ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Research and clinical practice clearly demonstrate that many reading disabilities are language-based. Because the language deficits associated with reading disabilities are often present during the preschool years, these deficits can serve as early indicators of risk for reading disabilities. This exchange briefly reviews the language basis of reading disabilities and provides a checklist of language deficits frequently associated with reading disabilities. It is intended that this checklist be used by professionals for the early identification of reading disabilities.
CLINICAL
EXCHANGE
The
Early
Identification
of
Language-Based
Reading
Disabilities
Hugh W.
Catts
University
of
Kansas, Lawrence
raditionally,
reading/learning
disabilities
have
been
identified
primarily
on
the
basis
of
reading
problems
(Critchley,
1970;
Thomson,
1984). As
a
result,
most children
with
these disabilities
have
not
been
identified
until
they
have
entered
school
and
experienced
significant
difficulties learning
to
read.
Unfortunately,
such
reading
failure
is
frequently
accompa-
nied
by
a
host
of
negative
consequences (Spear-Swerling
&
Sternberg,
1994;
Stanovich,
1986).
Children
who
encounter
early reading
problems
often
become less
motivated
to
read,
develop
lower
expectations
of
their abilities,
and gain
less
practice
reading
than
do
good
readers. Consequently,
they
may fall
farther
and
farther behind their
peers
in
reading
and
academic
achievement.
Current research
suggests,
however,
that
children at
risk
for
reading
disabilities
can be
identified
before
experienc-
ing
failure
in
learning
to read (Badian,
McAnulty,
Duffy,
&
Als,
1990;
Catts,
1991).
This
work
indicates
that
in
many
cases, reading
disabilities
are
language-based
disorders
(Catts,
1989a;
Kamhi
&
Catts,
1989;
Wagner
&
Torgesen,
ABSTRACT:
Research
and
clinical
practice
clearly
demonstrate
that
many
reading
disabilities
are
language-
based.
Because the
language deficits
associated
with
reading
disabilities
are
often
present
during
the
preschool
years,
these
deficits
can serve
as
early
indicators
of
risk
for
reading
disabilities.
This
exchange
briefly
reviews
the
language
basis
of reading
disabilities
and
provides
a
checklist
of
language
deficits frequently
associated
with
reading
disabilities.
It is
intended
that
this
checklist
be
used
by
professionals
for
the
early
identification
of
reading
disabilities.
KEY
WORDS:
reading
disabilities,
language impairments,
phonological
processing,
early
identification
1987).
These
disorders
generally
manifest
themselves
in
terms
of
difficulties
in
oral
language development.
Children
at
risk
for
reading
disabilities
may
demonstrate
early
problems
in
expressive
morphology
or
syntax
(Bishop
&
Adams,
1990;
Scarborough,
1990).
Others
may
have
difficulties
in
understanding
words
and
sentences
(Catts,
1993;
Menyuk,
Chesnick, Liebergott,
Korngold,
D'Agostino,
&
Belanger,
1991;
Tallal,
Curtiss,
&
Kaplan,
1989).
The
latter
problems
in the
comprehension of
language
can
significantly influence
children's
ability
to
understand
what
they
read.
Written
language
is
highly
decontextualized
and
its comprehension
relies heavily
on
a
well-developed
vocabulary
and
a
clear
understanding of
the
structural
components
(e.g.,
function
words)
and
rules
of
language
(e.g.,
syntax,
pragmatics).
Some
children
at
risk
for
reading
disabilities
will
not
show
early
difficulties
in
expressive
language
or compre-
hension. However,
many
of
these children
will
have
problems
in
what
has
been
termed phonological processing
(Catts,
1989b;
Wagner
&
Torgesen,
1987).
These
problems
include
a
lack
of
sensitivity
or
awareness
of
the
speech
sounds
in
words (e.g.,
phonological awareness)
(Bradley
&
Bryant,
1983;
Fletcher
et
al., 1994).
Phonological
process-
ing
deficits further
include problems
in word
retrieval
(Badian,
McAnulty,
Duffy,
&
Als,
1990;
Wolf,
1984),
verbal
short-term
memory
(Torgesen,
1985),
and
speech
production
(Catts,
1989c;
Snowling,
1981).
These various
deficits
have
been
shown
to
be
closely related
to
children's
ability
to
learn
to
recognize printed
words.
