Access to this full-text is provided by Frontiers.
Content available from Frontiers in Psychology
This content is subject to copyright.
MINI REVIEW
published: 02 April 2015
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00368
Edited by:
Gregory R. Maio,
Cardiff University, UK
Reviewed by:
Netta Weinstein,
University of Essex, UK
Ian Stephen,
Macquarie University, Australia
*Correspondence:
Andrew J. Elliot,
Department of Clinical and Social
Sciences in Psychology, University of
Rochester, Intercampus Drive,
Rochester, NY 14627, USA
andye@psych.rochester.edu
Specialty section:
This article was submitted to
Personality and Social Psychology, a
section of the journal Frontiers in
Psychology
Received: 25 November 2014
Accepted: 16 March 2015
Published: 02 April 2015
Citation:
Elliot AJ (2015) Color and
psychological functioning: a review of
theoretical and empirical work.
Front. Psychol. 6:368.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00368
Color and psychological functioning:
a review of theoretical and empirical
work
Andrew J. Elliot*
Department of Clinical and Social Sciences in Psychology, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA
In the past decade there has been increased interest in research on color and
psychological functioning. Important advances have been made in theoretical work and
empirical work, but there are also important weaknesses in both areas that must be
addressed for the literature to continue to develop apace. In this article, I provide brief
theoretical and empirical reviews of research in this area, in each instance beginning with
a historical background and recent advancements, and proceeding to an evaluation
focused on weaknesses that provide guidelines for future research. I conclude by
reiterating that the literature on color and psychological functioning is at a nascent stage
of development, and by recommending patience and prudence regarding conclusions
about theory, findings, and real-world application.
Keywords: color, psychological functioning, hue, lightness, chroma
The past decade has seen enhanced interest in research in the area of color and psychological
functioning. Progress has been made on both theoretical and empirical fronts, but there are also
weaknesses on both of these fronts that must be attended to for this research area to continue to
make progress. In the following, I briefly review both advances and weaknesses in the literature on
color and psychological functioning.
Theoretical Work
Background and Recent Developments
Color has fascinated scholars for millennia (Sloane, 1991;Gage, 1993). Theorizing on color and
psychological functioning has been present since Goethe (1810) penned his Theory of Colors, in
which he linked color categories (e.g., the “plus” colors of yellow, red–yellow, yellow–red) to emo-
tional responding (e.g., warmth, excitement). Goldstein (1942) expanded on Goethe’s intuitions,
positing that certain colors (e.g., red, yellow) produce systematic physiological reactions manifest in
emotional experience (e.g., negative arousal), cognitive orientation (e.g., outward focus), and overt
action (e.g., forceful behavior). Subsequent theorizing derived from Goldstein’s ideas has focused
on wavelength, positing that longer wavelength colors feel arousing or warm, whereas shorter wave-
length colors feel relaxing or cool (Nakashian, 1964;Crowley, 1993). Other conceptual statements
about color and psychological functioning have focused on general associations that people have
to colors and their corresponding influence on downstream affect, cognition, and behavior (e.g.,
black is associated with aggression and elicits aggressive behavior; Frank and Gilovich, 1988;Soldat
et al., 1997). Finally, much writing on color and psychological functioning has been completely
atheoretical, focused exclusively on finding answers to applied questions (e.g., “What wall color
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 1April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 368
Elliot Color and psychological functioning
facilitates worker alertness and productivity?”). The aforemen-
tioned theories and conceptual statements continue to motivate
research on color and psychological functioning. However, sev-
eral other promising theoretical frameworks have also emerged
in the past decade, and I review these frameworks in the
following.
Hill and Barton (2005) noted that in many non-human
animals, including primate species, dominance in aggressive
encounters (i.e., superior physical condition) is signaled by the
bright red of oxygenated blood visible on highly vascularized bare
skin. Artificial red (e.g., on leg bands) has likewise been shown to
signal dominance in non-human animals, mimicking the natural
physiological process (Cuthill et al., 1997). In humans in aggres-
sive encounters, a testosterone surge produces visible reddening
on the face and fear leads to pallor (Drummond and Quay, 2001;
Levenson, 2003). Hill and Barton (2005) posited that the parallel
between humans and non-humans present at the physiological
level may extend to artificial stimuli, such that wearing red in
sport contests may convey dominance and lead to a competitive
advantage.
Other theorists have also utilized a comparative approach in
positing links between skin coloration and the evaluation of con-
specifics. Changizi et al. (2006)andChangizi (2009)contend
that trichromatic vision evolved to enable primates, including
humans, to detect subtle changes in blood flow beneath the skin
that carry important information about the emotional state of
the conspecific. Increased red can convey anger, embarrassment,
or sexual arousal, whereas increased bluish or greenish tint can
convey illness or poor physiological condition. Thus, visual sensi-
tivity to these color modulations facilitates various forms of social
interaction. In similar fashion, Stephen et al. (2009)andStephen
and McKeegan (2010) propose that perceivers use information
about skin coloration (perhaps particularly from the face, Ta n
and Stephen, 2012) to make inferences about the attractiveness,
health, and dominance of conspecifics. Redness (from blood oxy-
genization) and yellowness (from carotenoids) are both seen as
facilitating positive judgments. Fink et al. (2006)andFink and
Matts (2007) posit that the homogeneity of skin coloration is an
important factor in evaluating the age, attractiveness, and health
of faces.
Elliot and Maier (2012) have proposed color-in-context the-
ory, which draws on social learning, as well as biology. Some
responses to color stimuli are presumed to be solely due to the
repeated pairing of color and particular concepts, messages, and
experiences. Others, however, are presumed to represent a bio-
logically engrained predisposition that is reinforced and shaped
by social learning. Through this social learning, color associ-
ations can be extended beyond natural bodily processes (e.g.,
blood flow modulations) to objects in close proximity to the body
(e.g., clothes, accessories). Thus, for example, red may not only
increase attractiveness evaluations when viewed on the face, but
also when viewed on a shirt or dress. As implied by the name of
the theory, the physical and psychological context in which color
is perceived is thought to influence its meaning and, accordingly,
responses to it. Thus, blue on a ribbon is positive (indicating first
place), but blue on a piece of meat is negative (indicating rot-
ten), and a red shirt may enhance the attractiveness of a potential
mate (red =sex/romance), but not of a person evaluating one’s
competence (red =failure/danger).
