Conference PaperPDF Available

Children and the Internet – A Preliminary Study in Uruguay

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Since 2007, almost every child in Uruguay has a laptop connected to the Internet. We investigated possible changes that such exposure to IT may have had on the children's ability to read, understand, search and analyze information. This paper reports the results of experiments carried out in 4 schools in and around Montevideo. The experiments consisted of children attempting to answer 'deep' questions in groups, children attempting to read beyond their expected levels in Spanish and English, and whether children would read better in groups than individually. The paper describes the design and limitations of these experiments, the results and their possible interpretation. It is suggested that children in groups can perform better at 'hard' problems than they can individually. It may be the opposite for 'easy' problems. It is suggested that the children studied in Uruguay are as good or better at reading than the corresponding standard recommended in the UK/USA. They are also shown to be capable of researching effectively using the Internet. The study raises several new research questions.
Content may be subject to copyright.
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Vol. 2 No. 15; August 2012
123
Children and the Internet A Preliminary Study in Uruguay
Sugata Mitra
Visiting Professor at MIT, Media Lab, Cambridge, MA, USA and also Professor at ECLS
Newcastle University
Newcastle upon Tyne, UK
Mabel Quiroga
Director of Log & Learn
Esmeralda 351 8B, Buenos Aires, Argentina
Abstract
Since 2007, almost every child in Uruguay has a laptop connected to the Internet. We investigated possible
changes that such exposure to IT may have had on the children’s ability to read, understand, search and analyze
information. This paper reports the results of experiments carried out in 4 schools in and around Montevideo. The
experiments consisted of children attempting to answer ‘deep’ questions in groups, children attempting to read
beyond their expected levels in Spanish and English, and whether children would read better in groups than
individually. The paper describes the design and limitations of these experiments, the results and their possible
interpretation. It is suggested that children in groups can perform better at hard’ problems than they can
individually. It may be the opposite for ‘easy’ problems. It is suggested that the children studied in Uruguay are
as good or better at reading than the corresponding standard recommended in the UK/USA. They are also shown
to be capable of researching effectively using the Internet. The study raises several new research questions.
1. Introduction
El Plan Ceibal is a national Uruguayan initiative to implement the “1 to 1” model to introduce Information and
Communication Technologies (ICT) in primary public education (http://www.ceibal.org.uy/). It has been in
operation since approximately 2007. As a result, almost every child in Uruguay’s primary schools has a laptop
and a free Internet connection, as do all primary school teachers.
We decided to investigate the effects these computers have had on the children’s reading comprehension,
information searching and information analysis skills.
2. Backgound
In the early days of eLearning, instructional material, mostly in the form of text available on a computer screen
was prevalent. It was supposed that individual learners would be capable of learning from this environment and,
indeed, that this would transform distance learning. Such misconceptions, and the resultant learning disasters,
persisted for years (Hara and Kling, 1999).
Collaborative and group learning has been researched since the 1970’s (see for example, Blumenfeld et. al. 1996).
Computer Supported Collaborative Learning (CSCL) is a relatively recent field of great interest. ‘The inclusion of
collaboration, computer mediation and distance education has problematized the very notion of learning and
called into question prevailing assumptions about how to study it.” (Stahl, Koschmann, & Suthers, 2006).
CSCL amongst children became possible only when computers became available to children and when Internet
bandwidths became useable for audio visual media and entertainment. Research on CSCL and children is
relatively recent (see, for example, Crook 1998). Regular interaction of children, computers and the Internet
brings in a new perspective to eLearning. The ‘Hole in the wall’ experiments (Mitra et.al, 2005; Mitra and Rana,
2001) consisted of computers embedded into walls of slums and villages and used by unsupervised groups of
children.
© Centre for Promoting Ideas, USA www.ijhssnet.com
124
These experiments showed that children in groups can learn to use computers and the Internet on their own, with
no prior knowledge and with none or very little literacy.
Subsequent experiments showed that such groups of unsupervised children can attain educational objectives by
themselves, in the absence of schools (Inamdar, 2006). Children have been shown to learn algebra by themselves
(Nicaud et al, 2004). Several instances of educational progress have been measured (Dangwal, 2005) and the
process by which such unsupervised and self organised learning happens, have been described (Dangwal and
Kapur, 2008).
