ArticlePDF Available

Group size affects social relationships in yellow-bellied marmots (Marmota flaviventris) Adriana A. Maldonado-Chaparro1

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Demographic variation, such as changes in population size, affects group-living conditions and thus creates new opportunities for individuals to interact socially. To understand how this variation in the social environment affects social structure, we used social network analysis to explore affiliative behaviors of nonpup (i.e., 1 year or older), female, yellow-bellied marmots (Marmota flaviventris). We examined 4 social attributes (outdegree, indegree, closeness centrality, and betweenness centrality) to measure social plasticity in response to group size variation. We found that, in response to increases in group size, individuals established fewer social connections than possible, which suggests that marmots experience constraints on sociality. Similarly, closeness and betweenness centrality decreased as group size increased, suggesting that females are expected to lose influence over other members of the group as group size increases, and there are substantial constraints on marmots transmitting information to others in large groups. Our results also suggest that group-level responses, such as behavioral plasticity, can be explained by individual-level mechanisms that evaluate the costs and benefits of sociality. Interestingly, the mechanistic basis of these group-level responses may, at times, follow patterns expected by chance. We propose that further research is necessary to uncover the mechanisms underlying the individual-level behavioral response. Like group size effects studied in other domains, formally considering group size effects on social structure may shed novel light on the constraints on sociality.
Content may be subject to copyright.
© The Author 2015. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of
the International Society for Behavioral Ecology. All rights reserved. For
permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com
The ocial journal of the
ISBE
International Society for Behavioral Ecology
Behavioral
Ecology
Original Article
Group size aects social relationships in
yellow-bellied marmots (Marmota flaviventris)
Adriana A.Maldonado-Chaparro,a LilahHubbard,a and Daniel T.Blumsteina,b
aDepartment of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of California, 621 Charles E.Young Drive
South, Los Angeles, CA 90095-1606, USA and bThe Rocky Mountain Biological Laboratory, Box 519,
Crested Butte, CO 81224, USA
Received 31 July 2014; revised 21 February 2015; accepted 26 February 2015.
Demographic variation, such as changes in population size, affects group-living conditions and thus creates new opportunities for
individuals to interact socially. To understand how this variation in the social environment affects social structure, we used social
network analysis to explore affiliative behaviors of nonpup (i.e., 1year or older), female, yellow-bellied marmots (Marmota flaviventris).
We examined 4 social attributes (outdegree, indegree, closeness centrality, and betweenness centrality) to measure social plasticity
in response to group size variation. We found that, in response to increases in group size, individuals established fewer social connec-
tions than possible, which suggests that marmots experience constraints on sociality. Similarly, closeness and betweenness central-
ity decreased as group size increased, suggesting that females are expected to lose influence over other members of the group as
group size increases, and there are substantial constraints on marmots transmitting information to others in large groups. Our results
also suggest that group-level responses, such as behavioral plasticity, can be explained by individual-level mechanisms that evaluate
the costs and benefits of sociality. Interestingly, the mechanistic basis of these group-level responses may, at times, follow patterns
expected by chance. We propose that further research is necessary to uncover the mechanisms underlying the individual-level behav-
ioral response. Like group size effects studied in other domains, formally considering group size effects on social structure may shed
novel light on the constraints on sociality.
Key words: centrality, degree, group size effects, social networks, yellow-bellied marmots.
INTRODUCTION
Ecological variation creates demographic opportunities that
allow individuals to aggregate and form social groups (Blumstein
2013). Ecological factors (e.g., food availability and climate) can
aect population characteristics, such as survival and reproduc-
tion, and thus drive variation in social groups and social systems
(Butler 1980; Crockett and Eisenberg 1987). Such population level
eects may increase or decrease with changes in group size and
may create new opportunities for individuals to interact if they
are not otherwise constrained. Therefore, in group-living animals,
sociodemographic variation may aect social structure (Griths
and Magurran 1997; Blumstein 2013; Schradin 2013). We view the
social system as the group of conspecifics that consistently interact
with each other and do so more often with each other than with
individuals of other groups (Kappeler and van Schaik 2002), the
social structure as the pattern of social interactions and the result-
ing relationships among individuals in the social system (Hinde
1976; Kappeler et al. 2013), and social organization as the size,
sexual composition, and cohesion of a social system (Kappeler and
van Schaik 2002; Kappeler etal. 2013).
Sociodemographic variation may dierently aect a popula-
tion’s or a species’ social organization, mating system, and social
structure. Previous studies have shown that increases in group size
can lead to the formation of complex societies that are character-
ized by a higher degree of morphological dimorphism and social
roles (Bourke 1999), can increase male mating success (Alexander
1974; Hovi et al. 1994), and can also be associated with reduced
ospring survival and declining birth rates after a certain group
size is reached (van Noordwijk and van Schaik 1999). Primate stud-
ies that focused on the relationship between group size and social
structure found that grooming increases with group size, but fur-
ther increases in group size leads to constraints: individuals have
insucient time to allocate to maintaining their social relationships
(Lehmann et al. 2007; Pollard and Blumstein 2008). Such find-
ings suggest that individuals can modify their behavior and conse-
quently alter the social relationships that emerge with changes in
group size. Thus, we aimed to explore how dierences in group
size are associated with variation in individuals’ social attributes
that aect group structure. By doing so, we sought to identify the
Address correspondence to D.T. Blumstein. E-mail: mar mots@ucla.edu.
Behavioral Ecology (2015), 00(00), 1–7. doi:10.1093/beheco/arv034
Behavioral Ecology Advance Access published April 15, 2015
Behavioral Ecology
mechanisms that permit females to behaviorally respond to changes
in their social environment.
The relationships that individuals can establish with other mem-
bers of the group may vary in the face of varying ecological and
social conditions. Such changes in an individual’s behavior as
a function of the conditions they experience are defined as phe-
notypic plasticity (Bradshaw 1965; Stearns 1989; Pigliucci 2001).
Plasticity of behavioral traits is known as behavioral plasticity
(West-Eberhard 1989; Dingemanse and Wolf 2013). Thus, social
plasticity can be defined as the ability of an individual to modify
its social behavior (social interactions and relationships) in response
to the social environment that it experiences (i.e., when found in
dierent sized groups). We suggest that individuals modify their
social behavior as a function of changes in their social environment
through social plasticity. By documenting the nature of these plastic
responses, we can understand how these behavioral changes alter
the overall social structure of the social system and identify poten-
tial constraints on sociality.
Here, we used a population of yellow-bellied marmots (Marmota
flaviventris; herein marmots), a diurnal and facultatively social
rodent, as a study system to investigate variation in social struc-
ture in response to changes in the social environment. The mar-
mot population around the Rocky Mountain Biological Laboratory
(RMBL) oers a great opportunity to address this question,
because females live in matrilines (i.e., kin groups), and the popu-
lation size has dramatically increased over the past decade. This
increase combined with a major population crash in 2011, prob-
ably as a result of warming spring temperatures and consequently
an increase in food availability during the marmots’ active season
(Ozgul et al. 2010), has resulted in substantial variation in group
size. Additionally, it has been suggested that marmots may increase
aliative behavior as group size increases to maintain social cohe-
sion (i.e., the degree to which members of a group are connected to
each other, Maldonado-Chaparro etal. forthcoming). Thus, in this
study, we will concentrate on aliative interactions because they
are often important for both group social cohesion and individual
fitness (Silk 2007b; Silk etal. 2009; Wey and Blumstein 2010, 2012)
and because these interactions also play an important role for dis-
persal decisions (Blumstein etal. 2009).
