ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

When people rapidly judge the truth of claims presented with or without related but nonprobative photos, the photos tend to inflate the subjective truth of those claims-a "truthiness" effect (Newman et al., 2012). For example, people more often judged the claim "Macadamia nuts are in the same evolutionary family as peaches" to be true when the claim appeared with a photo of a bowl of macadamia nuts than when it appeared alone. We report several replications of that effect and 3 qualitatively new findings: (a) in a within-subjects design, when people judged claims paired with a mix of related, unrelated, or no photos, related photos produced truthiness but unrelated photos had no significant effect relative to no photos; (b) in a mixed design, when people judged claims paired with related (or unrelated) and no photos, related photos produced truthiness and unrelated photos produced "falseness;" and (c) in a fully between design, when people judged claims paired with either related, unrelated, or no photos, neither truthiness nor falsiness occurred. Our results suggest that photos influence people's judgments when a discrepancy arises in the expected ease of processing, and also support a mechanism in which-against a backdrop of an expected standard-related photos help people generate pseudoevidence to support claims. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2015 APA, all rights reserved).
Content may be subject to copyright.
Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Learning, Memory, and Cognition
Truthiness and Falsiness of Trivia Claims Depend on
Judgmental Contexts
Eryn J. Newman, Maryanne Garry, Christian Unkelbach, Daniel M. Bernstein, D. Stephen
Lindsay, and Robert A. Nash
Online First Publication, March 30, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000099
CITATION
Newman, E. J., Garry, M., Unkelbach, C., Bernstein, D. M., Lindsay, D. S., & Nash, R. A. (2015,
March 30). Truthiness and Falsiness of Trivia Claims Depend on Judgmental Contexts.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. Advance online
publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000099
Truthiness and Falsiness of Trivia Claims Depend on Judgmental Contexts
Eryn J. Newman and Maryanne Garry
Victoria University of Wellington Christian Unkelbach
University of Cologne
Daniel M. Bernstein
Kwantlen Polytechnic University D. Stephen Lindsay
University of Victoria
Robert A. Nash
University of Surrey
When people rapidly judge the truth of claims presented with or without related but nonprobative
photos, the photos tend to inflate the subjective truth of those claims—a “truthiness” effect (Newman
et al., 2012). For example, people more often judged the claim “Macadamia nuts are in the same
evolutionary family as peaches” to be true when the claim appeared with a photo of a bowl of
macadamia nuts than when it appeared alone. We report several replications of that effect and 3
qualitatively new findings: (a) in a within-subjects design, when people judged claims paired with
a mix of related, unrelated, or no photos, related photos produced truthiness but unrelated photos had
no significant effect relative to no photos; (b) in a mixed design, when people judged claims paired
with related (or unrelated) and no photos, related photos produced truthiness and unrelated photos
produced “falseness;” and (c) in a fully between design, when people judged claims paired with
either related, unrelated, or no photos, neither truthiness nor falsiness occurred. Our results suggest
that photos influence people’s judgments when a discrepancy arises in the expected ease of
processing, and also support a mechanism in which—against a backdrop of an expected standard—
related photos help people generate pseudoevidence to support claims.
Keywords: cognitive fluency, photographs, truth judgments, truthiness
Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000099.supp
True or false: The liquid metal inside a thermometer is
magnesium. If you are like the participants in our studies, you
will find this judgment difficult. The cognitive psychology
literature suggests that you will try to arrive at a judgment about
the claim by retrieving information from memory—related
thoughts (“Which metals are liquid?”) and images (your high
school chemistry teacher)—to help you decide (Graesser &
Hemphill, 1991). The literature also suggests that you will
probably search for information confirming the hypothesis that
the claim is true (Gilbert, 1991; Nickerson, 1998). If your
retrieved thoughts and images are not diagnostic for a true/false
judgment, you may make a feeling-based judgment instead
(Reber & Unkelbach, 2010; see Schwarz, 2010, for a review). A
large body of work shows that these feeling-based judgments
are influenced by beliefs, expectations, and other aspects of
previous experience that shape processing in the moment,
which in turn influence true/false judgments (Begg, Anas, &
Farinacci, 1992; Bransford & Johnson, 1972; Jacoby, Kelley, &
Dywan, 1989; Mandler, 1980; Ozubko & Fugelsang, 2011;
Unkelbach, 2007).
Now suppose you evaluate the claim with the photo in the left
panel of Figure 1. Would this photo influence your answer? It
seems obvious that the photo should have no influence, because
it is nonprobative: It does not provide any evidence about the
nature of the liquid inside the thermometer. But recent research
suggests that the photo probably would influence your answer
(Newman, Garry, Bernstein, Kantner, & Lindsay, 2012). When
we asked people to judge the truth of difficult claims that
appeared with or without related nonprobative photos, photos
biased people toward believing the claims.
Eryn J. Newman and Maryanne Garry, School of Psychology, Victoria
University of Wellington; Christian Unkelbach, Social Cognition Center,
University of Cologne; Daniel M. Bernstein, Department of Psychology,
Kwantlen Polytechnic University; D. Stephen Lindsay, Department of
Psychology, University of Victoria; Robert A. Nash, Department of Psy-
chology, University of Surrey.
Eryn J. Newman is now in the Department of Psychology, University of
Southern California. Robert A. Nash is now in the School of Health
Sciences at Aston University.
We are grateful for the support of the New Zealand Government through
the Marsden Fund, administered by the Royal Society of New Zealand on
behalf of the Marsden Fund Council.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Eryn J.
Newman, Victoria University of Wellington, Psychology, Box 600, Wel-
lington, New Zealand. E-mail: eryn.newman@vuw.ac.nz
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Learning, Memory, and Cognition © 2015 American Psychological Association
2015, Vol. 41, No. 2, 000 0278-7393/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000099
1
Thus related nonprobative photos can produce a “truthiness”
1
effect: When making rapid judgments about the truth of a claim,
nonprobative photos nudge people toward believing that claim
(Newman et al., 2012). But why? If a picture of a thermometer
does not tell you whether the metal inside is magnesium, why
would it bias you to conclude the claim is true? Several lines of
research fit with the idea that photos help people generate pseu-
doevidence
2
about the claim, and suggest two broad ways photos
might have this effect.
First, nonprobative photos might promote truthiness in an auto-
matic, bottom-up way by providing rich semantic contexts for
target claims. There is evidence that such contexts can boost
conceptual processing and produce illusions of familiarity and
truth (see Ozubko & Fugelsang, 2011; Whittlesea, 1993). For
example, people tend to claim that they saw a target word (“test”)
earlier when the word appears in a semantically related sentence
(“The anxious student wrote a test”) rather than in a more neutral
sentence (“Later that afternoon she took a test”; Whittlesea, 1993).
Semantically related contexts produce easier conceptual process-
ing relative to neutral contexts, an experience that people may
interpret as evidence of familiarity. Likewise, semantically prim-
ing, repeating, or retrieving related information can produce illu-
sions of frequency, familiarity, and truth—presumably because of
increased ease of retrieval or cognitive availability (Begg et al.,
1992; Jacoby et al., 1989; Kelley & Lindsay, 1993; Ozubko &
Fugelsang, 2011; Tversky & Kahneman, 1973; Whittlesea, 2011).
People draw on the characteristics of a mental event when deciding
its accuracy, judging easily imagined propositions as more prob-
able and evaluating the vividness and detail of imagery as a cue to
reality (Johnson, 2006; Lindsay, 2008; Sherman, Cialdini,
Schwartzman, & Reynolds, 1985). Photos should be particularly
good at bootstrapping the generation of images related to a claim
and thereby fostering belief in that claim.
Second, nonprobative photos might promote truthiness because
people may “trawl” through the photo, deliberatively interpreting
information they find as support for a default bias to see the claim
as true. Such a process might be described as a confirmation bias
(Gilbert, Tafarodi, & Malone, 1993; Nickerson, 1998). For in-
stance, someone might examine the photo of a thermometer and
think, “I can see a liquid metal inside, and I think magnesium is a
metal.” By supporting selective searches for hypothesis-consistent
evidence, photos might produce a feeling of knowing the correct
answer and steer people away from spending extra effort consid-
ering reasons why the claim might be false (see Hart, 1965;
Thomas, Bulevich, & Dubois, 2012).
Both the conceptual fluency and trawling/confirmation bias
mechanisms (which are not mutually exclusive) hinge on the
relationship between the photo and the claim, and fit with our
earlier findings that semantically related photos can increase
truthiness (Newman et al., 2012). But consider now the right panel
of Figure 1. There is, of course, no obvious semantic relationship
between a thermometer and a lizard. What might be the effect of
such a discordant pairing on your answer? This is the question we
address in the eight experiments we present here, as part of our
ongoing exploration of the mechanisms underlying the effects of
photos on truth judgments.
