ArticlePDF Available

The Effects of Multiple Exemplar Instruction on the Relation Between Listener and Intraverbal Categorization Repertoires

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

We evaluated the effects of multiple exemplar instruction (MEI) on the relation between listener and intraverbal categorization repertoires of six typically developing preschool-age children using a nonconcurrent multiple-probe design across participants. After failing to emit intraverbal categorization responses following listener categorization training, participants were exposed to MEI in the form of alternating response forms (listener and intraverbal) during categorization training with novel stimulus sets. For two participants for whom there was some evidence of emergent intraverbal responding, responding was variable. For the remaining four participants, 32 to 99 MEI trial blocks produced minimal improvement in responding or no emergent responding at all. The results are discussed in terms of Skinner’s analysis of verbal behavior and naming theory.
Content may be subject to copyright.
The Effects of Multiple Exemplar Instruction
on the Relation Between Listener
and Intraverbal Categorization Repertoires
Sarah A. Lechago &James E. Carr &
April N. Kisamore &Laura L. Grow
Published online: 11 March 2015
#Association for Behavior Analysis International 2015
Abstract We evaluated the effects of multiple exemplar instruction (MEI) on the
relation between listener and intraverbal categorization repertoires of six typically
Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:7695
DOI 10.1007/s40616-015-0027-1
This article is based on a dissertation submitted by the first author, under the supervision of the second author,
in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the PhD degree in psychology at Western Michigan University.
We thank Jesse Slappey, Chiara Cunningham, Jennifer Davis, Tania Gonzales, Rachel Wagner, Dylan
Churchwell, Kerry Conde, Laura Reisdorf, Nicole Shriver, Jodie Newsome, Jacquelin Jackson, Sean
Reynolds, James Mellor, Jackie Hoag, Abby Mercure, Courtney Fox, Rob Long, Taylor Butts, Adam
Freeman, Chris Zielinski, Megan Baumgartner, and Cindy Han for their assistance with data collection and
conducting sessions. Finally, we thank Pat Oldham, Lori Sebright, Pamela Kelly, and Kathy Graff for their
invaluable on-site support.
S. A. Lechago (*)
Department of Psychology, University of HoustonClear Lake, 2700 Bay Area Boulevard, Suite 2617,
Houston, TX 77058-1098, USA
e-mail: lechago@uhcl.edu
J. E. Carr
Auburn University, Auburn, AL, USA
A. N. Kisamore
Western New England College, Springfield, MA, USA
L. L. Grow
St. Cloud State University, St. Cloud, MN, USA
Present Address:
J. E. Carr
Behavior Analyst Certification Board, 8051 Shaffer Parkway, Littleton, CO 80127, USA
Present Address:
A. N. Kisamore
Caldwell University, 120 Bloomfield Avenue, Caldwell, NJ 07006, USA
Present Address:
L. L. Grow
University of British Columbia, Vancouver campus, 2125 Main Mall, Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4, Canada
developing preschool-age children using a nonconcurrent multiple-probe design across
participants. After failing to emit intraverbal categorization responses following listener
categorization training, participants were exposed to MEI in the form of alternating
response forms (listener and intraverbal) during categorization training with novel
stimulus sets. For two participants for whom there was some evidence of emergent
intraverbal responding, responding was variable. For the remaining four participants,
32 to 99 MEI trial blocks produced minimal improvement in responding or no
emergent responding at all. The results are discussed in terms of Skinnersanalysis
of verbal behavior and naming theory.
Keywords Categorization .Intraverbal behavior.Listener behavior.Multiple exemplar
instruction .Naming
Curricular targets designed to teach verbal behavior are embedded within early and
intensive behavioral intervention (EIBI) programs for childhood autism and constitute a
large and critically important part of an individuals curriculum. Some EIBI programs
and some clinicians promote and utilize the Bverbal behavior approach^to teach
language or verbal behavior (Barbera and Rasmussen 2007; Sundberg and Partington
1998). According to the verbal behavior approach, it is not assumed that once an
individual learns to emit a word under the influence of one set of controlling variables,
she will automatically be able to emit the same word under a different set of controlling
variables. This analysis has implications for how language is taught. For example, if an
individual is taught to ask for water when she is water deprived (a mand), different
manipulations of antecedents and consequences are required to ensure she will be able
to emit the same response (water) when shown a picture of a glass of water and asked,
BWhat is this?^(a tact). There is support in the research literature that lends credence to
the assertion that the verbal operants are functionally independent from one another and
from listener behavior (Gilic and Greer 2011;LamarreandHolland1985;Migueletal.
2005; Petursdottir et al. 2008).
Despite existing evidence demonstrating functional independence across verbal
operants, it appears that typically developing individuals eventually learn to respond
across verbal operants without explicit training (Skinner 1957; Taylor and Harris 1995).
That is, they are able to emit a response under conditions in which they have not
received any direct training. However, such emergent responding between verbal
operants and between listener and speaker repertoires is notoriously deficient in many
individuals with language delays (Guess and Baer 1973;Kelleyetal.2007;Nuzzolo-
Gomez and Greer 2004). As such, it behooves researchers to develop procedures that
generate emergent responding across verbal operants. The result of such research might
enhance early intervention curricula such that verbal behavior is taught more compre-
hensively and efficiently. Multiple exemplar instruction (MEI) represents one way to
achieve emergent responding between verbal operants and between listener and speaker
repertoires.
One form of MEI consists of alternating instruction between two or more response
functions in a subset of exemplars, which results in emergent responding in initially
functionally independent verbal operants or response forms (Fiorile and Greer 2007;
Greer et al. 2005a,b; Nuzzolo-Gomez and Greer 2004). Several studies have examined
Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:7695 77
the effects of this specific form of MEI on emergence across verbal operants or
response forms. Nuzzolo-Gomez and Greer (2004) tested the effects of MEI on the
emergence of untrained mands or tacts of adjective-object pairs in four children with
autism. The experimenters presented three sets of adjective-object pairs (e.g., small cup,
third box, middle bowl). The participants were taught to either tact or mand for the first
or second set of adjective-object pairs. Baseline probes confirmed the absence of the
untaught verbal relation (i.e., mands if a tact was taught, and vice versa). MEI was then
conducted with a new set of adjective-object pairs. This condition consisted of alter-
nating mand and tact training trials with each adjective-object pair. After MEI, probes
were once again conducted with the untrained operant from the original set and a novel
set. The authors demonstrated that MEI resulted in high levels of correct responding on
the untrained operant from the original and novel sets.
Greer et al. (2005a,b) evaluated the effect of MEI on generating joint control
between novel dictation and intraverbal responding with children with language delays.
Usingapre-andpost-testsimilartotheoneemployedbyNuzzolo-Gomez
and Greer (2004), the authors observed low levels of the untrained response,
vocal or written spelling, in baseline. A subsequent MEI condition resulted in
high levels of emergent responding across participants. Greer et al. (2005a,b)
replicated these findings with children with language delays targeting vocal and
written spelling. It should be noted that the responses targeted in studies that
have investigated the effects of the aforementioned iteration of MEI were
relatively straightforward (e.g., Greer et al. 2005a,b). In other words, emergent
responding was examined across operants in which the target response form
was topographically identical (e.g., tacts and mands), across listener responses
in the form of pointing and one-word tacts, or across modalities of the same
responses (writing and orally spelling a word). Another point to note is that in
both the Nuzzolo-Gomez and Greer (2004)andGreeretal.(2005a,b)studies,
participants were probed only once during baseline before the MEI procedure
was introduced, making it challenging to discount maturation as a potential
confounding variable. Also, conducting multiple probes during the baseline
condition would have helped to control for practice effects. Nevertheless, this
line of research provides evidence that MEI may produce functional emergent
responding between verbal operants or different response forms (Fiorile and
Greer 2007; Greer et al. 2005a,b; Nuzzolo-Gomez and Greer 2004).
Categorization skills, which involve more complex forms of responding, are ubiq-
uitous in our daily functioning. They constitute a critical part of an individuals
academic, social, and professional success. For some learners, categorization skills
and emergent responding between categorization skills eventually come with apparent
relative ease, and minimal or no instruction.
However, for other learners, especially those with language delays and other
developmental disabilities, this emergent responding is not easily acquired.
