Content uploaded by James Edward Carr
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by James Edward Carr on May 22, 2015
Content may be subject to copyright.
The Effects of Multiple Exemplar Instruction
on the Relation Between Listener
and Intraverbal Categorization Repertoires
Sarah A. Lechago &James E. Carr &
April N. Kisamore &Laura L. Grow
Published online: 11 March 2015
#Association for Behavior Analysis International 2015
Abstract We evaluated the effects of multiple exemplar instruction (MEI) on the
relation between listener and intraverbal categorization repertoires of six typically
Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:76–95
DOI 10.1007/s40616-015-0027-1
This article is based on a dissertation submitted by the first author, under the supervision of the second author,
in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the PhD degree in psychology at Western Michigan University.
We thank Jesse Slappey, Chiara Cunningham, Jennifer Davis, Tania Gonzales, Rachel Wagner, Dylan
Churchwell, Kerry Conde, Laura Reisdorf, Nicole Shriver, Jodie Newsome, Jacquelin Jackson, Sean
Reynolds, James Mellor, Jackie Hoag, Abby Mercure, Courtney Fox, Rob Long, Taylor Butts, Adam
Freeman, Chris Zielinski, Megan Baumgartner, and Cindy Han for their assistance with data collection and
conducting sessions. Finally, we thank Pat Oldham, Lori Sebright, Pamela Kelly, and Kathy Graff for their
invaluable on-site support.
S. A. Lechago (*)
Department of Psychology, University of Houston—Clear Lake, 2700 Bay Area Boulevard, Suite 2617,
Houston, TX 77058-1098, USA
e-mail: lechago@uhcl.edu
J. E. Carr
Auburn University, Auburn, AL, USA
A. N. Kisamore
Western New England College, Springfield, MA, USA
L. L. Grow
St. Cloud State University, St. Cloud, MN, USA
Present Address:
J. E. Carr
Behavior Analyst Certification Board, 8051 Shaffer Parkway, Littleton, CO 80127, USA
Present Address:
A. N. Kisamore
Caldwell University, 120 Bloomfield Avenue, Caldwell, NJ 07006, USA
Present Address:
L. L. Grow
University of British Columbia, Vancouver campus, 2125 Main Mall, Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4, Canada
developing preschool-age children using a nonconcurrent multiple-probe design across
participants. After failing to emit intraverbal categorization responses following listener
categorization training, participants were exposed to MEI in the form of alternating
response forms (listener and intraverbal) during categorization training with novel
stimulus sets. For two participants for whom there was some evidence of emergent
intraverbal responding, responding was variable. For the remaining four participants,
32 to 99 MEI trial blocks produced minimal improvement in responding or no
emergent responding at all. The results are discussed in terms of Skinner’sanalysis
of verbal behavior and naming theory.
Keywords Categorization .Intraverbal behavior.Listener behavior.Multiple exemplar
instruction .Naming
Curricular targets designed to teach verbal behavior are embedded within early and
intensive behavioral intervention (EIBI) programs for childhood autism and constitute a
large and critically important part of an individual’s curriculum. Some EIBI programs
and some clinicians promote and utilize the Bverbal behavior approach^to teach
language or verbal behavior (Barbera and Rasmussen 2007; Sundberg and Partington
1998). According to the verbal behavior approach, it is not assumed that once an
individual learns to emit a word under the influence of one set of controlling variables,
she will automatically be able to emit the same word under a different set of controlling
variables. This analysis has implications for how language is taught. For example, if an
individual is taught to ask for water when she is water deprived (a mand), different
manipulations of antecedents and consequences are required to ensure she will be able
to emit the same response (water) when shown a picture of a glass of water and asked,
BWhat is this?^(a tact). There is support in the research literature that lends credence to
the assertion that the verbal operants are functionally independent from one another and
from listener behavior (Gilic and Greer 2011;LamarreandHolland1985;Migueletal.
2005; Petursdottir et al. 2008).
Despite existing evidence demonstrating functional independence across verbal
operants, it appears that typically developing individuals eventually learn to respond
across verbal operants without explicit training (Skinner 1957; Taylor and Harris 1995).
That is, they are able to emit a response under conditions in which they have not
received any direct training. However, such emergent responding between verbal
operants and between listener and speaker repertoires is notoriously deficient in many
individuals with language delays (Guess and Baer 1973;Kelleyetal.2007;Nuzzolo-
Gomez and Greer 2004). As such, it behooves researchers to develop procedures that
generate emergent responding across verbal operants. The result of such research might
enhance early intervention curricula such that verbal behavior is taught more compre-
hensively and efficiently. Multiple exemplar instruction (MEI) represents one way to
achieve emergent responding between verbal operants and between listener and speaker
repertoires.
One form of MEI consists of alternating instruction between two or more response
functions in a subset of exemplars, which results in emergent responding in initially
functionally independent verbal operants or response forms (Fiorile and Greer 2007;
Greer et al. 2005a,b; Nuzzolo-Gomez and Greer 2004). Several studies have examined
Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:76–95 77
the effects of this specific form of MEI on emergence across verbal operants or
response forms. Nuzzolo-Gomez and Greer (2004) tested the effects of MEI on the
emergence of untrained mands or tacts of adjective-object pairs in four children with
autism. The experimenters presented three sets of adjective-object pairs (e.g., small cup,
third box, middle bowl). The participants were taught to either tact or mand for the first
or second set of adjective-object pairs. Baseline probes confirmed the absence of the
untaught verbal relation (i.e., mands if a tact was taught, and vice versa). MEI was then
conducted with a new set of adjective-object pairs. This condition consisted of alter-
nating mand and tact training trials with each adjective-object pair. After MEI, probes
were once again conducted with the untrained operant from the original set and a novel
set. The authors demonstrated that MEI resulted in high levels of correct responding on
the untrained operant from the original and novel sets.