There are reasons to predict that relative to related photos,
unrelated photos will cause conceptual disfluency. That is, unre-
lated photos should make it more difficult for people to recall
related thoughts and images. Consistent with that idea, Lee and
Labroo (2004) reported that when people saw a target word that
was semantically unrelated to a preceding sentence (“The librarian
reached for the top shelf and pulled down a book; napkin”), they
rated that target word as less pleasant than when they saw a target
that was semantically related (“read”). The semantically related
sentence might help participants more quickly identify and com-
prehend the subsequent target word, and/or the semantically un-
related sentence might have prepared participants for a meaning
that mismatched the target and thereby slowed processing. Thus it
could be that the semantically unrelated words were relatively
disfluent compared to their semantically related counterparts, an
experience that people interpreted as a signal that the semantically
unrelated words were relatively less pleasant.
Parallel effects can occur with perceptual fluency and manipu-
lations such as degraded images, difficult fonts, and low contrast
1
From comedian Stephen Colbert, who defined truthiness as “truth that
comes from the gut, not books.” Incidentally, when Colbert discussed our
research on his show, he noted that, “Of course, everyone knows that the
metal in a thermometer is not magnesium: One taste and you know it is
made of dancing light and liquid time” (see tinyurl.com/truthiness2012).
2
We use “pseudoevidence” as an umbrella term for the kinds of non-
probative, nonsensical evidence people could extract from the photo rather
than as a process per se. That evidence could be semantic activation and an
experience of conceptual fluency or it could be a more active process
where people trawl through the photo interpreting details in the photo as
evidence the claim is true.
Figure 1. Example of related and unrelated photos and associated trivia claims. Thermometer photo courtesy
of Andres Rueda, Skink photo courtesy of William Cho, Creative Commons licenses. See the online article for
the color version of this figure.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
2NEWMAN ET AL.
colors that require people to invest more cognitive effort to make
sense of stimuli, which in turn can bias people toward evaluating
these stimuli more negatively (Diemand-Yauman, Oppenheimer,
& Vaughan, 2011; Hernandez & Preston, 2013; Petrova & Cial-
dini, 2005; Reber & Schwarz, 1999; see Yue, Castel, & Bjork,
2013). In the context of a truth judgment, additional cognitive
effort might be taken as a signal that the information being eval-
uated is false. Considered together research on conceptual and
perceptual fluency suggests that if unrelated photos lead to disflu-
ent processing relative to related photos, then they should produce
“falsiness.” That is, nonprobative, unrelated information should
bias people toward disbelieving a claim.
The fluency literature also tells us that the effect of photos
should depend not only on the semantic relationship between the
photo and the claim, but also the experimental context in which
they appear. That is, truthiness or falsiness may come about
because the photos make the claims feel relatively easy (or diffi-
cult) to process compared to the no photo items. Put simply, the
effect of photos may depend on the comparison of processing the
no photo items in the experiment. Indeed research suggests that a
feeling of easy retrieval or easy imagery is driven by a comparison
against a standard (Jacoby & Dallas, 1981; Unkelbach & Greif-
eneder, 2013). When it is easier than expected to retrieve some-
thing, people interpret that processing discrepancy as a cue to
truth; conversely, when it is more difficult than expected to re-
trieve something, people interpret this discrepancy the opposite
way. But when processing matches expectations, there is no dis-
crepancy to interpret. In other words, the no photo items may
produce a discrepancy in processing, making the claims with
photos easier (for related photos) or more difficult (for unrelated
photos) to process than expected. Moreover, processing standards
do not have to arise from sustained prior experiences; experimental
manipulations can forge them in the moment (Jacoby & Dallas,
1981; Westerman, 2008; Whittlesea & Williams, 2001a, 2001b).
For example, repetition is thought to produce truth because re-
peated statements are more easily processed, and people interpret
this processing fluency as a sign that statements are true (Bacon,
1979; Begg et al., 1992; Bernstein, 2005; Dechêne, Stahl, Hansen,
& Wänke, 2010; Kelley & Lindsay, 1993; Unkelbach, 2006). But
as is the case with some of psychological science’s well-known
effects—such as the mere-exposure effect—the effect of repetition
on truth disappears when repetition is manipulated between-
subjects (Dechêne, Stahl, Hansen, & Wänke, 2009; Roediger,
2008). In the experiments reported here, we examine the possibil-
ity that no photo items serve as a benchmark for processing by
manipulating the presence of photos within and between-subjects.
If the effect of photos depends on a comparison against a (no
photo) standard, we should expect to see the most pronounced
effects of photos in our within-subject designs.
But there are also reasons to predict that unrelated photos would
not produce falsiness in any of our experimental designs. As we
suggested earlier, when people are making a true/false decision
they might trawl through and selectively interpret information
from a related photo as evidence that the claim is true. It seems
unlikely that people would engage in this deliberate “trawling”
strategy with unrelated photos. It makes little sense to search a
photo of a lizard for evidence about magnesium’s putative role in
thermometers. Moreover, we know from research on confirmation
bias that if people encounter information inconsistent with the
hypothesis at hand, they often ignore it, allocate less weight to that
evidence, or even distort it to fit with their hypothesis (Darley &
Gross, 1983; Kuhn, 1989; Snyder & Cantor, 1979; see Nickerson,
1998, for a review; see also Fischhoff, 1975). Some research
indicates that adding related nonprobative information can sway
people’s judgments about others, but that adding unrelated infor-
mation produces the same effect as giving people no information
at all (Gill, Swann, & Silvera, 1998). Thus, a trawling mechanism
would predict that unrelated photos would have little if any effect
on people’s decisions.
We examined the effects of semantically related and unrelated
photos in eight experiments. In each, we showed people a series of
trivia claims that appeared with or without a photo that was or was
not semantically related to the claim.
Experiments 1–3
In Experiments 1–3 we examine the effects of semantically
unrelated photos on judgments of truth in a fully within-subjects
design. If the effects of photos depend on an expected standard of
processing, then they should have a particularly potent effect when
people experience related and unrelated photos within the same
experimental context.
Method
Participants. In Experiments 1–3, we used Amazon’s Me-
chanical Turk (MTurk; www.mturk.com/mturk) to recruit subjects
in the United States. MTurk is an online subject pool, in which
“workers” complete experiments and surveys and receive small
amounts of Amazon credit (they received $0.60 for this experi-
ment) that they can use to purchase things on amazon.com. Studies
run online using MTurk attract diverse subjects and tend to pro-
duce similar results to those run in a laboratory (Buhrmester,
Kwang, & Gosling, 2011; Germine et al., 2012; Mason & Suri,
2011; see also Lindsay, Allen, Chan, & Dahl, 2004). We prede-
termined a sample size of 200 subjects based on pilot testing.
Because of a quirk in the way MTurk assigns participants, there
were 208 participants in Experiment 1, 204 in Experiment 1, and
216 in Experiment 3.
Design. For Experiments 1–3, we used a single-factor (photo:
related, unrelated, no photo) within-subjects design (a table sum-
marizing all of our experimental designs appears in supplementary
materials).
Procedure. We used trivia claims from previous research to
assemble sets of difficult true–false trivia claims sampling general
knowledge (Newman et al., 2012; see also Nelson & Narens, 1980;
Unkelbach, 2007). People typically answer these critical claims
correctly 4060% of the time.
We used Qualtrics software to present 40 trivia claims to par-
ticipants. We told participants that sometimes they would see a
photo with these claims, and sometimes they would not. We did
not provide any instructions about how they should use the photo.
We asked participants to decide the truth of the claim “as quickly
as possible, but not so quickly that you start making errors.”
The claims appeared individually, in large black font against a
white background. In Experiments 1 and 2, to orient people to the
task, for the first 16 trials they saw easy trivia claims (which tend
to be answered correctly 80–100% of the time; e.g., “The player
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
3
TRUTHINESS AND FALSINESS
who guards the net in soccer is called the goalie”). Half these easy
claims appeared with a photograph, half with no photograph. To
ensure the practice phase did not teach participants a rule about the
relationship between truth and the presence of photos, we paired
photos equally often with true and false claims. Experiments 1
through 3 differed only in terms of this set of 16 practice items
seamlessly preceding the critical items: In Experiment 1, a random
half of the practice items appeared with a related photo and the
remainder appeared with no photo; in Experiment 2, a random half
of the 16 practice items appeared with an unrelated photo and the
remainder appeared with no photo; in Experiment 3 there were no
practice items. To anticipate, the pattern of means for the critical
items was the same in all three experiments.
Immediately after these easy practice trivia claims, the experi-
mental phase began. Participants saw 24 difficult trivia claims, half
true and half false. For one third of trials, a related nonprobative
photo depicted the grammatical subject of the claim. A few ex-
amples will convey the flavor of this phase: “Macadamia nuts are
in the same evolutionary family as peaches” sometimes appeared
with a photo of macadamia nuts; “Cactuses can reproduce by
parthenogenesis” with a photo of a cactus; and “The plastic things
on the ends of shoelaces are called aglets” with a photo of a shoe
with a shoelace. For another third of trials, a semantically unre-
lated nonprobative photo appeared with the claim: The claim about
macadamia nuts sometimes appeared with a photo of a trash can;
the claim about cactuses with a photo of a bicycle, and the claim
about shoelaces with a photo of a pig. For the other third of the
trials, people saw trivia claims presented without a photo. We used
the set of related nonprobative photos from Newman et al. (2012)
and created a new set of semantically unrelated nonprobative photos
for the experiments reported here. A semantically unrelated photo was
selected for each trivia claim. As a set, the unrelated photos repre-
sented a similar range of living and nonliving objects as the original
set of related photos from Newman et al. None of the photos revealed
the accuracy of the trivia claims. We randomized the order of claims
for each subject, and counterbalanced so that claims appeared equally
often with a related photo, unrelated photo, or no photo. We used an
online script to assign participants to conditions randomly.