This body of research seems to show that children in unsupervised groups can:
Learn to use computers and the Internet on their own
Use the Internet to search for information, read, understand and evaluate what they have found
Answer questions about curricular subjects they have not been taught
Compensate for inadequacies in the quality of school teaching (Mitra et al, 2008)
Based on these results, it is reasonable to expect significant changes in the children of Uruguay.
In what follows we describe a set of experiments to evaluate, or at least detect, these changes.
3. Experimental design and limitations
Due to limitations in time and finances, the experiments were planned over a three day period (in June 2011) in
four schools in and around Montevideo. We, therefore, do not have data from the rest of Uruguay. 78 children,
between 9 and 11 years old (average age 10, all from grade5), from the four schools participated in the study. This
sample size, though not large, does give us an indication of the direction in which future measurements need to be
made.
The experiments consisted of the following:
1. Children would form self organized learning environments (SOLEs) and find answers to questions posed
by us. A SOLE consists of a learning space, such as a classroom with the furniture rearranged to enable
groups of 3-5 children to interact with a computer and the Internet. They are given a question to
research. Children select their own groups, can change groups if they wish to, can talk to each other and
across groups. Groups can look at each other’s work. At the end of a given time period, usually between
30 and 45 minutes, each group would make a presentation on their findings.
2. Children would read and answer questions from reading materials at the USA grades 1, 3 and 5. Other
grades were not selected due to constraints of time. We chose tests from the United States for the US
system’s grades 1, 3 and 5. They were selected from www.superteacherworksheets.com.
3. The reading exercises would be carried out by children individually and also in groups.
4. The reading exercises would be carried out in Spanish as well as English. We used the English texts and
questions translated into Spanish for this purpose.
Ideally, each child should have gone through each of the 4 experiments above. However, this was not feasible due
to time, suitability and time-tabling problems. We then decided to conduct one or more of the experiments where
possible.
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Vol. 2 No. 15; August 2012
125
Photo 1. A group in a SOLE
4. Schools and experiments
Primary schools in Uruguay are identified by number. Our experiments were carried out in schools numbered 28,
31, 70 and 136. These schools are nearly identical in size and the demographics of the populations they serve.
This is useful in our context where not all the experiments can be carried out in all the schools. Since the schools
are similar, it would be reasonable to expect that the results of an experiment carried out in one school would be
similar to what we would have obtained if we had carried it out in the others.
The schools we had selected serve children from middle class backgrounds, with working parent(s). While the
homes where the children come from are not poor, they do have severe constraints on finance.
School 31:
This is the first school where the experiments were conducted. The first experiment consisted of the question,
‘How did the world begin? How will it end?’. The children were at this point learning about the solar system. The
experiment was conducted in a SOLE format (Mitra, 2009). This consisted of the children forming groups of 3, 4
or 5 of their choice. Each group then researched the question using one computer, to ensure that discussions take
place. The children are also allowed to talk to other groups and look at other’s work (see
http://sugatam.wikispaces.com/Papers+and+Articles, ‘Method ELSE for schools where children teach
themselves’). At the end of 30 minutes, the groups make a presentation of their findings. One child is elected by
the children as the ‘policeman’ to maintain law and order and generally help everybody.
In school 31, the children’s reports ranged from the Big Bang Theory to the ultimate demise of the Earth when the
Sun expands to engulf it. They had clear comprehension and had rejected alternative theories based on theology
and other schools of thought.
© Centre for Promoting Ideas, USA www.ijhssnet.com
126
After this they were given a grade 5 level text to read in groups. Of the 26 children present 13 were given 4 copies
of the text in English, while the other 13 were given 4 copies of the same text in Spanish. The English and
Spanish groups were separated so they could not interact.
School 28:
Here 19 children constructed a SOLE of 5 groups and researched the question, ‘Why do we slip on a wet
pavement but not on a dry one?’. After 30 minutes the groups reported their findings. The children’s answers
covered the basics of friction, Newton’s third law of action and reaction and they went on to electrostatic forces as
the cause of friction. They were fluent readers and clear speakers and seemed to have understood the concepts at
the elementary level, as reported by their teacher.
Next the children were given a grade 3 Spanish text to read, individually and answer the comprehension questions
at the end.
School 70:
Here 21 children constructed a SOLE of 5 groups and researched the question, ‘Why do we dream?’. After 30
minutes, the children’s research reports covered the neurology of dreams (the brain and neural activity continues
during sleep) and went on to the psychology of dreams including the work of Freud and Jung. They seemed to
have enjoyed this question and were willing to work on it for more time.
