ArticlePDF Available

Effectiveness of motor practice in lucid dreams: a comparison with physical and mental practice

Taylor & Francis
Journal of Sports Sciences
Authors:

Abstract

Motor practice in lucid dreams is a form of mental rehearsal where the dreamer can consciously rehearse motor skills in the dream state while being physically asleep. A previous pilot study showed that practice in lucid dreams can improve subsequent performance. This study aimed to replicate those findings with a different task (finger-tapping) and compare the effectiveness of lucid dream practice (LDP) not only to physical but also to mental practice (MP) in wakefulness. An online experiment was completed by 68 participants within four groups: LDP group, MP group, physical practice (PP) group and control (no practice) group. Pre-test was accomplished in the evening, post-test in the next morning, while the practice was done during the night. All three practice groups significantly improved their performance from pre-test to post-test, but no significant improvements were observed for the control group. Subjective sleep quality was not affected by night practice. This study thus corroborates the previous findings that practice in lucid dreams is effective in improving performance. Its effects seem to be similar to actual PP and MP in wakefulness. Future studies should establish reliable techniques for lucid dream induction and verify the effects of LDP in sleep laboratory conditions.
Effectiveness of motor practice in lucid dreams: a comparison with
physical and mental practice
TADAS STUMBRYS
1
, DANIEL ERLACHER
2
& MICHAEL SCHREDL
3
1
Institute of Sports and Sports Sciences, Heidelberg University, Heidelberg, Germany,
2
Institute of Sport Science, University
of Bern, Bern, Switzerland and
3
Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim/Heidelberg University,
Mannheim, Germany
(Accepted 13 March 2015)
Abstract
Motor practice in lucid dreams is a form of mental rehearsal where the dreamer can consciously rehearse motor skills in the
dream state while being physically asleep. A previous pilot study showed that practice in lucid dreams can improve
subsequent performance. This study aimed to replicate those ndings with a different task (nger-tapping) and compare
the effectiveness of lucid dream practice (LDP) not only to physical but also to mental practice (MP) in wakefulness. An
online experiment was completed by 68 participants within four groups: LDP group, MP group, physical practice (PP)
group and control (no practice) group. Pre-test was accomplished in the evening, post-test in the next morning, while the
practice was done during the night. All three practice groups signicantly improved their performance from pre-test to post-
test, but no signicant improvements were observed for the control group. Subjective sleep quality was not affected by night
practice. This study thus corroborates the previous ndings that practice in lucid dreams is effective in improving
performance. Its effects seem to be similar to actual PP and MP in wakefulness. Future studies should establish reliable
techniques for lucid dream induction and verify the effects of LDP in sleep laboratory conditions.
Keywords: lucid dreams, motor learning, lucid dream practice, mental practice, nger-tapping
Introduction
Mental practice (MP) is the cognitive rehearsal of a
physical activity in the absence of overt physical
movements (Richardson, 1967). It is a well-estab-
lished technique in sports (Morris, Spittle, & Watt,
2005). Several meta-analyses demonstrated that MP
signicantly improves performance, albeit to a smal-
ler extent than actual physical practice (PP)
(Driskell, Copper, & Moran, 1994; Feltz &
Landers, 1983). A novel and relatively unknown
type of mental rehearsal is motor practice in lucid
dreams (Erlacher, 2007).
Lucid dreams are dreams in which the dreamer is
asleep but aware that he or she is dreaming and often
can inuence the dream plot (LaBerge, 1985). This
ability to be aware in the dream state and deliber-
ately perform actions while physically asleep opens
up opportunities to use lucid dreams for sports prac-
tice, for example, to consciously rehearse specic
motor tasks without waking up (Tholey, 1990).
Practice in lucid dreams is similar to MP in wakeful-
ness: movements are rehearsed with a representation
of the body on a cognitive level without overt physi-
cal movements (cf. Erlacher, 2007).
The evidence suggests that imagined and executed
actions to some extent seem to share the same neural
structures (Decety, 1996). Motor and associated cor-
tical areas involved in programming and preparations
of actual movements are also active during mental
simulation of movement (reviews: Jeannerod, 2001;
Lotze & Halsband, 2006; Munzert, Lorey, &
Zentgraf, 2009), autonomic responses of imagined
actions mimic the actual autonomic responses during
the exercise (review: Guillot & Collet, 2005a), and the
timing of imagined and actual motor actions are clo-
sely related (reviews: Guillot & Collet, 2005b;
Guillot, Hoyek, Louis, & Collet, 2012). According
to the theory of neural simulation of action by
Jeannerod (2001), in general, covert actions are actual
actions, except for the fact that they are not executed.
The theory thus predicts a neural similarity between
the state where an action is simulated (S-state) and
the state of execution of this action. While for some S-
states, such as action observation, the functional
Correspondence: Tadas Stumbrys, Institute of Sports and Sports Sciences, Heidelberg University, Im Neuenheimer Feld 700, 69120 Heidelberg, Germany.
E-mail: tadas.stumbrys@issw.uni-heidelberg.de
Journal of Sports Sciences, 2015
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2015.1030342
© 2015 Taylor & Francis
Downloaded by [Universitaetsbibliothek Heidelberg] at 01:12 08 April 2015
equivalence has been called into question (Lorey
et al., 2013), the limited evidence from dream
research suggest that dreamed actions do seem to
share the same neural mechanisms with executed
actions (Erlacher & Schredl, 2008b). For example,
correspondences exist in underlying brain activity
(cf. Dresler et al., 2011), autonomic responses (cf.
Erlacher & Schredl, 2008a) and temporal dynamics
(cf. Erlacher, Schädlich, Stumbrys, & Schredl, 2014).
Empirical evidence on lucid dream practice
(LDP) is rather scarce. In a sample of 840 German
athletes from various sports, 57% stated that they
had at least one lucid dream in their life, 24%
reported frequent lucid dreams (one or more lucid
dreams per month), however only 9% of the lucid
dreamers used this dream state to practice sport
skills (Erlacher, Stumbrys, & Schredl, 20112012).
Yet, the majority of those who practiced had the
impression that the rehearsal within the lucid
dream improved their subsequent performance in
wakefulness. Several such anecdotal accounts on
how practice in lucid dreams improved waking per-
formance have also been reported in the literature
(e.g. Erlacher, 2007; LaBerge & Rheingold, 1990;
Tholey, 1990).
In a qualitative study, Tholey (1981) asked six
procient lucid dreamers to perform and practice
movements and complex sport skills, such as skiing
on gymnastics, with which they were already familiar
from waking life. The participants reported no dif-
culties while performing complex sport skills in their
lucid dreams and had an impression that their move-
ments improved following the practice.
Further, Erlacher and Schredl (2010) conducted a
pilot study (eld experiment) with a pre-post design
in which the participants were asked to practice a
simple motor task to toss 10-cent coins into a cup,
positioned at the distance of two meters, as many
times as possible out of 20 trials. Twenty participants
attempted to practice the task in a lucid dream on a
single night and seven of them succeeded. Their
performance was compared to a group which accom-
plished actual PP (n= 10) and a control group with-
out practice (n= 10). Both practice groups showed
signicant increases in hitting the target from pre-
test to post-test, while no increase was found for the
participants who did not practice the task. Although
the improvements following lucid dreaming practice
were somewhat lower in comparison to PP, the dif-
ferences were not signicant.
