ArticlePDF Available

Early Intervention for Toddlers With Language Delays: A Randomized Controlled Trial

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Early interventions for toddlers with expressive and receptive language delays have not resulted in positive expressive language outcomes. This randomized controlled trial tested the effects on language outcomes of a caregiver-implemented communication intervention targeting toddlers at risk for persistent language delays. Participants included 97 toddlers, who were between 24 and 42 months with language scores at least 1.33 SDs below the normative mean and no other developmental delays, and their caregivers. Toddlers were randomly assigned to the caregiver-implemented intervention or a usual-care control group. Caregivers and children participated in 28 sessions in which caregivers were taught to implement the intervention. The primary outcome was the Preschool Language Scale, Fourth Edition, a broad-based measure of language. Outcome measurement was not blinded. Caregivers in the intervention improved their use of all language facilitation strategies, such as matched turns (adjusted mean difference, intervention-control, 40; 95% confidence interval 34 to 46; P < .01). Children in the intervention group had significantly better receptive language skills (5.3; 95% confidence interval 0.15 to 10.4), but not broad-based expressive language skills (0.37, 95% confidence interval -4.5 to 5.3; P = .88). This trial provides preliminary evidence of the short-term effects of systematic caregiver instruction on caregiver use of language facilitation strategies and subsequent changes in children's language skills. Future research should investigate the ideal dosage levels for optimizing child outcomes and determine which language facilitation strategies are associated with specific child outcomes. Research on adaptations for families from culturally and linguistically diverse backgrounds is needed. Copyright © 2015 by the American Academy of Pediatrics.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Early Intervention for Toddlers With
Language Delays: A Randomized
Controlled Trial
Megan Y. Roberts, PhD, CCC-SLPa, Ann P. Kaiser, PhDb
abstract OBJECTIVE: Early interventions for toddlers with expressive and receptive language delays have
not resulted in positive expressive language outcomes. This randomized controlled trial tested
the effects on language outcomes of a caregiver-implemented communication intervention
targeting toddlers at risk for persistent language delays.
METHODS: Participants included 97 toddlers, who were between 24 and 42 months with language
scores at least 1.33 SDs below the normative mean and no other developmental delays, and their
caregivers. Toddlers were randomly assigned to the caregiver-implemented intervention or
a usual-care control group. Caregivers and childrenparticipatedin28sessionsinwhichcaregivers
were taught to implement the intervention. The primary outcome was the Preschool Language
Scale, Fourth Edition, a broad-based measure of language. Outcome measurement was not blinded.
RESULTS: Caregivers in the intervention improved their use of all language facilitation strategies,
such as matched turns (adjusted mean difference, intervention-control, 40; 95% condence
interval 34 to 46; P,.01). Children in the intervention group had signicantly better
receptive language skills (5.3; 95% condence interval 0.15 to 10.4), but not broad-based
expressive language skills (0.37, 95% condence interval 24.5 to 5.3; P= .88).
CONCLUSIONS: This trial provides preliminary evidence of the short-term effects of systematic
caregiver instruction on caregiver use of language facilitation strategies and subsequent
changes in childrens language skills. Future research should investigate the ideal dosage
levels for optimizing child outcomes and determine which language facilitation strategies are
associated with specic child outcomes. Research on adaptations for families from culturally
and linguistically diverse backgrounds is needed.
WHATS KNOWN ON THIS SUBJECT: Early
language delay is common in toddlers and is
associated with poor academic outcomes,
reading difculties, and persistent
communication problems. Despite these long-
term sequelae, few interventions for toddlers
with early language delays yield positive
expressive and receptive language results.
WHAT THIS STUDY ADDS: A28-sessionprogram
delivered over 3 months can enhance parent
language facilitation strategies. Unusually, the
small short-term benets were mainly in receptive,
rather than expressive, language and vocabulary.
Extended follow-up could determine the costs
versus benets of this promising approach.
aRoxelyn and Richard Pepper Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, School of Communication,
Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois; and bDepartment of Special Education, Peabody College, Vanderbilt
University, Nashville, Tennessee
Dr Roberts coordinated and supervised the data collection, carried out the analyses, and drafted
the initial manuscript; Dr Kaiser reviewed and revised the manuscript; and both authors
conceptualized and designed the study and approved the nal manuscript as submitted.
This trial has been registered at www.clinicaltrials.gov (identier NCT01975922) and What Works
Clearinghouse (identier R324A090181).
www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/peds.2014-2134
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2014-2134
Accepted for publication Jan 22, 2015
Address correspondence to Megan Y. Rober ts, PhD, CCC-SLP/L, The Roxelyn and Richard Pepper
Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, School of Communication, Northwestern
University, 2240 Campus Dr, Evanston, IL 60208. E-mail: megan.y.roberts@northwestern.edu
PEDIATRICS (ISSN Numbers: Print, 0031-4005; Online, 1098-4275).
Copyright © 2015 by the American Academy of Pediatrics
ARTICLE PEDIATRICS Volume 135, number 4, April 2015
by guest on September 13, 2017Downloaded from
Early language delay is common in
toddlers and is associated with
reading difculties,
1
persisting
communication problems,
2
and poor
school readiness.
3
Approximately
15% of 24-month-old children have
language delays that are not due to
any identiable etiology.
4
Although
the majority of toddlers with
expressive language delays are likely
to recover without intervention
(60%), toddlers with receptive and
expressive delays are substantially
less likely to recover spontaneously
(25%).
5
Despite this increased risk,
toddlers whose developmental delay
is restricted to language and who are
between 25% and 50% delayed in
that domain are not eligible to receive
early intervention services in half of
US states.
5
Only 3 intervention studies
examining children with receptive
and expressive language delays have
been published.
68
In the largest
randomized controlled trial of
a therapist-implemented intervention
for toddlers with language delays,
modest effects were found for
receptive language, and no effects
were found for expressive language.
6
However, the large range in intensity,
frequency, duration, and type of
therapy provided in the community-
based intervention limited the
interpretation of the results.
Caregiver-implemented interventions
for children with expressive and
receptive language delays have not
resulted in positive effects for
receptive and expressive language
skills.
7,8
The lack of effects in child
outcomes may be attributed to failure
to achieve differences in caregiver use
of language facilitation strategies
between intervention and control
groups
7
or the absence of systematic
caregiver instructional procedures
and measures of intervention
delity.
8
Given limitations in extant research,
the purpose of this study was to test
whether a 3-month caregiver-
implemented intervention targeting
toddlers at risk for persistent
language delays improved language
outcomes immediately after the end
of intervention. Identied evidence
gaps were addressed by targeting
toddlers with receptive and
expressive language delays, including
systematic caregiver instructional
procedures and measuring
caregiversuse of language facilitation
strategies continuously during
intervention to ensure that caregivers
were at high levels of delity.