We used social network analyses, which allowed us to statistically
analyze the structure and components of networks that involve
multiple types of interactions (Krause et al. 2009) and to study
direct as well as indirect relationships (Wasserman and Faust 1994;
Croft etal. 2008; Wey etal. 2008). We focused on 4 social network
attributes that describe an individual’s direct interactions and ability
to influence other members of the group and allow us to quantify
variation in an individual’s behavioral response: outdegree, inde-
gree, closeness centrality, and betweenness centrality. Outdegree
quantifies the number of other individuals with whom an indi-
vidual initiates interactions (Wasserman and Faust 1994), whereas
indegree specifies the number of other individuals that direct inter-
actions toward the subject (Wasserman and Faust 1994). Closeness
centrality quantifies connectedness of an individual in terms of its
direct and indirect interactions with every member of the group
(i.e., measures an individual’s influence), and betweenness centrality
indicates the ability of a member to control paths of information
(or disease) between members in a group. We chose these measures
because they permitted us understand group structure (Freeman
1979) and to characterize how extensively individuals are involved
in relationships with other individuals in the network. This permit-
ted us to capture the propensity of an individual to develop social
relationships as well as to quantify changes in the social dynam-
ics of female marmots that may drive biological processes such as
information or disease transmission.
We assumed that 1) changes in the social context oer new
opportunities to interact with potential members of the group,
2)such additional social interactions lead to new relationships that
may aect an individual’s social network, and 3) individuals are
able to optimize the assessment of alternative behavioral trade-os.
Given these assumptions, we hypothesized that females will adjust
their aliative social behavior in response to variation in group size
to balance the cost–benefit trade-os associated with social living.
However, because individuals must also distribute their time and
energy between social interactions and other activities, includ-
ing foraging, resting, and traveling around their home ranges, the
time available for interacting may act to constrain sociality (Dunbar
1992b; Pollard and Blumstein 2008; Blumstein 2013). Therefore,
we also predicted that, as group size increases, individuals will
increase the number of social partners until the cost of group
living (i.e., increased competition over access to resources and
mating opportunities, increased risk of disease, and increased pre-
dation risk) will exceed the benefit (i.e., protection from predators,
increased foraging success, and reduced infanticide probability)
and animals will be unable to engage in additional social relation-
ships. Also, assuming that not all individuals in the group choose to
increase the number of social partners (i.e., increase the number
of direct connections), we expected animals to be less closely con-
nected to other individuals in the group (i.e., lower closeness cen-
trality) and to have less control over information flow (i.e., lower
betweenness centrality).
METHODS
Monitoring social behavior and demography in
yellow-bellied marmots
Since 1962, marmots in and around the RMBL have been regu-
larly livetrapped and observed during the active season (between
mid-April and early September). Using baited live traps, we trapped
100% of the individuals in our population annually. Individuals
were given numbered ear tags the first time they were captured and
were marked with fur dye for identification from afar (Blumstein
et al. 2009). Additionally, we weighed (using a digital scale) and
sexed each individual. Individuals were classified into 3 age catego-
ries: pups (<1year), yearlings (1year old), and adults (≥2yearsold).
For these analyses, behavioral observations were conducted on
an average of 53 females per year over a period of 12 years. We
observed marmots from mid-April to early September, during
hours of peak activity (from 7:00 to 10:00 h in the morning and
16:00 to 19:00 h in the afternoon; Blumstein etal. 2009). Observers
sat quietly and observed marmots from about 20 to 150 m away
(Blumstein etal. 2009) with binoculars and 15–45× spotting scopes.
We recorded all observed social interactions (details in Wey and
Blumstein 2010). For each individual interaction, we recorded the
type (i.e., aliative or agonistic), the initiator and recipient, the
location, and the time of interaction. The number of hours of
observation per year over the study period (2002–2013) averaged
874 h but varied from 302 to 1270 h (Supplementary Table S1).
Quantifying the social environment
The social context of yellow-bellied marmots can be hierarchically
described. Marmots physically live in colony sites, a geographic
Page 2 of 7
Maldonado-Chaparro etal. • Group size eects on social structure
area that may contain one or more social groups that are found in
patches of suitable habitat. Social groups are a subset of 2 or more
individuals that live in close proximity in space and time and thus
are more associated among themselves than with other individuals
in the colony site. Not all individuals in a social group are observed
to interact above ground and thus animals in a social group may
or may not interact to form social networks. Thus, group size may
dier from social network size, which we defined as the set of indi-
viduals within a social group that were seen to behaviorally interact
with other members of the group during the study period. Asocial
network can be defined based on observations of aliative interac-
tions, agonistic interactions, orboth.
We focused on individuals found in 4 geographically distinct
areas (colony sites): Bench-River, Gothic Town, Marmot Meadow,
and Picnic that are patchily distributed between 2700 and 3100
m.a.s.l. Our Marmot Meadow and Picnic sites are located in higher
elevations (i.e., up valley) than our other 2 sites (i.e., down valley).
Within each colony site, we identified social groups based on the
marmot’s space-use overlap (Smith JE, Strelio CC, Blumstein DT,
unpublished data). To do this, we focused on nonpup (i.e., 1 year
or older) female and male marmots seen or trapped at least 5 times
in a year. Then we used Socprog (Whitehead 2009) to calculate
the simple ratio index (SRI, Cairns and Schwager 1987) from live-
trapping and observation data for each pair of marmots. We then
used the estimated SRI to identify the number and identity of the
individuals that belonged to a particular social group (i.e., mod-
ule) using the random walk algorithm on Map Equation (Rosvall
and Bergstrom 2008; Rosvall et al. 2009). For the purpose of this
study, we defined the social environment as the female group size
(the number of nonpup females present in a social group) because
we were interested in female sociality. Therefore, after identifying
the members of each social group, we removed all males from the
social group analysis to obtain the female group sizes. Between
2002 and 2013, we identified 86 social groups composed of 2 or
more nonpup females. Nonpup female group sizes varied from 2 to
18 individuals for the years under study (Supplementary Table S2).
Quantifying individual social attributes
We focused on aliative interactions (i.e., sit in body contact, sit in
proximity, grooming, and social play) recorded during the entire
active season to construct the aliative social matrix and the cor-
responding social network for each social group in each colony site
for each year from 2002 to 2013 (Figure1). Social networks con-
sisted of nodes (female marmots ≥1 year old) connected by directed
edges (i.e., observed aliative interactions between individuals). We
calculated the 4 social attributes for each nonpup female individual
in each social network (i.e., the connected components of the social
group). Outdegree was computed as the number of connections ini-
tiated by an individual (Wasserman and Faust 1994). Indegree was
the number of connections received by an individual (Wasserman
and Faust 1994). Closeness centrality was calculated by taking the
reciprocal of the sum of the shortest paths between the focal and
other individuals (or the sum of the reciprocals) (Wasserman and
Faust 1994; Wey etal. 2008; Wey and Blumstein 2012). Betweenness
centrality was the proportion of shortest path lengths between pairs
of other group members in which the focal individual was a point
on the path (Wasserman and Faust 1994; Wey etal. 2008; Wey and
Blumstein 2012). Outdegree and indegree were calculated using
directed, unweighted networks, whereas closeness and betweenness
centralities were calculated using undirected, unweighted networks.
All measurements were normalized to facilitate comparison
across networks of dierent sizes; thus, all of our measurements
ranged from 0 to 1.Indegree and outdegree were each divided by
n − 1 (the maximum number of possible connections), where n was
the total number of nodes in the network. For closeness, we multi-
plied the raw closeness by n − 1, where n was the number of nodes
in the graph, whereas for betweenness, we used 2× B/(n × n − 3×
2002 2004 2006
2008 2010 2012
Figure1
Examples of female (nonpup) yellow-bellied marmot (Marmota flaviventris) social networks in Marmot Meadow observed over dierent years. These networks
dier in size and structure. Gray nodes: yearling and adult females; solid lines: undirected aliative interactions.
Page 3 of 7
Behavioral Ecology
n + 2), where B is the raw betweenness and n is the number of
nodes in the graph (Freeman 1979). The unit of analysis was an
individual studied in a given year. All our calculations were con-
ducted in the iGraph package v.0.7.1 (Csardi and Nepusz 2006) in
R software v.3.1.1 (R Core Team 2014).