Meier and Robinson (2005)andMeier (in press)haveposited
a conceptual metaphor theory of color. From this perspective,
people talk and think about abstract concepts in concrete terms
grounded in perceptual experience (i.e., they use metaphors) to
help them understand and navigate their social world (Lakoff
and Johnson, 1999). Thus, anger entails reddening of the face,
so anger is metaphorically described as “seeing red,” and pos-
itive emotions and experiences are often depicted in terms of
lightness (rather than darkness), so lightness is metaphorically
linked to good (“seeing the light”) rather than bad (“in the dark”).
These metaphoric associations are presumed to have implications
for important outcomes such as morality judgments (e.g., white
things are viewed as pure) and stereotyping (e.g., dark faces are
viewed more negatively).
For many years it has been known that light directly influ-
ences physiology and increases arousal (see Cajochen, 2007,for
a review), but recently theorists have posited that such effects
are wavelength dependent. Blue light, in particular, is posited
to activate the melanopsin photoreceptor system which, in turn,
activates the brain structures involved in sub-cortical arousal
and higher-order attentional processing (Cajochen et al., 2005;
Lockley et al., 2006). As such, exposure to blue light is expected to
facilitate alertness and enhance performance on tasks requiring
sustained attention.
Evaluation and Recommendations
Drawing on recent theorizing in evolutionary psychology,
emotion science, retinal physiology, person perception, and
social cognition, the aforementioned conceptualizations repre-
sent important advances to the literature on color and psycho-
logical functioning. Nevertheless, theory in this area remains at a
nascent level of development, and the following weaknesses may
be identified.
First, the focus of theoretical work in this area is either
extremely specific or extremely general. A precise conceptual
proposition such as red signals dominance and leads to compet-
itive advantage in sports (Hill and Barton, 2005)isvaluablein
that it can be directly translated into a clear, testable hypothe-
sis; however, it is not clear how this specific hypothesis connects
to a broader understanding of color–performance relations in
achievement settings more generally. On the other end of the
spectrum, a general conceptualization such as color-in-context
theory (Elliot and Maier, 2012)isvaluableinthatitofferssev-
eral widely applicable premises; however, these premises are
only vaguely suggestive of precise hypotheses in specific con-
texts. What is needed are mid-level theoretical frameworks that
comprehensively, yet precisely explain and predict links between
color and psychological functioning in specific contexts (for
emerging developments, see Pazda and Greitemeyer, in press;
Spence, in press;Stephen and Perrett, in press).
Second, the extant theoretical work is limited in scope in
terms of range of hues, range of color properties, and direction
of influence. Most theorizing has focused on one hue, red, which
is understandable given its prominence in nature, on the body,
and in society (Changizi, 2009;Elliot and Maier, 2014); however,
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 2April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 368
Elliot Color and psychological functioning
other hues also carry important associations that undoubtedly
have downstream effects (e.g., blue: Labrecque and Milne, 2012;
green: Akers et al., 2012). Color has three basic properties: hue,
lightness, and chroma (Fairchild, 2013).Variationinanyorall
of these properties could influence downstream affect, cogni-
tion, or behavior, yet only hue is considered in most theorizing
(most likely because experientially, it is the most salient color
property). Lightness and chroma also undoubtedly have impli-
cations for psychological functioning (e.g., lightness: Kareklas
et al., 2014; chroma: Lee et al., 2013); lightness has received
some attention within conceptual metaphor theory (Meier, in
press;seealsoPrado-León and Rosales-Cinco, 2011), but chroma
has been almost entirely overlooked, as has the issue of combi-
nations of hue, lightness, and chroma. Finally, most theorizing
has focused on color as an independent variable rather than a
dependent variable; however, it is also likely that many situa-
tional and intrapersonal factors influence color perception (e.g.,
situational: Bubl et al., 2009; intrapersonal: Fetterman et al.,
2015).
Third, theorizing to date has focused primarily on main
effects, with only a modicum of attention allocated to the impor-
tant issue of moderation. As research literatures develop and
mature, they progress from a sole focus on “is” questions (“Does
X influence Y?”) to additionally considering “when” questions
(“Under what conditions does X influence Y and under what
conditions does X not influence Y?”). These “second generation”
questions (Zanna and Fazio, 1982, p. 283) can seem less excit-
ing and even deflating in that they posit boundary conditions
that constrain the generalizability of an effect. Nevertheless, this
step is invaluable in that it adds conceptual precision and clar-
ity, and begins to address the issue of real-world applicability. All
color effects undoubtedly depend on certain conditions – culture,
gender, age, type of task, variant of color, etc. – and acquiring
an understanding of these conditions will represent an impor-
tant marker of maturity for this literature (for movement in this
direction, see Schwarz and Singer, 2013;Tracy and Beall, 2014;
Bertrams et al., 2015;Buechner et al., in press;Young, in press).
Another, more succinct, way to state this third weakness is that
theorizing in this area needs to take context, in all its forms, more
seriously.
Empirical Work
Background and Recent Developments
Empirical work on color and psychological functioning dates
back to the late 19th century (Féré, 1887;seePressey, 1921,
for a review). A consistent feature of this work, from its incep-
tion to the past decade, is that it has been fraught with major
methodological problems that have precluded rigorous testing
and clear interpretation (O’Connor, 2011). One problem has
been a failure to attend to rudimentary scientific procedures
such as experimenter blindness to condition, identifying, and
excluding color deficient participants, and standardizing the
duration of color presentation or exposure. Another problem
has been a failure to specify and control for color at the spec-
tral level in manipulations. Without such specification, it is
impossible to know what precise combination of color proper-
ties was investigated, and without such control, the confounding
of focal and non-focal color properties is inevitable (Whitfield
and Wiltshire, 1990;Valdez and Mehrabian, 1994). Yet another
problem has been the use of underpowered samples. This prob-
lem, shared across scientific disciplines (Maxwell, 2004), can
lead to Type I errors, Type II errors, and inflated effect sizes
(Fraley and Vazire, 2014;Murayama et al., 2014). Together, these
methodological problems have greatly hampered progress in this
area.
