ArticlePDF Available

Preliminary analysis from a novel treatment targeting the exchange of new information within storytelling for people with nonfluent aphasia and their partners

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Background: Therapy for people with aphasia (PWA) can encompass a wide range of aims and methodologies from targeting the linguistic impairment to strategic compensation to optimise communication, interaction, and vocational rehabilitation. Aphasia rehabilitation ultimately has a social goal of optimising the communication of PWA within their typical environment. Therefore, across treatment type, one unifying area of interest relates to the generalisation of behaviours targeted in therapy to untrained tasks and contexts, particularly those related to everyday communication. Two important aspects of everyday communication relate to conveying new information and telling anecdotes/stories. Measures of transactional success in storytelling have previously demonstrated reliability and validity as an analytical method.
Content may be subject to copyright.
This article was downloaded by: [La Trobe University]
On: 04 May 2015, At: 17:22
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered
office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK
Click for updates
Aphasiology
Publication details, including instructions for authors and
subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/paph20
Preliminary analysis from a novel
treatment targeting the exchange of
new information within storytelling
for people with nonfluent aphasia and
their partners
Marcella Carraghera, Karen Sageb & Paul Conroyc
a Department of Human Communication Sciences, La Trobe
University, Melbourne, Australia
b Bristol Speech & Language Therapy Research Unit, University of
the West of England, Bristol, UK
c Neuroscience & Aphasia Research Unit, University of Manchester,
Manchester, UK
Published online: 15 Dec 2014.
To cite this article: Marcella Carragher, Karen Sage & Paul Conroy (2014): Preliminary analysis from
a novel treatment targeting the exchange of new information within storytelling for people with
nonfluent aphasia and their partners, Aphasiology, DOI: 10.1080/02687038.2014.988110
To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2014.988110
PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE
Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the
“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,
our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to
the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions
and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,
and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content
should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources
of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,
proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or
howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising
out of the use of the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any
substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,
systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &
Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-
and-conditions
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
Preliminary analysis from a novel treatment targeting the exchange of
new information within storytelling for people with nonfluent aphasia
and their partners
Marcella Carragher
a
*, Karen Sage
b
and Paul Conroy
c
a
Department of Human Communication Sciences, La Trobe University, Melbourne, Australia;
b
Bristol Speech & Language Therapy Research Unit, University of the West of England, Bristol,
UK;
c
Neuroscience & Aphasia Research Unit, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK
(Received 30 March 2014; accepted 12 November 2014)
Background: Therapy for people with aphasia (PWA) can encompass a wide range ofaims
and methodologies from targeting the linguistic impairment to strategic compensation to
optimise communication, interaction, and vocational rehabilitation. Aphasia rehabilitation
ultimately has a social goal of optimising the communication of PWAwithin their typical
environment. Therefore, across treatment type, one unifying area of interest relates to the
generalisation of behaviours targeted in therapy to untrained tasks and contexts, particu-
larly those related to everyday communication. Two important aspects of everyday
communication relate to conveying new information and telling anecdotes/stories.
Measures of transactional success in storytelling have previously demonstrated reliability
and validity as an analytical method.
Aims: The study aimed to extend previous work on transactional success in storytelling to
a programme of therapy targeting both the PWA andthe communication partner (CP). The
effects of therapy were measured in untrained novel storytelling tasks.
Methods & Procedures: Four participants with chronic nonfluent aphasia and their CPs
were recruited and a novel dual-focus treatment was administered. For the PWA, therapy
targeted storytelling using the principles of thinking for speakingand story grammar. For
the CP, therapy drew on the principles of conversation coaching to increase facilitative
behaviours within storytelling to aid coconstruction of the story.
Outcomes & Results: Outcomes were influenced by the complexity of the target
stimuli: following therapy, three PWA demonstrated increased transfer of new informa-
tion within untrained simple storytelling. For the untrained complex storytelling
stimuli, improvements were limited to two PWA. There were also changes in how
PWA produced stories (i.e., the order of events within the story), with simple stories
becoming more inline with the sequence used by control participants. Regarding the
CPs, again there was an effect of complexity: for the untrained simple story, three CPs
improved the accuracy of their understanding while for the untrained complex story,
two CPs improved. Interestingly, one CP consistently demonstrated a decrease in the
amount of information successfully understood following therapy, despite improved
information exchange by her partner with aphasia.
Conclusions: The preliminary findings of this dual-focused therapy offer promise in
terms of targeting a range of skills relating to both the PWA and CP, whilst the context
of information exchange and storytelling promotes likelihood of generalisation of
targeted behaviours to untrained tasks.
Keywords: nonfluent aphasia; generalisation; interactive storytelling; information exchange
*Corresponding author. Email: m.carragher@latrobe.edu.au
Aphasiology, 2014
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2014.988110
© 2014 Taylor & Francis
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
1. Introduction
Therapy for people with aphasia (PWA) encompasses a wide range of aims and methodolo-
gies from targeting the linguistic impairment (e.g., Carragher, Sage, & Conroy, 2013),
communication compensation (e.g., Hopper, Holland, & Rewega, 2002), interaction (e.g.,
Beeke, Maxim, Best, & Cooper, 2011), and vocational rehabilitation (e.g., Morris, Franklin,
Menger, & GD, 2011). Across all treatment approaches, one unifying area of interest relates to
the generalisation of behaviours targeted in therapy to untrained tasks and contexts, particu-
larly those related to everyday communication. However, capturing evidence of change
within everyday communication has proved difficult. Researchers within the field continue
to explore various methodologies in an attempt to reach consensus on the type of data we
should collect as well as optimal outcome measures and elicitation contexts in which to
measure the effects of therapy.
With regard to the question of which elicitation type and which data should we use to
measure the effects of treatment on everyday communication, one option relates to the
collection and analysis of naturally occurring conversation data. Certainly, conversation has
been observed to be the most common type of daily communication for PWA and matched
healthy control participants (Davidson, Worrall, & Hickson, 2003). Yet capturing evidence
of quantitative change in conversation has proved difficult, not least because no standar-
dised, quantitative measure of conversation exists (Beeke et al., 2011). Moreover, the high
demands of time and skill needed to carry out qualitative analysis of conversation present
challenges for service delivery in busy clinical settings (Bradley & Douglas, 2008).
Everyday communication is multifaceted, encompassing not only interaction but also
transaction (Davidson et al., 2003). One solution might be to use the naturally occurring
transactional opportunities in everyday communication to target treatment and to capture
evidence of change. Transactional communication encompasses various types of discourse
genreconversation, expository, procedural (Armstrong, 2000). Central to transactional
communication is the conveying of new information, an aspect that is often responsible
for conversational failure for PWA (Ramsberger & Rende, 2002). Conversation data do
not lend itself to the measurement of transactional success for a number of reasons: lack of
external criteria on which to judge transactional success (Ramsberger & Rende, 2002);
potential lack of clarity regarding a speakers target word or meaning (Armstrong, 2000);
potential for a dissociation between the information expressed by the speaker and how this
is understood by the communication partner (CP) (Ramsberger & Rende, 2002); as well as
the opportunity for speakers to draw on shared knowledge, which may not be expressed
explicitly. Thus, in order to measure transactional success, it is necessary to use a context
that shares similarities to conversation but, crucially, offers potential for externally set
criteria and standardisation. One such context is storytelling, which offers a broad plat-
form on which to base outcome measurement and treatment. There are several advantages
to using storytelling data within therapeutic studies:
Social perspective: storytelling is a means of self-expression (McAdams, 2001),
displaying and experiencing an evolving identity (Bierren, Kenyon, Ruth, Shroots, &
Svendson, 1996), engaging with others and passing on life experience (Randall, 2001).
Storytelling is a way in which we make sense of the world particularly during
challenging life transitions and traumatic events (Riessman, 1993), both of which are
relevant for individuals and their families living with the chronic effects of stroke.
Validity: like everyday conversation, interactive storytelling captures evidence of
speakersturn-taking and negotiating the pointof the story (Norrick, 2000).
2M. Carragher et al.
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
Furthermore, narrative stimuli are rich with options as to what will be commu-
nicated and the perspective adopted by the speaker. This presents choices to the
PWA regarding expression of story events through verbal and/or nonverbal means,
compared to more traditional language assessment that places constraints on pos-
sible linguistic responses and syntactic constructions (Hernandez-Sacristan &
Rosell-Clari, 2009).
Clinical validity: PWA engage in significantly less storytelling in daily life com-
pared to healthy controls (Davidson et al., 2003). Thus, the powerful benefits of
storytelling (as a way of engaging with others and as a means of coping) are
beyond the reach of a population who could benefit from this social activity. This
suggests that storytelling is a clinically valid context for treatment and outcome
measurement.
Linguistic perspective: production of narrative/storytelling encompasses a wealth of
linguistic skills. This includes macrolinguistic skills (e.g., the planning and sequen-
cing of information within a structured framework and tailored towards the listen-
ers perspective) and mircolinguistic skills (i.e., semantic and syntactic aspects of
production), which resonate throughout many language production activities in
daily life (Whitworth, 2010).
Methodological rigour: as an outcome measure, storytelling offers several advantages
including replicability, the potential for standardisation across participants, and an
opportunity for comparison of performances across individuals and to nonlanguage-
impaired control participants (Ramsberger & Menn, 2003; Ramsberger & Rende,
2002).
The current study stems from observations across the literature of mixed or underwhelm-
ing findings of generalisation following impairment-focused therapy. There is growing
evidence from therapy literature of the need to explicitly support PWA to generalise the
skills developed within therapy sessions to everyday communication (e.g., Carragher
et al., 2013; Whitworth, 2010). We hypothesised that targeting the combination of several
key ingredients might facilitate generalisation of behaviours targeted in therapy to an
untrained everyday communication task. These ingredients included the following:
(1) targeting microlinguistic and macrolinguistic skills and shaping nonverbal output
and compensatory strategies (e.g., reduced syntax and direct reported speech) to
optimise the communication of new information;
(2) using principles of thinking for speaking to improve narrative planning and
production for PWA;
(3) including the CP within the therapeutic focus to prime them to be receptive to the
PWAs compensated storytelling and to shape their own behaviours to optimise
uptakeof the PWAs improved skills; and
(4) targeting information exchange within the everyday activity of storytelling. We
hypothesised that positioning therapy within the everyday activity of information
exchange and storytelling would close the gap and present less of a challenge for
PWA to generalise their improved skills to everyday use.
The current study builds on work by Ramsberger and colleagues (Ramsberger & Menn,
2003; Ramsberger & Rende, 2002) by extending interactive storytelling to a therapy task.
The paper outlines the novel approach of Interactive Storytelling Therapy,a
Aphasiology 3
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
standardised approach to shaping and enhancing the exchange of new information
between PWA and their CPs within a storytelling context.
2. Aims of the study
The current study will present a preliminary analysis from a pilot study of the effects of
Interactive Storytelling Therapy on untrained tasks. Specifically, the study aimed to
answer the following questions:
Following therapy, do participants with a range of severities of nonfluent aphasia
successfully convey more information in response to untrained narrative stimuli?
Following therapy, are CPs more successful in interpreting new information within
untrained narrative tasks?
Following therapy, are there changes in the narrative structure used by the
PWA?