Next, 9 children worked in groups while the rest worked individually on a grade 1 level text in Spanish. They
answered questions on the text. Individuals handed in one answer sheet each while the groups handed in one
answer sheet per group.
School 136:
Here 26 children constructed a SOLE of 6 groups and researched the question, ‘How do we remember? Why do
we forget?’ After 30 minutes, the children’s reports ranged from neural network connections to the nature of
memory, long and short term memories and brain’s ability to categorize information by importance.
Next, 12 children formed 3 groups and were given a grade 3 text in Spanish, while the others were given the same
text individually. Each group handed in one answer sheet, while the individuals handed in one answer sheet each.
Photo2. Reading comprehension exercise
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Vol. 2 No. 15; August 2012
127
5. Results
The results are summarized in table 1.
School 28
School 31
School 70
Individual
Group
Individual
Group
Individual
Group
Group
Grade 1
text
Spanish
92.9
(n=12)
82.5
(n=9)
Grade 3
text
Spanish
48.3
(n=19)
82.3
(n=12)
Grade 5
text
Spanish
40
(n=13)
Grade 5
text
English
7.5
(n=13)
Table 1. All scores are out of 100. Each score reported here is the average for the answer sheets submitted for that
exercise
6. Discussion
It would have been ideal if we could have carried out all of the experiments in all of the schools studied.
However, this was not possible due to constraints of time and timetabling.
We could have conducted one single experiment in all the schools. However, our purpose was not to compare the
schools but to study the effect of computer use and collaborative work on reading comprehension. A single
experiment, with a single grade of text, would not have given us any idea of the performance across different
grades of texts. We would also not have been able to compare English and Spanish reading scores.
Keeping these difficulties in mind, we decided to administer different tests in each of the schools. For example,
grade 1 Spanish text was only tried in School 70, while grade 5 English text was only tried in School 31, and so
on.
6.1 The SOLE experience
In each of the schools, we noticed that the children had excellent searching skills. They were able to read web
pages in Spanish and were capable of summarizing the main findings. We have observed similar behavior in
schools in England, when children are allowed to research in groups and are used to the idea of doing so. Using
the SOLE approach would appear to be an effective method of self-organized instruction in Uruguay.
The children are confident of their ability to use computers and the Internet and would frequently solve each
other’s technical problems.
6.2 English
Children who cannot read English at all, were able to score 7.5% when attempting to read in groups. In a group of
4, when one child is unable to understand a word, others attempt to guess. Sometimes they would use English to
Spanish dictionaries. This raises an important research question: ‘Is reading comprehension in groups higher than
in the individuals comprising the group?’
When asked whether they liked working in groups with one computer, the children agreed enthusiastically. When
asked why, one child said, ‘Four brains are better than one’.
6.3 Reading comprehension in Spanish:
In school 28, the average individual score for reading a grade 3 text was 48.3%, while in school 31, when reading
a grade 5 text in groups, the average score was 40%.
© Centre for Promoting Ideas, USA www.ijhssnet.com
128
Once again, the data seems to hint at the fact that group reading comprehension is higher than individual. While
the schools where this data was collected were different, the demographics of the area and the children were
similar, as we have described in the design section above. We could not test school 28 for group reading
comprehension.
In school 70, the average of individual reading scores for a grade 1 text was 92.9, while the group reading score
for the same text was 82.5. This would seem to contradict the findings from the group scores in school 136. Upon
investigation we found that the children thought the grade 1 text was too easy for them. The groups became
listless and answered carelessly or at random. Individuals finished very quickly and went on to do other things.
This raises a research question, ‘Are individuals more effective at doing ‘easy’ tasks than groups?’ Could it then
be that the task of reading grade 5 text in school 31 was challenging enough for the groups to perform almost as
well (40%) as the individuals did with grade 3 text in school 28 (48.3%)?
One of us (SM) has worked on the SOLE methodology in England for several years. It is interesting to note that
when the group approach is explained to children, particularly that there is no competition at all (‘we will all try to
get to the answer together’) and they are asked ‘Would you like to try a hard question or an easy one?’, they often
say ‘very hard!’. However, when they are told to research silently and by themselves, they ask for an easy
question. There is fear of individual failure, which is generally absent in groups.