We hypothesized that toddlers in the
intervention group, relative to
toddlers in the control group, would
have higher scores on (1)
standardized measures of expressive
and receptive language (primary
outcome), (2) standardized caregiver-
report and observational measures of
expressive vocabulary (secondary
outcome), and (3) standardized
measures of receptive vocabulary
(secondary outcome) immediately
after the end of the 3-month
intervention. We also hypothesized
that caregivers in the intervention
group would use more language
facilitation strategies than caregivers
in the control group (primary
outcome) and that the stress level of
caregivers in the intervention group
would not be greater than caregivers
in the control group (secondary
outcome) immediately after the end
of the 3-month intervention.
METHODS
Trial Design
The Working on Rapid Language
Development project was
a randomized controlled trial
(NCT01975922). The study was
conducted in Nashville, Tennessee.
The trial was approved by Vanderbilt
Universitys Institutional Review
Board (090904), and all caregivers
provided written informed consent.
Participants
Toddlers and their caregivers were
recruited through the Tennessee
Early Intervention System, local
pediatriciansofces, and
advertisements placed in the local
Nashville Parent. Recruitment
occurred continuously from October
2009 to October 2013. Toddlers were
eligible for the trial if they were
between 24 and 42 months of age and
their expressive and/or receptive
language scores were at least 1.33 SD
below the normative mean of 10 on
the Bayley Scales of Infant and
Toddler Development, Third Edition
9
(scaled score #6). This score
corresponds to an expressive
vocabulary of ,8 words for a
24-month-old. Exclusion criteria
included intellectual disability,
hearing loss .40 dB, a major medical
condition, or a diagnosis of autism
spectrum disorder. Caregivers were
informed immediately after the
eligibility assessment if their children
met the inclusion criteria.
Randomization
Eligible toddlers were randomly
assigned to intervention or usual-care
control arms with a 1:1 ratio by using
a computer-generated random
number sequence. The randomization
sequence was concealed from the
study coordinator, who enrolled
participants and assigned
participants to study arms. Random
assignment occurred immediately
after the eligibility assessment.
Intervention
Once randomly assigned, participants
could not be naïve to the trial arm.
Caregivers in the control group did
not receive the intervention.
Information about the type and
amount of community-based
language intervention was collected
for all participants and is provided in
Table 1. All children in both trial arms
who had not previously been
evaluated were referred to the
Tennessee Early Intervention System
to determine their eligibility for
community-based services. The
intervention (Enhanced Milieu
Teaching
10
) was designed to promote
early language acquisition in
PEDIATRICS Volume 135, number 4, April 2015 687
by guest on September 13, 2017Downloaded from
everyday interactions. The
intervention included 2 components:
caregiver instruction and child
intervention. First, the caregiver
received individual instruction by
using the teach-model-coach-review
method (described below) to learn
how to use specic language
facilitation strategies at home with
their toddlers. Second, the caregiver
used 6 language facilitation strategies
during intervention sessions and
throughout the day with their child.
A summary of these intervention
components is provided in Fig 1.
A complete description of the
intervention is available in the
manualized intervention protocol.
11
Caregivers received 28 individual
instructional sessions (4 workshops,
24 practice sessions). One clinic
session and 1 home session occurred
weekly for 3 months. Missed sessions
were rescheduled. The format of each
session was standardized and
included (1) review of the strategy
taught in the workshop, (2) modeling
of the strategy by the interventionist
with the child, (3) caregiver practice
of the strategy with their child while
the interventionist provided coaching,
and (4) review of the session.
Modeling and practice occurred in
play interactions and routines such as
reading a book or eating a meal.
A video example of practice with
coaching is provided in Fig 2. Sessions
lasted 1 hour.
Because early language delay is highly
variable at young ages and caregivers
vary in their use of language
facilitation strategies, the intervention
was individualized in 2 ways. First,
specic language targets were chosen
for each child based on performance
during the baseline assessments. All
toddlers had either (1) single word
targets if they used ,50 total words
and 10 verbs during baseline or (2)
early word combination targets if they
used .50 total words but were not
combining words regularly. Second,
caregivers were taught the language
facilitation strategies in sequential
order. Performance was measured and
instruction continued to criterion
performance levels established for
each strategy. A new strategy was
taught after the caregiver reached
criterion levels on the previous
strategy. Criterion levels are provided
in Fig 1.
Trained interventionists provided the
caregiver instruction. The 2
interventionists received 30 hours of
individual instruction from M.Y.R. and
practiced implementing the child
intervention and the caregiver
instruction over a 6-month period
before beginning the intervention.
Fidelity of the implementation of the
intervention delivered by the
interventionists was recorded for
20% of sessions. The average delity
was 94% and exceeded 90% for all
intervention components across all
sessions.
Outcomes
Table 2 shows the primary and
secondary caregiver and child
outcomes. All outcome data were
collected at baseline and immediately
after the end of intervention
(3 months). Outcomes were
assessed in a clinic by a speech
language pathologist or a special
education teacher trained to delity.
These assessors were not naïve to the
trial arm; because of the behavioral
nature of the intervention, caregivers
knew the trial arm to which they
were assigned and made comments
during assessments that indicated
their trial arm assignment. To reduce
the potential for bias, procedural
delity was completed for 20% of all
assessments. In addition, all norm-
referenced assessments were scored
by 2 raters. Procedural delity and
scoring agreement exceeded 95% for
all assessments and did not vary by
trial arm, suggesting that the
potential for bias was low.
Caregiversuse of language
facilitation strategies was measured
during a 20-minute play-based
caregiverchild interaction in which
the caregiver and child played with
a standard set of toys in the clinic.
Interactions were coded for the
caregiver strategies listed and dened
TABLE 1 Participant Characteristics at Baseline
Variable Trial Arm
Intervention Control
n45 52
Age, months 30.3 (5.0) 30.6 (5.1)
Receiving additional speech therapy 20 14
Male 82 80
Race
African American 18 13
White 78 85
Other 4 2
Having receptive and expressive delay 69 72
Income 71 000 (35 000) 60 000 (52 000)
Mothers education
High school 39 44
Undergraduate degree 37 27
Graduate degree 24 29
Baseline scores
Cognitive skills, BSID-3
9
91.3 (8.4) 88.6 (7.6)
Expressive language, PLS-4
12
75.2 (7.9) 75.0 (7.2)
Receptive language, PLS-4
12
76.5 (17.3) 73.8 (15.2)
Expressive vocabulary, EOWPVT-3
13
60.9 (11.5) 59.8 (10.7)
Expressive vocabulary, NDW 19.0 (17.9) 17.2 (17.5)
Expressive vocabulary, MCDI
14
92.1 (105.1) 94.4 (97.2)
Data are expressed as mean (SD) or %. BSID-3, Bayley Scales of Infant Development, Third Edition; PLS-4, Preschool
Language Scale, Fourth Edition; EOWPVT-3, Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test, Third Edition; NDW, number of
different word roots in a 20-minute play interaction; MCDI, MacArthur Bates Communication Development Inventories.