Statistical analysis
To explore the potential relationship between group size and social
structure, we performed a series of regression analyses that allowed
us to identify group size eects. We used a reaction norm approach
(Pigliucci 2001) to isolate the eect of group size on standardized
network parameters. A simple linear reaction norm graph (i.e.,
straight line) contains 2 main characteristics: slope and elevation
(Pigliucci 2001); however, more complex relations can be explained
through nonlinear reaction norms (Koons etal. 2009). The slope
quantifies the population’s phenotypic plasticity, measured as the
change in phenotypic expression with respect to environmental
variation (Pigliucci 2001), and the elevation quantifies the aver-
age phenotypic response (Pigliucci 2001; Nussey etal. 2007). This
approach allowed us to ask if social plasticity was a mechanism that
explained variation in an individual’s social attributes.
To describe the behavioral response pattern for each of our
dependent variables, outdegree, indegree, closeness centrality, and
betweenness centrality, we fitted a set of candidate mixed eect
models that included linear and nonlinear relationships and per-
formed a model selection analysis (Table 1). In each model, the
dependent variable was modeled as a function of the year-spe-
cific social environment (i.e., female group size). We also included
age category as a factor to control for known behavioral dier-
ences between yearlings and adults (Wey and Blumstein 2010;
Maldonado-Chaparro et al. forthcoming). Additionally, and to
account for repeated measures on individuals, we included female
identity and year as random eects. The error structure of the
models varied for each of the dependent variables. Outdegree
and indegree were based on proportion data and therefore we fit-
ted a binomial model (logit link) (Noutdegree = 384; Nindegree= 384).
Closeness and betweenness centrality were arcsine square-root
transformed and we fitted a Gaussian model (identity link)
(Ncloseness = 395). Betweenness centrality contained 80% of zeros.
Thus, we focused only on the subset of our data where betweenness
was greater than 0 (N = 143). We identified the best model sup-
ported by the data by using the Akaike information criterion cor-
rected for small samples. For Gaussian models, we evaluated the
significance of fixed eects using the Satterthwaite’s approxima-
tion for degrees of freedom in the lmerTest package (Kuznetsova
etal. 2014). All of our models were analyzed using the lme4 pack-
age (Bates etal. 2013) and the gamm4 package (Wood and Scheipl
2013) in R software (R Core Team 2014).
Finally, we assessed if the observed pattern in each of our social
attributes diered from the pattern expected from social attri-
butes estimated for random networks. To do this, for each of our
observed social networks, we generated an equivalent Erdös–Rényi
(E–R) random graph using the same number of n nodes. The prob-
ability (P) in E–R graphs can vary between 0 and 1, where 0 rep-
resents an empty graph and 1 represents a complete graph. Thus,
we defined P as 0.5 to allow for the maximum uncertainty of a ran-
dom graph (Takahashi etal. 2012). Then, we calculated the node-
based indegree, outdegree, betweenness, and closeness in all of our
E–R networks. We used the values obtained through the equivalent
E–R graphs to create a data set that contained values expected by
chance. We then used these random values as the response variable
and fitted the best models that were selected for the observed val-
ues of outdegree, indegree, closeness, and betweenness. Finally, we
built the 95% confidence intervals of the regression lines for the
observed and the random data sets and determined if the confi-
dence intervals overlapped. If they overlapped, the observed group
size eect was expected by chance.
RESULTS
We constructed 86 nonpup female social networks based on
observed aliative interactions (some of the individuals in smaller
spatially defined groups were not observed to interact). The
observed networks had an average of 4.5 (±SD=3.0) female mar-
mots. As expected, our measures were somewhat correlated (i.e.,
indegree vs. outdegree, see Supplementary Figure S1), but we ana-
lyzed them independently because each one may reflect a dierent
social process.
Our regression analyses revealed that group size was always sig-
nificantly associated with an individual’s social attributes, whereas
age category was only sometimes significantly associated with an
individual’s social attributes. More precisely, outdegree and inde-
gree declined nonlinearly with group size. For each additional
member of the group, the probability that a female added an addi-
tional social partner initially decreased at an average of −0.339
(standard error [SE]=0.04; P<0.001) and then, for groups larger
than 10, increased at an average of 0.026 (SE =0.007; P<0.001)
social partners per additional individual (Figure 2a). Yearlings did
not significantly dier from adults in their average response (0.432;
SE= 0.251; P = 0.086). Our outdegree model explained 30.75%
of the variance.
Table1
Set of candidate models fitted for each of the 4 network
measures (outdegree, indegree, closeness centrality, and
betweenness centrality) calculated for members of the
female social networks in yellow-bellied marmot (Marmota
flaviventris)
Model df AICc
Outdegree
Age category 5 511.80
Group size + age category 6 429.36
Group size + group size2 + age category 7 418.22
s(Group size) 7 431.44
Indegree
Age category 5 507.41
Group size + age category 6 395.65
Group size + group size2 + age category 7 390.01
s(Group size) 7 397.71
Closeness centrality
Age category 5 383.19
Group size + age category 6194.81
Group size1 + group size2 + age category 7 145.28
s(Group size) 7 415.39
Betweenness centrality
Age category 5 105.69
Group size + age category 6 89.32
Group size1 + group size2 + age category 7 51.53
s(Group size) 7 61.71
The model in bold represents the selected model based on the Akaike
information criteria (AICc). Superscript 2 indicates squared group size
and subscripts 1 and 2 indicate the slope for group size below and above
the estimated breakpoint, respectively. s indicates the smooth function. df,
degrees of freedom.
Page 4 of 7
Maldonado-Chaparro etal. • Group size eects on social structure
Likewise, the probability that an individual received more social
interactions decreased at an average of −0.393 (SE = 0.044;
P < 0.001) and then, for groups larger than 10, increased at an
average of 0.023 (SE= 0.008; P =0.003) social partners per addi-
tional individual (Figure2b). Yearlings were more likely to receive
more ties as group size increased than adults (0.710; SE=0.264;
P=0.007). Our indegree model explained 39.77% of the variance.
Closeness centrality significantly decreased at a rate of −0.129
(SE = 0.009; P< 0.001) per additional individual. The slope sig-
nificantly changed from a steep to gradual decrease around a group
size of 8 individuals. After this break point, closeness centrality did
not significantly change (−0.009; SE=0.007; P<0.001) as a func-
tion of increased group size (Figure2c). Yearlings were significantly
closer to other members of the group than adults (0.107 ± 0.030;
P=0.007). The closeness model explained 55.51% of the variance.
Finally, betweenness centrality decreased quickly at small group
sizes (−0.950 ± 0.127; P< 0.001; Figure2d) and then it decreased
at lower rates for larger groups (>5 individuals) (−0.03 ± 0.008;
P<0.001; Figure2d). Yearling and adults did not significantly dif-
fer in their betweenness (0.014 ± 0.043; P = 0.747). Our between-
ness model explained 74.33% of the variance.
The observed reaction norms for our social attributes indegree,
outdegree, and closeness diered from that expected by chance
(Figure2ac). In other words, compared to a random process, mar-
mots were significantly more social at small group sizes. However,
as group size increased, marmots were significantly less likely to
initiate or receive connections or to have more central positions
than expected by chance. For betweenness, there were significant
deviations from random for groups smaller than 8 but not for larger
groups (Figure2d). In other words, for small group sizes, between-
ness centrality decreased at a significantly faster rate than expected
by chance, but this trend disappeared as group size increased.
DISCUSSION
Specific attributes of yellow-bellied marmot social relationships
are correlated with group size: marmots were less likely to add or
receive new social partners as group size increased, and their close-
ness and betweenness centralities decreased with increases in group
size. Thus, our results show that marmots are behaviorally flex-
ible and can adjust their social behavior to variation in their social
environment. Moreover, many of the identified patterns of behav-
ioral plasticity diered from those expected by chance, suggesting
that there are individual-level mechanisms that allow marmots to
balance the costs and benefits of maintaining social relationships.