Greer et al. (2005a,b) evaluated the effect of MEI on generating joint control
between novel dictation and intraverbal responding with children with language delays.
Usingapre-andpost-testsimilartotheoneemployedbyNuzzolo-Gomez
and Greer (2004), the authors observed low levels of the untrained response,
vocal or written spelling, in baseline. A subsequent MEI condition resulted in
high levels of emergent responding across participants. Greer et al. (2005a,b)
replicated these findings with children with language delays targeting vocal and
written spelling. It should be noted that the responses targeted in studies that
have investigated the effects of the aforementioned iteration of MEI were
relatively straightforward (e.g., Greer et al. 2005a,b). In other words, emergent
responding was examined across operants in which the target response form
was topographically identical (e.g., tacts and mands), across listener responses
in the form of pointing and one-word tacts, or across modalities of the same
responses (writing and orally spelling a word). Another point to note is that in
both the Nuzzolo-Gomez and Greer (2004)andGreeretal.(2005a,b)studies,
participants were probed only once during baseline before the MEI procedure
was introduced, making it challenging to discount maturation as a potential
confounding variable. Also, conducting multiple probes during the baseline
condition would have helped to control for practice effects. Nevertheless, this
line of research provides evidence that MEI may produce functional emergent
responding between verbal operants or different response forms (Fiorile and
Greer 2007; Greer et al. 2005a,b; Nuzzolo-Gomez and Greer 2004).
Categorization skills, which involve more complex forms of responding, are ubiq-
uitous in our daily functioning. They constitute a critical part of an individual’s
academic, social, and professional success. For some learners, categorization skills
and emergent responding between categorization skills eventually come with apparent
relative ease, and minimal or no instruction.
However, for other learners, especially those with language delays and other
developmental disabilities, this emergent responding is not easily acquired.
Therefore, it is important to identify those procedures that help establish emergent
responding between categorization skills. The present study contributes to this
endeavor by identifying the limitations of a specific iteration of the MEI procedure in
generating emergent responding between listener and intraverbal categorization
repertoires. This study was an extension of the Petursdottir et al. (2008) study, which
demonstrated that training listener categorization relations did not result in the
78 Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:76–95
emergence of intraverbal categorization relations, and vice versa, in typically develop-
ing preschool-age children. The purpose of the present study was to determine the
effects of MEI on emergent responding between intraverbal and listener categorization
repertoires. More specifically, the present study evaluated the effects of MEI on the
emergence (or lack thereof) of intraverbal categorization following listener categoriza-
tion training. The MEI procedure employed in this study was most similar to the
Nuzzolo-Gomez and Greer (2004) study in which there was one-to-one alternation
between the response types of interest per stimulus, with minor differences in the
alternation format of the response types.
Method
Participants, Setting, and Materials
Six typically developing children participated in the study; Doug (4 years, 7 months
old), Sophie (4 years, 2 months), Alex (4 years, 2 months), Rick (3 years, 10 months),
Mike (4 years), and Meredith (4 years, 2 months). English was the participants’primary
language and none had documented developmental delays.
Sessions were conducted at the participants’preschool in a partitioned corner of the
children’s dining area (Doug, Sophie, Alex, Rick) or in an empty preschool classroom
(Mike, Meredith). Sessions were conducted one to two times per day, five to ten times
per week. One to two graduate students, and one to two undergraduate research
assistants were present during each session.
Sessions were conducted at a child-sized table with three to four child-sized
chairs. Tokens were delivered for correct responding and appropriate sitting. A
small audio recorder was placed on the table within the child’s view. The audio
recorder was turned on during sessions to record participants’vocal behavior.
The participants had the opportunity to select a toy or snack from a large
plastic tub containing small age-appropriate toys and parent-approved snacks at
the end of each session.
Visual stimuli were presented to participants using three-ring binders. The stimuli
were placed on 8.5×11 in sheets that were presented in a horizontal manner to the
participants. Each sheet was placed inside a page protector. The novel stimuli were
outlines of maps of cities or countries and characters from foreign writing systems.
Novel stimuli were those stimuli that the participants were unable to tact or receptively
identify prior to this study. Familiar stimuli were those stimuli that the participants were
able to tact or receptively identify prior to this study. All the stimuli were in black and
white. The names of the stimuli were the corresponding names of the cities/countries or
characters. Some of the names of the stimuli were modified for ease of pronunciation
for the participants (e.g., Poro for Sapporo). The tact training binder contained the
stimuli used to train the participants to label (tact) each of the novel stimuli. One picture
(stimulus) was depicted on each sheet. The listener training,listener categorization
training,andMEI binders contained sheets of three stimuli to which participants would
point upon the experimenter’s request (see Fig. 1for a sample of the visual stimuli
presented during the study). Table 1contains the category names, stimuli names, and
set designation for the novel stimuli.
Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:76–95 79
Data Collection
Dependent Variables
The primary dependent variable was the emission of untrained intraverbal categoriza-
tion responses after initial listener categorization training before and after MEI. For
example, after the participant was trained to point to the Japanese character, BYo to^in
response to the instruction, BWhich one is Japanese?^, a probe for the participant’s
untrained emission of the vocal response BJapanese^in response to the instruction,
durbin (Africa) oria (Africa)
CATEGORY 1 – “JAPAN”
CATEGORY 2 – “AFRICA”
CATEGORY 3 – “EAST”
jeeling (E.India) cal (E. India)
poro (Japan) yoto (Japan)
Fig. 1 Three sample categories and their respective stimuli
80 Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:76–95
BYoto is ___^was conducted. Additionally, data were collected on responses during tact
and listener training of the exemplar stimuli, preexperimental listener and intraverbal
categorization training with familiar stimuli, listener categorization training of novel
stimuli, and listener and intraverbal categorization training of the MEI stimulus sets.