The present study aimed to replicate these nd-
ings with a different motor task (a sequential nger-
tapping task; cf. Karni et al., 1998; Walker,
Brakeeld, Morgan, Hobson, & Stickgold, 2002)
and compare the effectiveness of LDP not only to
awake PP but also to MP in wakefulness. It was
expected that all three types of practice will improve
subsequent performance. The gains from PP were
expected to be higher than the ones from MP (cf.
Driskell et al., 1994), whereas the gains from LDP
were expected to fall in between, as the cognitive
simulation in the dream state is much more realistic
(cf. Tholey, 1990) and the perception in lucid
dreams appears to be much more closer to actual
perception than to waking imagination (LaBerge &
Zimbardo, 2000).
Methods
Participants
The sample included 68 individuals (32 male and 36
female) who completed an online experiment. Their
ages ranged from 19 to 54 years, with the mean age
of 31.3 ± 7.3 years. Participants were recruited via
electronic advertisements (posted on lucid dream-
ing-related discussion boards, social networking
sites and via personal contacts) and assigned to one
of four groups: (1) LDP group (if they were frequent
lucid dreamers; n= 21); (2) MP group (n=15); (3)
PP group (n= 16); (4) control (no practice) group
(n= 16). The data of four participants from the LDP
group were excluded (reducing the sample to
n= 17) as three participants reported that they prac-
ticed the task only very briey (tapping the sequence
only 24 times) and one participant reported addi-
tional practice in the evening while awake. Group
characteristics are depicted in Table I. Participation
Table I. Group characteristics.
LDP (n= 17) MP (n= 15) PP (n= 16) Control (n= 16) Statistical test P
Age (y) 31.2 ± 8.1 32.1 ± 8.1 31.3 ± 6.0 30.1 ± 8.1 F
3,60
= 0.18 .909
Male/Female 11/6 6/9 7/9 6/10 χ
23
= 3.08 .379
Right-/Left-handers 13/4 15/0 15/1 15/1 χ
23
= 5.93 .115
VMIQ-2
External visual 31.4 ± 12.3 32.9 ± 10.3 33.3 ± 9.3 29.8 ± 9.6 F
3,60
= 0.36 .781
Internal visual 30.7 ± 11.4 30.1 ± 9.3 29.2 ± 11.4 27.1 ± 10.8 F
3,60
= 0.34 .800
Kinesthetic 28.8 ± 11.1 31.5 ± 9.3 24.8 ± 6.9 25.9 ± 9.3 F
3,60
= 1.63 .192
Lucid dreams/month 8.8 ± 7.0 0.5 ± 1.1 0.4 ± 0.7 0.3 ± 0.6 χ
23
= 36.1 <.001
Sleep quality 1.9 ± 0.7 1.9 ± 0.8 2.3 ± 0.8 1.8 ± 0.4 χ
23
= 3.70 .296
2T. Stumbrys et al.
Downloaded by [Universitaetsbibliothek Heidelberg] at 01:12 08 April 2015
was voluntary and unpaid. The study was conducted
according to the principles of the Declaration of
Helsinki. All participants signed an electronic
informed consent form and were free to withdraw
from the experiment at any time.
Motor task
A computerised online version of the sequential n-
ger-tapping task was used, which requires the parti-
cipant to press four keys on a computer keyboard
with a non-dominant hand producing a sequence of
ve elements as quickly and accurately as possible
for a period of 30 s (Walker et al., 2002). Each
sequence started and nished with the little nger;
index, middle and ring ngers were used once (e.g.
41324; cf. Karni et al., 1998). Four different
sequences were prepared for each hand, allowing
repeating the experiment up to four times. During
the initial (learning) phase, the participants were
asked to memorise the sequence shown on the
screen and tap it correctly 10 times. Each correct
key press produced a green dot on a corresponding
nger in a hand picture presented on the screen,
while an incorrect key press produced a red dot.
The assessment phase consisted of two test periods
of 30 s with a 30 s rest period in between. Each key
press produced a white dot. No other feedback was
provided. The number of correct sequences com-
pleted and the number of incorrect key presses
were recorded. The average scores of two test peri-
ods were calculated.
Procedure
The study was conducted as a eld experiment, that
is, the participants accomplished the procedure by
themselves in their home setting. Participants had to
ll out an initial online questionnaire, which
included demographical data (age, gender and coun-
try), questions about lucid dream recall, vividness of
motor imagery and handedness. Lucid dream fre-
quency was assessed on an 8-point scale (0 never;
1less than once a year; 2 about once a year; 3
about 24 times a year; 4 about once a month; 5
about 23 times a month; 6 about once a week and
7several times a week), with a high retest reliability
(r= .89; Stumbrys, Erlacher, & Schredl, 2013). To
ensure a clear understanding of lucid dreaming, a
denition was provided: In a lucid dream, one is
aware that one is dreaming during the dream. Thus
it is possible to wake up deliberately, or to inuence
the action of the dream actively, or to observe the
course of the dream passively.In order to obtain
units in frequency per month, the scale was recoded
using the class means: 0 0, 1 0.042, 2 0.083,
30.25, 4 1.0, 5 2.5, 6 4.0, 7 18.0 (see
Stumbrys et al., 2013). Further, the participants
lled a revised version of the Vividness of Motor
Imagery Questionnaire (VMIQ-2, Roberts, Callow,
Hardy, Markland, & Bringer, 2008), which assesses
three-factor (internal visual imagery, external visual
imagery and kinesthetic imagery) individual imagery
characteristics on a 5-point scale (ranging from 1
perfectly clear and vivid to 5 no image at all).
VMIQ-2 has demonstrated acceptable factorial
validity, construct validity and concurrent validity
(Roberts et al., 2008). The order in which VMIQ-2
imagery modalities were presented was randomised.
Lastly, the participants lled the Edinburgh
Handedness Inventory Short Form (EHI-SF,
Veale, 2014), which contains four items for which
the preference in the use of hands is scored on a 5-
point scale (from 1 always right to 5 always left).
The modication was shown to have good reliability,
factor score determinacy and correlation with the
original 10-item inventory (Oldeld, 1971).
After lling the initial questionnaire, the partici-
pants were assigned to one of the groups. The LDP
group was assembled from the participants with a
higher lucid dream frequency (23 or more lucid
dreams per month). Other participants were put on
the waiting list and randomly assigned to one of the
other groups after the LDP group completed the
experiment. Instructions to the participants were
sent by email. All participants were asked to choose
a time schedule for the experiment so that the time
difference between the evening pre-test and the
morning post-test would be 10 h. The MP and PP
participants were assigned corresponding practice
times (from the bed time) and durations as the
LDP group. Two lucid dreamers practiced the task
in two different dreams during the night, hence two
corresponding participants in MP and PP groups
were also asked to awaken and practice the task
twice during the night (although one PP participant
did only a single awakening). Further, all partici-
pants were asked to set an alarm clock to awaken at
least 30 min before the post-test time, so that their
performance would not be impaired by sleep inertia
(Tassi & Muzet, 2000).
