ArticlePDF Available

The Impact of Goal-setting on Worker Performance - Empirical Evidence from a Real-effort Production Experiment

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

This paper examines the influence of goal-setting on worker performance in an industrial production process. For empirical examination, we conducted a real-effort experiment at the Training Factory for Energy Productivity at the Technische Universität München. The participants’ performance was measured by checking for quantity and quality of the assembled products and furthermore by recording the consumed compressed air per finished good. In total four groups were defined, each group in a different experimental setting. This experiment is the first one ever conducted related to goal-setting in an industrial production setting and thus adds valuable results to academia and practitioners in the field of sustainable manufacturing. The major results are that even without financial incentives goal-setting improves worker performance by 12 to 15% compared to the situation where no goals were defined. This holds true for the groups which had to maximize either output quantity or output quality, as well as for the group which was obliged to be as energy efficient as possible.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Procedia CIRP 26 ( 2015 ) 127 132
Available online at www.sciencedirect.com
2212-8271 © 2015 Published by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).
Peer-review under responsibility of Assembly Technology and Factory Management/Technische Universität Berlin.
doi: 10.1016/j.procir.2015.02.086
ScienceDirect
12th Global Conference on Sustainable Manufacturing
The impact of goal-setting on worker performance - empirical
evidence from a real-effort production experiment
Sven Asmusa*, Florian Karlb, Alwine Mohnena, Gunther Reinhartb
aChair of Corporate Management, TUM School of Management, Technische Universität München, Arcisstrasse 21, 81369 Munich, Germany
bInstitute for Machine Tools and Industrial Management, Technische Univer sität München, Boltzmannstrasse 15, 85748 Garching, Germany
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +49 89 289 24074; fax: +49 89 289 24075. E-mail address: sven.asmus@tum.de
Abstract
This paper examines the influence of goal-setting on worker performance in an industrial production process. For empirical
examination, we conducted a real-effort experiment at the Training Factory for Energy Productivity at the Technische
Universität München. The participants’ performance was measured by checking for quantity and quality of the assembled
products and furthermore by recording the consumed compressed air per finished good. In total four groups were defined,
each group in a different experimental setting. This experiment is the first one ever conducted related to goal-setting in an
industrial production setting and thus adds valuable results to academia and practitioners in the field of sustainable
manufacturing. The major results are that even without financial incentives goal-setting improves worker performance by 12
to 15% compared to the situation where no goals were defined. This holds true for the groups which had to maximize either
output quantity or output quality, as well as for the group which was obliged to be as energy efficient as possible.
© 2014 The Authors. Published by Elsevier B.V.
Peer-review under responsibility of Assembly Technology and Factory Management/Technische Universität Berlin.
Keywords: goal-setting; sustainable manufacturing; industrial production
1. Introduction
Every relationship between a principle and an agent is
characterized by information asymmetry [1,2]. Since the
employer is not able to monitor the effort of the employees
permanently, certain measures to motivate the staff have to be
offered to the workforce. One suitable way to motivate is to
set goals [3]. The implementation of goals in everyday work
is manifold starting with sales goals for marketing managers
or production goals for blue collar worker in industrial
production [4]. From a research perspective, the impact of
goal-setting on human behavior is an interesting and already
well-examined topic. Starting in the late 1960’s [5,6], studies
focused on the effect of consciously set goals on the
performance of individuals with a major focus on the number
of solved tasks or produced units within a certain time frame.
Thereby most of the experimental studies focused on daily
office tasks like telephone services [7] or typing [8].
What has not been observed so far, is the effect of goal-setting
on real production tasks. Furthermore there is only little
knowledge about the impact of setting goals related to other
goal dimensions than output quantity. Given the above, the
presented study is examining if the findings of previous
studies about goal-setting are applicable to a real industrial
production setting and furthermore transferable explicitly to
other goal dimensions, in this case output quality and energy
efficiency as two of the major goals in modern production.
2. Literature and Hypotheses
The goal-setting theory is strongly affected by Ryan’s [9]
assumption that human behavior is driven by objectives, so-
called goals [10]. Taking this relationship into account,
primarily Latham and Locke [10,11,12], among others,
focused their research on the link between consciously set
goals and the observable task performance of individuals.
Thereby their major interest was on explaining and
forecasting human performance on several types of tasks and
furthermore on affecting someone’s performance by different
© 2015 Published by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).
Peer-review under responsibility of Assembly Technology and Factory Management/Technische Universität Berlin.
128 Sven Asmus et al. / Procedia CIRP 26 ( 2015 ) 127 – 132
types of measures [10]. The findings of numerous
experimental studies show that goal-setting strongly
influences human behavior and thus leads to changes in
performance [13,14,15,16]. This holds true for varying tasks,
different groups of participants and diverse experimental
settings. However, so far goal-setting in industrial production
processes is not well-examined. Based on the results of
previous experimental studies in non-production settings
hypothesis 1 is as follows:
H1: Goal-setting improves task performance in an industrial
production setting.
Our study not only aims to examine the effects of goal-
setting in industrial production, but moreover to distinguish
sharply the effects of goal-setting on different kind of goal
dimensions. The main focus of previous studies was on the
measure of the impact of goal-setting on task quantity, mostly
defined as the time needed to perform a certain task, either
doing calculations [5], specific reading tasks [14] or reacting
on a signal light [17]. The effects of goal-setting on other
important goal dimensions like output quality have not been
neglected. Energy consumption as an important factor for
industrial production processes, so far has not been examined
within a goal-setting situation. Hence, to strengthen the
explanatory power of goal-setting theory in a production
process, the presented experiment focuses on the impact of
goal-setting on the three described goal dimensions, output
quantity, output quality and energy consumption. Therefore
the sub-hypotheses of H1 are as follows:
H1a: Goal-setting increases the output quantity.