Does the difficulty level of the task given affect the performance of the groups in an inverse way (the harder the
task, the better the performance)? The depth with which the children researched the ‘hard’ questions in the SOLEs
in all the schools seems to indicate this. We could frame this as a research question, ‘Are groups of children able
to perform tasks that are beyond their current individual capabilities?’
In school 136, the average individual reading score was 72.7 while the average group score was 82.3, for a grade
3 text. Once again, given a moderately challenging task, the groups had performed better.
7. Conclusion
The ‘hole in the wall’ experiments of 1999-2004 (Mitra et al, 2006) continues to interest educators all over the
world. In these experiments, computers were embedded into walls of slums and villages of India for use by
unsupervised groups of children. It was reported that groups of children can learn to use computers and the
Internet on their own, irrespective of who or where they are, or even what language they know.
The data from Uruguay seems to indicate the reasons why the hole in the wall experiments produced the results
they did. At the time of the hole in the wall experiments, it was not possible to give each child a computer; laptops
were expensive and not common at all. These circumstantial reasons prompted children to work in groups and
figure out what was considered impossible at that time - computers, operating systems, applications and the
Internet. If children in groups have capabilities higher than their individual capabilities, as indicated by the data
from Uruguay, we have an explanation for the results of these earlier experiments.
Reading comprehension of children who participated in this study is clearly between grade 3 and grade 5. This is
close to key stage 3 in the British system (http://www.oup.com/oxed/pdf/GradeEquivalents.pdf ). The testing for
key stage 3 is done at age 14 in the UK (http://www.bbc.co.uk/schools/parents/national_curriculum_key_stages/ )
. The Uruguay children seem to have met, and perhaps exceeded, the UK standards.
Since there are no schools in Uruguay without computers, it is not possible to answer the question, ‘what would
have happened if schools did not have computers?
While, in the absence of control groups, it is impossible to say whether this achievement is due, at least in part, to
the laptops owned by the children, there is some indirect evidence that this may be the case. Measurements
conducted by the authors in northeastern England, with English speaking children, indicated that the children of
year 5 (10-11 year olds) were at a reading key stage of 2. These children are from a financially disadvantaged area
and have access to computers mostly in their school and not at home. This is lower than the observed scores in
Uruguay. This raises the question whether owning a computer and reading off its screen all the time will improve
reading comprehension in children.
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Vol. 2 No. 15; August 2012
129
We suggest on the basis of the above study that exposure to computers over the last several years has improved
the confidence levels, reading comprehension and information searching skills of the children of Uruguay.
Self organised learning amongst children could form the basis of new approaches to eLearning as well as open
and distance education in the future.
8. References
Blumenfeld, P., Marx,R., Soloway, E., Krajcik, J.(1996), Learning with Peers: From Small Group Cooperation
to Collaborative Communities, Educational Researcher Vol. 25, No. 8 (Nov., 1996), pp. 37-40
http://www.jstor.org/pss/1176492
Crook, C.(1998), Children as computer users: The case of collaborative learning, Computers Educ. Vol.30, No. 3
/ 4, pp237-247,
http://w3.nada.kth.se/kurser/kth/2D1624/PDF/Litteratur/children%20as%20computer%20users.pdf
Dangwal, R. & Kapur, P. (2008). Children's learning processes using unsupervised "hole in the wall" computers
in shared public spaces. Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 2008, 24, 3, 339-354.
Dangwal, R. (2005). Public computing, computer literacy and educational outcomes: children and computers in
rural India. In Looi C-K., Jonassen, D. & Ikeda, M. (Eds.) Towards sustainable and scalable educational
innovations informed by the learning sciences. Amsterdam, Berlin, Oxford, Tokyo $ Washington, DC:
IOS Press, 59-66.
Hara, N., Kling, R.(1999), Students' frustrations with a Web-based distance education course, First Monday,
Volume 4, Number 12
Inamdar, P. (2006). Computer skills development by children using hole in the wall computers in rural India.
Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 20, 3, 337-350
Jonesa,A., Issroffb, K.(2005), Learning technologies: Affective and social issues in computer-supported
collaborative learning, Computers & Education, Volume 44, Issue 4, May 2005, Pages 395-408,
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0360131504000570
Mitra, S. (2009). Remote presence: ‘beaming’ teachers where they cannot go. Journal of Emerging Technology
and Web Intelligence. 1, 1, 55-59.