LDP group. The participants could use any techni-
que to induce lucid dreams (cf. Stumbrys, Erlacher,
Schädlich, & Schredl, 2012), with the exception of
drug intake. After becoming lucid in a dream, they
had to start immediately practicing the task (repeat-
ing the memorised sequence) and continue the prac-
tice for as long as possible. The participants were
instructed to do the practice in 30 s (self-estimated)
intervals with 30 s (self-estimated) rest periods in
between. During the 30 s rest periods they were
allowed to apply techniques that prolong lucid
Effectiveness of motor practice in lucid dreams 3
Downloaded by [Universitaetsbibliothek Heidelberg] at 01:12 08 April 2015
dreams (e.g. spinning, hand rubbing; LaBerge,
1995).
MP group. Each participant was assigned one exact
practice time and duration from the LDP group. The
participants were asked to awaken 30 min before the
assigned practice time and keep themselves awake dur-
ing this period (to avoid possible effects of sleep iner-
tia). Then they were instructed to close their eyes and
start practicing the task in their mind without moving
their actual ngers during the practice. With their eyes
closed, they had to attempt to imagine themselves
producing the sequence by visualising the movement
of each nger. The participants were asked to try to feel
each movement of their ngers while repeating the
memorised sequence. The practice had to be accom-
plished in 30 s (self-estimated) intervals with 30 s (self-
estimated) rest periods in between.
PP group. As in the MP group, PP participants were
assigned exact practice times and durations from the
LDP group and asked to awaken 30 min before the
practice time and keep themselves awake during this
period. Then they were instructed to start practicing
the task physically, also in 30 s (self-estimated) inter-
vals with 30 s (self-estimated) rest periods in
between.
Control group. The participants were not asked to do
any practice (only pre-test in the evening and post-
test in the morning).
The experimental night procedure is depicted in
Figure 1. Upon completing the post-test, the parti-
cipants had to ll out a report, indicating their bed
times, and rating their sleep quality of the night on a
4-point scale (1 very good to 4 very bad). Three
practice groups were further asked to provide their
practice details. If unsuccessful, participants could
repeat the experiment with a difference sequence
(up to four times).
On one occasion (MP) no results were recorded
for the second post-test interval, whereas in two
other occasions (LDP and MP) the number of
correct sequences produced during one of the pre-
test intervals (in both cases the second interval) was
very low (2 sequences) as compared to the average
performance during the other three test intervals
(16.0 and 13.7 sequences, respectively). To avoid
possible distortions, the data from these intervals
were excluded (thus only a single test interval result
and not the average of the two test intervals were
used).
Statistical analysis
IBM SPSS Statistics 20 software was used for sta-
tistical analysis. One-way ANOVA were performed
to compare the characteristics of the four groups on
interval variables (age, scores on motor imagery
scales), whereas KruskallWallis test was used for
ordinal variables (sleep quality and lucid dream
frequency) and Chi-square for categorical variables
(gender and handedness). Two-way repeated mea-
sures ANOVA were conducted to compare the
performance from pre-test to post-test between
the groups. Differences between the groups were
investigated with post-hoc least signicant differ-
ence (LSD) tests. Student t-tests were used to
compare the performance from the pre-test to the
post-test for each individual group. Spearmansrho
correlations were used to check the associations
between changes in performance and potential con-
founding variables: lucid dream frequency and
repeated trials. G*Power 3.1.7 software (Faul,
Erdfelder, Buchner, & Lang, 2009) was used for
calculating effect sizes d. An alpha = .05 signi-
cance level was employed.
Results
Group characteristics
According to the EHI-SF results, six participants
were considered left-handed and were assigned to
do the motor task with the right hand, whereas the
rest 58 participants were right-handed and per-
formed the task with the left hand. There were no
signicant handedness differences between the
groups, as well as differences in motor imagery abil-
ities as measured by VMIQ-2 and reported sleep
quality of the night, although the groups differed in
their lucid dream frequency (Table I). LDP partici-
pants in average had 2.1 ± 1.1 (M ± s) trials.
10 h
30 min
30 min
MP ControlPP LDP
Morning awakening
Morning post-test
LD induct.
techniques
Evening pre-test
Going to bed
Night practice
Night awakening
Figure 1. Experimental protocol of the four groups.
4T. Stumbrys et al.
Downloaded by [Universitaetsbibliothek Heidelberg] at 01:12 08 April 2015
Practice times and durations
A few participants in MP and PP groups slightly
diverged from the original instructions (considering
their exact practice times and durations), but the
conditions were very much similar nevertheless.
The LDP group carried out their practice on average
at 6:07 ± 2:20 h since their bed time (range: 1:20
9:00), MP at 5:49 ± 2:17 h (range: 1:208:40) and
PP at 5:51 ± 2:23 h (range: 1:209:00). The LDP
group did 4.2 ± 6.3 blocks of 30 s practice (median:
2; range: 122), MP 5.3 ± 6.4 (median: 3; range: 1
22) and PP 3.8 ± 5.3 (median: 2; range: 122).
Three practice groups did not differ in their amount
of practice (F
3,60
= 0.08, P= .927) and duration
(F
3,60
= 0.28, P= .760).
Effects of practice
All three practice groups had signicant improve-
ments from pre-test to post-test in terms of the
number of correct sequences produced, averaged
across two 30 s test intervals, whereas the control
participants had only nonsignicant improvements
(Table II). There was a signicant time (pre-test to
post-test; F
1,60
= 50.12, P< .001) but not group
(F
3,60
= 0.48, P= .695) effect. Group × time inter-
action was signicant (F
3,60
= 3.43, P= .023), show-
ing that four groups improved differently from the
pre-test to post-test. Post-hoc LSD pair-wise com-
parisons showed signicant differences between the
LDP and control group (P= .003), as well as
between the PP and control group (P= .031), but
not between other pairs of the groups. Increases in
performance from pre-test to post-test were not asso-
ciated with lucid dream frequency (rho = .100,
P= .432). Further, no signicant association
between changes in performance and the number
of trials (rho =.473, P= .055) was found in the
LDP group, where most of participants attempted
the experiment more than once. Practice times
(since the bed time) were also not signicantly asso-
ciated with changes in performance (rho = .040,
P= .785).
LDP group made on average 5.3 ± 5.2 errors on
the pre-test, MP 5.5 ± 6.7, PP 5.0 ± 8.1 and control
group 8.3 ± 13.0. On the post-test the respective
error rates were 5.6 ± 5.0, 7.1 ± 7.8, 5.7 ± 9.7 and
10.9 ± 13.3. Differences between the two tests were
signicant for control group (more errors on the
post-test; t= 3.76, P= .002) but not for practice
groups (LDP: t= 0.23, P= .820; MP: t= 1.63,
P= .125; PP: t= 0.69, P= .504). Overall differences
for the change in the error rate between the four
groups were not signicant (F
3,60
= 0.95, P= .423).