Results and Discussion
Our primary aim was to examine the effects of semantic relat-
edness on truthiness and falsiness. To address this question we
calculated people’s bias (C) to say a claim was true (Stanislaw &
Todorov, 1999).
3
As the left panel of Figure 2 shows, pairing a
claim with a related nonprobative photo produced truthiness (as
shown by the negative value of C in Experiment 1). But unrelated
photos did not produce falsiness; instead, trials with unrelated
photos behaved more like trials with no photos.
Consistent with the pattern displayed in the figure, a one-way
analysis of variance (ANOVA) of C showed a main effect for
photo, F(2, 206) 4.10, p.02, p
2.04. Follow-up tests
showed that related photos produced more bias to say true than
unrelated photos or no photos, t
related-unrelated
(207) 2.30, p
.02, Cohen’s d.16, t
related-no photo
(207) 2.77, p.01, d.20.
In contrast, unrelated photos did not differ from no photos,
tunrelated-no photo(207) 0.47, p.64, d.04. We found no consistent
effects for photographs on d=but we report d=analyses for each
experiment in supplemental materials.
On the one hand, it is surprising that unrelated photos did not
produce falsiness given that they should have felt especially dif-
ficult to process compared to the related photos. On the other hand,
a critic might argue that unrelated photos failed to produce falsi-
ness because the combination of the practice task (comprised of
related and no photos) and experiment proper meant that unrelated
photos occurred rarely. That is, 16 practice items appeared with a
related photo or no photo, followed by a random series of 24 trivia
claims in the experiment proper of eight related, eight unrelated,
and eight no photo. Therefore, people saw trivia claims with no
photos 40% of the time; trivia claims with related photos 40% of
the time, and trivia claims with unrelated photos only 20% of the
time—in other words, when a photo appeared with a claim, two
thirds of the time it related to the claim and one third of the time
it did not. Such a mix might have led participants to “oversee”
relationships between some unrelated photos and their associated
claims, diluting the intended effect of the unrelated condition. To
address this issue, in Experiment 2 we reran Experiment 1 except
that the 16 practice items appeared with an unrelated photo or no
photo and in Experiment 3 we ran another version of Experiment
1 in which there was no practice task.
Did the practice phase bear on the effects we observed in
Experiment 1? The answer is no. Regardless of whether people
saw unrelated photos during the practice phase, or even when there
was no practice phase, Table 1 shows that we replicated the
primary findings in Experiment 1 (i.e., related photos produced
truthiness, and unrelated photos acted just like no photo trials).
A one-way ANOVA of C showed a main effect for photo: Exper-
iment 2, F(2, 202) 4.29, p.02, p
2.04, and Experiment 3, F(2,
214) 4.25, p.02, p
2.04. Related photos produced more bias
to say true than unrelated photos or no photos: Experiment 2,
t
related-unrelated
(203) 2.18, p.03, d.15; t
related-no photo
(203)
2.78, p.01, d.20 and Experiment 3 t
related-unrelated
(215) 2.85,
p.01, d.19; t
related-no photo
(215) 1.90, p.06, d.13. Bias
for unrelated did not differ from bias for no photos: Experiment 2,
t
unrelated-no photo
(203) .77, p.44 days .06 and Experiment 3,
t
unrelated-no photo
(215) 1.03, p.30, d.06.
In addition to these analyses we used data across Experiments 1–3
to arrive at a more precise estimate of the size of both the truthiness
and falsiness effects by subjecting them to random effects model mini
meta-analyses (Cumming, 2012). Those data appear in Figure 3. In
the first analysis we focused on truthiness, comparing the related
photo and no photo conditions to calculate an estimated raw effect
size of 0.16, 95% confidence interval (CI): 0.09, 0.23, z 4.27, p
.01. We ran a parallel analysis focusing on falsiness, this time com-
paring unrelated photo and no photo conditions, to calculate an
estimated raw effect size of 0.00, 95% CI: 0.07, 0.08, z .13, p
.90, a range of values that plausibly include falsiness (negative val-
ues), truthiness (positive values), or zero.
3
We corrected floor and ceiling responses by converting values of 1 to
.99 and values of 0 to .01 (Wickens, 2002). Note that for all experiments,
when we ran parallel analyses using the total proportion of true responses
for each condition we found the same pattern of results (see Stanislaw &
Todorov, 1999; Wickens, 2002).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
4NEWMAN ET AL.
Experiment 4
In Experiments 1–3 (and in our prior work on truthiness) par-
ticipants judged trivia claims as true or false. The primary aim of
Experiment 4 was to replicate the pattern in Experiments 1–3 using
a procedure in which participants rated each trivia claim on a scale
from 1 (definitely false)to9(definitely true). In Experiment 4, we
also examined generalizability by using a new set of trivia
claims—all of which were false—and on a sample of students at a
U.K. university who participated in one-on-one lab sessions. Fi-
nally, in Experiment 4 we also added a response latency measure,
which allowed us to examine the possibility that people were
simply ignoring the unrelated photos.
Method
Participants. Sixty-five University of Surrey undergraduates
participated in exchange for course credit.
Design. The design was identical to Experiments 1–3.
Procedure. We developed a new set of 87 difficult claims
with mean ratings that fell between 3 and 5 on a 7 point scale: 1
(definitely false)to7(definitely true), range 3.13 to 4.96 and
M3.91, SD 0.48. All the claims were false. We used E-Prime
software to present the 87 claims to participants, tested individu-
ally in the lab for course credit. One third of the claims appeared
with an accompanying related photo, one third with an unrelated
photo, and one third with no photo. We did not provide any
instructions about how participants should use the photos, but
simply asked them to be as quick and as accurate as possible.
Unlike our previous experiment in which we asked for binary
true–false judgments, this time we asked participants to rate their
confidence in the truth of each statement, using a scale from 1
(definitely false)to9(definitely true). Participants entered their
responses using the computer keyboard and we measured partici-
pants’ response latency for each claim.
Results and Discussion
Although we changed the response measure and the trivia
claims, we replicated the results from Experiment 1–3: Pairing a
claim with a related nonprobative photo produced truthiness, but
unrelated photos behaved more like trials with no photos. There
was a significant effect of photo on participants’ confidence rat-
ings, F(2, 63) 4.88, p.01, p
2.13. Related photos (M
5.25, SD .66) produced greater confidence ratings than did
unrelated photos (M5.11, SD .73) or no photos (M 5.06,
Table 1
Bias Data for Replications of Experiments 1, 5, and 7
Experiment Bias related photo Bias unrelated photo Bias no photo
Experiment 1
Experiment 2: Practice phase with
unrelated photos .13 (.76) .00 (.71) .06 (.81)
Experiment 3: With no practice phase .17 (.67) .01 (.70) .05 (.68)
Experiment 5
Experiment 6
Related condition .16 (.45) .04 (.46)
Unrelated condition .02 (.39) .11 (.42)
Experiment 7
Experiment 8 .00 (.43) .01 (.42) .06 (.33)
Note. Standard deviations are in parentheses. Negative bias (C) values show a bias to say true.
Figure 2. Bias for trivia claims presented with or without a photograph that was semantically related or
unrelated to the trivia claims. A negative value of C is a bias to say true. Error bars show 95% within-subject
confidence intervals (see Masson & Loftus, 2003) for the photo/no-photo effect for Experiments 1 (within-
subjects design) and 5 (mixed design) and the 95% between-subjects confidence intervals for each cell mean in
Experiments 7 (between-subjects design).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
5
TRUTHINESS AND FALSINESS
Figure 3. Forest plot of effect sizes for related and unrelated photos between studies (Derzon & Alford, 2013).
Each row represents one experiment, starting with the original experiment and then subsequent replications. The
top panel of the plot displays the effects for related photos. The bottom panel of the plot displays the effects for
unrelated photos. The location of each shape on the horizontal axis represents the raw effect size—the difference
between people’s response bias when they saw claims paired with a (related/unrelated) photo compared to when
they saw claims without photos. The lines extending either side of a shape represent a 95% confidence interval
(note that a 95% CI for a mean difference that excludes zero will be significantly different from zero using null
hypothesis testing). The black shape shows the result of the meta-analysis for each experiment, with the center
of the shape indicating the estimated effect size, and again the black lines extending either side of a shape
represent a 95% confidence interval. Finally, within each panel on the plot, data that fall to the right of the zero
line show truthiness (nonprobative related photos lead people to believe a claim) and data that fall to the left side
of the zero line show falsiness (nonprobative unrelated photos lead people to disbelieve a claim). Tables
presenting additional results from the meta-analyses can be found in supplementary materials. See the online
article for the color version of this figure.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
6NEWMAN ET AL.
