ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Response to Intervention (RTI) or Multi-Tiered Systems of Support (MTSS) is beginning to be implemented in preschool programs to improve outcomes and to reduce the need for special education services. The proportions of children in programs identified as struggling learners through universal screening have important implications for the feasibility of these approaches as well as for the way programs might allocate resources and staff implementing tiered models of intervention. The expected proportions of children who might be identified for higher tiers of instructional support in pre-kindergarten settings are relatively unknown. The proportions of children who would have been identified for higher tiers of instructional language/literacy support when using three different universal screening measures are described. Participants were 659 children participating in the Center for Response to Intervention in Early Childhood (CRTIEC) Tier 1 Study. Results indicated that the proportions of children at Tier 2 and Tier 3 performance levels were higher for children in low-income eligibility programs and varied by program-level characteristics including numbers of English language learners and children with special needs, as well as the universal screening measure used. Implications of these findings suggest the importance of increased focus on early literacy and language in Tier 1 instruction in programs serving high proportions of children at risk as a means of preventing reading failure in future years.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Journal of Early Intervention
2015, Vol. 36(4) 281 –291
© 2015 SAGE Publications
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1053815115579937
jei.sagepub.com
Article
Identifying Preschool Children
for Higher Tiers of Language
and Early Literacy Instruction
Within a Response to
Intervention Framework
Judith J. Carta1, Charles R. Greenwood1, Jane Atwater1,
Scott R. McConnell2, Howard Goldstein3,
and Ruth A. Kaminski4
Abstract
Response to Intervention (RTI) or Multi-Tiered Systems of Support (MTSS) is beginning to be
implemented in preschool programs to improve outcomes and to reduce the need for special
education services. The proportions of children in programs identified as struggling learners
through universal screening have important implications for the feasibility of these approaches
as well as for the way programs might allocate resources and staff implementing tiered models
of intervention. The expected proportions of children who might be identified for higher tiers
of instructional support in pre-kindergarten settings are relatively unknown. The proportions
of children who would have been identified for higher tiers of instructional language/literacy
support when using three different universal screening measures are described. Participants
were 659 children participating in the Center for Response to Intervention in Early Childhood
(CRTIEC) Tier 1 Study. Results indicated that the proportions of children at Tier 2 and Tier 3
performance levels were higher for children in low-income eligibility programs and varied by
program-level characteristics including numbers of English language learners and children with
special needs, as well as the universal screening measure used. Implications of these findings
suggest the importance of increased focus on early literacy and language in Tier 1 instruction in
programs serving high proportions of children at risk as a means of preventing reading failure
in future years.
Keywords
Response to Intervention, Multi-Tiered Systems of Support, universal screening, early literacy
assessment
1University of Kansas, Kansas City, MO, USA
2University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, USA
3The Ohio State University, Columbus, USA
4Dynamic Measurement Group, Eugene, OR, USA
Corresponding Author:
Judith J. Carta, University of Kansas, 444 Minnesota Avenue, Suite 300, Kansas City, KS, 66101 USA.
Email: carta@ku.edu
579937JEIXXX10.1177/1053815115579937Journal of Early InterventionCarta et al.
research-article2015
by guest on September 23, 2015jei.sagepub.comDownloaded from
282 Journal of Early Intervention 36(4)
Introduction
Although Response to Intervention (RTI) has been implemented in elementary grade settings for
several years, early education programs have only recently applied this approach with children
younger than kindergarten who may be struggling to learn language and early literacy skills
(Greenwood et al., 2011; McConnell & Missall, 2008). Whether some of the assumptions supporting
RTI in K-12 settings will fit pre-kindergarten (Pre-K) programs is still an open question. Although
there are many conceptual reasons why RTI should be a good match for early education (e.g., the
value placed on early identification/prevention of learning problems; the importance of individual-
izing instruction in preschool settings), some critical challenges and unanswered questions remain.
One major challenge for RTI in early education is the dearth of evidence-based Tier 1 curricula in
general and the infrequent implementation of evidence-based curricula to promote early literacy
more specifically (Guo, Sawyer, Justice, & Kaderavek, 2013). In addition, the scarcity of measures
for identification and progress monitoring in early literacy and language is another challenge of pre-
school RTI (Greenwood et al., 2011). Early education programs that seek to implement universal
screening to identify children who would benefit from higher tiers of instructional support are lim-
ited in their choices of measures. Yet, many Pre-K programs implementing tiered early literacy pro-
grams are moving ahead with universal screening with limited information on the soundness of these
measures or the ramifications for their programs of their choices in these measures.
One question about RTI models in Pre-K programs related to universal screening in early lit-
eracy and language is what proportions of children would be identified as potential candidates for
higher tiers of instructional support, and whether programs have the capacity and the infrastruc-
ture for addressing those needs. The proportion of children in a classroom or a program who are
identified for higher tiers of support has important implications for how RTI models are con-
ceived and delivered. When greater numbers of children are identified for a Tier 2 and 3 early
literacy intervention, more resources in terms of staff, materials, time, and cost will be required
to appropriately serve them. This concern is magnified for programs that already focus on chil-
dren who are at risk for meeting readiness goals for kindergarten (e.g., Title 1 preschool pro-
grams, many state-funded Pre-K programs, and Head Start). The available evidence indicates
that many children entering these programs are significantly below the mean on measures of
vocabulary (e.g., Zill & Resnick, 2006). Therefore, we might expect that the proportion of chil-
dren needing more than universal Tier 1 early literacy and language services would be much
higher in these early education programs than the 20% rule of thumb, often discussed for the
elementary grades (National Association of State Special Education Directors, 2008).
Therefore, in the present study, we sought to contribute to the literature by addressing ques-
tions about the proportions of children who qualify at levels of language and early literacy risk
greater than Tier 1 in preschool programs in a secondary analysis of a data from a larger investi-
gation (Greenwood et al., 2012). We sought to investigate how proportions of at-risk children
might vary across early childhood program types and when using various screening measures
used for identification. We expected that preschool RTI tier-level proportions in programs would
be influenced by child factors such as poverty, the language spoken at home, and special needs
status (e.g., Shanahan & Lonigan, 2008). We also expected proportions would be influenced by
the measures selected for use and the constructs, content, and methods that they reflected (e.g.,
Fletcher, Stuebing, Miciak, & Denton, 2012). This information about proportions of children
identified for greater instructional intensity is fundamental for programs seeking to address criti-
cal policy questions about feasibility and the resources, staff, time, and cost needed to implement
RTI. These data about relative proportions might also inform the sequence of steps a program
might take in beginning implementation of an RTI program designed to strengthen the overall
effectiveness of preschool instruction.
