ArticlePDF Available

Reading for pleasure and progress in vocabulary and mathematics

Wiley
British Educational Research Journal
Authors:

Abstract

This paper examines inequalities in attainment in vocabulary and mathematics at age 16 for a nationally representative cohort of people born in Britain in 1970 (the 1970 British Cohort Study). Our analytical sample is n = 3,583 cohort members who completed vocabulary and mathematics tests at age 16. We explore whether inequalities as a result of childhood social background are similar across the linguistic and mathematical domains, or whether they differ, and to what extent these inequalities are accounted for by families’ social class position, parents’ education and home reading resources, as well as by children's own reading for pleasure. As reading can be seen as an indicator of ‘cultural capital’, we also test the influence of an alternative indicator of cultural capital, playing a musical instrument. Our longitudinal analysis addresses the question of the extent to which differences in attainment are determined by age 10; and which factors are linked to a growth in differentials during adolescence. We show that childhood reading is linked to substantial cognitive progress between the ages of 10 and 16, whereas playing an instrument is not. Reading is most strongly linked to progress in vocabulary, with a weaker, but still substantial link to progress in mathematics. Strikingly, reading for pleasure is more strongly linked than parental education to cognitive progress in adolescence.
Reading for pleasure and attainment in vocabulary and
mathematics
Dr. Alice Sullivan (corresponding author)*
Director, 1970 British Cohort Study
Centre for Longitudinal Studies
Mr. Matt Brown
Survey Manager, 1970 British Cohort Study
Centre for Longitudinal Studies
*Contact details:
Centre for Longitudinal Studies
Institute of Education, University of London
55-59 Gordon Square
LONDON
WH1H 0NU
Tel: 020 76126661
Email: a.sullivan@ioe.ac.uk
Acknowledgements: We are grateful to the BCS70 cohort members for their
contribution over many years. Thanks to Tarek Mostafa for providing the analysis in
Appendix A1. Thanks to Dick Wiggins for comments on an earlier draft, and to two
anonymous reviewers.
1
Reading for pleasure and progress in vocabulary and
mathematics
Abstract
This paper examines inequalities in attainment in vocabulary and mathematics at age 16 for a
nationally representative cohort of people born in Britain in 1970 (the 1970 British Cohort
Study). Our analytical sample is n=3, 583 cohort members who completed vocabulary and
mathematics tests at age 16. We explore whether inequalities due to childhood social
background are similar across the linguistic and mathematical domains, or whether they
differ, and to what extent these inequalities are driven by families’ social class position,
parents’ education and home reading resources. We examine the role of children’s own
reading for pleasure controlling for all these background factors. As reading can be seen as an
indicator of ‘cultural capital’, we also test the influence of an alternative indicator of cultural
capital, playing a musical instrument. Our longitudinal analysis addresses the question of the
extent to which differences in attainment are determined by age 10; and which factors are
linked to a growth in differentials during adolescence. We show that childhood reading is
linked to substantial cognitive progress between the ages of 10 and 16, whereas playing an
instrument is not. Reading is most strongly linked to progress in vocabulary, with a weaker,
but still substantial link to progress in maths. Strikingly, reading for pleasure is more strongly
linked than parental education to cognitive progress in adolescence.
2
Keywords: Reading; longitudinal; BCS70; vocabulary; mathematics. Introduction
Persistent socio-economic inequalities in educational attainment and cognitive scores have
been documented by many studies over the years, and the explanation of these social
inequalities is one of the central problems within the sociology of education (Halsey et al.
1980). Debate continues regarding the mechanisms via which privileged families ensure the
educational and subsequent occupational success of their children, and the relative
importance of economic and cultural resources in determining class differentials in
educational outcomes.
Sociologists have put forward both cultural and economic explanations for educational
inequalities. The most prominent theory emphasising the importance of cultural resources,
Bourdieu’s theory of cultural reproduction (Bourdieu & Passeron [1977] 1990) has generated
much debate about the nature of cultural capital. Interpretations can be divided into the
‘status seeking’ and ‘information processing’ views (Ganzeboom 1982). According to the
status seeking view, children who display cultural attributes that are valued by the school are
rewarded by teachers with high attainment. In contrast, the ‘information processing’ view
suggests that particular cultural activities actually foster intellectual development. We have
argued that reading is distinctive because it develops linguistic ability and wider knowledge
(Sullivan 2002; 2007). Reading differs in this way from other cultural activities such as
music or going to galleries and museums (often termed ‘beaux arts’ participation). We know
that the home reading culture is important for children’s early cognitive scores (Byford et al.
2012). Past studies have also found that books in the home and children’s reading behaviour,
but not ‘beaux arts’ participation, help to explain social differentials in children’s outcomes
(De Graaf et al. 2000; Sullivan 2001) (Cheung & Andersen 2003; Georg 2004; Jaeger 2011;
Sullivan 2001).
The role of language has been neglected in most empirical studies of cultural reproduction,
yet Bourdieu’s discussion of cultural capital emphasises the importance of language as the
key to success in school. "Obvious in the literary disciplines but more subtle in the sciences,
the ability to manipulate academic language remains the principal factor in success in
examinations" (Bourdieu et al. 1994) (p.21). Hirsch (1983) also emphasises vocabulary,
though from a different perspective, on the grounds that knowledge of words is both an
adjunct to knowledge of concepts and assists further learning. It has been argued that reading
must be a particularly important driver of vocabulary development, given the paucity of
vocabulary used in speech compared to books, even comparing children’s books to adult
speech (Cunningham & Stanovich 1998).
Children pick up styles of speech, vocabulary and forms of reasoning simply by hearing their
parents talk, and also pick up reading habits through seeing their parents read, and having
reading materials readily available in the home (Sullivan 2007). We may expect this passive
cultural transmission to be most important in the case of linguistic skill, since studies have
found huge differences in the number of different words that children are exposed to in
middle and working class homes (Hart & Rinsley 1995). Vocabulary is transmitted within the
home almost constantly, without any conscious effort. In contrast, while parents may well
3
seek to promote their children’s success in subjects such as mathematics, this must typically
be done consciously, with discrete time set aside for the task. It is arguable therefore that we
should expect wider disparities according to parental cultural resources in children’s
linguistic ability than in their abilities in mathematics. On the other hand if reading ability
and vocabulary are central to further learning in all disciplines, we may expect the influences
on vocabulary development to also drive attainment in other subjects including mathematics.
Evidence for this view is provided by a recent study of monozygotic twins (Ritchie et al.
2014 in press) showing that learning to read well leads to improved general intelligence. In
this paper, we are able to address whether the socio-economic and cultural factors driving
vocabulary are essentially the same as those driving mathematics attainment, or whether they
differ.
Studies focussing on children’s reading have faced challenges in unpacking the reciprocal
relationship between ability and participation in reading (Cunningham & Stanovich 1998).
We know that children who read a lot perform well in school, but are they bright because they
read, or is it simply that they read because they are bright? Reviews of the literature find
extensive evidence for an association between reading frequency and reading attainment
(Twist et al. 2007), but note the difficulty in establishing whether reading frequency actually
leads to improved attainment in the absence of compelling longitudinal evidence (Clark & De
Zoysa 2012; Clark & Rumbold 2006; Department for Education 2012; Department for
Education Education Standards Research Team 2012). While some longitudinal studies on
reading exist, they have typically been small scale, covered relatively short periods, and
lacked controls for socio-economic background (Taylor et al. 1990).
The growth in cognitive inequalities according to socioeconomic status during childhood has
been established by analyses of the British cohort studies of 1946, 1958, 1970 and 2000
(Douglas 1964; Feinstein 2003; 2004; Fogelman & Goldstein 1976; Sullivan et al. 2013), but
for the 1970 cohort this has only so far been examined up to age 10. Here we analyse the
extent to which social inequalities in the 1970 cohort continued to grow during adolescence,
and which childhood resources drive this growth. Specifically, what is the role of reading for
pleasure during childhood and adolescence? As far as we are aware, ours is the first large
study to take a longitudinal approach to reading and cognitive development during
adolescence.
Research questions
In this paper, we focus on the potential role of both parents’ and children’s reading in
explaining differentials in cognitive test scores at age 16 for a cohort of children born in
1970. The BCS70 is a large, nationally representative, longitudinal birth cohort study,
containing rich measures of both cognition and home background, which provides some
strong advantages in tackling these questions.
4
We address the following questions.
1. Are inequalities due to parental social background similar across the domains of
vocabulary and mathematics, or do they differ? We hypothesize that parental
education (which is typically seen as reflecting cultural and cognitive resources) but
not occupational social class (which is an indicator of economic position) may be
more strongly linked to vocabulary than to maths attainment.