SD .68), t
related-unrelated
(64) 2.24, p.03, Cohen’s d.28,
t
related-no photo
(64) 3.00, p.01, d.37, whereas confidence
for unrelated and no photos was similar t
unrelated-no photo
(64)
0.88, p.38, d.11. We next coded participants’ responses as
“true” whenever their confidence ratings were 6 or above (i.e.,
greater than the scale’s midpoint), and we calculated the proportion of
statements rated as true. This analysis revealed the same main effect
of photo, F(2, 63) 7.39, p.01, p
2.19; t
related-unrelated
(64)
3.57, p.01, d.44, t
related-no photo
(64) 3.14, p.01, d.39,
t
unrelated-no photo
(64) 0.23, p.82, d.03.
That related photos produced truthiness replicates our prior
findings and shows that nonprobative photos that are related to
trivia claims can foster belief in those claims, true or false. But
why did unrelated photos have no effect relative to no photos? One
possibility is that people adopted a strategy of ignoring unrelated
photos, much as people in a Stroop task can adopt a strategy of
ignoring the word and focusing on the color (Kane & Engle, 2003;
Stroop, 1935; see also Besner, Stolz, & Boutilier, 1997). But there
are reasons to doubt that possibility. For one thing, participants
must have processed unrelated photos to some extent to determine
that they were unrelated. For another, in Experiment 4 we mea-
sured response latency and observed that responses were approx-
imately 300 ms slower when there was a photo (related or unre-
lated) than when there was no photo, which suggests that
participants did not ignore the unrelated photos.
4
Another expla-
nation is that presenting unrelated photos mixed with related
photos led participants to look for semantic relationships between
the claims and the photos, for both types of photo. For instance,
they might have looked at the thermometer claim paired with a
lizard and thought, “Well, the lizard is long and thin, like a
thermometer, and has a stripe up the center just like a thermom-
eter.” Such a strategy might have mitigated the perceived incon-
gruity between the photos and trivia claims, leading unrelated
photos to become more like related photos and thus diluting their
falsiness. This idea fits with research showing that people will find
or create meaning (Bartlett, 1932), especially when they are faced
with pairings that do not have an obvious semantic relationship
(e.g., novel metaphors; Grimshaw, Stewart, & Lauwereyns, 2011;
Lynott & Connell, 2010; see also Foster & Kokko, 2009).
Taken together, the findings from Experiments 1–4 suggest that in
the context of related photos, unrelated photos exert the same influ-
ence as no photos. But part of this context likely includes participants’
expectations that each photo will be meaningfully related to a claim.
Might that expectation modulate the effect of unrelated photos? If we
manipulated the semantic relatedness of photos between participants,
those shown unrelated photos should come to have minimal expec-
tations about meaningful relationships between photos and claims.
What then should be the effect of unrelated photos relative to no
photos? One possibility is that unrelated photos would make it more
difficult for people to bring to mind related ideas, producing disfluent
conceptual processing. If so, then we should see increased falsi-
ness relative to no-photo items among participants for whom
photos were always unrelated. A second possibility is that
setting people up to expect incongruity between photos and
claims might lead them to ignore all of the photos—a strategy
that should be much easier to apply when all the photos are
unrelated (Kane & Engle, 2003). We addressed these questions
in Experiments 5 and 6 (the latter was an exact replication of the
former).
Experiments 5 and 6
Method
Participants. We used MTurk to recruit 196 participants (Ex-
periment 5) and 185 participants (Experiment 6) in the United
States. They received $0.60 Amazon credit for participating.
Design. We used a 2 (photo: yes, no) 2 (relatedness: related,
unrelated) mixed design, manipulating the presence of a photo
within participants and relatedness of the photo between partici-
pants.
Procedure. We used the same procedure as Experiment 1
with the following changes. Immediately after seeing the easy
practice trivia claims, people saw 32 difficult trivia claims (half
true, half false). Half of the claims appeared with a photo; for half
of the participants the photo was always related to the claim
whereas for the remaining participants it was always unrelated to
the claim.
Results
As the middle panel of Figure 2 shows, related photos again
produced truthiness. But the figure also shows that, in contrast to
Experiments 1–3, unrelated photos produced falsiness.
A22 mixed ANOVA showed the pertinent Photo Related-
ness interaction, F(1, 194) 9.01, p.01 p
2.04; related photos
produced truthiness, t
related-no photo
(93) 2.12, p.04, d.23, but
unrelated photos produced falsiness, t
unrelated-no photo
(101) 2.12,
p.04, d.19. As Table 1 shows, these patterns replicated in
Experiment 6; there was a Photo Relatedness interaction, F(1,
183) 18.22, p.01 p
2.09; related photos produced truthiness,
t
related-no photo
(83) 4.08, p.01, d.44, but unrelated photos
produced falsiness, t
unrelated-no photo
(100) 1.94, p.06, d.19.
As in Experiments 1–3, we used data from both the primary
experiment and replication to calculate a more precise estimate of
the size of both the truthiness and falsiness effects by subjecting
these data to random effects model mini meta-analyses (see Figure
3; Cumming, 2012). The estimated raw effect size for truthiness
was 0.17, 95% CI: 0.10, 0.25, z 4.45, p.01, and for falsiness
it was 0.10, 95% CI: 0.16, 0.03], z 2.87, p.01.
Discussion
We found that when the semantic relatedness of photos to
claims was manipulated between participants, unrelated photos
produced falsiness—a pattern that fits with a fluency account,
suggesting that the unrelated photos may have produced an expe-
rience of disfluent processing. Taken together, Experiments 1–4
and 5–6 might lead us to conclude that although the effects of
unrelated photos depend on the experimental context in which they
appear, related photos produce truthiness regardless of context.
But in both of these experiments (and in Newman et al., 2012),
claims with related photos always appeared among claims without
4
Participants took longer to respond to claims presented with photos than to
those without photos, whereas response times for claims with related and
unrelated photos were similar (Mdn
related
photo 6899.28ms, Mdn
unrelated
photo 6979.28ms, Mdn
no photo
6604.97ms; Friedman’s
2
7.02, p
.03), Wilcoxon’s Z
related-unrelated
1.61, p.11; Z
related-no photo
2.51, p
.01; Z
unrelated-no photo
2.89, p.01).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
7
TRUTHINESS AND FALSINESS
photos; the same is true of claims with unrelated photos. Thus we
cannot rule out the possibility that truthiness also depends on
context. That is, perhaps what drives truthiness is that people
evaluate their processing experiences with photos against a bench-
mark of their experiences without photos (e.g., Dechêne, Stahl,
Hansen, & Wänke, 2009; Jacoby & Dallas, 1981; Roediger, 2008;
Unkelbach & Greifeneder, 2013). If truthiness (and falsiness)
depends on a standard, we should see that the pattern from Exper-
iments 5 and 6 disappears when people have no standard against
which to interpret the ease or difficulty of processing that accom-
panies claims paired with photos. Accordingly, in Experiments 7
and 8 (8 was an exact replication of 7), we manipulated the photo
factor entirely between participants.
Experiment 7 and 8
Method
Participants. We used MTurk to recruit 301 participants (Ex-
periment 7) and 301 participants (Experiment 8) in the United
States. They received a $0.60 Amazon credit.
Design. We used a single-factor (photo: related, unrelated, no
photo) between-participants design.
Procedure. Participants saw the same trivia claims as in Ex-
periments 5–6: 16 practice claims followed by 32 trivia claims.
The key difference in Experiments 7–8 is that we manipulated
the photo factor between participants. That is, a third of par-
ticipants saw the claims paired with related photos, a third saw
the claims paired with unrelated photos, and a final third saw
the claims paired with no photo. We gave participants the same
instructions as in the prior experiments except that we removed
any reference to the presence or absence of photos.
Results and Discussion
As the right side of Figure 2 shows, compared to when there was
no photo, related photos did not produce truthiness and unrelated
photos did not produce falsiness. In other words, there was no
effect of photo, F(2, 298) 0.75, p.47, p
2.01. As Table 1
shows, these patterns replicated in Experiment 8, F(2, 298) 1.02,
p.36, p
2.01. Together these findings fit with the idea that
truthiness and falsiness depend on expectations acquired in the
experimental context, and occur only when there is a discrepancy
in the expected ease of processing (Whittlesea & Williams, 1998).
As in our earlier experiments, we used both sets of data to
calculate a more precise estimate of the size of both the truthiness
and falsiness effects by subjecting them to random effects model
mini meta-analyses (see Figure 3; Cumming, 2012). The estimated
raw effect size for truthiness was 0.02, 95% CI: 0.11, 0.07, z
.48, p.63. The estimated raw effect size for falsiness was 0.06,
95% CI: 0.14, 0.02, z 1.44, p.15. These analyses show that the
effects of photos on truthiness or falsiness are nonexistent or trivial in
the context of a between-subjects design.