H1b: Goal-setting increases the output quality.
H1c: Goal-setting decreases the consumed energy per output
unit.
In 1990, Locke & Latham introduced the High
Performance Cycle (HPC) [18] which integrates the essential
elements of goal-setting theory as shown in figure 1.
Fig. 1. The High Performance Cycle (HPC).
While specificity and difficulty as the goal core
dimensions as well as certain moderators and mechanisms
influence the performance, the potential achievements e.g.
productivity or cost improvement have a strong impact on the
individuals’ satisfaction and the willingness to cope with
challenging tasks and objectives in the future [10,18]. Both
difficulty and specificity have been investigated extensively.
Several empirical studies show that the higher the difficulty of
a goal is, the higher is the performance of the individuals that
executed the task [13,16]. Notably, a performance increase is
only possible, until the limit of a persons’ ability is reached
[19]. Experiments show that specific goals lead to greater
achievements compared to non-specified or vague defined
goals [13,17]. Therefore so-called ‘do-your-best-goals’ are
not sufficient to generate optimal performance [12,16].
However, since specific goal-setting is not always easy to
implement, for instance due to a lack of information about the
potential range of performance outcomes, even broadly
formulated goals like maximizing the output or minimizing
the input can lead to significantly improved performance. Due
to the design of this experiment, our formulated goals are not
specified further than maximizing the output quantity,
maximizing the number of assembled goods having a defined
quality level and using as little energy as possible to perform
the task.
As one of the four mechanisms, persistence is integrated as
a potential mediator on task performance in the High
Performance Cycle [10,18]. In this regard, persistence can be
interpreted as an indicator for the development of task
performance over time or as a potential proxy how people
change their effort level due to certain time constraints while
performing a task. As already empirically proven, people
extend their effort when having tight time constraints
compared to situations with loose deadlines [5,11].
Based on the power curve model of Wright [20] and
innumerable other approaches to show and explain learning
curves, it is known that people are able to improve their
performance over time when doing repeated tasks.
Furthermore Dar-el et al. [21] found out that cognitive and
motorial elements occur in different stages when learning
industrial tasks through repetition. So far no clear evidence
exists on the interaction of goal-setting and learning effects on
task performance. Therefore it is of high interest, if goal-
setting leads to changes in the development of task
performance over time when performing repeated tasks due to
occurring learning effects. To examine the described
relationship, hypothesis 2 is as follows:
H2: Goal-setting intensifies learning effects and leads
therefore to additional task performance improvement
when doing repeated tasks.
3. Method
3.1. Experimental setting
Between July and August 2013 the presented experiment
was conducted at the Training Factory for Energy
Productivity at the Technische Universität München, Munich,
Germany. Since the production line of the training factory
used as the setting for this experiment is quite similar to real
industrial production, this experiment combines elements of
economic laboratory and field experiments. Because
129
Sven Asmus et al. / Procedia CIRP 26 ( 2015 ) 127 – 132
comparability of participants’ performance should be as high
as possible, we chose the final step of the whole production
chain as the setting for the experiment, where participants
were asked to assemble gearboxes. This process step ensured
the highest controllability of external parameters.
3.2. Participants
We invited 120 subjects, who were assigned to four
different treatment groups. More than 95% of the participants
were regular bachelor and master students of the two major
Munich universities with an average age of 24 years. To
ensure high comparability of the subject pools of the four
groups, the share of females was held equal between 20.00
and 23.33% in each group. Besides the set gender distribution,
participants were randomly assigned to the groups. With
about 92% of all subjects, the vast majority was right-handed.
Furthermore it was possible to adjust the used pneumatic
screwdriver for left or right-hand-use. Therefore no
distinction respectively special allocation to the groups based
on the handedness was made.
3.3. Task and goals
All participants were provided with the same workplace.
To assemble the gearboxes, the workplace was equipped with
a pneumatic screwdriver to screw bolts into the gearboxes (six
bolts per gearbox), 16 unassembled gearboxes, a large number
of bolts, a flow meter which displayed the accumulated
consumed compressed air, a pressure balancer to control the
pressure level of the pneumatic screwdriver, as well as a
digital torque wrench to check the torque level of the bolts in
the assembled gearboxes.
For evaluating participants’ performance, the number of
screwed bolts, the consumed compressed air, as well as the
average torque reach level of each assembled gearbox were
measured and noted down after each round. Based on the
treatment group participants were assigned to, they were
asked upfront to achieve certain goals as already described in
detail:
Group 1 (Control Group) - No goal.
Group 2 (Quantity Group) Quantity maximization
Group 3 (Quality Group) Quality maximization.
Group 4 (Energy Group) Energy consumption minimization.
As it can be seen above, group 1 as the control group, had
no goal to achieve, while group 2, 3 and 4 each had one
defined goal. The goal of group 2 was the maximization of the
output, measured through the number of assembled gearboxes
and the thereby installed bolts. The goal of group 4 was the
minimization of the input factor compressed air while
performing the assembly task. The goal of group 3 was more
complicated to formulate for that experiment, because the
potential measures for quality in production are both manifold
and difficult to implement. Finally the quality goal was set as
reaching an average bolt torque of 2.0 to 2.5 Nm for every
assembled gearbox and its six bolts. This level was set for two
reasons: on the one hand side to avoid potential releasing of
the bolts after a certain operating time due to a too low bolt
torque, on the other hand side to avoid potential damages to
the bolts and gearboxes due to a too high bolt torque.