688 ROBERTS and KAISER
by guest on September 13, 2017Downloaded from
in Table 2. Reliability was calculated
for 20% of all interactions, and point-
by-point interobserver agreement
exceeded 90% for each caregiver
behavior. Caregivers in the
intervention arm completed
a questionnaire related to the
structure and content of the
intervention and their perception of
the effect of the intervention on their
childrens communication skills.
Sample Size
We anticipated that 120 toddlers
would be eligible for the trial with
10% attrition. Assuming a baseline
covariate of 0.70 between the
primary outcome measure at baseline
and at the end of intervention,
a difference of 0.43 SD could be
detected with 90 toddlers (45 in each
arm) with 80% power at the 5%
signicance level.
Statistical Analyses
An intent-to-treat analysis was used
to analyze data in both randomization
arms. Mean outcomes were compared
between arms using linear regression
adjusted for the baseline measure of
the outcome variable, family income,
and child cognitive skills at baseline.
Differences in the rate of language
delay were compared by using x
2
analysis.
RESULTS
Figure 3 summarizes the ow of
participants through each stage of the
trial. Table 1 summarizes the
participant characteristics. Baseline
differences in group characteristics
were not large but generally favored
the intervention arm. As such, nal
analyses were adjusted for baseline
characteristics. Of the 97 eligible
toddlers, 45 were assigned to the
intervention group and 52 to the
control group. All toddlers in the
intervention group (n= 45) and 83%
of toddlers in the control group (n=
43) were retained after the 3-month
intervention period.
Child Outcomes
The analyses provided evidence for
improvement in all measures of
receptive language. The differences in
receptive language between the
FIGURE 1
Pictorial design of the Working on Rapid Language Development trial. PLS-4, Preschool Language Scale, Fourth Edition; EOWPVT-3, Expressive One-Word
Picture Vocabulary Test, Third Edition; PPVT, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Fourth Edition; NDW, number of different words in a 20-minute play
interaction; MCDI, MacArthur Bates Communication Development Inventories.
PEDIATRICS Volume 135, number 4, April 2015 689
by guest on September 13, 2017Downloaded from
intervention and control arms were
small, with effect sizes ranging from
0.27 to 0.35 (P= .04). Differences in
expressive language skills were not
consistent across measures. There
were only positive effects for the
observational measure of expressive
vocabulary (P= .01, effect size 0.38).
No positive effects for overall
expressive language skills, parent
report of expressive vocabulary, or
norm-referenced expressive
vocabulary were found. x
2
Analyses
indicated that toddlers in the
intervention arm were less likely to
meet the criterion for language delay
after intervention than toddlers in the
control arm (x
2
= 3.8, P= .05). After
the 3-month intervention period,
71% of toddlers in the control arm
were delayed, compared with 51% of
toddlers in the intervention arm.
Toddlers who did not receive
intervention had a 1.4 times greater
risk of having a language delay than
toddlers who received intervention.
Caregiver Outcomes
Table 3 shows that caregivers in the
intervention arm improved their use
of all language facilitation strategies.
The differences in caregiver use of
language facilitation strategies
between the intervention and control
arms were large, with effect sizes
ranging from 0.43 to 3.19 (P,.01).
There were no differences in the
amount of total stress reported by
caregivers in the intervention and
control arms (P= .15, effect size
20.16).
Intervention Evaluation
There were no known adverse events
or side effects for those children
enrolled in the intervention group.
The majority of caregivers (98%)
reported that the strategies helped
their childrens language skills. When
asked how comfortable they felt using
the strategies, 92% of caregivers
reported that they were very
comfortable and 8% were somewhat
comfortable. Caregivers reported
using the strategies an average of
17 hours per week (range 277 hours,
TABLE 2 Primary and Secondary Outcome Measures for the Working on Rapid Language Development Trial
Outcome Measure Description
Primary
Receptive and expressive language Preschool Language Scale, Fourth Edition
12
Expressive score: Expressive Communication subscale;
Receptive score: Auditory Comprehension subscale; both
yield a standard score of 100 (SD 15)
Caregiver use of strategies Observational coding of a 20-min play-based
caregiver-child interaction
Matched turns: percentage of adult utterances in response to
a childs utterance; Responsiveness: percentage of child
utterances to which the adult responded; Targets:
percentage of adult utterances that contained the childs
language target; Expansions: percentage of child utterances
to which the adult imitates and adds a word; Time Delays:
percentage of nonverbal prompting sequences that were
used correctly; Prompting: percentage of verbal prompting
sequences that were used correctly
Secondary
Caregiver stress Parenting Stress Index, Fourth Edition
15
Total Stress score
Receptive vocabulary Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Fourth Edition
16
Standard score with a normative mean of 100 (SD 15)
Expressive vocabulary MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development
Inventories: Words and Sentences
14
Total number of 680 words the caregiver reports that the
child says
Number of Different Word Roots Total number of different word roots the child says in a 20-min
play interaction with a research assistant
Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test,
Third Edition
13
Standard score with a normative mean of 100 (SD 15)
Presence of language delay Preschool Language Scale, Fourth Edition
12
Total standard score ,85
FIGURE 2
Video example of a parent practicing responding to and expanding child communication with
coaching from a therapist (Supplemental Video).
690 ROBERTS and KAISER
by guest on September 13, 2017Downloaded from
SD 18). The majority of caregivers
(98%) reported teaching the
language facilitation strategies to
another caregiver. The majority of
caregivers (98%) reported that they
preferred the home and clinic
location to home only or clinic only.
Caregivers ranked the strategies they
found most helpful in the following
order: (1) responsiveness, (2)
matched turns, (3) expansions, (4)
targets, (5) prompting, and (6) time
delays. Caregivers reported that
observing the interventionist model
the strategies and practicing the
strategies with coaching from the
interventionist were the most helpful
instructional methods. The average
cost of intervention was $3861 per
child, with a range of $2767 to $5653.
Intervention costs included travel
costs ($0.55/mile) and therapist
salary ($65/hour) for time spent in
instruction, traveling, preparing for,
and delivering the intervention
sessions.
