ArticlePDF Available

The many faces of narcissism: Narcissism factors and their predictive utility

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Previous research has often portrayed narcissism as a unitary construct, however more recent research suggests it may be multidimensional. This study was conducted to examine the utility of two measures of narcissism - the Narcissistic Pathological Inventory and the Pathological Narcissism Inventory, in jointly assessing a broader range of narcissism content. The sample consisted of 220 undergraduate students. Eight factors were extracted from an exploratory analysis labeled: Contingent Self-Esteem, Grandiose Fantasy, Leadership/Authority, Devaluing the Self, Grandiose Exhibitionism, Manipulative, Entitlement, and Superiority. It was found that these narcissism factors had differing relationships with self-esteem, depression, anxiety, and stress. Although a higher-order factor structure did not have satisfactory fit, it is maintained that these eight factors reflect the two higher order dimensions of adaptive and maladaptive narcissism. It is recommended that future researchers construct their studies based on a multidimensional conceptualisation of narcissism, and use multiple narcissism measures.
Content may be subject to copyright.
The Many Faces of Narcissism:
Narcissism Factors and Their Predictive Utility
Indako E. Clarke a, Lisa Karlov a, Nicholas J. Neale a
This is a pre-publication version of:
Clarke, I. E., Karlov, L., & Neale, N. J. (2015). The many faces of narcissism: Narcissism factors and
their predictive utility. Personality and Individual Differences, 81, 90-95. DOI:
10.1016/j.paid.2014.11.021
This article may not exactly replicate the authoritative document published in the journal. It is not the
copy of record. The exact copy of record can be accessed via the DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.11.021
a School of Psychology, The University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia
Corresponding Author:
Indako Clarke
School of Psychology
Brennan MacCallum Building (A18)
The University of Sydney
NSW 2006 Australia
Email: indako.clarke@sydney.edu.au
2
Abstract
Previous research has often portrayed narcissism as a unitary construct, however more recent research
suggests it may be multidimensional. This study was conducted to examine the utility of two measures
of narcissism the Narcissistic Pathological Inventory and the Pathological Narcissism Inventory, in
jointly assessing a broader range of narcissism content. The sample consisted of 220 undergraduate
students. Eight factors were extracted from an exploratory analysis labeled: Contingent Self-Esteem,
Grandiose Fantasy, Leadership/Authority, Devaluing the Self, Grandiose Exhibitionism, Manipulative,
Entitlement, and Superiority. It was found that these narcissism factors had differing relationships with
self-esteem, depression, anxiety, and stress. Although a higher-order factor structure did not have
satisfactory fit, it is maintained that these eight factors reflect the two higher order dimensions of
adaptive and maladaptive narcissism. It is recommended that future researchers construct their studies
based on a multidimensional conceptualisation of narcissism, and use multiple narcissism measures.
3
1. Introduction
1.1 Conceptual dimensionality
Research into the conceptual dimensionality of narcissism is complex, with different taxonomic
levels at which narcissism can be examined. There is not yet agreement regarding the number of
dimensions that make up the construct. According to Ackerman et al. (2011), narcissism may be
broadly conceptualized in two higher order dimensions adaptive and maladaptive. Adaptive
narcissism is related to psychological health and resilience (Sedikides, Rudich, Gregg, Kumashiro, &
Rusbult, 2004) and maladaptive narcissism is related to entitlement and negative affect (Pincus et al.,
2009). This may be analogous to normal versus pathological narcissism (Pincus & Lukowitsky, 2010).
Normal narcissism has been associated with the ability to promote a positive self-image, seek out self-
enhancing experiences in social environments, and assert dominance in achievement related contexts
(Ackerman et al., 2011; Pincus & Lukowitsky, 2010). Pathological narcissism is related to problematic
self-regulation processes, and can be further broken down into grandiose and vulnerable dimensions
(Ackerman et al., 2011; Miller, Gentile, Wilson, & Campbell, 2013).
The grandiose dimension is associated with elements of grandiosity, aggression and entitlement,
whereas the vulnerable dimension is associated with feelings of inadequacy, negative affect and
incompetence (Miller et al., 2011). This grandiose/vulnerable distinction is widely supported in the
clinical literature (Cain, Pincus, & Ansell, 2008; Pincus et al., 2009). This two-factor model focusses
only on a maladaptive or pathological conceptualization of narcissism.
Other research has put forward models that may capture more adaptive traits associated with
narcissism. Russ, Shedler, Bradley, and Westen (2008) three-factor model consists of
Grandiose/Malignant, Fragile and High-Functioning Exhibitionist factors. The
Grandiose/Malignant and Fragile dimensions are similar to the grandiose/vulnerable distinction. The
third factor, High-Functioning Exhibitionism, appears to capture more beneficial or adaptive
narcissistic traits such as leadership ability, and outgoingness, accompanied by an excessive sense of
4
self-importance (Russ et al., 2008). Ackerman et al. (2011) also proposed a model consisting of three
factors Leadership/Authority, Grandiose Exhibitionism and Entitlement/Exploitativeness.
Adaptive narcissism traits are reflected by the Leadership/Authority factor. When compared to the two-
factor model, these three-factor models encompass a wider range of traits associated with both adaptive
and maladaptive narcissism. However adaptive narcissism profiles are often uncorrelated with
maladaptive profiles, thus researchers have questioned the whether an adaptive dimension should be
considered in the measurement of narcissism at all (Ackerman et al., 2011).
1.2 Measuring narcissism
In personality research, trait narcissism is considered a heterogeneous construct, and its
measurement should reflect this (Miller et al., 2013). However narcissism measures are often still
interpreted as a global score rather than specific factor scores (Horvath & Morf, 2010; Sedikides et al.,
2004). The use of global scores may lead to the loss of more nuanced relationships between narcissistic
dimensions and other personality variables. Despite this, individual differences research has recognized
the importance of a multidimensional approach that utilizes factor scores and a combination of
measures (Maxwell, Donnellan, Hopwood, & Ackerman, 2011).
Criticisms of dominant measures of narcissism have emerged in light of dimensional
approaches to narcissism. The Narcissism Personality Inventory (NPI; Raskin & Terry, 1988) is
criticized due to the poor internal reliability of its sub-scales, and the tendency for researchers to sum
across the sub-scales to create a global score (Brown, Budzek, & Tamborski, 2009). Some argue that
specific sub-scales related to grandiosity and entitlement should replace the NPI altogether (Brown et
al., 2009). Others suggest that the NPI should not be replaced because it accounts for more variance in
trait narcissism than these individual sub-scales (Miller, Price, & Campbell, 2012), and has strong
convergence with expert ratings of narcissistic personality disorder (Miller, Gaughan, Pryor, Kamen, &
Campbell, 2009).
5
The NPI is also criticized for its lack of a consistent factorial structure (Maxwell et al., 2011).
Numerous factor solutions have been found, such as the Emmons four-factor solution (Emmons, 1984;
Rhodewalt & Morf, 1995) and Raskin and Terry’s (1988) seven-factor solution. More recently,
Kubarych, Deary, and Austin (2004) have put forward a revised three-factor solution relating to
‘Power’, ‘Exhibitionism’ and being a ‘Special Person.’ The Pathological Narcissism Inventory (PNI;
Pincus et al., 2009) was created to address the lack of measurement tools assessing pathological
narcissism (Cain, Pincus, & Ansell, 2008). In general, the NPI assesses adaptive aspects of narcissism
while the PNI assesses maladaptive aspects of narcissism (Cain et al., 2008; Pincus et al., 2009).