3. Method
3.1. Participants
Following ethical approval via standard UK protocols (NHS IRAS system), four PWA
were recruited. This study formed the third in a series of therapy studies targeting
incremental levels of language output in individuals with nonfluent aphasia. As part of
a larger group of participants (N= 9), these four participants had taken part in previous
therapy studies targeting verb retrieval (Carragher et al., 2013) and syntactic construction
(Carragher, Sage, & Conroy, in press). All participants presented with stroke-induced
chronic nonfluent aphasia. Presentation of nonfluent aphasia was confirmed on the basis
of converging evidence from clinical consensus, the results of standardised lexical
retrieval assessment (as indicated by a clinical score on the Boston Naming Test) and
impaired use of grammatical markers and syntactic structures in picture description
(Goodglass, Kaplan, & Barresi, 2001). Participants were at least 6 months postonset,
reducing the likelihood of further spontaneous recovery. As apraxia of speech often
co-occurs with nonfluent aphasia (McNeil, Robin, & Schmidt, 2008), presence of apraxic
errors did not form part of the exclusion criteria. Interparticipant variation existed for time
postonset, ranging from 26 to 80 months (mean: 51.5, SD: 27.5). The participants ranged
in age from 38 to 70 years (mean: 59.5, SD: 14.5); Table 1 provides background
information on the four participants with aphasia. In the current study, CPs participated
Table 1. Background information on the participants with aphasia.
Participants Gender
Age of leaving
education (years) Handedness Occupation
Age at time of
stroke (years)
TPO
(months)
BL Male 16 Right Pub manager 60 80
JH Female 23 Right Teacher 36 26
AT Female 16 Right Secretary 62 30
PM Male 16 Right Businessman 64 70
Note: TPO: time postonset.
4M. Carragher et al.
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
within the treatment. In each case, the CP was the PWAs husband or wife, had known the
PWA prior to the stroke, and had no history of neurological impairment. Written informed
consent was obtained for each PWA and his/her CP prior to commencement of the study.
3.2. Background assessment
Interparticipant variation existed for severity: noun naming (Boston Naming Test, Goodglass
et al., 2001) ranged from 16 to 36 from a maximum score of 60 (mean: 26.3, SD:10.0);verb
naming (The Object Action Naming Battery, Druks & Masterson, 2000) ranged from 30.5 to
59 from a maximum score of 100 (mean: 44.3, SD: 11.7). Further details of the PWA and their
performance on a battery of linguistic and cognitive assessments are provided in Carragher
et al. (2013).
3.3. Assessment stimuli
Pretherapy and posttherapy assessment consisted of interactive storytelling in response to
video stimuli. At each time point, the PWA watched a video clip in the absence of the CP;
the CP then returned to the room and the PWA recounted the story. No instructions or
limitations were placed on the participants during their discussion of the video clip, i.e.,
they were not directed to, nor restricted from, using gesture, pointing, drawing, or writing.
The only instruction issued to the CP was that the PWA had viewed a video clip, they
were asked to find out what happened in the clip, and that they would later report their
interpretation of it to the researcher. Assessment stimuli included a simple video narrative
and a complex video narrative. Drawing on Weinrich, McCall, Boser, and Viratas criteria
(2002), simple narratives were defined as video clips that involved only one to two actors,
one to two complicating actions and a resolution; complex narratives were defined as
video clips that involved more than two actors, four complicating actions, and a resolu-
tion. Data collected from control participants (N= 8) were used to distinguish simple
narrative video material from complex narrative material (see Outcome measures section
for more details on the collection and analysis of control data).
Assessment stimuli at both time points consisted of Mr. Bean
1
DVD footage. These
video clips were chosen for their minimal spoken language content, thereby minimising
the linguistic scaffolding available to the PWA in constructing the story. Cultural famil-
iarity was a further factor in the selection of assessment stimuli—“Mr. Beanclips contain
highly familiar/imageable concepts and humorous content which is watched by adults as
well as children. Similar to real-life communication, once the referent of Mr. Bean had
been established, the CP would have access to some shared knowledge about the
protagonist (e.g., Whitworth, 2010). In order to minimise the effects of memory or
practice, novel stimuli were used across pretherapy and posttherapy assessment although
they were based on the same comic character. CPs were not told in advance the subject or
nature of the narrative topics in the assessment video stimuli.
3.4. Therapy stimuli
For the therapy sessions, video clips were sourced from YouTube and viewed by PWA
using an iPad. The Mr. Bean video footage was not used within therapy sessions; they
were reserved for pretherapy and posttherapy assessment only. Therefore, outcome mea-
surement reflects participantsability to generalise behaviours targeted within therapy to
untrained, novel narrative stimuli. Video clips were selected for their interesting and
Aphasiology 5
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
newsworthy nature; they were often funny and therefore motivating for the couple to
discuss. The therapy video clips involved minimal or no use of language. As the PWA
presented across a range of aphasic severity, it was important that the video clips used
within the therapy sessions were capable of challenging the higher-level participants
whilst not alienating those PWA with fewer linguistic and communicative abilities.
Therefore, selection of the therapy stimuli erred on the side of complex narratives.
Within the therapy sessions, the higher-level participants were encouraged to include
details within their story construction whilst the participants with a more severe aphasic
impairment were encouraged to construct a more stripped-back story structure. See
Appendix 1 for an example of the video stimuli used during therapy. Outlined in the
following are the YouTube clips selected for the therapy sessions, the length of each clip,
and their current web address:
Seaplane fishing (00:54), http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=iY6AWs2QMbM
Pixar: Geris game (03:50), http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=9IYRC7g2ICg
Pixar: Pigeons (02:40), http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=oIlIVFBBbNw
Pixar: For the birds (03:00), http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=VkuBIrdi6eE
French clip (01:52), http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=3xAE6gjvQ7Q
3.5. Overview of Interactive Storytelling Therapy sessions
Therapy was delivered by the first author who is an experienced speech and language
therapist. In line with routine clinical intervention within the UK, participants received six
therapy sessions administered once a week. Each session lasted for approximately 1.5 hr.
Within each session, up to 45 min was dedicated to working with the PWA, up to 30 min
to working with the CP, and the remainder of the session used for video feedback and
discussion with the couple. The first therapy session focused on reflecting on current
storytelling behaviours before targeting these behaviours in subsequent practical sessions
(sessions 26). Figure 1 outlines the focus of therapy across sessions.
3.5.1. Session 1: reflection and goal-setting
The first treatment session focused on encouraging the PWA and their CP to reflect on the
baseline video-recording of their storytelling and to begin to increase their awareness of
various strategies and choices evident within their interactions. Video feedback was used to
facilitate discussion of the consequences of specific behaviours seen in the data. These
included strategies used by the PWA to convey events; strategies used by the CP to clarify
information or elicit further explanation; displays of negative emotion such as frustration;
alternatives to strategies seen in the video data; and, more broadly, sharing of the commu-
nicative burden and the overall effectiveness/success of the interaction. Couples were encour-
aged to extend their reflections beyond the recorded interactive storytelling to consider their
everyday conversations. During this initial session, therapy goals specific to each couple were
suggested, based on analysis of baseline interactive storytelling (see Appendix 2). The goals
were given brief descriptive, mnemonic labels (e.g., Drip drip and Pinpointsee Appendix 2
for definitions) to facilitate participants to remember their individual goals and also to aid
discussion of specific strategies within the therapy sessions. For the PWA, therapy goals
related to components of story grammar (Ramsberger & Menn, 2003; Ramsberger & Rende,
6M. Carragher et al.
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
2002), such as introducing key referents, while for the CP, therapy goals related to repairing
breakdowns in understanding.
3.5.2. Practical sessions 26: PWA
This part of the treatment drew upon the principles of thinking for speaking (Marshall,
2009) and story grammar (Rumelhart, 1975). The practical sessions began with the PWA
viewing a video clip in the absence of their partner (see Figure 1). The video clip was
repeated as often as requested (participants usually requested a maximum of three
repeated viewings). The therapist facilitated the PWA to segment the narrative into
main events, broadly conceptualised as the beginning, middle, and end sections of the
story. Where relevant, the PWA was prompted to begin by introducing the story (Set the
scene goal) by stating the main referent as well as other contextual information such as
location or tone of the story (e.g., funny or sad). Throughout this process, the PWA was
supported in his/her conceptualisation of the story through a visual record; the researcher
used this to record the ongoing construction of the story by writing down words/phrases
Figure 1. Overview of therapy sessions.
Aphasiology 7
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
produced by the PWA and using drawing to depict gesture. The visual record served as a
useful anchor by which the PWA could monitor their progression as they constructed the
story.
Having established the main referent of the story, the PWA was prompted to think about
what happened next in segments (corresponding to the Chunk it up and Drip drip goals). This
involved describing key information and actions relating to the main referent. The PWA was
encouraged to produce an agentverb construction, with the verb produced verbally or
through gesture, writing or drawing. The aim was to optimise (rather than correct) partici-
pantsoutput; therefore, any prompts or modelling provided by the therapist were carefully
built on the participants original output. For example, if the PWA gestured running,the
researcher prompted Who? followed by the gesture, with the aim of prompting the PWA to
produce a more contentful construction incorporating both verbal and nonverbal output
(related to the Show and Tell goal). If the PWA produced a content word in isolation (e.g.,
hungry), the researcher used wh-questions (e.g., who is hungry?) and modelling (e.g., bird
hungry) to facilitate the PWAs production of argument structure. In line with a previous
therapy study (Carragher et al., in press), all modelling of syntactic constructions involved
morphologically reduced structures to maintain focus on communicative value rather than
grammatical correctness. The PWA was also facilitated to use direct reported speech (Hengst,
Frame, Neuman-Stritzel, & Gannaway, 2005) to depict charactersreactions within the story
and to produce evaluative comments in grammatically simplified ways. When the opportunity
arose whilst constructing the story, the therapist modelled how the participant could make use
of direct reported speech to convey tone and emotion and to carry some of the lexical and
grammatical load of narrative production. Furthermore, written feedback was used to demon-
strate how direct reported speech eliminates the need for numerous function words and
morphology (e.g., girlcup of tea please rather than the girl said that she wanted a cup
of tea).
As the PWA progressed through the telling of each episode within the story, the
segmentation of the story was reinforced visually through the use of the visual record, i.e.,
clearly marking the first, second, third, fourth, etc. episodes of the story. This process was
repeated until the complete story had been discussed and sketched out in the visual record.
Throughout the story construction, the PWA was prompted to think selectively in terms of
what details to include or omit from the story to ultimately facilitate their partners
comprehension of the story. In particular, the PWA was encouraged to consider whether
a particular event or detail was key to understanding the story or more peripheral (issue of
selectivity raised by Marshall & Cairns, 2005).
By the end of this part of the session, the participant had produced the story three
times in total, with incremental withdrawal of support from the researcher:
(1) During the first telling, the PWA was maximally supported by the therapist to
segment the story into events, to prioritise establishing key referents, and to
combine verbal output with gesture, drawing, and writing. The therapist kept a
visual record of the story, which included key words, phrases, and drawings.
(2) In the second telling, the PWA was prompted to use the visual record to construct
the story. Moderate support was given to remind the PWA about the strategies
discussed and developed during the first story telling. Also at this stage, partici-
pants were facilitated to link together the various events within the story either
verbally (e.g., using connective devices such as and then) or nonverbally (e.g.,
using gestures or fingers to indicate first, second, third, etc.).
8M. Carragher et al.
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
(3) During the third telling of the story, the visual record was removed and PWA were
encouraged to construct the story independently, with the therapist providing
feedback or requesting clarification where necessary.
The aim here was not to foster rote-learning of a particular story. Rather, the approach was
to gradually withdraw support and to encourage independent use of key strategies to
support the PWA in constructing the story in an optimal, coherent manner with regard to
the sequencing of ideas and relevant information.