This suggests that the flow of information and/or disease may be
1.00
ab
cd
0.75
Outdegree
1.6
1.2
0.8
0.4
Closseness
0.0
0.5
1.0
1.5
2.0
Betweenness
0.50
0.25
0.00
1.00
0.75
Indegree
0.50
0.25
0.00
246810
Group size
12 14 16 18
246810
Group size
357911 13 15 17
Group size
12 14 16 18
246810
Group size
12 14 16 18
Figure2
Results of the generalized linear mixed models explaining the variation in the 4 network measures in response to changes in female marmot groups. Black
solid lines illustrate the regression line fitted with the observed network attributes. The confidence intervals are denoted as a gray shaded polygon. Dark gray
dashed lines illustrate the regression line fitted with the social attributes calculated from Erdös–Rényi random networks (P= 0.5). Confidence intervals are
denoted as a light gray shaded polygon. (a) Fitted line of the outdegree response and raw data, (b) fitted line of the indegree response and raw data, (c) fitted
line of the closeness response and raw data, and (d) fitted line of the betweenness response and raw data. Circles: aggregated values of each of the raw social
network measures.
Page 5 of 7
Behavioral Ecology
aected by the behavioral decisions made by individuals within a
group. Our results also indicated that age is a potentially important
factor that influences sociality. Yearlings are more interactive (i.e.,
they had a higher indegree and closeness) than adults, a finding
that supports previous research that showed that younger individu-
als are more sociable and have a potentially important role in main-
taining social cohesion (Wey and Blumstein 2010).
Although our observed patterns supported the existence of
behaviorally plastic responses, the comparisons between the
observed and the random patterns suggested that when there were
nonsignificant dierences, some elements of the emergent prop-
erties of social behavior at the group level may be explained by
alternative mechanisms that follow random processes. For example,
in house mice (Mus domesticus), and in red foxes (Vulpes vulpes), ran-
dom processes explain aspects of their spatial and social behavior
(Giuggioli etal. 2011; Perony etal. 2012). We know, however, that
animals often interact nonrandomly with group members (Kurvers
etal. 2014). Thus, significant dierences between the emergent pat-
terns of behavioral plasticity and the random expectations suggest
the existence of behavioral rules that govern social interactions.
The ability of marmots to behaviorally respond to changes in
their social environment may imply that individuals are able to
evaluate the costs and benefits of socially interacting under dier-
ent circumstances and avoid the costs of increased sociality. Our
analyses show that at small group sizes, individuals apparently work
to increase social interactions. Interestingly, as group sizes increases,
individuals either behave randomly or seem to avoid participating
in more social interactions. This may be a mechanism to avoid the
costs of increased sociality.
The rate of decline in the likelihood of making new social partners
as group size increased suggests that sociality in female yellow-bel-
lied marmots may entail net costs, such as increases in within-group
competition, spread of parasites, or possibly reproductive suppres-
sion (Alexander 1974; Krause and Ruxton 2002; Silk 2007a). For
example, in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis), intraspecific
competition may reduce individual food intake and therefore increas-
ing group size creates more costs than benefits (van Schaik and van
Noordwijk 1986). Alternatively, individuals may choose to selectively
interact with few individuals in the group, implying the existence
of social preferences among members of a group (Lehmann and
Boesch 2009) that may reduce the cost of group living. In marmots,
such preferences may emerge as a result of the kinship structure
that influences the aliative networks (Wey and Blumstein 2010).
Furthermore, restricting the number of individuals one interacts with
may also be an adaptation to minimize the spread of contact-trans-
mitted diseases and parasites by reducing the frequency of direct
contact with potentially infected individuals (i.e., decreasing infection
risk). This may be the case in yellow-bellied marmots, where parasite
load does not always increase with group size (Lopez et al. 2013),
thus suggesting that social species may have acquired an adaptation
to prevent the spread of parasites in large groups (Bordes etal. 2007).
Therefore, as group size increases, the trade-o between the bene-
fits and costs of sociality may determine the number, strength, and
nature of the social relationships among group members.
The decay in closeness values suggests that there may be an
inevitable loss of control over other members of the group as group
size increases. Therefore, females in larger groups have less influ-
ence over other individuals in their group. This has implications for
dominance relationships in larger groups. As individuals lose con-
trol, a single individual may not be able to exert dominance over
others. This might have practical demographic consequences if this
means that formerly dominant individuals are unable to suppress
reproduction of other females. Additionally, most animals have no
calculable betweenness, perhaps because females within a group
occupy more peripheral positions in the network. By contrast, mar-
mots with betweenness centrality values greater than 0 may serve as
links between individuals that are not directly connected, or between
subgroups within a social group, as has been described in bottlenose
dolphin (Tursiops spp.) networks (Lusseau and Newman 2004).
Taken together, our results paint a rather simple picture of female
marmot sociality, which in some ways is similar to male marmot soci-
ality (Olson and Blumstein 2010). The lack of social complexity may
be a product of proximate mechanisms such as temporal and cog-
nitive constraints that limit the number of social relationships that
an individual can maintain (Dunbar 1992b; Lehmann et al. 2007;
Pollard and Blumstein 2008; Sueur et al. 2011; Blumstein 2013),
thus aecting the social structure. Individuals must distribute their
time among various activities (foraging, vigilance, travel, etc.), mean-
ing that individuals are limited in the time that they may allocate to
social activities (Mitani 1989; Dunbar et al. 2009). Alternatively, the
neocortex size limits the amount of information that an individual
can process, which therefore limits the number of social relation-
ships that an individual can monitor (Dunbar 1992a; Lehmann etal.
2007). Therefore, individuals in a group are seemingly limited in the
number of social relationships that they can maintain.
Sociality is a key factor that aects the survival and reproduc-
tion of social species. We have shown that animals seemingly adjust
the specific nature of their social relationships according to changes
in their social environment. Such behavioral plasticity may alter
the interaction between behavior and sociodemography, which in
turn can aect population dynamics (Calhoun 1952). As popula-
tions fluctuate both naturally and as a result of human impacts, it is
important to understand the eects of these fluctuations on social-
ity in order to have a better insight of the role of sociality on the
relationship between temporal environmental variation and popu-
lation dynamics. Our statistical approach (i.e., reaction norms) to
study behavioral plasticity can be widely applied to social species
and, by doing so, may shed novel light on constraints on sociality.
SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL
Supplementary material can be found at http://www.beheco.
oxfordjournals.org/
FUNDING
A.M.-C. was supported by a Fulbright Fellowship. D.T.B. was
supported by the UCLA Academic Senate and Division of Life
Sciences, National Geographic Society, and National Science
Foundation (NSF-IDBR-0754247 and NSF-DEB-1119660 to
D.T.B.; and NSF-DBI 0242960, 0731346 to the Rocky Mountain
Biological Laboratory).
We thank all the marmoteers who helped collect data. The comments of
J. Fowler and several anonymous reviewers helped us improve previous
versions.
Handling editor: Alison Bell
REFERENCES
Alexander RD. 1974. The evolution of social behavior. Annu Rev Ecol Evol
Syst. 5:325–383.
Page 6 of 7
Maldonado-Chaparro etal. • Group size eects on social structure
Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B. 2013. Linear mixed-eect models using S4
classes. R package version 0.999999-2 [cited 2014 April 24]. Available
from: http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4
Blumstein DT. 2013. Yellow-bellied marmots: insights from an emergent
view of sociality. Phil Trans R Soc B. 368:20120349.
Blumstein DT, Wey TW, Tang K. 2009. A test of the social cohesion
hypothesis: interactive female marmots remain at home. Proc R Soc B.
276:3007–3012.
Bordes F, Blumstein DT, Morand S. 2007. Rodent sociality and parasite
diversity. Biol Lett. 3:692–694.
Bourke AFG. 1999. Colony size social complexity and reproductive conflict
in social insects. J Evol Biol. 12:245–257.