DISCUSSION
Main Findings
This is the rst trial to include
specic criterion levels for caregiver
use of language facilitation strategies
and to continuously monitor
caregiver use of these strategies. High
delity in caregiver use of language
facilitation strategies resulted in
FIGURE 3
Participant ow chart for the Working on Rapid Language Development trial.
TABLE 3 Outcome Comparisons
Outcome Mean (SD) for Trial Arms Adjusted
Intervention
(n= 45)
Control
(n= 43)
Mean Difference
(Intervention 2Control)
95% Condence
Interval
PEffect Size
Primary child outcomes
Expressive language, PLS-4
12
84.0 (13.9) 80.2 (12.0) 0.37 24.5 to 5.3 .88 0.03
Receptive language, PLS-4
12
86.3 (19.4) 77.3 (20.4) 5.3 0.15 to 10.4 .04 0.27
Secondary child outcomes: Expressive Vocabulary
MCDI
14
263.7 (172.6) 214.5 (146.3) 32.8 217.3 to 83.0 .20 0.21
NDW 54.9 (30.2) 38.0 (30.3) 11.4 2.5 to 20.4 .01 0.38
EOWPVT-3
13
75.7 (16.3) 70.0 (17.7) 3.5 24.2 to 11.12 .40 0.21
Secondary child outcomes: Receptive Vocabulary
PPVT-4
16
94.3 (13.6) 85.6 (16.7) 5.3 0.4 to 10.5 .04 0.35
Primary caregiver outcomes, %
Matched turns 74 (13) 32 (15) 40 34 to 46 ,.01 2.86
Responsiveness 85 (9) 80 (14) 5 1 to 10 .05 0.43
Targets 47 (21) 3 (4) 39 33 to 45 ,.01 2.55
Expansions 42 (18) 4 (4) 42 35 to 48 ,.01 3.19
Time delays 42 (38) 0 (0) 29 18 to 40 ,.01 1.07
Prompting 50 (38) 3 (9) 35 17 to 53 ,.01 1.25
Secondary caregiver outcomes
Stress, PSI
15
194.2 (43.4) 216.6 (38.2) 26.7 216 to 2.6 .15 20.16
Effect size calculated by dividing the adjusted mean difference by the pooled SD of the intervention and control arms. Adjusted analyses include baseline scores, household income, and child
cognitive scores as covariates. PLS-4, Preschool Language Scale, Fourth Edition; MCDI, MacArthur Bates Communication Development Inventories; NDW, number of different words in a 20-minute
play interaction; EOWPVT-3, Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test, Third Edition; PPVT, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Fourth Edition; PSI, Parenting Stress Index, Fourth Edition.
PEDIATRICS Volume 135, number 4, April 2015 691
by guest on September 13, 2017Downloaded from
small benets to receptive language
but not to the primary expressive
language outcome, although 1 of the 3
expressive vocabulary measures did
seem to benet. These results differ
from previous studies,
7,8
which did
not report positive expressive
language outcomes. Toddlers in the
intervention arm used 11 more
different words in a 20-minute
structured play interaction than
toddlers in the control arm. However,
differences between the intervention
and control arms were not observed
for other measures of expressive
language skills. Given the relatively
brief nature of the intervention
(3 months), it is not surprising that
only changes in the proximal measures
were evident immediately after the
end of intervention. Over time,
changes in these proximal measures
(number of different words in the
language sample) may result in
changes in the distal measures
(norm-referenced tests) of expressive
language. These positive outcomes
were achieved during 28 intervention
sessions over the course of the 3-
month intervention with a total
delivery cost of $3861 per family.
This cost and limited duration of
intervention are relatively modest
given the promising results.
Strengths
The components of the intervention
were naturalistic language facilitation
strategies similar to those commonly
used in speech-language therapy and
early intervention. Caregivers rated
the intervention strategies as easy to
use throughout the day. The
intervention approach was
individualized to accommodate
different rates of caregiver learning
and child targets, yet standardized
sufciently that all caregivers and
children received the same
intervention. The length and frequency
of the intervention were compatible
with community-based early
intervention service delivery models.
Unique features of this trial include
systematic and continuous monitoring
of caregiver use of language facilitation
strategies, inclusion of children with
receptive and expressive language
delays, and a standardized protocol for
teaching caregivers.
The research design included random
assignment, low attrition, and careful
and continuous monitoring of
caregiver use of the language
facilitation strategies. Intervention
delity was measured for the
therapist and for the caregiver,
ensuring a high level of quality
control. The manualized intervention
program included a replicable yet
individualized protocol. Taken
together, these methodological
strengths indicate high internal and
external validity, increasing the
likelihood that these positive results
will generalize to similar samples.
Limitations
The current ndings should be
considered in the context of several
limitations. First, the long-term
outcomes remain unknown. Longer-
term outcome measures may produce
different results. Follow-up is ongoing
and will provide additional information
about spontaneous recovery rates and
maintenance of language facilitation
strategies by caregivers. Second, the
majority of participants were from
mainstream US cultures. Thus, it is
unclear whether the results of this
study would generalize to caregivers
from other cultures with different
beliefs about child-rearing practices.
14
Third, participant recruitment took
place over 4 years and included
a variety of sources (self-referral,
pediatrician referral, early intervention
referral). It is possible that families
who chose to participate in a research
study may have been more motivated
than families from the general
population. Fourth, because assessors
and caregivers could not remain naïve
to the trial arm, the potential for bias
exists.
Interpretation
The instruction to caregivers had the
greatest effects on matched turns,
targets, and expansions. These
language facilitation strategies in
turn had the largest impact on
receptive language skills and
expressive vocabulary. Results of this
study have several important clinical
implications. Children with
expressive and receptive language
delays are likely to benet from early
intervention. This is the second trial
to nd positive effects on expressive
vocabulary outcomes for toddlers
with receptive and expressive
language delays.
6
Results from the
current trial indicate that the effects
of early intervention may be
maximized when the caregiver is
included in the intervention.
Teaching caregivers to use language
facilitation strategies is a cost-
effective way to provide early
intervention to this population.
However, teaching caregivers is
a complex process that requires
ongoing monitoring and coaching.
The current trial is the rst step in
investigating effective interventions
for a population of children with
language delays who are at risk for
persistent communication
difculties.