3.4. Procedure
As already described, the presented part of the experiment
consisted of four different experimental groups. As it can be
seen in figure 2, the sequence of the experiment was equal to
all participants.
Fig. 2. Sequence of the experi ment.
First of all a presentation and a video with a detailed
description of the task to perform, the workplace and the
available equipment had been shown. Subsequently a trial
round of five minutes started, in which participants should
become acquainted to the task. After that trial round again a
presentation was shown to familiarize participants even more
with the equipment available, to evaluate first own
experiences of the trial round and to clarify open questions.
Subsequently, the first round with a duration of five minutes
started, followed by a break of one minute. After that, the
same procedure as in round 1 started with the beginning of the
second round, followed again by a one minute break and
finally followed by the third and last round of the experiment.
While participants had the chance to use the breaks to relax,
the experimentator wrote down the results of all three goal
dimensions of the previous period on a scoring sheet.
After the final round all participants were provided with a
questionnaire asking for demographics and task-related
information. After finishing this questionnaire, participants
got paid their fixed compensation of 9€. Based on how fast
participants clicked through the presentations and filled out
the questions at the end, the total duration of the experiment
was between 40 and 55 minutes.
By showing presentations and videos to the participants
instead of letting the content be explained by the
experimentator, a highly standardized process for all
participants was secured, avoiding for instance influences
resulting from even unintentionally change of experimentator
130 Sven Asmus et al. / Procedia CIRP 26 ( 2015 ) 127 – 132
behavior from one participant to another and securing that
every participant got every information presented from the
same perspective and in the same sequence. The only
difference between group 1, having no goal and the other
three groups each having one goal defined, was that in the
presentations before every round, except of the trial round,
one slide explicitly mentioned the particular goal of that
group. No goal was mentioned to group 1, maximizing the
output to group 2, reaching the particular average bolt torque
for as many as possible assembled gearboxes to group 3 and
minimizing the used compressed air per assembled gearbox
respectively per bolt to group 4.
4. Results
4.1. The influence of goal-setting on task performance
For testing the first hypothesis about the general influence
of goal-setting on task performance, three two-sample t-tests
with equal variances had been conducted to test for significant
differences of the performance outcomes between the groups.
In the following, the results regarding the three sub-
hypotheses of H1 will be described and visualized for the
corresponding treatment group compared to the control group
(group 1).
In case of output quantity as the first goal dimension, the
results of group 1 are compared to group 2, as the group with
the goal of assembling as many gearboxes as possible in the
given time. With t(58) = -3.1862, p < .01, group 2, as the one
with the quantity objective, shows a significantly higher
performance than group 1 with no objective. Figure 3
visualizes the total number of bolts subsumed over all three
rounds comparing both groups. While the participants of
group 1 reached on average a total number of 88.6 bolts,
group 2 generated with 101.1 bolts more than 15% more
output in the three rounds.
Fig. 3. Comparison of the means of the total number of bolts over three
rounds for group 1 and 2.
Comparing the influence of goal-setting on task
performance regarding output quality, group 1 with no
objective and group 3 as the one with the quality objective are
taken into account. With t(58) = -0.5001, p > .05 no
significant difference between the groups does exist. With
about 24.1% target achievement, the group with the quality
objective reached a slightly higher share compared to the
control group with 21.5%, but not on a significant level.
To check whether or not goal-setting has a significant
influence on the participants performance regarding the
minimization of used compressed air for executing the task,
the consumed compressed air per bolt is compared for group 1
and group 4. With t(58) = 5.9636, p < .001 the difference is
highly significant. Group 4, as the group with the energy
saving objective, consumed with 6.23 liter per bolt
significantly less than group 1 which used on average 7.00
liter compressed air per bolt. The graphical results can be seen
in figure 4, where the average consumed compressed air level
per bolt over three rounds for group 1 and group 4 are shown.
Fig. 4. Comparison of the means of consu med compressed air per bolt over
three rounds for group 1 and 4.
Summarizing the results of hypothesis 1 and its sub-
hypothesis, goal-setting in the given industrial production
setting leads to a higher performance when subsuming the 3
experimental rounds. This counts for all three one-objective
groups compared to the control group. Regarding the
objectives output quantity and energy consumption, the
differences are very, respectively highly significant, while for
the quality goal the difference is not significant.
4.2. Goal-setting as an intensifier of learning effects when
performing repeated tasks
We now examine if goal-setting intensifies learning effects
and if therefore the change in task performance over the three
rounds is different if a goal is given or not. For that reason, we
ran panel regression analyses analyzing the performance
change of the control group and the respective comparison
group for every goal dimension.
First of all the change in produced quantity over the three
experimental rounds had been compared between group 1 as
the control group and group 2 as the group with the goal to
maximize the output quantity. With F(1,59) = 7.71, p < .01,
the F-statistics of the panel regression analysis shows that the
control group increased the output quantity from round to
131
Sven Asmus et al. / Procedia CIRP 26 ( 2015 ) 127 – 132
round significantly, with a coefficient of 1.42. With F(1,59) =
49.91, p < .001 group 2 increased the quantity as the
considered performance indicator significantly too, having a
much higher coefficient of 2.42. These results are visualized
on the left part of figure 5 where the rounds are displayed on
the x-axis while the number of bolts is shown on the y-axis. It
becomes clear that regarding the quantity, goal-setting works
as an intensifier of learning effects. Additionally is has to be
mentioned that the model’s validity for group 2 is with R² =
.4583 much higher than for group 1 with R² = .1156.
Fig. 5. Quantity and quality development over the three rounds comparing
group 1 and group 2 for quantity and group 1 and group 3 for quality.