Although some of the aforementioned problems remain (see
“Evaluation and Recommendations” below), others have been
rectified in recent work. This, coupled with advances in theory
development, has led to a surge in empirical activity. In the fol-
lowing, I review the diverse areas in which color work has been
conducted in the past decade, and the findings that have emerged.
Space considerations require me to constrain this review to a
brief mention of central findings within each area. I focus on
findings with humans (for reviews of research with non-human
animals, see Higham and Winters, in press;Setchell, in press)that
have been obtained in multiple (at least five) independent labs.
Tab l e 1 provides a summary, as well as representative examples
and specific references.
In research on color and selective attention, red stimuli have
been shown to receive an attentional advantage (see Folk, in
press, for a review). Research on color and alertness has shown
that blue light increases subjective alertness and performance on
attention-based tasks (see Chellappa et al., 2011,forareview).
Studies on color and athletic performance have linked wearing
red to better performance and perceived performance in sport
competitions and tasks (see Maier et al., in press,forareview).
In research on color and intellectual performance, viewing red
prior to a challenging cognitive task has been shown to under-
mine performance (see Shi et al., 2015, for a review). Research
focused on color and aggressiveness/dominance evaluation has
shown that viewing red on self or other increases appraisals of
aggressiveness and dominance (see Krenn, 2014,forareview).
Empirical work on color and avoidance motivation has linked
viewing red in achievement contexts to increased caution and
avoidance (see Elliot and Maier, 2014,forareview).Inresearch
on color and attraction, viewing red on or near a female has been
shown to enhance attraction in heterosexual males (see Pazda
and Greitemeyer, in press, for a review). Research on color and
store/company evaluation has shown that blue on stores/logos
increases quality and trustworthiness appraisals (see Labrecque
and Milne, 2012, for a review). Finally, empirical work on color
and eating/drinking has shown that red influences food and bev-
erage perception and consumption (see Spence, in press,fora
review).
Evaluation and Recommendations
The aforementioned findings represent important contributions
to the literature on color and psychological functioning, and
highlight the multidisciplinary nature of research in this area.
Nevertheless, much like the extant theoretical work, the extant
empirical work remains at a nascent level of development, due, in
part, to the following weaknesses.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 3April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 368
Elliot Color and psychological functioning
TABLE 1 | Research on color and psychological functioning.
Area of research Central finding Example References
Color and Selective Attention Red stimuli have been shown to receive an
attentional advantage
Participants’ visual search times were faster
for desaturated red (relative to several other
colored) targets
Lindsay et al., 2010;Tchernikov and Fallah, 2010;
Buechner et al., 2014;Pomerleau et al., 2014;Sokolik
et al., 2014 (cf. Becker et al., 2014;Folk, in press)
Color and Alertness Blue light has been shown to increase
subjective alertness and performance on
attention-based tasks
Participants exposed to blue (relative to
yellow) illumination reported greater mental
alertness
Lockley et al., 2006;Lehrl et al., 2007;Viola et al.,
2008;Cajochen et al., 2011;Taillard et al., 2012 (cf.
Vandewalle et al., 2007;Sahin and Figuerio, 2013)
Color and Athletic Performance Wearing red has been shown to enhance
performance and perceived performance in
sport competitions and tasks
Tae kwon do competitors wearing red
outperformed those wearing blue
Hill and Barton, 2005;Hagemann et al., 2008;Ilie et al.,
2008;Greenlees et al., 2013;Sorokowski et al., 2014
(cf. Caldwell and Burger, 2011;Garcia-Rubio et al.,
2011)
Color and Intellectual Performance Viewing red prior to a challenging cognitive
task has been shown to undermine
performance
Participants who viewed red (relative to
green or gray) on an intelligence test cover
performed worse on the test
Elliot et al., 2007;Gnambs et al., 2010;Zhang and Han,
2014;Shi et al., 2015;Thorstenson, in press (cf.
Yamazaki, 2010;Smajic et al., 2014)
Color and Aggressiveness/Dominance
Evaluation
Viewing red on self or other has been
shown to increase appraisals of
aggressiveness/dominance
Participants rated males wearing red
(relative to other chromatic colors) as more
dominant
Greenlees et al., 2008;Little and Hill, 2007;Feltman
and Elliot, 2011;Stephen et al., 2012a;Aiken and
Pascal, 2013 (cf. Sorokowski and Szmajke, 2007;
Furley et al., 2012)
Color and Avoidance Motivation Viewing red in achievement contexts has
been shown to increase caution and
avoidance
Participants who viewed red (relative to
green or gray) prior to an ostensible
intelligence test evidenced greater right
(versus left) frontal cortical activation
Elliot et al., 2007;Mehta and Zhu, 2009;Rutchick et al.,
2010;Tanaka and Tokuno, 2011;TenVeldenetal.,
2012 (cf. Elwood and Bode, 2014;Steele, 2014)
Color and Attraction Viewing red on or near a female has been
shown to increase attraction in
heterosexual males
Heterosexual males rated females wearing
red (relative to other chromatic colors) as
more attractive
Elliot and Niesta, 2008;Roberts et al., 2010;Stephen
and McKeegan, 2010;Guéguen and Jacob, 2014;Lin,
2014 (cf. Lynn et al., in press;Stephen et al., 2012b)
Color and Store/Company Evaluation Blue stores/logos have been shown to
increase quality and trustworthiness
appraisals
Participants rated websites featuring blue
(relative to green) as more trustworthy
Yüksel, 2009;Lee and Rao, 2010;Alberts and van der
Geest, 2011;Labrecque and Milne, 2012;Ridgway
and Myers, 2014 (cf. Barli et al., 2006;Chebat and
Morrin, 2007)
Color and Eating/Drinking Red has been shown to influence food and
beverage perception and consumption
Participants ate less chocolate chips from a
red (relative to blue or white) plate
Ross et al., 2008;Genschow et al., 2012;Guéguen,
2012;Bruno et al., 2013;Spence et al., 2014 (cf.