Therefore, it is important to identify those procedures that help establish emergent
responding between categorization skills. The present study contributes to this
endeavor by identifying the limitations of a specific iteration of the MEI procedure in
generating emergent responding between listener and intraverbal categorization
repertoires. This study was an extension of the Petursdottir et al. (2008) study, which
demonstrated that training listener categorization relations did not result in the
78 Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:7695
emergence of intraverbal categorization relations, and vice versa, in typically develop-
ing preschool-age children. The purpose of the present study was to determine the
effects of MEI on emergent responding between intraverbal and listener categorization
repertoires. More specifically, the present study evaluated the effects of MEI on the
emergence (or lack thereof) of intraverbal categorization following listener categoriza-
tion training. The MEI procedure employed in this study was most similar to the
Nuzzolo-Gomez and Greer (2004) study in which there was one-to-one alternation
between the response types of interest per stimulus, with minor differences in the
alternation format of the response types.
Method
Participants, Setting, and Materials
Six typically developing children participated in the study; Doug (4 years, 7 months
old), Sophie (4 years, 2 months), Alex (4 years, 2 months), Rick (3 years, 10 months),
Mike (4 years), and Meredith (4 years, 2 months). English was the participantsprimary
language and none had documented developmental delays.
Sessions were conducted at the participantspreschool in a partitioned corner of the
childrens dining area (Doug, Sophie, Alex, Rick) or in an empty preschool classroom
(Mike, Meredith). Sessions were conducted one to two times per day, five to ten times
per week. One to two graduate students, and one to two undergraduate research
assistants were present during each session.
Sessions were conducted at a child-sized table with three to four child-sized
chairs. Tokens were delivered for correct responding and appropriate sitting. A
small audio recorder was placed on the table within the childs view. The audio
recorder was turned on during sessions to record participantsvocal behavior.
The participants had the opportunity to select a toy or snack from a large
plastic tub containing small age-appropriate toys and parent-approved snacks at
the end of each session.
Visual stimuli were presented to participants using three-ring binders. The stimuli
were placed on 8.5×11 in sheets that were presented in a horizontal manner to the
participants. Each sheet was placed inside a page protector. The novel stimuli were
outlines of maps of cities or countries and characters from foreign writing systems.
Novel stimuli were those stimuli that the participants were unable to tact or receptively
identify prior to this study. Familiar stimuli were those stimuli that the participants were
able to tact or receptively identify prior to this study. All the stimuli were in black and
white. The names of the stimuli were the corresponding names of the cities/countries or
characters. Some of the names of the stimuli were modified for ease of pronunciation
for the participants (e.g., Poro for Sapporo). The tact training binder contained the
stimuli used to train the participants to label (tact) each of the novel stimuli. One picture
(stimulus) was depicted on each sheet. The listener training,listener categorization
training,andMEI binders contained sheets of three stimuli to which participants would
point upon the experimenters request (see Fig. 1for a sample of the visual stimuli
presented during the study). Table 1contains the category names, stimuli names, and
set designation for the novel stimuli.
Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:7695 79
Data Collection
Dependent Variables
The primary dependent variable was the emission of untrained intraverbal categoriza-
tion responses after initial listener categorization training before and after MEI. For
example, after the participant was trained to point to the Japanese character, BYo to^in
response to the instruction, BWhich one is Japanese?^, a probe for the participants
untrained emission of the vocal response BJapanese^in response to the instruction,
durbin (Africa) oria (Africa)
CATEGORY 1 – “JAPAN”
CATEGORY 2 – “AFRICA”
CATEGORY 3 – “EAST”
jeeling (E.India) cal (E. India)
poro (Japan) yoto (Japan)
Fig. 1 Three sample categories and their respective stimuli
80 Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:7695
BYoto is ___^was conducted. Additionally, data were collected on responses during tact
and listener training of the exemplar stimuli, preexperimental listener and intraverbal
categorization training with familiar stimuli, listener categorization training of novel
stimuli, and listener and intraverbal categorization training of the MEI stimulus sets.
Response Measurement
For all conditions, a response was counted as correct if the participant emitted the target
response (a pointing response for all listener targets and a vocal response for all speaker
targets) within 10 s of the experimenters instruction. A response was counted as
incorrect if the participant emitted an incorrect response or did not respond within
10 s of the instruction. Data were analyzed as the total number of correct responses out
of set number of possible responses as follows: (a) 8 responses for tact and listener
training and categorization training with familiar stimuli, (b) 20 responses for listener
categorization training with novel stimuli and intraverbal categorization probes, and (c)
32 responses for MEI.
Interobserver Agreement
A second observer independently collected interobserver agreement (IOA) data across
all experimental conditions. An agreement was scored for each trial in which the
experimenter and the observer both recorded the same correct or incorrect response.
Point-by-point agreement was calculated for each session by dividing the number of
agreements by the sum of agreements and disagreements and multiplying by 100. The
IOA results for each participant are depicted in Table 2. Agreement was assessed for at
least 88 % of sessions and averaged at least 99 % for each participant.
Tab l e 1 Category names, stimuli names, and set designation for novel stimuli
Category Members Set designation
South Cochin, Madras Set 1
Arabic Kaaf, Baa Set 1
Greek Pi, Mega Set 2
Africa Durbin, Oria Set 2
East Jeeling, Cal Set 3
Russian Shah, Yoo Set 3
Hebrew Mem, Shin MEI (A)
Sri Lanka Jaff, Mannar MEI (A)
Armenian Vev, Toh MEI (A)
Japan Yoto, Poro MEI (A)
Swit Eva, Laus MEI (B)
Rin Bu, Ka MEI (B)
German Lub, Bruck MEI (B)
Nese Yuk, Saan MEI (B)
Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:7695 81
Procedure
General Teaching Procedures
For the conditions that involved motor responses (listener training, listener categoriza-
tion training of familiar and novel stimuli), pointing to the correct response indepen-
dently within 10 s of the instruction (e.g., BWhich one is East?^), produced enthusiastic
praise from the experimenter (e.g., BWow! Thats right!^). For all conditions that
involved speaker behavior (tact training, intraverbal categorization training of familiar
and novel stimuli), emission of the correct response independently within 10 s of the
instruction (e.g., BWhat is it?^for tact training, BYoto is ___^for intraverbal categori-
zation training), produced enthusiastic praise from the experimenter. Tokens were
provided on an intermittent schedule of VR 3 contingent on appropriate attending,
imitation of the experimenters model prompts, and independent responses. If the
participant made an incorrect response during conditions that involved motor responses,
the experimenter provided gestural and verbal prompts (e.g., BIts this one. Can you
pointtoit?^). If the participant emitted the incorrect response during tact and intraverbal
categorization conditions, the experimenter provided an echoic prompt (e.g., BEast. Can
you say it?^). Imitation of the experimenters model produced praise (e.g., BYou re
right!^) and nonresponses or other responses resulted in additional gestural and verbal
prompts. After the participant was able to independently emit the correct responses with
reinforcement, the experimenter presented the stimuli again under extinction. This way,
if correct responses during subsequent probes for the primary dependent variable
(intraverbal categorization responding) did not occur or occurred at low rates, it could
be concluded with greater confidence that these outcomes were due to a failure to
acquire the response and not due to the transition from reinforcement to extinction.
Preliminary Procedures
Upon giving permission for their child to participate, parents were provided a
survey to nominate their childs favorite foods and toys. The results of the survey
Tab l e 2 Assessment results for interobserver agreement and procedural fidelity
Participant Interobserver agreement (IOA) Procedural fidelity Procedural fidelity IOA
Doug 95 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 75100)
97 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 75100)
63 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 62100)
Sophie 95 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 87100)
95 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 88100)
52 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 88100)
Alex 98 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 75100)
96 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 91100)
42 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 75100)
Rick 98 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 87100)
99 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 98100)
63 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 95100)
Mike 92 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 88100)
92 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 75100)
23 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 75100)
Meredith 88 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 88100)
88 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 87100)
17 % of sessions, M=99 %
(range 88100)
82 Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:7695
were used to select toys and snacks that were made available to participants after
each session.
Echoic Assessment The experimenter conducted an informal echoic assessment with
each participant to ensure that the participants were able to respond to the echoic
prompts that would be used in subsequent conditions.