CONCLUSIONS
Given the long-term negative
academic and social effects of
persistent communication difculties,
the outcomes of this trial provide
preliminary support for early
intervention for toddlers with
language delays. This trial provides
rigorous evidence of the effects of
systematic instruction to caregivers
on their use of language facilitation
strategies and subsequent changes in
their childrens receptive and
expressive language skills. The
outcomes are particularly promising
given the modest cost and limited
duration of the intervention. Long-
term follow-up of intervention effects
is ongoing and will assess the long-
term impact of early intervention on
child language skills. Whereas
caregiver use of language facilitation
692 ROBERTS and KAISER
by guest on September 13, 2017Downloaded from
strategies resulted in positive
receptive language and some
expressive vocabulary outcomes, the
amount and total dosage of caregiver
strategy use needed to optimize
language outcomes remains
unknown. Future research should
investigate specic caregiver
language facilitation strategies and
the amount of caregiver strategy use
that maximizes child language
outcomes.
FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no nancial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.
FUNDING: Supported by grant R324A090181 from the Institute of Education Sciences and UL1 TR000445 from the National Center for Advancing Translational
Sciences, NIH. Funded by the National Institutes of Health (NIH).
POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conicts of interest to disclose.
REFERENCES
1. Snowling M, Bishop DVM, Stothard SE. Is
preschool language impairment a risk
factor for dyslexia in adolescence?
J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2000;41(5):
587600
2. Preston JL, Frost SJ, Mencl WE, et al.
Early and late talkers: school-age
language, literacy and neurolinguistic
differences. Brain. 2010;133(pt 8):
21852195
3. Prior M, Bavin E, Ong B. Predictors of
school readiness in ve- to six-year-old
children from an Australian longitudinal
community sample. Educ Psychol. 2011;
31(1):316
4. Horwitz SM, Irwin JR, Briggs-Gowan MJ,
Bosson Heenan JM, Mendoza J, Carter
AS. Language delay in a community
cohort of young children. J Am Acad
Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2003;42(8):
932940
5. Law J, Boyle J, Harris F, Harkness A, Nye
C. Prevalence and natural history of
primary speech and language delay:
ndings from a systematic review of the
literature. Int J Lang Commun Disord.
2000;35(2):165188
6. Glogowska M, Roulstone S, Enderby P,
Peters TJ. Randomised controlled trial of
community based speech and language
therapy in preschool children. BMJ. 2000;
321(7266):923926
7. Law J, Kot A, Barnett G. A Comparison of
Two Methods of Providing Intervention to
Three Year Old Children With Expressive/
Receptive Language Impairment. London,
UK: City University London; 1999
8. Wake M, Tobin S, Girolametto L, et al.
Outcomes of population based language
promotion for slow to talk toddlers at
ages 2 and 3 years: Lets Learn Language
cluster randomised controlled trial. BMJ.
2011;343:d4741
9. Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant
Development III. San Antonio, TX:
Psychological Corporation; 2005
10. Kaiser AP. Parent-implemented language
intervention: an environmental system
perspective. In: Kaiser A, Gray D, eds.
Enhancing Childrens Communication:
Research Foundations for Intervention.
Baltimore, MD: P.H. Brookes; 1993:6384
11. Kaiser AP, Roberts MY. Parents as
communication partners: an evidence-
based strategy for improving parent
support for language and
communication in everyday settings.
Perspect Lang Learn Educ. 2013;20(3):
96111
12. Zimmerman IL, Steiner VG, Pond RE.
Preschool Language Scale. 4th ed. San
Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp.; 2002
13. Brownwell R. Expressive One-Word
Picture Vocabulary Test. 3rd ed. Novato,
CA: Academic Therapy Publications; 2000
14. Fenson L, Marchman V, Thal D, Dale P,
Reznick S, Bates E. MacArthur-Bates
Communicative Development Inventories.
Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes; 2007
15. Abidin RR. Parenting Stress Index.
Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment
Resources; 1995
16. Dunn LM, Dunn DM. Peabody Picture
Vocabulary Test. 4th ed. Minneapolis,
MN: Pearson Assessments; 2007
PEDIATRICS Volume 135, number 4, April 2015 693
by guest on September 13, 2017Downloaded from
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2014-2134
; originally published online March 2, 2015; 2015;135;686Pediatrics Megan Y. Roberts and Ann P. Kaiser
Controlled Trial
Early Intervention for Toddlers With Language Delays: A Randomized
Services
Updated Information & /content/135/4/686.full.html
including high resolution figures, can be found at:
Supplementary Material
html
/content/suppl/2015/03/25/peds.2014-2134.DCSupplemental.
Supplementary material can be found at:
References
/content/135/4/686.full.html#ref-list-1
at:
This article cites 8 articles, 2 of which can be accessed free
Subspecialty Collections
/cgi/collection/cognition:language:learning_disorders_sub
Cognition/Language/Learning Disorders /cgi/collection/development:behavioral_issues_sub
Developmental/Behavioral Pediatrics
the following collection(s):
This article, along with others on similar topics, appears in
Permissions & Licensing
/site/misc/Permissions.xhtml
tables) or in its entirety can be found online at:
Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures,
Reprints /site/misc/reprints.xhtml
Information about ordering reprints can be found online:
rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.
Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 2015 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All
and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elk
publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned, published,
PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly
by guest on September 13, 2017Downloaded from
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2014-2134
; originally published online March 2, 2015; 2015;135;686Pediatrics Megan Y. Roberts and Ann P. Kaiser
Controlled Trial
Early Intervention for Toddlers With Language Delays: A Randomized
/content/135/4/686.full.html
located on the World Wide Web at:
The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is
of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.
Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 2015 by the American Academy
published, and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point
publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned,
PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly
by guest on September 13, 2017Downloaded from
... Early delays should thus be closely monitored, and appropriate measures taken in due time. Here, parentand child-centered early interventions have been shown to be effective [59][60][61]. Future research will have to extend the current work by more closely studying developmental language disorders in multilingual children as well as further evaluating the role of cognitive abilities. Independent of the classification of cognitive abilities as co-occurring or contributing conditions to language disorders, all children with diagnosed developmental language disorders need to receive therapy tailored to their nonverbal cognitive performance levels. ...
Article
Full-text available
In recent years, there have been intense international discussions about the definition and terminology of language disorders in childhood, such as those sparked by the publications of the CATALISE consortium. To address this ongoing debate, a Delphi study was conducted in German-speaking countries. This study consisted of three survey waves and involved over 400 experts from relevant disciplines. As a result, a far-reaching consensus was achieved on essential definition criteria and terminology, presented in 23 statements. The German term ‘Sprachentwicklungsstörung’ was endorsed to refer to children with significant deviations from typical language development that can negatively impact social interactions, educational progress, and/or social participation and do not occur together with a potentially contributing impairment. A significant deviation from typical language development was defined as a child’s scores in standardized test procedures being ≥ 1.5 SD below the mean for children of the same age. The results of this Delphi study provide a proposal for a uniform use of terminology for language disorders in childhood in German-speaking countries.