Figure 5 shows on the right hand side the development of
the quality performance comparing group 1 and group 3. The
rounds are again displayed on the x-axis and the target
achievement in percent on the y-axis. It becomes clear that the
control group had a less steep learning curve from round 1 to
round 2 compared to the objective group, but both groups
decreased in their performance from round 2 to round 3.
Fig. 6. Energy consumption development over the three rounds comparing
group 1 and group 4.
Taking a closer look on the performance development of
group 1 and 4 related to the energy consumption, group 4 as
the group with the objective to use as little compressed air as
possible started in round 1 on a much lower consumption
level compared to the group 1 and had stronger learning
effects from round 1 to round 2 and from round 2 to round 3.
Nonetheless the learning effects of both groups are not
significant as it is illustrated in figure 6. This figure displays
the results of the panel regression with the rounds on the x-
axes and the energy consumption per bolt on the y-axes.
To prove whether or not goal-setting leads to higher
performance due to enhanced learning effects for the
objectives quality and energy too, panel regression analysis
for group 1 and the objective groups had been performed.
Contrary to output quantity, no significant improvements
based on learning effects over the three rounds did occur,
neither for output quality nor for the energy consumption.
Furthermore the validity of the models had been comparable
low with R² = .0156 for the panel regression of group 1
regarding quality and R² = .0161 for group 3 regarding quality
and R² = .0146 for the panel regression of group 1 related to
energy and R² = .0535 for group 4.
5. Discussion
This experiment aimed to show the effect of goal-setting
on human behavior. While this was already examined
extensively in several experimental studies [13,15] mostly for
output quantity as the measure of interest, other goal
dimensions were not included extensively. Analyzing the
results of the current study it becomes clear that our
hypothesis suggesting that goal-setting improves task
performance can be fully supported for the goal dimension
output quantity. With a significant higher output for the
quantity-objective group compared to the control group, the
results are in accordance with the existing literature
[22,23,24].
Regarding the goal dimension output quality, the results of
the experiment go into the same direction. Even the
performance differences between the control group and the
quality-objective group are not significant, the objective
group outperformed the control group by a 12% higher target
achievement seen in relative numbers. Even though the target
achievements are with 21.5% for the control group and 24.1%
for the objective group comparable low, the relative
performance difference between both groups is not negligible.
Of eminently importance, not only for the field of
sustainable manufacturing, are the findings regarding the
energy consumption goal. Comparing the results of group 1 as
the control group and group 4 having the objective to use as
less compressed air as possible to assemble the gear boxes,
the objective group used with 6.23 liter compressed air per
bolt significantly less than the control group with 7.00 liter
per bolt. The results for all three different observed goal
dimensions show that the goal-setting theory based on Latham
& Locke [10,11,12] is also applicable to other goal
dimensions than output quantity and works as well in an
industrial production setting.
Analyzing the results of hypothesis 2 that goal-setting
works as an intensifier of learning effects, a somewhat similar
statement can be derived for all three goal dimensions. For the
objective groups the learning curves of the participants appear
steeper compared to the control group. While the performance
132 Sven Asmus et al. / Procedia CIRP 26 ( 2015 ) 127 – 132
difference regarding quantity is significant comparing the
control group and the quantity-objective group, the
differences in the learning curves of energy consumption have
been demonstrated clearly in a figure, but are not statistically
significant. The same counts for the quality goal. While
control group and quality-objective group both improved their
level of task achievement from round 1 to round 2, their
performance dropped down from round 2 to round 3. Even the
performance increase to the second round was larger and the
decrease in performance from the second to the third round
smaller for the quality-objective group compared to the
control group, the differences are not statistically significant.
For all three goal dimensions it can be concluded that goal-
setting works as an intensifier of learning effects. The
strongest reinforcement has been measured for the quantity
goal.
6. Conclusion
First, this experiment aimed to assess if goal-setting theory
is applicable to other goal dimensions than output quantity.
Therefore output quality and energy consumption as further
relevant goal dimensions where integrated into the
experiment. Second, the experiment took place in a real
industrial production setting to figure out, if positive effects of
goal-setting are reproducible outside conventional
experimental laboratories. From the results we can conclude
that setting goals is one promising way to improve workers’
performance in industrial workplaces and that goal-setting
theory is of high relevance even far beyond conventional set
goals, being deployable to output quality and environmental
objectives, too. Due to scarce resources and increasing market
prices for energy, these results are of high relevance not only
for producing companies but more globally seen for the well-
being of our society. If the effects of goal-setting either on
traditional or currently emerging goals can be intensified by
incentives, has to be assessed in future research.
A further aim of the present study was about the
interaction of goal-setting and learning effects and its impact
on task performance. The results for all three goal dimensions,
output quantity, output quality and energy consumption
clearly indicate that goal-setting works as an intensifier of
learning effects. This counts especially for output quantity,
since the differences between control group and objective
group have been statistically significant. Future research
should further estimate the sustainability of goal-setting as an
intensifier of learning effects by conducting experimental
studies with extended task repetition.
The results furthermore show that goal-setting had
different impacts on the examined goal dimensions output
quantity, output quality and energy consumption. The reasons
for that can be manifold, e.g. related to the specificity of a
goal, the way a goal is framed or socio-demographic
characteristics of the people who are confronted with certain
goals. Even though it became clear that different goal
dimensions should not be treated equally, this field has to
undergo further research.
For the purpose of that study, the goals have not been
specified further than maximizing the output quantity,
reaching a certain quality level for as many as possible
assembled goods and using as little energy as possible to
perform the task. Specified goal level for every of the
dimensions should allow further statements about the concrete
impact of goal-setting.