Tact Tr a i n i n g During the tact training condition, the experimenter taught the partici-
pants to vocally tact the stimuli in set 1 (eight stimuli), set 2 (eight stimuli), and MEI A
(eight stimuli) for a total of 24 stimuli. Tacts were targeted one set at a time and one trial
block included all eight stimuli. Sets 1 and 2 included two categories with two stimuli
that corresponded to each category for a total of four target stimuli. Participants were
taught to tact stimuli from similar but untrained categories (eight additional stimuli
between sets 1 and 2 divided into four stimuli per set) that served as negative
comparisons. Additional sets of stimuli were trained if the participants required a
second implementation of MEI or if replication sets were required. During each trial,
the experimenter presented the stimulus on the table in front of the participant and
provided the instruction, BWhat is it?^Tact maintenance trials were typically conducted
every other experimental session in alternation with listener maintenance trials.
Maintenance trials were conducted to ensure that participants were able to tact and
point to members of the targeted categories throughout the study. After the participants
responded correctly with reinforcement at 100 %, trials were conducted under extinc-
tion. The mastery criterion was 100 % responding for all eight stimuli under extinction.
Listener Training The experimenter taught the participants to point to the stimuli in sets
1, 2, and MEI A. Additional sets of stimuli were trained as needed. The experimenter
presented the picture of the target stimulus along with two comparison stimuli and
instructed the participant to indicate the target stimulus (e.g., BShow me Yoto.^). The
mastery criterion was 100 % correct responding for all eight stimuli under extinction.
Listener and Intraverbal Categorization Training Trials with Familiar Stimuli These
trials were conducted with familiar stimuli to ensure instructional control of the trial
format over responding when testing with novel stimuli. Categorization testing with
familiar stimuli was conducted at the beginning of nearly every session in the entire
study except during MEI. Two stimuli from four categories comprised the eight stimuli
that were targeted. Categories that were targeted were animals, food, clothes, and toys.
Listener categorization training with familiar stimuli involved teaching the participants
to point to the stimulus that corresponded to the category indicated in the experi-
menters instruction (e.g., point to the cat in response to the instruction BWhich one is
an animal?^). After the participants responded correctly with reinforcement at 100 %,
trails were conducted under extinction. The mastery criterion was 100 % for all eight
stimuli under extinction. Intraverbal categorization training with familiar stimuli in-
volved teaching the participants to emit the name of the category that corresponded to
the stimulus indicated in the experimenters instruction (e.g., Saying BAn animal^in
response to the instruction, BA cat is ___.^). After the participants responded correctly
with reinforcement at 100 %, trials were conducted under extinction. The mastery
criterion was 100 % independent responding for all eight stimuli under extinction, for
Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:7695 83
both listener categorization training and intraverbal categorization training with familiar
stimuli.
Listener Categorization Training with Novel Stimuli This training was conducted with
one set of experimental stimuli (four stimuli were targeted and four stimuli served as
negative comparisons). Training was identical to the procedure described above except
that novel stimuli instead of familiar stimuli were used. After the participants responded
correctly with reinforcement at 100 %, trials were conducted under extinction. The
mastery criterion for listener categorization training sessions was 100 % independent
responding for all 20 stimuli under extinction.
Experimental Evaluation
A nonconcurrent multiple-probe design across participants was used to evaluate the
effects of MEI on the relation between intraverbal and listener categorization reper-
toires. The order of experimental conditions is depicted in Fig. 2.
Baseline Probes After listener categorization training was conducted with one set,
intraverbal categorization baseline probes were conducted. Listener categorization
training and intraverbal categorization probes were conducted separately. As an exam-
ple, after training the participant to point to outlines of Durbin and Oria in response to
the instruction, BWhich one is Africa?^during listener categorization training, a probe
during the intraverbal categorization baseline was conducted to determine if the
participant emitted the response BAfrica^in response to the instructions, BDurbin
is^and BOria is.^The order of stimulus presentation was randomly rotated across
20-trial blocks during the intraverbal categorization probes to ensure that responding
occurred under appropriate stimulus control (i.e., to ensure responding occurred under
the influence of the verbal instruction). Correct and incorrect responses did not result in
any differential consequences other than the presentation of a new trial. Baseline was
conducted until each stimulus within each of the categories (two categories, four
stimuli) was presented five times for a total of 20 trials.
MEI with set MEI A After the baseline condition, MEI was conducted with a second set
of stimuli (MEI A). Four categories (eight stimuli, two stimuli per category) were
targeted. Blocks of four trials were presented, each of which contained two trials of
intraverbal categorization training and two trials of listener categorization training
alternating between trial types. Only one stimulus was targeted per trial block. Data
were graphed and analyzed as the total number of correct responses out of a possible 16
correct per categorization type (listener and intraverbal) for a 32-trial block. Mastery
criterion was 100 % independent responding for one 32-trial block under extinction.
Depending on the participant, sessions lasted approximately 1530 min.
Post-MEI probes After MEI was conducted with set MEI A, post-MEI test probes for
intraverbal categorization responses were conducted again with sets 1 or 2. Before
intraverbal categorization probes were conducted, probes for listener categorization
responses were conducted to ensure the participants were still responding at mastery
84 Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:7695
Tact Training:
Novel Exemplars
Listener Training:
Novel Exemplars
Intraverbal/Listener
Categorization
Training:
Familiar Exemplars
Listener Cat.Training:
Novel Exemplars
(First Set)
MAINTENANCE
Intraverbal
Categorization Pre-
MEI Probe:
Novel Exemplars
(First Set)
Intraverbal
Categorization Probe:
Novel Exemplars
(First Set)
Listener
Categorization Probe:
Intraverbal
Categorization
Mastered:
(First Set)
Intraverbal
Categorization NOT
Mastered:
(First Set)
Listener
Categorization
Training:
(Second Set)
Intraverbal
Categorization Probe:
(Second Set)
MEI (B)
Listener
Categorization Probe:
(First Set)
Intraverbal
Categorization Probe:
(First Set)
Intraverbal Cat.Probe:
Novel Exemplars
(First Set)
MEI (A)
Novel Exemplars
(First Set)
Fig. 2 The sequence of training and testing conditions
Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:7695 85
criterion. If not, then listener categorization training was conducted until responding to
mastery criterion was reachieved. After this, probes for intraverbal categorization
responses were conducted. Probes were identical to those described under the baseline
condition. If responding for intraverbal categorization responses was at mastery crite-
rion (80 % or higher), then a new set was introduced and tested (e.g., if set 1 was
initially used, then listener categorization training and intraverbal categorization probes
were conducted with set 2). If responding met the mastery criterion during intraverbal
categorization probes for set 1, listener categorization training was conducted with a
new set (e.g., set 2). Prior to listener categorization training with set 3 for Sophie, an
intraverbal categorization probe was conducted with set 3. This step was conducted to
demonstrate that the participant was unable to emit intraverbal categorization responses
prior to listener categorization training. This step was not conducted with Doug due to
experimenter error. Given the esoteric nature of the targeted stimuli, however, it is
unlikely that Doug would be able to emit intraverbal categorization responses for
sets 2 and 3 in the absence of listener categorization training. Listener categori-
zation training then was conducted with set 2. Immediately after responding met
the mastery criterion during listener categorization training, probes for intraverbal
categorization responses were conducted for sets 2 or 3. Each stimulus in the set
(two categories, four stimuli total) was presented five times each for a total of 20
presentations. These data were analyzed and reported as the total number of
correct responses out of a possible 20 responses. If the probes for intraverbal
categorization responding were at 80 % or higher for 20 total presentations, the
participant was dismissed from the study.
MEI with set MEI B If responding during intraverbal categorization probes after MEI
for sets 1 or 2 was not at mastery criterion, additional MEI was conducted with a new
set (MEI B) which contained four categories. This initially involved tact and listener
training for all eight stimuli. After MEI was conducted to mastery criterion for set MEI
B, probes were conducted again with sets 1or 2. Before the intraverbal categorization
probes were conducted, probes for listener categorization responding were conducted
to ensure the participants were still responding at mastery level. As described above, if
intraverbal categorization responding was at mastery criterion (80 % or higher), then a
new set (e.g., set 2 or 3) was introduced at this time in an attempt to replicate the effects
of MEI. If mastery-level responding was not achieved after a second implementation of
MEI with set MEI B, then the participant was dismissed from the study.
Procedural Fidelity
A secondary observer scored a trial correct if the experimenter delivered the instruction,
prompts, and consequences appropriate to the phase and the childs response. A
procedural integrity score was then computed for each session as the percentage of
correctly implemented trials. Point-by-point IOA was assessed on all procedural fidelity
measures. Procedural fidelity scores and their corresponding IOA data are depicted in
Tab le 2. Procedural fidelity was assessed for at least 88 % of sessions and averaged at
least 99 % for each participant. Interobserver agreement on procedural fidelity data was
assessed for at least 17 % of sessions and averaged at least 99 % for each participant.