1.3 Dimensions of narcissism and their nomological networks
Grandiose and vulnerable narcissism have different associations with a number of personality
variables including the Five Factor Model. Broadly, both converge in their association with
antagonistic interpersonal style, and diverge in their relationships with neuroticism and extraversion
(Miller et al., 2011). Grandiose narcissism is positively related to extraversion and negatively related to
neuroticism and agreeableness; whereas vulnerable narcissism is positively related to neuroticism and
negatively related to extraversion and agreeableness (Miller et al., 2011). Other research has found that
vulnerable narcissism is also closely related to psychopathy and Machiavellianism, unlike grandiose
narcissism (Egan, Chan, & Shorter, 2014). Grandiose and vulnerable narcissism have divergent
associations with psychological distress. Grandiose narcissism has been found to have no significant
relationship, or a negative relationship with symptoms of distress and negative affect (Miller et al.,
2011; Sedikides et al., 2004). Vulnerable narcissism is correlated with depression, anxiety, hostility,
paranoia and interpersonal sensitivity (Miller et al., 2011). Ackerman et al.’s (2011)
Leadership/Authority factor related to a number of positive personality traits. This factor had no
association with negative personality traits (e.g. Neuroticism) or with maladaptive aspects of narcissism
such as entitlement or anti-social tendencies, with the exception of a moderate negative correlation with
agreeableness (Ackerman et al., 2011).
6
A significant hurdle for narcissism research is its complex relationship with self-esteem. There
is a fundamental difference between those high in self-esteem compared to those high in narcissism.
Self-esteem is considered intra-personal e.g. feeling confident and self-assured; whereas narcissism is
considered interpersonal e.g. feeling superior to others and arrogant (Rosenthal & Hooley, 2010).
Evidence suggests narcissism is associated with higher reported self-esteem (Horvath & Morf, 2010),
however this relationship may be only true of adaptive narcissism (Ackerman et al., 2011), whereas
maladaptive narcissistic traits may have an inverse relationship with self-esteem (Pincus et al., 2009).
1.4 Aims and hypotheses
There were two broad aims of this study. The first was to clarify the dimensionality of
narcissism through its measures: the NPI and PNI. It was hypothesised that both the NPI and PNI
would contain a mix of adaptive and maladaptive content, and that this would be reflected in a two-
higher order latent factor structure. The second aim was to examine these narcissism dimensions in
their nomological network, including self-esteem, personality, depression, stress and anxiety. It was
hypothesised that the dimensions would have differential relationships with self-esteem in particular. It
was hypothesised that adaptive narcissism would be positively correlated with self-esteem, and
maladaptive narcissism would be negatively correlated with self-esteem.
2. Method
2.1 Participants
220 first-year undergraduate psychology students at a large Australian university (156 females,
Mage = 19.25, SDage = 3.16) received course credit in exchange for participation. 70 participants (31.8%)
identified as being Anglo-Celtic, and 96 (43.6%) participants identified as Asian.
2.2 Measures
2.2.1. Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI; Raskin & Terry, 1988)
7
The NPI is a 40-item self-report questionnaire. The response format was modified to a six-point
scale ranging from ‘not at all like me’, to ‘very much like me’. This modification is becoming more
frequent (Egan & Lewis, 2011). Reliability for the NPI was α = .92.
2.2.2. Pathological Narcissism Inventory (PNI; Pincus et al., 2009)
The PNI is a 52-item self-report questionnaire with the same response format as the modified
NPI. The PNI was constructed with seven subscales: Contingent Self-Esteem, Exploitative, Self-
Sacrificing Self-Enhancement, Hiding the Self, Grandiose Fantasy, Devaluing, and Entitlement
Rage’ (Pincus et al., 2009). Reliabilities for the subscales were α = .91, .75, .76, .77, .88, .81, and .82
respectively.
2.2.3. Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSE; Rosenberg, 1965)
The RSE is a commonly used 10-item questionnaire using a four-point Likert scale. Reliability
for the RSE was α = .85.
2.2.4. Big Five Inventory (BFI; John, Donahue, & Kentle, 1991)
The BFI assesses the five-factor model of personality using a five-point Likert scale. Reliability
for Extraversion was α = .84, for Agreeableness was α = .71, α = .78 for Conscientiousness, α = .81 for
Neuroticism, and α = .73 for Openness to Experience.
2.2.5. Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS; Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995)
The short version of the DASS is a 21-item scale measuring depression, anxiety and stress using
a four-point response scale. Respondents indicate how much a statement applied to them over the past
week, from ‘did not apply’ to ‘applied very much’, e.g. ‘I found it hard to wind down’. Reliability for
Depression was α = .85, α = .74 for Anxiety, and α = .83 for Stress.
2.3 Procedure
8
Following approval by the University of Sydney Human Research Ethics Committee,
participants were recruited through a university-administered website. Questionnaires were
administered by computer under supervised conditions on university campus.
2.4 Data analysis
The data was examined for implausible responses based on extreme or invariant responding.
Based on this examination, all responses from one respondent were removed (N = 219). Given the
hypothesis that both the NPI and PNI contain a mix of adaptive and maladaptive narcissism content,
items from both scales were jointly assessed in an exploratory factor analysis (EFA). As there are
identical items in the NPI and PNI, duplicates were removed from analysis, leaving 86 items in total.
Factors with eigenvalues greater than one were examined in conjunction with the scree plot. EFAs were
conducted using maximum likelihood extraction with promax rotation. Items that had both
communalities <.16, and factor loadings of <|.3| on all factors were deleted from the final factor
solution. Items that loaded on more than one factor were assigned to the factor with the highest loading.
Parallel analysis of 1000 bootstrap samples was conducted using Marley Watkins Monte Carlo PCA for
Parallel Analysis. Eight of the observed eigenvalues were larger than the 95th percentile of random
eigenvalues (see Table 1). Based on the scree plot, results of the parallel analysis, and theory, eight
factors were extracted.
A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted to assess the hypothesised two-higher
order latent factor structure of the eight factors extracted from the EFA. In addition, a three-higher
order latent factor CFA was conducted, intended to reflect adaptive, grandiose, and vulnerable
dimensions of narcissism.
Finally, a series of hierarchical regressions predicting depression, anxiety, stress, and self-
esteem were conducted. Gender, age, and personality were entered into the first block, and each of the
eight narcissism factors were entered separately into the second block. Narcissism factors were entered
separately due to issues of multicollinearity.
9
3. Results
3.1 Factor analyses
Eight factors were extracted in an EFA of NPI and PNI items, explaining 45.40% of the
variance. The extracted factors were labeled ‘Contingent Self-Esteem’ (Factor I), ‘Grandiose Fantasy
(II), ‘Leadership/Authority’ (III), ‘Devaluing the Self’ (IV), ‘Grandiose Exhibitionism’ (V),
‘Manipulative’ (VI), ‘Entitlement’ (VII), and ‘Superiority’ (VIII). Factor loadings and intercorrelations
are shown in Table 1.