References
[1] Jensen MC, Meckling WH. Theory of the firm: Manageri al behavior,
agency costs and ownership structure. Journal of Financial Economics
1976; 3(4); 305-360.
[2] Feess E. Mikroökonomie. Eine spieltheoretische- und
anwendungsorientierte Einführung. 3rd ed. Marburg: Metropolis; 2004.
[3] Locke EA, Shaw KN, Saari LM, Latham GP. Goal setting and task
performance: 1969-1980. Psych ological Bulletin 1981; 90(1); 125-152.
[4] Ivancevich JM, Smith SV. Goal setting interview skills training:
Simulated and on-the-job analyses. Journal of Applied Psychology 1981;
66(6); 697-705.
[5] Bryan JF, Locke EA. Parkinson's Law as a goal-setting phenomenon.
Organizational Beha vior and Human Performance 1967; 2(3); 258-275.
[6] Locke EA. Towards a th eory of task motivation and incentives.
Organizational Behavior and Human Performance 1968; 3(2); 157-189.
[7] Kim JS, Hamner WC. Effect of performance feedback and goal setting in
productivity and s atisfaction in an organizational setting. Journal of
Applied Psychology 1976; 61(1); 48-57.
[8] Latham GP, Yukl GA. Effects of assigned and participative goal setting
on performance and job satisfaction. Journal of Applied Psychology 1976;
61(2); 166-171.
[9] Ryan TA. Intentional behavior. New York: Ronald Press; 1970.
[10] Locke EA, Latham GP. Building a practically useful theory of goal
setting and task motivation: A 35-year odyssey. American Psychologist
2002; 57(9); 705-717.
[11] Latham GP, Locke EA. Increasing producti vity with decreasing t ime
limits: A field replication of Parkinson's Law. Journal of Applied
Psychology 1975; 60(4); 524-526.
[12] Latham GP, Locke EA. New developments in goal setting and task
performance. New York: Brunner-Routledge; 2013.
[13] Bandura A, Cervone D. Self-evaluative and self-efficacy mechanisms
governing the motivational effects of goal systems. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology 1983; 45(5); 1017-1028.
[14] LaPorte RE, Nath R. Role of performance goals in prose learning.
Journal of Educational Psychology 1976; 68(3); 260-264.
[15] Latham GP, Locke EA, Erez M. Resolving scientific disputes by the
joint design of crucial experiments by the antagonists: Application to the
Erez-Latham dispute regarding partic ipation in goal setting. Journal of
Applied Psychology 1988; 73(4); 753-772.
[16] Locke EA. Motivation through conscious goal setting. Applied and
Preventive Psych ology 1996; 5(2); 117-124.
[17] Locke EA, Chah D, Harrison S, Lustgarten N. Separating the effects of
goal specificity from goal level. Organizational Behavior & Human
Decision Processes 1989; 43(2); 270-287.
[18] Locke EA, Latham GP. A theory of goal setting & task performance.
Prentice-Hall, Inc.; 1990.
[19] Erez M, Zidon I. Effects of goal acceptance on the relationship of goal
difficulty to performance. Journal of Applied Psychology 1984; 69(1);
69-78.
[20] Wright T. Factors affecting the cost of airplanes. Journal of Aeronautical
Science 1936; 3(2); 122128.
[21] Dar-el EM, Ayas K, Gi lad A. A dual-phase model for the individual
learning process in industrial tasks. IEE Transactions 1995; 27(3); 265-
271.
[22] Latham GP, Kinne SB. Improving job performance through training in
goal setting. Journal of Applied Psychology 1974; 59(2); 187-191.
[23] Latham GP, Yukl GA. Assigned versus participative goal setting with
educated and uneducated wood workers. Journal of Applied Psychology
1975; 60(3); 299-302.
[24] Locke EA, Latham GP. Goal setting: A motivational technique that
works! Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Pren tice Hall; 1984.
... In their study, Asmus et al. (2015) examined the influence of the supervisor supervision behavior of goal-setting on employee performance in an industrial production process using staff of a training factory for energy productivity in Munich, Germany. The findings revealed that the supervisor's behavior of goal setting improved workers' performance in industrial workplaces. ...
... These findings were consistent with the Y theory which suggests that employees who exhibit interest in work should be treated well to further enhance their performance. However, except for the study by Ibok and Umana (2013), all the other studies obliquely implied supervision behaviors in concepts such as recognition (Aryan & Singh, 2015;Atambo et al., 2012;Bradler et al. (2016) and goal-setting (Asmus et al., 2015;Teo & Low, 2016). This study further examined the relationship between supervisors' behaviors and job performance of teachers, considering supervision behaviors on the conception of Mathieu et al. (2016) as referring to person-oriented and task-oriented behaviors. ...
... For instance, Aryan (2015) revealed that the supervisor's behaviour of recognition had a positive significant influence on performance of the employees. Similarly, Asmus et al. (2015) found out that the supervisor's behavior of goal setting improved workers' performance in industrial workplaces. Likewise, Atambo et al. (2012) reported that the supervision behavior of recognizing the employee's accomplishments translated into improved performance both at the individual and organizational levels. ...
Article
Full-text available
The study investigated the relationship between head teachers’ supervision practices and job performance of teachers in government aided secondary schools in Western Uganda. Head teachers supervision practices were studied in terms of supervision behavior, support and communication. Job performance of teachers was considered in terms of classroom teaching, management of students, discipline as well as regularity and interpersonal relations. The study adopted a cross-sectional research design using the quantitative approach on a sample of 333 teachers. Data were collected using both the questionnaire. Data were analysed using quantitative. Inferential analysis showed that while supervision behaviors and support had a positive and significant influence on job performance of teachers, supervision communication did not. It was concluded that supervision behaviors and support are important for the job performance of teachers. Therefore, it is recommends that head teachers should provide effective supervision to teachers because such a practice has a significant positive influence on teachers’ job performance.