A
lack
of
awareness
of
the sounds
in
words
and/or
difficulties
storing
and
retrieving phonological information
may
impact
children's
ability
to
learn
sound-letter correspondence
and
its
use
in
decoding
printed
words
(Stanovich,
1988).
Most
of
the
language
deficits
described
above
can
be
observed
in
at-risk children
before
the
beginning
of
formal
reading
instruction.
As
a
result,
these deficits
may serve
as
86
LANGUAGE,
SPEECH,
AND
HEARING
SERVICES
IN
SCHOOLS
*
Vol.
28
0161-1461/97/2801-0086
American
Speech-Language-Hearing
Association
early
indicators
of
a
potential
reading
disability
and allow
us
to
identify children
who
are at
risk
before reading
instruction.
Once
identified,
early
intervention
may help
reduce reading
failure
and
the
negative consequences
of
this
failure
(Fey,
Catts,
&
Larrivee,
1995).
The
checklist
(Appendix)
is
designed
to
assist
teachers,
special educators,
psychologists,
speech-language patholo-
gists,
and
parents
in
the
identification
of
the
speech/
language problems
that
may
be indicative
of
risk
for
reading disabilities.
The
checklist
is
designed
to
be
used
with
children
at
the
end
of
kindergarten
or
beginning
of
first
grade.
Some
of
the
descriptors,
however,
may
also
characterize
older
children
with
language-based reading
problems.
No
single
descriptor
will
conclusively
identify
a
child
as
being
at
risk. However,
the
more
descriptors
that
are
checked, the more
likely
it
is
that
the
child
may
experience difficulties
in
learning to
read.
In
the case
of
a
child
receiving
a
large
number
of
checks,
a
full
evaluation
should
be
carried out.
See
Catts
(1996)
for
suggestions
for
diagnostic
procedures and
materials
for this
evaluation.
REFERENCES
Badian,
N.,
McAnulty,
G.,
Duffy,
F.,
&
Als,
H. (1990).
Prediction of
dyslexia
in
kindergarten boys.
Annals
of
Dyslexia,
41,
221-243.
Bishop,
D.,
&
Adams,
C.
(1990).
A
prospective
study
of
the
relationship
between specific language impairments, phonologi-
cal disorders,
and
reading
retardation.
Journal
of
Child
Psychology
and
Psychiatry,
21,
1027-1050.
Bradley,
L.,
&
Bryant,
P.
(1983).
Categorizing sounds
and
learning
to
read:
A
causal
connection.
Nature,
30,
788-790.
Catts,
H.
(1989a).
Defining dyslexia
as
a
developmental language
disorder.
Annals
of
Dyslexia,
39,
50-64.
Catts,
H.
(1989b).
Phonological
processing
deficits
and
reading
disabilities.
In A.
Kamhi
&
H.
Catts
(Eds.),
Reading
disabilities:
A
developmental
language
perspective.
Boston,
MA:
Allyn
&
Bacon.
Catts,
H.
(1989c). Speech
production deficits
in
developmental
dyslexia.
Journal
of
Speech
and
Hearing
Disorders,
54,
422-428.
Catts,
H.
(1991).
Early
identification
of
reading
disabilities.
Topics
in
Language
Disorders,
12,
1-16.
Catts,
H.
(1993).
The
relationship
between speech-language
impairments
and
reading
disabilities.
Journal
of
Speech
and
Hearing
Research,
36,
948-958.
Catts,
H.
(1996).
Defining dyslexia
as
a
developmental language
disorder:
An
expanded
view.
Topics
in
Language
Disorders,
16,
14-29.
Critchley, M.
(1970).
The
dyslexic
child.
London:
Heineman
Medical Books.
Fey,
M., Catts,
H.,
&
Larrivee,
L.
(1995).
Preparing
preschoolers
for
the
academic
and
social challenges
of
school.
In
M.
Fey,
J.
Windsor,
&
S.
Warren
(Eds.),
Language intervention:
Preschool
through
the
elementary
years
(pp.
3-37).
Baltimore,
MD:
Paul
Brooks.
Fletcher,
J.,
Shaywitz,
S.,
Shankweiler,
D.,
Katz,
L.
Liberman,
I., Stuebing,
K.,
Francis,
D.,
Fowler,
A.,
&
Shaywitz,
B.
(1994).
Cognitive
profiles
of
reading
disability: Comparisons
of
discrepancy
and
low
achievement
definitions.
Journal
of
Educational
Psychology,
86,
6-23.