Mitra, S., Dangwal, R. & Thadani, L. (2008). Effects of remoteness on the quality of education: a case study from
North Indian schools. Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 24, 2, 168-180.
Mitra, S., Dangwal, R., Chatterjee, S., Jha, S., Bisht, R.S. & Kapur, P. (2005). Acquisition of computer literacy on
shared public computers: Children and the “Hole in the wall”. Australasian Journal of Educational
Technology 2005, 21(3), 407-426.
Mitra, S. & Rana, V. (2001). Children and the Internet: experiments with minimally invasive education in India,
The British Journal of Educational Technology, 32, 2, pp 221-232
National Curriculum, UK. (2011)
http://www.direct.gov.uk/en/Parents/Schoolslearninganddevelopment/ExamsTestsAndTheCurriculum/D
G_4016665
Nicaud J.F., Bittar, M., Chaachoua, H., Inamdar, P. & Maffei, L. (2004). Experiments of Aplusix in four
countries, International Journal for Technology in Mathematics Education, 13, 2, 79-88.
Stahl, G., Koschmann, T., & Suthers, D. (2006). Computer-supported collaborative learning: An historical
perspective. In R. K. Sawyer (Ed.), Cambridge handbook of the learning sciences (pp. 409-426).
Cambridge, UK http://GerryStahl.net/cscl/CSCL_English.pdf
... In 1970 Illich stated that there was a need to "research on the possible use of technology to create institutions which serve personal, creative, and autonomous interaction" [18]. An example of this is Mitra's [21,22] Self-Organised Learning Environments (SOLEs), where the role of teachers shifts towards a facilitator of learning and the digital environment allows children to answer 'big questions'. This model has further progressed into the School in the Cloud (SitC), a network of SOLEs and 'grannies' that communicate with children over the internet to facilitate learning [9]. ...
... The social and material exclusion of people experiencing homelessness from mainstream education requires consideration of new models of learning and teaching [21,22], and new technologies to support learning networks and ecologies. Data collected in this study demonstrated the ways in which learning occurred and the variety of pathways taken. ...
... However, they can also replace food, are necessary for survival, let you forget your problems and are a reason for different communities of homeless to communicate and exchange information. When comparing this environment to Mitra's SOLEs [9,21,22] that occur mainly in schools, or since the development into the school in the cloud in specifically built learning environments, it becomes evident that there is no need for a designated teacher to pose the 'big question', and no need for specifically designed environments for learning to take place for this population. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Non-homeless youths outperform their homeless peers in school even if they live in extreme poverty. This disadvantage can have long-term consequences for engagement with and navigation of wider society. In this paper we examine how differences in achievement could be tackled outside of school through the re-envisioning of ecologies of digital education. Through interviews, design workshops, and a street visit with a total of 20 homeless young adults during a three-week engagement with a centre for people of low social stability in Bucharest, Romania, we examine the perceptions of education among street involved youth or adults. We identify the core values, aspirations, opportunities and barriers for education among these people, including survival, friendship, learning networks, and curiosity. These findings resulted in five implications for design: learning "happens", learning "works", designing for distanced learning, designing for the social politics of learning, and designing artefacts of everyday learning. These show the importance and necessity of educational reform in the field of HCI.
... In subsequent work (Dolan et al. 2013;Mitra and Crawley 2014;Mitra and Dangwal 2010;Mitra and Quiroga 2012), evidence of such learning resulted in the creation of ''selforganized learning environments'' (SOLEs). SOLEs can reproduce the environment of the hole in the wall inside schools or other enclosed locations and are widely in use by schools around the world. ...
... It is interesting to discuss why the rates of progress in this Bhutan study and in the older Indian experiments are so nearly identical. One of us (SM) has observed and documented unsupervised learning amongst groups of children using the Internet, in many different contexts and in many countries (Dolan et al. 2013;Mitra 2014;Mitra and Crawley 2014;Mitra and Dangwal 2010;Mitra and Quiroga 2012). Such learning is variously described as ''minimally invasive education'' and ''self-organized learning environments'' (SOLEs). ...