86 Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:7695
Results
Tact and Listener Training
The results of the tact and listener training conditions are depicted in Table 3. The table
includes the total number of trial blocks required to acquire all of the tact and listener
stimulus sets, the number of sets taught, the total number of stimuli, and the mean
number of trial blocks required to acquire each set. Participants acquired the tact sets in
92 to 147 trial blocks, and the listener sets in 4 to 8 trial blocks.
Listener and Intraverbal Categorization Training (Familiar Stimuli)
The results for the listener and intraverbal categorization training condition with
familiar stimuli for each participant are depicted in Table 2. The table includes the
total number of trial blocks required to acquire listener and intraverbal categorization
with familiar stimuli, whether the participant made errors with each categorization type
and any interesting collateral verbal behavior. Participants acquired the target listener
and intraverbal categorization relations in two to five trial blocks.
Tab l e 3 Results for tact training, listener training, and listener/intraverbal categorization training (familiar
stimuli)
Participant Tact training Listener training Listener/intraverbal categorization
training
Doug 95 trial blocks (M=23)
4 sets
32 stimuli
8 trial blocks (M=2)
4 sets
32 stimuli
2 trial blocks
No errors with either categorization type
Emitted an echoic and a tact during
a listener categorization trial
a
Sophie 122 trial blocks (M=26)
5 sets
40 stimuli
5 trial blocks (M=1)
5 sets
40 stimuli
3 trial blocks
Errors with intraverbal categorization
Alex 139 trial blocks (M=35)
4 sets
32 stimuli
7 trial blocks (M=2)
4 sets
32 stimuli
2 trial blocks
Errors with intraverbal categorization
Rick 92 trial blocks (M=23)
4 sets
32 stimuli
4 trial blocks (M=1)
4 sets
32 stimuli
4 trial blocks
Errors with intraverbal and listener
categorization
Mike 147 trial blocks (M=29)
5 sets
40 stimuli
5 trial blocks (M=1)
5 sets
40 stimuli
5 trial blocks
Errors with intraverbal categorization
Meredith 106 trial blocks (M=21)
5 sets
40 stimuli
Provided the name of the correct
category, instead of the name
of the stimulus itself during
a maintenance trial
a
7 trial blocks (M=1)
5 sets
40 stimuli
4 trial blocks
Errors with intraverbal and listener
categorization
a
Collateral verbal behavior of interest
Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:7695 87
Listener Categorization Training (Novel Stimuli)
The results for the listener categorization training condition with novel stimuli for each
participant are depicted in Table 4. The table includes the total number of sets taught
and the number of trials blocks required to master each set. Participants acquired the
target listener categorization relations in four to eight trial blocks per set.
Multiple Exemplar Instruction
Each participants MEI data are depicted in Table 4. The table includes the total number
of trial blocks required to acquire each MEI set and collateral verbal behavior of
interest. Participants completed MEI in 14 to 56 trial blocks per set.
Primary Experimental Evaluation (Intraverbal Categorization Probes)
Baseline (Pre-MEI)
Results for all six participantsperformance during intraverbal categorization probes
pre- and post-MEI training are depicted in Figs. 3and 4. During baseline, Doug scored
45 % correct for set 1, occasionally responding with targets from both categories. The
remaining five participants scored 0 % during baseline.
Post-MEI probes after MEI A
After ensuring listener categorization responses for set 1 and MEI A responses were
still at mastery, intraverbal categorization probes were conducted. Doug responded with
100 % accuracy under extinction during the first post-MEI probe. In attempts to
replicate these effects, probes were conducted with sets 2 and 3. Doug scored 50 %
Tab l e 4 Results for listener categorization training and multiple exemplar instruction
Participant Listener categorization training (novel stimuli) Multiple exemplar instruction
Doug Set 1=5 trial blocks
Set 2=4 trial blocks
Set 3=4 trial blocks
Set A= 14 trial blocks
Sophie Set 1=5 trial blocks
Set 3=7 trial blocks
Set A= 37 trial blocks
Set B= 33 trial blocks
Alex Set 2=4 trial blocks
Echoed the word BEast^in two trials
a
Set A= 43 trial blocks
Set B= 56 trial blocks
Rick Set 1=4 trial blocks Set A= 40 trial blocks
Set B= 32 trial blocks
b
Mike Set 1= 8 trial blocks Set A =17 trial blocks
Set B= 15 trial blocks
Meredith Set 1= 8 trial blocks Set A =20 trial blocks
Set B= 17 trial blocks
a
Collateral verbal behavior of interest
b
Modified mastery criterion
88 Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:7695
0
5
10
15
20
1234567
Set 1
Set 1
Set 2
Set 2
Set 3
Set 3
Set 3
Number of Correct Responses
Trial Blocks
Pre- Post-MEI
0
5
10
15
20
123456789
Sophie
Set 1 Set 1 Set 1
Set 1
Set 1
Set 3 Set 3
Set 3 Set 3
0
5
10
15
20
12345
Alex
Set 2 Set 2 Set 2 Set 2
Set 2
0
5
10
15
20
12345
Rick
Set 1 Set 1 Set 1 Set 1 Set 1
MEI (A)
MEI (B) Pre- LC
Training
Post- LC
Training
Doug
Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:7695 89
in the first set 2 probe. A second probe with set 2 resulted in a score of 90 %. In
response to these mixed results, another intraverbal categorization probe was conducted
with set 3. During the first probe with set 3, Doug scored 50 %, and with a second
probe, Doug scored 70 %. Given these mixed results, a third set 3 probe was conducted
in which Doug responded at 95 % correct.
For Sophie, two probes with set 1 each resulted in a score of 0 % correct. During
both probes, Sophie shrugged during each trial and said BIdontknowthese^or BI
0
5
10
15
20
Mike
MEI (A) MEI (B)
Set 1 Set 1 Set 1 Set 1Set 1 Set 1 Set 1
0
5
10
15
20
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
Meredith
Set 1 Set 1 Set 1 Set 1 Set 1 Set 1 Set 1
Pre-MEI
Trial Blocks
Number of Correct Responses
Post-MEI
Fig. 4 Number of correct responses during intraverbal categorization probes before and after multiple
exemplar instruction (MEI) for Mike and Meredith
Fig. 3 Number of correct responses during intraverbal categorization probes before and after multiple
exemplar instruction (MEI) for Doug, Sophie, Alex, and Rick. LC listener categorization
90 Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:7695
dontknow.^Alexs three post-MEI A probes with set 2 each resulted in scores of 0 %.
During the third post-MEI A probe, he repeatedly said, BIdontknow^and Bnothing.^
Ricks two post-MEI A probes with set 1 each resulted in scores of 0 %. During the first
of these probes, Rick emitted the exemplar and category names for stimuli targeted
during tact and MEI A training (e.g., brew, lanka, and yoo) for 8 out of the 20 trials.
Neither of the category names (brew and lanka) was correct for set 1. During the second
post-MEI A probe, he emitted three exemplar names (madras, senza, and chi) one time
each, and then names of random common stimuli not targeted in the study (e.g., apple
head and pancake) for 13 trials. Mikes two post-MEI A probes with set 1 each resulted
in a score of 0 %. On many occasions, he imitated the stimulus in the experimenters
instruction. Merediths two post-MEI A probes with set 1 each resulted in a score of
0%.
Post MEI (B)
A second implementation of MEI (MEI B) was conducted with Sophie, Alex, Rick,
Mike, and Meredith. During the first of these probes for set 1, Sophie scored 45 %.
During the second probe with set 1, Sophie scored 95 %. In an effort to replicate the
effects achieved with set 1, intraverbal categorization probes were conducted with set 3.
The first intraverbal categorization probe resulted in a score of 0 %. After listener
categorization training, the first intraverbal categorization probe resulted in a score of
0 %. Two more intraverbal categorization probes with set 3 resulted in a score of 50 %.
Alex scored 20 % on his post-MEI B intraverbal categorization probe with set 2 during
which he emitted category names targeted during MEI A and B. Rick, Mike, and
Meredith scored 0 %, during post-MEI B probes for set 1.
Discussion
The results of the present study extend the literature on MEI by demonstrating that it
was not reliably effective in producing emergent responding between listener and
intraverbal categorization repertoires in six typically developing children between the
ages of 3 and 4 years. It is possible that MEI failed to produce emergent responding
between the targeted verbal operants in the present study because of the more complex
nature of the repertoires involved (i.e., categorization). This study represents a starting
point for MEI procedures and their utility for producing emergent responding for more
complex forms of responding.