General Discussion
Across eight experiments, we found that the effects of nonpro-
bative photos on the perceived truth of trivia statements vary with
experimental context. We also found that the effects of nonproba-
tive photos depend on the semantic relationship between the photo
and the claim. When trivia statements were accompanied by a mix
of related, unrelated, and no photos, related photos produced
truthiness relative to no photos but unrelated photos had no effect
relative to no photos. When people had a no-photo standard against
which to evaluate either related or unrelated photos (Experiments
5–6), related photos increased the truth of claims (truthiness), and
unrelated photos decreased the truth of claims (falsiness). But
when we removed people’s ability to compare against a standard,
using a fully between-subjects design, neither related nor unrelated
nonprobative photos influenced true–false judgments. Considered
as a whole, this pattern of results suggests that photos influence
people’s judgments when a discrepancy arises in the expected ease
of processing—that is, when participants find claims with photos
easier (or more difficult) to evaluate compared to claims without
photos (Hansen, Dechêne, & Wänke, 2008; Westerman, 2008;
Whittlesea & Williams, 2001a, 2001b).
Of course, one might argue that these effects of photos are
produced by some kind of demand characteristic. But there are at
least three reasons why we don’t think that is the case. First, we
told participants that the study was about visual and verbal learn-
ing, so participants did not know the purpose of the study. Second,
if participants did figure out our hypothesis, we wouldn’t neces-
sarily expect them to act in a way that would promote the effect.
In fact, the literature on fluency shows that when people detect the
source of fluent processing (or they can “see through” the manip-
ulation), they tend to discount that fluency experience and try to
counteract any influence that source of fluency might have on their
judgments (e.g., Jacoby & Whitehouse, 1989; Oppenheimer,
2004). In other words, this literature suggests that if participants
had some inkling of the idea that photos could influence their
decisions, we would expect them to say “false” more when they
saw a related photo paired with a claim. But we see the opposite
pattern of results. Third, if demand characteristics were driving
truthiness, then photos should have a systematic effect not just
across studies but across all of our items. In our earlier work, we
found instead that photos produced truthiness for unfamiliar
claims—that is, in conditions of uncertainty, which are conditions
that also make people susceptible to fluency effects (see Newman
et al., 2012; Schwarz, 2010; Unkelbach, 2007).
Rather, these findings support a mechanism in which—against a
backdrop of an expected standard—related photos help people
generate pseudoevidence to support the claim. Related photos
might help people generate pseudoevidence by facilitating concep-
tual processing, perhaps increasing cognitive availability and (or)
helping people trawl for evidence (Kelley & Lindsay, 1993; Nick-
erson, 1998; Tversky & Kahneman, 1973; Whittlesea, 2011). Our
anecdotal observations of a small number of lab-tested pilot par-
ticipants (who provided think-aloud protocols while performing
the procedure of Experiments 5–6) also fit with such a mechanism.
For instance, when faced with the claim “Macadamia nuts are in
the same evolutionary family as peaches” paired with a photo of
macadamia nuts, one subject said, “I’m going to go with yes
because they kind of look like peaches, so that would make sense.”
The finding that unrelated photos produced falsiness in Exper-
iments 5 and 6 fits with the idea that unrelated photos should not
help people generate confirming pseudoevidence and may actually
generate a feeling of incongruity or difficult processing when
evaluating the claim. Nonetheless, it is puzzling that unrelated
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
8NEWMAN ET AL.
photos did not produce falsiness in Experiments 1–4; instead,
participants seemed to treat them as no photo items. One reason
why unrelated photos did not produce falsiness is that presenting
them along with related photos led participants to look for seman-
tic relationships between the claims and the unrelated photos. This
kind of process might dilute the perceived incongruity between the
unrelated photos and trivia claims, leading unrelated photos to
become more like related photos and thus reducing their falsiness.
Indeed, people have a tendency to find meaning (Bartlett, 1932),
especially when they are faced with pairings that do not have an
obvious semantic relationship (e.g., novel metaphors; Grimshaw,
Stewart, & Lauwereyns, 2011; Lynott & Connell, 2010; see also
Foster & Kokko, 2009). We think this is an intriguing result
worthy of future research.
Photos did not produce truthiness or falsiness in Experiments 7
and 8. That finding is consistent with research on discrepant
fluency, where conceptually fluent items have the most pro-
nounced effects when they are presented against a backdrop of
relatively disfluent items (when ease of processing is manipulated
within participants; Hansen et al., 2008; Westerman, 2008;
Whittlesea & Williams, 2001a, 2001b). But there are at least two
other possibilities. First, there are many cues that people could
draw on to decide whether a claim is true (e.g., credibility of the
source of the claim, familiarity, logical coherence) and it is pos-
sible that the within-subject design produces truthiness because it
emphasizes photos as the important cue, because their presence
changes across trials (see Schwarz, in press). Such an emphasis is
not placed on photos in the between-subjects design (the presence or
absence of photos is constant across trials), so people may turn to
other cues they notice changing across items such as the familiarity of
a name or the ease or difficulty of a statement (see Newman et al.,
2012). An interesting avenue for future research might be to disen-
tangle these accounts of the between-subjects data.
A second and less interesting account of the between-subjects
data is that people simply ignored the photos and that is why they
had little influence on people’s judgments. But there are several
reasons why the ignoring hypothesis is not a compelling explana-
tion of the results of Experiment 7 and 8. For instance, although
people might find the unrelated photos distracting, it is unclear
why people would be motivated to ignore the related photos in the
between-subjects condition. In addition, eye-tracking studies show
that we tend to be drawn to visual information, so it would take
cognitive effort and some kind of explicit strategy to ignore the
photos (e.g., Sargent, 2007). In fact, we should be able to detect if
people are engaging in this kind of strategy by examining people’s
responses and response times across trials. Although we do not
have response times for Experiments 7 and 8, we did collect
response times when we ran a between-subjects replication of
some of our earlier photo research (we conducted a conceptual
replication of Experiment 1 from Newman et al., 2012, manipu-
lating the presence of photos between-subjects). If the ignoring
hypothesis is true, we should have seen a difference in people’s
responses and RTs across trials. That is, people should initially
show a photo effect, or spend more time on photo trials, an effect
that should fade across trials as people refine their ignoring strat-
egy. In contrast to the ignoring hypothesis, we found that if we
compare the photo and no photo trials, there were no differences in
people’s response times for the first quarter block and the last
quarter block. In fact, in both blocks and across all trials, people
took slightly longer to respond to photo trials (a pattern consistent
with our within-subject data in Experiment 4). That is, people
started the same way they ended—they did not learn to ignore
across trials. Moreover, there was no evidence that photos caused
diminishing truthiness over trials. Both of these findings are at
odds with the proposal that people learned to use some kind of
ignoring strategy in the between-subjects design.
In all of our analyses we examined the effect of photos relative
to the no photo condition. So what we have demonstrated is that
the difference in response bias between photo and no photo con-
ditions varied across experimental contexts. But we could also ask
Figure 4. Forest plot of mean bias (c) scores (an estimate from a meta-analysis for each experiment) for related
photo, unrelated photo and no photo conditions, plotted for each experimental design. Tables presenting
additional results from the meta-analyses can be found in supplementary materials. See the online article for the
color version of this figure.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
9
TRUTHINESS AND FALSINESS
how response bias in each condition—the absolute bias for related,
unrelated and no photo trials—varied across experimental con-
texts. This question gains piquancy from the fact that the response
bias to no-photo items (which served as our baseline control
condition) varied across conditions (see Figure 4 for a forest plot
of estimates of C against the benchmark of zero).
Lindsay and Kantner (2011) observed that old/new recognition
memory response bias tends to be conservative when the stimuli
are scans of paintings. Subsequent work (Lindsay, Fallow, &
Kantner, in press) found that when the stimuli include a mix of
paintings and words, bias was conservative on paintings and liberal
on words. But in a between-subjects design, although bias contin-
ued to be conservative on paintings it was neutral on words. It may
be that the liberal recognition bias on words in the mixed case was
an artifact of participants’ reluctance to classify paintings as old;
that is, participants were reluctant to claim that paintings were old,
but they also did not want to say “No” to too many probes so they
compensated by being liberal on words. We find a similar pattern
in the data here: When claims were a mix of related, unrelated, and
no photos, bias was neutral for claims presented with an unrelated
photo or with no photo and liberal for claims presented with
related photos. But in a between-subjects design, bias was neutral
for all conditions. Taken together, these findings show that abso-
lute response bias varies according to attributes of the stimuli, as
well as the context in which those stimuli appear.
Taken together with our prior work, the eight experiments
reported here suggest that nonprobative photographs do more than
simply decorate claims: They wield a significant and immediate
influence on people’s judgments. As with many effects in the
cognitive psychology literature, our photo-truthiness and falsiness
effects depend on the way in which people process and interpret
photos when evaluating the truth of claims.
References
Bacon, F. T. (1979). Credibility of repeated statements: Memory for trivia.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5,
241–252. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.5.3.241
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social
psychology. London, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Begg, I. M., Anas, A., & Farinacci, S. (1992). Dissociation of processes in
belief: Source recollection, statement familiarity, and the illusion of
truth. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 446458.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.121.4.446
Bernstein, D. M. (2005). Making sense of memory. Canadian Journal of
Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimen-
tale, 59, 199–208. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0087475
Besner, D., Stolz, J. A., & Boutilier, C. (1997). The stroop effect and the
myth of automaticity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 221–225.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03209396
Bransford, J. D., & Johnson, M. K. (1972). Contextual prerequisites for
understanding: Some investigations of comprehension and recall. Jour-
nal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 11, 717–726. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(72)80006-9
Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon’s Mechan-
ical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Per-
spectives on Psychological Science, 6, 3–5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
1745691610393980
Cumming, G. (2012). Understanding the new statistics: Effect sizes, con-
fidence intervals, and meta-analysis. New York, NY: Routledge.