2. Are inequalities due to parental social background explained by parental reading
environment, behaviour and ability? We hypothesise that the link between parental
education and children’s test scores is more likely to be accounted for in this way than
the link between parental social class and children’s test scores.
3. Is the influence of parental reading environment, behaviour and ability explained by
children’s own reading behaviour?
4. Which factors are linked to changing test scores between the ages of 10 and 16? In
particular, is the child’s own reading linked to cognitive progress? We hypothesise
that children’s reading should be more strongly linked than children’s music
participation to cognitive progress. We also hypothesise that childhood reading should
be more strongly linked to progress in vocabulary than to progress in maths, since
reading directly exposes children to new vocabulary. Nevertheless, we expect that
reading will develop mathematical abilities to some degree, since reading ability
facilitates learning in all subjects.
Data and variables
The 1970 British Cohort Study (BCS70) follows the lives of more than 17,000 people born in
England, Scotland and Wales in a single week of 1970 (Elliott & Shepherd 2006). Over the
course of cohort members’ lives, the BCS70 has collected information on health, physical,
educational and social development, and economic circumstances among other factors. Since
the birth survey in 1970, there have been eight surveys (or ‘waves’) at ages 5, 10, 16, 26, 30,
34, 38 and 42. An understanding of the educational progress of this cohort during their
childhood is vital to understanding their later life course trajectories.
The 1970 cohort study is rich in cognitive test scores throughout the early years, and the early
test scores (up to age ten) have been analysed extensively, including influential work by
Feinstein (2003; 2004). The cognitive scores at age ten have also been shown to be important
for adult outcomes, including in employment (Breen & Goldthorpe 2001) and health (Batty et
al. 2007). To date, almost no research has been carried out using the age 16 test scores
(though see (Duncan et al. 2012)), due to a lack of awareness of the existence of these scores
among the research community, and because the arithmetic dataset was not deposited until
2008. We aim to encourage wider use of these variables by researchers.
5
Because we exploit data from all of the childhood waves of the study, including the age 16
wave, the problem of missing data must be addressed. The age 16 survey employed sixteen
separate survey instruments, and unfortunately coincided with a teachers’ strike which
affected the completion of those instruments, including cognitive tests, that were
administered via schools (Dodgeon 2008). This led to substantial instrument non-response.
Nevertheless, the 1986 sample is more representative in terms of the birth characteristics of
the sample in 1970 than any other wave of the study excluding the birth wave (Mostafa &
Wiggins 2014). Appendix A1 shows logit response models for response to both the arithmetic
and vocabulary tests in terms of the birth characteristics of the 1970 sample. Respondents
who took both tests were no more highly selected than the 1986 sample as a whole. Levels of
missing data for the variables used in our analysis are provided in table 1. As list-wise
deletion was not a practical option, we use multiple imputation to ‘fill-in’ values of any
missing items in the variables selected for our analysis adopting Schafer’s algorithm under
the assumption of ‘missing at random’ (MAR). All reported analyses are averaged across
twenty replicates based upon Rubin’s Rule for the efficiency of estimation under a reported
degree of missingness across the whole data of just under 20% (Little & Rubin 1987). The
analytical sample consists of the 3, 583 cohort members who completed the age 16
vocabulary and maths tests.
It is important to acknowledge that people’s levels of motivation and compliance, as well as
potential stereotype-threat (Croizet & Claire 1998; Spencer et al. 1999) will affect their
scores in cognitive tests. We also acknowledge that multiple-choice tests do not capture the
full range of academic skills, and girls tend to fare worse in multiple-choice tests than in
other forms of assessment (Gipps & Murphy 1994). We do not interpret the tests used here as
providing an estimation of innate intelligence. They are simply tests of attainment based on
the capability and motivation to complete a particular task under given conditions.
Analytical Strategy
We will first present a descriptive analysis of children’s attainment trajectories. Subsequently
we will model mathematics and vocabulary scores at age 16 as a function of family
characteristics, childhood reading behaviour and children’s prior attainment scores. This will
be presented as a series of multivariate linear regression models. The outcome and
explanatory variables to be used in these analyses are described below.
Outcome variables: Mathematics and vocabulary at age 16
Influences on cognitive scores and changes in these scores may be expected to differ
according to the nature of the assessment, and the demands the assessment makes on
processing capacity or problem solving as opposed to knowledge (Richards & Sacker 2003).
Here we examine attainment scores in vocabulary and maths. Vocabulary reflects linguistic
competence, which we expect to be developed within the home and through reading rather
than primarily through schoolwork. An advantage for our purposes of the vocabulary test
6
used here is that it is purely a test of linguistic competence, with no verbal reasoning element.
The vocabulary test is purely dependent on recall, while the maths test can be seen as a test of
problem solving, although of course background knowledge is important here too.
Maths attainment was assessed using the Applied Psychology Unit (UPU) Arithmetic test - a
30 minute assessment comprising 60 multiple choice items covering arithmetic, probabilities
and area (Closs & Hutchings 1976; Dodgeon 2008). Vocabulary was assessed using a 75 item
multiple choice test. Table 1 shows descriptive statistics for the mathematics and vocabulary
tests.
TABLE 1
Explanatory variables
This section outlines the predictors to be used in the regression models presented in the
results section of this paper. The descriptives for the variables in our regression analyses are
shown in tables 2 and 3.
TABLE 2
TABLE 3
Model 1: Socio-economic background, sex and siblings
Social class is coded according to the NS-SEC schema (Goldthorpe 1997) at age ten (in
1980). NS-SEC is an occupational schema, and determines class position in terms of
employment relations. It reflects not just income, but longer term economic security, stability
and prospects, as reflected in a person’s labour market position. It also reflects power in terms
of relationships of authority, control and autonomy within the workplace (Goldthorpe &
McKnight 2006). We adopt a ‘dominance’ approach to household social class, taking the
mother or father’s occupation whichever is higher, using the three category classificationi.
Parents’ education is coded as the highest qualification of the mother or father (whichever of
the two is higher).
Position in the birth order is a well established predictor of child outcomes (Nisbet 1953).
Sex is included to test whether development in maths and vocabulary differed between boys
and girls.
Model 2: Model 1 + Home reading culture
We are able to provide a relatively thorough operationalisation of the home reading culture
compared to many previous studies. We include not just reading to the child, but also parental
reading behaviour, newspapers (broadsheet or tabloid) in the home and parental reading
ability.
7
When the cohort members were aged 5, mothers were asked on how many days of the last 7
the child had been read to. In 1986, the mother was asked whether she and her husband read
books. Seeing parents reading may affect children’s attitudes to reading, and parents’ reading
habits are also likely to be positively linked to parents’ reading ability. Mothers were also
asked which newspapers were in the home and were thus available for the teenager to read.
We are able to differentiate between broadsheet and tabloid households and those who did not
have newspapers at home. The prose style of tabloid (then as now) was simpler and geared
towards a lower reading age and smaller vocabulary than the broadsheets. During the 1980s,
newspaper readership was high, and the type of newspaper read was (as it remains) a strong
cultural identifier (Chan & Goldthorpe 2007).
In the absence of a reading assessment for parents, we rely on a self-reported measure of
reading difficulties. Mothers were asked whether they or their husband had reading
difficulties, either when learning to read or currently (maternal self-completion 1986).
Positive responses to these items are low, with 5% of mothers admitting to any difficulties for
themselves, and 4% for their husbands. Item non-response was high on these questions (6%
for mothers and 9% for fathers) and, in preliminary analyses, was found to be significantly
associated with poor performance in both assessments suggesting that those with reading
difficulties may have been embarrassed or reluctant to report this. We acknowledge also that
subjective reporting of difficulties tends to be much lower than actual tested difficulties
(Bynner & Parsons 2006). Here we use a binary variable which indicates whether the mother
reported that either she herself or the father had difficulties with reading. Although parental
reading ability and habits and reading materials available in the home were captured when the
cohort member was age 16, we consider these to be variables which would be unlikely to be
subject to significant change during the preceding years of the cohort member’s life and
therefore do not see it as problematic to treat these variables as predictors of outcomes at age
16. We acknowledge the drawback that we have no measure of books in the home, a variable
which has been shown to be a powerful predictor of children’s educational attainment (Chiu
& Chow 2010; Evans et al. 2010).
Model 3: Model 2 + Cohort member’s own reading and playing a musical instrument
The 1980 self-completion pupil questionnaire includes items on reading books and going to
the library. The 1986 cohort member self-completion questionnaires contained items on
reading books and newspapers. Book reading declined between the ages of ten and 16. Some
difference may be due to the earlier variable being reported by mothers while the later
variable is self-reported, but it is also likely that there was a genuine decline in reading
among teenagers, perhaps partly due to a lack of availability and promotion of suitable books
for this age group. For example, libraries during the 1980s typically devoted very little space
to books aimed at adolescents. This decline in reading for pleasure as children get older is in
line with previous research (Clark & Rumbold 2006).