Indices of fit for both the two- and three-higher order factor CFAs suggested that the models
had unsatisfactory fit (CFI = .84 and .83 respectively; RMSEA = .16 and .17). Further fit statistics are
available from the first author.
3.2 Descriptive statistics
The descriptive statistics for factor scores and additional variables of interest are shown in
Table 2.
3.3 Correlations
The correlations between factor scores and variables of interest are shown in Table 3. The
correlations show that each of the narcissism factors have differential relationships with psychological
distress, self-esteem, and personality. Leadership/Authority, Grandiose Exhibitionism, and Superiority
are correlated with higher self-esteem, and are not significantly correlated with psychological distress.
On the other hand, Contingent Self-Esteem, Grandiose Fantasy, and Devaluing the Self are correlated
with lower self-esteem, and higher reported levels of psychological distress.
3.4 Regression analyses
Four separate hierarchical regression analyses were conducted with Self-Esteem, Depression,
Anxiety and Stress as each of the outcome variables. Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness,
Neuroticism, Openness to Experience, Gender, and Age were entered into the first model as control
10
variables. The eight narcissism factors were entered into the second model to assess the incremental
predictive validity of these factors. The results of these analyses are shown in Table 4. The results show
that the narcissism factors account for a significant amount of additional variance in Self-Esteem,
Depression, Anxiety, and Stress, over and above personality, gender and age. Betas and semi-partial
correlations for the regression analyses are shown in Table 5. Devaluing the Self appears to be most
detrimental to psychological wellbeing, as it significantly predicts Self-Esteem, Depression, and Stress.
4. Discussion
The results of the present study provide some support for the heterogeneity of narcissism. Eight
narcissism factors were identified: Contingent Self-Esteem, Grandiose Fantasy, Leadership/Authority,
Devaluing the Self, Grandiose Exhibitionism, Manipulative, Entitlement, and Superiority. These factors
had differing relationships with other variables. The findings may help clarify the complex relationship
between narcissism and self-esteem (Cain et al., 2008; Rosenthal & Hooley, 2010).
Although a higher-order factor structure did not have satisfactory fit, it is maintained that the
factors identified in this study reflect a multidimensional conceptualisation of narcissism: Adaptive and
Maladaptive Narcissism. It is proposed that Maladaptive Narcissism is characterised by grandiosity
used as a compensation for low self-esteem. This may be best reflected by the Contingent Self-Esteem,
and Devaluing the Self factors. These factors positively predicted Depression and Stress, and
negatively predicted Self-Esteem over and above Neuroticism. These factors were also inversely
correlated with Extraversion, supporting Ronningstam’s (2005) ‘Shy narcissist’ typology, characterised
by interpersonal inhibitions, and low tolerance for exhibitionism. On the other hand, Adaptive
Narcissism is characterised by a sense of superiority and a preference for leadership roles. The
Leadership/Authority and Superiority factors may best reflect this dimension. These factors were
positively correlated with Self-Esteem, and negatively correlated with Neuroticism. They had no
association with Depression, Anxiety, and Stress. Based on these findings, it may be that Adaptive
Narcissism content is a distinctly different to ‘Narcissism’.
11
This study had several limitations. The use of self-report measures is of particular concern for
individuals who might display narcissistic traits. It is likely that self-deception is a critical aspect of
narcissistic functioning. The second limitation is related to issues of generalizability of the present
findings beyond an undergraduate sample. Additionally, the sample size restricted the types of analyses
that could be conducted regression was chosen over more sophisticated structural equation modelling
due to insufficient N. Research in larger, non-student samples is warranted.
It is recommended that future research should utilize multiple narcissism measures, in order to
gain a more holistic view of narcissism as a multi-dimensional construct. Another important area of
future research concerns the continuity of narcissism traits. In a taxometric analysis, Fossati et al.
(2005) found there was a discontinuity in the distribution of the DSM-IV NPD criteria (APA, 2000).
However, in a separate taxometric analysis, Foster and Campbell (2007) found support for a
dimensional view of narcissism. This is particularly important given the qualitatively different
associations of ‘Adaptive’ Narcissism with other variables. Further research in this area may change
our conceptions of both trait and clinical narcissism.
5. Conclusions
This study contributes to factor analyses of narcissism measures. It follows on from the
proposals for an adaptive/maladaptive distinction between narcissism measures and facets (Ackerman
et al., 2011). Most importantly, this study has been an empirical investigation of narcissism factors, and
provides evidence for a multidimensional conceptualisation of narcissism. Finally, evidence is provided
for the utility of narcissism factors in the prediction of psychological outcomes.
12
References
Ackerman, R. A., Witt, E. A., Donnellan, M. B., Trzesniewski, K. H., Robins, R. W., & Kashy, D. A.
(2011). What does the Narcissistic Personality Inventory really measure? Assessment, 18(1), 67-
87. doi:10.1177/1073191110382845
Brown, R. P., Budzek, K., & Tamborski, M. (2009). On the meaning and measure of narcissism.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35(7), 951-964. doi:10.1177/0146167209335461
Cain, N. M., Pincus, A. L., & Ansell, E. B. (2008). Narcissism at the crossroads: Phenotypic
description of pathological narcissism across clinical theory, social/personality psychology, and
psychiatric diagnosis. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(4), 638-656.
doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.09.006
Egan, V., Chan, S., & Shorter, G. W. (2014). The dark triad, happiness and subjective well-being.
Personality and Individual Differences, 67, 17-22. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.004
Egan, V., & Lewis, M. (2011). Neuroticism and agreeableness differentiate emotional and narcissistic
expressions of aggression. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(6), 845- 850.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.01.007
Emmons, R. A. (1984). Factor analysis and construct validity of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 48(3), 291-300. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa4803_11
Fossati, A., Beauchaine, T. P., Grazioli, F., Carretta, I., Cortinovis, F., & Maffei, C. (2005). A latent
structure analysis of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition,
Narcissistic Personality Disorder criteria. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 46(5), 361-367.
doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2004.11.006
Foster, J. D., & Campbell, W. K. (2007). Are there such things as “narcissists” in social psychology? A
taxometric analysis of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory. Personality and Individual
Differences, 43(6), 1321-1332. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.04.003
13
Horvath, S., & Morf, C. C. (2010). To be grandiose or not to be worthless: Different routes to self-
enhancement for narcissism and self-esteem. Journal of Research in Personality, 44(5), 585-592.
doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2010.07.002
John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., & Kentle, R. L. (1991). The Big Five Inventory Versions 4a and 54.
Berkeley, CA: University of California, Berkeley, Institute of Personality and Social Research.
Kubarych, T. S., Deary, I. J., & Austin, E. J. (2004). The Narcissistic Personality Inventory: Factor
structure in a non-clinical sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 36(4), 857-872.
doi:10.1016/S0191-8869%2803%2900158-2
Lovibond, P. F., & Lovibond, S. H. (1995). Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (2nd ed.).
Sydney, Australia: The Psychology Foundation of Australia.
Maxwell, K., Donnellan, M. B., Hopwood, C. J., & Ackerman, R. A. (2011). The two faces of
Narcissus? An empirical comparison of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory and the
Pathological Narcissism Inventory. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(5), 577- 582.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.11.031
Miller, J. D., Gaughan, E. T., Pryor, L. R., Kamen, C., & Campbell, W. K. (2009). Is research using the
Narcissistic Personality Inventory relevant for understanding Narcissistic Personality Disorder?