The overall goal of the larger Center for Response to Intervention in Early Childhood
(CRTIEC) Tier 1 study was to examine the quality of Tier 1 being implemented across a large
by guest on September 23, 2015jei.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Carta et al. 283
number of classrooms in early education programs that were typical in each of four geographic
sites (see Greenwood et al., 2012). In general, we sought to investigate the quality and quantity
of early literacy/language instruction that might serve as the foundation for a multi-tiered system
of support. In the current study, we report the range of children being served across these typical
programs and the proportions of them that might be identified for higher tiers of instruction if
they were universally screened with multiple measures at the beginning of the school year.
We addressed the following research questions from an RTI perspective:
Research Question 1: What were the variations in children’s language/early literacy risk
status overall and by specific programs?
Research Question 2: What proportions of children were identified for language/early liter-
acy risk by different measures overall and within specific programs?
Research Question 3: What proportions of children were identified for higher than Tier 1
instructional support by the three universal screening measures by program, English language
learner (ELL), and individualized education plan (IEP) status?
Research Question 4: What was the concordance of agreement across measures in identify-
ing individual children for higher tiers of language/literacy support?
Method
Sample
Participants were 659 children with informed consent enrolled in 65 Pre-K classes from pro-
grams in four states that were involved in the CRTIEC Multi-Site Study of Tier 1 instruction
during the 2009-2010 school year (see Greenwood et al., 2012, for details). Programs participat-
ing were typical in each of the four geographic sites involved in the CRTIEC project (Kansas
City, MO/KS; Columbus, OH; Eugene–Springfield, OR; and Minneapolis, MN).
Only early education programs that reported that language and literacy were an instructional
focus and that could identify a specific curriculum they used for language and literacy instruction
for the majority of preschool-aged children were recruited. We selected programs where children
attended for at least 12 hr per week, where the majority of early literacy instruction occurred in
English and where children communicated primarily in either English or Spanish. We included
programs that served children with IEPs but did not include programs in which the majority of
children had disabilities or developmental delays.
This enrollment resulted in 65 classrooms that represented four types of programs: 20 (30.7%)
were from State-Funded Pre-K programs; 20 (30.7%) were Title 1; 17 (26.1%) were from Head
Start; and 8 (12.3%) were Tuition-Based (see Table 1). The majority of classrooms were from
half-day (39, 60%) versus full-day (26, 40%) classrooms. Private-Tuition and Title 1 classrooms
were more likely to be full-day programs, whereas Head Start and Pre-K were more likely to be
half-day programs.
Children and parents. All children in the selected programs who were at least 4 years old and were
identified in their Pre-K year and all children in the selected classrooms and their parents meeting
these criteria were individually recruited. The mean age of the children was 4.6 years (SD = .32
years) at the first assessment. Eighty-one percent were 4-year-olds, 19% were 5-year-olds.
Children’s and parents’ socio-demographics varied by sites (see Greenwood et al., 2012). The
total sample was balanced by gender and included 36% African American, 31% White, 20%
Hispanic/Latino, 10% multi-race, and 3% Asian. Approximately 23% of the children were ELLs.
This varied considerably by program type with 38% of children in State-Funded Pre-K programs
being ELLs and 0% of children in the Tuition-Funded programs (see Table 2). The mean percent-
age of children eligible for early childhood special education (with IEPs) was 11%. This also
by guest on September 23, 2015jei.sagepub.comDownloaded from
284 Journal of Early Intervention 36(4)
Table 2. Percentage of ELLs and Children With IEPs Across Program Types.
Program type % of ELL % of non-ELLs % of children with IEPs % of children without IEPs
State-Funded Pre-K 38 62 13 87
Head Start 20 80 15 85
Title I 13 87 5 95
Tuition-Based 0 100 3 97
Total sample 23 87 11 89
Note. ELL = English language learners; IEP = Individualized Education Plan.
varied across program types with 15% of children in Head Start programs having IEPs and 3% of
children in Tuition-Based programs. With regard to parent/caregiver educational attainment in the
overall sample, 22% of parents reported having less than high school, 23% had high school diplo-
mas or general education development (GED), and 55% reported education beyond high school.
Teachers. All teachers in these classrooms were recruited for participation, and those enrolled
provided informed consent. These 65 teachers reported 9.9 mean years of teaching experience.
The majority of teachers reported having a 4-year degree (47.7%) in early childhood education;
7.4% had a 2-year degree. The proportion of teachers having a graduate degree was 38.2%;
18.5% of these were early childhood degrees. Only 2.4% had a Child Development Associates
(CDA) degree, and 4.3% had no degree.
Design and Procedures
In addition to child/family and teacher characteristics, measures used for this report focused on
oral language and early literacy performance to identify children who might have been appropri-
ate candidates for more intensified Tier 2 and Tier 3 interventions in these areas. A lead measure-
ment director planned and supervised implementation of the multi-site data collection. Staff
assessors in local sites were trained to meet calibration standards on all measures and to meet
pre-specified levels of procedural and measurement reliability.
Measurement
The universal screening measures used in the fall included the Get Ready to Read (GRTR;
Whitehurst & Lonigan, 2001), an early literacy screener; and an early version of the Picture
Naming and Sound Identification measures in the Individual Growth and Development Indicators
Table 1. Number of Classroom by Program Types by Site.
Sites
Program type Kansas Ohio Oregon Minnesota Total % of sample
State Pre-K 14 1 5 20 30.7%
Head Start 17 17 26.1%
Title 1 6 11 3 20 30.7%
Tuition-Based 8 8 12.3%
Full-day programs 12 1 13 26 40%
Half-day programs 20 16 3 39 60%
Total 20 12 17 16 65
% of sample 30.7% 18.4% 26.1% 24.6% 100%
by guest on September 23, 2015jei.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Carta et al. 285
(IGDIs 2.0; see McConnell & Greenwood, 2013). The two IGDI measures (Picture Naming and
Sound Identification) were collected in the fall, midyear, and spring of the academic year, but
only fall assessment data are included in this report.