Bradshaw AD. 1965. Evolutionary significance of phenotypic plasticity in
plants. Adv Genet. 13:115–155.
Butler RG. 1980. Population size social behavior and dispersal in house
mice: a quantitative investigation. Anim Behav. 28:78–85.
Cairns SJ, Schwager SJ. 1987. A comparison of association indices. Anim
Behav. 35:1454–1469.
Calhoun JB. 1952. The social aspects of population dynamics. J Mammal.
33:139–159.
Crockett CM, Eisenberg JF. 1987. Howlers: variation in group size and
demography. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RR,
Struhsker TT, editors. Primate societies. Chicago (IL): The University of
Chicago Press.
Croft DP, James R, Krause J. 2008. Exploring animal social networks.
Princeton (NJ): Princeton University Press.
Csardi G, Nepusz T. 2006. The igraph software package for complex net-
work research. InterJournal Complex Systems, p. 1695.
Dingemanse NJ, Wolf M. 2013. Between-individual dierences in behav-
ioral plasticity within populations: causes and consequences. Anim Behav.
25:81–89.
Dunbar RI. 1992a. Neocortex size as a constraint on group size in primates.
J Hum Evol. 22:469–493.
Dunbar RI. 1992b. Time: a hidden constraint on the behavioral ecology of
baboons. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 31:35–49.
Dunbar RIM, Korstjens AH, Lehmann J. 2009. Time as an ecological con-
straint. Biol Rev. 84:413–429.
Freeman LC. 1979. Centrality in social networks: I. Conceptual clarifica-
tion. Soc Netw. 1:215–239.
Giuggioli L, Potts JR, Harris S. 2011. Animal interactions and the emer-
gence of territoriality. PLoS Comput Biol. 7:e1002008.
Griths SW, Magurran AE. 1997. Schooling preferences for familiar fish
vary with group size in a wild guppy population. Proc R Soc Lond B.
264:547–551.
Hinde RA. 1976. Interactions relationships and social structure. Man. 11:1–17.
Hovi M, Alatalo RV, Hoglund J, Lundberg A, Rintamaki PT. 1994. Lek
centre attracts black grouse females. Proc R Soc Lond B. 258:303–305.
Kappeler PM, Barrett L, Blumstein DT, Clutton-Brock TH. 2013.
Constraints and flexibility in mammalian social behavior: introduction
and synthesis. Phil Trans R Soc B. 368:20120337.
Kappeler PM, van Schaik CP. 2002. Evolution of primate social systems.
Int J Primatol. 23:707–740.
Koons DN, Pavard S, Baudisch A, Metcalf JE. 2009. Is life-history buer-
ing or lability adaptive in stochastic environments? Oikos. 118:972–980.
Krause J, Lusseau D, James R. 2009. Animal social networks: an introduc-
tion. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 63:967–973.
Krause J, Ruxton GD. 2002. Living in groups. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Kurvers RH, Krause J, Croft DP, Wilson AD, Wolf M. 2014. The evolu-
tionary and ecological consequences of animal social networks: emerging
issues. Trends Ecol Evol. 29:326–335.
Kuznetsova A, Brockho PB, Christensen RHB. 2014. lmerTest: tests for
random and fixed eects for linear mixed eect models (lmer objects
of lme4 package). Version 2.0-6 [cited 2014 May 24]. Available from:
http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lmerTest/index.html.
Lehmann J, Boesch C. 2009. Sociality of the dispersing sex: the nature of
social bonds in West African female chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Anim
Behav. 77:377–387.
Lehmann J, Korstjens AH, Dunbar RIM. 2007. Group size grooming and
social cohesion in primates. Anim Behav. 74:1617–1629.
Lopez J, Wey TW, Blumstein DT. 2013. Patterns of parasite prevalence and
individual infection in yellow-bellied marmots. J Zool. 291:296–303.
Lusseau D, Newman ME. 2004. Identifying the role that animals play in
their social networks. Proc R Soc B. 271:S477–S481.
Maldonado-Chaparro AA, Martin JGA, Armitage KB, Oli MK, Blumstein
DT. 2015. Environmentally-induced phenotypic variation in wild yellow-
bellied marmots. J Mamm.
Mitani JC. 1989. Orangutan activity budgets: monthly variations and the
eects of body size parturition and sociality. Am J Primatol. 18:87–100.
van Noordwijk MA, van Schaik CP. 1999. The eects of dominance rank
and group size on female lifetime reproductive success in wild long-tailed
macaques, Macaca fascicularis. Primates. 40:105–130.
Nussey DH, Wilson AJ, Brommer JE. 2007. The evolutionary ecology
of individual phenotypic plasticity in wild populations. J Evol Biol.
20:831–844.
Olson LE, Blumstein DT. 2010. Applying the coalitionary-traits metric:
sociality without cooperation in male yellow-bellied marmots. Behav
Ecol. 5:957–996.
Ozgul A, Childs DZ, Oli MK, Armitage KB, Blumstein DT, Olson LE,
Tuljapurkar S, Coulson T. 2010. Coupled dynamics of body mass
and population growth in response to environmental change. Nature.
466:482–485.
Perony N, Tessone CJ, König B, Schweitzer F. 2012. How random is social
behaviour? Disentangling social complexity through the study of a wild
house mouse population. PLoS Comput Biol. 8:e1002786.
Pigliucci M. 2001. Phenotypic plasticity: beyond nature and nurture.
Baltimore (MD): Johns Hopkins University Press.
Pollard KA, Blumstein DT. 2008. Time allocation and the evolution of
group size. Anim Behav. 76:1683–1699.
R Core Team. 2014. R: a language and environment for statistical comput-
ing. Vienna (Austria): R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available
from: www.r-project.org.
Rosvall M, Axelsson D, Bergstrom CT. 2009. The map equation. Eur Phys
J Spec Top. 178:13–23.
Rosvall M, Bergstrom CT. 2008. Maps of information flow reveal com-
munity structure in complex networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA.
105:1118–1123.
van Schaik CP, van Noordwijk MA. 1986. The hidden costs of social-
ity: intra-group variation in feeding strategies in Sumatran long-tailed
macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Behavior. 99:296–315.
Schradin C. 2013. Intraspecific variation in social organization by genetic
variation developmental plasticity social flexibility or entirely extrinsic
factors. Phil Trans R Soc B. 368:20120346.
Silk JB. 2007a. The adaptive value of sociality in mammalian groups. Phil
Trans R Soc B. 362:539–559.
Silk JB. 2007b. Social components of fitness in primate groups. Science.
317:1347–1351.
Silk JB, Beehner JC, Bergman TJ, Crockford C, Engh AL, Moscovice LR,
Wittig RM, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. 2009. The benefits of social capi-
tal: close social bonds among female baboons enhance ospring survival.
Proc R Soc B. 276:3099–3104.
Stearns SC. 1989. The evolutionary significance of phenotypic plasticity.
Bioscience. 39:436–445.
Sueur C, Deneubourg JL, Petit O, Couzin ID. 2011. Group size, grooming
and fission in primates: a modeling approach based on group structure. J
Theor Biol. 273:156–166.
Takahashi DY, Sato JR, Ferreira CE, Fujita A. 2012. Discriminating dier-
ent classes of biological networks by analyzing the graphs spectra distri-
bution. PLoS One. 7:e49949.
Wasserman S, Faust K. 1994. Social network analysis: methods and applica-
tions. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge University Press.
West-Eberhard MJ. 1989. Phenotypic plasticity and the origins of diversity.
Annu Rev Ecol Syst. 20:249–278.
Wey TW, Blumstein DT. 2010. Social cohesion in yellow-bellied mar-
mots is established through age and kin structuring. Anim Behav.
79:1343–1352.
Wey TW, Blumstein DT. 2012. Social attributes and associated perfor-
mance measures in marmots: bigger male bullies and weakly aliating
females have higher annual reproductive success. Behav Ecol Sociobiol.
66:1075–1085.