Kamhi,
A.,
&
Catts, H.
(1989).
Reading
disabilities:
A
develop-
mental
language perspective.
Boston,
MA:
Allyn
&
Bacon.
Menyuk,
P.,
Chesnick,
M.,
Liebergott,
J.,
Korngold,
B.,
D'Agostino,
R.,
&
Belanger,
A.
(1991).
Predicting reading
problems
in
at-risk
children.
Journal
of
Speech
and
Hearing
Research,
34,
893-903.
Scarborough,
H.
(1990).
Very
early
language
deficits
in
dyslexic
children.
Child
Development,
61,
1728-1743.
Snowling,
M.
(1981).
Phonemic
deficits
in
developmental
dyslexia.
Psychological
Research,
43,
219-234.
Spear-Swerling,
L.,
&
Sternberg,
R.
(1994). The
road not taken:
An
integrative
theoretical
model
of
reading
disabilities.
Journal
of
Learning
Disabilities,
27,
91-103.
Stanovich,
K.
(1986).
"Matthew effects"
in reading:
Some
consequences
of
individual
differences
in
the
acquisition
of
literacy.
Reading Research
Quarterly,
21,
360-407.
Stanovich,
K.
(1988).
The
right
and wrong
places
to
look
for
the
locus
of
reading disabilities.
Annals
of
Dyslexia,
38.
154-177.
Tallal,
P.,
Curtiss,
S.,
&
Kaplan,
R.
(1989).
The
San
Diego
longitudinal
study:
Evaluating
the
outcomes
of
preschool
impairments
in
language
development. Final report
NINCDS.
Washington,
DC.
Thomson,
M.
(1984).
Developmental dyslexia:
Its
nature,
assessment,
and
remediation.
London:
Edward
Arnold.
Torgesen,
J.
(1985).
Memory
processes in reading disordered
children.
Journal
of
Learning
Disabilities,
18,
35-357.
Wagner,
R.,
&
Torgesen,
J.
(1987). The
nature
of
phonological
processing
and
its
causal
role
in
the
acquisition
of
reading
skills.
Psychological
Bulletin,
101,
199-212.
Wolf,
M.
(1984). Naming,
reading,
and
the dyslexias:
A
longitudi-
nal
overview.
Annals
of
Dyslexia,
34,
87-136.
Received
July
12,
1995
Accepted
November
27,
1995
Contact
author:
Hugh
W.
Catts, Speech-Language-Hearing:
Sciences
and
Disorders,
The
University
of
Kansas,
3031
Dole,
Lawrence,
KS
66045-2181.
Catts
87
APPENDIX
A.
EARLY
IDENTIFICATION
OF
LANGUAGE-BASED
READING
DISABILITIES:
A
CHECKLIST
'
Child's
Name:_
___
Birthday:
Date
Completed:
Age:
This
checklist
is
designed
to
identify children
who
are at
risk
for
language-based
reading
disabilities.
It is
intended
for
use
with
children
at the
end
of
kindergarten or
beginning
of
first
grade. Each
of
the
descriptors
listed
below
should
be
carefully
considered
and
those
that
characterize
the
child's
behavior/history
should
be
checked.
A
child
receiving
a
large number
of
checks
should
be
referred for
a
more
in-depth evaluation.
Speech Sound
Awareness
L
doesn't
understand
and
enjoy
rhymes
J
doesn't
easily recognize
that
words
may
begin
with
the
same
sound
JL
has
difficulty counting
the
syllables
in
spoken words
U
has
problem
clapping
hands
or
tapping
feet
in
rhythm
with
songs
and/or rhymes
IJ
demonstrates
problems
learning
sound-letter
correspondences
Word
Retrieval
I
has
difficulty
retrieving
a
specific
word
(e.g.,
calls
a
sheep
a
"goat"
or
says
"you
know,
a
woolly animal")
LO
shows
poor
memory
for classmates'
names
I
speech
is
hesitant,
filled
with
pauses
or
vocalizations
(e.g.,
"um,"
"you
know")
Li
frequently
uses
words
lacking
specificity
(e.g.,
"stuff," "thing,"
"what
you
call
it")
L
has
a
problem
remembering/retrieving
verbal sequences
(e.g.,
days
of
the
week,
alphabet)
Verbal
Memory
LI
has
difficulty
remembering
instructions or
directions
LO
shows
problems learning
names
of
people
or
places
i
has
difficulty
remembering
the
words
to
songs or poems
LO
has
problems learning
a
second
language
Speech
Production/Perception
LO
has
problems
saying common
words
with
difficult
sound
patterns
(e.g.,
animal,
cinnamon, specific)
Li
mishears
and
subsequently mispronounces
words
or
names
I
confuses
a
similar
sounding
word
with
another
word
(e.g.,
saying
"The
Entire
State
Building
is
in
New
York")
L
combines sound
patterns
of similar
words (e.g.,
saying
"escavator" for
escalator)
L
shows
frequent
slips
of
the
tongue
(e.g.,
saying
"brue
blush"
for
blue
brush.)