Darley, J. M., & Gross, P. H. (1983). A hypothesis-confirming bias in
labeling effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44,
20–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.20
Dechêne, A., Stahl, C., Hansen, J., & Wänke, M. (2009). Mix me a list:
Context moderates the truth effect and the mere-exposure effect. Journal
of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 1117–1122. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.jesp.2009.06.019
Dechêne, A., Stahl, C., Hansen, J., & Wänke, M. (2010). The truth about
the truth: A meta-analytic review of the truth effect. Personality and
Social Psychology Review, 14, 238–257. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
1088868309352251
Derzon, J. H., & Alford, A. A. (2013). Forest plots in Excel: Moving
beyond a clump of trees to a forest of visual information. Practical
Assessment, Research & Evaluation, 18, 1–9.
Diemand-Yauman, C., Oppenheimer, D. M., & Vaughan, E. B. (2011).
Fortune favors the bold (and the italicized): Effects of disfluency on
educational outcomes. Cognition, 118, 111–115. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.cognition.2010.09.012
Fischhoff, B. (1975). Hindsight does not equal foresight:The effect of
outcome knowledge on judgment under uncertainty. Journal of Exper-
imental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1, 288–299.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.1.3.288
Foster, K. R., & Kokko, H. (2009). The evolution of superstitious and
superstition-like behaviour. Proceedings Biological Sciences, 276, 31–
37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.0981
Germine, L., Nakayama, K., Duchaine, B. C., Chabris, C. F., Chatterjee,
G., & Wilmer, J. B. (2012). Is the Web as good as the lab? Comparable
performance from Web and lab in cognitive/perceptual experiments.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 847–857. http://dx.doi.org/
10.3758/s13423-012-0296-9
Gilbert, D. T. (1991). How mental systems believe. American Psycholo-
gist, 46, 107–119. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.46.2.107
Gilbert, D. T., Tafarodi, R. W., & Malone, P. S. (1993). You can’t not
believe everything you read. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 65, 221–233. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.65.2.221
Gill, M. J., Swann, W. B., Jr., & Silvera, D. H. (1998). On the genesis of
confidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1101–
1114. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.5.1101
Graesser, A. C., & Hemphill, D. (1991). Question answering in the context
of scientific mechanisms. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 186
209. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(91)90003-3
Grimshaw, G. M., Stewart, J., & Lauwereyns, J. (2011, April). The sub-
jective nature of meaning. Poster presented at the meeting of the Cog-
nitive Neuroscience Society, San Francisco, CA.
Hansen, J., Dechêne, A., & Wänke, M. (2008). Discrepant fluency in-
creases subjective truth. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44,
687–691. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2007.04.005
Hart, J. T. (1965). Memory and the feeling-of-knowing experience. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 56, 208–216. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
h0022263
Hernandez, I., & Preston, J. L. (2013). Disfluency disrupts the confirmation
bias. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 178–182. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2012.08.010
Jacoby, L. L., & Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship between autobio-
graphical memory and perceptual learning. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 110, 306–340. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-
3445.110.3.306
Jacoby, L. L., Kelley, C. M., & Dywan, J. (1989). Memory attributions. In
H. L. Roediger, III, & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and
consciousness: Essays in honour of Endel Tulving (pp. 391–422). Hills-
dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Jacoby, L. L., & Whitehouse, K. (1989). An illusion of memory: False
recognition influenced by unconscious perception. Journal of Experi-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
10 NEWMAN ET AL.
mental Psychology: General, 118, 126–135. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
0096-3445.118.2.126
Johnson, M. K. (2006). Memory and reality. American Psychologist, 61,
760–771. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.61.8.760
Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working-memory capacity and the
control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect, response com-
petition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 132, 47–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445
.132.1.47
Kelley, C. M., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Remembering mistaken for
knowing: Ease of retrieval as a basis for confidence in answers to
general knowledge questions. Journal of Memory and Language, 32,
1–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1993.1001
Kuhn, D. (1989). Children and adults as intuitive scientists. Psychological
Review, 96, 674689. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.96.4.674
Lee, A. Y., & Labroo, A. (2004). Effects of conceptual and perceptual
fluency on affective judgment. Journal of Marketing Research, 41,
151–165. http://dx.doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.41.2.151.28665
Lindsay, D. S. (2008). Source monitoring. In H. L. Roediger, III (Ed.),
Cognitive psychology of memory. Vol. 2 of Learning and memory: A
comprehensive reference, 4 vols. (J. Byrne, Ed.) (pp. 325–348). Oxford,
UK: Elsevier.
Lindsay, D. S., Allen, B. P., Chan, J. C. K., & Dahl, L. C. (2004).
Eyewitness suggestibility and source similarity: Intrusions of details
from one event into memory reports of another event. Journal of
Memory and Language, 50, 96–111. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml
.2003.08.007
Lindsay, D. S., & Kantner, J. (2011). A search for influences of feedback
on recognition of music, poetry, and art. In P. Higham & J. Leboe (Eds.),
Constructions of remembering and metacognition: Essays in honor of
Bruce Whittlesea (pp. 137–154). Houndmills, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.
Lindsay, D. S., Fallow, K. M., & Kantner, J. (in press). Recognition
memory bias is conservative for paintings and we don’t know why. In
D. S. Lindsay, C. M. Kelley, A. P. Yonelinas, & H. L. Roediger III
(Eds.), Remembering: Attributions, processes and control in human
memory (in honor of Larry L. Jacoby). New York: Psychology Press.
Lynott, D., & Connell, L. (2010). Embodied conceptual combination.
Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 212. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2010
.00212
Mandler, G. (1980). Recognizing: The judgment of previous occurrence.
Psychological Review, 87, 252–271. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-
295X.87.3.252
Mason, W., & Suri, S. (2011). Conducting behavioral research on Ama-
zon’s Mechanical Turk. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 1–23. http://
dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13428-011-0124-6
Masson, M. E. J., & Loftus, G. R. (2003). Using confidence intervals for
graphically based data interpretation. Canadian Journal of Experimental
Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 57, 203–
220. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0087426
Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L. (1980). Norms of 300 general- information
questions: Accuracy of recall, latency of recall, and feeling-of-knowing
ratings. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 19, 338–368.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90266-2
Newman, E. J., Garry, M., Bernstein, D. M., Kantner, J., & Lindsay, D. S.
(2012). Nonprobative photographs (or words) inflate truthiness. Psycho-
nomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 969–974. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/
s13423-012-0292-0
Nickerson, R. S. (1998). Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in
many guises. Review of General Psychology, 2, 175–220. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.2.175
Oppenheimer, D. M. (2004). Spontaneous discounting of availability in
frequency judgment tasks. Psychological Science, 15, 100–105. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01502005.x
Ozubko, J. D., & Fugelsang, J. (2011). Remembering makes evidence
compelling: Retrieval from memory can give rise to the illusion of truth.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cogni-
tion, 37, 270–276. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021323
Petrova, P. K., & Cialdini, R. B. (2005). Fluency of consumption imagery
and the backfire of imagery appeals. Journal of Consumer Research, 32,
442–452.
Reber, R., & Schwarz, N. (1999). Effects of perceptual fluency on judg-
ments of truth. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal,
8, 338–342. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/ccog.1999.0386
Reber, R., & Unkelbach, C. (2010). The epistemic status of processing
fluency as source for judgments of truth. Review of Philosophy and
Psychology, 1, 563–581. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s13164-010-0039-7
Roediger, H. L., III. (2008). Relativity of remembering: Why the laws of
memory vanished. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 225–254. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190139
Sargent, S. L. (2007). Image effects on selective exposure to computer-
mediated news stories. Computers in Human Behavior, 23, 705–726.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2004.11.005
Schwarz, N. (2010). Meaning in context: Metacognitive experiences. In B.
Mesquita, L. F. Barrett, & E. R. Smith (Eds.), The mind in context (pp.
105–125). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Schwarz, N. (in press). APA handbook of personality and social psychol-
ogy: Attitudes and social cognition. Washington, DC: American Psy-
chological Association.
Sherman, S. J., Cialdini, R. B., Schwartzman, D. F., & Reynolds, K. D.
(1985). Imagining can heighten or lower the perceived likelihood of
contracting a disease: The mediating effect of ease of imagery. Person-
ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11, 118–127. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1177/0146167285111011
Snyder, M., & Cantor, N. (1979). Testing hypotheses about other people:
The use of historical knowledge. Journal of Experimental Social Psy-
chology, 15, 330–342. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(79)90042-8
Stanislaw, H., & Todorov, N. (1999). Calculation of signal detection theory
measures. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 31,
137–149. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03207704
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/h0054651
Thomas, A. K., Bulevich, J. B., & Dubois, S. J. (2012). An analysis of the
determinants of the feeling of knowing. Consciousness and Cognition:
An International Journal, 21, 1681–1694. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
.concog.2012.09.005
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging
frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 207–232. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(73)90033-9
Unkelbach, C. (2006). The learned interpretation of cognitive fluency.