Piqueras-Fiszman et al., 2012;Van Ittersum and
Wansink, 2012)
The review of findings was restricted to those that have been supported by a minimum of five independent laboratories. The references are to representative articles within each area of research; articles with supportive
findings area listed first, followed by articles with non-supportive findings (indicated by cf.).
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 4April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 368
Elliot Color and psychological functioning
First, although in some research in this area color proper-
ties are controlled for at the spectral level, in most research it
(still) is not. Color control is typically done improperly at the
device (rather than the spectral) level, is impossible to imple-
ment (e.g., in web-based platform studies), or is ignored alto-
gether. Color control is admittedly difficult, as it requires tech-
nical equipment for color assessment and presentation, as well
as the expertise to use it. Nevertheless, careful color control is
essential if systematic scientific work is to be conducted in this
area. Findings from uncontrolled research can be informative in
initial explorations of color hypotheses, but such work is inher-
ently fraught with interpretational ambiguity (Whitfield and
Wiltshire, 1990;Elliot and Maier, 2014)thatmustbesubsequently
addressed.
Second, color perception is not only a function of lightness,
chroma, and hue, but also of factors such as viewing distance
and angle, amount and type of ambient light, and presence of
other colors in the immediate background and general envi-
ronmental surround (Hunt and Pointer, 2011;Brainard and
Radonji´
c, 2014;Fairchild, 2015). In basic color science research
(e.g., on color physics, color physiology, color appearance mod-
eling, etcetera; see Gegenfurtner and Ennis, in press;Johnson,
in press;Stockman and Brainard, in press), these factors are
carefully specified and controlled for in order to establish stan-
dardized participant viewing conditions. These factors have been
largely ignored and allowed to vary in research on color and psy-
chological functioning, with unknown consequences. An impor-
tant next step for research in this area is to move to incor-
porate these more rigorous standardization procedures widely
utilized by basic color scientists. With regard to both this and the
aforementioned weakness, it should be acknowledged that exact
and complete control is not actually possible in color research,
given the multitude of factors that influence color perception
(Committee on Colorimetry of the Optical Society of America,
1953) and our current level of knowledge about and ability to
control them (Fairchild, 2015). As such, the standard that must
be embraced and used as a guideline in this work is to control
color properties and viewing conditions to the extent possible
given current technology, and to keep up with advances in the
field that will increasingly afford more precise and efficient color
management.
Third, although in some research in this area, large, fully
powered samples are used, much of the research remains
underpowered. This is a problem in general, but it is partic-
ularly a problem when the initial demonstration of an effect
is underpowered (e.g., Elliot and Niesta, 2008), because ini-
tial work is often used as a guide for determining sample size
in subsequent work (both heuristically and via power analy-
sis). Underpowered samples commonly produce overestimated
effect size estimates (Ioannidis, 2008), and basing subsequent
sample sizes on such estimates simply perpetuates the prob-
lem. Small sample sizes can also lead researchers to prema-
turely conclude that a hypothesis is disconfirmed, overlooking a
potentially important advance (Murayama et al., 2014). Findings
from small sampled studies should be considered preliminary;
running large sampled studies with carefully controlled color
stimuli is essential if a robust scientific literature is to be devel-
oped. Furthermore, as the “evidentiary value movement” (Finkel
et al., 2015) makes inroads in the empirical sciences, color
scientists would do well to be at the leading edge of imple-
menting such rigorous practices as publically archiving research
materials and data, designating exploratory from confirmatory
analyses, supplementing or even replacing significant testing
with “new statistics” (Cumming, 2014), and even preregistering
research protocols and analyses (see Finkel et al., 2015,foran
overview).
Conclusion
In both reviewing advances in and identifying weaknesses of the
literature on color and psychological functioning, it is impor-
tant to bear in mind that the existing theoretical and empirical
work is at an early stage of development. It is premature to offer
any bold theoretical statements, definitive empirical pronounce-
ments, or impassioned calls for application; rather, it is best to
be patient and to humbly acknowledge that color psychology is a
uniquely complex area of inquiry (Kuehni, 2012;Fairchild, 2013)
that is only beginning to come into its own. Findings from color
research can be provocative and media friendly, and the pub-
lic (and the field as well) can be tempted to reach conclusions
before the science is fully in place. There is considerable promise
in research on color and psychological functioning, but consider-
ably more theoretical and empirical work needs to be done before
the full extent of this promise can be discerned and, hopefully,
fulfilled.
References
Aiken, K. D., and Pascal, V. J. (2013). Seeing red, feeling red: how a change in field
color influences perceptions. Int. J. Sport Soc. 3, 107–120.
Akers, A., Barton, J., Cossey, R., Gainsford, P., Griffin, M., and Micklewright,
D. (2012). Visual color perception in green exercise: positive effects of
mood on perceived exertion. Environ. Sci. Technol. 46, 8661–8666. doi:
10.1021/es301685g
Alberts, W., and van der Geest, T. M. (2011). Color matters: color as trustworthi-
ness cue in websites. Tech. Comm. 58, 149–160.
Barli, Ö., Bilgili, B., and Dane, ¸S. (2006). Association of consumers’ sex and eyed-
ness and lighting and wall color of a store with price attraction and perceived
quality of goods and inside visual appeal. Percept. Motor Skill 103, 447–450. doi:
10.2466/PMS.103.6.447-450
Becker, S. I., Valuch, C., and Ansorge, U. (2014). Color priming in pop-out
search depends on the relative color of the target. Front. Psychol. 5:289. doi:
10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00289
Bertrams, A., Baumeister, R. F., Englert, C., andFurley, P. (2015). Ego depletion in
color priming research: self-control strength moderates the detrimental effect
of red on cognitive test performance. Pers. Soc. Psychol. B. 41, 311–322. doi:
10.1177/0146167214564968
Brainard, D. H., and Radonji´
c, A. (2014). “Color constancy” in The New Visual
Neurosciences, eds J. Werner and L. Chalupa (Cambridge, MA; MIT Press),
545–556.
Bruno, N., Martani, M., Corsini, C., and Oleari, C. (2013). The effect of the color
red on consuming food does not depend on achromatic (Michelson) con-
trast and extends to rubbing cream on the skin. Appetite 71, 307–313. doi:
10.1016/j.appet.2013.08.012
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 5April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 368
Elliot Color and psychological functioning
Bubl, E., Kern, E., Ebert, D., Bach, M., and Tebartz van Elst, L. (2009).