Wey TW, Blumstein DT, Shen W, Jordán F. 2008. Social network analysis of
animal behavior: a promising tool for the study of sociality. Anim Behav.
75:333–344.
Whitehead H. 2009. SOCPROG programs: analyzing animal social struc-
tures. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 63:765–778.
Wood S, Scheipl F. 2013. gamm4: generalized additive mixed models using
mgcv and lme4. Version 0.2-2 [cited 2014 May 24]. Available from:
http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/gamm4/index.html.
Page 7 of 7
... This population of marmots is ideal for studying gut microbial variation due to their spatial variability and an available half-decade dataset consisting of microbial samples from well-studied individuals (Pfau et al. 2023). This marmot population is subdivided into to higher-and lower-elevation colonies, which differ in the length of the growing season and environmental harshness (Maldonado-Chaparro, Hubbard, and Blumstein 2015;Philson et al. 2024), with documented differences in life history traits between the colonies. Moreover, prior work found significant associations between individual fitness and social behavior, environmental harshness, and age (e.g., Jamieson and Armitage 1987;Montero et al. 2020;Wey and Blumstein 2012;Maldonado-Chaparro, Hubbard, and Blumstein 2015;Philson and Blumstein 2023), giving us context to ask about a diversity of variables that might influence the microbiome in this system. ...
... This marmot population is subdivided into to higher-and lower-elevation colonies, which differ in the length of the growing season and environmental harshness (Maldonado-Chaparro, Hubbard, and Blumstein 2015;Philson et al. 2024), with documented differences in life history traits between the colonies. Moreover, prior work found significant associations between individual fitness and social behavior, environmental harshness, and age (e.g., Jamieson and Armitage 1987;Montero et al. 2020;Wey and Blumstein 2012;Maldonado-Chaparro, Hubbard, and Blumstein 2015;Philson and Blumstein 2023), giving us context to ask about a diversity of variables that might influence the microbiome in this system. Importantly, initial studies of these marmots' gut microbiomes show that variation in microbial abundance is associated with individual mass gain rate, which is necessary for hibernation survival (Degregori et al. 2024), and with social behavior, including associations between alpha diversity and pathogenetic microbes and social isolation (Pfau et al. 2023). ...
... Colonies were grouped into two core areas designated as "higher elevation" or "lower elevation" sites (Philson and Blumstein 2023). The higher elevation area experiences harsher weather conditions than the lower elevation areas (Blumstein 2006;Maldonado-Chaparro, Hubbard, and Blumstein 2015;Van Vuren and Armitage 1991). To uniquely mark individuals and collect microbiome samples, from late-May (when a majority of the snow has melted, and the marmots have had time to start eating natural vegetation) to mid-September, we placed Tomahawk live traps at burrow entrances to live capture marmots with the goal of once every 2 weeks. ...
Article
Full-text available
The gut microbiome has a well‐documented relationship with host fitness, physiology, and behavior. However, most of what is known comes from captive animals where diets and environments are more homogeneous or controlled. Studies in wild populations that experience dynamic environments and have natural life history variation are less common but are key to understanding the drivers of variation in the gut microbiome. Here we examine a wild population of yellow‐bellied marmots (Marmota flaviventer), an obligate winter hibernator, to quantify multivariate associations between host‐associated factors (e.g., age, sex, environmental harshness, and social behavior) and gut microbial composition. Across 5 years and 143 individuals, we found that males had a higher relative abundance of microbes associated with mass gain and cellulose digestion, which suggests a metabolic investment in mass gain (such as phylum Firmicutes and family Lachnospiraceae). By contrast, females had higher relative abundances of microbes associated with inflammation and metabolism (from microbial groups such as Tenericutes and Ruminococcus), possibly reflecting the importance of lactation and offspring investment. Post hoc analyses of lactating females showed a negative relationship with the abundance of microbes associated with mass gain but a positive relationship with microbes associated with metabolic energy, suggesting a trade‐off between investment in pups and maternal mass gain. Older animals also had reduced Proteobacteria relative abundance, a phylum associated with reduced inflammation. Results demonstrate that sex and age‐based traits, not sociality or environmental harshness, are associated with microbe‐mediated metabolism and inflammation in a wild, hibernating mammal.
... In a social context, this can translate to larger group sizes (Caughley, 1964;Pépin & Gerard, 2008;Vander Wal et al., 2013;Webber & Vander Wal, 2021), with correspondingly many effects on other social network properties, such as measures of centrality (Newman, 2018). A range of studies demonstrate how variation in group size can shape social structure (Balasubramaniam et al., 2014;Maldonado-Chaparro et al., 2015;Nunn et al., 2015;Webber & Vander Wal, 2020). For instance, in yellow-bellied marmots (Marmota flaviventris), network density decreased with increasing group size (Maldonado-Chaparro et al., 2015), as individuals may have a limited capacity to establish more social connections as the availability of conspecifics increases (e.g. ...
... A range of studies demonstrate how variation in group size can shape social structure (Balasubramaniam et al., 2014;Maldonado-Chaparro et al., 2015;Nunn et al., 2015;Webber & Vander Wal, 2020). For instance, in yellow-bellied marmots (Marmota flaviventris), network density decreased with increasing group size (Maldonado-Chaparro et al., 2015), as individuals may have a limited capacity to establish more social connections as the availability of conspecifics increases (e.g. due to time constraints, García et al., 2021 or cognitive constraints, Dunbar, 1992). ...
... We find that network edge density increased with increasing population size (Figure 3a) which is in contrast to findings in other species such as in yellow-bellied marmots (Maldonado-Chaparro et al., 2015) or many primate groups (Sueur et al., 2011) where network edge density decreases with increasing group size. Great tits freely mix with conspecifics and may prefer to forage at locations with high densities, thus increasing their visiting frequency and the probability of social encounters. ...
Article
Full-text available
The structure of animal societies is a key determinant of many ecological and evolutionary processes. Yet, we know relatively little about the factors and mechanisms that underpin detailed social structure. Among other factors, social structure can be influenced by habitat configuration. By shaping animal movement decisions, heterogeneity in habitat features, such as vegetation and the availability of resources, can influence the spatiotemporal distribution of individuals and subsequently key socioecological properties such as the local population size and density. Differences in local population size and density can impact opportunities for social associations and may thus drive substantial variation in local social structure. Here, we investigated spatiotemporal variation in population size at 65 distinct locations in a small songbird, the great tit (Parus major) and its effect on social network structure. We first explored the within‐location consistency of population size from weekly samples and whether the observed variation in local population size was predicted by the underlying habitat configuration. Next, we created social networks from the birds' foraging associations at each location for each week and examined if local population size affected social structure. We show that population size is highly repeatable within locations across weeks and years and that some of the observed variation in local population size was predicted by the underlying habitat, with locations closer to the forest edge having on average larger population sizes. Furthermore, we show that local population size affected social structure inferred by four global network metrics. Using simple simulations, we then reveal that much of the observed social structure is shaped by social processes. Across different population sizes, the birds' social structure was largely explained by their preference to forage in flocks. In addition, over and above effects of social foraging, social preferences between birds (i.e. social relationships) shaped certain network features such as the extent of realized social connections. Our findings thus suggest that individual social decisions substantially contribute to shaping certain social network features over and above effects of population size alone.
... The drivers of individual sociality in this system are multicausal and previous studies have suggested age (Wey and Blumstein 2010;St. Lawrence et al. 2022), body mass (Ozgul et al. 2010;Armitage 2014;Kroeger et al. 2018), and social group size (Wey and Blumstein 2012;Maldonado-Chaparro et al. 2015a) have important links with sociality. Individual social connections are also a potential driver of mass gain rate in this system (Philson et al. 2022). ...