We also include variables indicating whether the cohort member played a musical instrument
at age 5 and age 10. Playing an instrument would not be expected to directly develop skills in
maths or vocabulary, but can be seen as a more general indicator of the cultural climate of the
8
home, and arguably represents ‘status seeking’ behaviour, in the sense that parents who
encourage their children to play an instrument are complying with a strong social norm
within the educated middle-classes.
Model 4: Model 3 + Cognitive tests at five and ten
The cohort members took age-appropriate tests at age five and ten. These are included in our
final model, to assess cognitive progress between the ages of 10 and 16. Descriptive statistics
for these tests are shown in table 4. We use Principal Components Analysis (PCA) to extract a
single scale for cognition at each age (5 and 10). Full details of the tests used and the PCA
analysis are provided in Parsons (2014).
TABLE 4
Age five tests
The cohort members took five tests designed to capture verbal and non-verbal skills:
Copying designs (Rutter et al. 1970); English picture vocabulary (Brimer & Dunn 1962);
Human figure drawing (draw-a-man) (Goodenough 1926; Harris 1963); Complete a profile;
and Schonell graded reading (Golding 1975).
Age ten tests
The eight tests taken at age ten were: Shortened Edinburgh reading test (Godfrey Thompson
Unit 1978); Pictorial language comprehension test; Friendly maths test; Spelling (dictation
task); British Ability Scales (BAS) (Elliott et al. 1979; Hill 2005): Two verbal subscales
(word definitions and word similarities) and two non-verbal subscales (digit recall and
matrices) (Butler et al. 1980).
Results
We begin with a simple graphical presentation of children’s trajectories over time, before
presenting the results of our regression analyses.
Attainment trajectories
First we look at children’s progress in vocabulary and mathematics in terms of their
percentile rankings. Figure 1 shows vocabulary trajectories between the ages of five and 16
comparing frequent readers to infrequent readers and the children of at least one graduate to
those whose parents had no qualifications. Young people who read books most frequently at
10 and 16 (20%) are classified as high readers. Those who were in the lowest book reading
category at both ages or the lowest at one and second lowest at the other (21%) were
categorised as low readers.
9
FIGURE 1
High readers started out in the 57th percentile at age 5, and increased their position by 5
percentiles by age 10 and a further 7 percentiles by age 16, meaning that by age 16, the high
readers had caught up with the children of graduates. The least frequent readers saw a decline
of 8 percentiles over the same period.
The children of both graduates and parents with no qualifications maintained a roughly
constant position between the ages of 5 and 10. Interestingly, the children of parents with no
qualifications maintained a roughly constant position between 5 and 16, with a slight increase
in their mean rank at 10 and a slight decline by 16. However, the gap between the children of
graduates and the children of the unqualified grew during the secondary school years, as the
children of graduates improved their position.
FIGURE 2
Turning to children’s maths trajectories, we must first note that there was no maths test at age
5, so we use a non-verbal score (Copying Designs) as a baseline. The children of graduates
improve their position between 5 and 16 by 3 percentiles (as opposed to 5 for vocabulary).
The position of the children of parents with no qualifications barely changed between 5 and
16. The gap between frequent and infrequent readers remained fairly constant between 5 and
16.
In summary, these charts suggest that parents’ education and children’s reading make a
difference to children’s progress in both vocabulary and maths during the secondary school
years. However, the role of children’s reading appears to be particularly important for
vocabulary development. In the analyses to follow, we test whether these descriptive results
hold given an extensive set of control variables.
Multivariate Regression Analysis
We present a multivariate regression analysis (also known as multivariate general linear
model or multivariate response model), which treats the outcome variables jointly, and hence
uses only those cases with data for both test scores at age 16. This allows us to compare
model fit across the two outcomes, and to examine differences in the predictors across the
two outcomes. The dependent variables are treated as percentage test scores, in order to make
the coefficients interpretable as percentage point differences. We have run parallel analyses
(available on request) treating the dependent variables as standardised z scores, which did not
change the results in any substantial way.
TABLE 5
Table 5 shows the multivariate regression analysis results. In Model 1 we control only for sex
and family background. Model 1 shows no link between gender and either maths or
10
vocabulary scores. Parents’ qualifications are significantly linked to both test scores, with the
children of more highly educated parents achieving higher scores. Children who had a parent
with a degree scored around 13 percentage points higher than those whose parents had no
qualifications in both vocabulary and maths. The advantage associated with managerial and
professional occupations (‘the salariat’) compared to the manual/routine class was
considerably smaller, 4 percentage points for vocabulary and 5 for maths. Older siblings are
negative for both vocabulary and maths, with a 2 percentage point disadvantage for each
additional older sibling.
In model 1, we can also note that model fit is weak for both maths and vocabulary, but fit is
stronger in the case of vocabulary (R2 = 0.15) than for maths (0.10), suggesting that ascribed
social characteristics are more important predictors of variability in vocabulary than in maths.
In model 2, we introduce variables related to the home reading climate. How often the child
was read to at age five is significantly positive for both vocabulary and maths, with an
advantage of 1.2 and 0.9 percentage points respectively for each additional day the child was
read to in the last week. Parents’ reading books in their spare time is also significantly
positive forvocabulary (an advantage of 1.6 percentage points), but not for maths. Parents’
self-reported reading problems are significantly negative for both scores (-2.9 percentage
points for vocabulary, -4.9 for maths). Having broadsheet newspapers in the home is linked to
higher scores on both vocabulary (4.9) and maths (4.4), while tabloids in the home were not
significant compared to having no national newspapers in the home.
In this model, the coefficients for both parental education and socialsocial class are somewhat
reduced, but remain consistently statistically significant. In model 2, the improvement in
model fit is greatest for vocabulary (R2 increases from 0.15 to 0.20, compared to 0.10 to 0.12
for maths) suggesting that the home reading climate accounts for more of the variability in
vocabulary than for maths.
Model 3 introduces the child’s own reading behaviour. Book reading at 10 and 16 and
newspaper reading at 16 are powerfully linked to both vocabulary and maths. Reading often
at age 10 is linked to an advantage of 13 percentage points in vocabulary and 14 in maths.
Reading more than weekly at 16 is linked to a further advantage of 7 percentage points in
vocabulary and 3 in maths, while reading a newspaper more than weekly at age 16 is
associated with an additional 5 percentage points in vocabulary and 7 in maths. This model
also includes playing a musical instrument at 10 and 16. Playing an instrument is linked to
test scores in this model, but much less strongly than reading is. Playing an instrument at 10
is linked to an advantage of only 1 percentage point in vocabulary and 2 percentage points in
maths. Playing an instrument at 16 is not linked to a statistically significant advantage in
maths, but is linked to a 1.5 percentage point advantage in vocabulary.
Interestingly, gender becomes significant for both subjects in this model, with positive
coefficients for boys. This suggests that, while boys’ absolute performance was not different
from girls’ in either maths or vocabulary, boys performed at higher levels than girls for any
11
given level of recreational reading – in other words, boys performed as well as girls, despite
not reading as much as girls. The influence of variables reflecting the parents’ reading culture
is reduced in this model, suggesting that the influence of parents’ reading culture is somewhat
explained by the child’s own reading. However, the negative link between tabloid newspapers
and vocabulary (a 1 percentage point reduction in scores) becomes statistically significant in
this model.
Model 3 also shows a substantially improved model fit for vocabulary (from R2 = 0.2 to
0.32), with a smaller improvement for maths (0.12 to 0.18), suggesting that the child’s own
reading is most important for vocabulary development. In particular, the cohort member’s
reading at age 16 was more strongly linked to vocabulary than to mathematics attainment.
Model four introduces the cohort member’s test scores at the ages of five and ten. Principal
Components Analysis (PCA) is used to extract a single standardised Z score for cognition at
each age (5 and 10).. The inclusion of the age five and ten test scores in the model means that
it becomes a model of how far the predictions of model 3 had already been established by age
ten, and how far they continued to be reflected in changes in the child’s test scores between
age ten and age 16. Essentially it is a model of progress, with the proviso that the tests taken
at ages five and ten were not the same as those taken at 16, although vocabulary was
measured at both 5 and 10, and maths was also measured at age 10. Coefficients in this model
could be biased if measurement error in the cognitive tests at ages 5 and 10 are linked to other
variables of interest (Jerrim & Vignoles 2013). Therefore, we have attempted to minimise the
risk of spurious results due to measurement error in any given test by including the full set of
test scores at both ages in our analysis.