Seeing gray when feeling blue? Depression can be measures in the eye
of the diseased. Biol. Psychiat. 68, 205–208. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.
02.009
Buechner, V. L., Maier, M. A., Lichtenfeld, S., and Elliot, A. J. (in press). Emotion
expression and color: their joint influence on perceptions of male attractiveness
and social position. Curr. Psychol.
Buechner, V. L., Maier, M. A., Lichtenfeld, S., and Schwarz, S. (2014). Red – take a
closer look. PLoS ONE 9:e108111. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0108111
Cajochen, C. (2007). Alerting effects of light. Sleep Med. Rev. 11, 453–464. doi:
10.1016/j.smrv.2007.07.009
Cajochen, C., Frey, S., Anders, D., Späti, J., Bues, M., Pross, A., et al. (2011). Evening
exposure to a light-emitting diodes (LED)-backlit computer screen affects cir-
cadian physiology and cognitive performance. J. Appl. Phsysoil . 110, 1432–1438.
doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00165.2011
Cajochen, C., Münch, M., Kobialka, S., Kräuchi, K., Steiner, R., Oelhafen, P., et al.
(2005). High sensitivity of human melatonin, alertness, thermoregulation, and
heart rate to short wavelength light. J. Clin. Endocr. Metab. 90, 1311–1316. doi:
10.1210/jc.2004-0957
Caldwell, D. F., and Burger, J. M. (2011). On thin ice: does uniform color really
affect aggression in professional hockey? Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 2, 306–310. doi:
10.1177/1948550610389824
Changizi, M. (2009). The Vision Revolution. Dallas, T X: Benbella.
Changizi, M. A., Zhang, Q., and Shimojo, S. (2006). Bare skin, blood and the
evolution of primate colour vision. Biol. Lett. 2, 217–221. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.
2006.0440
Chebat, J. C., and Morrin, M. (2007). Colors and cultures: exploring the effects
of mall décor on consumer perceptions. J. Bus. Res. 60, 189–196. doi:
10.1016/j.jbusres.2006.11.003
Chellappa, S. L., Steiner, R., Blattner, P., Oelhafen, P., Götz, T., and Cajochen, C.
(2011). Non-visual effects of light on melatonin, alertness, and cognitive per-
formance: can blue-enriched light keep us alert? PLoS ONE 26:e16429. doi:
10.1371/journal.pone.0016429
Committee on Colorimetry of the Optical Society of America. (1953). The Science
of Color. Washington, DC: Optical Society of America.
Crowley, A. E. (1993). The two dimensional impact of color on shopping. Market.
Lett. 4, 59–69. doi: 10.1007/BF00994188
Cumming, G. (2014). The new statistics: why and how. Psychol. Sci. 25, 7–29. doi:
10.1177/0956797613504966
Cuthill, I. C., Hunt, S., Cleary, C., and Clark, C. (1997). Color bands, dominance,
and body mass regulation in male zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata). Proc. R.
Soc. Lond. B. Sci. 264, 1093–1099. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1997.0151
Drummond, P. D., and Quay, S. H. (2001). The effect of expressing anger on
cardiovascular reactivity and facial blood flow in Chinese and Caucasians.
Psychophysiology 38, 190–196. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3820190
Elliot, A. J., and Maier, M. A. (2012). Color-in-context theory. Adv.
Exp. Soc. Psychol. 45, 61–125. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-394286-9.
00002-0
Elliot, A. J., and Maier, M. A. (2014). Color psychology: effects of perceiving color
on psychological functioning in humans. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 65, 95–120. doi:
10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115035
Elliot, A. J., Maier, M. A., Moller, A. C., Friedman, R., and Meinhardt, J.
(2007). Color and psychological functioning: the effect of red on performance
attainment. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 136, 154–168. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.136.
1.154
Elliot, A. J., and Niesta, D. (2008). Romantic red: red enhances men’s attrac-
tion to women. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 95, 1150–1164. doi: 10.1037/0022-
3514.95.5.1150
Elwood, J. A., and Bode, J. (2014). Student preferences vis-à-vis teacher feed-
back in university EFL writing classes in Japan. System 42, 333–343. doi:
10.1016/j.system.2013.12.023
Fairchild, M. D. (2013). Color Appearance Models, 3rd Edn. New York, NY: Wiley
Press. doi: 10.1002/9781118653128
Fairchild, M. D. (2015). Seeing, adapting to, and reproducing the appearance of
nature. Appl. Optics 54, B107–B116. doi: 10.1364/AO.54.00B107
Feltman, R., and Elliot, A. J. (2011). The influence of red on perceptions of
dominance and threat in a competitive context. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 33,
308–314.
Fetterman, A. K., Liu, T., and Robinson, M. D. (2015). Extending color psychology
to the personality realm: interpersonal hostility varied by red preferences and
perceptual biases. J. Personal. 83, 106–116. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12087
Féré, C. (1887). Note sur les conditions physiologiques des émotions. Revue Phil.
24, 561–581.
Fink, B., Grammer, K., and Matts, P. J. (2006). Visible skin color distribution plays
a role in the perception of age, attractiveness, and health in female faces. Evol.
Hum. Behav. 27, 433–442. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.08.007
Fink, B., and Matts, P. J. (2007). The effects of skin colour distribution and topog-
raphy cues on the perception of female age and health. J. Eur. Acad. Derm. 22,
493–498. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-3083.2007.02512.x
Finkel, E. J., Eastwick, P. W., and Reis, H. T. (2015). Best research practices
in psychology: Illustrating epistemological and pragmatic considerations with
the case of relationship science. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 108, 275–297. doi:
10.1037/pspi0000007
Folk, C. L. (in press). “The role of color in the voluntary and involuntary guid-
ance of selective attention,” in Handbook of Color Psychology,edsA.Elliot,M.
Fairchild, and A. Franklin (C ambridge: Cambridge University Press).