Response Measurement
For all conditions, a response was counted as correct if the participant emitted the target
response (a pointing response for all listener targets and a vocal response for all speaker
targets) within 10 s of the experimenter’s instruction. A response was counted as
incorrect if the participant emitted an incorrect response or did not respond within
10 s of the instruction. Data were analyzed as the total number of correct responses out
of set number of possible responses as follows: (a) 8 responses for tact and listener
training and categorization training with familiar stimuli, (b) 20 responses for listener
categorization training with novel stimuli and intraverbal categorization probes, and (c)
32 responses for MEI.
Interobserver Agreement
A second observer independently collected interobserver agreement (IOA) data across
all experimental conditions. An agreement was scored for each trial in which the
experimenter and the observer both recorded the same correct or incorrect response.
Point-by-point agreement was calculated for each session by dividing the number of
agreements by the sum of agreements and disagreements and multiplying by 100. The
IOA results for each participant are depicted in Table 2. Agreement was assessed for at
least 88 % of sessions and averaged at least 99 % for each participant.
Tab l e 1 Category names, stimuli names, and set designation for novel stimuli
Category Members Set designation
South Cochin, Madras Set 1
Arabic Kaaf, Baa Set 1
Greek Pi, Mega Set 2
Africa Durbin, Oria Set 2
East Jeeling, Cal Set 3
Russian Shah, Yoo Set 3
Hebrew Mem, Shin MEI (A)
Sri Lanka Jaff, Mannar MEI (A)
Armenian Vev, T’oh MEI (A)
Japan Yoto, Poro MEI (A)
Swit Eva, Laus MEI (B)
Rin Bu, Ka MEI (B)
German Lub, Bruck MEI (B)
Nese Yuk, Saan MEI (B)
Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:76–95 81
Procedure
General Teaching Procedures
For the conditions that involved motor responses (listener training, listener categoriza-
tion training of familiar and novel stimuli), pointing to the correct response indepen-
dently within 10 s of the instruction (e.g., BWhich one is East?^), produced enthusiastic
praise from the experimenter (e.g., BWow! That’s right!^). For all conditions that
involved speaker behavior (tact training, intraverbal categorization training of familiar
and novel stimuli), emission of the correct response independently within 10 s of the
instruction (e.g., BWhat is it?^for tact training, BYoto is ___^for intraverbal categori-
zation training), produced enthusiastic praise from the experimenter. Tokens were
provided on an intermittent schedule of VR 3 contingent on appropriate attending,
imitation of the experimenter’s model prompts, and independent responses. If the
participant made an incorrect response during conditions that involved motor responses,
the experimenter provided gestural and verbal prompts (e.g., BIt’s this one. Can you
pointtoit?^). If the participant emitted the incorrect response during tact and intraverbal
categorization conditions, the experimenter provided an echoic prompt (e.g., BEast. Can
you say it?^). Imitation of the experimenter’s model produced praise (e.g., BYou ’re
right!^) and nonresponses or other responses resulted in additional gestural and verbal
prompts. After the participant was able to independently emit the correct responses with
reinforcement, the experimenter presented the stimuli again under extinction. This way,
if correct responses during subsequent probes for the primary dependent variable
(intraverbal categorization responding) did not occur or occurred at low rates, it could
be concluded with greater confidence that these outcomes were due to a failure to
acquire the response and not due to the transition from reinforcement to extinction.
Preliminary Procedures
Upon giving permission for their child to participate, parents were provided a
survey to nominate their child’s favorite foods and toys. The results of the survey
Tab l e 2 Assessment results for interobserver agreement and procedural fidelity
Participant Interobserver agreement (IOA) Procedural fidelity Procedural fidelity IOA
Doug 95 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 75–100)
97 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 75–100)
63 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 62–100)
Sophie 95 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 87–100)
95 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 88–100)
52 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 88–100)
Alex 98 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 75–100)
96 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 91–100)
42 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 75–100)
Rick 98 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 87–100)
99 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 98–100)
63 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 95–100)
Mike 92 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 88–100)
92 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 75–100)
23 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 75–100)
Meredith 88 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 88–100)
88 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 87–100)
17 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 88–100)
82 Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:76–95
were used to select toys and snacks that were made available to participants after
each session.
Echoic Assessment The experimenter conducted an informal echoic assessment with
each participant to ensure that the participants were able to respond to the echoic
prompts that would be used in subsequent conditions.
Tact Tr a i n i n g During the tact training condition, the experimenter taught the partici-
pants to vocally tact the stimuli in set 1 (eight stimuli), set 2 (eight stimuli), and MEI A
(eight stimuli) for a total of 24 stimuli. Tacts were targeted one set at a time and one trial
block included all eight stimuli. Sets 1 and 2 included two categories with two stimuli
that corresponded to each category for a total of four target stimuli. Participants were
taught to tact stimuli from similar but untrained categories (eight additional stimuli
between sets 1 and 2 divided into four stimuli per set) that served as negative
comparisons. Additional sets of stimuli were trained if the participants required a
second implementation of MEI or if replication sets were required. During each trial,
the experimenter presented the stimulus on the table in front of the participant and
provided the instruction, BWhat is it?^Tact maintenance trials were typically conducted
every other experimental session in alternation with listener maintenance trials.
Maintenance trials were conducted to ensure that participants were able to tact and
point to members of the targeted categories throughout the study. After the participants
responded correctly with reinforcement at 100 %, trials were conducted under extinc-
tion. The mastery criterion was 100 % responding for all eight stimuli under extinction.
Listener Training The experimenter taught the participants to point to the stimuli in sets
1, 2, and MEI A. Additional sets of stimuli were trained as needed. The experimenter
presented the picture of the target stimulus along with two comparison stimuli and
instructed the participant to indicate the target stimulus (e.g., BShow me Yoto.^). The
mastery criterion was 100 % correct responding for all eight stimuli under extinction.