Doug may have engaged in covert responding that produced the emergent intraverbal
categorization responding observed during baseline and post-MEI probes. A consider-
ation of the Naming theory is warranted. When an individual has learned to respond to a
given stimulus as both a speaker and a listener, it can be said that he has acquired naming
(Horne and Lowe 1996; Lowe et al. 2002). A critical feature of the naming account is
that many stimuli may come to produce a common speaker and listener response,
implicating the naming relation in the establishment of categorization (Horne et al.
2004;Loweetal.2005;Migueletal.2008). During listener categorization training,
when the experimenter asked Doug, BWhich one is Africa?,^he was taught to point to
the outlines of the cities Durbin and Oria. While pointing to Durbin and Oria, he may
Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:7695 91
have responded to his own listener behavior (pointing to the picture of Durbin and Oria)
as a speaker and covertly tacted the exemplar and echoed the corresponding category
(BDurbin, Africa^and BOria, Africa^). During intraverbal categorization probes, the
experimenter asked Doug BDurbin is ___?,^during which time he may have engaged in
covert responding BDurbin, Africa,^established during listener categorization training,
and then emitted the response BAfrica.^Teaching Doug to respond to both Durbin and
Oria as a listener to a common name (Africa) may have then resulted in Doug
responding to both Durbin and Oria as a speaker using a common name (Africa). The
auditory stimulus BDurbin,^served as a stimulus common to both instructional contexts,
possibly resulting in stimulus control over responding in both instructional contexts
(listener and intraverbal categorization) and influencing the establishment of emergent
responding. While consideration of the Naming theory offers a cohesive and parsimo-
nious explanation for Dougs performance, this account remains speculative as there
was no evidence of covert responding and functional control was not demonstrated over
the post-MEI response increases.
Sophie required exposure to two sets of MEI, which yielded comparatively little
emergent responding. The reverse categorization relation was originally examined with
Sophie. That is, the intraverbal categorization repertoire was trained and listener
categorization responses were probed. These data are not graphed. When the experi-
menter probed listener categorization responses during baseline, she demonstrated
100 % emergent responding providing evidence of emergent responding between the
two categorization types in one direction (from intraverbal to listener), indicating an
emerging Naming relation for Sophie. For Sophie, perhaps hearing both the exemplar
name and the category name (e.g., BDurbin is Africa^) exerted greater control over
responding during listener categorization probe trials than was the case with the reverse
relation. During listener categorization training, the experimenter provided only the
auditory stimulus of the category name (e.g., BAfrica^). Maybe learners with a more
extensive history with respect to categorization, like Doug, are more likely to tact,
overtly or covertly, the exemplar name (e.g., BDurbin^) during listener categorization
training trials than those learners who have less experience with categorization. As
such, the learner produces an additional relevant auditory stimulus during listener
categorization (BDurbin^) that will be present during intraverbal categorization probes
(BDurbin is ____^), which would subsequently increase the likelihood of emergent
responding between the two categorization types.
Rick and Alex required a large number of training trials to learn two sets of MEI.
Despite training two sets of MEI, there was very little to no emergent responding,
suggesting a weak or nonexistent naming repertoire. Rick was nearly 1 year younger
than Doug and the only participant under the age of 4 years at the start of the study. This
may be important when considering the types of skills typically targeted with children
between the ages of 3.5 and 4.5 years in daycare and preschool, which commonly
include tacts, listener responses (matching and pointing), and categorization. Three to
9 months of experience with these skills can make a difference with respect to
developing the verbal behavior necessary to establish a naming repertoire and produce
emergent responding.
Mike and Meredith required training with two sets of MEI, which ultimately did not
produce emergent responding between listener and intraverbal categorization reper-
toires. Mike and Meredith acquired mastery of MEI in few trials and their performances
92 Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:7695
with this task resembled that of Dougs. However, it may have been the case that not
only could Doug produce the relevant verbal behavior (e.g., tact the name of the
stimulus and echo the name of the category during listener categorization training
trials) but perhaps he also used a rehearsal strategy such that he was able to later emit
the responses during intraverbal probe trials. It may have been the case that Mike and
Meredith were not employing a rehearsal strategy, or at least not doing so consistently,
and this contributed to the failure to produce emergent intraverbal categorization
responding. Three to 4-year-old children typically do not use rehearsal strategies and
often just keep track of physical stimuli (Novak and Pelaez 2004). At around 5 years of
age, children start to use rehearsal as a strategy (Novak and Pelaez 2004). Mike and
Meredith were close to 4 years, and Doug was 4 years and 7 months at the start of the
study. This age difference with Doug may be significant in that Doug may have already
begun to use rehearsal strategies, while Mike and Meredith had not.
A few limitations of the current study are worth acknowledging. One limitation may
have been the probe trial format failing to effectively evoke desired responding,
especially as it pertained to Sophies and Mikes performances. Just as members of a
response class can produce the same consequence, so too can the topographically
varied members of a functional stimulus class influence the same response (McIlvane
and Dube 2003). Researchers endeavor to use stimuli that they assume will control the
responding of the participant. Sometimes there are features of stimuli that the researcher
may not detect or consider that control the responding of the participant. One of these
features may include trial format or presentation. Although the format of the listener
and intraverbal categorization trials during training with familiar stimuli was the same
as during listener training and intraverbal probes with novel stimuli, the trials were
presented in an alternating format, as was the case during MEI. However, during
listener categorization training with novel stimuli and during intraverbal categorization
probes with novel stimuli, these trials were presented all at once and not in an
alternating format. It may have been the case that these presentation differences
ultimately negatively impacted their performances. Future studies may, for example,
present the listener categorization trials and the intraverbal categorization trials with
familiar stimuli separately, mimicking the training and probe trial formats, instead of
presenting them in an alternating trial format as was done in the current study, which
mimicked the MEI trial format.
A second possible limitation may have been the one-to-one alternating trials format
of the MEI training procedure. Due to the fact that each stimulus was targeted in four-
trial blocks with alternation between response type, and hence close temporal proximity
of trials of each response type, it could have been the case that intraverbal categoriza-
tion responding came under echoic control of the experimenter emitting the stimulus
name, and not under control of the intraverbal frame. However, due to the stringent
response criteria (responding within 10 s of the experimenters instruction), and rapid
alternation between trial blocks, this is highly unlikely, and it is more likely that
responding during intraverbal categorization trials was under discriminative control
of the intraverbal frame.
A third limitation may have been that the mastery criterion for MEI was especially
stringent (100 % correct trials under extinction) and the number of targets included in
MEI (32 targets) may have been too great for this young population. As a result, it took
a long time for participants to reach the mastery criteria, which may have affected
Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:7695 93
motivation during experimental trials. A modification of the mastery criterion for MEI
was even required for one participant. It may have been the case that waning motivation
over time affected attending responses which in turn negatively impacted intraverbal
categorization responding. Future research studies in this area may consider using half
the number of targets during MEI training (e.g., 16 trials, two categories) and reducing
the MEI mastery criterion to 80 or 90 %.
Future research might also examine the effects of directly training exemplar and
category tacts during MEI to generate emergent responding between listener and
intraverbal categorization repertoires. During listener categorization training trials, it
may be more effective to train the pointing response, and the autoclitic frame BStimulus
is Category.^For example, the experimenter would provide the discriminative stimu-
lus, BWhich one is Africa?^and then train the participant to point to BDurbin,^then
vocalize the statement BDurbin is Africa.^Although previous research has indicated
that echoic, multiple-tact, and receptive discrimination training does not significantly
influence the emission of intraverbal behavior (Miguel et al. 2005; Petursdottir et al.
2014), perhaps embedding this training as an autoclitic frame in an alternating format
with intraverbal categorization trials in the context of MEI may teach the child to emit
relevant responses that would generate stimulus control over categorization responding
and result in emergent intraverbal categorization.
Finally, future research may also want to examine the effects of this studys
iteration of MEI and its effectiveness in producing emergent listener categori-
zation responses after training intraverbal categorization responses. It may be
the case that this studys iteration of MEI is effective contingent on a given
training sequence (expressive before receptive instead of receptive before ex-
pressive). As mentioned previously, Sophie was able to emit all listener cate-
gorization responses after initial intraverbal categorization training. For individ-
uals who do not demonstrate emergence, it may be the case that this studys
MEI procedure is effective in producing emergent responding if the expressive
(intraverbal) categorization response were trained first. The outcomes of a study
like this may also make some helpful contributions to the literature on expres-
sive and listener responses and training sequences (Petursdottir and Carr 2011).