Journal of Research in Personality, 43(3), 482-488. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2009.02.001
Miller, J. D., Gentile, B., Wilson, L., & Campbell, W. K. (2013). Grandiose and vulnerable narcissism
and the DSM5 pathological personality trait model. Journal of Personality Assessment, 95(3),
284-290. doi:10.1080/00223891.2012.685907
Miller, J. D., Hoffman, B. J., Gaughan, E. T., Gentile, B., Maples, J., & Keith Campbell, W. (2011).
Grandiose and vulnerable narcissism: A nomological network analysis. Journal of personality,
79(5), 1013-1042. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00711.x
14
Miller, J. D., Price, J., & Campbell, W. K. (2012). Is the Narcissistic Personality Inventory still
relevant? A test of independent grandiosity and entitlement scales in the assessment of
narcissism. Assessment, 19(1), 8-13. doi:10.1177/1073191111429390
Pincus, A. L., Ansell, E. B., Pimentel, C. A., Cain, N. M., Wright, A. G. C., & Levy, K. N. (2009).
Initial construction and validation of the Pathological Narcissism Inventory. Psychological
Assessment, 21(3), 365-379. doi:10.1037/a0016530
Pincus, A. L., & Lukowitsky, M. R. (2010). Pathological narcissism and narcissistic personality
disorder. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 421-446.
doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131215
Raskin, R., & Terry, H. (1988). A principal-components analysis of the Narcissistic Personality
Inventory and further evidence of its construct validity. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 54(5), 890-902. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.5.890
Rhodewalt, F., & Morf, C. C. (1995). Self and interpersonal correlates of the Narcissistic Personality
Inventory: A review and new findings. Journal of Research in Personality, 29(1), 1-23.
doi:10.1006/jrpe.1995.1001
Ronningstam, E. F. (2005). Identifying and Understanding the Narcissistic Personality. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University
Press.
Rosenthal, S. A., & Hooley, J. M. (2010). Narcissism assessment in social-personality research: Does
the association between narcissism and psychological health result from a confound with self-
esteem? Journal of Research in Personality, 44(4), 453-465. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2010.05.008
Russ, E., Shedler, J., Bradley, R., & Westen, D. (2008). Refining the construct of Narcissistic
Personality Disorder: Diagnostic criteria and subtypes. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(11),
15
1473-1481. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07030376
Sedikides, C., Rudich, E. A., Gregg, A. P., Kumashiro, M., & Rusbult, C. (2004). Are normal
narcissists psychologically healthy?: Self-esteem matters. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 87(3), 400-416. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.3.400
16
Table 1
Rotated factor loadings for NPI and PNI joint EFA, eigenvalues, and factor intercorrelations.
I
II
III
IV
V
VI
VIII
PNI40 I am disappointed when people don’t notice me
.79
PNI16 When others don’t notice me, I start to feel worthless
.77
PNI36 It’s hard for me to feel good about myself unless I know other people like me
.77
PNI30 It’s hard to feel good about myself unless I know other people admire me
.75
PNI48 I need others to acknowledge me
.73
PNI08 When people don’t notice me, I start to feel bad about myself
.72
PNI32 I am preoccupied with thoughts and concerns that most people are not interested in
me
.71
PNI37 It irritates me when people don’t notice how good a person I am
.63
PNI47 When others don’t respond to me in the way I would like them to, it is hard for me to
still feel okay with myself
.59
PNI05 It’s hard to feel good about myself when I’m alone
.59
PNI19 I sometimes need important others in my life to reassure me of my self-worth
.57
PNI34 Sometimes I avoid people because I’m concerned they won’t acknowledge what I do
for them
.54
PNI33 I like to have friends who rely on my because it makes me feel important
.45
PNI02 My self-esteem fluctuates a lot
.43
PNI39 I try to show what a good person I am through my sacrifices
.42
PNI22 I feel important when others rely on me
.40
PNI45 I often fantasize about being recognized for my accomplishments
.79
PNI31 I often fantasize about being rewarded for my efforts
.74
PNI14 I often fantasize about having a huge impact on the world around me
.74
PNI42 I often fantasize about performing heroic deeds
.69
PNI26 I often fantasize about accomplishing things that are probably beyond my means
.68
PNI01 I often fantasize about being admired and respected
.62
PNI41 I often find myself envying others’ accomplishments
.60
PNI44 It’s important to show people I can do it on my own, even if I have doubts inside
.47
PNI43 I help others in order to prove I’m a good person
.40
PNI25 Sacrificing for others makes me the better person
.33
NPI36 I am a born leader
.81
NPI33 I would prefer to be a leader
.79
NPI10 I see myself as a good leader
.76
NPI11 I am assertive
.64
NPI32 People always seem to recognize my authority
.64
NPI12 I like having authority over people
.62
NPI18 I want to amount to something in the eyes of the world
.56
NPI17 I like to take responsibility for making decisions
.51
NPI27 I have a strong will to power
.44
NPI23 Everybody likes to hear my stories
.40
PNI21 When others don’t meet my expectations, I often feel ashamed about what I wanted
.71
PNI24 When others disappoint me, I often get angry at myself
.66
PNI03 I sometimes feel ashamed about my expectations of others when they disappoint me
.61
PNI50 When others get a glimpse of my needs, I feel anxious and ashamed
.57
PNI17 Sometimes I avoid people because I’m concerned that they’ll disappoint me
.57
PNI46 I can’t stand relying on other people because it makes me feel weak
.54
PNI27 Sometimes I avoid people because I’m afraid they won’t do what I want them to
.52
PNI09 I often hide my needs for fear that others will see me as needy and dependent
.50
PNI51 Sometimes it’s easier to be alone than to face not getting everything I want from
others
.47
PNI28 It’s hard to show others the weaknesses I feel inside
.44
17
I
II
III
IV
V
VI
VIII
PNI07 I hate asking for help
.34
NPI29 I like to look at myself in the mirror
.75
NPI19 I like to look at my body
.71
NPI30 I really like to be the center of attention
.65
NPI15 I like to display my body
.63
NPI20 I am apt to show off if I get the chance
.61
NPI07 I like to be the center of attention
.53
NPI28 I like to start new fads and fashions
.48
NPI04 I know that I am good because everybody keeps telling me so
.37
NPI31 I can live my life in any way that I want to
.33
NPI13 I find it easy to manipulate people
.84
NPI35 I can make anybody believe anything I want them to
.75
NPI06 I can usually talk my way out of anything
.71
NPI01 I have a natural talent for influencing people
.67
PNI10 I can make anyone believe anything I want them to
.67
NPI16 I can read people like a book
.56
PNI11 I get mad when other people don’t notice all that I do for them
PNI20 When I do things for other people, I expect them to do things for me
NPI25 I will never be satisfied until I get all that I deserve
PNI52 I can get pretty angry when others disagree with me
PNI18 I typically get very angry when I’m unable to get what I want from others
NPI24 I expect a great deal from other people
PNI12 I get annoyed by people who are not interested in what I say or do
NPI26 I like to be complimented
NPI38 I get upset when people don’t notice how I look when I go out in public
NPI14 I insist upon getting the respect that is due to me
PNI29 I get angry when criticized
NPI37 I wish somebody would someday write my biography
NPI34 I am going to be a great person
.76
NPI40 I am an extraordinary person
.73
NPI08 I will be a success
.61
NPI09 I think I am a special person
.58
NPI39 I am more capable than other people
.46
NPI05 If I ruled the world it would be a much better place
.36
Eigenvalue No.