GRTR. The GRTR is a brief, 20-item screener that measures print knowledge, emergent writing,
and phonological awareness and that includes cut points for identifying children with weak and
very weak language and early literacy skills. Reliability is reported to be .78 for alpha and .80 for
split-half. Classification accuracy (predictive validity) of the measure with the Test of Preschool
Early Literacy (TOPEL; Lonigan, Wagner, & Torgesen, 2007) ranged from 68% to 86% (Wilson
& Lonigan, 2009). Validity is reported to range from .58 to .69 (Phillips, Lonigan, & Wyatt,
2009). Recommended cut point ranges for the GRTR scores are 9 to 20 (average and above) =
Tier 1; 6 to 8 (weak skills) = Tier 2; and 0 to 5 (very weak skills) = Tier 3.
IGDIs. Picture Naming and Sound Identification IGDIs developed by the CRTIEC research team
were used (McConnell & Greenwood, 2013). The Picture Naming IGDI is an individually admin-
istered, untimed task in which a child is presented with a set of pictures, each depicting a familiar
object, and the child is asked to name each picture as quickly as possible (Bradfield et al., 2013).
Administration consists of 4 sample cards and 40 test cards with a child’s score being the number
of pictures correctly named out of 40. This score is converted into a Rasch scale score and a cor-
rect card equivalent score. Picture Naming has a person-level (as opposed to item level) reliabil-
ity score of .81 and criterion validity correlation coefficients of .62 with the Peabody Picture
Vocabulary Test (PPVT; Dunn & Dunn, 2007) and .69 with the Clinical Evaluation of Language
Fundamentals–Preschool (2nd ed.; CELF-P2; Wiig, Secord, & Semel, 2004) expressive vocabu-
lary subtest. A cut score of 28 produced a 70% classification accuracy of being in Tier 1. This
score was associated with a balance in classification accuracy of 70% between correctly identi-
fied for Tier 2/3 (sensitivity) versus falsely identified for Tier 2/3 (selectivity). The criterion used
as the basis for classification accuracy was teachers’ ratings of students’ vocabulary skill as
defined by a three-tier rubric classification developed by the authors.
The Sound Identification IGDI is an individually administered, untimed task in which the
child identified a sound that the letter makes from three choices (Wackerle-Hollman, Schmitt,
Bradfield, Rodriguez, & McConnell, 2013). Like in Picture Naming, items on the Sound
Identification IGDI are presented on individual cards, but in this case, each card depicts three
letters (upper and lower case). The child is asked to point to the letter that makes the sound
modeled by the assessor. The score on this measure is the number of letters correctly identified.
This score is converted into a Rasch scale score and a correct card count equivalent score. The
person-level Rasch reliability score is .60. Sound Identification has a criterion validity correla-
tion coefficient of .54 with the TOPEL Print Knowledge subtest (Lonigan et al., 2007). A cut
score of 10 on Sound Identification was associated with a probability of .74 of being in Tier 1.
This score was associated with a balance in classification accuracy of 70% for Tier 2/3 cor-
rectly identifying children for Tier 2/3 (sensitivity) versus incorrectly identifying Tier 2/3
(selectivity). As with the Picture Naming, the criterion for classification accuracy was teach-
ers’ ratings of child skills on a three-tier rubric, this time rating children on their phonological
awareness skills. Each of the IGDI assessors met pre-specified qualifications based on meeting
a protocol assessing their administration fidelity.
Results
Research Question 1
Children’s overall GRTR mean score was 11.2 (SD = 4.3), higher than the lower cut point limit
of 9 on the GRTR, indicating that, on average, children in the sample were in the low normative
by guest on September 23, 2015jei.sagepub.comDownloaded from
286 Journal of Early Intervention 36(4)
range on the GRTR screener (see Table 3). Also, pairwise comparisons across programs indicated
that preschoolers in the income-eligible programs (i.e., Pre-K, Head Start, and Title 1) had lower
GRTR mean scores compared with those in the Tuition-Based programs.
Overall means on the Picture Naming and Sound Identification IGDIs indicated that, on aver-
age, children’s scores were actually below the normative range. The Picture Naming score for the
entire sample (M = 26.7, SD = 9.0) fell below the lower Tier 1 cut point score of 28 (see Table 3).
As with the GRTR, pairwise comparisons indicated that children in the Tuition-based programs
scored higher on Picture Naming than those in Pre-K (23.4) and Title 1 (27.6) programs who
scored below the Tier 1 cut point (see Table 3).
The overall mean score on Sound Identification (M = 9.9, SD = 5.8) was equivalent to the
lower Tier 1 cut point of 10 for this measure. Mean scores of children from Pre-K (9.6), Head
Start (9.1), and Title 1 (9.7) were all below the Sound Identification cut point for Tier 1. Pairwise
comparisons for Sound Identification indicated that the Tuition-Based program mean of 13.4 was
significantly higher than those of each of the other three programs (see Table 3).
Research Question 2
As might be expected, given the differences in overall means by screening measures reported
above, the proportions of children identified for Tier 2/3 support varied by measure. The propor-
tions of children identified for higher levels of instructional support across the entire sample were
29.9% using the GRTR, 36.4% using Picture Naming, and 48.1% using the Sound Identification
measure. Looking at proportions identified within specific programs, the smallest proportions of
children were identified in the Tuition-based program (see Table 3). Proportions of children also
differed considerably for each measure by program. For example, although the GRTR identified
approximately 30% of the entire sample for higher tiers of support, approximately 40% of chil-
dren in Head Start programs were identified for Tier 2/3 using this measure and fewer than 10%
in Tuition-Based programs.
The Picture Naming measure identified a higher proportion of children across the entire sam-
ple (36.4%) than did the GRTR. Using Picture Naming, the proportions identified for Tier 2/3
support varied considerably across program types with 50.2% identified in Pre-K programs and
only 8.3% in Tuition-Based programs. The Sound Identification measure identified the highest
proportion of children across the entire sample (48.1%) with variation across programs ranging
from 51.1% in Head Start programs and 27.8% in Tuition-Based programs.
Table 3. Mean Screening Scores for Children in Preschool Programs.