Previous studies found MEI to be effective when the trained responses was topo-
graphically identical to the probed responses (Nuzzolo-Gomez and Greer 2004)or
when acquisition of the necessary behavior to produce emergent responding was more
straightforward, as is the case between pointing and matching responses, and tact
responses (Greer et al. 2005a,b). However, generating emergent responding between
categorization repertoires may be more complex and may require the previously
described modifications to the MEI procedure to successfully generate emergent
responding between listener and intraverbal categorization.
References
Barbera, M. L., & Rasmussen, T. (2007). The verbal behavior approach: How to teach children with autism
and related disorders. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Fiorile, C. A., & Greer, R. D. (2007). The induction of naming in children with no prior tact responses as a
function of multiple exemplar histories of instruction. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 23,7187.
94 Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:7695
Gilic, L., & Greer, R. D. (2011). Establishing naming in typically developing two-year-old children as a
function of multiple exemplar speaker and listener experiences. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 27,157
177.
Greer, R. D., Stolfi, L., Chavez-Brown, M., & Rivera-Valdes, C. (2005a). The emergence of the listener to
speaker component of naming in children as a function of multiple exemplar instruction. The Analysis of
Verbal Behavior, 21,123134.
Greer, R. D., Yaun, L., & Gautreaux, G. (2005b). Novel dictation and intraverbal responses as a function of a
multiple exemplar instructional history. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 21,99116 .
Guess, D., & Baer, D. M. (1973). An analysis of individual differences in generalization between receptive
and productive language in retarded children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 6,311329.
Horne, P. J., & Lowe, C. F. (1996). On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 185241.
Horne, P. J., Lowe, C. F., & Randle, V. R. L. (2004). Naming and categorization in young children: II. Listener
behavior training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 81, 267288.
Kelley, M. E., Shillingsburg, M. A., Castro, M. J., Addison, L. R., & LaRue, R. H., Jr. (2007). Further
evaluation of emerging speech in children with developmental disabilities: training verbal behavior.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40,431445.
Lamarre, J., & Holland, J. G. (1985). The functional independence of mands and tacts. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43,519.
Lowe, C. F., Horne, P. J., Harris, F. D. A., & Randle, R. L. (2002). Naming and categorization in young
children: vocal tact training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78, 527549.
Lowe, C. F., Horne, P. J., & Hughes, J. C. (2005). Naming and categorization in young children: III. Vocal tact
training and transfer of function. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 83,4765.
McIlvane, W. J., & Dube, W. V. (2003). Stimulus control topography coherence theory: foundations and
extensions. The Behavior Analyst, 26,195213.
Miguel, C. F., Petursdottir, A. I., & Carr, J. E. (2005). The effects of multiple-tact and receptive-discrimination
training on the acquisition of intraverbal behavior. TheAnalysisofVerbalBehavior,21,2741.
Miguel, C. F., Petursdottir, A. I., Carr, J. E., & Michael, J. (2008). The role of naming in stimulus
categorization by preschool children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 89, 383405.
Novak, G., & Pelaez, M. (2004). Child and adolescent development. Thousand Oaks: Sage.
Nuzzolo-Gomez, R., & Greer, R. D. (2004). Emergence of untaught mands or tacts of novel adjective-object
pairs as a function of instructional history. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 20,6376.
Petursdottir, A. I., & Carr, J. E. (2011). A review of recommendations for sequencing receptive and expressive
language instruction. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44,859876.
Petursdottir, A. I., Carr, J. E., Lechago, S. A., & Almason, S. M. (2008). An evaluation of intraverbal training
and listener training for teaching categorization skills. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41,5368.
Petursdottir, A. I., Lepper, T. L., & Peterson, S. P. (2014). Effects of collateral response requirements and
exemplar training on listener training outcomes in children. The Psychological Record, 64,703717.
Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.
Sundberg,M.L.,&Partington,J.W.(1998).Teaching language to children with autism or other develop-
mental disabilities. Pleasant Hill: Behavior Analysts.
Taylor, B. A., & Harris, S. L. (1995). Teaching children with autism to seek information: acquisition of novel
information and generalization of responding. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28,314.
Analysis Verbal Behav (2015) 31:7695 95
... Estudos posteriores que utilizaram o MEI com o objetivo de estabelecer IRV avaliaram diversas relações emergentes, como ecoico, tato impuro e mando (Guerra & Almeida-Verdu, 2020), respostas autoclíticas (Luke et al., 2011) e respostas de ouvinte e intraverbais (Lechago et al., 2015). Participaram destes estudos populações com diferentes perfis: pessoas com desenvolvimento típico (Lechago et (Cahill & Greer, 2014), com um quadro de deficiência intelectual (Nuzzolo-Gomez & Greer, 2004) e com deficiência auditiva (Merlin et al., 2019;Rique et al., 2017). ...
... Participaram destes estudos populações com diferentes perfis: pessoas com desenvolvimento típico (Lechago et (Cahill & Greer, 2014), com um quadro de deficiência intelectual (Nuzzolo-Gomez & Greer, 2004) e com deficiência auditiva (Merlin et al., 2019;Rique et al., 2017). Também foram realizadas diferentes manipulações na estrutura do treino de MEI, como a implementação de tentativas lineares 3 (Guerra & Almeida-Verdu, 2020;Merlin et al., 2019;Rique et al., 2017), treino de intraverbais de categorização e AVMTS (Lechago et al., 2015) e tentativas de respostas de ouvinte (seguimento de instruções) e tato impuro (Luke et al., 2011). ...
... Uma análise geral do objetivo do treino de MEI nos 24 artigos selecionados para análise mostrou que a metade (N=12) empregou este procedimento para induzir BiN (Fiorile & Greer, 2007;Gilic & Greer, 2011;Greer & Du, 2015;Greer et al., 2011;Greer et al., 2007;Greer, Stolfi et al., 2005;Hawkins et al., 2009;Hawkins et al., 2007;Olaff et al., 2017;Pereira et al., 2018;Pereira et al., 2016: Santos & Souza, 2016, sendo que Greer e Du (2015) buscaram estabelecer nomeação por exclusão (NE) 5 utilizando uma variação do MEI, o ensino por exclusão via múltiplos exemplares (exclusion multiple exemplar instruction -EMEI). A outra metade utilizou o MEI para promover IRV (Cahill & Greer, 2014;Eby et al., 2010;Greer & Yuan, 2008;Greer, Yuan et al., 2005;Guerra & Almeida-Verdu, 2020;Lechago et al., 2015;Luke et al., 2011;Merlin et al., 2019;Nuzzolo-Gomez & Greer, 2004;Rique et al., 2017;Sidener et al., 2010;Singer-Dudek et al., 2017). ...
Article
Full-text available
Neste trabalho se realizou uma revisão sistemática de estudos experimentais que têm investigado o ensino por múltiplos exemplares (MEI) na indução de ‘nomeação bidirecional’ (BiN) e na promoção da ‘interdependência entre repertórios verbais’ (IRV), buscando identificar e analisar o perfil dos participantes, a estrutura dos testes e os repertórios testados, o objetivo e a estrutura do treino de MEI, e os resultados obtidos. Uma busca nas bases de dados Wiley, PUBMED, SciELO, Web of Science e Scopus, utilizando o termo ‘multiple exemplar’, resultou em 24 artigos selecionados para análise (12 dos quais empregaram MEI para induzir BiN e 12 para promover IRV). De forma geral, observou-se que: (1) os participantes foram principalmente crianças com desenvolvimento atípico; (2) a maioria das estruturas de teste para avaliar BiN utilizou um treino de emparelhamento ao modelo por identidade com tato do estímulo modelo pelo experimentador (IDMTS+tato) antes dos testes de ouvinte e falante, e para avaliar IRV pré e pós-testes dos repertórios treinados no MEI; e foram testados principalmente ‘nomeação unidirecional de falante’, mando e tato puro; (3) as estruturas de MEI mais utilizadas foram: a rotação de tentativas de IDMTS+tato, ouvinte, tatos puro e impuro (para BiN), e rotação de tentativas de mando e tato puro (para IRV); (4) os resultados foram positivos para o estabelecimento de IRV e ‘nomeação unidirecional de falante’. Discute-se que não foram realizados testes adequados de ‘nomeação bidirecional conjunta’ ou ‘nomeação bidirecional incidental’, sendo sugeridas pesquisas para superar esta limitação e avançar o conhecimento sobre a IRV. Palavras-chave: ensino por múltiplos exemplares; nomeação bidirecional; interdependência entre repertórios verbais.