An increase of 1 standard deviation in cognitive scores at age 5 was associated with an
increase of 1 percentage point in vocabulary, but no significant gain for maths. The age 10
score was more highly predictive of attainment at 16, as would be expected. A one standard
deviation increase in age 10 scores was linked to gains of 9 percentage points in vocabulary
and 13 percentage points in maths. Model fit improves substantially, to R2 = 0.61 for
vocabulary and 0.49 for maths.
Many variables that were significant in model 3 become non-significant or marginally
significant in model 4 because they are linked to absolute attainment in the test scores at age
16, but not to progress between ten and 16. Parents’ education remains significant, but the
coefficient is much reduced in size (the advantage due to a parental degree falls from 8 to 2.5
percentage points for vocabulary and from 9 to 2 percentage points for maths). Social class is
non-significant in this model. The negative influence of elder siblings remains significant for
both maths and vocabulary, with a one percentage point disadvantage in progress in maths
and vocabulary for each additional elder sibling.
The influence of the home reading environment is much reduced, although broadsheet
newspapers in the home and having been read to at age 5 remain significantly positive for
vocabulary, though not for maths. Overall though, the influence of the home reading
12
environment has been largely mediated by the combination of the children’s own reading
behaviour and by their early attainment scores.
Importantly, the coefficients for cohort member’s own reading behaviour, including reading
books and newspapers, remain large and highly statistically significant in model 4. This
suggests that it is not just the case that academically able children read more, but that leisure
reading is linked to greater cognitive progress during the teenage years. This is in marked
contrast to playing a musical instrument, which becomes non-significant in this model, apart
from a borderline significant negative relationship between playing an instrument at age 10
and vocabulary development.
Taking the three variables reflecting childhood reading together, being in the top categories
for all three adds up to a gain of 10 percentage points in vocabulary and 5 percentage points
in maths. This compares to a difference associated with a parental degree of 2.5 percentage
points for vocabulary and 2 percentage points for maths. In other words, the influence of
reading on growth in academic attainment scores is substantial. Given that parental education
has typically been found to be the strongest determinant of children’s educational and
cognitive outcomes, the fact that childhood reading matters more than parental education for
cognitive progress during the secondary school years is striking.
Finally, in additional analysis (available on request) we tested whether there was an
interaction between childhood reading and parents’ education. This was to assess whether the
cognitive return to reading differed according to parents’ educational level. We found that
there was a positive interaction for reading at age 16 (but not age 10) only for vocabulary.
This differential may suggest that 16 year olds with more educated parents had access to
higher quality reading materials, but unfortunately we lack the data to test this hypothesis
directly.
Conclusions
Our initial descriptive analyses showed that differentials in vocabulary scores between the
children of graduates and the children of parents with no qualifications increased sharply
between the ages of ten and 16, but differentials for maths were stable. However, our
subsequent regression analyses showed that differentials in test scores at age 16 due to
parental social background did not vary greatly across the domains of mathematics and
vocabulary. We hypothesised that parental education would be more strongly linked to
vocabulary than to maths scores, but this was not the case. Rather, parental education was
much more strongly linked than parental social class to both vocabulary and maths scores.
This finding broadly supports Bourdieu’s emphasis on cultural resources, suggesting that they
matter more than economic resources, at least for cognitive outcomes.
The home reading culture, including reading to the child, parents’ reading books and
newspapers, and parental reading problems, was linked to children’s test scores, and this had
a role in mediating the influence of parents’ education, and also to some extent in mediating
13
parents’ social class. In order to interpret the influence of the home reading culture on
children’s outcomes, it is important to acknowledge that elements of this culture are likely to
be strongly related to one another. For example, a mother who struggles with reading is likely
to struggle to read to her child, and unlikely to read in her leisure time. Given the prevalence
of adult literacy problems in Britain, (National Audit Office 2008) this is likely to be an
important aspect of the educational disadvantage suffered by many children. Eight per cent of
the BCS70 cohort were assessed as having poor basic skills in literacy at age 34, with strong
evidence of intergenerational transmission of poor literacy and numeracy to their own
children (Bynner & Parsons 2006)
Children’s own reading behaviour was strongly linked to test scores in maths and vocabulary,
and this accounted for some of the influence of parents’ reading. Our findings support other
work suggesting that children’s leisure reading is important for educational attainment and
social mobility (Taylor 2011), and suggest that the mechanism for this is increased
development of cognitive skills. Once we controlled for the child’s test scores at age five and
ten, the influence of the child’s own reading remained large and statistically significant,
suggesting that the positive link between leisure reading and cognitive outcomes is not purely
due to more able children being more likely to read a lot, but that reading for pleasure is
actually linked to increased progress over time., We found a stronger link between reading
and progress in vocabulary development than in mathematics. This is in line with our
hypothesis, based on the grounds that books directly expose readers to new words, and
therefore reading should influence vocabulary directly, whereas the influence on attainment
in other areas would be indirect, via the fact that improved reading ability improves an
individual’s ability to learn across the whole curriculum. We expected this indirect influence
of reading on progress in mathematics to be weaker than the hypothesised direct effect on
vocabulary. The differential influence of reading on the two cognitive scores can therefore be
seen as tentatively supporting a causal interpretation of the role of reading in vocabulary
development. As a further check on the interpretation of the effect of reading, we included
playing a musical instrument in our models. While it could be argued that reading simply
proxies a particular cultural milieu within the family, or child characteristics such as diligence
or concentration, we would argue that the lack of a significant association between playing a
musical instrument (which should also proxy such characteristics) and cognitive progress
does not accord with this view. In summary, although statistical regression analyses can never
prove causality, we suggest that the combined evidence of temporal ordering and differential
effects in line with theory makes a causal interpretation of the role of reading more plausible
than the alternative explanations in this case.
From a policy perspective, our findings strongly back the need to support and encourage
children’s reading in their leisure time, especially given that the available evidence on trends
over time suggests that children’s reading for pleasure has declined in recent years (Clark &
Rumbold 2006). Reasons for this decline in reading for pleasure may include increases in
competing demands on young people’s time, including homework, organised activities and
the internet. However, there is scope for new technologies to be exploited to provide greater
access to books. Supporting young children in becoming confident readers is clearly
14
necessary but not sufficient to achieve the goal of encouraging reading for pleasure
throughout childhood and adolescence. Schools need to foster a love of reading as well as
teaching reading as a skill. In light of the decline in leisure reading between the ages of ten
and 16, our findings suggest the particular need to support teenagers’ reading. We would also
argue that supporting reading for pleasure among disadvantaged children could potentially
provide a powerful tool in closing education attainment gaps (Connelly et al. 2014). Further
research is needed on effective approaches to promoting reading for pleasure, but it seems
clear that library services both within and outside schools are necessary to promoting reading,
and sharing knowledge with individual young people about books they may enjoy, especially
for those children who do not live in homes that are lined with books. The lack of library
provision in many British schools is a cause for concern (APPG 2014). By definition, no one
can be forced to read for pleasure, but practices such as silent reading periods in school may
help to establish reading as a habit to be enjoyed.
In this paper, we have attempted to shed new light on educational inequalities and cultural
reproduction processes by examining the role of reading in attainment trajectories over time,
and the distinctive link between cultural resources and vocabulary. In future work, we intend
to assess the role of cognition in determining educational attainment and life chances,
including the question of whether vocabulary plays a distinctive role in determining
educational attainment and social reproduction and mobility.
15
Bibliography
APPG (2014) The beating heart of the school: Improving educational attainment through
school libraries and librarians (London, Libraries All Party Parliamentary Group).
Batty, G. D., Deary, I. J., Schoon, I. & Gale, C. R. (2007) Mental ability across childhood in
relation to risk factors for premature mortality in early life: the 1970 British Cohort Study,
Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 61, 977-1003.
Bourdieu, P., Passeron, J.-C. & Saint-Martin, M. (1994) Academic Discourse (Cambridge,
Polity Press).
Bourdieu, P. & Passeron, J. C. ([1977] 1990) Reproduction in education, society and culture
(London ; Beverly Hills, Sage Publications).
Breen, R. & Goldthorpe, J. H. (2001) Class, mobility and merit - The experience of two
British birth cohorts, European Sociological Review, 17(2), 81-101.
Brimer, M. A. & Dunn, L. M. (1962) English Picture Vocabulary TestEducational Evaluation
Enterprises).
Butler, B., Despotidou, S. & Shepherd, P. (1980) 1970 British Cohort Study (BCS70): Ten
Year Follow-up (London, Social Statistics Research Unit, City University).
Byford, M., Kuh, D. & Richards, M. (2012) Parenting practices and intergenerational
associations in cognitive ability, International Journal of Epidemiology, 41, 263-272.
Bynner, J. & Parsons, S. (2006) New light on literacy and numeracy (London, NRDC
Research Report).
Chan, T. W. & Goldthorpe, J. H. (2007) Social Status and Newspaper Readership, American
Journal of Sociology, 112(4), 1095-1134.