Screener Statistic Pre-K (PK)
Head Start
(HS)
Title 1
(T1)
Tuition-
Based (TB)
Entire
sample
Tier 1 cut
points
GRTR M11.0 10.3 11.1 14.4a,b,c 11.2 9.0
SD 4.2 4.3 4.2 4.1 4.3
PN M23.4d,e 28.7 27.6 32.1a,c 26.7 28.0
SD 10.5 8.3 6.8 4.0 9.0
SI M9.6 9.1 9.7 13.4a,b,c 9.9 10.0
SD 6.0 5.2 5.4 5.9 5.8
Note. GRTR = Get Ready to Read; PN = Picture Naming; SI = Sound Identification.
aTB versus PK < .001.
bTB versus HS < .001.
cTB versus T1 < .001.
dPK versus HS < .001.
ePK versus T1 < .001.
by guest on September 23, 2015jei.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Carta et al. 287
Research Question 3
The proportions of children identified for higher than Tier 1 instructional support were 29.9%,
36.4%, and 48.1%, as measured by the GRTR, Picture Naming, and Sound Identification screen-
ers (see Table 4). As previously noted, for all three measures, proportions were lowest for the
income-eligible programs compared with Tuition-Based. The proportions identified for higher
tiers were generally larger when the two IGDIs were used than when the GRTR was employed,
with more children identified for Tier 2/3 services using the IGDI measures than when the GRTR
was used (with the exception of Picture Naming [26.6%] in Head Start versus GRTR at 39.6%).
Across all programs, the highest proportion of children were identified with the Sound
Identification measure.
Children whose home language was other than English were identified in much greater pro-
portions (see Table 3) than children whose home language was English. For example, 46.3% of
ELLs were identified for Tier 2/3 using the GRTR measure, compared with 24.9% for children
who were not ELL. The difference between ELL and not ELL in proportions identified for higher
tiers of support was the greatest for the Picture Naming measure with 81.2% of ELL children
identified and 23.3% of not-ELL children identified. The smallest difference in proportions
between ELL and not ELL occurred for the Sound Identification measure with 55.7% of ELL
children identified for Tier 2/3 and 45.9% of not-ELL children identified.
The proportion of children with IEPs identified for higher tiers of instructional support by the
GRTR was 41.2%, compared with 24.9% of children without IEPs; 63.2% of children with IEPs
were identified using the Sound Identification measure compared with 46.4% without IEPs.
Interestingly, similar proportions of children with and without IEPs were identified using the
Picture Naming measure, at 35.3% with IEPs versus 36.7% without IEPs.
Table 4. Proportion of Children Classified at Risk Using Three Universal Screening Measures (N = 659).
Program type ELL IEP
Screeners Statistic All Pre-K Head Start Title 1 Tuition-Based ELL Not ELL Yes No
Get Ready to Read
Tier 1 (9-20) Count 462 176 84 136 66 80 383 40 423
% 70.1 69.0 60.4 70.5 91.7 53.7 75.1 58.8 71.6
Tier 2 (6-8) Count 129 50 35 40 4 38 91 17 111
% 19.6 19.6 25.2 20.7 5.6 25.5 17.8 25.0 18.8
Tier 3 (0-5) Count 68 29 20 17 2 31 36 11 57
% 10.3 11.4 14.4 8.8 2.8 20.8 7.1 16.2 9.6
Tiers 2 and 3
(0-8)
Count 197 79 55 57 6 69 127 28 168
% 29.9 31.0 39.6 29.5 8.3 46.3 24.9 41.2 28.4
Picture Naming
Tier 1 (28-40) Count 419 127 102 124 66 28 391 44 374
% 63.6 49.8 73.4 64.2 91.7 18.8 76.7 64.7 63.3
Tiers 2 and 3
(0-27)
Count 240 128 37 69 6 121 119 24 217
% 36.4 50.2 26.6 35.8 8.3 81.2 23.3 35.3 36.7
Sound Identification
Tier 1 (10-20) Count 342 127 68 95 52 66 276 25 317
% 51.9 49.8 48.9 49.2 72.2 44.3 54.1 36.8 53.6
Tiers 2 and 3
(0-9)
Count 317 128 71 98 20 83 234 43 274
% 48.1 50.2 51.1 50.8 27.8 55.7 45.9 63.2 46.4
Total Count 659
Note. ELL = English Language Learner; IEP = Individualized Education Plan; Pre-K = pre-kindergarten.
by guest on September 23, 2015jei.sagepub.comDownloaded from
288 Journal of Early Intervention 36(4)
Research Question 4
The concordance among the three screening measures in identifying children for Tier 2 or 3 ser-
vices was 63% (415 out of 659 children) overall. For 37% of the children, there was a lack of
agreement across the three measures in identifying the children for Tier 2/3. The best pairwise
concordance was between the GRTR and the Picture Naming IGDI, with 72% of children classi-
fied in agreement. The least concordance among screening measures was found between Picture
Naming and Sound Identification, at 63%, the first a vocabulary and the second a phonological
awareness measure.
Discussion
The purpose of this investigation was to address questions about the proportions of children
identified by language and early literacy universal screening measures across a range of early
education programs. The measurement model based on the RTI approach used three screening
measures selected for their brevity and repeatability making them efficient and feasible for pro-
gram-wide universal screening.
The primary question we sought to answer was whether the proportions identified for higher
tiers of instructional support would approximate those expected in a three-tier RTI framework.
An assumption of RTI approaches is that the proportion of children identified for higher tiers will
be less than the proportion identified for core instruction (Tier 1). If this assumption is flipped,
and higher proportions are identified for the resource-intensive Tier 2/3 than for Tier 1, an RTI-
driven program would not be feasible within the presumed constraints and costs of implementing
RTI in most typical preschool programs.
Additional questions examined the variation in proportions by measures overall, by programs,
and by ELL and IEP status. We sought to describe the range of differences in the proportion of
identified children that might arise given use of different screening measures, and thus, the prac-
tical implications for preschool RTI feasibility and implementation. It was not our purpose to
examine the superiority of the screeners on differences in domain content or accuracy in deter-
mining risk (e.g., Wilson & Lonigan, 2009).
On the three screeners, children scored, on average, close to the cut score separating Tier 1
from Tier 2/3. The proportion of children identified across the entire sample for Tier 2/3 overall
was as low as 30% when using the GRTR and was as high as 48% when using the Sound
Identification IGDI. The smallest proportions identified for Tier 2/3 were in the Tuition-Based
program with 8.3% indicated on both the GRTR and the Sound Identification measures. The
highest proportions of children were identified for Tier 2/3 by the Sound Identification IGDI, and
this was true across all the programs (range was 27.8% within Tuition-Based programs and
51.1% within Head Start programs). The proportions of children indicated among ELL and IEP
status children were the highest of all. And the concordance among the measures was as low as
63% between the Picture Naming and Sound Identification, and as high as 72% between the
GRTR and Picture Naming, indicating a sizable lack of concordance.