Discussion
The present study corroborates the ndings of a pilot
study by Erlacher and Schredl (2010) that motor
practice in lucid dreams enhances subsequent per-
formance. All three types of practice lead to signi-
cant improvements from the pre-test to the post-test,
whereas only a small but not statistically signicant
improvement was found in the control (no practice)
group. LDP and PP participants had signicantly
higher improvements as compared to the control
group who did only the test and retest. No signi-
cant differences were found between the three dif-
ferent practices, as well as between the MP and
control group. LDP resulted in highest average
gains (+20%), followed by PP (+17%) and MP
(+12%), however the effect size was highest for PP
(1.57), followed by MP (1.16) and LDP (0.91). All
these effect sizes are considered large (0.8) accord-
ing to Cohen (1992).
Before discussing the ndings, several methodolo-
gical issues have to be acknowledged. Firstly, the
study was conducted as a eld experiment and there-
fore the experimental control was lacking (i.e. only
the test procedure but not the experimental treat-
ment can be controlled by the experimenter online).
For example, it is possible that some participants did
not adhere to the instructions correctly. Although
instructions sent to the participants were written as
clearly as possible, in a few cases the participants
slightly diverged (e.g. by awakening for practice at a
slightly different time, by doing a somewhat different
number of practice intervals, or by forgetting to do
rest periods during LDP). To have a better experi-
mental control, sleep laboratory studies are
Table II. Effects of practice on motor task performance (number of correct sequences averaged across two 30 s periods per test).
Pre-test Post-test T-test
MsSEM M sSEM Change in % T PEffect size
Lucid dream practice 17.1 4.1 1.00 20.5 4.7 1.14 +20% 3.75 .001 0.91
Mental practice 15.4 4.9 1.27 17.3 5.6 1.45 +12% 4.46 <.001 1.16
Physical practice 16.2 5.8 1.45 18.9 6.4 1.60 +17% 6.25 <.001 1.57
Control (no practice) 17.1 6.8 1.70 17.9 7.7 1.93 +5% 1.56 .070 0.39
Note: One-tailed T-tests were used.
Effectiveness of motor practice in lucid dreams 5
Downloaded by [Universitaetsbibliothek Heidelberg] at 01:12 08 April 2015
recommended where actions in lucid dreams can be
monitored by using eye movements (cf. Erlacher
et al., 2014). Yet such sleep laboratory studies are
always affected by small sample sizes (limited usually
only to a few successful participants), because it is
very difcult to recruit procient lucid dreamers. In
the general population, only 5% of people have at
least one lucid dream a week (Schredl & Erlacher,
2011), which is necessary for sleep laboratory stu-
dies, often restricted only to a few nights. The big-
gest advantage of online eld experiments is that
they allow the recruitment of participants from all
over the world (e.g., 21 lucid dreamers who com-
pleted the present study represented 11 different
countries) and therefore samples can be much
higher. Secondly, the assignment to the experimental
groups was not completely randomised, as the LDP
group was selected by lucid dream frequency.
Improvements in performance, however, were not
associated with lucid dream frequency. Further, a
number of participants who registered for the experi-
ment did not complete it. Participation in the study
was quite demanding for MP and PP participants, as
they had to awaken at a certain time during the night
and wait for half an hour before starting to practice.
Lucid dreamers could have had an additional stress
to be successful(i.e. to be able to have a lucid
dream and practice the task in it). Thus, this might
have resulted that only certain (e.g. highly moti-
vated) individuals completed the study, and there-
fore the ndings should be interpreted cautious.
Thirdly, most participants in the LDP group and a
few participants in the other practice groups did the
experiment a few times (up to four). While each time
a different sequence was used, there is a possibility
that some transfer (positive or negative) in learning
had occurred. The possibility to have a task which
could be repeatable with a different variation was
important to increase the chances of success for
lucid dreamers for example, out of 21 lucid drea-
mers who completed the study, only 6 were success-
ful on their rst trial. Yet multiple trials did not seem
to inuence the performance: No signicant associa-
tion was found between the changes in performance
and the number of trials in LDP group. The trend,
in fact, was negative: Further trials were associated
with lesser performance improvements. Fourthly,
practice times and durations for all practice condi-
tions were derived from the lucid dreamersesti-
mates of time, which might be somewhat
inaccurate. For example, lucid dreamers and parti-
cipants in the two other intervention groups might
have considerably deviated in their actual practice
times and durations. Sleep laboratory research, how-
ever, demonstrated that time in dreams is perceived
very similarly as to when awake and while motor
actions might take longer times in lucid dreams,
this difference exists only for absolute but not for
relative durations, which is crucial for motor skill
learning (Erlacher et al., 2014). Under- and over-
estimations of motor imagery durations also happen
during MP (Guillot & Collet, 2005b; Guillot et al.,
2012). In this study, participants in all three practice
groups were asked to estimate their practice
durations.
As in the previous study (Erlacher & Schredl, 2010),
LDP was found to improve subsequent performance
in wakefulness. This group had the highest gains but
the lowest effect size, which suggests that its effects
might be more variable. The previous study (Erlacher
& Schredl, 2010) with a different aiming task found
higher improvements (+43% for LDP and +88% for
PP), but the reanalysis of the data showed that effect
sizes for practices were similar (LDP: 1.24; PP: 1.32)
and the effects of PP showed greater variability than of
LDP. Considering the results of the two studies
together, LDP appears to be similar or slightly less
effective than PP. The comparison with MP is some-
what less clear. LDP showed signicantly greater
improvements as compared to control group, whereas
MP did not signicantly differ from any of groups.
LDP resulted in somewhat higher performance gains
as compared to MP, yet somewhat lower effect size,
which might be considered a better indicator of
improvement than the percentage gains taking into
account fairly large standard deviations (Table II).
Previous studies with similar nger-tapping tasks
have shown that MP improves performance and the
gains are similar or slightly lower as compared to PP
(e.g. Debarnot, Creveaux, Collet, Doyon, & Guillot,
2009; Nyberg, Eriksson, Larsson, & Marklund, 2006).
The present study corroborates those ndings and
suggests that the effectiveness of LDP might be similar
to the effectiveness of MP, however more research is
needed.
Both lucid dream and wakeful MPs are cognitive
rehearsals with a representation of the body without
overt physical movements and both seem to some
extent to share the same neural mechanisms that
produce actual movements (Decety, 1996; Erlacher
& Schredl, 2008b), supporting the notion of func-
tional equivalence (Jeannerod, 2001). Thus, lucidly
dreamed and imagined movements would have simi-
lar functional outcomes with actual physical move-
ments, allowing motor learning to occur. A recent
functional magnetic resonance imaging/near-infra-
red spectroscopy study showed that brain activity in
the sensorimotor cortex is similar during imagined
and lucidly dreamed movement (Dresler et al.,
2011). The perception in lucid dreams, however,
seems to be much closer to actual perception and
both are quite distinct from imagination (LaBerge &
Zimbardo, 2000). In lucid dreams the simulation is
experienced as real (not just existing in imagination)
6T. Stumbrys et al.