Cheung, S.-Y. & Andersen, R. (2003) Time to read: Family resources and educational
outcomes in Britain, Journal of Comparative Family Studies, 34(3), 413-433.
Chiu, M. M. & Chow, B. W. Y. (2010) Culture, motivation and reading achievement: High
school students in 41 countries, Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 579-592.
Clark, C. & De Zoysa, S. (2012) Mapping the interrelationships of reading enjoyment,
attitudes, behaviour and attainment (London, National Literacy Trust).
Clark, C. & Rumbold, K. (2006) Reading for pleasure: A research overview (London,
National Literacy Trust).
Closs, S. J. & Hutchings, M. J. (1976) APU arithmetic test (London, Hodder and Stoughton).
Connelly, R., Sullivan, A. & Jerrim, J. (2014) Primary and secondary education and poverty:
Anti poverty strategy review (London, Joseph Rowntree Foundation).
Croizet, J.-C. & Claire, T. (1998) Extending the concept of stereotype threat to social class:
The intellectual underperformance of students from low socioeconomic backgrounds,
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(6), 588-594.
Cunningham, A. E. & Stanovich, K. E. (1998) What reading does for the mind, American
Educator, 22(1-2), 8-15.
De Graaf, N. D., De Graaf, P. M. & Kraaykamp, G. (2000) Parental Cultural Capital and
Educational Attainment in the Netherlands: A Refinement of the Cultural Capital Perspective,
Sociology of Education, 73, 92-111.
Department for Education (2012) Research Evidence on Reading for Pleasure (London, DfE
http://www.education.gov.uk/schools/teachingandlearning/pedagogy/b00192950/encouragin
g-reading-for-pleasure/what-the-research-says-on-reading-for-pleasure).
Department for Education Education Standards Research Team (2012) Research Evidence
on Reading for Pleasure (London, DfE
http://www.education.gov.uk/schools/teachingandlearning/pedagogy/b00192950/encouragin
g-reading-for-pleasure/what-the-research-says-on-reading-for-pleasure).
Dodgeon, B. (2008) Guide to the Dataset: BCS70 16 year follow up: APU arithmetic test
(London, Centre for Longitudinal Studies).
Douglas, J. W. B. (1964) The Home and the School (London, MacGibbon and Kee).
Duncan, G. J., Bergman, L., Duckworth, K., Kokko, K., Lyyra, A.-L., Metzger, M., Pulkkinen,
L. & Simonton, S. (2012) The Role of Child Skills and Behaviors in the Intergenerational
16
Transmission of Inequality: A Cross-National Study, in: J. Ermisch, M. Jäntti & T. Smeeding
(Eds) From Parents to Children: The Intergenerational Transmission of Advantage (New
York, Russell Sage Foundation).
Elliott, C. D., Murray, D. J. & Pearson, L. S. (1979) British Ability Scales (Slough, NFER).
Elliott, J. & Shepherd, P. (2006) Cohort Profile: 1970 British birth cohort (BCS70),
International Journal of Epidemiology, 35(4), 836-843.
Evans, M. D. R., Kelley, J., Sikora, J. & Treiman, D. J. (2010) Family scholarly culture and
educational success: Books and schooling in 27 nations, Research in social stratification
and mobility, 28, 171-197.
Feinstein, L. (2003) Inequality in the Early Cognitive Development of British Children in the
1970 Cohort, Economica, 70(1), 73-97.
Feinstein, L. (2004) Mobility in pupils' cognitive attainment during school life, Oxford Review
of Economic Policy, 20(2), 213-229.
Fogelman, K. R. & Goldstein, H. (1976) Social Factors Associated with Changes in
Educational Attainment between 7 and 11 Years of Age, Educational Studies, 2, 95-109.
Ganzeboom, H. (1982) Explaining Differential Participation in High-Cultural Activities - A
Confrontation of Information-Processing and Status-Seeking Theories, in: W. Raub (Ed)
Theoretical Models and Empirical Analyses: Contributions to the Explanation of Individual
Actions and Collective Phenomena (Utrecht, E.S. Publications), 186-205.
Georg, W. (2004) Cultural capital and social inequality in the life course, European
Sociological Review, 20(4), 333-344.
Gipps, C. & Murphy, P. (1994) A fair test? : assessment, achievement and equity (Milton
Keynes, Open University Press).
Godfrey Thompson Unit (1978) Edinburgh Reading Test (Sevenoaks, Hodder and
Stoughton).
Golding, J. (1975) The 1970 Birth Cohort 5-Year Follow-up: Guide to the dataset (University
of Bristol, Institute of Child Health).
Goldthorpe, J. & McKnight, A. (2006) The economic basis of social class, in: S. Morgan, D.
B. Grusky & G. S. Fields (Eds) Mobility and Inequality: Frontiers of research from sociology
and economics (Stanford, Stanford University Press).
Goldthorpe, J. H. (1997) The 'Goldthorpe' class schema: some observations on conceptual
and operational issues in relation to the ESRC review of government social classifications,
in: D. Rose & K. O'Reilly (Eds) Constructing Classes: Towards a New Social Classification
for the UK (Swindon, ESRC/ONS).
Goodenough, F. L. (1926) The measurement of intelligence by drawings (New York, World
Book Company).
Halsey, A. H., Heath, A. & Ridge, J. (1980) Origins and Destinations: family, class, and
education in modern Britain (Oxford, OUP).
Harris, D. B. (1963) Children's drawings as measures of intellectual maturity (New York,
Harcourt, Brace and World).
Hart, B. & Rinsley, T. R. (1995) Meaningful differences in the everyday experiences of young
American children (Baltimore, M.D., Paul H. Brookes).
Hill, V. (2005) Through the past darkly: A review of the British Ability Scales Second
Edition, Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 10, 87-98.
Hirsch, E. D. (1983) Cultural Literacy, The American Scholar, 52(2), 159-169.
Jaeger, M. M. (2011) Does Cultural Capital Really Affect Academic Achievement? New
evidence from combined sibling and panel data, Sociology of Education, 84(4), 281-298.
Jerrim, J. & Vignoles, V. (2013) Social mobility, regression to the mean and the cognitive
development of high ability children from disadvantaged homes, Journal of the Royal
Statistical Society (Series A), 176(4), 887-906.
Lareau, A. (2003) Unequal childhoods : class, race, and family life (Berkeley, University of
California Press).
Little, R. J. A. & Rubin, D. B. (1987) Statistical analysis with incomplete data, (New York,
Wiley).
17
Mostafa, T. & Wiggins, D. (2014) Handling attrition and non-response in the 1970 British
Cohort Study, CLS Working Paper, 2014/2.
National Audit Office (2008) Skills for Life: Progress in improving adult literacy and numeracy
(London, House of Commons Public Accounts Committee).
Nisbet, J. (1953) Family Environment and Intelligence, Eugenics Review, XLV, 31-42.
Parsons, S. (2014) Childhood cognition in the 1970 British Cohort Study, CLS Data Note.
Richards, M. & Sacker, A. (2003) Lifetime antecedents of cognitive reserve, Journal of
clinical and experimental neuropsychology, 25(5), 614-624.
Ritchie, S. J., Bates, T. C. & Plomin, R. (2014 in press) Does Learning to Read Improve
Intelligence? A Longitudinal Multivariate Analysis in Identical Twins From Age 7 to 16, Child
development.
Rutter, M., Tizard, J. & Whitmore, K. (1970) Education, Health and Behaviour (London,
Longmans).
Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M. & Quinn, D. M. (1999) Stereotype threat and women's math
performance, Journal of experimental social psychology, 35(1), 4-28.
Sullivan, A. (2001) Cultural Capital and Educational Attainment, Sociology, 35(4), 893-912.
Sullivan, A. (2002) Bourdieu and Education: How Useful is Bourdieu's Theory for
Researchers?, Netherlands' Journal of Social Sciences, 38(2), 144-166.
Sullivan, A. (2007) Cultural Capital, Cultural Knowledge, and Ability, Sociological Research
Online, 12(6).
Sullivan, A., Ketende, S. & Joshi, H. (2013) Social class and inequalities in early cognitive
scores, Sociology, 47(6), 1187-1206.
Taylor, B. M., Frye, B. J. & Maruyama, G. M. (1990) Time spent reading and reading growth,
American Educational Research Journal, 27(2), 351-362.
Taylor, M. (2011) Life course outcomes of cultural practices -
instrumental benefits?, paper presented at British Sociological Association annual
conference (London School of Economics, London, England.
Twist, L., Schagan, I. & Hodgson, C. (2007) Progress in International Reading Literacy
Study (PIRLS): Reader and Reading National Report for England 2006 (Slough, NFER and
DCSF).
18
Tables and Figures
Table 1: Age 16 Arithmetic and Vocabulary Scores – Percentage points
N Min. Max. Mean Std.