3.5.3. Practical sessions 26: CPs
The CP then rejoined the therapy session in order to discuss the video clip with their
partner with aphasia. At this point, the CP became the focus of therapy intervention (see
Figure 1). Therapy sessions were video recorded with the participantsconsent in order to
facilitate later reflection. The therapist prompted the CP to recall the therapy goals agreed
at the start of the intervention; as therapy progressed over a number of weeks, this
discussion expanded to include topics that had arisen in earlier sessions. As the couple
began to discuss the story, the therapist intervened on a needs-basis when a trouble source
arose that the CP struggled to resolve. For example, the therapist offered a diagnosis of the
problem (i.e., relating to a lexical search, confusion regarding a referent, or a broader issue
regarding which part of the story was currently being discussed) and facilitated the CP to
select one of the targeted goal behaviours to employ, e.g., Move along or Stop and check
(see Appendix 2). If the CP struggled to select a strategy, the therapist suggested an
appropriate strategy and modelled this behaviour as needed. The therapist did not inter-
vene if the PWA omitted important details of the story or confirmed details about the story
that were incorrect; the goal of therapy related to the exchange and negotiation of
information between the couples rather than conveying specific details.
3.5.4. Practical sessions 26: the couple
Once the couple had finished discussing the story, the CP watched the target YouTube
video clip and then together the couple viewed the video recording of them discussing the
story (Figure 1). This enabled both the PWA and CP to engage in offline evaluation of the
strategies employed within the task. Discussion focused on the agreed goals for each
individual; where relevant, discussion included any novel issues that had arisen during the
session and goals were agreed for each couple to focus on in the homework task and in
the subsequent therapy session. The homework task consisted of each couple setting aside
time to each practice using goal behaviour in an everyday conversation and reflecting on
whether the strategy had been useful. The types of conversations in which participants
were encouraged to use the goals behaviours included catching up on what the PWA did
at the stroke group, relaying a conversation with a neighbour, etc. Stimuli were not
provided for the homework task; rather, the focus was placed on each couple developing
an awareness of opportunities for changed interactive behaviours and selecting strategies
as needed.
3.6. Outcome measures
Outcome measurement focused on transactional success (i.e., exchange of new informa-
tion) in comparison to control data. Control participants (N= 8) viewed the Mr. Bean
Aphasiology 9
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
video clips (used in pretherapy and posttherapy assessment) and were asked to describe
what happened. The control participants were nonlanguage-impaired, native English
speakers. They were not matched to the PWA in the current study but represented a
varied sample with respect to age (mean: 42 years; range: 1764), years of full-time
education (mean: 16 years; range: 1121) and gender (four males and four females). The
control participantsdescriptions of the Mr. Bean video clips varied regarding quantity of
description as well as the details provided (e.g., one control participant described Mr.
Bean driving a yellow car, another described Mr. Bean driving a yellow Mini, while
another simply reported Mr. Bean drove into a car park and omitted any details relating to
the car). In order to condense the control participantsdescriptions to the core story
components, written transcripts of the control participantsdescriptions were analysed for
the most commonly reported content words. Those content words that were reported by at
least 50% of control participants were interpreted as forming essential components or
ideas of the target story. Thus, content words that were produced by at least 50% of the
control participants were labelled salient content words. In this way, the control data
provided a maximum score for each Mr. Bean video clip. These salientcontent words
were used to develop model narratives for each clip consisting of the crucial parts of story
structure, i.e., setting, complicating actions, and resolution (Labov, 1972). The target
components for each assessment video clip are shown in the shaded columns in
Appendix 3. A similar measure of transactional success in storytelling had demonstrated
high validity and reliability as a method of analysis (Ramsberger & Menn, 2003;
Ramsberger & Rende, 2002).
3.7. Data analysis
Using data from control participants, it was possible to segment each assessment narrative
into distinct story segments, with a core group of target content words within each
segment. In the baseline storytelling data, differences in main ideas reported reflect
difference in the complexity of the narrative stimuli and are in line with data from the
control participants, i.e., control participants, PWA, and CPs produced more narrative
output for complex stimuli.
For each assessment point, storytelling data for each couple (i.e., Mr. Bean stimuli)
were transcribed by individuals who were blind to the aim of the treatment and the
sampling point; the first author verified accuracy of all transcripts. For the PWA, the
written transcripts were analysed to identify instances when they produced (either verbally
or nonverbally) a main idea that corresponded to those content words most frequently
produced across control participants (see Appendix 3 for the maximum score achievable
for each stimulus). For the CPs, written transcripts of their reporting of the story were
subjected to the same analysis (see Appendix 3). In this way, PWA and the CPs were
credited only for the information that was deemed essential across control participants.
4. Results
4.1. Number of salient ideas communicated by the PWA
It was not possible to carry out statistical analysis given (1) the inherent variability in
sampling phenomena such as information exchange within narrative production, (2) lack
of data on stability of information exchange over sampling periods, and (3) the lack of
rigidly defined accurate/inaccurate responses, i.e., CPsretelling of the narratives were
10 M. Carragher et al.
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
compared to the salient content words produced by the control participants; close syno-
nyms were accepted as correct. Raw and percentage data are presented for each PWA
and CP.
For the PWA, Table 2 outlines the raw and percentage data for the changes in number
of main ideas reported. Data are presented separately for the simple and complex narrative
stimuli. Following therapy, the simple narrative condition demonstrated numeric improve-
ments across the board: three participants improved their communication of salient ideas
(AT, BL, and JH) whilst the remaining participant (PM) demonstrated a slight drop of
0.5%. The complex narrative condition proved more challenging with only two partici-
pants (AT and PM) demonstrating increases in the number of salient ideas they expressed,
whilst two participants demonstrated numeric decreases (BL and JH).
4.2. Number of salient ideas communicated by the CPs
Following therapy, for the simple narrative stimuli, three CPs demonstrated numeric
increases in the number of main ideas they had successfully understood (partners of AT,
BL, and JH) whilst the remaining CP (partner of PM) demonstrated a drop of 12.8% (see
Table 3). For the complex narrative stimuli, two CPs (partners of AT and BL) demon-
strated numeric increases in the number of main ideas they understood, whilst two CPs
demonstrated numeric decreases (partners of PM and JH).
The pre/posttherapy data for simple and complex narrative stimuli were collapsed
together for each time point in order to compare mean change for each PWA and his/her
Table 2. Comparison of salient content words reported by the participants with aphasia in
pretherapy and posttherapy storytelling (% data shown in brackets).
Stimuli PWA Pretherapy Posttherapy Difference (%)
Simple AT 9 (60.0%) 11 (84.6%) +24.6
PM 7 (46.7%) 6 (46.2%) 0.5
BL 6 (40.0%) 6 (46.2%) +6.2
JH 6 (40.0%) 8 (61.6%) +21.6
Complex AT 12 (44.4%) 21 (67.7%) +23.3
PM 13 (48.2%) 18 (58.1%) +9.9
BL 8 (29.6%) 8 (25.8%) 3.8
JH 13 (48.2%) 14 (45.2%) 3.0
Table 3. Comparison of salient content words reported by the CPs in pretherapy and posttherapy
storytelling (% data shown in brackets).
Stimuli CPs Pretherapy Posttherapy Difference (%)
Simple AT 9 (60.0%) 9 (69.2%) +9.2
PM 10 (66.7%) 7 (53.9%) 12.8
BL 4 (26.7%) 7 (53.9%) +27.2
JH 10 (66.7%) 12 (92.3%) +25.6
Complex AT 4 (14.8%) 16 (51.6%) +36.8
PM 13 (48.2%) 13 (41.9%) 6.2
BL 3 (11.1%) 8 (25.8%) +14.7
JH 16 (59.3%) 18 (58.1%) 1.2
Aphasiology 11
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
CP. Interestingly, the changes in the number of salient ideas observed in the performance
of the PWA do not appear to reliably predict changes in how many ideas will be under-
stood by their CPs (see Table 4 and Figure 2):
Following therapy, for the simple narrative AT improved her communication of
salient ideas by 24.6% whilst her CP improved by a smaller margin (9.2%). For
complex narrative, the reverse is true: AT produced 23.3% more salient ideas but
her CP was correct in interpreting her expression by 36.8%.
For PM, he communicated slightly less salient ideas for simple narrative after
therapy (0.5%); his partner also demonstrated a decrease in the number of ideas
she successfully understood (12.8%). For complex narrative, PM produced more
salient ideas following therapy (9.9%), but this did not result in his partner
successfully interpreting these (6.2%).
Following therapy, BL increased his communication of salient ideas for simple
narrative (6.2%), which his partner responded to by successfully understanding
27.2% more of these salient ideas. For complex narrative, BL produced less salient
ideas (3.8%), but his partner was able to compensate for this and understood
14.7% more salient ideas compared to pretherapy levels.
JH communicated 21.6% more salient ideas for simple narrative following therapy
and this was also reflected in her partners understanding (which increased by
25.6%). However, for complex stimuli, her communication of salient ideas reduced
by 3% and her partners understanding of these salient ideas also reduced by 1.2%.
4.3. Narrative sequence
For each PWA, therapy had emphasised and provided opportunities to practice narrative
planning skills (e.g., beginning by introducing the main referent, building up the story
event-by-event) and expanding content of output using verbal and nonverbal means (e.g.,
pairing a spoken agent with a gestured action to fill the verb slot within the construction).
Given the emphasis on narrative planning and structure, we analysed the output of the
PWA to investigate whether the order in which they delivered each narrative changed
following therapy. The control data provided a guide for the order in which the control
participants described each event within the story. Storytelling data from the PWA were
coded to identify each content word produced and to which scene it corresponded within
the target template (which represented an amalgamation of the control participants
production).
For the simple narrative, pretherapy data suggest that some participants (most notably
AT and JH) reported narrative events out of sequence (see Table 5). This may have
Table 4. Comparison of mean change in number of salient content words
reported by the participants with aphasia and their CPs in pretherapy and
posttherapy storytelling.
Participant Mean difference: PWA (%) Mean difference: CP (%)
AT +24.0 +23.0
PM +4.7 9.5
BL +1.2 +20.9
JH +9.3 +12.2
12 M. Carragher et al.
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
Figure 2. Percentage of mean change following therapy for each PWA and CP within untrained
simple and complex narratives.
Aphasiology 13
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
contributed to breakdowns in understanding between the PWA and his/her CP. Following
therapy, all participants improved in their narrative sequencing, i.e., sequencing more
closely followed the structure used by control participants and less needed to revise their
narrative production.
The complex narrative stimuli proved to be more challenging in terms of participants
planning and sequencing (see Table 6). Participants AT and JH produced narrative that
was more closely aligned to the sequence produced by the control participants, i.e.,
reporting the first scene of the target narrative, followed by the second scene, third,
Table 5. Simple narrative: how the participants with aphasia organised and relayed the narrative
events in pretherapy and posttherapy storytelling compared to the structure used by control
participants.
Time point Scene Controls AT PM BL JH
Pretherapy Scene 1 ✓✓
Scene 2 3✓✓3
Scene 3 2✓✓1
Scene 4 4✓✓
122
23
Posttherapy Scene 1 ✓✓
Scene 2 ✓✓
Scene 3 ✓✓
Scene 4 ✓✓31
Table 6. Complex narrative: how the participants with aphasia organised and relayed the narrative
events in pretherapy and posttherapy storytelling compared to the structure used by control
participants.
Time point Scene Controls AT PM BL JH
Pretherapy Scene 1 ✓✓
Scene 2 ✓✓
Scene 3 ✓✓5✓✓
Scene 4 5356
Scene 5 6465
Scene 6 2✓✓6
4
Posttherapy Scene 1 ✓✓
Scene 2 ✓✓3✓✓
Scene 3 ✓✓11
Scene 4 522
Scene 5 4366
Scene 6 ✓✓54
53
4
3
4
6
14 M. Carragher et al.
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
fourth, etc. However, participants PM and BL produced complex narratives that were less
aligned to control narrative sequence compared to their pretherapy level.