... Os dois participantes que passaram pelas sondas de tato (pré e pós-tratamento) emitiram tatos dos estímulos aprendidos, resultado que contrasta com estudos anteriores que falharam em demonstrar a emergência de tatos a partir do treino de ouvinte (Bao et al., 2017;DeSouza et al., 2019;Lechago et al., 2015). ...
... Além disso, não houve aumento no número de respostas corretas tateadas entre os conjuntos de estímulos. Estes resultados estão de acordo com de outros estudos que não encontraram evidência da transferência de controle das respostas de ouvinte para as de tato (e.g., Bao et al., 2017;Delfs et al., 2014;Ingvarsson et al., 2012;Lechago et al., 2015;Petursdottir & Carr, 2011;Sprinkle & Miguel, 2012;mas ver DeSouza et al., 2019;Kobari-Wright & Miguel, 2014). Futuras investigações podem incorporar o uso de sondas múltiplas de tato durante os tratamentos a fim de comparar a emergência do repertório nas duas condições de ensino, com e sem exigência de resposta ecoica. ...
... Allan et al. (2015) successfully demonstrated the emergence of reverse intraverbals with autistic children using a procedure similar to MEI. In contrast, Lechago et al. (2015) showed that MEI was not successful in inducing emergent intraverbal responding following listener training. In their experiment, however, participants were taught listener discriminations rather than tacts, their study evaluated categorization, and their MEI procedure did not include tact training though it did include the intraverbal. ...
Article
This study tested for the emergence of listener discriminations and intraverbal vocal responses following tact training with four autistic children. All participants were trained to tact the name and the favorite food of two contrived cartoon monsters in the presence of a picture of the monster (e.g., “What is the name of this monster?” – “Max” and “What food does the monster eat?” – “Sweets”) to evaluate the effects of emergent listener discriminations and emergent intraverbal vocal responses. Once criterion was met on the tact training, participants were tested for emergent listener discriminations (e.g., “Who eats sweets?” And “Who is Max?”) and emergent intraverbal vocal responses (e.g., “What food does Max eat?” – “Sweets” and “Who eats sweets?” – “Max” in the absence of the picture). After training, all four participants engaged in emergent listener responding but only one participant engaged in emergent intraverbal responding. Multiple exemplar instruction (MEI) was used to teach those who could not engage in emergent intraverbal responding, and it was demonstrated to be effective. These findings are educationally significant because efficiency of instruction is important to maximize instructional impact, and to reduce the time and resource-intensive nature of behavior-analytic programming.
... Hence, even though listener training can lead to emergent intraverbal behavior for some participants with ASD, it is not always effective. Similar findings have been reported in studies with typically developing children (Cortez et al., 2020;Lechago et al., 2015;Petursdottir & Haflidadottir, 2009;Petursdottir et al., 2008b;Petursdottir et al., 2014). ...
Article
Full-text available
We evaluated whether intraverbal and reverse intraverbal behavior emerged following listener training in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Six participants were each taught three sets of three “when?” questions in listener training. A multiple baseline design across behaviors (stimulus sets) was used to assess the effects of listener training. Results showed that intraverbal behavior emerged following listener training for five out of six participants. One participant received additional listener training and intraverbal training before intraverbal behavior emerged. Furthermore, reverse intraverbal responding occurred across all three sets of questions for three of the six participants. Establishing listener behavior may be a pathway for emergent intraverbal and reverse intraverbal responding in children with ASD. Future research could examine what skill repertoire may facilitate such transfer.
... MEI har også blitt benyttet for å etablere emergente intraverbaler hos typisk utviklede barn (f.eks. Lechago et al., 2015) og hos barn med autisme (f.eks. Allan et al., 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
An overall goal of this paper is to offer an overview of procedures to establish stimulus control to strengthen verbal behavior, as well as encourage further research on stimulus control and the establishment of verbal behavior. Children with developmental delays often show major challenges in acquiring language or verbal behavior. Therefore, it is crucial to introduce effective language training as early as possible. Language training largely involves establishing stimulus control in the form of simple and complex discriminations, including compound stimulus control. First, we show how stimulus control can be applied to establish verbal behavior. Second, we propose some possible solutions to difficulties with the establishment of stimulus control, such as when overshadowing and blocking describe the phenomena. Finally, we describe some effective procedures based on stimulus control technology to establish basic verbal operants, such as mand, echoic, tact, and intraverbal, as well as how generative language learning can be established. Key words: stimulus control, discrimination training, verbal behavior, procedures to establish verbal behavior, generative language learning
... 할 수 있을 뿐만 아니라 발달장애아동의 언어행동적 특성을 이해하고 현장에서 아동의 다양한 언어특성에 따라 어떤 교수방법을 활용해야 하는지를 결정하는 과정에서 도움 이 될 것이다. 따라서 지금까지 연구된 발달장애아동의 언어행동 관련 국내 중재연구에 대해 리뷰하고 이를 토대로 발달장애아동의 언어행동을 기능적으로 개선하기 위한 중 재방법과 그 효과성의 정도를 분석하는 것은 매우 중요한 과제라고 할 수 있다 (Tincani, Miller, Lorah, & Nepo, 2020 (Horne & Lowe, 1996)으로서 주로 우발 적 언어 습득 능력을 일컫는 네이밍에 효과적인 방법인데 (최진혁, 김대용, 이상아, 2017), 특히 인트라버벌을 증가시키기 위한 중재 방법으로 연구되었다 (Lechago, Carr, Kisamore, & Grow, 2015). 택트의 자극통제 전이 절차(transfer of stimulus control procedure)도 인트라버벌의 획득을 위한 중재 방법으로 지속적으로 연구되어왔다 (Coon & Miguel, 2012;Goldsmith, LeBlanc, & Sautter, 2007;Luciano, 1986;Partington & Bailey, 1993 Daegu University <Abstract> In this study, we attempted to review quantitatively research of verbal behavioral intervention for improving the verbal expression ability of children with developmental disabilities. ...
Chapter
This chapter provides a comprehensive analysis of the strategies for teaching verbal behavior to children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD). It explores the fundamental role of language and communication in human development with a focus on the functionality of verbal behavior. By breaking down verbal behavior into teachable units, such as mands, tacts, echoics, and intraverbals, the chapter underscores the necessity of a tailored, functional communication approach in the remediation of language delays and the fostering of an adequate communicative repertoire in children with autism.
Article
Full-text available
For decades, Skinner's analysis of verbal behavior has been applied in a variety of contexts. One critically important topic in this area is the extent to which some verbal operants may be learned indirectly, as a result of learning other verbal operants. This phenomenon is often referred to as emergent verbal behavior and is critical to our understanding of how language is learned in a generative fashion across the lifespan. Emergent intraverbal repertoires are especially important because responses under some degree of intraverbal control may constitute a majority of responses in a fully formed verbal repertoire. Recent literature reviews suggest that there are many published studies on emergent intraverbals, but this body of literature is highly heterogeneous. To provide an overview of this literature and map out the various tactics used across studies, we conducted a scoping review. We identified 99 experiments in total on emergent intraverbals contained 79 articles in total. Findings are summarized in terms of populations and independent variables studied, procedural variations, and recommendations.
Article
Full-text available
Intraverbal emergence has been broadly studied. The aim of this paper was analyzing discriminative and related processes involved in that emergence. The variables and results of all known articles that demonstrated emergence were analyzed by comparing the discriminative and related procedures used by the researchers and the emergence outcomes. Discriminative processes involved in learning simple and conditional discriminations, the correlation between stimuli to establish stimulus-stimulus relations, the previous acquisition of the responses of the emergent intraverbals, the previous history with stimuli of the sort of the involved stimuli, the effect of repeating probes, the optimal sequence of teaching and probing, the negative transfer of learning a second response to the same stimulus, and the effects of symmetry were found to explain most emergence results. The lack of some of the related factors resulted in failures to obtain emergence. The successful procedures suggest techniques for promoting the emergence of intraverbals in typically developing children as well as in persons with learning difficulties or developmental delays. Because of the nature of intraverbals, most instances of emergence evidence reasoning.