Li
has
difficulty
with
tongue
twisters
(e.g., she
sells
seashells)
Comprehension
L
only
responds
to
part of
a
multiple element request
or
instruction
L
requests multiple repetitions
of
instructions/directions
with
little
improvement
in
comprehension
L
relies too
much
on
context
to
understand
what
is
said
II
has
difficulty
understanding questions
L
fails
to
understand age-appropriate
stories
L
has
difficulty
making inferences,
predicting
outcomes, drawing
conclusions
L
lacks
understanding
of
spatial
terms
such
as
left-right,
front-back
Expressive
Language
L
talks
in
short
sentences
O
makes
errors
in
grammar
(e.g.,
"he
goed
to
the
store"
or
"me
want
that")
O
lacks
variety
in
vocabulary
(e.g.,
uses
"good"
to mean
happy,
kind,
polite)
II
has
difficulty
giving directions
or
explanations
(e.g., may
show
multiple revisions
or
dead
ends)
L
relates
stories or events
in
a
disorganized
or
incomplete
manner
L
may have
much
to say,
but provides little
specific
detail
L
has
difficulty
with
the
rules
of
conversation,
such
as
turn taking,
staying
on
topic,
indicating
when
he/she
does
not
understand
January
1997
88
LANGUAGE,
SPEECH,
AND
HEARING
SERVICES
IN
SCHOOLS
Vol.
28
Other
Important
Factors
OI has
a
prior
history
of
problems
in
language comprehension and/or production
L
has
a
family
history
of
spoken or
written
language
problems
[O
has
limited
exposure
to
literacy
in
the home
O]
lacks
interest
in
books
and
shared
reading
activities
O
does
not
engage
readily
in
pretend
play
Comments
This
checklist
was
prepared
by
Hugh
W.
Catts,
University
of
Kansas. Some
descriptors
have
been
taken
from
Language
for
Learning:
A
Checklist
for
Language
Difficulties,
Melbourne, Australia:
OZ
Child.
The
American Speech-Language-Hearing
Association
grants
permission
to photocopy this
checklist for
professional
use.
Catts
89
... Early language difficulties indicate risk for dyslexia (Catts, 1997), and many children with language impairment have poor phonological awareness (Zourou et al., 2010) that results in poor decoding skills (Catts et al., 2005). Up to 84% of children who receive speech-language services in schools have co-occurring characteristics of dyslexia (McArthur et al., 2000;Werfel & Krimm, 2017). ...
... If a primary "red flag" that parents and teachers look for is, in fact, not a red flag at all, many children who present with actual red flags (e.g., phonemic awareness deficits and other spoken language difficulties but not letter reversals) may be overlooked for referral. SLPs need to ensure that parents and educators are aware that early language difficulties often precede identification of dyslexia (Catts, 1997). ...
Article
Purpose: The purpose of this exploratory study was to evaluate speech-language pathologists' (SLPs') conceptions and misconceptions about dyslexia. Method: Participants were 86 school-based SLPs. They completed an online survey on which they rated their agreement and disagreement with true and false statements related to the scientific evidence about the nature of dyslexia and interventions for dyslexia, as well as common misconceptions about dyslexia. Results: There was considerable variability among SLPs' agreement and disagreement with the statements. Critically, despite abundant contrary evidence in the literature, many SLPs believe that dyslexia involves a visual processing deficit. Conclusions: These findings suggest that many school-based SLPs hold misconceptions about dyslexia, especially those related to dyslexia being a visual disorder. The identified misconceptions may contribute to some SLPs' reluctance to incorporate reading and prereading skills into speech-language assessment and intervention. SLPs need greater knowledge of dyslexia to provide more effective evaluations and intervention services.
... TD = typically developing children. (Catts, 1997;Ha et al., 2019;Kim, 2014;Kim et al., 2007;Kim et al., 2020;Pae, 1996;Paul et al., 2017 ...