Listener and Intraverbal Categorization Training Trials with Familiar Stimuli These
trials were conducted with familiar stimuli to ensure instructional control of the trial
format over responding when testing with novel stimuli. Categorization testing with
familiar stimuli was conducted at the beginning of nearly every session in the entire
study except during MEI. Two stimuli from four categories comprised the eight stimuli
that were targeted. Categories that were targeted were animals, food, clothes, and toys.
Listener categorization training with familiar stimuli involved teaching the participants
to point to the stimulus that corresponded to the category indicated in the experi-
menter’s instruction (e.g., point to the cat in response to the instruction BWhich one is
an animal?^). After the participants responded correctly with reinforcement at 100 %,
trails were conducted under extinction. The mastery criterion was 100 % for all eight
stimuli under extinction. Intraverbal categorization training with familiar stimuli in-
volved teaching the participants to emit the name of the category that corresponded to
the stimulus indicated in the experimenter’s instruction (e.g., Saying BAn animal^in
response to the instruction, BA cat is ___.^). After the participants responded correctly
with reinforcement at 100 %, trials were conducted under extinction. The mastery
criterion was 100 % independent responding for all eight stimuli under extinction, for
Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:76–95 83
both listener categorization training and intraverbal categorization training with familiar
stimuli.
Listener Categorization Training with Novel Stimuli This training was conducted with
one set of experimental stimuli (four stimuli were targeted and four stimuli served as
negative comparisons). Training was identical to the procedure described above except
that novel stimuli instead of familiar stimuli were used. After the participants responded
correctly with reinforcement at 100 %, trials were conducted under extinction. The
mastery criterion for listener categorization training sessions was 100 % independent
responding for all 20 stimuli under extinction.
Experimental Evaluation
A nonconcurrent multiple-probe design across participants was used to evaluate the
effects of MEI on the relation between intraverbal and listener categorization reper-
toires. The order of experimental conditions is depicted in Fig. 2.
Baseline Probes After listener categorization training was conducted with one set,
intraverbal categorization baseline probes were conducted. Listener categorization
training and intraverbal categorization probes were conducted separately. As an exam-
ple, after training the participant to point to outlines of Durbin and Oria in response to
the instruction, BWhich one is Africa?^during listener categorization training, a probe
during the intraverbal categorization baseline was conducted to determine if the
participant emitted the response BAfrica^in response to the instructions, BDurbin
is…^and BOria is….^The order of stimulus presentation was randomly rotated across
20-trial blocks during the intraverbal categorization probes to ensure that responding
occurred under appropriate stimulus control (i.e., to ensure responding occurred under
the influence of the verbal instruction). Correct and incorrect responses did not result in
any differential consequences other than the presentation of a new trial. Baseline was
conducted until each stimulus within each of the categories (two categories, four
stimuli) was presented five times for a total of 20 trials.
MEI with set MEI A After the baseline condition, MEI was conducted with a second set
of stimuli (MEI A). Four categories (eight stimuli, two stimuli per category) were
targeted. Blocks of four trials were presented, each of which contained two trials of
intraverbal categorization training and two trials of listener categorization training
alternating between trial types. Only one stimulus was targeted per trial block. Data
were graphed and analyzed as the total number of correct responses out of a possible 16
correct per categorization type (listener and intraverbal) for a 32-trial block. Mastery
criterion was 100 % independent responding for one 32-trial block under extinction.
Depending on the participant, sessions lasted approximately 15–30 min.
Post-MEI probes After MEI was conducted with set MEI A, post-MEI test probes for
intraverbal categorization responses were conducted again with sets 1 or 2. Before
intraverbal categorization probes were conducted, probes for listener categorization
responses were conducted to ensure the participants were still responding at mastery
84 Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:76–95
Tact Training:
Novel Exemplars
Listener Training:
Novel Exemplars
Intraverbal/Listener
Categorization
Training:
Familiar Exemplars
Listener Cat.Training:
Novel Exemplars
(First Set)
MAINTENANCE
Intraverbal
Categorization Pre-
MEI Probe:
Novel Exemplars
(First Set)
Intraverbal
Categorization Probe:
Novel Exemplars
(First Set)
Listener
Categorization Probe:
Intraverbal
Categorization
Mastered:
(First Set)
Intraverbal
Categorization NOT
Mastered:
(First Set)
Listener
Categorization
Training:
(Second Set)
Intraverbal
Categorization Probe:
(Second Set)
MEI (B)
Listener
Categorization Probe:
(First Set)
Intraverbal
Categorization Probe:
(First Set)
Intraverbal Cat.Probe:
Novel Exemplars
(First Set)
MEI (A)
Novel Exemplars
(First Set)
Fig. 2 The sequence of training and testing conditions
Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:76–95 85
criterion. If not, then listener categorization training was conducted until responding to
mastery criterion was reachieved. After this, probes for intraverbal categorization
responses were conducted. Probes were identical to those described under the baseline
condition. If responding for intraverbal categorization responses was at mastery crite-
rion (80 % or higher), then a new set was introduced and tested (e.g., if set 1 was
initially used, then listener categorization training and intraverbal categorization probes
were conducted with set 2). If responding met the mastery criterion during intraverbal
categorization probes for set 1, listener categorization training was conducted with a
new set (e.g., set 2). Prior to listener categorization training with set 3 for Sophie, an
intraverbal categorization probe was conducted with set 3. This step was conducted to
demonstrate that the participant was unable to emit intraverbal categorization responses
prior to listener categorization training. This step was not conducted with Doug due to
experimenter error. Given the esoteric nature of the targeted stimuli, however, it is
unlikely that Doug would be able to emit intraverbal categorization responses for
sets 2 and 3 in the absence of listener categorization training. Listener categori-
zation training then was conducted with set 2. Immediately after responding met
the mastery criterion during listener categorization training, probes for intraverbal
categorization responses were conducted for sets 2 or 3. Each stimulus in the set
(two categories, four stimuli total) was presented five times each for a total of 20
presentations. These data were analyzed and reported as the total number of
correct responses out of a possible 20 responses. If the probes for intraverbal
categorization responding were at 80 % or higher for 20 total presentations, the
participant was dismissed from the study.