Downloaded by [Universitaetsbibliothek Heidelberg] at 01:12 08 April 2015
and therefore it has been suggested that LDP should
be more effective than MP performed in wakeful
imagination (Tholey, 1990). The present study,
however, did not show a clear difference between
the two types of practice.
All three types of practice increased performance
speed without compromising accuracy the error
rate did not signicantly differ between the two
tests, which is consistent with previous research (cf.
Walker et al., 2002). Only the control group showed
a signicant increase in the error rate, which perhaps
might be linked to changes in motivation (i.e. less
motivated to retake the same test without any prac-
tice in between). A nger-tapping study by Debarnot
et al. (2009), for example, also found somewhat
increased error rate during the retest, although the
difference did not reach signicance.
In comparison with the earlier study (Erlacher &
Schredl, 2010), this study involved a larger sample
and practice times were matched. In the previous
study, the PP group accomplished practice in the
evening, whereas lucid dreamers practiced at some
later point at night. Sleep memory consolidation
research shows that improvements in performance
seem to be associated with various sleep parameters,
such as a higher amount of rapid eye movement
(REM) sleep (Fischer, Hallschmid, Elsner, & Born,
2002) or a greater proportion of time spent in Stage
2 sleep (Walker et al., 2002). Therefore, the PP
group might have had an advantage in the previous
study. In this study, the practice times were
matched. PP and MP participants had a small dis-
advantage to awaken at night and wait 30 min before
starting their practice. The LDP group, however,
also had to awaken after a lucid dream (to write
down the dream) and several lucid dreamers indi-
cated that they used Wake-up-Back-To-Bed method
for lucid dream induction, which requires to awaken
at night and stay awake for some time (cf. Stumbrys
et al., 2012). Thus, the conditions were comparable
and the awakenings did not seem to signicantly
disturb the sleep all groups rated their subjective
sleep quality similarly. While there are some indica-
tions that the brain seems to be more active during
lucid as compared to non-lucid REM sleep (Dresler
et al., 2012; Voss, Holzmann, Tuin, & Hobson,
2009), there appears to be no evidence in the litera-
ture about negative effects of frequent lucid dream-
ing on sleep quality.
In summary, the present study corroborates the
ndings that motor practice in lucid dreams
improves subsequent performance in wakefulness.
No signicant differences were found when compar-
ing the effectiveness of LDP to PP and MP in wake-
fulness. While further research with more complex
skills is very much needed, current research with
simple motor skills, such as nger-tapping or coin-
tossing, shows that LDP gives an additional oppor-
tunity to athletes to practice specic sport skills dur-
ing the night time when physiologically asleep. LDP
provides a more realistic simulation of the waking
environment than MP (cf. LaBerge & Zimbardo,
2000; Tholey, 1990) and could be alternatively
used when an athlete is injured, unable to practice
physically or actions are dangerous. LDP may be
more benecial for those athletes who have lucid
dreams more often but lack vivid wakeful imagina-
tion. While only a limited number of athletes have
lucid dreams on a frequent basis (Erlacher et al.,
20112012), there is a wide range of techniques
that can be used for lucid dream induction
(Stumbrys et al., 2012), yet none of them has been
veried to induce lucid dreams reliably and consis-
tently. This is one the main challenges facing lucid
dream research. Future studies should establish reli-
able techniques for lucid dream induction and exam-
ine the effects of LDP in controllable sleep
laboratory conditions.
Acknowledgement
The authors would like to thank Laurens Van Keer
from Snoozon.com for developing the online nger-
tapping application.
References
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin,112(1),
155159.
Debarnot, U., Creveaux, T., Collet, C., Doyon, J., & Guillot, A.
(2009). Sleep contribution to motor memory consolidation: A
motor imagery study. Sleep,32(12), 15591565.
Decety, J. (1996). Do imagined and executed actions share the
same neural substrate? Cognitive Brain Research,3(2), 8793.
Dresler, M., Koch, S. P., Wehrle, R., Spoormaker, V. I.,
Holsboer, F., Steiger, A., & Czisch, M. (2011). Dreamed
movement elicits activation in the sensorimotor cortex.
Current Biology,21(21), 18331837.
Dresler, M., Wehrle, R., Spoormaker, V. I., Koch, S. P.,
Holsboer, F., Steiger, A., & Czisch, M. (2012). Neural corre-
lates of dream lucidity obtained from contrasting lucid versus
non-lucid REM sleep: A combined EEG/fMRI case study.
Sleep,35(7), 10171020.
Driskell, J. E., Copper, C., & Moran, A. (1994). Does mental
practice enhance performance? Journal of Applied Psychology,79
(4), 481492.
Erlacher, D. (2007). Motorisches Lernen im luziden Traum:
Phänomenologische und experimentelle Betrachtungen [Motor
learning in lucid dreams: Phenomenological and experimental
aspects]. Saarbrücken: VDM.
Erlacher, D., Schädlich, M., Stumbrys, T., & Schredl, M. (2014).
Time for actions in lucid dreams: Effects of task modality,
length, and complexity. Frontiers in Psychology,4, 1013.
Erlacher, D., & Schredl, M. (2008a). Cardiovascular responses to
dreamed physical exercise during REM lucid dreaming.
Dreaming,18(2), 112121.
Erlacher, D., & Schredl, M. (2008b). Do REM (lucid) dreamed
and executed actions share the same neural substrate?
International Journal of Dream Research,1(1), 713.
Effectiveness of motor practice in lucid dreams 7
Downloaded by [Universitaetsbibliothek Heidelberg] at 01:12 08 April 2015
Erlacher, D., & Schredl, M. (2010). Practicing a motor task in a
lucid dream enhances subsequent performance: A pilot study.
The Sport Psychologist,24(2), 157167.
Erlacher, D., Stumbrys, T., & Schredl, M. (20112012). Frequency
of lucid dreams and lucid dream practice in German athletes.
Imagination, Cognition and Personality,31(3), 237246.
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009).
Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for corre-
lation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods,
41(4), 11491160.
Feltz, D. L., & Landers, D. M. (1983). The effects of mental
practice on motor skill learning and performance: A meta-
analysis. Journal of Sport Psychology,5(1), 2557.
Fischer, S., Hallschmid, M., Elsner, A. L., & Born, J. (2002).
Sleep forms memory for nger skills. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,99(18),
1198711991.
Guillot, A., & Collet, C. (2005a). Contribution from neurophy-
siological and psychological methods to the study of motor
imagery. Brain Research Reviews,50(2), 387397.
Guillot, A., & Collet, C. (2005b). Duration of mentally simu-
lated movement: A review. Journal of Motor Behavior,37(1),
1020.
Guillot, A., Hoyek, N., Louis, M., & Collet, C. (2012).
Understanding the timing of motor imagery: Recent ndings
and future directions. International Review of Sport and Exercise
Psychology,5(1), 322.
Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: A unifying
mechanism for motor cognition. NeuroImage,14(1), S103
S109.
Karni, A., Meyer, G., Rey-Hipolito, C., Jezzard, P., Adams, M.
M., Turner, R., & Ungerleider, L. G. (1998). The acquisition
of skilled motor performance: Fast and slow experience-driven
changes in primary motor cortex. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,95(3),
861868.