Observed
eigenvalues
Random eigenvalues
Factor intercorrelations
I
II
III
IV
V
VI
VIII
1
15.711
2.537
I: Contingent Self-Esteem
2
8.777
2.419
II: Grandiose Fantasy
.60
3
4.023
2.328
III: Leadership/Authority
.09
.27
4
3.348
2.251
IV: Devaluing the Self
.60
.46
.08
5
2.867
2.183
V: Grandiose Exhibitionism
.21
.28
.50
.12
6
2.615
2.120
VI: Manipulative
.10
.14
.56
.18
.30
7
2.234
2.062
VII: Entitlement
.58
.49
.35
.36
.52
.27
8
2.115
2.010
VIII: Superiority
.09
.28
.55
-.06
.51
.40
Note. N = 219; NPI = Narcissistic Personality Inventory; PNI = Pathological Narcissism Inventory. Factor loadings <|.3| are not shown.
All factor intercorrelations r >|.13| are significant at p <.05 (two-tailed).
18
Table 2
Descriptive statistics for narcissism factors and variables of interest
Mean
SD
Extraversion
17.16
5.55
Agreeableness
23.92
4.80
Conscientiousness
19.34
5.46
Neuroticism
17.63
5.47
Openness to Experience
24.97
5.22
Self-Esteem
18.51
4.65
Depression
5.49
4.51
Anxiety
4.44
3.81
Stress
7.19
4.78
Contingent Self-Esteem
2.15
.88
Grandiose Fantasy
2.72
.93
Leadership/Authority
2.38
.86
Devaluing the Self
1.99
.87
Grandiose Exhibitionism
1.93
.85
Manipulative
2.19
.98
Entitlement
2.07
.78
Superiority
2.50
.92
Note. N = 219.
Table 3
Correlations between narcissism factors and variables of interest.
Gender
Age
Extraversion
Agreeableness
Conscientiousness
Neuroticism
Openness to
Experience
Depression
Anxiety
Stress
Self-Esteem
Contingent Self-Esteem
.00
-.17*
-.18**
-.04
-.13
.42**
.05
.49**
.38**
.35**
-.49**
Grandiose Fantasy
-.06
-.06
.02
.01
-.10
.29**
.21**
.28**
.25**
.25**
-.26**
Leadership/Authority
-.19**
-.03
.47**
-.04
.16*
-.18**
.30**
-.06
-.09
.08
.27**
Devaluing the Self
-.10
-.08
-.25**
-.17*
-.15*
.40**
.05
.53**
.37**
.39**
-.52**
Grandiose Exhibitionism
-.27**
.01
.23**
-.15*
-.15*
-.11
.04
.01
-.04
.03
.28**
Manipulative
-.08
-.01
.27**
-.17*
-.07
-.10
.28**
.08
.08
.17**
.07
Entitlement
-.17*
.00
.01
-.23**
-.06
.25**
-.02
.25**
.19**
.28**
-.10
Superiority
-.27**
.06
.15*
.01
.07
-.19**
.22**
-.03
.00
.05
.40**
Note. N = 219; Gender coded 0 = male, 1 = female.
* Significant at p <.05.
** Significant at p <.01.
Table 4
Model summaries for four hierarchical regression analyses.
Outcome Variable
R2
ΔR2
F7,211
ΔF8,203
p
Self-Esteem
Model 1
.44
-
23.97
-
<.001
Model 2
.64
.20
24.49
14.34
<.001
Depression
Model 1
.31
-
13.44
-
<.001
Model 2
.43
.12
10.13
5.32
<.001
Anxiety
Model 1
.24
-
9.28
-
<.001
Model 2
.30
.07
5.85
2.41
.02
Stress
Model 1
.36
-
16.72
-
<.001
Model 2
.42
.06
9.64
2.58
.01
Note. N = 219; Model 1 predictor variables: Age, Gender, Extraversion, Agreeableness,
Conscientiousness, Neuroticism, Openness to Experience; Model 2 additional predictor variables:
Contingent Self-Esteem, Grandiose Fantasy, Leadership/Authority, Devaluing the Self, Grandiose
Exhibitionism, Manipulative, Entitlement, Superiority.
21
Table 5
Betas and semi-partial correlations between narcissism factors and outcome variables in full
hierarchical regression model (Model 2).
Self-Esteem
Depression
Anxiety
Stress
β
sr
β
sr
β
sr
β
sr
Contingent Self-Esteem
-.27**
-.16
.21*
.13
.14
.08
.02
.01
Grandiose Fantasy
-.06
-.04
-.06
-.05
-.01
-.004
-.07
-.05
Leadership/Authority
-.09
-.05
-.03
-.02
-.16
-.09
.01
.01
Devaluing the Self
-.16**
-.11
.26**
.18
.16
.11
.18*
.12
Grandiose Exhibitionism
.21**
.15
-.03
-.02
-.02
-.02
-.05
-.03
Manipulative
-.08
-.06
.08
.06
.13
.09
.11
.08
Entitlement
.05
.03
-.03
-.02
-.01
-.01
.05
.03
Superiority
.30**
.21
.07
.05
.12
.08
.11
.08
Note. N = 219; Model 2 variables (see Table 4).
* β significant at p<.05.
** β significant at p<.01.
... However, consistent with the idea that self-regulation is central to narcissism (Morf & Rhodewalt, 2001), research has shown that regulatory modes may also underlie different narcissistic feelings (Boldero et al., 2015). In turn, narcissism affects various individuals' well-being outcomes (Clarke et al., 2015). It is thus possible that narcissism could mediate the effect of the regulatory modes on positivity. ...
... In other words, depending on the type of narcissism experienced (see also Boldero et al., 2015) by locomotors (i.e., individuals who have a high locomotion orientation) and assessors (i.e., individuals who have a high assessment orientation), the effects of regulatory modes on positivity may vary. Accordingly, given its multidimensional nature, narcissism is presumed to have opposite effects on wellbeing outcomes, depending on its "adaptive" or "maladaptive" nature (e.g., Clarke et al., 2015). An aim of the present work was to examine different dimensions of narcissism in mediating the effects of regulatory modes on positivity. ...
... Literature in clinical and personality psychology has widely supported a multidimensional conceptualization of narcissism (e.g., Miller et al., 2011;Pincus et al., 2009;Wink, 1991). Accordingly, Clarke et al. (2015) identified eight dimensions that reflected adaptive or maladaptive content of narcissism, corresponding to: (1) Leadership/Authority, (2) Superiority, (3) Grandiose Exhibitionism, (4) Contingent Self-Esteem, (5) Devaluing the Self, (6) Grandiose Fantasy, (7) Manipulative, and (8) Entitlement. Of such dimensions, Clarke et al. (2015) suggests that Leadership/Authority and Superiority would reflect adaptive narcissism, as they were positively correlated with selfesteem, and negatively correlated with neuroticism. ...