Deviation
Arithmetic 3583 0 100 61.6 19.4
Vocabulary 3583 0 96 53.5 14.9
Table 2: Descriptive statistics for categorical regressors
Imputed
%
Original
N
%
missing
Child sex
Male 46.1 1653
Female 53.9 1930
Missing -0 0
Parental social class (NS-SEC)
Managerial / professional 29.9 1044
Intermediate 28.1 835
Routine and manual 38.5 1322
Not working 3.6 128
Missing -254 7.1
Parental highest qualification (Age 10)
No qualifications 27.1 972
Vocational/Other 19.1 684
O-Levels/A-levels 34.4 1113
Degree+ 19.3 692
Missing -122 3.4
Parental reading (Age 16 survey)
At least one parent reads books 88.7 2127
Missing -1040 29.0
Parental reading problems (Maternal
report - Age 16 survey)
At least one parent has reading problem 9.6 345
Missing -970 27.1
Reading materials available in home
(Age 16)
Tabloid paper 43.6 1562
Broadsheet paper 11.8 423
Missing -970 27.1
How often reads books (Age 10)
Often 54.9 1962
Sometimes 39.8 1034
Never or hardly ever 5.3 161
Missing -426 11.9
How often reads newspapers (Age 16)
Rarely or never 11.6 414
Less than once a week 7.1 256
19
Once a week 20.6 562
More than once a week 60.7 2130
Missing -221 6.2
How often reads books (Age 16)
Rarely or never 33.3 1192
Less than once a week 20.6 736
Once a week 16.9 472
More than once a week 29.2 967
Missing -216 6.0
Plays musical instrument
Age 10 46.5 1662
Missing -436
Age 16 24.0 861
Missing -249 6.9
Table 3: Descriptive statistics for continuous regressors
Imputed
mean
Original
N
%
missing
Birth order 1.8 3163 11.7
Number of days child read to in past
week (age 5) 4.5 2872 19.8
20
Table 4: Age 5 and 10 test scores – Imputed where missing
Original
NMin. Max. Mean. Std.
Deviation
%
missing
Age 5
Copying designs 2968 0 8 4.9 1.8 17.2%
English picture
vocabulary 2775 0 62 38.1 12.0 22.6%
Human figure
drawing 2931 1 22 10.5 2.8 18.2%
Complete-a-profile 2876 0 16 6.9 3.6 19.7%
Schonell graded
reading 2962 0 49 1.7 4.0 17.3%
Age 10
Edinburgh Reading
Test 2690 0 64 39.7 13.0 24.9%
Pictorial Language
Score 2904 24 100 63.0 9.6 19.0%
Friendly Maths Test 2690 3 72 45.9 11.2 24.9%
Spelling score 2880 0 56 36.3 9.7 19.6%
BAS word definitions 2675 0 29 10.8 4.7 25.3%
BAS word
similarities 2662 0 20 12.3 2.3 25.7%
BAS digit recall score 2671 7 34 22.5 3.8 25.5%
BAS Matrices 2666 0 28 16.2 4.9 25.6%
21
Table 5: Multivariate regression analysis of vocabulary and mathematics % scores (n for all models
=3, 583)
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
Maths Vocab Maths Vocab Maths Vocab Maths Vocab
Intercept 57.7** 50.9** 54.6** 46.3** 35.6** 29.6** 57.6** 46.3**
Sex (Male) 0.6 -0.1 0.8 0.2 3.4** 2.9** 0.4 0.6
Social class (Ref =
Manual/Routine)
Managerial / professional 4.8** 3.9** 3.6** 2.6** 3.2** 2.1** 0.4 -0.1
Intermediate 2.2** 2.0** 1.8* 1.5** 1.7* 1.4** 0.2 0.3
Not working 0.6 0.8 1.3 1.4 1.7 1.2 0.5 0.5
Highest parental qual (Ref=None)
Vocational/Other 3.9** 2.8** 3.4** 2.2** 3.1** 2.0** 0.9 0.3
O-Levels/A-levels 5.7** 5.6** 4.7** 4.5** 4.2** 3.9** 0.6 1.2**
Degree+ 13.2** 12.6** 10.9** 9.7** 9.4** 8.2** 2.0* 2.5**
Birthorder -2.1** -2.2** -1.8** -1.9** -1.6** -1.6** -0.8** -1.0**
Age 5 - number of days child read
to in past week 0.9** 1.2** 0.5** 0.9** -0.2 0.3**
Age 16 - Parent(s) read books 1.3 1.6* 0.5 0.7 -0.9 -0.4
Age 16 - Parental reading
problems -4.9** -2.9** -4.1** -2.2** -1.2 0.0
Age 16 - Tabloid newspaper in
home 0.2 -0.1 -0.9 -0.9* -0.3 -0.4
Age 16 - Broadsheet newspaper in
home 4.4** 4.9** 3.5** 3.7** 1.0 1.8**
Age 10 - How often reads books
(Ref = Never or hardly ever)
Often 13.9** 12.7** 2.6* 4.2**
Sometimes 10.3** 7.8** 3.4** 2.6**
Age 16 - How often reads
newspapers (Ref = Rarely or
never)
Less than once a week 3.3* 2.0* 1.5 0.5
Once a week 3.2** 1.7* 2.1* 0.8
More than once a week 6.9** 4.9** 2.8** 1.8**
Age 16 - How often reads books
(Ref = Rarely or never)
Less than once a week 2.9** 2.7** 0.7 1.0*
Once a week 1.6 3.1** 0.4 2.2**
More than once a week 3.0** 6.5** -0.4 4.0**
Age 10 - Plays musical instrument 2.3** 1.1* 0.1 -0.7
Age 16 - Plays musical instrument 1.1 1.5** -0.3 0.4
Age 5 ability score 0.5 1.0**
Age 10 ability score 12.6** 9.2**
Adjusted R20.10 0.15 0.12 0.20 0.18 0.31 0.48 0.61
22
Figure 1: Vocabulary trajectories
N=3583, data imputed where missing.
Age 5: English Picture Vocabulary; Age 10: the vocabulary subscale of the Pictorial
Language Score; Age 16: Vocabulary score.
23
Figure 2: Mathematics trajectories
N=3583, data imputed where missing.
Age 5: Copying Designs; Age 10: Friendly Maths test; Age 16: Arithmetic test.
24
Appendix Table A1: Odds ratios of Logit response models for BCS70 1986 school tests*
Gender (reference: Men)
Women 1.37*** (0.057)
Marital status (reference: Single)
Married 1.90*** (0.265)
Mother lives in London in 1970 (reference: not in London)
In London 0.60*** (0.043)
Parity (reference: 0)
1 0.83*** (0.042)
2 0.75*** (0.05)
3+ 0.55*** (0.045)
Lactation (reference: attempted)
No 0.94 (0.042)
Mother’s age at Delivery (reference: less than 20)
20-24 1.25*(0.107)
25-29 1.35*** (0.122)
30-34 1.61*** (0.163)
35 or more 1.51*** (0.187)
Mother’s age at completion of education (reference: 14 or less)
15 1.2 (0.133)
16 1.38** (0.162)
17 1.27 (0.166)
18 + 1.29 (0.166)
Father’s social class (reference: SC 1)
SC2 1 (0.104)
SC3 non-manual 1 (0.108)
SC3 manual 0.84 (0.086)
SC4 0.76*(0.087)
SC5 0.55*** (0.079)
Other 0.82 (0.116)
Father’s age at completion of education (reference: 14 or less)
15 0.99 (0.095)
16 1.04 (0.112)
17 1.31*(0.16)
18 or more 1.02 (0.116)
N15270
pseudo R20.024
Exponentiated coefficients; Standard errors in parentheses, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001
*With thanks to Tarek Mostafa. This logit response model can be compared to identical models for each wave of
BCS70 in Mostafa and Wiggins (2014).
25
26
27
i We treat ‘not working’ as a separate category as NS-SEC is not available in BCS70 prior to 1980,
so non-workers could not be categorised according to their previous NS-SEC.
... The practice engagement theory (Reder, 1994;Reder et al., 2020) suggests active everyday engagement in reading and writing to be essential for the development of reading skills. As books expose readers to new words, reading books is expected to be directly related to vocabulary and reading comprehension, whereas an indirect effect on achievement in other school subjects is also anticipated (i.e., due to the fact that enhanced reading ability strengthens an individual's capacity to learn across the entire curriculum; Sullivan & Brown, 2015). In sum, reading experience can support different components of verbal skills (see Mol & Bus, 2011, for a review) and subject-specific content knowledge (e.g., Hirsch, 2003). ...