Psychological Science, 17, 339–345. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
9280.2006.01708.x
Unkelbach, C. (2007). Reversing the truth effect: Learning the interpreta-
tion of processing fluency in judgments of truth. Journal of Experimen-
tal Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 219–230. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.33.1.219
Unkelbach, C., & Greifeneder, R. (2013). A general model of fluency
effects in judgment and decision making. In C. Unkelbach & R. Greif-
eneder (Eds.), The experience of thinking (pp. 11–32). London: Psychol-
ogy Press.
Westerman, D. L. (2008). Relative fluency and illusions of recognition
memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 1196–1200. http://dx.doi
.org/10.3758/PBR.15.6.1196
Whittlesea, B. W. A. (1993). Illusions of familiarity. Journal of Experi-
mental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1235–1253.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.19.6.1235
Whittlesea, B. W. A. (2011). Remembering under the influence of uncon-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
11
TRUTHINESS AND FALSINESS
scious expectations. In P. A. Higham & J. P. Leboe (Eds.), Constructions
of remembering and metacognition: Essays in honor of Bruce Whittlesea
(pp. 225–236). Houndmills, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.
Whittlesea, B. W., & Williams, L. D. (1998). Why do strangers feel
familiar, but friends don’t? A discrepancy-attribution account of feelings
of familiarity. Acta Psychologica, 98, 141–165.
Whittlesea, B. W. A., & Williams, L. D. (2001a). The discrepancy-
attribution hypothesis: I. The heuristic basis of feelings of familiarity.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cogni-
tion, 27, 3–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.27.1.3
Whittlesea, B. W. A., & Williams, L. D. (2001b). The discrepancy-
attribution hypothesis: II. Expectation, uncertainty, surprise, and feelings
of familiarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
and Cognition, 27, 14–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.27.1.14
Wickens. (2002). Elementary signal detection theory. New York, NY:
Oxford University Press.
Yue, C. L., Castel, A. D., & Bjork, R. A. (2013). When disfluency is—and
is not—a desirable difficulty: The influence of typeface clarity on
metacognitive judgments and memory. Memory & Cognition, 41, 229
241. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13421-012-0255-8
Received August 14, 2013
Revision received November 10, 2014
Accepted November 17, 2014
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
12 NEWMAN ET AL.
... The first aim of the current study is to investigate the influence of deepfakes on people's credibility judgments of a news article. Given that audio-visual information tends to garner higher credibility than textual information based on the truthiness effect (Newman et al. 2015) and realism heuristic (Sundar 2007), fake news involving deepfakes can be particularly problematic than text or photo-based fake news (Hwang, Ryu, and Jeong 2021). However, if people can distinguish deepfakes from authentic videos and thus not use deepfakes as evidence for fake news claims, the concern over deepfakes can be reduced. ...
... Research supports this tactic because people are more easily attuned to visuals than text (Pieters and Wedel 2004). Third, a photo can enhance the credibility of written reports due to the truthiness effect (Newman et al. 2015). By serving as pseudo-evidence to a claim, a picture that is somewhat relevant but not probative can increase people's credibility toward the claim. ...
... The theoretical importance of studying deepfakes concerns the aforementioned truthiness effect (Newman et al. 2015) and the realism heuristic (Sundar 2007) people can bring in when judging the deepfake-based misinformation. The realism heuristic refers to a mental shortcut when evaluating information that audio-visual modality content is deemed more credible than text modality content. ...
Article
With rapid technical advancements, deepfakes-i.e., hyper-realistic fake videos using face swaps-have become more widespread and easier to create, challenging the old notion of "seeing is believing." Despite raised concerns over potential impacts of deepfakes on people's credibility toward audiovisual evidence in journalism, systematic investigation of the topic has been lacking. This study conducted an experiment (N = 230) that tested (1) how a news article using deepfake video (vs. real video) affects news credibility and viral behavioral intentions and (2) whether, based on signaling theory, obtaining knowledge about the low cost of producing deepfakes reduces the impact of deepfake news. Results show that people whose pre-existing attitudes toward controversial issues (abortion, marijuana legalization) are congruent with the advocated position of a news article are more likely to believe and be willing to share deepfake news as much as real video news. In addition, educating participants about the low cost of producing deepfakes was effective in reducing the credibility and viral behavioral intention of deepfake news for those who have congruent issue attitudes. This study provides evidence for differing levels of susceptibility for deepfake news and the importance of media literacy education regarding deepfakes that would prevent biased reasoning.
... The truth status of this claim is likely ambiguous for most people who have little information available to determine the correct response. However, a growing literature suggests that if a photo of a hippopotamus appeared with this trivia question, people may be more inclined to think the question was true compared to if it appeared alone (Abed et al., 2017;Fenn et al., 2013;Newman et al., 2012;Newman et al., 2015;Newman et al., 2020). Regardless of whether the claim is true or false, photos can make less-known trivia claims feel more truthful, even if the photo lacks useful information (i.e., the color of the milk). ...
... Regardless of whether the claim is true or false, photos can make less-known trivia claims feel more truthful, even if the photo lacks useful information (i.e., the color of the milk). Importantly, the photo must be contextually related to the claim for this effect to occur (see Newman et al., 2015). This bias towards truth in the presence of related, but nonprobative (i.e., not indicative of truth) information such as photos has been coined truthiness. ...
... Subsequently, truthiness occurs when people use this fluency as a heuristic (mental shortcut) for truth (see also Tversky & Kahneman, 1973). Specifically, people can mistake the ease with which they process the trivia claim as evidence that the claim is true rather than correctly attributing that ease to the presence of the non-probative but related photograph (Newman et al., 2015;Schwarz & Newman, 2017; see also fluency misattribution, Jacoby & Whitehouse, 1989). ...
Article
Full-text available
When semantically-related photos appear with true-or-false trivia claims, people more often rate the claims as true compared to when photos are absent-truthiness. This occurs even when the photos lack information useful for assessing veracity. We tested whether truthiness changed in magnitude as a function of participants' age in a diverse sample using materials appropriate for all ages. We tested participants (N = 414; Age range = 3-87 years) in two culturally diverse environments: a community science center (First language: English (61.4%); Mandarin/Cantonese (11.6%); Spanish (6%), other (21%); ethnicity: unreported) and a psychology lab (First language: English (64.4%); Punjabi (9.8%); Mandarin/Cantonese (7.4%); other (18.4%); ethnicity: Caucasian (38%); South Asian (30.7%); Asian (22.7%); other/unreported (8.6%). Participants rated trivia claims as true or false. Half the claims appeared with a semantically related photo, and half appeared without a photo. Results showed that participants of all ages more often rated claims as true when claims appeared with a photo; however, this truthiness effect was stable across the lifespan. If truthiness age differences exist, they are likely negligible in the general population. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
... Some studies have shown that when participants encounter information in tandem with a photograph, they rate that information as feeling more subjectively true than they would have otherwise, even when the photograph does not provide evidence in support of the relevant information (Newman, Garry, Bernstein, Kantner, & Lindsay, 2012;Newman et al., 2015). ...
Preprint
Digital technologies have changed the everyday use of human memory. When information is saved or made readily available online, there is less need to encode or maintain access to that information within the biological structures of memory. People increasingly depend on the Internet and various digital devices to learn and remember, but the implications and consequences of this dependence remain largely unknown. The present chapter provides an overview of research to date on memory in the digital age. It focuses in particular on issues related to transactive memory, cognitive offloading, photo taking, social media use, and learning in the classroom.
... In published articles discussing deepfake societal implications, the authors have brought many anecdotal shreds of evidence leading to the prospect of mass production and diffusion of deepfakes by malicious actors and how it could present extremely serious challenges to society [19]. Since the social perspectives are based on "seeing is believing" or "a picture is worth a thousands of words", images and videos have much stronger persuasive power than text; thus citizens have comparatively weak defenses against visual deception of this kind [42]. Particularly the research by [59] investigated how people can be deceived by political deepfake, and it turns out that society is more likely to feel uncertain than to be completely misled by deepfakes. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Deepfakes are synthetic content generated using advanced deep learning and AI technologies. The advancement of technology has created opportunities for anyone to create and share deepfakes much easier. This may lead to societal concerns based on how com�munities engage with it. However, there is limited research available to understand how communities perceive deepfakes. We examined deepfake conversations on Reddit from 2018 to 2021—including major topics and their temporal changes as well as implications of these conversations. Using a mixed-method approach—topic modeling and qualitative coding, we found 6,638 posts and 86,425 comments discussing concerns of the believable nature of deep�fakes and how platforms moderate them. We also found Reddit conversations to be pro-deepfake and building a community that supports creating and sharing deepfake artifacts and building a marketplace regardless of the consequences. Possible implications derived from qualitative codes indicate that deepfake conversations raise societal concerns. We propose that there are implications for Human Computer Interaction (HCI) to mitigate the harm created from deepfakes.