5. Discussion
The current study aimed to extend previous work on transactional communication in story-
telling (Ramsberger & Menn, 2003; Ramsberger & Rende, 2002) by conducting preliminary
analyses of the effect of a novel intervention targeting transactional success within storytelling
for people with nonfluent aphasia and their CPs. Interactive Storytelling Therapy targets and
optimises the coconstruction of stories by two people (Bronken, Kirkevold, Martinsen, &
Kvigne, 2012), a feature that may be especially important when one speaker has aphasia.
Therapy consists of a dual-focus targeting both the PWA and the CP in order to optimise the
exchange of new information. By using video clips to stimulate storytelling, the therapy
approach offers a method of standardising treatment sessions whilst maintaining features of
everyday interaction, e.g., the CP is blind to the video content thus simulating a real-life
communicative situation in which the PWA is imparting new information. Interactive
Storytelling Therapy establishes storytelling as a shared communicative activity between
speakers. In this way, it differs from narrative therapy (e.g., Whitworth, 2010), which focuses
solely on the production of the PWA, thereby omitting features of storytelling such as to
whom the story is addressed and how the teller and the recipient interact to achieve mutual
understanding (Goodwin, 1995).
Drawing on the principles of thinking for speaking, PWA were facilitated to segment
video narrative into distinct events, to selectively highlight specific details of the story and to
use a combination of verbal (e.g., syntactically reduced utterances and direct reported speech)
and nonverbal resources (e.g., gesture, writing, and drawing) in order to convey new
information to their partner. Components of story grammar (e.g., setting the scene by
introducing main characters) were used to facilitate narrative planning and production. For
the CPs, therapy drew on the principles of conversation coaching to educate partners on their
role within the interaction and ultimately increase facilitative behaviours within storytelling. It
was hypothesised that the sum of these strands of therapy would be improved negotiation and
construction of shared understanding within storytelling. Therapy was administered once a
week (each session lasting approximately 1.5 hr) for 6 weeks. This decision was based on
typical clinical intervention within the UK. However, optimal intensity/dosages for this
specific treatment are as yet unknown.
Effects of therapy were analysed within untrained narrative tasks by measuring changes in
the number of main ideas, which were (1) successfully communicated by the PWA and (2)
successfully understood by the CP. Video stimuli used within baseline and posttherapy
assessment were novel stimuli not previously seen by the participants. Therefore, any changes
would represent generalisation of behaviours targeted in therapy to untrained tasks. For the
PWA, three participants demonstrated increased communication of salient ideas within
posttherapy simple narrative, and two participants demonstrated increases in complex narra-
tive. For the CPs, numeric increases in simple narrative were seen for three participants, and
for the complex narrative, two participants demonstrated numeric gains. PMs partner was
unique in that she demonstrated a decrease in the amount of information she successfully
interpreted across both simple and complex posttherapy storytelling. This is intriguing given
that her partner with aphasia PM improved in his conveying of main ideas. However, as
Fergadiotis and Harris Wright (2011, p. 1414) argue discourse is successfully produced when
the listener is able to reconstruct the message sent and interpret its intended meaning(p.
1414). Therefore, PMs increased ability to communicate salient ideas from the target story
Aphasiology 15
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
was not sufficient in its own right to increase his partners understanding of the story. This
highlights the need for further work to uncover the reasons behind the discrepancy between
PM and his partners response to therapy. Furthermore, this finding underlines the importance
of explicitly supporting PWA to generalise the behaviours targeted within therapy for use in
everyday communication. PMs partner highlights the need to work with CPs to create space
within the interaction for the PWA to make use of their improved linguistic and commu-
nicative resources and to shape CPsbehaviours to accommodate a speaker with an impaired
language system.
Whilst the participant numbers within the current study are small and the therapy dose
relatively low, preliminary results appear to be promising, suggesting that the combination
of targeting both the PWA and CP within an everyday communicative activity assists in
the negotiation and transfer of new information relative to comparable narratives obtained
at baseline. The approach of evaluating CPsretelling of a narrative to which they were
blind appeared to be a promising outcome measure that was both engaging and of interest
to all of these participants and represented a middle ground between experimentally
controlled tasks for eliciting monologic aphasic data and the more ecological but uncon-
strained sampling of conversation data. Further related research would be aided by
establishing more precise measures relating to narrative complexity through closer match-
ing of related narratives (e.g., ensuring that simple narratives are matched for identical
numbers of complications and key words) This could allow for use of nonparametric
analyses of apparent differences between pretherapy and posttherapy narrative samples in
order to more formally evaluate whether differences are statistically significant.
The current study represents an attempt to develop some degree of standardisation within
an interactive therapy protocol to promote generalisation to untrained tasks. Given the
tradition of interactive and conversation analysis therapy methods of having been highly
data driven and individualised in terms of therapy focus, the method described here represents
an attempt to develop a standardised template for intervention delivery and measurement.
Storytelling plays a vital role in making sense of the world, particularly in the wake of a
traumatic life experience (Kellas & Trees, 2006). Evidence suggests PWA engage signifi-
cantly less in storytelling than their healthy counterparts (Davidson et al., 2003); thus, story-
telling presents a psychosocially and clinically valid context for therapeutic focus. The
method evaluated within the current study has been characterised as a template consisting
of (1) working with the PWA to deliver new information in the context of storytelling and (2)
working with the CP to collaborate in the construction of the story. While the precise advice
and recommended strategies for a particular couple are tailored and individualised, this will be
within the limits of the central task of information exchange. This move towards some flexible
standardisation may support clinical application of this method, given that it is a defined
protocol that can be applied in a time-efficient manner without preplanning. Similarly, use of
first session information exchange measures can serve as baseline measures for posttherapy
evaluation that has ease of use and real-world clinical plausibility. Further research is
warranted in order to identify the exact mechanisms of change (or lack of change in the
case of PMs partner). This could include analysis of the specific goal behaviours that
participants made use of following therapy.
The ultimate goal of aphasia rehabilitation is a social one: to optimize the commu-
nication between the person with aphasia and his or her environment(Van De Sandt-
Koenderman, Van Der Meulen, & Ribbers, 2012, p. S1). The range of aphasia therapies
has been conceptualised as deficit-focused, functional/disability-focused or participant-
focused (World Health Organisation [WHO], 2001). This study represents an attempt to
combine elements from impairment-focused therapy (i.e., thinking for speaking and story
16 M. Carragher et al.
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
grammar) and a disability-focused therapy (i.e., conversation coaching targeting the CP)
in order to target the exchange of new information within storytelling. The inclusion of the
CP within therapy acknowledges the important roles played by both the PWA and the CP
in constructing shared understanding. Employing therapy techniques from various
approaches reflects clinical practice where therapists combine all approaches at their
disposal in supporting a PWA and their family through aphasia rehabilitation. Therapy
stimuli were sourced from YouTube and viewed using an iPad, thus utilising widely
available technology to create interesting, age-appropriate materials. Whilst further
research is required to expand this model of treatment delivery and outcome measurement
to a larger group of participants, the current study offers a novel approach whereby an
important aspect of everyday communicationconveying new informationis targeted
through the production patterns of the PWA and shaping facilitative behaviours of the CP.
Such intervention may have implications for establishing and maintaining relationships, a
sense of achievement for the PWA and CP, and, more broadly, quality of life.
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to the participants with aphasia and their families for their time, effort, and
enthusiasm in taking part in the study. This work would not have been possible without the support
of speech and language therapists who cascaded information about the study to potential
participants.
Funding
This work is supported by a Research Bursary from the Stroke Association (TSA JRTF 2009/01).
Note
1. Mr. Bean is a socially inept character who gets himself into embarrassing, comic scenarios, such
as becoming frightened in front of others on a high diving board in a swimming pool.
References
Armstrong, E. (2000). Aphasic discourse analysis: The story so far. Aphasiology,14, 875892.
doi:10.1080/02687030050127685
Beeke, S., Maxim, J., Best, W., & Cooper, F. (2011). Redesigning therapy for agrammatism: Initial
findings from the ongoing evaluation of a conversation-based intervention study. Journal of
Neurolinguistics,24, 222236. doi:10.1016/j.jneuroling.2010.03.002
Bierren, J., Kenyon, G., Ruth, J., Shroots, J., & Svendson, J. (1996). Aging and biography:
Explorations in adult development. New York, NY: Springer.
Bradley, M., & Douglas, J. (2008). Conversation partner training Its role in aphasia. Acquiring
Knowledge in Speech, Language and Hearing,10,1821.
Bronken, B., Kirkevold, M., Martinsen, R., & Kvigne, K. (2012). The aphasic storyteller: Co-
constructing stories to promote psychosocial well-being after stroke. Qualitative Health
Research,22, 13031316. doi:10.1177/1049732312450366
Carragher, M., Sage, K., & Conroy, P. (2013). The effects of verb retrieval therapy for people with
non-fluent aphasia: Evidence from assessment tasks and conversation. Neuropsychological
Rehabilitation,23, 846887. doi:10.1080/09602011.2013.832335
Carragher, M., Sage, K., & Conroy, P. (in press). Outcomes of treatment targeting syntax production
in people with Brocas-type aphasia: Evidence from psycholinguistic assessment tasks and
everyday conversation. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders.
Davidson, B., Worrall, L., & Hickson, L. (2003). Identifying the communication activities of older
people with aphasia: Evidence from naturalistic observation. Aphasiology,17, 243264.
doi:10.1080/729255457
Druks, J., & Masterson, J. (2000). The Object Action Naming Battery. Hove: Psychology Press.
Aphasiology 17
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
Fergadiotis, G., & Harris Wright, H. (2011). Lexical diversity for adults with and without aphasia
across discourse elicitation tasks. Aphasiology,25(11), 14141430. doi:10.1080/
02687038.2011.603898
Goodglass, H., Kaplan, E., & Barresi, B. (2001). The Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination
(BDAE). Baltimore, MD: Lippincott.
Goodwin, C. (1995). Co-constructing meaning in conversations with an aphasic man. Research on
Language and Social Interactions,28, 233260. doi:10.1207/s15327973rlsi2803_4
Hengst, J., Frame, S., Neuman-Stritzel, T., & Gannaway, R. (2005). Using otherswords:
Conversational use of reported speech by individuals with aphasia and their communication
partners. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research,48, 137156. doi:10.1044/1092-
4388(2005/011)
Hernandez-Sacristan, C., & Rosell-Clari, V. (2009). Syntax and conversation in aphasia. A strategic
restrictive use of Spanish and Catalan connector QUE by aphasic speakers. Clinical Linguistics
& Phonetics,23, 717741. doi:10.3109/02699200903063053
Hopper, T., Holland, A., & Rewega, M. (2002). Conversational coaching: Treatment outcomes and
future directions. Aphasiology,16, 745761. doi:10.1080/02687030244000059
Kellas, J., & Trees, A. (2006). Finding meaning in difficult family experiences: Sense-making and
interaction processes during joint family storytelling. Journal of Family Communication,6,
4976. doi:10.1207/s15327698jfc0601_4
Labov, W. (1972). The transformation of experience in narrative syntax. In W. Labov (Ed.),
Language and the inner city. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania.