... In sum, reading experience can support different components of verbal skills (see Mol & Bus, 2011, for a review) and subject-specific content knowledge (e.g., Hirsch, 2003). Leisure reading has been associated with greater vocabulary knowledge (e.g., Sullivan & Brown, 2015;van der Kleij et al., 2022), reading comprehension (e.g., Locher & Pfost, 2020;Pfost et al., 2013;Pfost & Heyne, 2023;Torppa et al., 2020), and mathematical skills (e.g., Sullivan & Brown, 2015). Furthermore, relations between leisure reading and empathy, fantasy, and theory of mind (Mumper & Gerrig, 2017), as well as prosocial behavior and social adjustment in adolescents from Grade 6 to Grade 9 (Lenhart et al., 2023), have been documented. ...
... In sum, reading experience can support different components of verbal skills (see Mol & Bus, 2011, for a review) and subject-specific content knowledge (e.g., Hirsch, 2003). Leisure reading has been associated with greater vocabulary knowledge (e.g., Sullivan & Brown, 2015;van der Kleij et al., 2022), reading comprehension (e.g., Locher & Pfost, 2020;Pfost et al., 2013;Pfost & Heyne, 2023;Torppa et al., 2020), and mathematical skills (e.g., Sullivan & Brown, 2015). Furthermore, relations between leisure reading and empathy, fantasy, and theory of mind (Mumper & Gerrig, 2017), as well as prosocial behavior and social adjustment in adolescents from Grade 6 to Grade 9 (Lenhart et al., 2023), have been documented. ...
Article
Number of books at home is often associated with reading, mathematics, and science skills. Number of children’s own books has rarely been considered in previous studies as a separate variable. The present study aimed to investigate relations between both of these indicators and cognitive and socio-emotional outcomes in secondary school students, while analysing mechanisms underlying relations of these indicators with outcomes. Based on cross-sectional data of a German sample of 407 secondary school students (Mage = 12.72 years, SDage = 0.38, 51.1% girls) and using multiple linear regressions, the number of books at home was found to be related to students’ cognitive skills, while the number of students’ own books was mainly associated their socio-emotional skills. Relations between the number of books, the number of students’ own books, and students’ outcomes mostly did not change when students’ and parental behaviors were considered. Thus, future studies are needed with the aim to investigate relations between the number of books and outcomes in a longitudinal manner and to explore further potential mediators behind these relations.
... There is a substantial body of international research that highlights benefits of reading for pleasure for children and their communities. Children who read for pleasure are more likely to have advanced vocabulary development (Sullivan & Brown, 2015), enhanced school performance, especially if they read fiction (Jerrim & Moss, 2019), more developed brain function (Sun, Sahakian et al., 2023), demonstrate prosocial behaviours at school (Mak & Fancourt, 2020a), fit in better through connecting with others (Wilhelm & Smith, 2016), and make healthier choices around eating and smoking (Mak & Fancourt, 2020b). While these qualities are often seen as individual benefits, they are also qualities of healthy societies along with other traits associated with reading for pleasure that are fundamental to social cohesion and communal life. ...
... Other studies have reported gendered differences in the extent and nature of boys' and girls' reading (Scholes et al., 2021), effects on reading of family socio-economic status (Sullivan & Brown, 2015) and predictable patterns across standardised categories of ethnicity (Meissel, Reese & Turnbull, 2019). ...
Research
Full-text available
The study finds that reading with others and discussing their reading boosts children's resilience and wellbeing, especially during difficult times like a pandemic. Older children who had experiences of shared reading with whānau and friends reported greater resilience during lockdown. The study examined data from the Growing Up in New Zealand (GUiNZ) study and focus groups of children of similar ages. This allowed researchers to investigate how reading experiences from birth affected wellbeing at age 11 during lockdown. Children who engaged more with reading, read with others, and had positive attitudes towards reading reported better health and wellbeing during lockdown. Prior positive reading experiences did not reduce depression and anxiety scores, although there was a reduction in depression scores when more reading together with others occurred in bubbles during lockdown. However, the benefits of reading are not equally accessible to all children. High-income households and European ethnicities are more likely to have beneficial reading experiences and access to more resources for reading. Early reading experiences were prevalent for those making more positive wellbeing reports during lockdown, even when adjusted for income and ethnicity. This points to the need for equalising children’s access to the enjoyment and beneficial outcomes of reading for pleasure through resourcing and opportunity. Public resources like libraries and schools can help bridge these gaps, and whānau need access to reading materials that are culturally relevant for their children.
... For preservice teachers, this means that enhancing mathematical literacy skills involves integrating knowledge from various disciplines-such as history, geography, economics, biology, agriculture, culinary arts, and social studies-with the prerequisite knowledge and skills from mathematics classes (McCrone & Dossey, 2007;Steen et al., 2007). Central to this interdisciplinary approach is the role of reading skills, which are pivotal in acquiring the necessary background knowledge for success in mathematics and other academic subjects (Erbeli et al., 2021;Reinke et al., 1997;Sullivan & Brown, 2015). These insights underscore the potential of an interdisciplinary approach to fostering mathematical literacy, with a RC playing a substantial role in the acquisition and development of mathematical literacy skills. ...
... These findings coincide with the conclusion by Arofah and Ningsi (2021) that an inclination towards learning mathematics is positively influenced by an increase in reading interest and study habits. Additionally, Reinke et al., (1997) and Sullivan and Brown (2015) state that reading skills play a crucial role in achieving success in mathematics, and that children's reading behaviour is closely tied to their mathematics test scores. Therefore, the responsibility of fostering a robust RC in schools rests on teachers who have the authority to cultivate such an environment (Aramide, 2023). ...
... The benefits of RfP have been highlighted in the contemporary literature, revealing its key role in educational and personal development Mak & Fancourt, 2020;Vanden Dool & Simpson, 2021). Consistent engagement with pleasurable reading has been shown to enhance not only literacy-based skills, but also social skills, cultural awareness, and empathy for others Ivey & Johnston, 2024;Mak & Fancourt, 2020;Sullivan & Brown, 2015). RfP also promotes action against the Matthew Effect, which posits that differences in reading level are accumulated and widened over time (Stanovich, 2009). ...
Article
Full-text available
Proficiency in literacy provides an essential foundation for citizens to participate fully and effectively in society. Research highlights a strong correlation between frequent reading and strong literacy outcomes for students and emphasises the benefits of a Reading for Pleasure (RfP) pedagogy on student reading motivation and attainment. As teachers are responsible for students’ literate development, the ability to teach literacy and the development of a literate identity thus form integral components of initial teacher education. There are studies revealing the important connection between teachers’ literate identities and their students’ reading engagement and literacy outcomes; however, less is known about the formation of pre-service teachers’ literate identities. This case study sought to investigate the impact of pre-service teacher engagement with Reading for Pleasure practices as they reflected on their literate identities. During the study, four student teachers in their final year of a Bachelor of Education (Primary) degree in an urban university in NSW, Australia, met weekly for six weeks to engage with Reading for Pleasure practices. Data were collected through participant observation notes, focus group discussions, and a questionnaire completed at the start and the end of the study. This study was informed with a consideration of complexity theory, and an analysis was made of participant’s reflections on RfP, as expressed through the intersecting systems within which initial teacher education is situated. Framed by the understanding that knowledge is socially constructed, this paper outlines how participants found benefit in Reading for Pleasure in forming their literate identities.
... Clark and Foster (2005) survey a large sample of primary and secondary graders in England and provide reasons for reading, including fun, increased knowledge, and life skills. Sullivan and Brown (2015) mention that adolescents get cognitive benefits from leisure reading, including academic achievement in vocabulary. Panero et al. (2016) find that reading fiction materials is associated with the enhancement of empathy among schoolchildren. ...
Article
Full-text available
This review delves into the intricate interplay between family dynamics and children's reading proficiency and intellectual growth. In recent decades, researchers have increasingly recognized the pivotal role of familial environments in shaping children's cognitive development, particularly in the realm of literacy acquisition. Drawing upon a wide array of scholarly literature, this paper synthesizes existing research to elucidate the multifaceted influences exerted by family-related factors on children's reading skills and overall intellectual advancement.The review identifies and examines various dimensions of family life that impact children's reading abilities, encompassing parental involvement in literacy activities, the quality of parent-child interactions, socioeconomic status, family structure, parental education level, and cultural factors. Additionally, it scrutinizes the significance of home literacy environments, including access to books, reading materials, and conducive learning settings, in fostering children's language acquisition and cognitive development.