Article
Full-text available
This article describes a study under the Reaching the Unreached component of the Chiphen Rigpel project between the governments of Bhutan and India. This initiative is an attempt to provide computer literacy to children of Bhutan through setting up “hole in the wall” (HiWEL) Playground Learning Station(s) (PLSs). The study described here involves 14 sites (the experimental group), where PLSs are installed, and another 8 sites (the control group) without PLSs, but having similar demographic and socioeconomic profiling as the experimental group. This article compares the acquisition of computer literacy of school-going children in India with those in Bhutan. Results indicate that Bhutanese children have acquired computer literacy on their own and that their level of computer skills acquisition is similar to that of Indian children when the scores are normalized. This study was done over a one-year period, with results reported for eight months. In addition to verifying the earlier Indian results, this article hints at the fact that, irrespective of ethnicity, culture, and country, an identical—and possibly universal—self-organized learning mechanism seems to work with children vis-à-vis the Internet.
... It was previously known that SOLEs pedagogy could assist learners in learning ahead of their time [53,61,64]. However, it was poorly known that SOLEs pedagogy could improve all aspects of metacognitive skills, and the current study contributes to understanding the effect of SOLEs pedagogy on different scholastic constructs. ...
Article
Full-text available
The development of metacognitive skills should be holistic, which implies that it should focus on different (metacognitive skills) aspects. However, the literature indicates that metacognitive skills are often investigated in general terms with little focus on their aspects. In response to the established findings this far, the current study aimed to examine the effect of Self-organized learning environments (SOLEs) pedagogy on the different aspects of learners' metacognitive skills in Physical Sciences classrooms. The study employed the quasi-experimental design in which four (two urban and two rural) groups (155 participants) were involved. Data were gleaned using a metacognition-self assessment scale (MSAS) questionnaire and analysed using descriptive (means and effect size) and inferential (t-test) statistics. The results indicate that SOLEs pedagogy improved all aspects of metacognitive skills, but empathy towards others improved more than other aspects such as Respect shown to me, Respect shown to others, and Respect shown towards problem-solving. The current study concludes that SOLEs pedagogy can holistically develop metacognitive skills as it improves all aspects of metacognitive skills. In addition, SOLEs pedagogy makes learners empathetic toward each other during the learning process, which results in a conducive learning environment.
... If the software is more complex, a brief overview or introductory video, for example, can help establish some base skills. Essentially, the aim is that students have a chance to explore the new technology or applications themselves; they can learn with and from one another as they discover the possibilities and potential of new software (Mitra & Quiroga, 2012). ...
Chapter
This chapter explores the Bridge21 activity model designed to support the development of an innovative twenty-first-century learning environment in second-level schools. Over the past 10 years, the model has been developed, trialled and tested with over 14,000 students and over 2000 teachers, both in informal and formal educational scenarios. Research conducted at Trinity College Dublin has evaluated and underpinned the development of this learning model. The Bridge21 learning model is a pragmatic approach to team-based technology-mediated learning. It is designed to leverage current technology and to release students’ potential through a structured move away from individualised, teacher-led learning. Essential elements of the model include technology-mediated, project-based, teamwork and reflection. This chapter introduces the Bridge21 activity model and provides approaches and techniques to those who wish to design Bridge21 learning experiences. It empowers schools to build on what already works well for teachers and students while creating the space for innovative ideas and alternative approaches to teaching and learning. It presents a shift in focus from the teaching of individual subjects, to the teaching of key competencies and twenty-first-century learning skills.
... By implementing a SOLE activity, students should get motivated to study a variety of ideas and subjects [21], and the mentioned data seems to confirm that. Research has also shown that learning in unsupervised groups can make up for inadequate teaching in school [22] and a study in England showed that many students react to SOLE positively and they view it as different from what they perceive as a normal lesson [19]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The relation between learners’ metacognitive skills and conceptual understanding is often portrayed as an input-output relation when dealing with science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) and non-STEM disciplines. However, studies indicate that not all pedagogies yield positive correlations between learners’ metacognitive skills and conceptual understanding, particularly in the science (S) strand of STEM. On the other hand, it has been revealed that self-organised learning environments (SOLEs) pedagogy incorporates the characteristics of some of the learning models that were found to induce positive correlations between learners’ metacognitive skills and conceptual understanding in other non-STEM disciplines. Due to the lack of established research, particularly around the “S” in STEM, in the study reported on here we investigated the correlation between metacognitive skills and conceptual understanding under SOLEs pedagogy. We employed a quasi-experimental design which included 2 experimental and 2 control groups. Data were collected from 155 participants using the sciences (strand of STEM) performance test and metacognition self-assessment scale (MSAS) questionnaire. Data were statistically analysed using Pearson’s product-moment correlation coefficient (r), means (¯x), and t test. The results indicate a positive correlation between learners’ metacognitive skills and conceptual understanding of sciences when SOLEs pedagogy was employed.