Marshall, J. (2009). Framing ideas in aphasia: The need for thinking therapy. International Journal
of Language & Communication Disorders,44,114. doi:10.1080/13682820802683507
Marshall, J., & Cairns, D. (2005). Therapy for sentence processing problems in aphasia: Working on
thinking for speaking. Aphasiology,19, 10091020. doi:10.1080/02687030544000218
McAdams, D. (2001). The psychology of life stories. Review of General Psychology,5, 100122.
doi:10.1037/1089-2680.5.2.100
McNeil, M., Robin, D., & Schmidt, R. (2008). Apraxia of speech: Definition and differential
diagnosis. In M. McNeil (Ed.), Clinical management of sensorimotor speech disorders (2nd
ed.). New York, NY: Thieme Medical.
Morris, J., Franklin, S., Menger, F., & GD. (2011). Returning to work with aphasia: A case study.
Aphasiology,25,890907. doi:10.1080/02687038.2010.549568
Norrick, N. R. (2000). Conversational narrative: Storytelling in everyday talk. Amsterdam: John
Benjamins.
Ramsberger, G., & Menn, M. (2003). Co-constructing Lucy: Adding a social perspective to the
assessment of communicative success in aphasia. In G. Goodwin (Ed.), Conversation and brain
damage. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Ramsberger, G., & Rende, B. (2002). Measuring transactional success in the conversation of people
with aphasia. Aphasiology,16, 337353. doi:10.1080/02687040143000636
Randall, W. (2001). Acquiring a narrative perspective on aging, identity and everyday life. In G.
Kenyon, P. Clark, & B. De Vries (Eds.), Narrative gerontology: Theory, research and practice.
New York, NY: Springer.
Riessman, C. (1993). Narrative analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Rumelhart, D. (1975). Notes on a schema for stories. In D. Bobrow & A. Collins (Eds.),
Representation and Understanding: Studies in Cognitive Science. New York, NY: Academic
Press.
Van De Sandt-Koenderman, M., Van Der Meulen, I., & Ribbers, G. (2012). Aphasia rehabilitation:
More than treating the language disorder. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation,93,
S1S3. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2011.08.037
Weinrich, M., McCall, D., Boser, K. I., & Virata, T. (2002). Narrative and procedural discourse
production by severely aphasic patients. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair,16, 249274.
doi:10.1177/154596802401105199
Whitworth, A. (2010). Using narrative as a bridge: Linking language processing models with real-
life communication. Seminars in Speech and Language,31,6475. doi:10.1055/s-0029-
1244954
World Health Organisation (WHO). (2001). International classification of functioning, disability
and health (ICF). Geneva: Author.
18 M. Carragher et al.
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
Appendix 1. Example of video stimuli used during therapy
Geris game (Pixar short film) is a 3:50 minute video clip, http://www.youtube.com/watch?v
=9IYRC7g2ICg
Summary: Its autumn and an elderly man is in the park alone setting up a game of chess. He
proceeds to play with his own alias as an opponent. As he moves to each side of the chessboard, he
plays as a different character”—on one side of the board he wears his glasses and is a timid
character; on the other side of the board he takes off his glasses and is a competitive and somewhat
aggressive character. As the game progresses, the competitive character (without the glasses) is
winning. The timid character (with glasses) pretends to have a heart attack and, while his oppo-
nentis distracted, switches the chessboard so that he is winning. Once the game resumes, the
competitive character realises he is no longer winning the game and he resigns. As the prize, he
hands over a set of false teeth. As the camera pans over from the park, the man is seen sitting alone
at the chessboard.
Appendix 2.Individual goals for PWA and their conversation partners
Initials PWA CP Goals for therapy
AT Chunk it up: think about the story in smaller, more
manageable chunks
Set the scene: detail the initial contextual information about
the story or give a general impression of the tone of the
story
Drip drip: tell the story bit by bit, leaving time for partner to
ask questions
AT-CP Stop and check: check your understanding as you go along
by asking questions and summarising what youve
understood
Move along: during an unproductive lexical search for
PWA, keep the conversation moving by briefly
summarising the story so far and prompting PWA to tell
you the next part
Who does what: establish how many people are involved in
the story and their role within the story
Pinpoint: be specific about what you understand and what
you dont understand
PM Set the scene: detail the initial contextual information about
the story or give a general impression of the tone of the
story
Drip drip: tell the story bit by bit, leaving time for partner to
ask questions
Show and tell: use gesture or acting in combination with
speech to convey parts of the story
PM-CP Move on: if you know the word the PWA is trying to say,
keep the conversation going. If you dont know the word,
ask questions such as Do you mean?
Who does what: establish how many people are involved in
the story and their role within the story
Pinpoint: be specific about what you understand and what
you dont understand
(continued )
Aphasiology 19
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
Appendix 2. (Continued).
Initials PWA CP Goals for therapy
BL Set the scene: detail the initial contextual information about
the story or give a general impression of the tone of the
story
Chunk it up: think about the story in smaller, more
manageable chunks
Drip drip: tell the story bit by bit, leaving time for partner to
ask questions
Show and tell: use gesture or acting in combination with
speech to convey parts of the story
BL-CP Stop and check: check your understanding as you go along
by asking questions and summarising what youve
understood
Who does what: establish how many people are involved in
the story and their role within the story
Pinpoint: be specific about what you understand and what
you dont understand
JH Stop and listen: use conversation partners questions to
clarify details of the story with yes/no responses
Set the scene: detail the initial contextual information about
the story or give a general impression of the tone of the
story
Chunk it up: think about the story in smaller, more
manageable chunks
Drip drip: tell the story bit by bit, leaving time for partner to
ask questions
JH-CP Go for the jugular: establish the basic details/events first
and then enquire specifically about background
information or more fine-grained detail
20 M. Carragher et al.
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
Appendix 3.CPsretelling of stories, scored in comparison to the most frequently occurring content words produced by control
participants
Pretherapy simple narrative
target ATsCP PMsCP BLsCP JHsCP
Mr. Bean drives into a car
park in a mini
Mr. Bean (1)
drives (1)
car park/parking lot (1)
mini (1)
Well Mr. Bean (1)with
Rowan Atkinson in it was
a TV programme and I
assuming that this this
part of a TV programme
which shows Atkinson
as Mr. Bean driving (1)
into a car park (1) with his
usual incompetence
He saw a car (1) going into a
car park (1) the bloke
who turned out to be
Rowan Atkinson (1)
Rowan Atkinson (1) driving
(1) a car (1)
Right I think Ron Atkinson
was driving (1) a mini (1)
into a car park (1)
He parks too far from the
ticket machine and cant
reach his ticket
parks/pulls up (1)
too far/not close enough (1)
ticket machine (1)
cant reach (1)
ticket (1)
not being able to reach (1) the
ticket machine (1)
couldnt reach (1) the ticket
(1) the ticket machine (1)
I dont know was he going
somewhere to pay for
something and using his
plastic card
but it didnt show him going
into the car park it showed
him reaching (1) for a
ticket (1) to press the
button and he couldnt
press it
He uses a grabber to get the
ticket
grabber/stick/litter picker (1)
get/grab/pull out (1)
ticket (1)
so he has some sort of
extended arm (1) and
then gets (1) the ticket (1)
out of the machine
so he got something I
presume out of the car
like a grabber thing a pick-
me-up thing (1) to pull out
(1) the ticket (1)
and he used his stick (1) to
either push the plastic card
in or type out
something or other I dont
know I dont really know
so then he went inside the car
and he found a litter-
picking stick (1), reached
out of the car to press the
ticket, got (1) his ticket (1)
He drives recklessly into the
car park
drives (1)
recklessly/quickly (1)
car park (1)
and drives off (1) I dont
know whether hes coming
in or going out
and then he went to park (1)
the car somewhere and
its was a black and
yellow and black car
and then drove (1) into the
car park (1) and then it
ended and the car was
either yellow or cream
Total: 15 9 10 4 10
(continued )
Aphasiology 21
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
Appendix 3. (Continued).
Posttherapy simple narrative
target ATsCP PMsCP BLsCP JHsCP
Mr. Bean is in the pool,
looking around
Mr. Bean/he (1)
arrives (1)
pool (1)
looking/had a look around
(1)
Mr. Bean (1) eh Mr. Bean the
scene appears to be Mr. Bean
walks into a swimming pool (1)
Right so back to the swimming
pool (1) Mr. Bean (1) again
so I assume from that it was
going to be funny hes
standing on the side of the pool
wearing his trunks
Right what I got Rowan
Atkinson (1) and he said slide
at first well he said there were
two slides at first didnthebut
I- I never carried on with two
slides and then I found out it
were in swimming baths and the
slides were in the swimming
baths (1)
Right Mr. Bean (1) again hes
still in the swimming pool (1) but
hes sort of on the side
looking round (1) at the
excitement
He spots an elephant slide in
the childrens pool and
decides to go on it
spots/sees (1)
elephant (1)
slide (1)
go on it/have a go/drawn to
(1)
and sees (1) a couple of kids with
toy elephants (1) or real
elephants I suppose toy elephants
playing about and he wants to
get involved (1)
and there were two lads coming
from down the slide (1) I
presume young lads and he
thought hed go up (1) Mr. Bean
thought hed go up,
(see reference to slidesabove) (1) and he spots (1) that theres two
elephant (1) slides (1) with trunk-
therestrunks with slides and he
sees them and he thinks Ill go
on there (1)
As hes about to slide into the
water, the lifeguard blows
the whistle
sat on/got on/go on/have a
play/slide down (1)
lifeguard (1)
blows whistle/whistles/
stop/get off
(1)
so in his clumsy way he clambers
(1) onto the slide and then .
where theyre all possibly
jumbled together hes about to
join into this and the attendant (1)
comes along and says oi were
not having this (1) youre
its dangerous or whatever it is
started going up (1) but he got
stopped (1) by the instructor (1)
who said he couldntand for
children only
Anyway it sounded like he were
coming down (1) and he
shouldnt have been he were
doing something wrong and
lifeguard (1) or something like
life guard whatever at
swimming baths were
complaining to him telling
him he hadnt to do it (1) no no
no
so he goes over there and hes like
going up (1) the steps and then
he gets to the top sort of thing
and the life guard (1) whistles (1)
and says youre not allowed on
there (1) its only for young
children or whatever
Mr. Bean climbs back up the
slide
climbs back/gets off/pulls
himself up (1)
slide (1)
and thats it and he doesnt (1)
thatsi
t
and that was the end of it and then he got sent off (1) did he and then hes sort of a bit stunned
and sort of starts to get back
down but hes losing his footing
in sort of stumbling and then
but then he just gets down (1)
while the life guard watching and
doesnt go on the slide
Total: 13 9 7 7 12
(continued )
22 M. Carragher et al.
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
Appendix 3. (Continued).
Pretherapy complex narrative
target ATsCP PMsCP BLsCP JHsCP
Mr. Bean notices the diving
board and climbs to the
highest level
Mr. Bean (1)
notices/sees (1)
diving board (1)
climbs/heads (1)
top/highest level (1)
Another Mr. Bean (1) story
hes at the swimming pool
decided to show off finds
himself on the top deck (1) .