Article
Full-text available
Positivity (i.e., the individual tendency to positively approach life experiences) has proven to be an effective construct applied in positive psychology. However, individuals’ self-regulation may have contrasting effects on positivity. We specifically examined whether positivity could be partially explained through two aspects of motivation concerned with self-regulation: locomotion (i.e., a motivational orientation concerned with movement) and assessment (i.e., a motivational orientation concerned with comparison and evaluation). Furthermore, based on previous literature that found a link between these aspects and narcissism, we examined whether “adaptive” and “maladaptive” dimensions of narcissism could mediate the effects of locomotion and assessment on increased or decreased positivity. Narcissism was defined by previous research as adaptive or maladaptive insofar as it leads or does not lead to increased psychological well-being. We estimated a mediation model with multiple independent variables and multiple mediators in a cross-sectional study with self-reported data from 190 university students. We found that both locomotion and assessment were associated with adaptive narcissism, which in turn was positively associated with positivity. However, assessment was also associated with maladaptive narcissism, which in turn was negatively associated with positivity. Relationships between aspects of self-regulation, narcissism, and positivity can have significant implications which will be discussed.
... Nevertheless, a growing body of research connects narcissism to some positive outcomes, such as increased leadership effectiveness and confidence (Pincus & Lukowitsky, 2010). Given this dichotomy, scholars have called for more research examining the multiple facets of narcissism (Back et al., 2013;Brown et al., 2009;Clarke et al., 2015). Thotigh narcissism is often considered pathological in nature, as with liarcissistic personality disorder (Ainerican Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013), it also exists as a non-pathological trait with various personal and relati(mal implications (Derry et al., 2017; Morf & Rhodewalt, 2001). ...
... the influences and practical implications of trait narcissism. The findings of this study address the call for increased understanding of narcissism as multidimensional (Back et al., 2013;Brown et al., 2009;Clarke et al., 2015) and may assist mental health counselors iii providing proper conceptualization and treatment for clients with high trait narcissism. Miller and Campbell (2010) described how narcissistic itidividlials' actions and thoughts are often purposed to increase their own self-esteem, Indeed, individuals higher in trait narcissism engage iii increased selfpromotion (Raskin & Shaw, 1988) and are found less likable over time as self-.J ournal of Mental Health Counseling centeredness is exhibited (Paulhus, 1998). ...
Article
Full-text available
Research concerning the role of narcissism in influencing perceptions of situations is sparse. In this study, the daily influences of two components of trait narcissism, admiration and rivalry, were explored using narrative life-logging cameras. We examined the influences of admiration and rivalry on perceptions of situation desire, situation choice, momentary self-esteem, authenticity, and the DIAMONDS (Duty, Intellect, Adversity, Mating, pOsitivity, Negativity, Deception, and Sociality) characteristics of situations among a sample of undergraduate students. Rivalry was found to be predictive of lower situation desire and choice, while admiration and overall narcissism were not. Admiration showed a significant positive association with both momentary self-esteem and momentary authenticity, while rivalry showed a negative association with these two variables. Patterns emerged demonstrating that the two facets, admiration and rivalry, differentially predict perceptions of the DIAMONDS characteristics of situations. This research further illuminates the need to study narcissism as multidimensional and provides various implications for counselors working with clients who display narcissistic tendencies.
... The NPI is a self-report measure in which participants choose the most representative of two statements for a total of 40 statement pairs. Though debated (see Samuel & Widiger, 2008), the NPI is supported to have adequate construct validity when the global score is used (which explains greater variance in narcissism; Clarke, Karlov, & Neale, 2015). The NPI is argued to contain more psychometric data than the Pathological Narcissism Inventory (Cain et al., 2008). ...
... Furthermore, narcissism was measured only as a personality trait via a scale with debated validity (Samuel & Wi-diger, 2008). However, the NPI is generally preferred to the Pathological Narcissism Index in samples that would not score in the clinically significant range (Clarke et al., 2015;Pincus et al., 2009). Given that narcissism exists on a spectrum from populationwide personality traits, which may actually be adaptive (Leaf et al., 1990), to a debilitating personality disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 2013;Emmons, 1987), recruiting and assessing for clinically significant levels of narcissistic personality disorder would be beneficial to further clarify the relationship between the right MPFC and narcissism. ...
Article
Though debated, it has been argued that self-face recognition is an indicator of consciousness of self typically called self-awareness. Evidence from behavioral and neuronal studies suggests that self-recognition is associated with activation in the right prefrontal cortex (PFC). Although previous studies have examined neurological correlates of deficits in self-recognition (e.g., autism, schizophrenia), the current study attempted to examine neurological correlates of inflated self-focus. Thus, this study used transcranial magnetic stimulation to assess neurological correlates of self-recognition in different regions of the PFC and to assess whether narcissistic personality traits would be correlated with reaction time (RT) among 11 healthy participants. It was found that RT for self-face identification was positively correlated with narcissistic personality traits (p = .034). It was also found that the interaction between brain region and RT for self-faces was significant after narcissism was controlled for, demonstrating an association between narcissistic traits and self-recognition (p = .036). The results support previous research indicating the right PFC is necessary for self-face recognition. Furthermore, increases in narcissistic traits decreased self-recognition RT, and self-recognition appeared to be mediated via right PFC regions.
... The empirical research complements this notion, where research studies have consistently found that narcissistic individuals distort their perceptions and reconstruct their past to maintain a positive self-image (Foster & Campbell, 2005;Holtzman et al., 2010;Rhodewalt & Eddings, 2002). Clarke, Karlov and Neale (2015) stress that research that relies solely on selfreported data from narcissistic individuals is of particular concern given that selfdeception is a critical feature of narcissistic functioning. For these reasons, studies have implemented triangulation in narcissism research (NPI) where multiple perspectives and their convergences are considered in order to understand the personality construct Campbell et al., 2002). ...
Thesis
Despite its longevity as a personality construct, theoretical understandings of gender differences in narcissistic presentation are underdeveloped given the overemphasis of grandiose features indicative of the male gender. The existing literature is also fragmented across empirical and clinical subfields, with inconsistent conceptualisations regarding an assumed heterogeneous construct encompassing grandiose and vulnerable features. In this context, this thesis aims to enhance theoretical knowledge regarding gender differences in grandiose and vulnerable narcissism through undertaking three distinct but interrelated studies. The focus was specifically on parenting styles in the development of narcissism and variances in self-esteem regulation within Intimate Partner Violence, and the gender bias of narcissistic pathology as captured in the psychiatric nomenclature. Results demonstrate that hypothetical patients with vulnerable narcissism symptomatology are being (mis)diagnosed as having other ‘vulnerable disorders’, findings which may contribute to the observed gender bias in the psychiatric nomenclature (Study 1). Converging evidence demonstrates gender differences linking females to vulnerable features of narcissism (Study 2 and Study 3). Retrospective accounts of childrearing experiences generated findings which associated different parenting styles with manifestations of narcissism and partner violence outcomes in each gender, further elucidating the underling construct of grandiose and vulnerable narcissism (Study 2). The complexity of narcissism is revealed, as gender roles were perceived to shape self-regulatory strategies in females to obtain their self-worth (Study 3). It is concluded that gender socialisation processes play an important role in producing these gender differences, impacting on the diagnostic assessment, development, and manifestation of narcissism. It is recommended that a significant theoretical re-synthesis is required to capture gender issues in narcissism at the level of conceptualisation and clinical treatment, and integrate the disjointed subfields. Limitations of the thesis are identified and suggestions for future research made.