Article
Objectives: This study aimed to develop the “Speech Language Early Literacy Screening Checklist” in order to identify speech, language, and early literacy difficulties in children aged 5-6 years before entering elementary school. This study examined the reliability, validity, sensitivity, specificity, positive predictive value, and negative predictive value to verify the accuracy and efficiency of the checklist as a screening tool.Methods: Out of the 2,880 children who participated in the initial screening test, 155 underwent diagnostic deep tests. A total of 110 children who exhibited difficulties in language, speech, and early literacy areas were classified as the high-risk group, while those who demonstrated appropriate development in all areas were classified as the general group.Results: The Cronbach’s α value for all checklist items was .884. The content validity index rated by six language development experts was 0.70 or higher for all 36 items, with an average content validity score of 0.91. Clinical validity was confirmed through the total score differences between the general and high-risk groups classified by the diagnostic deep tests. The receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve results for the screening checklist showed an area under the curve (AUC) of 0.858, sensitivity of 84.4%, and specificity of 76.4%. The positive predictive value (PPV) calculated from the cut-off score was 92.3%.Conclusion: The “Speech Language Early Literacy Screening Checklist” developed to identify children at high risk of language development delays before school entry has shown appropriate reliability, validity, and classification accuracy. This study suggests that the implementation of this checklist will contribute to preventing school failure by effectively identifying at-risk children aged 5-6.
... As principals become more knowledgeable about dyslexia and appropriate intervention, they are better able to recognize the aspects of intervention that are necessary to improve skills in students with dyslexia. First, principals should encourage identification of students with dyslexia or reading disabilities as early as possible because early intervention proves important in the remediation of difficulties that students face in academics (Catts, 1993(Catts, , 1997Ferrer et al., 2015;IDA, 2017a;Lyon & Chhabra, 2004;Poulsen, 2018;Snowling & Hulme, 2012;Walsh et al., 2006;Washburn et al., 2013). See Principal recommendation number 5 (Table 9). ...
Article
Kindergarten through second‐grade elementary schools that best serve students with dyslexia have principals who are knowledgeable about dyslexia and understand the best practices for providing intervention for students with dyslexia. In this study, three styles of leadership were examined to understand the implication that leadership has on intervention for dyslexia: transformational, instructional, and integrated leadership. However, many students in elementary schools have difficulty learning to read despite good leadership by the principal, with 5–20% of students being diagnosed with dyslexia. While these students need phonetic, multisensory intervention to build necessary reading skills, this study found that many principals lack knowledge of this specialized instruction. The purpose of this research was to explore variables that determine the school‐based level of appropriate intervention for students with dyslexia. A questionnaire assessing leadership skills, knowledge, and beliefs about dyslexia, preparation in reading disorders and/or dyslexia received from degree programs and professional development, and services provided to students with dyslexia was given to K‐2 principals serving in schools across the United States. Results indicated that regardless of leadership style, principals who have greater knowledge and more correct beliefs about dyslexia provide more appropriate school‐based services for students with dyslexia. Eight detailed K‐2 principal/practitioner recommendations are included based upon this key finding.
... The preceding work by Abu-Rabia (2000) and Saeigh-Haddad et al. (2003a, 2003b has aligned with studies on oral language and comprehension that were not specific to the Arabic language. Both Catts (1997) and Snowling (2005) have demonstrated that difficulties in oral language were predictors of future deficiencies in reading comprehension, resulting from limited phonological awareness, difficulties with word retrieval, poor verbal retention, and limited speech production or perception. Additionally, mild difficulties in oral language have resulted in mild difficulties in reading while major oral language deficiency eISSN: 2550-2131 ISSN: 1675-8021 192 severely impeded reading comprehension (Kaiser, Roberts, & McLeod, 2011). ...
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study is to investigate the effects of Arabic diglossia on the development of classical Arabic language acquisition amongst bilingual learners in a private school in Lebanon. The study compares the Arabic language (L1) performance to that of English as a Foreign Language (EFL) in accordance with Bialystok’s model. One hundred-forty participants; (n=140), ranged from five to eleven years of age, with a mean of eight years were sampled. A two-stage random sampling technique was applied, while ensuring that students with contrastive academic achievement were included within the study. The participants sampled for this correlational research were learners enrolled at a private middle school. Participants were given two standardized measures so as to establish the trend of development in oral skills for both classical and colloquial Arabic, determine the grade level at which convergence occurs between the two forms of acquisition and finally compare the degree of classical Arabic (L1) acquisition with respect to their EFL acquisition. The findings indicated interrelatedness between Arabic diglossia and the late oral development of classical Arabic, whereas participants showed a higher degree of comfort with English than with their mother language. Recommendations for future directions and research are also given.