Fraley, R. C., and Vazire, S. (2014). The N-pact factor: evaluating the quality of
empirical journals with respect to sample size and statistical power.PLoSONE
9:e109019. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0109019
Frank, M. G., and Gilovich, T. (1988). The dark side of self and social perception:
black uniforms and aggression in professional sports. J.Pers.Soc.Psychol.54,
74–85. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.1.74
Furley, P., Dicks, M., and Memmert, D. (2012). Nonverbal behavior in soccer: the
influence of dominant and submissive body language on the impression for-
mation and expectancy of success of soccer players. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 34,
61–82.
Gage, J. (1993). Color and Culture: Practice and Meaning from Antiquity to
Abst ra ct io n. Be rkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Garcia-Rubio, M. A., Picazo-Tadeo, A. J., and González-Gómez, F. (2011). Does a
red shirt improve sporting performance? Evidence from Spanish football. Appl.
Econ. Lett. 18, 1001–1004. doi: 10.1080/13504851.2010.520666
Gegenfurtner, K. R., and Ennis, R. (in press). “Fundamentals of color vision II:
higher order color processing,” in Handbook of Color Psychology, eds A. Elliot,
M. Fairchild, and A. Franklin (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).
Genschow, O., Reutner, L., and Wänke, M. (2012). The color red reduces snack food
and soft Drink intake. Appetite 58, 699–702. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.12.023
Gnambs, T., Appel, M., and Batinic, B. (2010). Color red in web-based knowledge
testing. Comput. Hum. Behav. 26, 1625–1631. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2010.06.010
Goethe, W. (1810). Theory of Colors. London: Frank Cass.
Goldstein, K. (1942). Some experimental observations concerning the influence of
colors on the function of the organism. Occup. Ther. Rehab. 21, 147–151. doi:
10.1097/00002060-194206000-00002
Greenlees, I. A., Eynon, M., and Thelwell, R. C. (2013). Color of soccer goalkeep-
ers’ uniforms influences the outcomed of penalty kicks. Percept. Mot. Skill. 116,
1–10. doi: 10.2466/30.24.PMS.117x14z6
Greenlees, I., Leyland, A., Thelwell, R., and Filby, W. (2008). Soccer penalty
takers’ uniform color and pre-penalty kick gaze affect the impressions
formed of them by opposing goalkeepers. J. Sport Sci. 26, 569–576. doi:
10.1080/02640410701744446
Guéguen, N. (2012). Color and women attractiveness: when red clothed women are
perceived to have more intense sexual intent. J. Soc. Psychol. 152, 261–265. doi:
10.1080/00224545.2011.605398
Guéguen, N., and Jacob, C. (2014). Coffee cup color and evaluation of a beverage’s
“warmth quality.” Color Res. Appl. 39, 79–81. doi: 10.1002/col.21757
Hagemann, N., Strauss, B., and Leißing, J. (2008). When the referee sees red.
Psychol. Sci. 19, 769–771. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02155.x
Higham, J. P., and Winters, S. (in press). “Color and mate choice in non-human
animals,” in Handbook of Color Psychology, edsA.Elliot,M.Fairchild,andA.
Franklin (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).
Hill, R. A., and Barton, R. A. (2005). Red enhances human performance in contests.
Nature 435, 293. doi: 10.1038/435293a
Hunt, R. W. G., and Pointer, M. R. (2011). Measuring Colour,4thEdn.NewYork,
NY: WileyPress. doi: 10.1002/9781119975595
Ilie, A., Ioan, S., Zagrean, L., and Moldovan, M. (2008). Better to be red
than blue in virtual competition. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 11, 375–377. doi:
10.1089/cpb.2007.0122
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 6April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 368
Elliot Color and psychological functioning
Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2008). Why most discovered true associations are inflated.
Epidemiology 19, 640–648. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e31818131e7
Johnson, G. M. (in press). “Color appearance phenomena and visual illusions,”
in Handbook of Color Psychology, edsA.Elliot,M.Fairchild,andA.Franklin
(Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).
Kareklas, I., Brunel, F. F., and Coulter, R. A. (2014). Judgment is not color blind:
the impact of automatic color preference on product advertising preferences.
J. Consum. Psychol. 24, 87–95. doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2013.09.005
Krenn, B. (2014). The impact of uniform color on judging tack-
les in association football. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 15, 222–225. doi:
10.1016/j.psychsport.2013.11.007
Kuehni, R. (2012). Color: An Introduction to Practice and Principles, 3rd Edn.
New York, NY: Wiley. doi: 10.1002/9781118533567
Labrecque, L. L., and Milne, G. R. (2012). Exciting red and competent blue:
the importance of color in marketing. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 40, 711–727. doi:
10.1007/s11747-010-0245-y
Lakoff, G., and Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the Flesh: The Embodied Mind
and its Challenges to Western Thought. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Lee, S., Lee, K., Lee, S., and Song, J. (2013). Origins of human color preference for
food. J. Food Eng. 119, 508–515. doi: 10.1016/j.jfoodeng.2013.06.021
Lee, S., and Rao, V. S. (2010). Color and store choice in electronic commerce: the
explanatory role of trust. J. Electr. Commer. Res. 11, 110–126.
Lehrl, S., Gerstmeyer, K., Jacob, J. H., Frieling, H., Henkel, A. W., Meyrer, R.,
et al. (2007). Blue light improves cognitive performance. J. Neural Trans. 114,
457–460. doi: 10.1007/s00702-006-0621-4
Levenson, R. W. (2003). Blood, sweat, and fears: the automatic architecture of
emotion. Ann. N. Y. Acad Sci. 1000, 348–366. doi: 10.1196/annals.1280.016
Lin, H. (2014). Red-colored products enhance the attractiveness of women.
Displays 35, 202–205. doi: 10.1016/j.displa.2014.05.009
Lindsay, D. T., Brown, A. M., Reijnen, E., Rich, A. N., Kuzmova, Y. I., and Wolfe,
J. M. (2010). Color channels, not color appearance of color categories, guide
visual search for desaturated color targets. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1208–1214. doi:
10.1177/0956797610379861
Little, A. C., and Hill, R. A. (2007). Attribution to red suggests special role in
dominance signaling. J. Evol. Psychol. 5, 161–168. doi: 10.1556/JEP.2007.1008
Lockley, S. W., Evans, E. E., Scheer, F. A., Brainard, G. C., Czeisler, C. A., and
Aeschbach, D. (2006). Short-wavelength sensitivity for the direct effects of light
on alertness, vigilance, and the waking electroencephalogram in humans. Sleep
29, 161–168.