... Goal setting theory stressed on importance of clarity of the goal to perform at the highest level and achieve success (Asmus et al., 2015). This theory magnifies importance of strategic alignment for performance improvement. ...
... Psychologist Edwin Locke's goal setting theory is one of the most popular models of employee mental readiness through strategic alignment. This theory believes that goal-setting process is collaborative effort that involves all parties imagining and working towards a common aim in tandem (Asmus et al., 2015). Goal setting theory outlines five requirements for goals as it need to be clear, specific, and easy to understand, push employees (challenging), pursues the objective wholeheartedly from its inception, provides feedback and direction throughout the process to maintain momentum or encourage improvement and set reasonable expectations and should divide larger projects into smaller, easier to tackle tasks with steps, milestones, and regular review (Teo & Low, 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
Strategic alignment deals with organizational strategic fit with functional areas strategies, such as human resources management strategy. It is based on goal setting premise which believes in collaborative effort that involves all parties imagining and working towards a common aim in tandem. Strategic alignment in this study takes assumption of goal setting theory stressed on importance of clarity of the goal to perform at the highest level and achieve success. The purpose of this study was to look into the impact of strategic alignment on organizational performance. Clarity in the aim, role clarity, and process clarity were discovered and examined for the measurement of organizational strategic alignment. The study took a quantitative approach with descriptive and explanatory research designs. Three Ethiopian universities were chosen based on generation of establishment to include 365 personnel in the sample using a random selection technique. Descriptive statistical tools such as mean and standard deviation were used, whereas structural equation models were used for confirmatory factor analysis and path analysis. According to the study’s findings, goal clarity, role clarity, and process clarity all have a significant and favorable effect on organizational performance in higher education. Findings of the study reveal also that organizational performance varies among study institution based on implementation level of strategic alignment. Based on the study’s findings, it is recommended that organizational leaders outline organizational strategic intents with specific goals. Thus, it is recommended that governing bodies need to promote defined roles and processes for all workers.
... Nkata (2020) views the directive inspection approach as affecting the teachers' professional development and consequently learner achievement. On the other hand, Anna (2015) sees the directive inspection approach as a means of developing professional acuity which helps them to improve faster. Sullivan and Glanz (2020), report that the directive inspection approach is instrumental to new teachers in becoming familiar and confident in their career and Anyagre (2014) similarly reports that veteran teachers in the USA enjoyed it because they realised instructional effectiveness in a short time. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated the effect of the directive and non-directive school inspection approaches on teacher instructional effectiveness. While teacher instructional effectiveness was studied in terms of reporting on pupil academic achievements, teacher attitudes, effective planning, attendance and pedagogical approaches, the directive School inspection approach was studied in terms of directing and evaluation of teacher knowledge yet the non-directive approach was studied in terms of team planning and consultations. A cross-sectional survey mixed research design using the quantitative and qualitative approaches on a sample of 178 people namely teachers, head teachers, school inspectors and education officers was adopted. While quantitative data was collected using survey questionnaires and analysed using inferential analysis, qualitative data was collected through interviews, focus group discussions and analysed by grouping the responses in themes which were merged with quantitative findings. The findings showed that all the attributes of directive inspection approach were positively insignificant to teacher instructional effectiveness explaining that they had no impact on teacher instructional effectiveness. Similarly, team planning had a positive and insignificant relationship meaning that it had no effect on teacher instructional effectiveness. In conclusion, only team planning had effect on teacher instructional effectiveness and consultation did not have any effect. It was recommended that school inspectors should use the non-directive inspection approach because of sub-variable team planning that had a positive effect on teacher instructional effectiveness.
... In the view of effective goal-setting ability is pivotal in achieving any task successfully. 31 In other words, learners with goalsetting skills (clearly defined and realistic goals) would be able to cope with learning demands or pressure. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aims to examine the extent to which learning efficacy and goal setting will interact with academic burnout among learners in the post covid-19 pandemic era. The quantitative research design was adopted for this study. A total of 1, 407 adolescents in Ibadan, Oyo State, Nigeria (male = 38.8%; female 61.2%) aged between 12 and 20 (mean = 15: SD = 4.24) responded to an online survey developed by the researchers, which was open for two months. Data were analysed using inferential statistics of linear logistics and regression analysis. The findings revealed that all the factors had a strong association with academic burnout, learning efficacy (OR = 1.02, 95% CI {1.01–1.04}) and goal setting (OR = 1.04, 95% CI {1.01–1.06}). Further, learning efficacy made the highest contribution to academic burnout (Beta = -.322, t= -12.629, p<0.05), and goal setting (Beta = -.234, t= -9.184, p<0.05). By implication, learning efficacy and goal setting played a significant role in reducing academic burnout of adolescent learners in the post-pandemic era for effective learning outcomes. It was, therefore, recommended based on this outcome that schools should employ professional school counsellors who can assist learners by adopting psychological interventions that are helpful to manage academic burnout to navigate adolescent learners through their academic life. Keywords: Adolescent Learners, Academic Goal Setting, Burnout, Learning Efficacy, Post-Covid-19 Era
... Findings on H2, are supported by Klein et al.,(2012), who states that Goal Commitment is associated with persistence and may therefore lead people who have (proactive) the most ambitious goals to persist in attaining them despite of all the challenges. On H3, the finding is in line with prior studies done by Locke and Latham (1984) and Asmus et al., (2015). In their study on the impact of goal-setting on worker performance, it became clear that their finding suggests that goal-setting and commitment improves task performance which leads to higher output quantity. ...