... Efektivitas intervensi EMT pertama kali dibuktikan oleh Hemmeter & Kaiser (1994) pada anak yang mengalami keterlambatan perkembangan dan A. P. Kaiser & Hester (1994) pada anak yang mengalami keterlambatan bahasa. Seiring dengan perkembangan, EMT juga mulai banyak digunakan pada kasus lainnya seperti autism spectrum disorder (Kasari et al., 2014;Kim et al., 2020;Logan et al., 2023), specific language impairment (Roberts & Kaiser, 2015), dan keterlambatan bahasa pada anak dengan SES rendah (Hatcher & Page, 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Enhanced Milieu Teaching (EMT) merupakan salah satu intervensi yang dapat meningkatkan keterampilan dalam mengekspresikan bahasa verbal pada anak usia dini. Penelitian ini merupakan studi kasus tunggal yang bertujuan untuk menguji efektivitas intervensi EMT terhadap keterampilan bahasa anak dengan gangguan perkembangan bahasa. Partisipannya ialah seorang anak berusia 4 tahun yang mengalami gangguan perkembangan bahasa. Program intervensi EMT diberikan selama dua bulan. Hasil dari analisis visual yang dilakukan menunjukkan terdapat peningkatan keterampilan partisipan dalam mengekspresikan bahasa dan perluasan panjang kalimat yang diucapkan.
... Für Österreich liegen keine Evaluierungen vor. Effizienzstudien insbesondere für frühe und durch Eltern umgesetzte Sprachfördermaßnahmen belegen übereinstimmend ihre Wirksamkeit [11][12][13]. Zudem gibt es vielfältige Hinweise auf ein hohes Kosten-Nutzen-Verhältnis früher Intervention [14]. Nicht zuletzt erlauben Sprachscreenings häufig die Erkennung nichtsichtbarer Entwicklungsstörungen (z. ...
Article
Full-text available
Zusammenfassung Sprachliche Entwicklungsverläufe stehen in deutlichem Zusammenhang mit der Entwicklung von sozialen Kompetenzen, Beziehungen mit Gleichaltrigen, psychischer Gesundheit, schulischem Lernen und beruflichen Chancen. Eine frühe Erkennung von erhöhtem Risiko für Sprachentwicklungsprobleme erlaubt eine gezielte weitere Abklärung und eine abgestufte Intervention. Die Umsetzung von Sprachförderstrategien in der Familie ist effektiv, in weiterer Folge kann sprachtherapeutische Unterstützung erforderlich sein. In Oberösterreich wurden in langjähriger Forschungsarbeit in der direkten Anwendung in kinderärztlichen Praxen zwei treffsichere und praxistaugliche Sprachscreenings entwickelt, die nun für den Einsatz für 2‑ und 3‑jährige Kinder zur Verfügung stehen. Adaptierungen im Sinne einer Tablet-basierten und somit papierfreien Umsetzung sind in Erprobung. Zudem wird aktuell ein Online-Elterntraining zur frühen Sprachförderung evaluiert.
... There have been numerous studies conducted to identify risk factors, predictors, and prevalence of language disorders in children (3,(6)(7)(8)(9)(10). Additionally, there is a considerable body of research focused on interventions for children with language delay (11)(12)(13). Some studies have reported outcome predictors specifically related to children with delayed expressive language (14,15). ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction To assess the association between a three-month interruption of language intervention programs and the language performance of children with language delay during the COVID-19 pandemic, and to identify which children are more vulnerable to such interruptions. Materials and methods This is a retrospective study involving 33 children with language delay who experienced a three-month suspension of language interventions due to the COVID-19 pandemic. We collected their demographic data and language performance scores from the Comprehensive Developmental Inventory for Infants and Toddlers—Diagnostic test (CDIIT-DT) at four different time points. The scores were analyzed using a Wilcoxon Signed Ranks test. Results The median scores of language comprehension and overall language ability showed a decreasing trend during the interruption period. However, resuming interventions post-interruption showed a statistically significant increase in all language domains. Children in the borderline delay group (CDIIT-DT DQ scores between 71 and 85) were more likely to experience a decline in their language abilities during the interruption. Discussion This is the first study to reveal a decreasing trend in language performance during interruption periods, and highlighting the significance of post-interruption language interventions in facilitating improvements. Furthermore, our study brings attention to the heightened vulnerability of children exhibiting borderline language delay in overall language ability tests when faced with interruptions in language interventions.
... Caregiver-implemented interventions increase families access to evidence-based practices for individuals with developmental disabilities, which may lead to children making significant improvements in their behavioral outcomes and increase the likelihood that these improvements will generalize to new settings, situations, or stimuli and maintain across time (Meadan et al., 2009;Kaiser et al., 2000;Roberts & Kaiser, 2015). Previous research has demonstrated the efficacy of training caregivers to implement interventions for improving leisure and play skills, daily living skills, and promoting communication for children with developmental disabilities (Barton & Fettig, 2013;Symon, 2005). ...
Article
Full-text available
Children with developmental disabilities often have difficulty completing complex multistep tasks, such as daily living skills or leisure and play skills, which often occur in children’s homes. The use of visual supports can increase the predictability of routines and clarify behavioral expectations for children with developmental disabilities. Thus, increasing the likelihood that these children will independently engage in these complex tasks. Caregivers are important stakeholders, especially when it comes to interventions that occur in the family home. Therefore, it is important for researchers to continue to evaluate the effectiveness of caregiver-implemented interventions with visual supports in the home setting. The purpose of the current review was to systematically evaluate the use of caregiver-implemented visual support interventions using the categories of visual supports identified by (Rutherford et al., Autism 24:447–469, 2020). We reviewed 16 studies with 37 participants that examined caregiver-implemented visual supports in the home. We found that visual schedules were the most commonly used visual support. In the studies that included visual schedules, researchers instructed caregivers to use visual schedules to promote their child’s completion of daily living skills most often. Based on these findings, we discuss considerations for implementing these interventions, quality and social validity of caregiver-implemented visual support interventions, and areas of future research.
... Hampton et al. (2017) conducted a follow-up study of children with DLD who participated in a randomized controlled trial of the effects of Enhanced Milieu Teaching (M. Y. Roberts & Kaiser, 2015). Significant group differences in receptive language scores immediately post-intervention were nonsignificant by 6-month and 12-month follow-up timepoints, thus demonstrating fade-out of effects. ...