Article
The “hole-in-the-wall” experiments of 1999, as named by the popular media, started with an Internet-connected computer being embedded in a wall facing a slum in Kalkaji, New Delhi, India. Several studies showed that groups of children, when given access to the Internet, can learn by themselves. Children’s academic marks improved, and their interest in learning new things increased, resulting in a significant decrease in school dropouts and increase in school attendance. Soft skills, such as confidence, communication, and self-regulation improved. This article traces the history of this experiment, the formation of “hole-in-the-wall education” (HiWEL) as an organization, technological and pedagogical advances, and how this simple idea is touching the lives of millions of children.
Article
Full-text available
We converge the disciplines of context-aware computing, human-computer interaction and pedagogic research practice to propose an agenda for the use of embedded sensing for novel learning spaces. In this case, embedded sensing is the identification and analysis of in-the moment individual, group and class level behavioural data from students engaged in physical learning activities. Our work is motivated by the challenges and opportunities for teachers inherent in the rise of the design, development and evaluation of novel learning spaces augmented with multidevice technology. We present a framework for the use of embedded sensing, its relationship related and emerging work in the fields of social learning analytics and smart learning, and a practical illustration of SOLE (Sugata Mitra’s Self Organised Learning Environments). Our agenda addresses the conceptualization, data collection, and analysis of learning; zooming in on hard-to-identify individual-within-group learning processes. For the educational researcher, we propose a context-sensitive, dynamic and situated approach that can inform analytic frameworks and development of tools for sense-making. For the teacher-inquirer, the smart teacher, we propose that this approach directly addresses issues linked to the complexity of the ‘what and how’ of education-based evaluation and assessment of students in unstructured and multidevice learning spaces more broadly.
Chapter
Many people are currently considering education in the 21st century as something quite different from what we have been used to. Some, like Jenkins (2002), consider that it is now outside the classroom and should not be ‘controlled’ in traditional ways. Others, such as Mitra et al. (2013), consider education as an ‘emergent’ phenomenon, something that will, with a little less help than usual, become a very effective self-organising structure. With the advent of two new intelligences as advocated by Gardner (1999-existential intelligence and naturalist intelligence), how could education in the closed system of a spaceship or extra-terrestrial settlement take into account new thinking and the connectedness of humanity in its acquisition and practice? How might teachers function in this environment and how much might depend on the computer? How might AI emerge as a powerful factor in the development of human beings carrying the future of the species into hostile environments? These questions and more are considered from the perspective of education—a human right? And a child’s freedom to make choices concerning her future, her education and a ‘career’, in terms different to those currently understood.
Article
Full-text available
span>Earlier work, often referred to as the "hole in the wall" experiments, has shown that groups of children can learn to use public computers on their own. This paper presents the method and results of an experiment conducted to investigate whether such unsupervised group learning in shared public spaces is universal. The experiment was conducted with "hole in the wall" (minimally invasive education, or MIE) computers in 17 locations in rural India. Focus groups in each location were tested for computer literacy for 9 months. Results, which are discussed in the paper, show that groups of children can learn to use computers and the Internet on their own, irrespective of who or where they are. Furthermore, such group self instruction is as effective as traditional classroom instruction, whilst this learning is considerably less expensive and is independent of teachers and schools. The results point to a new pedagogy for children's education in those circumstances where schools and teachers are either absent or not effective due to any reason.</p
Article
Full-text available
This paper studies the relationship between geographic remoteness and the quality of primary education in rural Northern India. It then discusses this relationship in the context of educational technology. There is a significant negative correlation between the quality of education and the distance of a school from the nearest urban centre. No correlation was seen between the quality of education and average number of students per teacher or per classroom. The authors suggest that teacher migration and desire for migration are directly responsible for poorer quality of education in remote areas. The role of technology, therefore, becomes vital in such areas. We propose that appropriate educational technology, if designed for that purpose, will be needed to improve the quality of education in non-urban regions where good teachers will not remain.