Several important findings were generated by these analyses with important implications for
early education programs adopting RTI frameworks. One finding was that approximately 30% to
35% were identified for higher tiers of support rather than the 20% often indicated for RTI mod-
els implemented in K-12 settings. Another finding was that the proportions of children identified
for Tier 2/3 services were much larger in income-eligible programs compared with Tuition-Based
programs, and this most likely reflects the sizable numbers of children in income-eligible pro-
grams who are ELLs and have IEPs. A striking finding was that more than 81% of ELL children
were identified by the Picture Naming IGDI. Of course, the Picture Naming measure captures
only a child’s expressive vocabulary knowledge in English. A new set of Spanish IGDIs will
by guest on September 23, 2015jei.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Carta et al. 289
allow programs to measure a child’s oral language and early literacy skills in Spanish, and pro-
grams will be able to assess children who are native Spanish speakers in both English and Spanish
(see Wackerle-Hollman, 2014). For children with IEPs, Picture Naming typically was not nearly
the challenge as it was for ELLs, with 35.3% identified by this measure. For children with IEPs,
Sound Identification was much more likely to identify children needing Tier 2/3 (63.2% chil-
dren). Across all screening measures for ELL children and those with IEPs, these subsample
proportions were vastly larger than the theoretical 20% one would expect, indicating a greater
need for resources for the programs serving these children and a reconfigured Tier 1 foundation
to prevent and reduce these numbers. In contrast, Tuition-Based programs with fewer ELL chil-
dren and those receiving early childhood special education had much smaller proportions need-
ing Tier 2/3 services.
It should not be surprising that screening measures did not consistently identify similar pro-
portions of children as being at risk for literacy and language problems. The higher proportion
identified by the Sound Identification measure is most likely a reflection of the fact that many
children are not exposed to instruction on letter–sound correspondence until the year before they
enter kindergarten. Therefore, children without classroom instruction in this aspect of early lit-
eracy may have a higher likelihood of obtaining a positive screen than they would on Picture
Naming, as children may have opportunities to learn vocabulary in their home and classroom
environments prior to receiving specific classroom oral language instruction. These content dif-
ferences tapped by the measures have important implications for programs deciding what they
are planning to teach and aligning content domains appropriately.
Limitations
Several study design limitations need consideration. First, although based on a multi-site study
with a large student sample, the findings reported by program types cannot be considered nation-
ally representative, because the four program types were not equally represented in the study’s
sampling plan due to limitations in resources. As a result, program types in the study were con-
founded by site and regional factors. However, the participating programs were qualified mem-
bers of well-known, national early education programs with their accompanying standards and
criteria for program membership, program quality, and accountability.
Because the Tuition-Based programs were all for-profit service providers, they conformed to
their own unique standards; and thus, they were less likely typical of any particular class of early
education program than were the other three programs. We argue that the meaningfulness of the
program type comparison was in accounting for variation in the numbers of children at language
and early literacy risk based on expected socio-demographic differences associated with the each
program type. Thus, program type differences were due to factors beyond those associated with
program differences in philosophy, pedagogy, or service model.
Second, because Picture Naming and Sound Identification cut points separating Tier 2 and 3
risk levels were not available, we were not able to separate the proportions of children in each
tier. These cut points await further research. Third, results were limited to only one measurement
occasion at the beginning of the year. Additional research is needed on this important aspect of
how universal screeners might identify children at different times of the school year (after
exposed to several weeks of Tier 1 early literacy/language instruction).
Implications
The proportions of children needing more intensive tiers of intervention revealed in this study
clearly raise some serious questions of how an RTI model might be designed and implemented in
early education settings—especially those serving children with low-income eligibility
by guest on September 23, 2015jei.sagepub.comDownloaded from
290 Journal of Early Intervention 36(4)
requirements. We understand that these proportions were influenced by poverty and home lan-
guage differences, for example, as well as the universal screening measure used for identifica-
tion, the domains they tapped, and classification accuracy psychometrics.
These high proportions of children at early literacy/language risk are not news to those who
are familiar with Pre-K programs serving low-income children. However, organizing instruction
that addresses the early literacy/language gaps prior to kindergarten is the real challenge in these
programs. Faced with large numbers of children with risk but limited resources and capacity for
serving them using the RTI approach, programs will need help knowing how to approach the
challenge, how to begin, and how to make subsequent decisions leading to implementation of the
RTI approach and attaining the promised benefits.
Establishing an effective Tier 1 combined with local screening data to quantify the proportions
of children needing higher tiers of support is usually the first step in implementing an RTI
approach (Greenwood, Horner, Kratochwill, & Clements, 2008). Subsequent steps strengthening
Tier 1 based on local screening data also will be needed to improve local outcomes and reduce
the numbers needing Tier 2 and 3 supports. Fortunately, the problem of needed enhancement of
Tier 1 is not the lack of knowledge of what needs to be taught and learned prior to kindergarten
(e.g., Shanahan & Lonigan, 2008). The heart of the challenge is the curriculum, methods, imple-
mentation standards, and professional development needed to teach children these skills.
Review and selection of an evidence-based curriculum to be implemented is an early step in
reducing the proportions of children at risk. Understanding the socio-demographic risk of the
children being served as in this study also helps explain the extent that supports for ELL children
will be needed as part of Tier 1. Screening data in this study also helped identify Sound
Identification in the phonological awareness domain as an area where Tier 1 instruction is likely
needed by all students, regardless of home language environment.
Conclusion
Clearly, regardless of the measure being used, large proportions of children in the United States
will enter kindergarten with significant delays in language and early literacy. “Business as usual”
in early education that typically waits until the early elementary grades to identify children as
struggling learners and only then provides more intensified instruction to these children is inter-
vention that is “too little and too late.” Although the promise of reaching greater effectiveness in
Pre-K instruction through RTI is challenging, we argue for greater efforts to provide the neces-
sary infrastructure tools and supports needed by local programs. Successful RTI implementation
efforts will require more refinement in screening and progress monitoring tools, a greater selec-
tion of evidence-based interventions, and more attention to professional development that insures
high-fidelity implementation of language and early literacy practices.
Authors’ Note
The opinions presented in this article are those of the authors, and no official endorsement of the Institute
of Education Sciences should be inferred.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or
publication of this article.
Funding
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publi-
cation of this article: This work was conducted by the Center for Response to Intervention in Early Childhood
by guest on September 23, 2015jei.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Carta et al. 291
supported by Grant R324C080011 to the University of Kansas (Charles Greenwood and Judith Carta,
Principal Investigators), from the National Center for Special Education Research, Institute of Education
Sciences, U.S. Department of Education.
References
Bradfield, T., Besner, A., Wackerle-Hollman, A., Albano, A., Rodriguez, M., & McConnell, S. (2013).