Article
Purpose: Early language and communication interventions for children with language impairments have been shown to be effective in assessments administered immediately after treatment. The purpose of the current systematic review and meta-analysis was to assess the overall durability of those effects over time and whether durability was related to outcome type, etiology of child language impairments, implementer of intervention, magnitude of posttest effects, time between intervention and follow-up, and study risk of bias. Method: We conducted a systematic search of online databases and reference lists to identify experimental and quasi-experimental group design studies. All studies tested the effects of early communication interventions at least 3 months post-intervention. Participants were children 0-5 years old with language impairments. Coders identified study features and rated methodological quality indicators for all studies. Effect sizes at long-term timepoints and associations with potential moderators were estimated using multilevel meta-analysis with robust variance estimation. Results: Twenty studies with 129 long-term outcome effect sizes met inclusion criteria. Studies included children with developmental language disorders or language impairment associated with autism. The overall average effect size was small and significant (g = .22, p = .002). Effect size estimates were larger for prelinguistic outcomes (g = .36, p < .001) than for linguistic outcomes (g = .14, p = .101). Significant factors were the posttest effect sizes, the risk of bias for randomized trials, and etiology of language impairment for linguistic outcomes. Time post-intervention did not significantly predict long-term effect sizes. Conclusions: Outcomes of early language and communication interventions appear to persist for at least several months post-intervention. More research is needed with collection and evaluation of long-term outcomes, a focus on measurement, and consistency of primary study reporting. Supplemental material: https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.23589648.
... There is mounting evidence that early language intervention is likely to lead to better outcomes for children (Roberts & Kaiser, 2015;Wake et al., 2011). Some researchers (Larson, 2016) have suggested that universal language screening for young children would allow for timely identification of language disorders, hearing loss, autism spectrum disorders (ASD), and genetic disorders (Kaiser et al., 2022). ...
Article
Early screening for language problems is a priority given the importance of language for success in school and interpersonal relationships. The paucity of reliable behavioral instruments for this age group prompted the development of a new touchscreen language screener for 2-year-olds that relies on language comprehension. Developmental literature guided selection of age-appropriate markers of language disorder risk that are culturally and dialectally neutral and could be reliably assessed. Items extend beyond products of linguistic knowledge (vocabulary and syntax) and tap the process by which children learn language, also known as fast mapping. After piloting an extensive set of items (139), two phases of testing with over 500 children aged 2; 0-2; 11 were conducted to choose the final 40-item set. Rasch analysis was used to select the best fitting and least redundant items. Norms were created based on 270 children. Sufficient test-retest reliability, Cronbach's alpha, and convergent validity with the MB-CDI and PPVT are reported. This quick behavioral measure of language capabilities could support research studies and facilitate the early detection of language problems.
... For example, studies can examine whether parent coaching (Roberts & Kaiser, 2015) can alter neural activities of the child as well as parentchild neural attunement (Nguyen et al., 2021) and whether changes in neural activities are associated with language developmental outcome (Cychosz, 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
The speech register that adults especially caregivers use when interacting with infants and toddlers, that is, infant‐directed speech (IDS) or baby talk, has been reported to facilitate language development throughout the early years. However, the neural mechanisms as well as why IDS results in such a developmental faciliatory effect remain to be investigated. The current study uses functional near‐infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) to evaluate two alternative hypotheses of such a facilitative effect, that IDS serves to enhance linguistic contrastiveness or to attract the child's attention. Behavioral and fNIRS data were acquired from twenty‐seven Cantonese‐learning toddlers 15–20 months of age when their parents spoke to them in either an IDS or adult‐directed speech (ADS) register in a naturalistic task in which the child learned four disyllabic pseudowords. fNIRS results showed significantly greater neural responses to IDS than ADS register in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (L‐dlPFC), but opposite response patterns in the bilateral inferior frontal gyrus (IFG). The differences in fNIRS responses to IDS and to ADS in the L‐dlPFC and the left parietal cortex (L‐PC) showed significantly positive correlations with the differences in the behavioral word‐learning performance of toddlers. The same fNIRS measures in the L‐dlPFC and right PC (R‐PC) of toddlers were significantly correlated with pitch range differences of parents between the two speech conditions. Together, our results suggest that the dynamic prosody in IDS increased toddlers’ attention through greater involvement of the left frontoparietal network that facilitated word learning, compared to ADS. Research Highlights This study for the first time examined the neural mechanisms of how infant‐directed speech (IDS) facilitates word learning in toddlers. Using fNIRS, we identified the cortical regions that were directly involved in IDS processing. Our results suggest that IDS facilitates word learning by engaging a right‐lateralized prosody processing and top‐down attentional mechanisms in the left frontoparietal networks. The language network including the inferior frontal gyrus and temporal cortex was not directly involved in IDS processing to support word learning.
Article
Language and drawing each have their own sets of symbols that they use to convey messages. It is still unclear whether the development of these two expression mechanisms is similar or distinct. There are various approaches to supporting language and communication skills, including structured and naturalistic methods. The goal of this in-depth case study is to find out how a combined intervention affects the language and drawing abilities of a three-year-old girl with developmental delay. Structured implementations and milieu teaching strategies were used in the same session, but not simultaneously. The participant's language and communication skills improved at the end of the programme. Interestingly, even though there was no drawing-related goal, her drawings evolved too. Even though this is a single-subject case study, it suggests that if a child's cognitive and language skills develop, his/her drawing skills do as well.
Article
Introduction: Caregiver-implemented language programmes are effective for alleviating early language difficulties. This study examined the efficacy of a self-directed video-based caregiver-implement language programme in Chinese families. Method: This study consisted of two stages. In stage 1, 31 caregiver-child dyads (typically developing children) completed the training programme (group 1) in the form of six video-based training modules. In stage 2, 28 caregiver-child dyads (children with language difficulties) receiving active speech therapy were randomly assigned to the training (group 2) and control arms (group 3). Group 2 received the same training as group 1 in addition to their regular therapy while group 3 was kept as status quo. Caregivers completed a quiz on their knowledge of language facilitation techniques (LFTs) and submitted caregiver-child interaction videos at the start and end of the training. Outcome measures included programme completion rate, quiz scores, and use of LFTs and children's communication skills in the videos. A pre-post design and a between-group design were adopted in the stage 1 and 2 studies, respectively. Results: A completion rate of about 60% in both stages was noted. Significantly higher post-training knowledge scores were found in groups 1 and 2. General but nonsignificant growth in use of parallel talk and gesture, and significant gains in children's vocalization in the training arm were observed. Conclusion: The self-directed video-based training programme would be useful in imparting information to caregivers. However, the modest improvements in the use of LFTs suggested direct coaching appeared to still play a significant role in enhancing the actual implementation of LFTs. Further investigation on a larger scale is required to evaluate the effectiveness of the training programme for promoting the wider use of this mode as a preventive measure.