... Narcissism has frequently been shown to be a complex and multi-dimensional construct (Clarke et al., 2015). Accordingly, we explored whether the relationship between narcissism and entrepreneurship differs across the three dimensions of narcissism proposed by Ackerman et al. (2011). ...
Article
Full-text available
Widespread attention is being paid to the alleged rise of narcissism in people in general and business leaders in particular. Surprisingly, hardly any studies have focused on the link between narcissism and entrepreneurship. Using self-reported data from 4798 respondents from three countries, we explore the associations between trait narcissism and six different entrepreneurial aspects that represent the entire entrepreneurial process. Overall, our findings suggest that a positive link exists between narcissism and entrepreneurship that is particularly salient in the early stage of the entrepreneurial process (e.g., entrepreneurial intention) and in the individual aspects of entrepreneurship (e.g., entrepreneurial orientation, well-being of the entrepreneur). Our additional analyses reveal that the adaptive aspect of narcissism (i.e., leadership/authority) is most consistently linked to entrepreneurship and that the links between narcissism and entrepreneurship are predominately linear. Finally, our findings are largely robust when different sets of controls are added.
Article
Full-text available
Penelitian ini bertujuan untuk menentukan hubungan antara self-esteem dengan narsistik. Partisipan dalam penelitian ini adalah siswa-siswi kelas 12 IPS SMAK Penabur Cirebon dengan jumlah 58 orang. Pengumpulan data menggunakan skala self-esteem yang terdiri dari 51 butir dan skala narsistik yang terdiri dari 19 butir dan keduanya telah memenuhi syarat reliabilitas yang baik. Analisis data menggunakan teknik korelasi Pearson product moment. Hasil penelitian ini menunjukan koefisien korelasi sebesar -0.749 dengan nilai signifikansi 0,000 (p< 0,05), artinya terdapat hubungan negatif yang signifikan antara self-esteem dengan narsistik yang berarti semakin tinggi self-esteem maka akan semakin rendah narsistik, demikian sebaliknya semakin rendah self-esteem maka semakin tinggi narsistik. Melalui penelitian ini diharapkan remaja dapat mempertahankan self-esteem yang tinggi dan memiliki perilaku yang membuat self-esteem tetap stabil dengan mengetahui kelebihan dan kekurangan pada diri sendiri, agar menghindari munculnya perilaku narsistik.
Article
Purpose Utilizing a self-regulatory perspective, the authors examine how narcissism influences perceived negative inequity and the downstream effects on self-enhancement motivation and organizational citizenship behaviors (OCBs) over time. Design/methodology/approach A total of 689 useable three-wave panel data were obtained via Amazon Mechanical Turk during a three-month period. A latent growth curve modeling approach using EQS 6.4 for Windows was employed to test the hypothesized model. Findings Results illustrate that individuals with higher levels of narcissism perceive higher levels of perceived negative inequity and then form higher levels of self-enhancement motivation, which prompt more OCB directed toward the organization (OCBO) than OCB directed toward individuals (OCBI). When perceived negative inequity increases over time, narcissists experience a faster increase in self-enhancement motivation, which also leads to a faster increase in OCBO compared to the increase in OCBI. Originality/value Theoretically, this study provides theoretical and empirical insights into understanding the process through which narcissists' OCBs are motivated. Practically, this study offers several practical recommendations that help managers manage OCBs effectively in the organization.
Article
The study of emotional intelligence (EI) and its relationship with the dark triad has emerged as a popular research area. However, the complex nature of the dark triad and EI, including multiple measures for assessment, has led to inconsistent findings. A systematic review was conducted to focus on the multifaceted nature of the dark triad traits. Included studies must have been conducted with adult samples using standardized EI and dark triad measures. Forty-eight studies were identified; all bar one reported overall negative associations between the dark triad and EI. These associations were more complicated than expected. Further examination found these relationships significantly differed when examined at the facet level. Our results highlight that future research requires more than a one-size-fits-all approach.
Article
Purpose The primary purpose is to investigate the relationship between narcissism and managers' overconfidence in listed companies' risk-taking. Design/methodology/approach In this study, two criteria of signature and reward are used to measure manager's narcissism; manager's overconfidence, using multiple regression models and finally to measure companies' risk-taking by using companies' monthly returns. Multiple regression is employed to test the model using a sample of 890 firm-year participation on the Tehran Stock Exchange from 2012 to 2017 with panel data and model with fixed effects. Findings The findings indicate that the CEO's narcissism and the board of directors positively and significantly affect corporate risk-taking. Also, managers' overconfidence has a positive and significant relationship with corporate risk-taking. Originality/value The results of this study identified other factors affecting companies' risk-taking. This study also contributed to the development of the literature on narcissism, overconfidence and corporate risk-taking.
Book
Full-text available
Bu bilimsel kitapta; geçmişten günümüze liderlik kavramı, liderlik teorileri, liderlik tarzları, klasik liderlik tarzları, modern liderlik tarzları, post-modern liderlik tarzları, liderliğin yeni paradigmaları, değerler yönünde liderlik tarzları, destekleyici liderlik tarzları, insan odaklı liderlik tarzları, kaotik liderlik tarzları, negatif liderlik tarzları derli toplu, geniş bir şekilde gruplandırılarak paylaşılmaya çalışılmıştır. Kitap içeriği; Liderlik Kavramına Genel Bir Bakış, Liderlikle İlgili Temel Teoriler, Otokratik Liderlik, Demokratik Liderlik, Bürokratik Liderlik, Dönüşümcü Liderlik, Etkileşimci Liderlik, Karizmatik Liderlik, Durumsal Liderlik, Holistik (Bütünleştirici) Liderlik, Stratejik Liderlik, Vizyoner Liderlik, Öğretimsel Liderlik, Siyasi Liderlik, Girişimci Liderlik, Yenilikçi Liderlik, Kültürel Liderlik, Teknolojik Liderlik, Çift Yetenekli Liderlik, Kuantum Liderlik, Etik Liderlik, Manevi Liderlik, Otantik Liderlik, Koçluk Stili Liderlik, Mentorluk Tarzı Liderlik, Güçlendirici Liderlik, Takım Liderliği, Kolaylaştırıcı Liderlik, İlham Verici Liderlik, Hizmetkâr Liderlik, Empatik Liderlik, Paternalist (Babacan) Liderlik, Dağıtımcı Liderlik, Öz Liderlik, Kriz Liderliği, Adaptif Liderlik, Toksik Liderlik, Saldırgan Liderlik, Karanlık Liderlik, Yıkıcı Liderlik, Diktatör Liderlik, Narsistik Liderlik, Örneklerle Liderlik Becerileri ve Liderlik Tarzlarının İş Davranışlarına Etkisi olmak üzere 44 bölümden oluşmaktadır.