... A theme woven through these potential explanations is how environmental change and seasonal shifts influence summer and winter survival (Saether and Bakke 2000;Gaillard and Yoccoz 2003;Prather et al. 2023) because population densities are associated with social group size (Carneiro 1967;Griffiths and Magurran 1997). For example, population size is associated with group size, and group size is strongly associated with individual social network measures (Wasserman and Faust 1994), particularly in our system (Maldonado-Chaparro et al. 2015a). In this system, winter survival is largely driven by conditions during the preceding active season and the impact of continued environmental change, and seasonal shifts are likely to be mainly negative, whereas summer survival is likely to be positively impacted (Cordes et al. 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Across animal systems, abiotic environmental features, including timing of seasonal events and weather patterns, affect fitness. An individual’s degree of social integration also has fitness consequences, but we lack an understanding of how abiotic features relate to patterns of individual sociality. A deeper understanding of this relationship could be developed from studying systems where these two links with fitness have already been identified. We explored the relationship between individual social behavior and seasonal timing, seasonal length, and weather patterns. We used social network analysis on a sixteen-year dataset of a wild population of hibernating yellow-bellied marmots (Marmota flaviventer). We fit a series of generalized linear mixed models and found that longer growing seasons before winter hibernation and longer winters were associated with increased individual sociality in the following spring. However, later snowmelt was associated with decreased sociality that spring. We found no relationship between individual sociality and various measures of precipitation and temperature. This suggests that seasonal timing and length may be a more important driver of sociality than weather patterns in this system, both as a lag and contemporary effect. Seasonal timing and length may mediate the opportunity or intensity of social interactions. The entwined relationships between the seasonal schedule and weather, and the seemingly contradictory role of winter length and snowmelt, suggests the timing of seasons and its relationship with sociality is complex and further exploration of environment-sociality relationships is required across taxa. Significance statement While the adaptive benefits of social behavior are well studied, less is known about how features of the abiotic environment drive variation in individual social behavior. Given increasing stochasticity in the timing of seasonal events and weather patterns, mapping the environment-sociality relationship will provide important insights to the drivers of sociality in the wild. This is particularly salient for species most vulnerable to climate and environmental change, such as seasonal hibernators, like yellow-bellied marmots (Marmota flaviventer). We found that features of seasonal duration were positively associated with increased sociality, whereas the timing of seasonal onset was negatively associated. This work provides empirical evidence towards an important gap in the behavioral ecology literature.
... Following intraspecies research on the yellowbellied marmot (Marmota flaviventris), it was found that females living in groups had higher reproductive success compared to solitary ones. However, exceptionally large groups led to reduced success due to increased competition among females (Maldonado-Chaparro et al., 2015). For every species, depending on size, food source, and ecology, the balance between competition over food and predator protection is diYerent (Silk, 2007). ...
... Observed pairwise relatedness values within a warren are >0.3, but after a period of population growth warrens may split with some females starting a new warren, which alters relatedness values (Surridge et al. 1999). Similarly, studies on yellow-bellied marmots have shown that as the social group becomes larger, relatedness values decrease (Maldonado-Chaparro et al. 2015). Estimating relatedness patterns over multiple years could provide further insight into social dynamics in this boodie population, particularly during population fluctuations that are characteristic of arid environments. ...
Article
Animal social relationships affect animal survival, reproduction, and resource exploitation, and are important to translocation success, but little is known on how they are impacted in reintroduced populations. Here, we investigate the social genetic structure in a reintroduced population of boodies (Bettongia lesueur), one of few social burrowing marsupial species in Australia. Pairwise relatedness of male and female individuals located within seven warrens was determined using 12 microsatellite markers, while mitochondrial DNA was used to identify maternal ancestral lineage. Females were significantly more related within warrens than between warrens, while the difference among males was not significant. We also found evidence of fine-scale genetic structure up to distances of 2 km in females consistent with patterns found in natural populations. Although mean relatedness values were low, our results support the hypothesis that boodies exhibit a female social structure. Multiple maternal lineages were present within all warrens with one exception, suggesting boodies appear to be non-selective when living with individuals from different ancestral backgrounds. We found a significant positive relationship between female sex bias and the number of active entrances in each warren. Together, these results suggest that social structuring should be a key consideration in future reintroductions of boodies.
... Social groups are composed of one or more adult males and females (>2 years old), yearlings (1 year old) and pups (<1 year old) [31]. Colonies are composed of one or multiple social groups that vary in composition and size [32]. Although groups are kin structured, adult females compete with other females for resources and reproductive success [30]. ...
Article
Full-text available
In group-living species, reproductive variation among individuals of the same sex is widespread. By identifying the mechanisms underlying this reproductive skew, we gain fundamental insights into the evolution and maintenance of sociality. A common mechanism, social control, is typically studied by quantifying dominance, which is one of many attributes of sociality that describes how individuals exert influence on others and is an incomprehensive measure of social control as it accounts only for direct relationships. Here, we use the global reaching centrality (GRC), which quantifies the degree of hierarchy in a social network by accounting for both direct and indirect social relationships. Using a wild, free-living population of adult female yellow-bellied marmots (Marmota flaviventris), we found a positive relationship between the reproductive skew index and GRC: more despotic social groups have higher reproductive skew. The GRC was stronger predictor for skew than traditional measures of social control (i.e. dominance). This allows deeper insights into the diverse ways individuals control other group members’ reproduction, a core component in the evolution of sociality. Future studies of skew across taxa may profit by using more comprehensive, network-based measures of social control.
... Networks were less dense in larger choruses, which was expected based on the increase in noise levels, and this finding corresponds with results from previous studies of the effects of group size on network density in animal social networks [76]. Variation in communication characteristics also affected the structure of the network as a whole. ...
Article
Full-text available
Communication takes place within a network of multiple signallers and receivers. Social network analysis provides tools to quantify how an individual’s social positioning affects group dynamics and the subsequent biological consequences. However, network analysis is rarely applied to animal communication, likely due to the logistical difficulties of monitoring natural communication networks. We generated a simulated communication network to investigate how variation in individual communication behaviours generates network effects, and how this communication network’s structure feeds back to affect future signalling interactions. We simulated competitive acoustic signalling interactions among chorusing individuals and varied several parameters related to communication and chorus size to examine their effects on calling output and social connections. Larger choruses had higher noise levels, and this reduced network density and altered the relationships between individual traits and communication network position. Hearing sensitivity interacted with chorus size to affect both individuals’ positions in the network and the acoustic output of the chorus. Physical proximity to competitors influenced signalling, but a distinctive communication network structure emerged when signal active space was limited. Our model raises novel predictions about communication networks that could be tested experimentally and identifies aspects of information processing in complex environments that remain to be investigated. This article is part of the theme issue ‘The power of sound: unravelling how acoustic communication shapes group dynamics’.
... (iv) Intercept-only model: random effects as in (iii); no environmental fixed effects other than an intercept were included. The best-fitting model was model (iii), the heritability model (italicized engage in less affiliative behaviour and more agonistic behaviour-as suggested for yellow-bellied marmots-potentially giving juveniles fewer opportunities to engage in grooming behaviour [77]. An alternative hypothesis is that larger groups contain more close-in-age juveniles who provide opportunities for non-grooming social interactions such as play. ...
Article
Full-text available
Affiliative social bonds are linked to fitness components in many social mammals. However, despite their importance, little is known about how the tendency to form social bonds develops in young animals, or if the timing of development is heritable and thus can evolve. Using four decades of longitudinal observational data from a wild baboon population, we assessed the environmental determinants of an important social developmental milestone in baboons—the age at which a young animal first grooms a conspecific—and we assessed how the rates at which offspring groom their mothers develops during the juvenile period. We found that grooming development differs between the sexes: female infants groom at an earlier age and reach equal rates of grooming with their mother earlier than males. We also found that age at first grooming for both sexes is weakly heritable (h² = 0.043, 95% CI: 0.002–0.110). These results show that sex differences in grooming emerge at a young age; that strong, equitable social relationships between mothers and daughters begin very early in life; and that age at first grooming is heritable and therefore can be shaped by natural selection.