MEI with set MEI B If responding during intraverbal categorization probes after MEI
for sets 1 or 2 was not at mastery criterion, additional MEI was conducted with a new
set (MEI B) which contained four categories. This initially involved tact and listener
training for all eight stimuli. After MEI was conducted to mastery criterion for set MEI
B, probes were conducted again with sets 1or 2. Before the intraverbal categorization
probes were conducted, probes for listener categorization responding were conducted
to ensure the participants were still responding at mastery level. As described above, if
intraverbal categorization responding was at mastery criterion (80 % or higher), then a
new set (e.g., set 2 or 3) was introduced at this time in an attempt to replicate the effects
of MEI. If mastery-level responding was not achieved after a second implementation of
MEI with set MEI B, then the participant was dismissed from the study.
Procedural Fidelity
A secondary observer scored a trial correct if the experimenter delivered the instruction,
prompts, and consequences appropriate to the phase and the child’s response. A
procedural integrity score was then computed for each session as the percentage of
correctly implemented trials. Point-by-point IOA was assessed on all procedural fidelity
measures. Procedural fidelity scores and their corresponding IOA data are depicted in
Tab le 2. Procedural fidelity was assessed for at least 88 % of sessions and averaged at
least 99 % for each participant. Interobserver agreement on procedural fidelity data was
assessed for at least 17 % of sessions and averaged at least 99 % for each participant.
86 Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:76–95
Results
Tact and Listener Training
The results of the tact and listener training conditions are depicted in Table 3. The table
includes the total number of trial blocks required to acquire all of the tact and listener
stimulus sets, the number of sets taught, the total number of stimuli, and the mean
number of trial blocks required to acquire each set. Participants acquired the tact sets in
92 to 147 trial blocks, and the listener sets in 4 to 8 trial blocks.
Listener and Intraverbal Categorization Training (Familiar Stimuli)
The results for the listener and intraverbal categorization training condition with
familiar stimuli for each participant are depicted in Table 2. The table includes the
total number of trial blocks required to acquire listener and intraverbal categorization
with familiar stimuli, whether the participant made errors with each categorization type
and any interesting collateral verbal behavior. Participants acquired the target listener
and intraverbal categorization relations in two to five trial blocks.
Tab l e 3 Results for tact training, listener training, and listener/intraverbal categorization training (familiar
stimuli)
Participant Tact training Listener training Listener/intraverbal categorization
training
Doug 95 trial blocks (M=23)
4 sets
32 stimuli
8 trial blocks (M=2)
4 sets
32 stimuli
2 trial blocks
No errors with either categorization type
Emitted an echoic and a tact during
a listener categorization trial
a
Sophie 122 trial blocks (M=26)
5 sets
40 stimuli
5 trial blocks (M=1)
5 sets
40 stimuli
3 trial blocks
Errors with intraverbal categorization
Alex 139 trial blocks (M=35)
4 sets
32 stimuli
7 trial blocks (M=2)
4 sets
32 stimuli
2 trial blocks
Errors with intraverbal categorization
Rick 92 trial blocks (M=23)
4 sets
32 stimuli
4 trial blocks (M=1)
4 sets
32 stimuli
4 trial blocks
Errors with intraverbal and listener
categorization
Mike 147 trial blocks (M=29)
5 sets
40 stimuli
5 trial blocks (M=1)
5 sets
40 stimuli
5 trial blocks
Errors with intraverbal categorization
Meredith 106 trial blocks (M=21)
5 sets
40 stimuli
Provided the name of the correct
category, instead of the name
of the stimulus itself during
a maintenance trial
a
7 trial blocks (M=1)
5 sets
40 stimuli
4 trial blocks
Errors with intraverbal and listener
categorization
a
Collateral verbal behavior of interest
Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:76–95 87
Listener Categorization Training (Novel Stimuli)
The results for the listener categorization training condition with novel stimuli for each
participant are depicted in Table 4. The table includes the total number of sets taught
and the number of trials blocks required to master each set. Participants acquired the
target listener categorization relations in four to eight trial blocks per set.
Multiple Exemplar Instruction
Each participant’s MEI data are depicted in Table 4. The table includes the total number
of trial blocks required to acquire each MEI set and collateral verbal behavior of
interest. Participants completed MEI in 14 to 56 trial blocks per set.
Primary Experimental Evaluation (Intraverbal Categorization Probes)
Baseline (Pre-MEI)
Results for all six participants’performance during intraverbal categorization probes
pre- and post-MEI training are depicted in Figs. 3and 4. During baseline, Doug scored
45 % correct for set 1, occasionally responding with targets from both categories. The
remaining five participants scored 0 % during baseline.