Article
Full-text available
El artículo examina las habilidades de literacidad en estudiantes de la Universidad de Guadalajara de dos programas académicos: Licenciatura en Letras Hispánicas y Administración Gubernamental y Políticas Públicas. A través de la “prueba de entrada y salida”, se recopilaron datos de estudiantes de una universidad pública que cursaban el primer o segundo semestre. Se utilizó un cuestionario previamente validado y adaptado para obtener información detallada sobre las capacidades de resumen, lectura crítica y argumentación de los participantes. La muestra estuvo compuesta por 44 estudiantes. El cuestionario constaba de veinte ítems distribuidos en tres secciones principales. Los estudiantes leyeron un artículo seleccionado y luego, debían elaborar un resumen, identificar la tesis del autor y presentar una argumentación. La evaluación de sus habilidades de literacidad fue realizada por un experto en la materia, quien utilizó una rúbrica integrada en el instrumento de diagnóstico. Los resultados indicaron que los estudiantes de Letras Hispánicas exhibieron una mayor habilidad para la argumentación, mientras que aquellos de Administración Gubernamental y Políticas Públicas demostraron mayor destreza en la elaboración de resúmenes. Sin embargo, ninguno de los grupos fue capaz de identificar correctamente la tesis del artículo ni de producir un texto coherente y cohesivo acorde al nivel. Estos hallazgos sugieren una mejora en la enseñanza de habilidades de literacidad dentro de ambos programas académicos.
Article
Purpose To become good at anything we need to practice. However, in the classroom context, much of the reading time is devoted to instruction rather than practice. Reading practice should be daily. Text choice, motivation and reading for pleasure all interact to support implicit learning. Design/methodology/approach Review and synthesise the literature discussing independent reading in the primary classroom. Findings The more a student reads, the more their reading improves. Independent reading promotes implicit learning. Outcomes of independent reading include motivation to read more, expanded vocabulary, grammar and spelling knowledge, the ability to read fluently, improved comprehension skills, increased general knowledge and overall academic achievement. Writing and oral language skills can also improve if opportunities are created for talking about books and independent reading is used as inspiration and models for writing. Research limitations/implications The research is based on existing literature. The limit of 3,000 words restricts the depth of the paper. Practical implications The finding supports teachers to include independent reading in their daily timetable. Suggestions for establishing independent reading are provided at the end of the paper. Social implications Engagement in reading “acts as a mediator of gender and socio-economic status and… can help leverage social change” (OECD, 2021, p. 28). Independent reading benefits students in multiple ways and helps close the gap between those who have access to books at home and those who do not. Originality/value A great deal of emphasis is placed on extrinsic reading motivation and test scores. Independent reading provides the opportunity for growth and development in all areas of literacy learning through implicit learning and enjoyment.
Preprint
Full-text available
Izveštaj je rezultat pilot istraživanja kulture razvoja čitalačkih navika u porodici, koje je provedeno u periodu od oktobar-novembar 2024., u okviru istoimenog projekta Fondacije POINT. Pilot istraživanje je obavljeno kao kvantitativno i obuhvatilo je 1453 roditelja dece predškolskog uzrasta iz preko 10 gradova u Srbiji. U fokusu istraživanja bile su postojeće prakse i stavovi roditelja prema čitanju sa svojom decom / deci uzrasta do šest godina.
Article
Passive constructions in English ( be+past participle ) and French ( être+past participle ) were inventoried on a text-per-text basis in a bi-directional French/English corpus. The corpus consisted of 70 (35 × 2) original source texts in each language, 70 (35 × 2) target texts produced by human translators, and the same number of target texts obtained by machine translation with DeepL Pro. Passive constructions were found to be about 70% more frequent in original English than in original French. In the corpora of translated texts, no large disparities were observed, with the exception of French texts translated from English by DeepL, which contained around 40% more passive constructions than original French texts. Correlations between frequencies of analogous passive constructions in pairs of source and target texts were statistically significant and stronger for machine translations than for human translations. Manual analysis of random samples of source/target pairs showed less regular correspondence between the two analogous constructions as counterparts for one another, and greater diversity among alternative translation counterparts in human translation compared with machine translation.
Article
Full-text available
This paper explores the role of the family-in particular family background, family structure and social and cultural resources-in explaining educational achievement in Britain. We use extensive panel data to track the relationship between family, characteristics and factors associated with educational attainment for individuals from birth to age 33. Our results show that although social class is an important predictor of academic ability and educational outcomes, cultural and social resources in the family also play an independent role. Social resources in the family have direct and lasting positive effects on academic success and also play an indirect role by affecting childhood reading habits, which in turn affect educational success. Parental cultural practices are less important to early academic success, though they are positively related to the eventual likelihood of obtaining a university degree. In contrast,- family size and structure are related to academic achievement early in life but have no direct effect on final educational outcomes. We conclude that the role of cultural and social resources in the family should not be dismissed when attempting to explain educational outcomes.
Article
Full-text available
Bourdieu’s theory of cultural reproduction has been highly influential within the sociology of education. This paper will provide a critical introduction to Bourdieu’s theory regarding the cultural reproduction of educational advantage, and an overview of the empirical literature on cultural reproduction. It will be argued that the ‘grand theory’ of cultural reproduction is unhelpful. On the other hand, the concept of cultural capital, though ill-defined, has proved useful for empirical researchers.
Article
Full-text available
Research emphasising the importance of parenting behaviours and aspirations for child outcomes has been seized on by policymakers to suggest the responsibility of the worst off themselves for low levels of social mobility. This article provides a critique of the way in which research evidence has been used to support the dominant policy discourse in this area, as well as an empirical analysis. We use the Millennium Cohort Study to interrogate the relationship between social class and attainment in the early years of schooling. We investigate the extent to which social class inequalities in early cognitive scores can be accounted for by parental education, income, family social resources and parental behaviours. We conclude that social class remains an important concept for both researchers and policymakers, and that the link between structural inequalities and inequalities in children's cognitive scores cannot be readily accounted for in terms of individual parenting behaviours.
Article
Full-text available
Evidence from twin studies points to substantial environmental influences on intelligence, but the specifics of this influence are unclear. This study examined one developmental process that potentially causes intelligence differences: learning to read. In 1,890 twin pairs tested at 7, 9, 10, 12, and 16 years, a cross-lagged monozygotic-differences design was used to test for associations of earlier within-pair reading ability differences with subsequent intelligence differences. The results showed several such associations, which were not explained by differences in reading exposure and were not restricted to verbal cognitive domains. The study highlights the potentially important influence of reading ability, driven by the nonshared environment, on intellectual development and raises theoretical questions about the mechanism of this influence.
Article
The 1970 British Cohort Study (BCS70) is a continuing multi-purpose, multidisciplinary longitudinal study based on a sample of over 17,000 babies born in England, Wales and Scotland in 1970. The study has collected detailed information from the cohort members on various aspects of their lives, including their family circumstances at birth, education, employment, housing and partnership histories. There have been nine sweeps of data collection so far: at birth and at ages 5, 10, 16, 26, 30, 34, 38 and most recently age 42 (2012). This paper studies the extent of attrition in BCS70 and how it affects sample composition over time. We examine the determinants of response then construct inverse probability weights. In the last section, we use a simulation study to illustrate the effectiveness of weights and imputations in dealing with unit non-response and item missingness respectively. Our findings show that when the predictive power of the response models is weak, the efficacy of non-response weights is undermined. Further, multiple imputations are effective in reducing the bias resulting from item missingness when the magnitude of the bias is high and the imputation models are well specified.
Book
Class does make a difference in the lives and futures of American children. Drawing on in-depth observations of black and white middle-class, working-class, and poor families, Unequal Childhoods explores this fact, offering a picture of childhood today. Here are the frenetic families managing their children's hectic schedules of "leisure" activities; and here are families with plenty of time but little economic security. Lareau shows how middle-class parents, whether black or white, engage in a process of "concerted cultivation" designed to draw out children's talents and skills, while working-class and poor families rely on "the accomplishment of natural growth," in which a child's development unfolds spontaneously—as long as basic comfort, food, and shelter are provided. Each of these approaches to childrearing brings its own benefits and its own drawbacks. In identifying and analyzing differences between the two, Lareau demonstrates the power, and limits, of social class in shaping the lives of America's children. The first edition of Unequal Childhoods was an instant classic, portraying in riveting detail the unexpected ways in which social class influences parenting in white and African American families. A decade later, Annette Lareau has revisited the same families and interviewed the original subjects to examine the impact of social class in the transition to adulthood.
Article
. A gap in cognitive skill between richer and poorer children is evident from a very early age. Some studies have also suggested that highly able children from disadvantaged homes are overtaken by their rich but less able peers before the age of 10 years, in terms of their cognitive skill. This finding has become a widely cited ‘fact’ within the academic literature and has had a major influence on public policy and political debate. We show that this finding is vulnerable to a spurious statistical artefact known as regression to the mean and we propose the application of an alternative methodology to address this problem. After applying some simple adjustments for regression to the mean to data from the Millennium Cohort Study, we no longer find convincing evidence that able but disadvantaged pupils fall behind their more advantaged but less able peers.