LaBerge, S. (1985). Lucid dreaming. The power of being awake and
aware in your dreams. Los Angeles, CA: Tarcher.
LaBerge, S. (1995). Prolonging lucid dreams. NightLight,7(34),
2227.
LaBerge, S., & Rheingold, H. (1990). Exploring the world of lucid
dreaming (p. 277). New York, NY: Ballantine Books.
LaBerge, S., & Zimbardo, P. (2000, April 10). Smooth tracking
eye-movements discriminate both dreaming and perception from
imagination. Abstract of talk presented at the Toward a
Science of Consciousness Conference IV, Tucson.
Lorey, B., Naumann, T., Pilgramm, S., Petermann, C., Bischoff,
M., Zentgraf, K., Munzert, J. (2013). How equivalent are
the action execution, imagery, and observation of intransitive
movements? Revisiting the concept of somatotopy during
action simulation. Brain and Cognition,81(1), 139150.
Lotze, M., & Halsband, U. (2006). Motor imagery. Journal of
Physiology Paris,99(46), 386395.
Morris, T., Spittle, M., & Watt, A. P. (2005). Imagery in sport.
Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Munzert, J., Lorey, B., & Zentgraf, K. (2009). Cognitive motor
processes: The role of motor imagery in the study of motor
representations. Brain Research Reviews,60(2), 306326.
Nyberg, L., Eriksson, J., Larsson, A., & Marklund, P. (2006).
Learning by doing versus learning by thinking: An fMRI
study of motor and mental training. Neuropsychologia,44(5),
711717.
Oldeld, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handed-
ness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia,9(1),
97113.
Richardson, A. (1967). Mental practice: A review and discussion.
Part I. Research Quarterly. American Association for Health,
Physical Education and Recreation,38(1), 95107.
Roberts, R., Callow, N., Hardy, L., Markland, D., & Bringer, J.
(2008). Movement imagery ability: Development and assess-
ment of a revised version of the vividness of movement imagery
questionnaire. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology,30(2),
200221.
Schredl, M., & Erlacher, D. (2011). Frequency of lucid dreaming
in a representative German sample. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
112(1), 104108.
Stumbrys, T., Erlacher, D., Schädlich, M., & Schredl, M. (2012).
Induction of lucid dreams: A systematic review of evidence.
Consciousness and Cognition,21(3), 14561475.
Stumbrys, T., Erlacher, D., & Schredl, M. (2013). Reliability and
stability of lucid dream and nightmare frequency scales.
International Journal of Dream Research,6(2), 5356.
Tassi, P., & Muzet, A. (2000). Sleep inertia. Sleep Medicine
Reviews,4(4), 341353.
Tholey, P. (1981). Empirische Untersuchungen über Klarträume
[Empirical study on lucid dreams]. Gestalt Theory,3(1/2),
2162.
Tholey, P. (1990). Applications of lucid dreaming in sports.
Lucidity Letter,9(2), 617.
Veale, J. F. (2014). Edinburgh handedness inventory short form: A
revised version based on conrmatory factor analysis. Laterality:
Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition,19(2), 164177.
Voss, U., Holzmann, R., Tuin, I., & Hobson, J. A. (2009). Lucid
dreaming: A state of consciousness with features of both waking
and non-lucid dreaming. Sleep,32(9), 11911200.
Walker, M. P., Brakeeld, T., Morgan, A., Hobson, J. A., &
Stickgold, R. (2002). Practice with sleep makes perfect:
Sleep-dependent motor skill learning. Neuron,35(1), 205211.
8T. Stumbrys et al.
Downloaded by [Universitaetsbibliothek Heidelberg] at 01:12 08 April 2015
... Regular moderate exercise enhances the circulation of immune cells (e.g., white blood cells, lymphocytes) that help the body fight infections and reduce the risk of illness. Physical activity helps lower systemic inflammation by reducing inflammatory markers like Creactive protein, improving immune regulation [12] . Exercise slows the age-related decline in immune function (immunesenescence), keeping the immune system more resilient in older adults. ...
... Aerobic Exercise (e.g., running, cycling, swimming) [12,23] Aerobic exercise helps promote longer periods of REM sleep, which is the sleep stage where most dreaming occurs. The extended time in REM can lead to more vivid, detailed, and memorable dreams. ...
... Sometimes, studies confirm the actuality of these and other ideas. For example, it was demonstrated that LD A WAY TO OPERATE A SMART HOME FROM LUCID DREAMS 4 might improve motor skills [24]- [26] and waking mood [27]- [29]; prevent nightmares [30], [31], help people overcome phobias, anxiety [32]- [34], and everyday life problems [35], [36]; reduce chronic pain [37] and depression [38]. In 2020, Mallett used a brain-computer interface based on electroencephalography (EEG) to reveal the possibility of controlling virtual 3D objects from LDs [39]. ...
... It is another topic that is not fully understood yet. When some studies reveal positive effects of LD practice [24], [25], [34]- [38], [26]- [33], LD methods may significantly affect sleep with unknown consequences [50], [56]. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Smart home systems facilitate humans' domestic environments and enhance their quality of life. Voice control of devices is usually available right after one awakens in the morning. In this study, we tested a way to operate smart home technologies even before awakening with the help of electromyographical (EMG) signals from dreams. A software program was tuned to translate and digitally vocalize EMG spikes into commands for the voice control of devices. One advanced lucid dreaming practitioner was trained to use these commands to operate lights, electric kettle, and a radio. The goal was to achieve the same tasks in LD under polysomnographic observation in laboratory conditions. LDs was verified by pre-agreed eye movements in REM sleep. The participant in three confirmed LDs was able to control smart home units despite sleep paralysis. It was possible to control light, the electric kettle, and radio in real-time while still being in REM sleep. The experiment demonstrated a new practical application of LD-specifically, technological everyday life tasks could be performed while asleep. In the future, the same tasks could be performed directly (without voice commands) by transmitting EMG, electroencephalography, and electrooculography patterns from LDs. With development, these technologies may open new perspectives for LD application like online shopping, entertaining, education, and jobs.