Post-MEI probes after MEI A
After ensuring listener categorization responses for set 1 and MEI A responses were
still at mastery, intraverbal categorization probes were conducted. Doug responded with
100 % accuracy under extinction during the first post-MEI probe. In attempts to
replicate these effects, probes were conducted with sets 2 and 3. Doug scored 50 %
Tab l e 4 Results for listener categorization training and multiple exemplar instruction
Participant Listener categorization training (novel stimuli) Multiple exemplar instruction
Doug Set 1=5 trial blocks
Set 2=4 trial blocks
Set 3=4 trial blocks
Set A= 14 trial blocks
Sophie Set 1=5 trial blocks
Set 3=7 trial blocks
Set A= 37 trial blocks
Set B= 33 trial blocks
Alex Set 2=4 trial blocks
Echoed the word BEast^in two trials
a
Set A= 43 trial blocks
Set B= 56 trial blocks
Rick Set 1=4 trial blocks Set A= 40 trial blocks
Set B= 32 trial blocks
b
Mike Set 1= 8 trial blocks Set A =17 trial blocks
Set B= 15 trial blocks
Meredith Set 1= 8 trial blocks Set A =20 trial blocks
Set B= 17 trial blocks
a
Collateral verbal behavior of interest
b
Modified mastery criterion
88 Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:76–95
0
5
10
15
20
1234567
Set 1
Set 1
Set 2
Set 2
Set 3
Set 3
Set 3
Number of Correct Responses
Trial Blocks
Pre- Post-MEI
0
5
10
15
20
123456789
Sophie
Set 1 Set 1 Set 1
Set 1
Set 1
Set 3 Set 3
Set 3 Set 3
0
5
10
15
20
12345
Alex
Set 2 Set 2 Set 2 Set 2
Set 2
0
5
10
15
20
12345
Rick
Set 1 Set 1 Set 1 Set 1 Set 1
MEI (A)
MEI (B) Pre- LC
Training
Post- LC
Training
Doug
Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:76–95 89
in the first set 2 probe. A second probe with set 2 resulted in a score of 90 %. In
response to these mixed results, another intraverbal categorization probe was conducted
with set 3. During the first probe with set 3, Doug scored 50 %, and with a second
probe, Doug scored 70 %. Given these mixed results, a third set 3 probe was conducted
in which Doug responded at 95 % correct.
For Sophie, two probes with set 1 each resulted in a score of 0 % correct. During
both probes, Sophie shrugged during each trial and said BIdon’tknowthese^or BI
0
5
10
15
20
Mike
MEI (A) MEI (B)
Set 1 Set 1 Set 1 Set 1Set 1 Set 1 Set 1
0
5
10
15
20
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
Meredith
Set 1 Set 1 Set 1 Set 1 Set 1 Set 1 Set 1
Pre-MEI
Trial Blocks
Number of Correct Responses
Post-MEI
Fig. 4 Number of correct responses during intraverbal categorization probes before and after multiple
exemplar instruction (MEI) for Mike and Meredith
Fig. 3 Number of correct responses during intraverbal categorization probes before and after multiple
exemplar instruction (MEI) for Doug, Sophie, Alex, and Rick. LC listener categorization
90 Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:76–95
don’tknow.^Alex’s three post-MEI A probes with set 2 each resulted in scores of 0 %.
During the third post-MEI A probe, he repeatedly said, BIdon’tknow^and Bnothing.^
Rick’s two post-MEI A probes with set 1 each resulted in scores of 0 %. During the first
of these probes, Rick emitted the exemplar and category names for stimuli targeted
during tact and MEI A training (e.g., brew, lanka, and yoo) for 8 out of the 20 trials.
Neither of the category names (brew and lanka) was correct for set 1. During the second
post-MEI A probe, he emitted three exemplar names (madras, senza, and chi) one time
each, and then names of random common stimuli not targeted in the study (e.g., apple
head and pancake) for 13 trials. Mike’s two post-MEI A probes with set 1 each resulted
in a score of 0 %. On many occasions, he imitated the stimulus in the experimenter’s
instruction. Meredith’s two post-MEI A probes with set 1 each resulted in a score of
0%.
Post MEI (B)
A second implementation of MEI (MEI B) was conducted with Sophie, Alex, Rick,
Mike, and Meredith. During the first of these probes for set 1, Sophie scored 45 %.
During the second probe with set 1, Sophie scored 95 %. In an effort to replicate the
effects achieved with set 1, intraverbal categorization probes were conducted with set 3.
The first intraverbal categorization probe resulted in a score of 0 %. After listener
categorization training, the first intraverbal categorization probe resulted in a score of
0 %. Two more intraverbal categorization probes with set 3 resulted in a score of 50 %.
Alex scored 20 % on his post-MEI B intraverbal categorization probe with set 2 during
which he emitted category names targeted during MEI A and B. Rick, Mike, and
Meredith scored 0 %, during post-MEI B probes for set 1.
Discussion
The results of the present study extend the literature on MEI by demonstrating that it
was not reliably effective in producing emergent responding between listener and
intraverbal categorization repertoires in six typically developing children between the
ages of 3 and 4 years. It is possible that MEI failed to produce emergent responding
between the targeted verbal operants in the present study because of the more complex
nature of the repertoires involved (i.e., categorization). This study represents a starting
point for MEI procedures and their utility for producing emergent responding for more
complex forms of responding.
Doug may have engaged in covert responding that produced the emergent intraverbal
categorization responding observed during baseline and post-MEI probes. A consider-
ation of the Naming theory is warranted. When an individual has learned to respond to a
given stimulus as both a speaker and a listener, it can be said that he has acquired naming
(Horne and Lowe 1996; Lowe et al. 2002). A critical feature of the naming account is
that many stimuli may come to produce a common speaker and listener response,
implicating the naming relation in the establishment of categorization (Horne et al.
2004;Loweetal.2005;Migueletal.2008). During listener categorization training,
when the experimenter asked Doug, BWhich one is Africa?,^he was taught to point to
the outlines of the cities Durbin and Oria. While pointing to Durbin and Oria, he may
Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:76–95 91
have responded to his own listener behavior (pointing to the picture of Durbin and Oria)
as a speaker and covertly tacted the exemplar and echoed the corresponding category
(BDurbin, Africa^and BOria, Africa^). During intraverbal categorization probes, the
experimenter asked Doug BDurbin is ___?,^during which time he may have engaged in
covert responding BDurbin, Africa,^established during listener categorization training,
and then emitted the response BAfrica.^Teaching Doug to respond to both Durbin and
Oria as a listener to a common name (Africa) may have then resulted in Doug
responding to both Durbin and Oria as a speaker using a common name (Africa). The
auditory stimulus BDurbin,^served as a stimulus common to both instructional contexts,
possibly resulting in stimulus control over responding in both instructional contexts
(listener and intraverbal categorization) and influencing the establishment of emergent
responding. While consideration of the Naming theory offers a cohesive and parsimo-
nious explanation for Doug’s performance, this account remains speculative as there
was no evidence of covert responding and functional control was not demonstrated over
the post-MEI response increases.