... From the diagram above, Chandrasekar (2011) maintains that employees are to be involved in setting performance and goal, either through their immediate supervisor or as part of an organisation formal performance management process. This become necessary as every relationship between a principal and an agent is characterized by information asymmetry, and since the employer is not able to monitor the effort of the employees permanently, certain measures to motivate staff have to be offered to the workforce (Asmus, Karl, Mohnen & Reinhart, 2015), while goals setting present the best way to achieve this (Locke, Shaw, Saari & Latham ,1981) From the diagram, with the millennia generation entering into employment in vast number, present a challenge of how to retain them, their career aspirations, attitude to work and with their uncomfortability with rigid corporate structures (PwS, 2011). Thus, poaching also creates the need for organisation to be flexible in managing their talent, so as not to lose them cheaply to rival organisations that are not ready to invest in training and at the same time, need already made talent On-the-job behaviour that will make them succeed, and help replacing their retiring boomer generation (Cooke, 2000;PwS, 2011;Nappinnai & Premavathy, 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
The paradigm shift in the world of work has created a new dimension in the labour market vis a vis employment relation; a point of reference is the flexibility of work and labour deployment in the Nigeria labour market. The study examines flexibility of work in employment relations and deployment of labour in the Nigeria labour market within the purview of relationship and the benefits to the employers and employees. Hence, the study takes an exploratory approach by reviewing extant literatures to understand the dynamics of work flexibility in employment relations and its influence on different patterns of labour deployment. The findings from the review of extant literatures revealed that there are various dimensions to flexibility of work in employment relations(workplace flexibility, workforce flexibility, low flexibility and mutual flexibility) and labor deployment(temporary worker, part time workers triangular employees etc.) in the Nigeria labour market and it often benefit the employer more because of the objective of maximizing return on investment by creating financial rigidity on the employee through cutting or reduction in the personnel cost or overhead cost. This study therefore recommends that a win-win flexible work dimension for deploying of workers for flexible work arrangement should be adopted to achieve a healthy employment relations practice in the Nigeria labour market.
Chapter
As discussed in the previous chapters, the implementation of learning factories has become increasingly widespread throughout the world in recent years. These learning factories come in various sizes, serving different purposes, scopes, and levels of sophistication, all aimed at providing a comprehensive learning experience for learners from both industry and academia in the field of production. The upcoming three chapters aim to provide a comprehensive overview of the diverse range of existing learning factories.
Article
Full-text available
This study talks about the challenges faced by the project manager to implement a digitized supply chain quickly and ways to overcome them in the healthcare industry. A combination of the Best-Worst Method and fuzzy DEMATEL is used to study the factors that would pose a challenge to a Project Manager. The output from the Best-Worst Method is fed as the input to the fuzzy DEMATEL method. The study showed that Presenteeism is one of the most influential factors in the system. It also shows that a Project Manager must be someone with a good knowledge of the business and its working. The findings indicate that for a project to become successful during novel working conditions, the Project Manager and the project team as a whole are responsible and need to come together.
Article
Full-text available
Penelitian ini bertujuan untuk mengetahui hubungan antara literasi digital, regulasi diri dengan produktivitas dosen di Kota Kediri dalam situasi normal baru. Adapun teknik pengambilan sampel menggunakan insidental sampling dan didapatkan sampel sebesar 111 orang. Teknik analisis menggunakan korelasi berganda. Pada uji hipotesis mayor dianalisis menggunakan regresi dan didapatkan nilai sig 0,000 < 0,05. Berdasarkan hasil pada model summary menunjukkan skor R 0,861, jika skor R mendekati 1 dikatakan hubungan antar variabel tergolong kuat. Dapat disimpulkan, hipotesis mayor terbukti bahwa ada hubungan kuat antara literasi digital, regulasi diri dengan produktivitas dosen. Pada uji hipotesis minor dianalisis menggunakan korelasi product moment didapatkan nilai sig 0,000 < 0,05 dengan koefisien korelasi sebesar 0,487 artinya ada hubungan antara literasi digital dengan produktivitas dosen. Selanjutnya, didapatkan 0,000 < 0,05 dengan koefisien korelasi 0,858 artinya ada hubungan positif antara regulasi diri dengan produktivitas dosen. Terdapat sumbangan efektif literasi digital dan regulasi diri terhadap produktivitas dosen sebesar 74,2%. Dengan demikian, dosen yang memiliki literasi digital dan regulasi diri dalam situasi normal baru mampu menunjukkan produktivitas kerja, sehingga pemahaman terhadap teknologi dan pengelolaan diri secara efektif berperan penting terhadap pencapaian produktivitas kerja dalam situasi normal baru. Kata Kunci: Literasi digital, regulasi diri, produktivitas dosen, situasi normal baru.