Redefining Individual Growth and Development Indicators: Oral language. Assessment for Effective
Intervention, 39, 233-244.
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—4th edition. Bloomington, MN:
Pearson.
Fletcher, J. M., Stuebing, K. K., Miciak, J., & Denton, C. (2012, February). Agreement and convergence
of indicators of Response to Intervention. Poster presentation, Pacific Coast Research Conference, San
Diego, CA.
Greenwood, C. R., Bradfield, T., Kaminski, R., Linas, M., Carta, J. J., & Nylander, D. (2011). The Response
to Intervention (RTI) approach in early childhood. Focus on Exceptional Children, 43(9), 1-22.
Greenwood, C. R., Carta, J. J., Atwater, J., Goldstein, H., Kaminski, R., & McConnell, S. R. (2012). Is a
Response to Intervention (RTI) approach to preschool language and early literacy instruction needed?
Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 33, 48-64.
Greenwood, C. R., Horner, R., Kratochwill, T., & Clements, M. (2008). Introduction. In C. R. Greenwood,
T. Kratochwill, & M. Clements (Eds.), School-wide prevention models: Lessons learned in elementary
schools (pp. 3-30). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Guo, Y., Sawyer, B. E., Justice, L. M., & Kaderavek, J. N. (2013). Quality of the literacy environment
in inclusive early childhood special education classrooms. Journal of Early Intervention, 35, 40-60.
doi:10.1177/1053815113500343
Lonigan, C. J., Wagner, D. A., & Torgesen, J. K. (2007). Test of Preschool Early Literacy (TOPEL). Austin,
TX: Pro-Ed.
McConnell, S. R., & Greenwood, C. R. (2013). General outcome measures in early childhood and the
Individual Growth and Development Indicators (IGDIs). In V. Buysse & E. Peisner-Feinberg (Eds.),
Handbook of Response to Intervention (RTI) in early childhood (pp. 143-154). Baltimore, MD:
Brookes.
McConnell, S. R., & Missall, K. N. (2008). Best practices in monitoring progress for preschool children. In
A. Thomas & J. Grimes (Eds.), Best practices in school psychology (5th ed., pp. 561-573). Washington,
DC: National Association of School Psychologists.
National Association of State Special Education Directors. (2008). Response to Intervention: Blueprints for
implementation. Alexandria, VA: Author.
Phillips, B. M., Lonigan, C. J., & Wyatt, M. A. (2009). Predictive validity of the Get Ready to Read! screener:
Concurrent and long-term relations with reading-related skills. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42,
133-147.
Shanahan, T., & Lonigan, C. J. (2008). Developing early literacy: A Report of the National Early Literacy
Panel. Retrieved from http://www.nifl.gov/publications/pdf/NELPReport09.pdf
Wackerle-Hollman, A. (2014). Spanish individual growth and development project. Retrieved from http://
www.cehd.umn.edu/CEED/projects/spanishigdis/default.html
Wackerle-Hollman, A., Schmitt, B., Bradfield, T., Rodriguez, M., & McConnell, S. (2013). Redefining
Individual Growth and Development Indicators: Phonological awareness. Journal of Learning
Disabilities. Advance online publication. doi:10.1177/0022219413510181
Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (2001). Get Ready to Read. Columbus, OH: Pearson.
Wiig, E., Secord, W., & Semel, E. (2004). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals—Preschool 2nd
edition. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.
Wilson, S. B., & Lonigan, C. J. (2009). An evaluation of two emergent literacy screening tools for preschool
children. Annals of Dyslexia, 59, 115-131. doi:10.1007/s11881-009-0026-9
Zill, N., & Resnick, G. (2006). Emergent literacy of low-income children in Head Start: Relationships with child
and family characteristics, program factors, and classroom quality. In D. K. Dickinson & S. B. Neuman
(Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (Vol. 2, pp. 347-371). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
by guest on September 23, 2015jei.sagepub.comDownloaded from
... To most effectively address all students and provide robust prevention, schools may adopt a multitiered system of support (MTSS) approach such as Responsiveness to Intervention, whereby students are provided effective instruction and effective classroom placement procedures before academic risk compounds into failure and subsequently engage with evidence-based interventions to remediate any remaining deficits (Benner et al., 2013;Lurz & Walsh, 2022;Siegel, 2020). In fact, according to Carta et al. (2015), children from low-income families were disproportionately represented at higher levels of intervention to remediate risk. The authors recommended that schools increase their focus on robust early literacy practices to prevent higher rates of disability diagnoses and reading challenges in the future. ...
Article
Full-text available
Reading comprehension is critical for early learners’ academic success. Derived relations are an important feature of reading comprehension within the science of behavior because reading comprehension involves experientially acquired derived stimulus relations (Helou-Care, 2008; Neves in Behavioral Interventions 38:376–400, 2023, Park, 2005; da Silva in Experimental Analysis of Human Behavior Bulletin 24:1–8, 2006). That is, derived relations pertain to a learner demonstrating comprehension by making untrained connections to previously trained stimuli (e.g., words). For example, a learner who is trained that “kitten” is a type of “cat” and that all “cats meow” should then be able to demonstrate the untrained connection that “kittens meow.” The purpose of this study was to investigate the effects of the Corrective Reading Comprehension (CR) curriculum, which employs an instructional sequence involving derived relations, on reading comprehension responses entailing derived relations. Four typically developing first-grade students were the participants in this study and the students were divided into two dyads. The principal researcher instructed each dyad on each of the five lessons of CR until mastery was obtained and then conducted postprobes on the dependent measures that were metaphors, derived relations from sentences, derived relations from letters/numbers, and implicit/explicit comprehension questions. The researchers used a delayed multiple probe design across 1st-grade dyads to assess the effect of five CR lessons on derived relations and reading comprehension responses of the participants. Posttest assessments showed gains for all four participants. The article concludes with a discussion of the main findings, implications for future research, and the limitations of this study.