... The preceding work by Abu-Rabia (2000) and Saeigh-Haddad et al. (2003a, 2003b has aligned with studies on oral language and comprehension that were not specific to the Arabic language. Both Catts (1997) and Snowling (2005) have demonstrated that difficulties in oral language were predictors of future deficiencies in reading comprehension, resulting from limited phonological awareness, difficulties with word retrieval, poor verbal retention, and limited speech production or perception. Additionally, mild difficulties in oral language have resulted in mild difficulties in reading while major oral language deficiency eISSN: 2550-2131 ISSN: 1675-8021 5 severely impeded reading comprehension (Kaiser, Roberts, & McLeod, 2011). ...
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study is to investigate the effects of Arabic diglossia on the development of classical Arabic language acquisition amongst bilingual learners in a private school in Lebanon. The study compares the Arabic language (L1) performance to that of English as a Foreign Language (EFL) in accordance with Bialystok's model. One hundred-forty participants; (n=140), ranged from five to eleven years of age, with a mean of eight years were sampled. A two-stage random sampling technique was applied, while ensuring that students with contrastive academic achievement were included within the study. The participants sampled for this correlational research were learners enrolled at a private middle school. Participants were given two standardized measures so as to establish the trend of development in oral skills for both classical and colloquial Arabic, determine the grade level at which convergence occurs between the two forms of acquisition and finally compare the degree of classical Arabic (L1) acquisition with respect to their EFL acquisition. The findings indicated interrelatedness between Arabic diglossia and the late oral development of classical Arabic, whereas participants showed a higher degree of comfort with English than with their mother language. Recommendations for future directions and research are also given.
Article
Bu çalışmada, öğrenme güçlüğü riski taşıyan çocukların erken evrede tespit edilebilmesi amacıyla, Öğrenme Güçlüğü Erken Belirtileri Tarama Ölçeği (ÖGEBTÖ)’nün geliştirilmesine ve bu ölçeğin psikometrik özelliklerinin kapsamlı bir şekilde incelenmesine yönelik bir çalışma gerçekleştirilmiştir. Okul öncesi dönemde öğrenme güçlüğü olabilecek öğrencilerin belirlenmesi erken müdahale için önemlidir. Bu çalışmanın yapılmasının temel nedeni, Türkiye'de öğrenme güçlüğünün erken belirtilerini kapsamlı bir şekilde değerlendiren bir ölçeğin bulunmamasıdır. Bu araştırma, geriye dönük betimsel tarama deseninde gerçekleştirilen bir ölçek geliştirme çalışmasıdır. Araştırmaya çocuğu öğrenme güçlüğü olan toplam 1429 öğrenci velisi katılmıştır. Gerçekleştirilen geçerlik ve güvenirlik analizlerinin sonuçları, ÖGEBTÖ geçerli ve güvenilir bir araç olarak kabul edilmesi için yeterli kanıt sağlamaktadır. Bu araştırmada, ÖGEBTÖ; Dil Gelişimi ve İletişim, Bilişsel Beceriler, Psikomotor Gelişim Becerileri ve Sosyal Duygusal Beceriler şeklinde dört alt faktörden ve 52 maddeden oluşmuştur. Bulgular literatür ölçütlerine göre değerlendirip tartışılmıştır. Ülkemizde öğrenme güçlüğü erken belirtileri gelişimsel belirtileri tarayan standardize olan araç ilk araçtır.