Article
Full-text available
The Aplusix system has been designed for helping students to learn algebra. Its capacity to tell the students whether their calculations are correct or not, to provide families of exercises of a chosen level, and to give scores after tests allows this system to be used in the regular functioning of the class. Its capacity to record the students' actions and to replay the students' work is a valuable feature for the researcher in mathematics education. In this paper, we describe four experiments conducted in four different countries with different goals: remediation piloted by researchers in Italy; remediation integrated in the regular functioning of classes in Brazil, collaborative learning in India, and learning and use during the entire school year in France. (Contains 4 tables and 5 figures.)
Conference Paper
Full-text available
This paper reports the research findings from a national research program conducted in rural India. In this research, children were provided unconditional access to public, outdoor computer. Evaluation was conducted on the children's ability to learn to operate the computer and the effect of such playground computing on educational outcome. The results suggest that playground computers might have an important role to play in improving the outcomes of elementary education and in imparting critical life skills.
Article
Earlier research by Mitra and colleagues on the use of computers by young children revealed that children are able to learn basic computing skills irrespective of their social, cultural, intellectual and religious backgrounds (Mitra & Rana, 2001). The present paper is an attempt to identify the varied aspects of a learning environment that impact upon the learning process enabled by "hole in the wall" computers. The study covers 250 children in the age group 8 to 14 years using qualitative and anecdotal evidence given by children and research consultants in the field. The evidence indicates that the environment in which the child learns is of importance for it brings together a host of different yet interrelated aspects of learning. The findings suggest a pedagogic tool for alternative methods to teaching in school settings, and strengthen the view that students should be engaged in a thinking curriculum, wherein everyone learns from everyone else, and no student is deprived of the opportunity for making contributions and appreciating the contributions of others.
Article
span>Earlier work often referred to as the "hole in the wall" experiments has shown that groups of children can learn to use public computers on their own. This paper presents the method and results of an experiment conducted to investigate whether such unsupervised group learning in shared public spaces can improve children's performance in school examinations. The experiment was conducted with "hole in the wall" (minimally invasive education, or MIE) kiosks in the rural Sindhudurg District of Maharashtra State, India. 103 children of the Grade 8 level, across 3 villages, were administered the curricular examination for 'Computer Science' for that grade. The results show that children who had learned at MIE kiosks were able to complete this curricular examination without being taught the subject. They scored only marginally lower than children who had been taught the 'Computers' curriculum in school throughout the school year. The results of this study throw new light on pedagogy for bridging the digital divide. It poses the question that similar learning may well be observed in whole or part in other subjects of the school curriculum.</p
Article
Urban children all over the world seem to acquire computing skills without adult intervention. Indeed this form of self-instruction has produced hackers?‘children who can penetrate high tech security systems. Is this kind of learning dependent only on the availability of technology? We provided slum children in New Delhi with Internet access in their settlement. The paper describes the results obtained in the first month of unsupervised and unguided access. It is observed that children seem to understand and use the technology fluently. Language and formal education do not seem to make any significant difference.
Article
This paper is concerned with affective issues in learning technologies in a collaborative context. Traditionally in learning there has been a division between cognition and affect: where cognition is concerned with skills and processes such as thinking and problem-solving and affect with emotional areas such as motivation, attitudes, feelings. Affective issues have been viewed as somewhat problematic in studying learning, so although it is well known that learner attitude, motivation, and emotional state are very important, they have often been excluded from the frame of research, or studied separately from cognitive learning. This position is gradually changing and this paper considers what previous research has been conducted in these areas. It discusses the role of affective factors in three main areas of collaboration: in settings where learners are co-located, in on-line communities and to support and develop socio-emotional skills. It considers relevant developments in these areas, what the outcomes have been and suggests important directions for future research.
Article
A modern enthusiasm for pupils to learn collaboratively within early education is identified. It is shown that educational practice is in harmony with theories of learning promoted by developmental psychologists and also with studies of classroom interventions evaluating cooperative learning regimes. However, observations of children's spontaneous interactions during routine small group work imply that the quality of collaboration is typically rather poor. This paper considers whether the cultivation of true collaborative learning is a realistic ambition with very young children. An analysis of the social dynamic at the heart of this form of learning suggests that it is well within the reach of children as a form of social exchange. However, it may be hard for them to exercise that dynamic under the particular formats demanded of schooled problem solving. It is argued that new technology offers a special potential for supporting the development of collaborative learning in early education. However, the scope of the resourcing required extends beyond meeting the traditional formats of circumscribed group work: designers for this style of learning need to address collaboration managed as a broader communal concern within the very fabric of educational settings.