... Previous meta-analytic studies have concluded that MP programs have a positive effect on sport-specific motor skills (Lindsay et al., 2021) and motor performance (Driskell et al., 1994;Simonsmeier et al., 2021). In addition, several studies found that MP had similar results on performance and learning as PP (Doussoulin & Rehbein, 2011;Stumbrys et al., 2016;Truong et al., 2022). Moreover, additional findings suggest that the combination of MP and PP Online ISSN:2659-5699 DOI: 10.24310/jpehm.5.2.2023 can enhance performance to a greater extent compared to using PP alone (Allami et al., 2008;Behrendt et al., 2021;Hird et al., 1991;Matsuo et al., 2020;Simonsmeier et al., 2021;Wriessnegger et al., 2008) and MP alone (Lindsay et al., 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: Previous research has reached positive conclusions regarding the effects of mental practice on performance and learning of a motor skill. The purpose of this study was to use the aggregate data meta-analytic approach to assess the impact of physical practice (PP), mental practice (MP), and the combination of both on acquisition, retention, and transfer tests in motor skill performance. Methodology: Twenty-seven studies published up to 2022 were included by searching six databases. Random effects model using the standardized mean difference effect size (ES) was used to pool results. Results: A total of 42 ES, were calculated and separated into pairwise comparisons for acquisition, retention, and transfer phase. In the acquisition phase, it was found that MP was more effective than no practice (ES=0.508; n=25; CI=0.29,0.72), PP was more effective than no practice (ES=1.78; n=15; CI=0.97,2.60), CP was more effective than no practice (ES=1.16; n=12; CI=0.57,1.75), PP was more effective than MP (ES=-1.16; n=23; CI=-1.88,-0.45), PP had similar results as CP (ES=-0.01; n=16; CI=-0.31,0.28), and CP was more effective than MP (ES=0.61; n=12; CI=0.17,1.04). In the retention phase, it was found that MP was more effective than no practice (ES=1.11; n=5; CI=0.44,1.79), PP was more effective than no practice (ES=1.03; n=4; CI=0.08, 1.99), PP was more effective than MP (ES=-1.29; n=9; CI=-3.12,0.54), PP had similar results as CP (ES=0.16; n=8; CI=-0.29,0.63), CP had similar results as MP (ES=-0.06; n=3; CI=-1.22,1.09). In the transfer phase, it was found that MP was more effective than no practice (ES=1.12; n=5; CI=0.01,1.59), PP had similar results as no practice (ES=0.41; n=5; CI=-0.02,0.85), and PP was more effective than MP (ES=0.50; n=6; CI=0.12,0.87). Age, skill level, type of mental practice, total of sessions, and type of skill were considered as possible moderator variables. Conclusions: Mental practice does not replace physical practice, however, under some conditions, physical practice can be complemented with mental practice.
Article
Full-text available
In addition to being aware of being in a dream, dreamers in a lucid dream can sometimes also intentionally execute predetermined actions and manipulate the dream’s narrative. These features open the opportunity to use lucid dreaming (LD) as a recreational, therapeutic and skill acquisition tool. This study conducted semistructured interviews with nine adolescent lucid dreamers to gain insight into their LD experiences and the role these play in their waking lives. Reflexive thematic analysis revealed that LD provided participants with a space where they felt safe to explore movements and behaviors without the repercussions they would experience if they were awake. During LD they trained motor skills, addressed emotional conflicts, prepared for difficult waking life situations, and engaged in fun activities. These experiences promoted motor learning, self-efficacy, personal growth, and mental well-being. This study also highlighted that training motor skills in a lucid dream may at times heighten waking self-efficacy but not necessarily motor performance. LD brought participants a sense of freedom, liberation, and empowerment, alongside positive emotions, and a temporary release from the burdens of their waking lives. Advanced dream manipulation skills may not be mandatory for benefiting from LD, suggesting that LD may be a valuable tool for novice lucid dreamers and those with limited dream manipulation. Adolescence is a period marked by self-evaluations and the development of one’s own identity. LD may be a valuable tool to support these developmental processes and adolescents’ well-being.
Article
During lucid dreaming (LD), dreamers are aware of experiencing a dream and may consciously influence its content. This study used an online questionnaire to investigate the LD frequency and applications in 193 adolescent athletes and non-athletes (17.40 ± 2.09 years; 46% athletes, 54% controls). Given the critical role that sleep plays in adolescent health, development, and performance, associations of LD with various sleep parameters were also explored. LD is prominent in adolescents (67.4% experienced it at least once, 30.0% once a month or more, 12.9% at least once a week), but similar in terms of frequency and uses between athletes and non-athletes. A higher proportion of those who practiced sports/dance during LD reported improved waking self-efficacy (57.1%) over sport performance (42.9%). There was no indication that chronotype preference may influence LD nor that LD may be detrimental to adolescent sleep. Athletes and controls had similar sleep durations, daytime sleepiness, and sleep disturbances frequency, but athletes reported higher sleep quality. Despite, on average, meeting the minimum sleep recommendation guidelines for their age, a relatively large proportion of adolescents did not attain sufficient sleep, particularly on weeknights (47.4% 14–17 years; 20.0% 18–21 years), suggesting that restricted sleep remains prevalent in adolescent populations
Article
Empirical investigations that search for a link between dreaming and sleep-dependent memory consolidation have focused on testing for an association between dreaming of what was learned, and improved memory performance for learned material. Empirical support for this is mixed, perhaps owing to the inherent challenges presented by the nature of dreams, and methodological inconsistencies. The purpose of this paper is to address critically prevalent assumptions and practices, with the aim of clarifying and enhancing research on this topic, chiefly by providing a theoretical synthesis of existing models and evidence. Also, it recommends the method of Targeted Memory Reactivation (TMR) as a means for investigating if dream content can be linked to specific cued activations. Other recommendations to enhance research practice and enquiry on this subject are also provided, focusing on the HOW and WHY we search for memory sources in dreams, and what purpose (if any) they might serve.
Chapter
This chapter aims to elaborate on motor learning in lucid dreams. Lucid dreaming training is the systematic repetition of a sports action with the experienced “dream body” in a lucid dream, with the aim of practicing movement sequences. It does not represent a traditional training method, however, anecdotal reports, questionnaire studies, qualitative studies, and experimental investigations show that training in a lucid dream can improve movement sequences for waking life. The mode of action is explained with the simulation theory, in which cognitive movement executions are understood as a simulation of the actual movement. For lucid dreaming, there are indications at various levels that suggest that movements in the dream represent a simulation. The findings so far are thus quite promising, which allows for some concrete recommendations for sports practice to be derived.
Article
Full-text available
Lucid dreams (LDs) can be characterized by the presence of metacognition during dreams. LDs may have initially low perception vividness. In this study, we compare the effectiveness of eight techniques for increasing perception vividness, or deepening techniques (DTs), in LDs and the effectiveness of the general principles on which these DTs are based. In separate online experiments, volunteers were asked to perform each of the eight DTs during LDs (one DT per LD) and then evaluate how much perception vividness changed. A total of 858 reports were received. The most effective DT was palpation in LDs, both in general (88% of successful reports, p value < .001) and for achieving hyper-vivid perceptions (25% of all reports). The most ineffective DT was falling upside down (46% of successful reports, p value = .458). The most effective DT types were the suggestion (75% of successful reports, p value < .001) and sensory DT types (73% of successful reports, p value < .001). The least effective DT type was the vestibular DT type (53% of successful reports, p value = .411). A significant difference was found between the best and worst DTs. The results can be useful for LD practitioners. Further research should compare all other DTs and identify patterns among them to gain a better understanding of REM sleep and aid further LD research.