Article
Full-text available
Organizations nowadays are under immense external pressure due to advancements in information technology, making it precarious. It also inserts extra pressure to keep the employees motivated and productive. Therefore, while information technology benefits the organization, it also challenges the organization and employees more. In order to meet these challenges, many organizations have begun to flatten their organizational structures and decentralized their management approaches. This study collected 336 valid questionnaires from 20 service companies. The reliability and validity of the questionnaire were tested. In addition, the exploratory factor analysis and confirmatory factor analysis. Relevant analysis and empirical analysis were also carried out using hierarchical regression. The study finds that (1) Goal-setting participation positively affects employees’ proactive behavior. (2) Perceived insider status plays a mediating role between goal-setting participation and employee proactive behavior. (3) The power distance positively modifies the goal-setting participation in the relationship of employee’s perceived insider status. (4) Power distance positively moderates perceived insider status in the relationship of goal-setting participation on employee proactive behavior through perceived insider status. This research applies goal-setting theory and social cognition theory to build a theoretical framework for the influence mechanism of goal-setting participation on employee’s proactive behavior. Expands the application scope of fundamental theoretical research and improve understanding of the relationship between goal-setting participation and employee’s proactive behavior. The research conclusions help organizations understand the formation mechanism of employees’ proactive behaviors, strengthen the focus on goal-setting participation, and optimize the relationship between leaders and employees.
Article
Full-text available
Reports an error in the the article "Effects of Assigned and Participative Goal Setting on Performance and Job Satisfaction" by Gary P. Latham and Gary A. Yukl (Journal of Applied Psychology. Vol 61(2) Apr 1976, 166-171). The last paragraph in the Results section subtitled "Manipulation Check and Reliability of Measures" on page 168 was a repetition of the previous paragraph. It should be changed to read as follows: The internal consistency of the satisfaction measure was .83, The stability of the satisfaction scale, as estimated by the correlation between the before and after measures of satisfaction, was .70 (p < .01). Evaluated the job performance of 41 female typists under participative or assigned goal setting conditions over a 10-wk period. Significant productivity improvement occurred in both conditions during the 2nd 5 wks. There was no significant difference between conditions with respect to goal difficulty or frequency of goal attainment. Job satisfaction declined slightly in both conditions. Individual trait measures such as need for independence did not moderate the effects of either type of goal setting.
Article
Full-text available
The authors summarize 35 years of empirical research on goal-setting theory. They describe the core findings of the theory, the mechanisms by which goals operate, moderators of goal effects, the relation of goals and satisfaction, and the role of goals as mediators of incentives. The external validity and practical significance of goal-setting theory are explained, and new directions in goal-setting research are discussed. The relationships of goal setting to other theories are described as are the theory’s limitations.
Article
Full-text available
: A review of both laboratory and field studies on the effect of setting goals when learning or performing a task found that specific, challenging goals led more often to higher performance than easy goals, 'do your best' goals or no goals. This is one of the most robust and replicable findings in the psychological literature, with 90% of the studies showing positive or partially positive results. The main mechanisms by which goals affect performance are by directing attention, mobilizing effort, increasing persistence, and motivating strategy development. Goal setting is most likely to improve task performance when the goals are specific and sufficiently challenging, when the subjects have sufficient ability (and ability differences are controlled), when feedback is provided to show progress in relation to the goal, when rewards such as money are given for goal attainment, when the exerimenter manager is supportive, and when the assigned goals are actually accepted by the individual. No reliable individual differences have emerged in goal setting studies, probably because goals were typically assigned rather than self-set. Need for achievement and self esteem may be the most promising individual difference variables. (Author)
Article
This paper integrates elements from the theory of agency, the theory of property rights and the theory of finance to develop a theory of the ownership structure of the firm. We define the concept of agency costs, show its relationship to the 'separation and control' issue, investigate the nature of the agency costs generated by the existence of debt and outside equity, demonstrate who bears the costs and why, and investigate the Pareto optimality of their existence. We also provide a new definition of the firm, and show how our analysis of the factors influencing the creation and issuance of debt and equity claims is a special case of the supply side of the completeness of markets problem.
Article
Two experiments were designed to test the effects of different time limits on time taken to complete a task (Parkinson's law) and to determine whether goal-setting mediated the effects of time limits on performance rate. It was hypothesized that the different time limits would produce differences in performance rate only if and to the degree that different goals were set by Ss in the different conditions. Two groups were run in each experiment. On the first three trials, the Excess-time Ss were given twice as much time as was needed to complete an addition task, and the Minimum-time Ss were given just enough time to finish it. On the fourth trial, the Ss in both groups were allowed to work at their own pace, and on the fifth trial the Ss in both groups were asked to work as fast as their estimated “fastest possible” time. Both groups in both experiments performed in accordance with Parkinson's Law on trials 1 to 3 (the Excess Ss taking longer to complete the task), and the hypothesis that goal setting mediated the Parkinson effect was supported. It was also found, however, that the same goal could mean different things in different contexts. For instance, the Ss in the Excess group who were trying to work “quickly but not as fast as possible” on trials 1 to 3 worked at a significantly slower pace on these trials than the Minimum Ss who said they were working for the same goal during this period. Although the Parkinson effect did not extend to trials 4 and 5, goals were related to performance within groups on these trials.
Article
This paper summarizes and integrates research concerned with a long-neglected topic in psychology: the relationship between conscious goals and intentions and task performance. The basic promise of this research is that an individual's conscious ideas regulate his actions. Studies are cited demonstrating that: (1) hard goals produce a higher level of performance (output) than easy goals; (2) specific hard goals produce a higher level of output than a goal of “do your best”; and (3) behavioral intentions regulate choice behavior. The theory also views goals and intentions as mediators of the effects of incentives on task performance. Evidence is presented supporting the view that monetary incentives, time limits, and knowledge of results do not affect performance level independently of the individual's goals and intentions. A theoretical analysis supports the same view with respect to three other incentives: participation, competition, and praise and reproof. Finally, behavioral intentions were found to mediate the effects of money and “verbal reinforcement” on choice behavior. It is concluded that any adequate theory of task motivation must take account of the individual's conscious goals and intentions. The applied implications of the theory are discussed.