Sophie required exposure to two sets of MEI, which yielded comparatively little
emergent responding. The reverse categorization relation was originally examined with
Sophie. That is, the intraverbal categorization repertoire was trained and listener
categorization responses were probed. These data are not graphed. When the experi-
menter probed listener categorization responses during baseline, she demonstrated
100 % emergent responding providing evidence of emergent responding between the
two categorization types in one direction (from intraverbal to listener), indicating an
emerging Naming relation for Sophie. For Sophie, perhaps hearing both the exemplar
name and the category name (e.g., BDurbin is Africa^) exerted greater control over
responding during listener categorization probe trials than was the case with the reverse
relation. During listener categorization training, the experimenter provided only the
auditory stimulus of the category name (e.g., BAfrica^). Maybe learners with a more
extensive history with respect to categorization, like Doug, are more likely to tact,
overtly or covertly, the exemplar name (e.g., BDurbin^) during listener categorization
training trials than those learners who have less experience with categorization. As
such, the learner produces an additional relevant auditory stimulus during listener
categorization (BDurbin^) that will be present during intraverbal categorization probes
(BDurbin is ____^), which would subsequently increase the likelihood of emergent
responding between the two categorization types.
Rick and Alex required a large number of training trials to learn two sets of MEI.
Despite training two sets of MEI, there was very little to no emergent responding,
suggesting a weak or nonexistent naming repertoire. Rick was nearly 1 year younger
than Doug and the only participant under the age of 4 years at the start of the study. This
may be important when considering the types of skills typically targeted with children
between the ages of 3.5 and 4.5 years in daycare and preschool, which commonly
include tacts, listener responses (matching and pointing), and categorization. Three to
9 months of experience with these skills can make a difference with respect to
developing the verbal behavior necessary to establish a naming repertoire and produce
emergent responding.
Mike and Meredith required training with two sets of MEI, which ultimately did not
produce emergent responding between listener and intraverbal categorization reper-
toires. Mike and Meredith acquired mastery of MEI in few trials and their performances
92 Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:76–95
with this task resembled that of Doug’s. However, it may have been the case that not
only could Doug produce the relevant verbal behavior (e.g., tact the name of the
stimulus and echo the name of the category during listener categorization training
trials) but perhaps he also used a rehearsal strategy such that he was able to later emit
the responses during intraverbal probe trials. It may have been the case that Mike and
Meredith were not employing a rehearsal strategy, or at least not doing so consistently,
and this contributed to the failure to produce emergent intraverbal categorization
responding. Three to 4-year-old children typically do not use rehearsal strategies and
often just keep track of physical stimuli (Novak and Pelaez 2004). At around 5 years of
age, children start to use rehearsal as a strategy (Novak and Pelaez 2004). Mike and
Meredith were close to 4 years, and Doug was 4 years and 7 months at the start of the
study. This age difference with Doug may be significant in that Doug may have already
begun to use rehearsal strategies, while Mike and Meredith had not.
A few limitations of the current study are worth acknowledging. One limitation may
have been the probe trial format failing to effectively evoke desired responding,
especially as it pertained to Sophie’s and Mike’s performances. Just as members of a
response class can produce the same consequence, so too can the topographically
varied members of a functional stimulus class influence the same response (McIlvane
and Dube 2003). Researchers endeavor to use stimuli that they assume will control the
responding of the participant. Sometimes there are features of stimuli that the researcher
may not detect or consider that control the responding of the participant. One of these
features may include trial format or presentation. Although the format of the listener
and intraverbal categorization trials during training with familiar stimuli was the same
as during listener training and intraverbal probes with novel stimuli, the trials were
presented in an alternating format, as was the case during MEI. However, during
listener categorization training with novel stimuli and during intraverbal categorization
probes with novel stimuli, these trials were presented all at once and not in an
alternating format. It may have been the case that these presentation differences
ultimately negatively impacted their performances. Future studies may, for example,
present the listener categorization trials and the intraverbal categorization trials with
familiar stimuli separately, mimicking the training and probe trial formats, instead of
presenting them in an alternating trial format as was done in the current study, which
mimicked the MEI trial format.
A second possible limitation may have been the one-to-one alternating trials format
of the MEI training procedure. Due to the fact that each stimulus was targeted in four-
trial blocks with alternation between response type, and hence close temporal proximity
of trials of each response type, it could have been the case that intraverbal categoriza-
tion responding came under echoic control of the experimenter emitting the stimulus
name, and not under control of the intraverbal frame. However, due to the stringent
response criteria (responding within 10 s of the experimenter’s instruction), and rapid
alternation between trial blocks, this is highly unlikely, and it is more likely that
responding during intraverbal categorization trials was under discriminative control
of the intraverbal frame.
A third limitation may have been that the mastery criterion for MEI was especially
stringent (100 % correct trials under extinction) and the number of targets included in
MEI (32 targets) may have been too great for this young population. As a result, it took
a long time for participants to reach the mastery criteria, which may have affected
Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:76–95 93
motivation during experimental trials. A modification of the mastery criterion for MEI
was even required for one participant. It may have been the case that waning motivation
over time affected attending responses which in turn negatively impacted intraverbal
categorization responding. Future research studies in this area may consider using half
the number of targets during MEI training (e.g., 16 trials, two categories) and reducing
the MEI mastery criterion to 80 or 90 %.