Article
Full-text available
Objective. In lucid dreams (LDs), it is possible to maintain consciousness and achieve predetermined goals while in REM sleep. Since the 1980s, scientists have been searching for LD application areas. The current study is the first to demonstrate a method for controlling a car avatar with feedback from LDs. Support for this possibility could be used for personal applications and LD studies. Method. Five experienced LD practitioners participated in the present study. Electromyographical (EMG) sensors were used to detect leg and arm muscle movements, and red diodes were put in front of participants’ eyes to indicate obstacles. Software converted EMG impulses into movements of a virtual car, which was chosen as the avatar. The study was conducted under laboratory conditions with polysomnographic LD verification (pre-agreed eye signals were recorded during REM sleep). Results. The participants spent one to four nights in the laboratory and induced 18 confirmed LDs. In 12 LDs, they drove a virtual car, successfully avoided obstacles, or both. All results were observed in real-time and recorded by the research team. Discussion. The results show an achievable way to control the functions of a computer program with feedback from LDs. In theory, many more options for controlling and feedback besides EMG sensors and diodes could be used. With development, all these technologies could help control more sophisticated objects in virtual and physical realms. The two-way communication from LDs with computers observed in this study could facilitate new studies because people could correct the settings and functions of devices by themselves from LDs. This would also foster communication between people while asleep.
Article
Full-text available
Lucid dream and nightmare frequencies vary greatly between individuals and reliable instruments are needed to assess these differences. The present study aimed to examine the reliability of eight-point scales for measuring lucid dream and nightmare frequencies. The scales were administered twice (with a four-week interval) to 93 sport students. A re-test reliability r=.89 (p<.001) for the lucid dream frequency was found and for the nightmare frequency r=.75 (p<.001). Both eight-point scales appear to be reliable measures for assessing individual differences in lucid dream and nightmare frequencies.
Article
Full-text available
The relationship between time in dreams and real time has intrigued scientists for centuries. The question if actions in dreams take the same time as in wakefulness can be tested by using lucid dreams where the dreamer is able to mark time intervals with prearranged eye movements that can be objectively identified in EOG recordings. Previous research showed an equivalence of time for counting in lucid dreams and in wakefulness (LaBerge, 1985; Erlacher and Schredl, 2004), but Erlacher and Schredl (2004) found that performing squats required about 40% more time in lucid dreams than in the waking state. To find out if the task modality, the task length, or the task complexity results in prolonged times in lucid dreams, an experiment with three different conditions was conducted. In the first condition, five proficient lucid dreamers spent one to three non-consecutive nights in the sleep laboratory. Participants counted to 10, 20, and 30 in wakefulness and in their lucid dreams. Lucidity and task intervals were time stamped with left-right-left-right eye movements. The same procedure was used for the second condition where eight lucid dreamers had to walk 10, 20, or 30 steps. In the third condition, eight lucid dreamers performed a gymnastics routine, which in the waking state lasted the same time as walking 10 steps. Again, we found that performing a motor task in a lucid dream requires more time than in wakefulness. Longer durations in the dream state were present for all three tasks, but significant differences were found only for the tasks with motor activity (walking and gymnastics). However, no difference was found for relative times (no disproportional time effects) and a more complex motor task did not result in more prolonged times. Longer durations in lucid dreams might be related to the lack of muscular feedback or slower neural processing during REM sleep. Future studies should explore factors that might be associated with prolonged durations.
Article
Full-text available
While the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory has been widely used, there have been few studies assessing its factorial validity. There is evidence that the original instructions and response options are difficult to understand. Using simplified instructions and response options, the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory was administered on a sample of 1514 participants using an online questionnaire. In accordance with previous research, a model of the 10-item inventory had poor fit for the data. This study also detected model misspecification in the previously-proposed 7-item modification. A 4-item Edinburgh Handedness Inventory - Short Form had good model fit with items modelled as both continuous and ordinal. Despite its brevity, it showed very good reliability, factor score determinacy, and correlation with scores on the 10-item inventory. By eliminating items that were modelled with considerable measurement error, the short form alleviates the concern of the 10-item inventory over-categorising mixed handers. Evidence was found for factorial invariance across level of education, age groups, and regions (USA and Australia/New Zealand). There generally appeared to be invariance across genders for the 4-item inventory. The proposed Edinburgh Handedness Inventory - Short Form measures a single handedness factor with an inventory that has brief and simple instructions and a small number of items.
Article
Investigations into the process of mentally practicing a perceptual motor skill began about 30 years ago. However, there has been a marked increase in the amount of interest in this area of research in the last 10 years. This is the first of two articles, which together aim to present a critical review of work so far completed, possible explanations of the phenomenon, and an indication of the directions that future research might take. In this article the evidence for improvement under conditions of mental practice is reviewed.
Article
Nocturnal dreams can be considered as a kind of simulation of the real world on a higher cognitive level. Within lucid dreams, the dreamer is able to control the ongoing dream content and is free to do what he or she wants. In this pilot study, the possibility of practicing a simple motor task in a lucid dream was studied. Forty participants were assigned to a lucid dream practice group, a physical practice group and a control group. The motor task was to toss 10-cent coins into a cup and hit as many as possible out of 20 tosses. Waking performance was measured in the evening and on the next morning by the participants at home. The 20 volunteers in the lucid dream practice group attempted to carry out the motor task in a lucid dream on a single night. Seven participants succeeded in having a lucid dream and practiced the experimental task. This group of seven showed a significant improvement in performance (from 3.7 to 5.3); the other 13 subjects showed no improvement (from 3.4 to 2.9). Comparing all four groups, the physical practice group demonstrated the highest enhancement in performance followed by the successful lucid dream practice group. Both groups had statistically significant higher improvements in contrast to the nondreaming group and the control group. Even though the experimental design is not able to explain if specific effects (motor learning) or unspecific effects (motivation) caused the improvement, the results of this study showed that rehearsing in a lucid dream enhances subsequent performance in wakefulness. To clarify the factors which increased performance after lucid dream practice and to control for confounding factors, it is suggested that sleep laboratory studies should be conducted in the future. The possibilities of lucid dream practice for professional sports will be discussed.
Book
Ch. 1. Introduction : the power of imagination -- Ch. 2. Definitions : what is imagery? -- Ch. 3. Theories : how does imagery work? -- Ch. 4. Imagery-ability and imagery-use assessment -- Ch. 5. Imagery research -- Ch. 6. Imagery perspectives -- Ch. 7. Psychophysiological research on imagery -- Ch. 8. Strategies for applying imagery -- Ch. 9. Uses for imagery -- Ch. 10. Technical aids to imagery -- Ch. 11. Injury rehabilitation and imagery -- Ch. 12. Exercise and imagery -- Ch. 13. Future directions in research and practice<br /
Article
Jeannerod (2001) hypothesized that action execution, imagery, and observation are functionally equivalent. This led to the major prediction that these motor states are based on the same action-specific and even effector-specific motor representations. The present study examined whether hand and foot movements are represented in a somatotopic manner during action execution, imagery, and action observation. The experiment contained ten conditions: three execution conditions, three imagery conditions, three observation conditions, and one baseline condition. In the nine experimental conditions, participants had to execute, observe, or imagine right-hand extension/flexion movements or right-foot extension/flexion movements. The fMRI results showed a somatotopic organization within the contralateral premotor and primary motor cortex during motor imagery and motor execution. However, there was no clear somatotopic organization of action observation in the given regions of interest within the contralateral hemisphere, although observation of these movements activated these areas significantly.