... The breadth of academic inquiry in this area spans the effectiveness of various instructional strategies to the significance of the home literacy environment (HLE). Key considerations include the design and implementation of teacher-led interventions (Carta et al., 2014;McConnell et al., 2014), the continuous evolution of these strategies to keep pace with changing educational landscapes (Kaminski and Powell-Smith, 2017;Phillips et al., 2021), and dynamic evaluations regarding the success and challenges of such interventions (Gustafson et al., 2014;Greenwood et al., 2017). Within this overarching topic, three primary facets stood out. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Early reading has gained significant attention in the academic community. With the increasing volume of literature on this subject, it has become crucial to assess the current research landscape and identify emerging trends. Methods This study utilized the dynamic topic model to analyze a corpus of 1,638 articles obtained from the Web of Science Core Collection to furnish a lucid understanding of the prevailing research and forecast possible future directions. Results Our in-depth assessment discerned 11 cardinal topics, among which notable ones were interventions' impacts on early reading competencies; foundational elements of early reading: phonological awareness, letters, and, spelling; and early literacy proficiencies in children with autism spectrum disorder. Although most topics have received consistent research attention, there has been a marked increase in some topics' popularity, such as foundational elements of early reading and early literary proficiencies in children with autism spectrum disorder. Conversely, other topics exhibited a downturn. Discussion This analytical endeavor has yielded indispensable insights for scholars, decision-makers, and field practitioners, steering them toward pivotal research interrogatives, focal interest zones, and prospective research avenues. As per our extensive survey, this paper is a pioneering holistic purview of the seminal areas of early reading that highlights expected scholarly directions.
... The fact is like preparation that they need to face the wider community. But, it needs further research on the economic status of children in rural and urban areas (Carta et al., 2015), as well as the intensity of instruction provided by parents (families) and support to literacy (Borisova et al., 2017;Morrow et al., 2006) because the available data does not provide this information. The need for children aged 5-6 years to read is the most potent encouragement for children to have alphabet knowledge (Oncu & Unluer, 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
Alphabetic skills were one of the early literacy skills. Several studies on alphabetic skills have been conducted in Indonesia. Meta-analysis was used to describe the children's alphabet skills aged 3 to 6 years as an aggregate. The maximum likelihood estimation method and random effect design were performed to aggregate children's alphabetic skills that can be generalized in Indonesia. The articles were obtained from research published between 2014 and 2021. Those were accessed and collected through a collection of bachelor's thesis on Google Scholar and university digital libraries. The meta-analysis was carried out with JASP. The results showed that the ability to recognize letters among children aged 3 to 6 years in Indonesia was 50.16 (scale 0-100). Despite the fact that mastering letters has an impact on children’s growth in early literacy, the study’s findings show that youngsters in Indonesia still have a low level of letter recognition. These results encourage to improve the quality of kindergartens literacy instruction in Indonesia
... Departing from the explanation, early childhood education programs can serve as institutional education that prevents reading inability among children with low social and economic status. Reading failure in the future thus can be prevented by performing screening in the form of assessment in the school, performing normative monitoring, and providing early intervention to the children at risk (Carta et al., 2015;Coyne & Harn, 2006). Therefore, teachers should implement the prevention-oriented approach by performing valid early literacy assessments (Invernizzi et al., 2004;Justice et al., 2002;Lonigan et al., 2011). ...
Article
p style="text-align: justify;">The study aims to identify how kindergarten teachers perform early literacy assessments in the classroom and the challenges these teachers have in performing such assessments. During the study, the data were gathered through an in-depth interview in the form of a forum group discussion (FGD). Then, the phenomenological data were attained from 30 public and private Kindergarten teachers. The researchers could illustrate how these kindergarten teachers assessed their children's early literacy through these data. Furthermore, the study results show that the teachers' literacy knowledge has been sufficient and that the literacy programs for the children have been variously designed in each school. The teachers' techniques in performing the early literacy assessment are observation and documentation (portfolio), and the measurement of literacy skills itself refers to the scope of literacy. Concerning the findings, numerous obstacles and expectations that kindergarten teachers have are also discussed within the study.</p
... ELLs that fit within the response to intervention and multi-tiered systems of support frameworks (e.g., Carta et al., 2015;Samson & Lesaux, 2009). This study's results further support the use of Tutoring Buddy as a Tier 2 intervention-specifically, Tutoring Buddy can be utilized to assess, teach, and monitor LSK acquisition for ELL preschoolers who are not accessing the universal preschool curriculum. ...
Article
The importance of letter sound knowledge (LSK) as a precursor to later literacy skills has been well‐documented. Since English language learners (ELLs), or students who first acquired a language other than English, continue to underperform in reading compared to their English‐speaking peers, they are particularly at‐risk for reading and academic difficulties. The current study examines the utility and acceptability of teaching letter sounds to four ELL preschoolers through a software‐based incremental rehearsal tool (Tutoring Buddy). All students demonstrated increases in LSK, with large effect sizes derived through percentage of all nonoverlapping data analyses. Increases were also noted in the students' generalization of letter sounds to measures of letter sound fluency and nonsense word fluency, signified by small to large effect sizes. Large effect sizes generated for real word fluency measures indicated that all students were able to apply LSK to decoding real, consonant‐vowel‐consonant words. All students rated Tutoring Buddy as helpful for their letter sound learning, and varied in how enjoyable they found the intervention to be. Overall, the results support the use of Tutoring Buddy as an effective and acceptable method for teaching letter sounds to young ELLs. Implications for school‐based professionals working with ELL and future directions for research are discussed.
Article
Engagement and academic achievement are generally correlated among elementary school students without learning disabilities (LDs). However, it is unclear if this pattern holds for students with LDs, who have lower achievement and engagement than their peers. This study examined whether links between achievement and student-reported behavioral engagement operate differentially for students with and without LDs in a nationally representative U.S. sample ( N ≈ 15,660). Multiple group path analyses demonstrated that associations between third- and fourth-grade achievement were stronger for students with LDs, compared with students without LDs. For children without LDs, engagement mediated all pathways from third-grade to fourth-grade achievement. For students with LDs, engagement only mediated the pathway from third-grade math to fourth-grade reading. Overall, our study emphasizes the crucial role of early achievement for later school success, particularly for students with LDs. We discuss the importance of early intervention to support LD students’ academic achievement and engagement.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose Phonological awareness and alphabet knowledge are fundamental building blocks for literacy development. We identified preschoolers with persistent delays in these skills and evaluated the efficacy of a supplemental curriculum to remediate deficits in early literacy skills. Method Using a cluster design, 21 classrooms were randomly assigned to early literacy versus language intervention conditions. Sixty children identified through fall semester assessments of phonological awareness and alphabet knowledge were enrolled in small-group instruction. Teachers completed eLearning modules, used a digital platform to enter data and facilitate data-based decision making, received practice-based coaching, and delivered 12 weekly units of an early literacy curriculum. Comparison teachers delivered similarly administered small-group language instruction. Results Large effects were evident for letter sounds, phoneme segmentation, first sound, and first sound fluency measures (d = 0.92, 4.77, 1.15, and 1.50, respectively), and nonsignificant, small effects for letter naming and blending measures (d = 0.26 and 0.27). Discussion This early literacy intervention package had practical benefits, with 90% of experimental group preschoolers exceeding the phonemic awareness benchmark for beginning kindergarten compared to 45% in the comparison group, for example. Providing preschool teachers with tools and support for implementing a tiered approach to early literacy intervention holds promise for producing impressive gains in skills required for children to succeed in later schooling.