Lynn, M., Giebelhausen, M., Garcia, S., Li, Y., and Patumanon, I. (in press).
Clothing color and tipping: an attempted replication and extension. J. Hosp.
Tourism Res. doi: 10.1177/1096348013504001
Maier, M. A., Hill, R., Elliot, A. J., and Barton, R. A. (in press). “Color in achieve-
ment contexts in humans,” in Handbook of Color Psychology,edsA.Elliot,M.
Fairchild, and A. Franklin (C ambridge: Cambridge University Press).
Maxwell, S. (2004). The persistence of underpowered studies in psychologi-
cal research: causes and consequences. Psychol. Methods 9, 147–163. doi:
10.1037/1082-989X.9.2.147
Mehta, R., and Zhu, R. (2009). Blue or red? Exploring the effect of color on
cognitive task performances. Science 323, 1226–1229. doi: 10.1126/science.
1169144
Meier, B. P. (in press). “Do metaphors color our perception of social life?,” in
Handbook of Color sychology, eds A. Elliot, M. Fairchild, and A. Franklin
(Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).
Meier, B. P., and Robinson, M. D. (2005). The metaphorical representation of affect.
Metaphor Symbol. 20, 239–257. doi: 10.1207/s15327868ms2004_1
Murayama, K., Pekrun, R., and Fiedler, K. (2014). Research practices that can pre-
vent an inflation of false-positive rates. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 18, 107–118.
doi: 10.1177/1088868313496330
Nakashian, J. S. (1964). The effects of red and green surroundings on behavior.
J. Gen. Psychol.70, 143–162. doi: 10.1080/00221309.1964.9920584
O’Connor, Z. (2011). Colour psychology and colour therapy: caveat emptor. Color
Res. Appl. 36, 229–334. doi: 10.1002/col.20597
Pazda, A. D., and Greitemeyer, T. (in press). “Color in romantic contexts in
humans,” in Handbook of Color Psychology,edsA.Elliot,M.Fairchild,andA.
Franklin (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).
Piqueras-Fiszman, B., Alcaide, J., Roura, E., and Spence, C. ( 2012). Is it the plate or
is it the food? Assessing the influence of the color (black or white) and shape of
the plate on the perception of food placed on it. Food Qual. Prefer. 24, 205–208.
doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2011.08.011
Pomerleau, V. J., Fortier-Gauthier, U., Corriveau, I., Dell’Acqua, R., and Jolicœur,
P. (2014). Colour-specific differences in attentional deployment for equilumi-
nant pop-out colours: e vidence from lateralized potentials. Int. J. Psychophysiol.
91, 194–205. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.10.016
Prado-León, L. R., and Rosales-Cinco, R. A. (2011). “Effects of lightness and sat-
uration on color associations in the Mexican population,” in New Directions in
Colour Studies, eds C. Biggam, C. Hough, C . Kay, and D. Simmons (Amsterdam,
NL: John Benjamins Publishing Company), 389–394.
Pressey, S. L. (1921). The influence of color upon mental and motor efficiency. Am.
J. Psychol. 32, 327–356. doi: 10.2307/1413999
Ridgway, J., and Myers, B. (2014). A study on brand personality: consumers’ per-
ceptions of colours used in fashion brand logos. Int. J. Fash. Des. Tech. Educ. 7,
50–57. doi: 10.1080/17543266.2013.877987
Roberts, S. C., Owen, R. C., and Havlicek, J. (2010). Distinguishing between per-
ceiver and wearer effe ctsin clothing color-associated attributions. Evol. Psychol.
8, 350–364.
Ross, C. F., Bohlscheid, J., and Weller, K. (2008). Influence of visual
masking technique on the assessment of 2 red wines by trained con-
sumer assessors. J. Food Sci. 73, S279–S285. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-3841.2008.
00824.x
Rutchick, A. M., Slepian, M. L., and Ferris, B. D. (2010). The pen is mightier
than the word: object priming of evaluative standards. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 40,
704–708. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.753
Sahin, L., and Figuerio, M. G. (2013). Alerting effects of short-wavelength (blue)
and long-wavelength (red) lights in the afternoon. Physiol. Behav. 116, 1–7. doi:
10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.03.014
Schwarz, S., and Singer, M. (2013). Romantic red revisited: red enhances men’s
attraction to young, but not menopausal women. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 49,
161–164. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.08.004
Setchell, J. (in press). “Color in competition contexts in non-human animals,” in
Handbook of Color Psychology, eds A. Elliot, M. Fairchild, and A. Franklin
(Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).
Shi, J., Zhang, C., and Jiang, f. (2015). Does red undermine individuals’ intellec-
tual performance? A test in China. Int. J. Psychol. 50, 81–84. doi: 10.1002/ijop.
12076
Sloane, P. (1991). Primary Sources, Selected Writings on Color from Aristotle to
Albers. New York, NY: Design Press.
Smajic, A., Merritt, S., Banister, C., and Blinebry, A. (2014). The red effect, anxiety,
and exam performance: a multistudy examination. Teach. Psychol. 41, 37–43.
doi: 10.1177/0098628313514176
Sokolik, K., Magee, R. G., and Ivory, J. D. (2014). Red-hot and ice-cold ads: the
influence of web ads’ warm and cool colors on click-through ways. J. Interact.
Advert. 14, 31–37. doi: 10.1080/15252019.2014.907757
Soldat, A. S., Sinclair, R. C., and Mark, M. M. (1997). Color as an environmen-
tal processing cue: external affective cues can directly affect processing strategy
without affecting mood. Soc. Cogn. 15, 55–71. doi: 10.1521/soco.1997.15.1.55
Sorokowski, P., and Szmajke, A. (2007). How does the “red wins: effect work? The
role of sportswear colour during sport competitions. Pol. J. Appl. Psychol. 5,
71–79.
Sorokowski, P., Szmajke, A., Hamamura, T., Jiang, F., and Sorakowska, A. (2014).