Future research might also examine the effects of directly training exemplar and
category tacts during MEI to generate emergent responding between listener and
intraverbal categorization repertoires. During listener categorization training trials, it
may be more effective to train the pointing response, and the autoclitic frame BStimulus
is Category.^For example, the experimenter would provide the discriminative stimu-
lus, BWhich one is Africa?^and then train the participant to point to BDurbin,^then
vocalize the statement BDurbin is Africa.^Although previous research has indicated
that echoic, multiple-tact, and receptive discrimination training does not significantly
influence the emission of intraverbal behavior (Miguel et al. 2005; Petursdottir et al.
2014), perhaps embedding this training as an autoclitic frame in an alternating format
with intraverbal categorization trials in the context of MEI may teach the child to emit
relevant responses that would generate stimulus control over categorization responding
and result in emergent intraverbal categorization.
Finally, future research may also want to examine the effects of this study’s
iteration of MEI and its effectiveness in producing emergent listener categori-
zation responses after training intraverbal categorization responses. It may be
the case that this study’s iteration of MEI is effective contingent on a given
training sequence (expressive before receptive instead of receptive before ex-
pressive). As mentioned previously, Sophie was able to emit all listener cate-
gorization responses after initial intraverbal categorization training. For individ-
uals who do not demonstrate emergence, it may be the case that this study’s
MEI procedure is effective in producing emergent responding if the expressive
(intraverbal) categorization response were trained first. The outcomes of a study
like this may also make some helpful contributions to the literature on expres-
sive and listener responses and training sequences (Petursdottir and Carr 2011).
Previous studies found MEI to be effective when the trained responses was topo-
graphically identical to the probed responses (Nuzzolo-Gomez and Greer 2004)or
when acquisition of the necessary behavior to produce emergent responding was more
straightforward, as is the case between pointing and matching responses, and tact
responses (Greer et al. 2005a,b). However, generating emergent responding between
categorization repertoires may be more complex and may require the previously
described modifications to the MEI procedure to successfully generate emergent
responding between listener and intraverbal categorization.
References
Barbera, M. L., & Rasmussen, T. (2007). The verbal behavior approach: How to teach children with autism
and related disorders. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Fiorile, C. A., & Greer, R. D. (2007). The induction of naming in children with no prior tact responses as a
function of multiple exemplar histories of instruction. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 23,71–87.
94 Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:76–95
Gilic, L., & Greer, R. D. (2011). Establishing naming in typically developing two-year-old children as a
function of multiple exemplar speaker and listener experiences. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 27,157–
177.
Greer, R. D., Stolfi, L., Chavez-Brown, M., & Rivera-Valdes, C. (2005a). The emergence of the listener to
speaker component of naming in children as a function of multiple exemplar instruction. The Analysis of
Verbal Behavior, 21,123–134.
Greer, R. D., Yaun, L., & Gautreaux, G. (2005b). Novel dictation and intraverbal responses as a function of a
multiple exemplar instructional history. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 21,99–116 .
Guess, D., & Baer, D. M. (1973). An analysis of individual differences in generalization between receptive
and productive language in retarded children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 6,311–329.
Horne, P. J., & Lowe, C. F. (1996). On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 185–241.
Horne, P. J., Lowe, C. F., & Randle, V. R. L. (2004). Naming and categorization in young children: II. Listener
behavior training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 81, 267–288.
Kelley, M. E., Shillingsburg, M. A., Castro, M. J., Addison, L. R., & LaRue, R. H., Jr. (2007). Further
evaluation of emerging speech in children with developmental disabilities: training verbal behavior.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40,431–445.
Lamarre, J., & Holland, J. G. (1985). The functional independence of mands and tacts. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43,5–19.
Lowe, C. F., Horne, P. J., Harris, F. D. A., & Randle, R. L. (2002). Naming and categorization in young
children: vocal tact training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78, 527–549.
Lowe, C. F., Horne, P. J., & Hughes, J. C. (2005). Naming and categorization in young children: III. Vocal tact
training and transfer of function. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 83,47–65.
McIlvane, W. J., & Dube, W. V. (2003). Stimulus control topography coherence theory: foundations and
extensions. The Behavior Analyst, 26,195–213.
Miguel, C. F., Petursdottir, A. I., & Carr, J. E. (2005). The effects of multiple-tact and receptive-discrimination
training on the acquisition of intraverbal behavior. TheAnalysisofVerbalBehavior,21,27–41.
Miguel, C. F., Petursdottir, A. I., Carr, J. E., & Michael, J. (2008). The role of naming in stimulus
categorization by preschool children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 89, 383–405.
Novak, G., & Pelaez, M. (2004). Child and adolescent development. Thousand Oaks: Sage.
Nuzzolo-Gomez, R., & Greer, R. D. (2004). Emergence of untaught mands or tacts of novel adjective-object
pairs as a function of instructional history. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 20,63–76.
Petursdottir, A. I., & Carr, J. E. (2011). A review of recommendations for sequencing receptive and expressive
language instruction. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44,859–876.
Petursdottir, A. I., Carr, J. E., Lechago, S. A., & Almason, S. M. (2008). An evaluation of intraverbal training
and listener training for teaching categorization skills. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41,53–68.
Petursdottir, A. I., Lepper, T. L., & Peterson, S. P. (2014). Effects of collateral response requirements and
exemplar training on listener training outcomes in children. The Psychological Record, 64,703–717.
Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.
Sundberg,M.L.,&Partington,J.W.(1998).Teaching language to children with autism or other develop-
mental disabilities. Pleasant Hill: Behavior Analysts.
Taylor, B. A., & Harris, S. L. (1995). Teaching children with autism to seek information: acquisition of novel
information and generalization of responding. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28,3–14.
Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:76–95 95