... In published articles discussing deepfake societal implications, the authors have brought many anecdotal shreds of evidence leading to the prospect of mass production and diffusion of deepfakes by malicious actors and how it could present extremely serious challenges to society [19]. Since the social perspectives are based on "seeing is believing" or "a picture is worth a thousands of words", images and videos have much stronger persuasive power than text; thus citizens have comparatively weak defenses against visual deception of this kind [42]. Particularly the research by [59] investigated how people can be deceived by political deepfake, and it turns out that society is more likely to feel uncertain than to be completely misled by deepfakes. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Deepfakes are synthetic content generated using advanced deep learning and AI technologies. The advancement of technology has created opportunities for anyone to create and share deepfakes much easier. This may lead to societal concerns based on how communities engage with it. However, there is limited research available to understand how communities perceive deepfakes. We examined deepfake conversations on Reddit from 2018 to 2021 -- including major topics and their temporal changes as well as implications of these conversations. Using a mixed-method approach -- topic modeling and qualitative coding, we found 6,638 posts and 86,425 comments discussing concerns of the believable nature of deepfakes and how platforms moderate them. We also found Reddit conversations to be pro-deepfake and building a community that supports creating and sharing deepfake artifacts and building a marketplace regardless of the consequences. Possible implications derived from qualitative codes indicate that deepfake conversations raise societal concerns. We propose that there are implications for Human Computer Interaction (HCI) to mitigate the harm created from deepfakes.
... Des affirmations insérées dans un contexte conceptuel cohérent ont tendance à être jugées plus vraies que des affirmations insérées dans un contexte conceptuel moins cohérent (Parks & Toth, 2006). Des affirmations accompagnées d'une illustration non-corroborante (i.e., elle n'apporte pas d'information en plus de l'affirmation) sont jugées plus vraies que des affirmations présentées seules (Newman, Jalbert, Schwarz, & Ly, 2020 ;Newman et al., 2015 ;Newman, Gary, Bernstein, Kantner, & Lindsay, 2012). Du fait de cette famille d'« effets de vérité », il a été suggéré que tous reposent essentiellement sur un mécanisme commun, soit la fluence. ...
Thesis
La prolifération rapide de fausses informations est une face obscure de la diffusion massive d’informations. Comprendre comment nous jugeons la vérité des informations que nous rencontrons s’avère crucial. L’exposition répétée aux informations augmente la tendance à les juger vraies. Cet effet de vérité est couramment expliqué par la familiarité, qui serait incorrectement attribuée à la vérité des informations en l’absence de recollection, soit le souvenir précis d’y avoir été exposé avant. Nous pointons des limites de cette hypothèse et proposons une alternative : l’hypothèse de correspondance duale, qui suppose que nous évaluons la vérité des informations à travers leur correspondance avec des contenus récupérés en mémoire. La récupération de ces contenus peut être basée sur la familiarité comme sur la recollection, et ce en particulier lorsque nous ne pouvons pas récupérer des indices de vérité. Dégrader la recollection devrait ainsi augmenter l’effet de vérité pour l’hypothèse de familiarité, mais le diminuer pour l’hypothèse de correspondance duale. Nous avons estimé les mérites relatifs des deux hypothèses en manipulant l’attention à l’encodage et le délai. Les deux hypothèses expliquent aussi bien certains résultats ; des résultats que nous pensions attendus sous les deux hypothèses n’ont pas été mis en évidence ; et l’hypothèse de correspondance duale rend mieux compte de certains résultats que l’hypothèse de familiarité, mais l’inverse est aussi le cas. Dans un volet plus appliqué de la thèse, nous suggérons que l’effet de vérité pourrait exister avec des théories du complot dans des réanalyses corrélationnelles de deux enquêtes à grande échelle. L’hypothèse de correspondance duale est une alternative pertinente à l’hypothèse de familiarité, mais la confrontation des deux hypothèses est à poursuivre pour mieux comprendre les processus de mémoire impliqués dans l’effet de vérité. Cet effet pourrait en outre être impliqué dans des phénomènes sociétaux comme le conspirationnisme, invitant à doter les études d’une plus grande validité externe.
... In many published articles discussing deepfake's social implications, the authors have brought many anecdotal shreds of evidence leading to the prospect of mass production and diffusion of deepfakes by malicious actors and how it could present extremely serious challenges to society [32]. Since the social perspectives are based on "seeing is believing" or "a picture worth thousands of words," images and videos have much stronger persuasive power than text where citizens have comparatively weak defenses against visual deception of this kind [80]. Particularly the research by [113] tried to understand how people can be deceived by political deepfakes, and it turns out that society is more likely to feel uncertain than to be completely misled by deepfakes. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Deepfakes created in the form of image, audio and video by leveraging AI are far more realistic to identify if its being synthetically created by replacing someone else's voice or video. This is an emerging concern as the implications of such technology may distress the society. Currently, much of the scholarly research is centered on the technology of deepfake, but sparse in understanding how the emergence of deep-fakes impacts society. In this chapter, we provide (1) an understanding of deepfake related research with a focus on societal implications through a systematic review and (2) an empirical understanding of the societal implications of deepfake by analyzing Reddit conversations related to deepfakes from 2018 to 2021. A systematic review found 787 deepfake related research, yet only 88 were depicting any social implications side of deepfakes. The majority were literature reviews and less focused on empirical evidence. Our empirical study provided evidence of possible implications concerning society. We finally provide 5 directions to mitigate deepfake societal harms.
Article
Full-text available
Some research suggests people are overconfident because of personality characteristics, lack of insight, or because overconfidence is beneficial in its own right. But other research fits with the possibility that fluent experience in the moment can rapidly drive overconfidence. For example, fluency can push people to become overconfident in their ability to throw a dart, know how rainbows form or predict the future value of a commodity. But surely there are limits to overconfidence. That is, even in the face of fluency manipulations known to increase feelings of confidence, reasonable people would reject the thought that they, for example, might be able to land a plane in an emergency. To address this question, we conducted two experiments comprising a total of 780 people. We asked some people (but not others) to watch a trivially informative video of a pilot landing a plane before they rated their confidence in their own ability to land a plane. We found watching the video inflated people's confidence that they could land a plane. Our findings extend prior work by suggesting that increased semantic context creates illusions not just of prior experience or understanding—but also of the ability to actually do something implausible.
Article
Why do consumers sometimes fall for spurious claims – e.g., brain training games that prevent cognitive decline, toning sneakers that sculpt one's body, flower essence that cures depression – and how can consumers protect themselves in the modern world where information is shared quickly and easily? As cognitive scientists, we view this problem through the lens of what we know, more generally, about how people evaluate information for its veracity, and how people update their beliefs. That is, the same processes that support true belief can also encourage people to sometimes believe misleading or false information. Anchoring on the large literature on truth and belief updating allows predictions about consumer behavior; it also highlights possible solutions while casting doubt on other possible responses to misleading communications.
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter reviews the context sensitivity of people's inferences from subjective experiences, focusing on inferences of ease of recall and processing fluency.
Article
B. W. A. Whittlesea and L. D. Williams (1998, 2000) proposed the discrepancy-attribution hypothesis to explain the source of feelings of familiarity. By that hypothesis, people chronically evaluate the coherence of their processing. When the quality of processing is perceived as being discrepant from that which could be expected, people engage in an attributional process; the feeling of familiarity occurs when perceived discrepancy is attributed to prior experience. In the present article, the authors provide convergent evidence for that hypothesis and show that it can also explain feelings of familiarity for nonlinguistic stimuli. They demonstrate that the perception of discrepancy is not automatic but instead depends critically on the attitude that people adopt toward their processing, given the task and context. The connection between the discrepancy-attribution hypothesis and the "revelation effect" is also explored (e.g., D. L. Westerman & R. L. Greene, 1996).
Chapter
The stream of consciousness has many tributaries. At any given moment some are dry beds, others gently burbling brooks, yet others gushing torrents. The main current is not always mindful of the sources of its flow, such that, for example, the waters of the wellspring of memory may mingle with the freshet of insight. But, to push the metaphor past the breaking point, we’d be at sea if we were completely unable to distinguish observation from expectation, reality from wish or fear, inherent ease from familiarity, etc. Thus the mind/brain needs mechanisms for monitoring (albeit imperfectly and at varying levels of specificity) the sources of influence on its own productions.
Chapter
The editors of this volume asked all us contributors to say something nice about the honouree. Okay: he writes long, convoluted articles packed with experiments (probably some kind of compensation for his shortness); he does have a nice ‘conference’ voice (resulting from liberal application of scotch to his tonsils); and he may be nearly as smart as he thinks he is. Best I can do.
Article
In the accompanying article (B. W. A. Whittlesea & L. D. Williams, 2001), surprising violation of an expectation was observed to cause an illusion of familiarity. The authors interpreted that evidence as support for the discrepancy-attribution hypothesis. This article extended the scope of that hypothesis, investigating the consequences of surprising validation of expectations. Subjects were shown recognition probes as completions of sentence stems. Their expectations were manipulated by presenting predictive, nonpredictive, and inconsistent stems. Predictive stems caused an illusion of familiarity, but only when the subjects also experienced uncertainty about the outcome. That is, as predicted by the discrepancy-attribution hypothesis, feelings of familiarity occurred only when processing of a recognition target caused surprise. The article provides a discussion of the ways in which a perception of discrepancy can coma about, as well as the origin and nature of unconscious expectations.