Rowan Atkinsons (1) gone to a
swimming pool hes runs up to
the either diving board (1) or the
view I didnt get that out of it
properly he went up some steps
I havent a clue something
about Rowan Atkinson (1)
on a diving board (1)
Right I think Ron Atkinsons in the
swimming baths and heshe goes
up some steps to get up to the high
diving board (1) which is theres two
diving boards and he goes on the
highest one (1)
He peers over the edge and
becomes afraid as he
realises the height and
holds on to the rail
peers over/looks down (1)
edge (1)
afraid/panicked/frightened
(1)
holds on (1)
rail (1)
too high (1) for him and he got to the I presume the edge
(1) of the diving board didnt
like it (1), flapped his arms then
he turned round as if he was going
to perhaps dive backwards
and then hes there trying to he looks
over the edge (1) and hes too scared
(1) so he yelps out and jumps back
and sort of holds onto (1) the
handle rail (1) hes quivering and
hes all scared
Two boys appear on the
diving board
two (1)
boys (1)
appear/come up (1)
and I imagine the lads (1) are
showing off and . Winding
him up and diving off the
board Im not quite sure if they
were spring board or at the top
and then two (1) lads (1) two children
appeared (1) not quite sure
where they came from
and he had mates thatsitI
dont know what he were
doing with his hand up there
like that ((mimics BLs raised
hand))
and then theres two (1) lads (1) who
come up (1) oh and hes wearing
trunks that have got blue and orange
and maybe some other colours on
but then he
The boys look impatient so
Mr. Bean pretends not to
be afraid and has to dive in
impatient/check watches (1)
pretends (1)
dive in/jump off (1)
he were he said he were
frightened (1) or nervous or
because hes so scared theyre sort of
sort of taking the mick out of him
a bit and saying you know tapping
their arms as if theyre waiting for
him(1) to get off and hes sort of
hes really scared so
Mr. Bean eases down onto his
front and hangs off the
board by his hands
eases down/lowers/climbs
down (1)
hangs off/holds (1)
onto/dangles/clings (1)
diving board (1)
hand (1)
and he bent down (1) to put his hands
down
eventually he ends up hanging (1)
off the edge of the diving board (1)
(continued )
Aphasiology 23
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
Appendix 3. (Continued).
Pretherapy complex narrative
target ATsCP PMsCP BLsCP JHsCP
One of the boys stamps on
Mr. Beans hand and he
falls into the pool
one (1)
boys/lads (1)
stamps (1)
hand/finger (1)
falls (1)
pool (1)
and one (1) of the children (1) stamped
(1) on his hands (1) but he still went
in the water (1) in a fashion
and he were on about his feet I
dont know what he was
talking about with his feet
cant think of owt else he
didnt tell me much did he
and then they stand on (1) one one of
his hands (1) on his fingers and then
he drops (1) into the water (1) and
sort of dives
Total: 27 4 13 3 16
Posttherapy complex
narrative target ATsCP PMsCP BLsCP JHsCP
Mr. Bean is in the pool and
realises his trunks have
come off and are floating
in the water
Mr. Bean (1)
realises/notices (1)
trunks (1)
come off/lost (1)
floating (1)
Mr. Beans (1) in the pool
swimming or trying to swim in
his usual probably incompetent
way til he discovers a pair of
trunks (1) floating (1) about
and he thought well Im
struggling now cos Iminthe
altogether (1)
Back at the pool again Mr.
Beans (1) in the water
without any trunks (1) on
theyve fallen off (1) not quite
sure why they would have but
he wouldnt have probably know
that
Well Rowan Atkinson (1) jumped in
pool, lost (1) his trunks (1)
It was Mr. Bean (1) or Rowan
Atkinson not Ron Atkinson
and he was it was like a
like a follow-on of the diving
into the pool one which we did
ages ago so hes in the pool
and his trunk- and he realises (1)
hes swimming about and he
realises that he hasnt got his
trunks (1) on and theyre on the
side
He swims over to get his
trunks but a little girl picks
them out of the water
swims (1)
get (1)
trunks (1)
little/little/young (1)
girl/child (1)
picks out/grabs (1)
before he could do anything a little
(1) girl (1) who he was probably
swimming next to picks up (1)
the trunks (1) so hes left then
without the
without his trunks (1), a little (1)
girl (1) who had got (1) them was
walking off with them
a child (1) picked (1) them (1) up
run off (1) with them
and then theres a couple with a
young (1) child (1) who pick (1)
up his trunks (1) and take them
away so hes obviously
panicking a bit
(continued )
24 M. Carragher et al.
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
Appendix 3. (Continued).
Posttherapy complex
narrative target ATsCP PMsCP BLsCP JHsCP
The lifeguard blows the
whistle to tell everyone to
get out of the pool so Mr.
Bean hides underwater
lifeguard (1)
blows whistle (1)
everybody out/get out (1)
pool (1)
hides/ducks down (1)
underwater (1)
by this time for some reason and
I should have asked because
thats my fault the pool cleared
and Mr. Bean was left in there
whether its closing time or
whatever Im not sure and the
only people there are two pool
attendants (1) so hesand
one of them is a female .so
Mr. Bean is struggling to keep
out of sight if you will hes
swimming about in the altogether
because he keeping under the
water (1) presumably to keep out
of sight (1) but having to surface
keeps surfacing
the instructor (1) and an assistant
were on the pool side the
pool- they got more people in
and they called time (1) it must
have been time to go or
whatever to get out so they
get called out Mr. Bean didnt
want to get out he was
embarrassed cos he hadnt got
his trunks on he looked very
sheepish
but he just stays in the water and
then but then its the end of the
day so the whistle gets blown
(1) to get out of the pool (1)
so he doesnt, he tries to
hide (1) under the water (1) about
three times and they keep
blowing the whistle (1) and
nobody knows hes hid
When everyone has left the
pool, Mr. Bean tries to
sneak out of the pool
tries (1)
sneak out (1)
pool (1)
and for some reason I should have
asked again the two pool
attendants left but one of them
so he tries (1) to get
out (1)
eventually everybody had got out of
the pool he got out (1)
and then everyone seems to have
gone away so he sort of
comes out (1) of the pool and
then tries (1) to make his way to
the changing rooms
(continued )
Aphasiology 25
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
Appendix 3. (Continued).
Posttherapy complex
narrative target ATsCP PMsCP BLsCP JHsCP
He hides from the female
lifeguard who has come
back into the pool
hides (1)
female (1)
lifeguard (1)
comes back (1)
pool (1)
and one of thems a woman (1)
so thats my fault I should
have asked for more detail
shouldntIthe I cantI
cannot rem- Mr. Bean then
gets out of the pool thinking
hes safe but by this time well
hell have got out of the pool
wont he but then the woman
thats right the woman would see
him and he he starts running
around trying to escape her
the instructor walked away but the
assistant (1) was still around
but hadnt noticed that he hadnt
got any trunks on
A group of girls come out of
the changing room, see Mr.
Bean and scream so he
runs off
group (1)
girls/schoolgirls (1)
come out/come in (1)
see (1)
scream (1)
runs off/runs away (1)
and by this time there are more kids
(1) have appeared (1) into the
pool area so he then he dives
back in again so hes back in the
same position he was in before
and that appears to be the
story [great, anything else?]
well I cant remember I think its
cos I didnt ask properly what
actually happened between Mr.
Bean, the girl picking Mr. Beans
trunks up and these two
attendants appearing and
disappearing whether Mr.
Bean was out of the pool by that
stage and when the kids arrive
its all a bit of a bit of a sort of
intermix if you will
Mr. Bean walked towards the
changing room as some girls-
young girls (1) came out (1)
and he got all embarrassed and
they laughed a bit and that
was the end of it
and then then Atkinson got out
naked and he were in front of
all people (1) that were watching
or on side of baths and they
were in costumes they were all
people waiting to go into
baths or been in baths and
thats it [asked to explain BLs
drawing] well that was Rowan
Atkinson but he scrubbed him
out and that was little boy on
side and they were trunks there
little boy got his trunks out of
baths run off and then he went up
here this is supposed to be
Rowan Atkinson and I presume
this is audience and they were all
in swimming gear so I
presume they were waiting to go
in or they had just got out or
something [and what happened at
the end?] I dont know
but then theres a big group (1) of
school girls (1) outside who see
(1) himnaked and start
screaming (1) and hes about a
bit ((gestures startled)) and then
thatsit
Total: 31 16 13 8 18
26 M. Carragher et al.
Downloaded by [La Trobe University] at 17:22 04 May 2015
... Of interest to the present study was the nding that of the 25 studies reviewed by Dipper et al. (2020), only six studies reported gains in discourse macrostructure aer treatment either explicitly targeting discourse macrostructure, that is, the overall story structure and information (Osiejuk, 1991;Carragher et al., 2015) or aer multilevel therapies focusing on any combination of two of the levels, that is, single word level, and/or sentence level, and/or discourse level (Penn and Beecham, 1992;Dietz et al., 2018) or focusing on all three levels (single word, sentence, discourse) (Whitworth, 2010;Whitworth et al., 2015). Of these studies, only four reported face-to-face delivery of discourse treatment by a speech-language therapist (SLT) (Penn and Beecham, 1992;Whitworth, 2010;Carragher et al., 2015;Whitworth et al., 2015) and three studies based the treatment activities on some aspects of personal narrative content either selected by the participant with aphasia (Osiejuk, 1991;Dietz et al., 2018) or prompted by the clinician (e.g., "what did you do this morning") (Penn and Beecham, 1992). ...
... Of interest to the present study was the nding that of the 25 studies reviewed by Dipper et al. (2020), only six studies reported gains in discourse macrostructure aer treatment either explicitly targeting discourse macrostructure, that is, the overall story structure and information (Osiejuk, 1991;Carragher et al., 2015) or aer multilevel therapies focusing on any combination of two of the levels, that is, single word level, and/or sentence level, and/or discourse level (Penn and Beecham, 1992;Dietz et al., 2018) or focusing on all three levels (single word, sentence, discourse) (Whitworth, 2010;Whitworth et al., 2015). Of these studies, only four reported face-to-face delivery of discourse treatment by a speech-language therapist (SLT) (Penn and Beecham, 1992;Whitworth, 2010;Carragher et al., 2015;Whitworth et al., 2015) and three studies based the treatment activities on some aspects of personal narrative content either selected by the participant with aphasia (Osiejuk, 1991;Dietz et al., 2018) or prompted by the clinician (e.g., "what did you do this morning") (Penn and Beecham, 1992). In most cases (Osiejuk, 1991;Whitworth, 2010;Carragher et al., 2015;Whitworth et al., 2015), the goal of discourse treatment was to improve story grammar elements (i.e., setting, initiating event, direct consequence, etc.). ...
... Of these studies, only four reported face-to-face delivery of discourse treatment by a speech-language therapist (SLT) (Penn and Beecham, 1992;Whitworth, 2010;Carragher et al., 2015;Whitworth et al., 2015) and three studies based the treatment activities on some aspects of personal narrative content either selected by the participant with aphasia (Osiejuk, 1991;Dietz et al., 2018) or prompted by the clinician (e.g., "what did you do this morning") (Penn and Beecham, 1992). In most cases (Osiejuk, 1991;Whitworth, 2010;Carragher et al., 2015;Whitworth et al., 2015), the goal of discourse treatment was to improve story grammar elements (i.e., setting, initiating event, direct consequence, etc.). Moreover, discourse treatment in the above-mentioned studies was primarily delivered individually (Osiejuk, 1991;Penn and Beecham, 1992;Whitworth, 2010;Whitworth et al., 2015;Dietz et al., 2018) or in dyads with a communication partner (Carragher et al., 2015) and no study had an outcome measure to formerly assess post-treatment improvement in functional communication in everyday life. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: People with aphasia (PWA) face challenges in sharing personal stories due to communication difficulties. Discourse treatment in aphasia focusing on personal narrative macrostructure has yet to receive the attention this warrants of researchers and clinicians. Emerging person-centered approaches involve coproduction and codesign with PWA for meaningful discourse treatments. Few studies explore discourse treatment's impact on functional communication. This pilot study aims to explore whether the use of the coproduction approach in the development of a multilevel personal narrative intervention at the group level increased the production of macrostructure elements in trained and untrained narrative discourse contexts, improved aphasia severity and functional communication skills, and advanced quality of life of the participants with aphasia. Methods: An ABA design was followed featuring a pre-treatment baseline assessment phase, a treatment phase, and a post-treatment assessment phase immediately after treatment was completed. Three people with chronic stroke-induced aphasia, three communication partners, and a moderator took part in the study. All participants were members of a university-led community aphasia communication group. The research protocol consisted of eleven, two-hour, weekly sessions over an 11-week block. Nine treatment sessions were carried out following codesign and coproduction methods that focused on participants with aphasia producing words, sentences, and total communication strategies to express macrostructure elements in their personal stories. Assessment measures were collected at baseline and post-treatment to evaluate improvements in trained and untrained narrative abilities, aphasia severity, functional communication, and the impact of aphasia on quality of life. Results: Multilevel personal narrative therapy improved the narrative skills of the participants with aphasia at the macrostructural level of narrative discourse. Improvements were also observed in functional communication and quality of life post-treatment. Discussion: The involvement of participants with aphasia in the codesign and coproduction of the treatment content for the group intervention facilitated improvement in narrative skills, functional communication, and overall quality of life with aphasia. It is recommended that researchers and clinicians consider using content from the personal narratives of clients with aphasia to build discourse treatment and adopt codesign and coproduction approaches, when designing interventions for people with chronic aphasia, to improve communication outcomes in everyday life.