Article
This systematic replication was designed to teach nonvocal children with autism to mand for missing items using an Augmentative Alternative Communication (AAC) system. Consistent with Rodriguez et al. (2017) and Wójcik et al. (2020), we used the interrupted chain procedure with EO‐present and EO‐absent trials. Consistent with Wójcik et al., 2020, we used sufficient exemplar training and activity schedules to establish manding for missing items. Participants were three children with autism, and the design was a nonconcurrent multiple‐baseline design across participants. Following training, all participants requested the missing items during EO‐present trials correctly and refrained from making requests during EO‐absent trials, suggesting that requesting using the AAC system was established as a verbal operant controlled by the appropriate EO, hence, was established as a mand. Correct requesting behavior transferred to new tasks, across skill domains, across people, to new settings, and across time.
Article
Full-text available
We evaluated the effects of collateral response requirements during listener training on the emergence of vocal foreign-language tacts and intraverbals among 4- and 5-year-old children. In Experiment 1, participants were first exposed to auditory-visual match-to-sample training without collateral response requirements. Four participants did not perform to criterion in probes for derived vocal responses, and were exposed to a two-phase intervention that involved adding echoic and native-language tact requirements to match-to-sample trials. Performance did not improve as a result of the intervention. However, all participants passed tact probes after receiving direct tact and intraverbal training with a subset of the stimuli, and two of four participants also passed the intraverbal probes. Experiment 2 addressed potential limitations of Experiment 1 with three additional participants, but collateral response requirements still failed to affect the emergence of tacts and intraverbals.
Article
Full-text available
Naming is a verbal developmental capability and cusp that allows children to acquire listener and speaker functions without direct instruction (e.g., incidental learning of words for objects). We screened 19 typically developing 2- and 3-year-old children for the presence of Naming for 3-dimensional objects. All 9 3-year-olds had Naming, and 8 of 10 2-year-olds lacked Naming. For the 2-year-old children who lacked Naming, we used multiple-probe designs (2 groups of 4 children) to test the effect of multiple exemplar instruction (MEI) across speaker and listener responses on the emergence of Naming. Prior to the MEI, the children could not emit untaught listener or speaker responses following match-to-sample instruction with novel stimuli, during which they had heard the experimenter tact the stimuli. After MEI with a different set of novel stimuli, the children emitted listener and speaker responses when probed with the original stimuli, in the absence of any further instruction with those stimuli. Seven of 8 children acquired the speaker and listener responses of Naming at 83% to 100% accuracy. We discuss the basic and applied science implications.
Article
Full-text available
Stimulus control topography refers to qualitative differences among members of a functional stimulus class. Stimulus control topography coherence refers to the degree of concordance between the stimulus properties specified as relevant by the individual arranging a reinforcement contingency (behavior analyst, experimenter, teacher, etc.) and the stimulus properties that come to control the behavior of the organism (experimental subject, student, etc.) that experiences those contingencies. This paper summarizes the rationale for analyses of discrimination learning outcomes in terms of stimulus control topography coherence and briefly reviews some of the foundational studies that led to this perspective. We also suggest directions for future research, including pursuit of conceptual and methodological challenges to a complete stimulus control topography coherence analysis of processes involved in discriminated and generalized operants.
Article
Full-text available
The phenomenon identified as naming is a key stage of language function that is missing in many children with autism and other language delay diagnoses. We identified four children with autism, who, prior to the implementation of this experiment, did not have the naming repertoire (either speaker to listener or listener to speaker) and who had no tact responses for two- or three-dimensional stimuli. Tact training alone did not result in a naming repertoire or echoic-to-tact responses for these students. We then provided multiple exemplar instruction (MEI) across speaker and listener repertoires for a subset of stimuli (the teaching set) that resulted in untaught response components of naming and the capability to acquire naming after learning tacts for subsequent sets of stimuli. We used a delayed multiple-baseline probe design with stimuli counterbalanced across participants. The results showed that for all four students, mastery of tacts alone (the baseline or initial training condition) was not sufficient for the naming or echoic-to-tact repertoires to emerge. Following MEI the naming repertoire emerged for all four students for the initial set of stimuli. In addition, we tested for naming with novel stimuli that were probed prior to the MEI and naming also emerged following tact instruction alone for these sets. The results are discussed in terms of the role of naming in the incidental acquisition of verbal functions as part of the speaker-as-own-listener repertoire.
Article
Full-text available
We tested the effect of multiple exemplar instruction on the transfer of stimulus function for unfamiliar pictures across listener responses (i.e., matching and pointing) and speaker responses (i.e., pure tacts and impure tacts). Three preschool students, who were 3- and 4-year-old males and did not have the listener to speaker component of the naming repertoire, participated in the experiment. The dependent variable was numbers of correct responses to probe trials of both untaught listener responses ("point to__") and speaker responses (tact and impure tacts) following mastery of matching responses for two sets of five unfamiliar pictures (Set 1 and Set 3). After each participant mastered matching (e.g., "match Labrador") for Set 1 pictures they were probed on the three untaught responses to Set 1 words. That is, they were asked to point to Labrador, tact the picture of Labrador, and respond to the picture of a Labrador and the question "What is this?" Next, the participants were taught mastery of all four types of responses using MEI for a second set of five pictures (Set 2) and probed again on the 3 untaught Set 1 responses. Finally, matching responses were taught to mastery for a novel set of pictures (Set 3) and then probed on the three untaught responses. The results showed that untaught speaker responses emerged at 60% to 85% for two participants, and 40%-70% for one participant. We discuss the role of instructional history in the development of the listener to speaker component of naming.
Article
Full-text available
We tested the effect of multiple exemplar instruction (MEI) on acquisition of joint spelling responses, vocal to written and vice versa, for three sets of five words by four kindergarteners with language delays using a delayed multiple probe design. First, students were taught to spell Set 1 as either vocal or written responses (two vocal and two written) and probed on untaught responses. Next students were taught Set 2 using MEI (i.e., alternating responses) and again probed untaught responses for Set 1. Finally, Set 3 was taught in a single response and students were probed on untaught responses. Two students spelled none of Set 1 untaught responses before MEI, while two spelled the words at 60% accuracy or 10% accuracy. After MEI on Set 2, all students spelled untaught responses for Set 1 at 80% to 100% accuracy and Set 3 at 80% to 100% accuracy. The MEI resulted in joint stimulus function such that formerly independent responses came under the same stimulus control. We replicated these results with four other kindergartners with autism who performed academically above their typically developing peers. The results are discussed in terms of Skinner's treatment of the independence of the two verbal operants.
Chapter
This chapter focuses on the psychological, particularly cognitive, dimension of the evolution of consciousness. After introducing the concept of psychological development, I discuss some of the challenges in researching the evolution of consciousness from the psychological standpoint and point to the need for a transdisciplinary approach. I present an overview of child and adolescent cognitive development pointing to the limitations of Piaget’s model, and then introduce some evidence of widespread changes in thinking occurring across the knowledge sector over the last hundred years: megatrends of the mind. The purpose of the chapter is to create conceptual bridges between psychological development and the futures of education.
Article
In three experiments, 2- to 4-year-old children, following pretraining with everyday objects, were presented with arbitrary stimuli of differing shapes. In Experiment 1A, 9 subjects were trained one common tact response, "zag," to three of these and a second tact, "vek," to another three. In category match-to-sample Test 1, 4 subjects sorted accurately when required only to look at the sample before selecting from five comparisons. The remaining 5 subjects succeeded in Test 2, in which they were required to tact the sample before selecting comparisons. Experiment 1B showed, for 2 of these subjects, that tact training with 12 arbitrary stimuli established two six-member classes that were still intact 6 weeks later. In Experiment 2, 3 new subjects participated in a common tact training procedure that ensured that none of the exemplars from the same class were presented together prior to the test for three-member classes. Two subjects passed category Test I and the third passed Test 2. Tests showed subjects' listener behavior in response to hearing /zog/ and /vek/ to be in place. These experiments indicate that common naming is effective in establishing arbitrary stimulus classes and that category match-to-sample testing provides a robust measure of categorization.
Article
The present version is more than twice as long as the James Lectures and contains many changes made to conform with recent progress in the experimental analysis of behavior, human and otherwise. Although the emphasis is not upon experimental or statistical facts, the book is not theoretical in the usual sense. It makes no appeal to hypothetical explanatory entities. The ultimate aim is the prediction and control of verbal behavior. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)