Article
Background Awareness of stuttering is likely to depend upon the development of the metalinguistic skill to discriminate between fluent speech and stuttering and the ability to identify one’s own speech as fluent or stuttered. Presently, little is known about these abilities in individuals with Down syndrome (DS). Purpose This study investigates whether individuals with DS and typically developing (TD) children who stutter and who do not stutter differ in their ability to discriminate between fluent speech and stuttering. The second purpose of this study is to discover if this ability is correlated with their self-identification ability. Method An experiment to investigate awareness with tasks for discrimination of stuttering and self-identification was developed. It was administered to 28 individuals (7-19 years) with DS, 17 of them stutter and 11 do not, and 20 TD children (3-10 years), 8 of them stutter and 12 do not. Skills to discriminate stuttering were compared between these groups and correlated with self-identification within these groups. The influence of stuttering severity and developmental/chronological age on their ability to discriminate was also investigated. Results The ability to discriminate does not differ significantly between the DS and TD group, but is highly influenced by developmental age. This ability correlates with self-identification but only for the TD individuals who speak fluently. Conclusion The ability to discriminate matures around the age of 7 and conscious awareness may rely on this ability. Differences between the present findings and earlier studies suggest that differentiation in levels and types of awareness is warranted.
Book
Full-text available
يلقى هذا الكتاب الضوء علي العمليات الفونولوجية ويعرفها ويشرحها ويوضح العلاقة بينها وبين القراءة والكتابة وصعوباتهما وذلك لتعرف كيفية قياسها وكيفية تعرف المشكلات المحددة المتعلقة بها وكيفية التغلب علي تلك الصعوبات لتحسين عملية القراءة والكتابة والتخلص من الصعوبات الخاصة، يشتمل هذا الكتاب علي
Article
Full-text available
Three bodies of research that have developed in relative isolation center on each of three kinds of phonological processing: phonological awareness, awareness of the sound structure of language; phonological recoding in lexical access, recoding written symbols into a sound-based representational system to get from the written word to its lexical referent; and phonetic recoding in working memory, recoding written symbols into a sound-based representational system to maintain them efficiently in working memory. In this review we integrate these bodies of research and address the interdependent issues of the nature of phonological abilities and their causal roles in the acquisition of reading skills. Phonological ability seems to be general across tasks that purport to measure the three kinds of phonological processing, and this generality apparently is independent of general cognitive ability. However, the generality of phonological ability is not complete, and there is an empirical basis for distinguishing phonological awareness and phonetic recoding in working memory. Our review supports a causal role for phonological awareness in learning to read, and suggests the possibility of similar causal roles for phonological recoding in lexical access and phonetic recoding in working memory. Most researchers have neglected the probable causal role of learning to read in the development of phonological skills. It is no longer enough to ask whether phonological skills play a causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. The question now is which aspects of phonological processing (e.g., awareness, recoding in lexical access, recoding in working memory) are causally related to which aspects of reading (e.g., word recognition, word analysis, sentence comprehension), at which point in their codevelopment, and what are the directions of these causal relations?
Article
Full-text available
This study followed 163 boys from kindergarten through fourth grade. A battery of neurospychological and preacademic tests and electrophysiological measures (BEAM) were administered in kindergarten, and reading tests at grade 4, in an attempt to delineate precursors of dyslexia. Three of the kindergarten tasks (giving sounds associated with letters, rapid naming of numbers, and finger localization) differentiated dyslexics from normal readers with 98 percent correct classification. The tasks primarily involve grapheme-phoneme associations, storage and retrieval of phonological information in long-term memory, and verbal labeling. Results are interpreted as confirming the role played by phonological processing tasks in the prediction of dyslexia. Preliminary BEAM results for visual evoked potential topography suggest a significant increment in the distribution of this potential in the left parietal and frontal region, and, for auditory evoked potential topography, a significant difference between the two groups in the right posterior hemisphere.
Article
Full-text available
Examined the validity of distinguishing children with reading disabilities according to discrepancy and low-achievement definitions by obtaining 4 assessments of expected reading achievement and 2 assessments of actual reading achievement for 199 children (aged 7.5–9.5 yrs). These assessments were used to subdivide the sample into discrepancy and low-achievement definitional groups who were compared on 9 cognitive variables related to reading proficiency. Results did not support the validity of discrepancy vs low achievement definitions. Although differences between Ss with impaired reading and Ss without impaired reading were large, differences between Ss with impaired reading who met IQ-based discrepancy definitions and those who met low reading achievement definitions were small or not significant. Measures of phonological awareness were robust indicators of differences between Ss with impaired reading and Ss without impaired reading regardless of how reading disability was defined. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
A FRAMEWORK for conceptualizing the development of individual differences in reading ability is presented that synthesizes a great deal of the research literature. The framework places special emphasis on the effects of reading on cognitive development and on "bootstrapping" relationships involving reading. Of key importance are the concepts of reciprocal relationships-situations where the causal connection between reading ability and the efficiency of a cognitive process is bidirectional-and organism-environment correlation-the fact that differentially advantaged organisms are exposed to nonrandom distributions of environmental quality. Hypotheses are advanced to explain how these mechanisms operate to create rich-getricher and poor-get-poorer patterns of reading achievement. The framework is used to explicate some persisting problems in the literature on reading disability and to conceptualize remediation efforts in reading.