... Colonies were grouped into two core areas designated as 'higher elevation' or 'lower elevation' sites [46]. About 300 m higher, the higher elevation sites experienced harsher weather conditions than the lower elevation areas [61][62][63]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The gut microbiome has a well-documented relationship with host fitness. Greater microbial diversity and abundance of specific microbes have been associated with improved fitness outcomes. Intestinal microbes also may be associated with patterns of social behaviour. However, these associations have been largely studied in captive animal models; we know less about microbiome composition as a potential driver of individual social behaviour and position in the wild. We used linear mixed models to quantify the relationship between fecal microbial composition, diversity and social network traits in a wild population of yellow-bellied marmots (Marmota flaviventer). We focused our analyses on microbes previously linked to sociability and neurobehavioural alterations in captive rodents, primates and humans. Using 5 years of data, we found microbial diversity (Shannon–Wiener and Faith's phylogenetic diversity) has a modest yet statistically significant negative relationship with the number of social interactions an individual engaged in. We also found a negative relationship between Streptococcus spp. relative abundance and two social network measures (clustering coefficient and embeddedness) that quantify an individual's position relative to others in their social group. These findings highlight a potentially consequential relationship between microbial composition and social behaviour in a wild social mammal.
Article
Full-text available
Interindividual social variation allows inferences linking social ties to fitness, but also raises questions about what drives the variation. We examined how group-level and individual factors (group size, availability of relatives, age, dominance rank) relate to inter-individual social variation in wild blue monkeys, and how these relationships reflect the social partners included in the analysis. Using focal follow data from adult females in 12 groups over 13 years, we examined five sociality measures: number of total, strong and weak ties, total tie strength, and tie evenness. We used linear mixed models to assess how group-level and individual factors influenced these measures, repeating the analysis for ‘peer’ partners (other adult females) only and for ‘non-peers’ (juveniles and adult males) to evaluate how including different partner sets affected the results. The number of available partners influenced all social measures regardless of partner criteria, while presence of relatives affected only the number of strong ties and total tie strength. Age and dominance rank affected number of ties to non-peer social partners, but had no significant effect on ties to peers. Infant presence increased total tie strength among peers, but reduced it for all partners and non-peers. The consistent effect of number of available partners conflicts with the hypothesis that time constraints limit sociality. Partner inclusion criteria influenced the strength and direction of some effects, showing that this analytical choice can influence conclusions, particularly when factors like age, rank, and maternity differentially affect ties to various partner classes.
Article
Full-text available
Phenotypic plasticity, the ability of an individual to modify its phenotype according to the conditions it experiences, is a source of between-individual variation and a mechanism by which individuals can cope with environmental change. Plasticity is expected to evolve in response to environmental heterogeneity, such as seasonality and year-to-year variation. We aimed to characterize patterns of phenotypic change in morphological (body mass), life-history (reproductive success and litter size), and social (embeddedness) traits of female yellow-bellied marmots (Marmota flaviventris) in response to climatic and social variation. We used data collected over 36 years on a population of yellow-bellied marmots studied in Colorado. We used mixed effect models to explore phenotypically plastic responses and tested for individual variation in mean trait values (i.e., intercept) and in plasticity (i.e., slope). All examined traits were plastic, and the population’s average plastic response often differed between spatially distinct colonies that varied systematically in timing of snowmelt, among age classes, and between females with different previous reproductive experiences. Moreover, we showed individual differences in June mass and pup mass plasticity. We suggest that plasticity plays a key role buffering the effects of continuous changes in environmental conditions.
Article
Full-text available
Patterns of infection and prevalence result from complex interactions between hosts and parasites, the effects of which are likely to vary by species. We investigated the effects of age, sex and season on the likelihood of individual infection, and the effects of host population size, sex ratio and age structure on parasite prevalence. We capitalized on data from a long‐term study of yellow‐bellied marmots Marmota flaviventris potentially infected with fecal–orally transmitted intestinal parasites (Ascaris sp., Eimeria spp. and Entamoeba sp.), ectoparasitic fleas Thrassis stanfordi, and a flea‐ and louse‐transmitted blood parasite Trypanosoma lewisi. Patterns of individual‐ and group‐level infection varied widely by parasite. Yearlings were more likely to be infected with Tr. lewisi and Ascaris. Yearlings were also slightly more likely than adults to have Eimeria, but female yearlings had higher infection levels than female adults, while male yearlings had lower infection levels than male adults. Entamoeba infection decreased as the season progressed. Adults and males were more likely to be infected with Th. stanfordi. Ascaris prevalence increased with colony size. There were no significant relationships between colony size and prevalence of Entamoeba, Tr. lewisi, Eimeria or Thrassis. There was a small, but significant positive correlation between male‐biased sex ratio and prevalence of fleas. The host population's age structure affected the prevalence of infection of Ascaris and Eimeria. Overall intestinal parasite diversity increased with colony size. Taken together, our results show a great deal of variation in the likelihood of individual infection and patterns of parasite prevalence in marmots.
Article
The intuitive background for measures of structural centrality in social networks is reviewed and existing measures are evaluated in terms of their consistency with intuitions and their interpretability.
Article
The broad limits of mature colony size in social insect species are likely to be set by ecological factors. However, any change in colony size has a number of important social consequences. The most fundamental is a change in the expected reproductive potential of workers. If colony size rises, workers experience a fall in their chances of becoming replacement reproductives and, it is shown, increasing selection for mutual inhibition of one another's reproduction (worker policing). As workers' reproductive potential falls, the degree of dimorphism between reproductive and worker castes (morphological skew) can rise. This helps explain why small societies have low morphological skew and tend to be simple in organization, whereas large societies have high morphological skew and tend to be complex. The social consequences of change in colony size may also alter colony size itself in a process of positive feedback. For these reasons, small societies should be characterized by intense, direct conflict over reproduction and caste determination. By contrast, conflict in large societies should predominantly be over brood composition, and members of these societies should be relatively compliant to manipulation of their caste. Colony size therefore deserves fuller recognition as a key determinant, along with kin structure, of social complexity, the reproductive potential of helpers, the degree of caste differentiation, and the nature of within-group conflict.
Article
The conceptual framework presented involves three levels-interactions, relationships (described by the content, quality and patterning of interactions) and structure (described by the content, quality and patterning of relationships). At each level it is necessary to abstract generalisations valid over a range of instances, and to seek for organisational principles which account for the patterning at that level. 'Institutions' appear both as abstractions from the surface structure and as organisational principles.
Article
According to behavioural ecology theory, sociality evolves when the net benefits of close association with conspecifics exceed the costs. The nature and relative magnitude of the benefits and costs of sociality are expected to vary across species and habitats. When sociality is favoured, animals may form groups that range from small pair-bonded units to huge aggregations. The size and composition of social groups have diverse effects on morphology and behaviour, ranging from the extent of sexual dimorphism to brain size, and the structure of social relationships. This general argument implies that sociality has fitness consequences for individuals. However, for most mammalian species, especially long-lived animals like primates, there are sizable gaps in the chain of evidence that links sociality and social bonds to fitness outcomes. These gaps reflect the difficulty of quantifying the cumulative effects of behavioural interactions on fitness and the lack of information about the nature of social relationships among individuals in most taxa. Here, I review what is known about the reproductive consequences of sociality for mammals.
Article
The first generation of research on animal social networks was primarily aimed at introducing the concept of social networks to the fields of animal behaviour and behavioural ecology. More recently, a diverse body of evidence has shown that social fine structure matters on a broader scale than initially expected, affecting many key ecological and evolutionary processes. Here, we review this development. We discuss the effects of social network structure on evolutionary dynamics (genetic drift, fixation probabilities, and frequency-dependent selection) and social evolution (cooperation and between-individual behavioural differences). We discuss how social network structure can affect important coevolutionary processes (host–pathogen interactions and mutualisms) and population stability. We also discuss the potentially important, but poorly studied, role of social network structure on dispersal and invasion. Throughout, we highlight important areas for future research.