Article
Full-text available
Narcissism can be expressed in grandiose or vulnerable forms. We examined whether positive psychological states (defined by the Oxford Happiness Inventory (OHI) and the Diener Satisfaction With Life (SWL) scales) assisted differentiation relative to general personality traits and the ''the Dark Triad'' (psychopathy, narcissism, and Machiavellianism, measured by the D12 and Short Dark Triad (SD3) indices) for 840 persons primarily from the UK, USA and Canada. The best fitting structural equation model comprised two latent variables, one of positive mood (comprising total scores on the OHI and SWL scales), and another forming a ''dark dyad'' of Machiavellianism and psychopathy (predicted by low agreeableness and lower positive mood), with narcissism regarded as a separate construct correlated with the dark dyad. Latent positive mood was primarily predicted by higher emotional stability and extraversion. Narcissism was predicted by lower emotional stability, lower agreeableness, and higher extraversion. Latent profile analysis identified four groups in the data: ''unhappy but not narcissistic'', ''vulnerable narcissism'', ''happy non-narcissism'' and ''grandiose narcissism''. Our results suggest more problematic narcissism can be identified by reference to measures indexing positive mood states and general personality traits.
Article
Full-text available
The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Personality Disorders (4th ed., American Psychiatric Association, 2000) personality disorders (PDs) that will be included in the DSM-5 will be diagnosed in an entirely different manner; the explicit criterion sets will be replaced with impairments in self and interpersonal functioning and personality traits from a 25-trait dimensional model of personality pathology. From a trait perspective, narcissistic personality disorder (NPD), the focus of this study, is assessed using 2 specific traits: grandiosity and attention seeking. Using a sample collected online from Amazon's Mechanical Turk (MTurk; N = 306), we examined the relations among traits from a new measure of DSM-5's trait model-the Personality Inventory for DSM-5 (PID5; Krueger, Derringer, Markon, Watson, & Skodol, in press)-and grandiose and vulnerable narcissism. The 25 traits from PID5 captured a significant portion of the variance in grandiose and vulnerable factors, although the 2 specific facets designated for the assessment of NPD fared substantially better in the assessment of grandiose rather than vulnerable narcissism. These results are discussed in the context of improving the DSM-5's ability to capture both narcissism dimensions.
Article
Shame, narcissism, and self-esteem ostensibly generate an ego threat construct claimed to underlie provocation, frustration, and self-reported aggression. Measures of these variables were examined in relation to the five factor model of personality to clarify their actual influence on aggression. Results from 150 individuals found an integrated ego threat construct could not be generated from the data that notionally indexed the components of this concept. Shame, provocation, and frustration loaded with hostility on an affective aggression dimension, whereas subscales of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory, anger, and verbal and physical aggression from the Aggression Questionnaire produced a narcissistic aggression dimension. Self-esteem did not relate to either factor. Path analysis found affective aggression predicted by Neuroticism alone, whereas narcissistic aggression was underpinned by low agreeableness, extraversion, and masculinity. Our study shows that ego psychology models exaggerate emotion’s influence on expressive aggression.
Article
ABSTRACT Evidence has accrued to suggest that there are 2 distinct dimensions of narcissism, which are often labeled grandiose and vulnerable narcissism. Although individuals high on either of these dimensions interact with others in an antagonistic manner, they differ on other central constructs (e.g., Neuroticism, Extraversion). In the current study, we conducted an exploratory factor analysis of 3 prominent self-report measures of narcissism (N=858) to examine the convergent and discriminant validity of the resultant factors. A 2-factor structure was found, which supported the notion that these scales include content consistent with 2 relatively distinct constructs: grandiose and vulnerable narcissism. We then compared the similarity of the nomological networks of these dimensions in relation to indices of personality, interpersonal behavior, and psychopathology in a sample of undergraduates (n=238). Overall, the nomological networks of vulnerable and grandiose narcissism were unrelated. The current results support the need for a more explicit parsing of the narcissism construct at the level of conceptualization and assessment.
Article
Areas of convergence and divergence between the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI; Raskin & Terry, 1988) and the Pathological Narcissism Inventory (PNI; Pincus et al., 2009) were evaluated in a sample of 586 college students. Summary scores for the NPI and PNI were not strongly correlated (r = .22) but correlations between certain subscales of these two inventories were larger (e.g., r = .71 for scales measuring Exploitativeness). Both measures had a similar level of correlation with the Narcissistic Personality Disorder scale from the Personality Diagnostic Questionnaire-4 (Hyler, 1994) (r = .40 and .35, respectively). The NPI and PNI diverged, however, with respect to their associations with Explicit Self-Esteem. Self-esteem was negatively associated with the PNI but positively associated with the NPI (r = −.34 versus r = .26). Collectively, the results highlight the need for precision when discussing the personality characteristics associated with narcissism.
Article
Both narcissists and high self-esteem individuals engage in active self-enhancement to support their positive self-views. However, while narcissists want to assert their superiority, high self-esteem individuals desire to be valued by the social community. These different self-goals suggest that only narcissists can afford to engage in forceful and brazen self-enhancement strategies. Consistent with expectation, in two studies, narcissists exploited self-enhancement opportunities primarily by augmenting self-ratings on positive traits. Individuals with genuine high self-esteem in contrast, self-presented more moderately and also used the more socially accepted discounting of negative traits. Subsequent increased accessibility of positive self-information, only shown by narcissists, indicates that their desire for self-worth is hard to fulfill. These findings continue to illuminate the distinction between narcissism and self-esteem.
Article
Most research on narcissism has been conducted using the narcissistic personality inventory (NPI). However, the generalizability of findings from the NPI to narcissistic personality disorder (NPD) is unknown. The current study uses two samples (48 clinical outpatients; 49 undergraduates) to address this question by examining the correlations between the NPI and interview ratings of NPD. The profiles generated by both assessments in relation to measures of general personality traits are compared to expert and meta-analytically derived profiles of NPD. The NPI and NPD assess overlapping constructs as they are significantly correlated and generate similar personality profiles. The measures diverge primarily in that NPI narcissism includes traits related to certain facets of Extraversion, which is consistent with expert conceptualizations of NPD.
Article
Narcissism is typically viewed as a dimensional construct in social psychology. Direct evidence supporting this position is lacking, however, and recent research suggests that clinical measures of narcissism exhibit categorical properties. It is therefore unclear whether social psychological researchers should conceptualize narcissism as a category or continuum. To help remedy this, the latent structure of narcissism—measured by the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI)—was examined using 3895 participants and three taxometric procedures. Results suggest that NPI scores are distributed dimensionally. There is no apparent shift from “normal” to “narcissist” observed across the NPI continuum. This is consistent with the prevailing view of narcissism in social psychology and suggests that narcissism is structured similar to other aspects of general personality. This also suggests a difference in how narcissism is structured in clinical versus social psychology (134 words).
Article
Influential social and personality psychology research indicates that narcissism is related to psychological health. Such inferences are open to question, however, because they nearly all rely on the same self-report instrument—the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI; [77] and [78])—to operationalize and measure narcissism. This is problematic because numerous NPI items do not appear to correspond to common definitions or manifestations of narcissism, and may instead be indicative of self-esteem. Two studies demonstrate that the NPI’s confound with self-esteem accounts for the purported relationship between narcissism and psychological health. This suggests that inferences about narcissism and psychological health may need to be reinterpreted. Results also highlight the need for measures that correspond more directly to core components of narcissism.