Article
Full-text available
This article describes the Teach-Model-Coach-Review approach for teaching parents to implement Enhanced Milieu Teaching (EMT), an evidence-based naturalistic intervention for young children with language impairment. The article discusses the evidence for parent training as an effective early language intervention approach, the principles and procedures of EMT, the empirical basis of the Teach-Model-Coach-Review approach, and the skills needed to implement this approach.
Article
Full-text available
To determine the benefits of a low intensity parent-toddler language promotion programme delivered to toddlers identified as slow to talk on screening in universal services. Cluster randomised trial nested in a population based survey. Three local government areas in Melbourne, Australia. Parents attending 12 month well child checks over a six month period completed a baseline questionnaire. At 18 months, children at or below the 20th centile on an expressive vocabulary checklist entered the trial. Maternal and child health centres (clusters) were randomly allocated to intervention (modified "You Make the Difference" programme over six weekly sessions) or control ("usual care") arms. The primary outcome was expressive language (Preschool Language Scale-4) at 2 and 3 years; secondary outcomes were receptive language at 2 and 3 years, vocabulary checklist raw score at 2 and 3 years, Expressive Vocabulary Test at 3 years, and Child Behavior Checklist/1.5-5 raw score at 2 and 3 years. 1217 parents completed the baseline survey; 1138 (93.5%) completed the 18 month checklist, when 301 (26.4%) children had vocabulary scores at or below the 20th centile and were randomised (158 intervention, 143 control). 115 (73%) intervention parents attended at least one session (mean 4.5 sessions), and most reported high satisfaction with the programme. Interim outcomes at age 2 years were similar in the two groups. Similarly, at age 3 years, adjusted mean differences (intervention-control) were -2.4 (95% confidence interval -6.2 to 1.4; P=0.21) for expressive language; -0.3 (-4.2 to 3.7; P=0.90) for receptive language; 4.1 (-2.3 to 10.6; P=0.21) for vocabulary checklist; -0.5 (-4.4 to 3.4; P=0.80) for Expressive Vocabulary Test; -0.1 (-1.6 to 1.4; P=0.86) for externalising behaviour problems; and -0.1 (-1.3 to 1.2; P=0. 92) for internalising behaviour problems. This community based programme targeting slow to talk toddlers was feasible and acceptable, but little evidence was found that it improved language or behaviour either immediately or at age 3 years. Current Controlled Trials ISRCTN20953675.
Article
Objective: To document the prevalence of expressive language delay in relation to age and gender in 12- to 39-month-old children. To document the characteristics, particularly social competence and emotional/behavioral problems, related to deficits in expressive language. Method: Parents of an age- and sex-stratified random sample of children born at Yale New Haven Hospital between July 1995 and September 1997 who lived in the New Haven Meriden Standard Metropolitan Statistical Area were enrolled when their children were 12 to 39 months of age (79.8% participation; N = 1,189). The main outcome for these analyses is expressive language delay measured by the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory, short forms. Results: Expressive language delays range from 13.5% in 18- to 23-month-olds to 17.5% in children 30 to 36 months of age. By 18 to 23 months, children are more likely to experience delays if they come from environments characterized by low education, low expressiveness, poverty, high levels of parenting stress, and parents who report worry about their children's language problems. When social competence is adjusted for in the multivariable model, behavior problems are no longer associated with language delay, suggesting that poor social competence rather than behavior problems may be the critical early correlate of low expressive language development. Conclusions: Expressive language delays are prevalent problems that appear to be associated with poor social competence. Given that such problems may be risk factors for social and emotional problems, early identification is critical.
Article
This paper reports on school readiness (SR) and its predictors in five‐ to six‐year‐old children from a prospective, longitudinal study of children from eight months to seven years (the Early Language in Victoria Study – ELVS). The ELVS children came from a representative sample of children recruited though the State Government Infant and Child Health Centres at the age of eight months who were studied at yearly intervals with a combination of parent surveys and face‐to‐face assessments. The study had a focus on language, pre‐literacy and behavioural development. In pre‐school and preparatory grade, teachers of the children completed a brief questionnaire rating SR characteristics, including cognitive, language and personal/social competencies. The data bank on these children provided a set of hypothesised child and family predictors of the SR score which were tested via factor analysis and regression analyses. Significant predictors of SR in the equation were evident from two years of age, and were all related to language and pre‐literacy factors from two to six years. Twelve per cent of the children showed language impairment (LI) at five years according to their scores on the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, 4th edition (CELF‐4). In an additional analysis we found that these children were significantly lower on SR; they were also of lower social class compared with the remainder of the sample. The most influential factors in readiness for school were child language competencies and pre‐literacy capacities, including phonemic awareness and letter knowledge. Preparation for school involving systematic emphasis on language and pre‐literacy enrichment is recommended for all children before school entry and particularly for children at risk, including those coming from socio‐economic disadvantage and those with delayed and impaired early language development. This addition to pre‐school programmes can have lasting positive effects into pre‐school and beyond, and prevent early school failure.
Article
The prevalence and the natural history of primary speech and language delays were two of four domains covered in a systematic review of the literature related to screening for speech and language delay carried out for the NHS in the UK. The structure and process of the full literature review is introduced and criteria for inclusion in the two domains are specié ed. The resulting data set gave 16 prevalence estimates generated from 21 publications and 12 natural history studies generated from 18 publications. Results are summarized for six subdivisions of primary speech and language delays: (1) speech and/or language, (2) language only, (3) speech only, (4) expression with comprehension, (5) expression only and (6) comprehension only. Combination of the data suggests that both concurrent and predictive case deé nition can be problematic. Prediction improves if language is taken independently of speech and if express- ive and receptive language are taken together. The results are discussed in terms of the need to develop a model of prevalence based on risk of subsequent diYculties.
Article
The literacy skills of 56 school leavers from the Bishop and Edmundson (1987) cohort of preschoolers with specific language impairment (SLI) were assessed at 15 years. The SLI group performed worse on tests of reading, spelling, and reading comprehension than age-matched controls and the literacy outcomes were particularly poor for those with Performance IQ less than 100. The rate of specific reading retardation in the SLI group had increased between the ages of 8½ and 15 years and there had been a substantial drop in reading accuracy, relative to age. However, over 35% had reading skills within the normal range and those who had had isolated impairments of expressive phonology had a particularly good outcome. Our findings highlight the limitations of discrepancy definitions of dyslexia that do not take account of the changing demands of reading over time. We argue that children's phonological difficulties place them at risk of literacy failure at the outset of reading and that later, impairments of other language skills compromise development to adult levels of fluency.