“Red wins,” “black wins,” “blue loses” effects are in the eye of the beholder,
but they are culturally niversal: a cross-cultural analysis of the influence of
outfit colours on sports performance. Pol. Psychol. Bull. 45, 318–325. doi:
10.2478/ppb-2014-0039
Spence, C. (in press). “Eating with our eyes,” in Handbook of Color Psychology,
eds A. Elliot, M. Fairchild, and A. Franklin (Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press).
Spence, C., Velasco, C., and Knoeferle, K. (2014). A large sampled study on the
influence of the multisensory environment on the wine drinking experience.
Flavour 3, 8. doi: 10.1186/2044-7248-3-8
Steele, K. M. (2014). Failure to replicate the Mehta and Zhu (2009) color-
priming effect on anagram solution times. Psychon. B. Rev. 21, 771–776. doi:
10.3758/s13423-013-0548-3
Stephen, I. D., Law Smith, M. J., Stirrat, M. R., and Perrett, D. I. (2009). Facial skin
coloration affects perceived health of human faces. Int. J. Primatol. 30, 845–857.
doi: 10.1007/s10764-009-9380-z
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 7April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 368
Elliot Color and psychological functioning
Stephen, I. D., and McKeegan, A. M. (2010). Lip colour affects perceived sex
typicality and attractiveness of human faces. Perception 39, 1104–1110. doi:
10.1068/p6730
Stephen, I. D., Oldham, F. H., Perrett, D. I., and Barton, R. A. (2012a). Redness
enhances perceived aggression, dominance and attractiveness in men’s faces.
Evol. Psychol. 10, 562–572.
Stephen, I. D., Scott, I. M. L., Coetzee, V., Pound, N., Perrett, D. I., and Penton-
Voak, I. S. (2012b). Cross-cultural effects of color, but not morphological
masculinity, on perceived attractiveness of men’s faces. Evol. Hum. Behav. 33,
260–267. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.10.003
Stephen, I. D., and Perrett, D. I. (in press). “Color and face perception,” in
Handbook of Color Psychology,edsA.Elliot,M.Fairchild,andA.Franklin
(Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).
Stockman, A., and Brainard, D. H. (in press). “Fundamentals of color vision I: pro-
cessing in the eye,” in Handbook of Color Psychology, eds A. Elliot, M. Fairchild,
and A. Franklin (C ambridge: Cambridge University Press).
Taillard, J., Capelli, A., Sagaspe, P., Anund, A., and Akerstadt, T. (2012).
In-car nocturnal blue light exposure improves motorway driving: a ran-
domized controlled trial. PLoS ONE 7:e46750. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.
0046750
Tan, K. W., and Stephen, I. D. (2012). Colour detection thresholds in faces and
colour patches. Perception 42, 733–741. doi: 10.1068/p7499
Tanaka, A., and Tokuno, Y. (2011). The effect of the color red on avoidance
motivation. Soc. Behav. Pers. 39, 287–288. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2011.39.2.287
Tchernikov, I., and Fallah, M. (2010). A color hierarchy for auto-
matic target selection. PLoS ONE 5:e9338. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.
0009338
Thorstenson, C. A. (in press). Functional equivalence of the color red and enacted
avoidance behavior? Replication and empirical integration. Soc. Psychol.
Tracy, J. L., and Beall, A. T. (2014). The impact of weather on women’s tendency
to wear red pink when at high risk for conception. PLoS ONE 9:e88852. doi:
10.1371/journal.pone.0088852
Ten Velden, F. S., Baas, M., Shalvi, S., Preenen, P. T. Y., and De Dreu, C. K. W.
(2012). In competitive interaction displays of red increase actors’ competitive
approach and perceivers’ withdrawal. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 48, 1205–1208. doi:
10.1016/j.jesp.2012.04.004
Valdez, P., and Mehrabian, A. (1994). Effects of color on emotions. J. Exp. Psychol.
Gen. 123, 394–409. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.123.4.394
Van Ittersum, K., and Wansink, B. (2012). Plate size and color suggestability, The
Deboeuf Illusion’s bias on serving and eating behavior. J. Consum. Res. 39,
215–228. doi: 10.1086/662615
Vandewalle, G., Schmidt, C., Albouy, G., Sterpenich, V., Darsaud, A., Rauchs, G.,
et al. (2007). Brain responses to violet, blue, and green monochromatic light
exposures in humans: prominent role of blue light and the brainstem. PLoS
ONE 11:e1247. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001247
Viola, A. U., James, L. M., Schlangen, L. J. M., and Dijk, D. J. (2008). Blue-
enriched white lightin the workplace improves self-reported alertness, per-
formance and sleep quality. Scan. J. Work Environ. Health 34, 297–306. doi:
10.5271/sjweh.1268
Whitfield, T. W., and Wiltshire, T. J. (1990). Color psychology: a critical review.
Gen. Soc. Gen. Psychol. 116, 385–411.
Yamazaki, A. K. (2010). An analysis of background-color effects on the scores of a
computer-based English test. KES Part II LNI. 6277, 630–636. doi: 10.1007/978-
3-642-15390-7_65
Young, S. (in press). The effect of red on male perceptions of female attractiveness:
moderationby baseline attractiveness of female faces. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol.
Yüksel, A. (2009). Exterior color and perceived retail crowding: effects on tourists’
shoppingquality inferences and approach behaviors. J. Qual. Assur. Hosp.
Tourism 10, 233–254. doi: 10.1080/15280080903183383
Zanna, M. P., and Fazio, R. H. (1982). “The attitude behavior relation: Moving
toward a third generation of research,” in The Ontario Symposium,Vol.2,eds
M. Zanna, E. T. Higgins, and C. Herman (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates, Inc.), 283–301.
Zhang, T., and Han, B. (2014). Experience reverses the red effect among Chinese
stockbrokers. PLoS ONE 9:e89193. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0089193
Conflict of Interest Statement: The author declares that the research was con-
ducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be
construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Copyright © 2015 Elliot. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of
the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or repro-
duction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are
credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with
accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which
does not comply with these terms.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 8April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 368
Available via license: CC BY 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.