... Broadly, discourse interventions may be viewed as falling within two categories, i.e., those that actively target and seek to improve discourse structure or organization, and even the rules involved in discourse, and those that use discourse as a context for practicing microstructure targets with the intention of increasing generalization. Examples of the former include Interactive Storytelling Therapy [20], NARNIA (a Novel Approach to Real-life Communication: Narrative Intervention in Aphasia [21], and LUNA (Language Underpins Narrative in Aphasia [22] and are explored further below. In contrast, the latter category uses discourse as a context to practice and embed microlinguistic skills such as word finding, without explicit therapeutic focus on discourse structure or organization. ...
... To explore this idea further, the following sections will focus specifically on some of the studies in the evidence base that illustrate intentional targeting. Beginning with those aimed primarily at discourse macrostructure, two studies are of interest [15,20] and involve five participants in total. Both studies reported gains in word produc-tion and in discourse macrostructure. ...
... In particular, complex treatments require a theoretical rationale linked to components of intervention in order to explain the expected mechanisms of change [38]. Only three of the 25 studies in the review by Dipper and colleagues [4] explicitly mentioned a theoretical framework for discourse macrostructure [20,21,40], and these authors drew on varied sources ( [42] for cohesion; [14,43,44] for story grammar). ...
... • Referential communication tasks (n = 4: Devanga et al., 2021;Hengst et al., 2008Hengst et al., , 2010Hoover et al., 2017). • Retell and recount therapies (n = 5: Carragher et al., 2015;Cartwright & Elliott, 2009;Mason et al., 2020;Nickels et al., 2016;Nykänen et al., 2013). • Comprehensive communication training programmes, in which co-constructed communication therapy tasks are embedded within the programme (n = 4: Parola et al., 2019;Sim et al., 2013;Togher et al., 2013Togher et al., , 2016. ...
... In general, participants with ABI were given the role of having to recount/retell the stimuli content. The CPs were most commonly other participants with ABI (Cartwright & Elliott, 2009;Mason et al., 2020;Nickels et al., 2016), with two studies using spouses (Carragher et al., 2015;Nykänen et al., 2013). The speech pathologist acted as the mediator, intervening on a needs-basis to help repair communication breakdowns, suggest scaffolding techniques, provide multimodal cueing (Cartwright & Elliott, 2009;Mason et al., 2020;Nickels et al., 2016), and guide participants to use compensatory communication strategies (Carragher et al., 2015;Nykänen et al., 2013). ...
... The CPs were most commonly other participants with ABI (Cartwright & Elliott, 2009;Mason et al., 2020;Nickels et al., 2016), with two studies using spouses (Carragher et al., 2015;Nykänen et al., 2013). The speech pathologist acted as the mediator, intervening on a needs-basis to help repair communication breakdowns, suggest scaffolding techniques, provide multimodal cueing (Cartwright & Elliott, 2009;Mason et al., 2020;Nickels et al., 2016), and guide participants to use compensatory communication strategies (Carragher et al., 2015;Nykänen et al., 2013). Various stimuli were used including YouTube videos (Carragher et al., 2015), television show episodes (Cartwright & Elliott, 2009;Mason et al., 2020;Nickels et al., 2016), pictures, objects, situations and local news stories (Nykänen et al., 2013). ...
Article
Background: Meaningful, varied, joyful conversation is an important therapy target for adults with language or cognitive-communication disorders following acquired brain injury (ABI). However, the complexity of daily communication is often reduced to component parts within intervention programmes, with mixed evidence of generalization to everyday conversation. Interventions targeting co-construction of communication within a dyad offer a structured way in which to retain and treat elements of everyday conversation for individuals and their communication partner (CP). Such interventions exist but they are variably labelled, target different ABI populations and have not been synthesized. Aims: To identify the nature, scope and effects of intervention studies targeting co-constructed communication in adults with ABI. Method: This systematic review was completed using PRISMA Guidelines. Six databases (MEDLINE, Embase, CINAHL, Scopus, LLBA, PsychInfo) were searched and 1210 studies were screened. Data were extracted and studies were rated for methodological quality and completeness of reporting. Outcome measures and effects of treatment were collated through descriptive synthesis. Main contribution: This review highlights an emerging evidence base in relation to an intervention approach that targets everyday communication. Co-constructed communication interventions have been reported by 13 studies, from a total of 206 participants with post-stroke aphasia, traumatic brain injury and progressive language impairments. These interventions take a range of formats, including referential communication tasks, retell/recount therapies and communication training programmes. Methodological quality evaluation indicated mostly low-level study designs. Heterogeneity was identified in primary outcome measures, with 28 unique primary outcome measures reported across studies. Most studies demonstrated change in task-specific or broad communication outcome measures. Conclusions: Co-constructed communication interventions may offer clinicians a systematic, protocolized, replicable way to target everyday communication for adults with ABI. More high-quality, experimental designs with complete reporting and psychometrically sound outcome measures are needed to strengthen the evidence base. What this paper adds: What is already known on this subject Everyday conversation is an important therapy target for adults with ABI, but there is mixed evidence of therapy gains generalizing to everyday life. Many interventions reduce conversation to component parts such as naming or sentence construction. A different approach is needed to capture the social, dyadic, interactive and multifaceted nature of conversation. We propose the term 'co-constructed communication interventions' as a therapy genre targeting semi-structured dialogue. These interventions retain elements of everyday conversation (such as multimodal communication and situating tasks within dyads), combined with experimental elements (where stimuli prompt interactions and responses can be scored against normative data). What this paper adds to existing knowledge This review proposes and describes a distinct genre of discourse intervention within the current evidence base with a novel operational definition of 'co-constructed communication'. What are the potential or actual clinical implications of this work? Co-constructed communication interventions directly target elements of everyday communication by situating the therapy goals within a dyadic, interactive, multimodal task. A range of intervention tasks have been identified, including collaborative storytelling and problem-solving. This review will be of interest to clinicians working with adults with ABI; co-constructed communication interventions may offer a useful, replicable way to target aspects of everyday communication. This synthesis of the current evidence base encourages clinicians' informed, evidence-based decisions around these interventions.
... A related systematic review of 25 sentence production studies (Poirier et al., 2023) also concluded that sentencelevel treatments targeting verbs, sentence structure, and/or morphology often resulted in generalization with 72 of the 84 (86%) participants represented in their review showing generalization to untreated items and 43 of 59 (72%) demonstrating narrative discourse improvements, primarily evaluated through story retell. Across the Hickin et al. (2022) and Poirier et al. (2023) reviews, the treatment approaches that demonstrated the strongest evidence for generalization of treatment effects to untreated sentences were Verb Network Strengthening Treatment (Edmonds, 2014), TUF (Thompson & Shapiro, 2005), and mapping therapies (Carragher et al., 2015;Rochon et al., 2005). ...
... Treatment procedures varied widely across studies, but in all cases, participants were encouraged to expand their individual utterances and increase the length and content of their narrative responses. Albeit interpretation of the current studies was complicated by a lack of consistency in reporting, in general, Dipper et al. identified increases in word production, including the number of verb productions in the cases where that was isolated (Carragher et al., 2015;Falconer & Antonucci, 2012;Goral & Kempler, 2009;Hoover et al., 2015;Whitworth, 2010). Notably, most studies included in the review did not assess discourse beyond a picture description task so use of verbs during spontaneous conversation and other discourse genres (e.g., procedural discourse) remains unclear. ...
Article
Purpose Generalization has been defined and instantiated in a variety of ways over the last half-century, and this lack of consistency has created challenges for speech-language pathologists to plan for, implement, and measure generalization in aphasia treatment protocols. This tutorial provides an overview of generalization with a focus on how it relates to aphasia intervention, including a synthesis of existing principles of generalization and examples of how these can be embedded in approaches to aphasia treatment in clinical and research settings. Method Three articles collectively listing 20 principles of generalization formed the foundation for this tutorial. The seminal work of Stokes and Baer (1977) focused attention on generalization in behavioral change following treatment. Two aphasia-specific resources identified principles of generalization in relation to aphasia treatment (Coppens & Patterson, 2018; Thompson, 1989). A selective literature review was conducted to identify evidence-based examples of each of these 20 principles from the extant literature. Results Five principles of generalization were synthesized from the original list of 20. Each principle was supported by studies drawn from the aphasia treatment literature to exemplify its application. Conclusions Generalization is an essential aspect of meaningful aphasia intervention. Successful generalization requires the same dedication to strategic planning and outcome measurement as the direct training aspect of intervention. Although not all people with aphasia are likely to benefit equally from each of the principles reviewed herein, our synthesis provides information to consider for maximizing generalization of aphasia treatment outcomes. Supplemental Material https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.24714399
... to facilitate reliable analysis of various micro-and macrolinguistic discourse variables (vs. unstructured conversation; Carragher et al., 2015;Christensen et al., 2009). Participants previewed the wordless picture book. ...