Article
In research in the cognitive and neurosciences, a co-occurrence between naming and reading disorders has been found in children and aphasic adults. Evidence from a completed cross-sectional study will be briefly summarized and an ongoing longitudinal study will be presented to suggest that factors disrupting specific stages of the naming process can impede the development of children's reading in particular, perhaps predictable, ways. Based on the components of a neurolinguistic model of naming, a battery of naming and reading tests was administered to a longitudinal sample of 115 children before, during, and after reading acquisition. Preliminary trends indicate that poor readers are significantly different (p<.001) from average readers on all naming tests except those emphasizingreceptive vocabulary perception. Tests emphasizing retrieval rate are best able to predict patterns of naming performance and errors characterize specific subgroups of the dyslexias.
Article
A coherent conception of dyslexia has been difficult to arrive at because research findings have continually created logical paradoxes for the psychometric definition of reading disability. One such paradox is that cognitive research has undermined the assumption of specificity in the definition of dyslexia. The assumption of specificity is the underlying premise that the cognitive problems characteristic of the dyslexic child are reasonably specific to the reading task and do not implicate broader domains of cognitive functioning. This paper demonstrates how to develop hypotheses about the cognitive deficits of dyslexic children that do not undermine the assumption of specificity. Phonological awareness is explored as one candidate process.It is argued that in order to avoid the pitfalls surrounding the assumption of specificity three things are important: 1) We must understand the operation of Matthew effects in education (rich-get-richer and poor-get-poorer effects); 2) Strict psychometric criteria must be used to define dyslexia; 3) We must continually be aware that dyslexia does not demarcate a discrete category, separate from other groups of poor readers, but represents the outcome of the application of an arbitrary criterion in a continuous distribution.
Article
Despite recent developments in research and theory, investigators and practitioners continue to rely on rather traditional definitions of dyslexia. This paper discusses some of the problems with traditional definitions and reviews a rapidly growing body of research that suggests a more comprehensive definition. According to this definition, dyslexia is a developmental language disorder that involves a deficit(s) in phonological processing. This disorder manifests itself in various phonological difficulties as well as a specific reading disability.
Article
Tested the hypothesis that the experiences that a child has with rhyme before he/she goes to school might have an effect on later success in learning to read and write. Two experimental situations were used: a longitudinal study and an intensive training program in sound categorization or other forms of categorization. 368 children's skills at sound categorization were measured before they started to read and then related to their progress in reading, spelling, and mathematics over 4 yrs. At the end of initial testing and during the 4 yrs Ss' IQ, reading, spelling, and mathematical abilities were tested. There were high correlations between initial sound categorization scores and Ss' reading and spelling over 3 yrs. At the onset of study, 65 Ss who could not read and had low sound-categorization skills were divided into 4 groups. Two received 2 yrs of training in categorizing sounds. Group 1 was taught that the same word shared common beginning, middle, and end sounds with other words and could be categorized in different ways. Group 2 was also taught how each common sound was represented by a letter of the alphabet. The other groups served as controls. Group 3 was taught only that the same word could be classified in several ways. At the end of training, Group 1 was ahead of Group 3 and Group 2 was ahead of Group 1 in reading and spelling. This suggests that training in sound categorization is more effective when it also involves an explicit connection with the alphabet. Results support the hypothesis. (5 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Dyslexia has recently been defined as a developmental language disorder (Catts, 1989a; Kamhi & Catts, 1989). In this article, the language basis of dyslexia and the current research that supports and extends it are reviewed. Research continues to indicate that phonological processing deficits lie at the core of dyslexia. However, recent evidence also suggests that these deficits may underlie the difficulties of many poor readers who do not meet the traditional definitions of dyslexia (i.e., low achievers). Rather than collapse dyslexics and low achievers into a single diagnostic category, it is argued that these groups or groups like them may be differentiated on the basis of higher-level language abilities. The role of higher-level language functioning and phonological processing in reading is further considered in an expanded view of the language basis of reading disabilities. The implications this view has for the early identification and remediation of reading disabilities are also presented. (C) 1996 Aspen Publishers, Inc.