Article
Engaging, focusing, and persisting in the completion of tasks are among the skills needed for school success. Tracking whether a child is learning cognitive problem-solving skills is essential in knowing if they are acquiring skills important for development and school readiness; and if not, how they are responding to early intervention. Use of the Early Cognitive Problem-Solving Indicator (EPSI) was documented by data for 2,614 children (6–42 months of age) collected by the early childhood staff from 45 programs. Results indicated that the EPSI was (a) scalable across programs, assessors, and assessment occasions, (b) reliable, (c) sensitive to growth over months of age, (d) comprised a dynamic continuum of skills within and across skills over time, and (e) moderated by children’s disability status but not gender or home language. Implications for research and practice are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Seeking to achieve greater effectiveness in educating the nation's youth, the Response to Intervention (RTI) approach is increasingly being implemented in US schools (Berkeley, Bender, Peaster, & Saunders, 2009; Walker & Shinn, 2010). The approach is a paradigm shift in K–12 education that is affecting early education, early intervention, and early childhood special education as well. The shift moves practice away from the tradi-tional model of waiting for students to qualify for special education before serving them to one of intervening immediately to prevent developmental delays and challenges from becoming disabilities. Supporting implementation of RTI are favorable policies in the No Child Left Behind Act (NCLB, 2001) and the Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement Act (IDEA, 2004) that have put the spotlight on improving students' results through early and sustained use of evidence-based practice. While not specifically addressed in early childhood special education policy, RTI is supported by federal and state accountability policies requiring annual reporting of individual child progress and an expectation of improving results for children served (Head Start for School Readiness Act, 2007; US Department of Education [USDE], 2006). The policies and reports of professional organizations and advocacy groups like the National Association of State Directors of Special Education, National Association of School Psychologists, and the National Center for Learning Disabilities (NCLD) also sup-port the RTI model. These organizations, among others, (e.g., National Association of State Special Education Directors, 2008; Shinn & Walker, 2010; Thomas & Grimes, 2008) embrace RTI as a science-based practice and have made RTI knowledge and practice part of their professional expectations and advocacy. The Council for Exceptional Children has a position statement on RTI (CEC, 2007), and the Division of Early Childhood (DEC), in conjunction with the National Association of the Education of Young Childhood (NAEYC) and the Head Start Association are currently working on a joint early childhood position statement on RTI.
Article
Full-text available
Learning to read is one of the most important indicators of academic achievement. The development of early literacy skills during the preschool years is associated with improved reading outcomes in later grades. One of these skill areas, phonological awareness, shows particular importance because of its strong link to later reading success. Presented here are two studies that describe the development and revision of four measures of phonological awareness skills: Individual Growth and Development Indicators Sound Blending, Syllable Sameness, Rhyming, and Alliteration 2.0. The authors discuss the measure development process, revision, and utility within an early childhood Response to Intervention framework.
Article
Full-text available
Preschool experience plays a role in children’s development. However, for programs with language and early literacy goals, the question remains whether preschool instructional experiences are sufficiently effective to achieve these goals for all children. In a multisite study, the authors conducted a process-product description of preschool instruction, and children’s growth and outcomes in typical programs (i.e., Pre-K, Title 1, Head Start, Tuition-Based) using a response to intervention (RTI) perspective. Results indicated that (a) students in their preschool year prior to kindergarten made small gains, but students starting the year in lower Tier 2 and 3 performance levels did not close initial skills gaps, (b) variations were noted by program types with varying sociodemographics and instructional processes, and (c) the quality of instruction (Tier 1) received by all was low with room for improvement. Implications for future research on the application of the RTI approach and potential benefits are discussed.
Article
Language skills developed during preschool contribute strongly to later reading and academic achievement. Effective preschool assessment and intervention should focus on core components of language development, specifically oral language skills. The Early Language and Literacy Individual Growth and Development Indicators (IGDIs) are a set of measures that have demonstrated utility in language and early literacy assessment, evaluation, and intervention studies. The purpose of this article is to describe two studies conducted to build on the existing oral language IGDI, Picture Naming, to increase utility for instructional decision making, particularly within an early childhood Response to Intervention framework. Study 1 outlines the initial development and piloting of potential measures and Study 2 describes the revision and continued analysis of the most promising measures. Results of Study 1 and Study 2 indicate that Picture Naming has continued utility as a measure of oral language skill development and that one newly developed oral language IGDI, Definitional Vocabulary, has potential utility.
Article
The purpose of this study was to examine the quality of the literacy environment in inclusive early childhood special education (ECSE) classrooms (N = 54). The first aim was to describe the quality of the literacy environment in terms of structure (i.e., book materials and print/writing materials) and instruction (i.e., instructional support). The second aim was to examine the interrelationships among teacher and classroom factors and the quality of the literacy environment. Results showed that, on average, the quality of the structural literacy environment was low to moderate, and the quality of the instructional literacy environment was generally low. Only one factor, the number of children who were dual-language learners, related to the quality of the structural literacy environment, whereas the quality of the instructional literacy environment was positively associated with two teacher-level factors (teacher education and self-efficacy) and negatively associated with one teacher-level factor (the number of language and literacy workshops attended). Study findings suggest the need to examine ways to improve the literacy environment of ECSE classrooms and to better understand sources of variance with respect to the literacy environment.
Book
Introduction The motivation for this Handbook arose from a conversation with Don Palmer, who raised the question of whether organization theories in general have life cycles. Given the proliferation of theoretical paradigms, do organization theories build into coherent conceptual frameworks supported by diligently conducted empirical work, or do they fragment into proliferated confusion? That conversation never proceeded to a comparative assessment of organization theories. But it did lead to the present volume. It seemed, in late 2004, when the idea of a Handbook was mooted, an appropriate moment to take stock of the institutional perspective on organizations because we were approaching the thirtieth anniversary of seminal papers that not only triggered revitalization of interest in the role of institutions but became known as the new institutionalism. It is important at the outset to set down certain scope conditions for this volume: 1. Our interest is in understanding organizations. How 2. ...