Article
Purpose Although individuals with aphasia commonly exhibit challenges in executive functioning (EF) and spoken discourse, there is limited research exploring connections between these abilities within this specific population. Therefore, this study investigated the relationship between verbal and nonverbal EF and narrative productions in aphasia using a multilevel linguistic approach. Method Participants included 22 persons with aphasia (PWA) and 24 age- and education-matched, neurologically healthy controls (NHC). All participants completed assessments for EF and a story retelling task. Narrative samples were analyzed for microlinguistic (productivity, lexical and syntactic features, semantic content, word and sentence errors) and macrolinguistic (coherence, informational content, organization, and language use) characteristics. Correlational analyses were employed to explore the relationships among narrative variables. EF factors, extracted from principal component analysis, were used as predictive variables in hierarchical stepwise regression analyses to evaluate their role in predicting narrative performances of PWA and NHC. Results Relative to NHC, PWA exhibited impaired narrative performance affecting both microlinguistic and macrolinguistic levels. Breakdowns at the structural level (i.e., reduced productivity, syntax, lexical retrieval, and diversity) correlated with impaired story completeness, organization, and connectedness; this relationship was more prominent for PWA. Three EF factors representing (1) verbal EF, (2) verbal and nonverbal fluency, and (3) nonverbal EF were extracted. Factors 1 and 2 largely predicted narrative performance, whereas Factor 3 (i.e., nonverbal EF) contributed prominently to predicting macrolinguistic discourse performance in both groups although accounting for less variance in the data. Overall, lower EF scores, particularly verbal EF variables, predicted poor narrative performance in both groups. Conclusions Our results indicate that both linguistic and extralinguistic cognitive abilities play a role in story retelling performances among PWA. Notably, both verbal and nonverbal EF skills were found to be correlated with narrative abilities. However, the extent of their contributions varied depending on the discourse levels assessed. These findings provide a significant contribution to our understanding of the cognitive factors associated with breakdowns in discourse among PWA and highlight the importance of comprehensive assessment of EF and discourse within this population. Supplemental Material https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.26485627
Article
Full-text available
Background Aphasia impacts communication and relationships. While counselling is increasingly recognised as a component of the speech‐language therapy role, the success of conversation partner training is typically measured in terms of communication alone. This scoping review aimed to describe how successful conversation is currently measured with couples—with and without aphasia, to inform the development of an ecologically valid measure for couples with aphasia. Methods and Procedures The scoping review was reported in alignment with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta‐Analysis Extensions for Scoping Reviews (PRISMA‐SCR). MEDLINE, EMBASE, CINAHL and PsycINFO databases were searched for conversation treatment studies for couples with and without aphasia. Patient‐reported outcome measures (PROMs) were extracted from included studies and screened against the three‐tier model of situated language to shortlist those that measure everyday communication. Items within shortlisted PROMs were further described using the refined International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health linking rules. Results Following screening and full‐text review, 46 studies were included, consisting of 24 studies conducted with couples with aphasia and 22 studies conducted with couples without aphasia. For couples with aphasia, 13 PROMs were identified that measured everyday communication. Of these, 23% were dyadic (i.e., measured from the perspectives of both members of the couple); however, they usually only appraised the communication behaviours of the person with aphasia. For couples without aphasia, eight PROMs were identified that measured everyday communication; all were dyadic and measured both attitudes and communication behaviours of both partners. Conclusion Conversation relies on the interaction of two people, and success in conversation is best rated by those having the conversation. The use of PROMs is recommended as part of person and relationship‐centred practice; however, there is currently no validated PROM for conversation in aphasia that considers the perspectives and behaviours of both the person with aphasia and their communication partner. The PROM items identified in this study will form the basis of future research to develop a PROM for couples' conversations in aphasia. WHAT THIS PAPER ADDS What is already known on the subject Communication partner training (CPT) for couples, where one person has aphasia, can improve conversation skills and enhance relationships. Clinician‐rated measures, such as conversation analysis, have been used to measure the outcomes of CPT. However, there is a lack of person‐centred or self‐rated outcome measurement instruments. that is, patient‐reported outcome measures (PROMs) for couples' conversations in aphasia. What this paper adds to existing knowledge We have identified outcome measurement instruments used in conversation treatment studies for couples with and without aphasia. We found that most PROMs used in aphasia treatment studies were not dyadic, that is, they did not include the self‐report of both communication partners. In contrast, the PROMs used with couples without aphasia were dyadic and contained items that measured a more complex range of both partners' emotions, behaviours and attitudes. What are the potential or actual clinical implications of this work? This study provides insights into the content and characteristics of PROMs for couples' conversation therapy and may assist clinicians in selecting outcome measurement instruments in their practice.
Article
Background The complexity of communication presents challenges for clinical assessment, outcome measurement and intervention for people with acquired brain injury. For the purposes of assessment or treatment, this complexity is usually managed by isolating specific linguistic functions or speech acts from the interactional context. Separating linguistic functions from their interactional context can lead to discourse being viewed as a static entity comprised of discrete features, rather than as a dynamic process of co‐constructing meaning. The ecological validity of discourse assessments which rely on the deconstruction of linguistic functions is unclear. Previous studies have reported assessment tasks that preserve some of the dialogic features of communication, but as yet, these tasks have not been identified as a distinct genre of assessment. We suggest the term ‘co‐constructed communication’ to describe tasks which are specifically designed to capture the dynamic, jointly produced nature of communication within a replicable assessment task. Aims To identify and summarize how co‐constructed communication has been assessed with individuals with non‐progressive acquired communication disability regarding task design, measures and psychometric robustness. Methods A scoping review methodology was used to identity relevant studies. Systematic database searches were conducted on studies published before July 2021. Studies in the yield were assessed against eligibility criteria, with 37 studies identified as eligible for inclusion. Main contribution This is the first time that co‐constructed communication has been defined as a genre of discourse assessment for stroke and traumatic brain injury populations. Co‐constructed communication has been assessed for 144 individuals with aphasia and 111 with cognitive–communication disability. Five categories of co‐constructed communication tasks were identified, ranging in complexity. Variability exists in how these assessment tasks are labelled and measured. Assessment measures require further psychometric profiling, specifically regarding test–retest reliability and validity. Conclusions Co‐constructed communication is a discourse genre which offers researchers and clinicians a replicable method to assess language and communication in an experimentally rigorous way, within an ecologically valid context, bridging the gap between experimental and ecological assessment approaches. What this paper adds What is already known on this subject Standardized assessments of language skills and monologue offer reliable, replicable ways to measure language. However, isolating language from an interactional context fundamentally changes the behaviour under study. This raises questions about the ecological validity of the measures we routinely use to determine diagnoses, guide treatment planning and measure the success of treatment. What this study adds to the existing knowledge This review highlights studies that conceptualize, and often quantify, interaction by combining experimental rigour and aspects of everyday dialogue. This is the first time this genre of discourse assessment has been identified. We propose the term ‘co‐constructed communication’ to describe this genre and provide an operational definition for the term. What are the practical and clinical implications of this study? Co‐constructed communication assessment tasks require refinement, particularly regarding aspects of psychometric robustness. In the future, these tasks offer pragmatic, meaningful ways to capture the effect and impact of aphasia and cognitive–communication disability within interaction.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose Conversation-focused speech-language pathology services are a top priority for people living with aphasia, but little is known about how researchers measure conversation as an outcome of treatment. This scoping review was undertaken to systematically review the evidence regarding the measurement of conversation in aphasia studies and to identify current practices and existing gaps. Method A systematic literature search was conducted for studies published between January 1995 and September 2019 in multiple electronic databases. Covidence software was used to manage search results, study selection, and data charting processes. Data were extracted from each study and then collated and organized to elucidate the breadth of approaches, tools, or procedures oriented to measuring conversation as an outcome and identify gaps in the existing literature. Results The systematic search of the literature resulted in 1,244 studies. A total of 64 studies met inclusion criteria and were included in the review. The review summarizes the various tools and procedures used to measure conversation as an outcome of aphasia intervention, including variations in data collection and analysis procedures. The review also evaluates the quality of conversation measures in terms of psychometric properties and informal measures of validity. There was a total of 211 measures used across the 64 studies. Conclusions While there was no clear measure that was objectively superior, several measures show promise and warrant future exploration. Some of the orientations, conceptualizations, and procedures we have presented can be seen as options that might be included in a future conversation-focused core outcome set. Supplemental Material https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.21514062
Article
Full-text available
Background: Capturing evidence of the effects of therapy within everyday communication is the holy grail of aphasia treatment design and evaluation. Whilst impaired sentence production is a predominant symptom of Broca's-type aphasia, the effects of sentence production therapy on everyday conversation have not been investigated. Given the context-sensitive nature of spoken production by people with aphasia, it is difficult to extrapolate implications for everyday conversation based on results from task-based assessment (such as picture description, story retell or interview). Thus, there are strong theoretical and clinical motivations to investigate generalization from sentence production treatment to everyday conversation. Aims: To evaluate a theoretically driven treatment focused on the language production skills of participants with post-stroke Broca's aphasia and to track outcomes from psycholinguistic assessment tasks to everyday conversation. Methods & procedures: A case series design was utilized with pragmatic selection of participants with chronic aphasia undergoing the same assessment and treatment procedures. Nine participants with Broca's aphasia and their main conversation partners took part in the study. Treatment was implemented once weekly over 8 weeks and targeted production of basic syntax-two, three and four constituent constructions-through principles of mapping and reduced syntax treatment. Use of different possible exemplars for nouns, particularly pronouns, was trained together with use of both light and heavy verbs. Participants had the opportunity to 'top-up' therapy practise by completely a homework task that mirrored the therapy task. Outcomes & results: Syntactic well-formedness was assessed in samples of constrained sentence production, narrative retell and naturally occurring conversations at baseline, 1 week post-treatment, and 1 month post-treatment. Treatment showed strong direct effects in trained and untrained sentence construction tasks, with some generalization to narrative retell tasks. There was little evidence of change in everyday conversation. Conclusions & implications: Improvement in language production in constrained assessment tasks may not impact on everyday conversations. Implications for further research are discussed, e.g. the need for bridging interventions between constrained and unconstrained contexts of language production. Clinical implications include the potential to streamline therapy planning and delivery by making use of rich, hybrid therapies to treat individuals with similar symptom profiles but with a range of underlying deficits.
Article
Full-text available
Despite often impressive improvements on linguistic assessments, there is a lack of evidence of significant generalisation from impairment-focused aphasia therapy to everyday communication. The aim of the current study was to investigate the effect of a verb retrieval therapy across a range of levels of language production. Nine participants with chronic non-fluent stroke aphasia were recruited into this case series. Baseline assessment included naming a range of verbs (i.e., action verbs, semantically light verbs and personally relevant verbs) and sentence production. Multiple samples of conversation were collected from each participant and his/her partner. Consecutively failed verbs were divided across treatment and control sets; these sets were matched for salient psycholinguistic variables such as frequency, imageability and argument structure. A multi-component verb retrieval therapy was delivered, consisting of semantic feature analysis, gesture production and phonemic cueing. Following therapy, participants demonstrated significant and sustained gains in naming treated verbs; more modest effects were seen in untreated verbs. Mixed patterns of generalisation were evident in assessment of sentence production. In conversation, while group analysis suggested a lack of change, individual analyses indicated increased verb retrieval for three participants and qualitative changes related to the syntactic contexts of verbs retrieved.
Article
Reported speech, wherein one quotes or paraphrases the speech of another, has been studied extensively as a set of linguistic and discourse practices. Researchers agree that reported speech is pervasive, found across languages, and used in diverse contexts. However, to date, there have been no studies of the use of reported speech among individuals with aphasia. Grounded in an interactional sociolinguistic perspective, the study presented here documents and analyzes the use of reported speech by 7 adults with mild to moderately severe aphasia and their routine communication partners. Each of the 7 pairs was videotaped in 4 everyday activities at home or around the community, yielding over 27 hr of conversational interaction for analysis. A coding scheme was developed that identified 5 types of explicitly marked reported speech: direct, indirect, projected, indexed, and undecided. Analysis of the data documented reported speech as a common discourse practice used successfully by the individuals with aphasia and their communication partners. All participants produced reported speech at least once, and across all observations the target pairs produced 400 reported speech episodes (RSEs), 149 by individuals with aphasia and 251 by their communication partners. For all participants, direct and indirect forms were the most prevalent (70% of RSEs). Situated discourse analysis of specific episodes of reported speech used by 3 of the pairs provides detailed portraits of the diverse interactional, referential, social, and discourse functions of reported speech and explores ways that the pairs used reported speech to successfully frame talk despite their ongoing management of aphasia.
Article
Background : There is evidence that language production requires specialised conceptual processes, or “thinking for speaking” (Slobin, 1996). These processes generate the pre-verbal message, which according to Levelt (1989, 1999) has propositional structure and perspective and is specifically adapted for the target language. Aims/Main Contribution : This paper presents evidence that thinking for speaking may be impaired in aphasia. Even when not impaired, its complexities may prevent people with aphasia from revealing grammatical competencies. This may explain why therapy often fails to bring about improvements in open speaking conditions, such as narrative, which impose heavy message-level demands. Conclusions : It is argued that, with some individuals, therapy should target thinking for speaking skills. Two therapy studies are reviewed that support this conclusion.