Content uploaded by Serge H Ahmed
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Serge H Ahmed on Mar 25, 2015
Content may be subject to copyright.
Available via license: CC BY 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.
PSYCHIATRY
EDITORIAL
published: 13 February 2015
doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00020
Alternative models of addiction
Hanna Pickard1*, Serge H. Ahmed 2and Bennett Foddy 3
1University of Birmingham, Birmingham, UK
2University of Bordeaux: IMN CNRS UMR 5293, Bordeaux, France
3NewYork University, NewYork, NY, USA
*Correspondence: h.pickard@gmail.com
Edited and reviewed by:
Giovanni Addolorato, Catholic University of Rome, Italy
Keywords: drugs, addiction, disease, choice, compulsion, self-control, substance abuse, substance dependence
For much of the twentieth century, theories of addictive behav-
ior and motivation were polarized between two models. The first
model viewed addiction as a moral failure for which addicts
are rightly held responsible and judged accordingly. The second
model, in contrast, viewed addiction as a specific brain disease
caused by neurobiological adaptations occurring in response to
chronic drug or alcohol use, and over which addicts have no
choice or control. As our capacity to observe neurobiological phe-
nomena improved, the second model became scientific orthodoxy,
increasingly dominating addiction research and informing public
understandings of addiction. The articles in this research topic aim
to move beyond the polarization between the competing moral
and disease models of addiction.
In the opening article of this e-book “Addiction and Choice:
Theory and New Data,” Heyman (1) examines new data on the
ways that addicts recover, and argues that recovery from addic-
tion is better predicted by a model in which addicts choose to use
drugs, rather than one in which they are compelled to do so by
a disease. This theme is echoed in other papers in this collection.
Satel and Lilienfeld (2) in “Addiction and the Brain-Disease Fal-
lacy” directly challenge the disease model, drawing on historical
and clinical data to argue that addicts respond to incentives and
use drugs for reasons, and so addictive behavior must be under-
stood as a choice. In “Intertemporal Bargaining in Addiction,”
Ainslie (3) reprises his large body of work on the inherent weak-
nesses of the human capacity for choice, exploring its relevance
to questions of the nature of responsibility and our justifica-
tion in holding addicts accountable for addictive behavior and its
consequences.
Other authors in this volume seek to understand the ways
in which these choices can be pathologically impaired by addic-
tion. For example, Dill and Holton’s (4) article “The Addict In Us
All” contrasts ordinary choices with what they call the “incentive
salience”choices, which are typical in addictive consumption, and
involve extreme cravings for drugs and strong motivation to con-
sume, even when consumption is neither experienced nor judged
as desirable. Henden et al. (5) in “Addiction: Choice or Compul-
sion?” chart a course between the moral and disease model by
arguing that addictive behavior can be labeled both voluntary and
compulsive, if this is understood as involving repeated decisions,
which can lead to maladaptive and self-destructive behavioral
outcomes.
The importance of negative outcomes to understanding addic-
tion is emphasized by both Levy (6) and Wakefield and Schmitz (7).
In “Addiction is Not a Brain Disease (And it Matters),” Levy argues
that while addiction does produce neurological dysfunction, this
is not enough to make it a disease. In Levy’s eyes, disease neces-
sarily involves impairment, and impairment must be understood
relative to the social and practical context in which addicts live. In
contrast, Wakefield and Shmitz’s article “How Many People Have
Alcohol Use Disorders?”points out that by focusing too heavily on
the negative health correlates of chronic alcohol use, DSM-IV and
DSM-V diagnostic criteria risk diagnosing non-addicts suffering
the ill-effects of long-term use with an addictive “disease.”
The social and practical context of addiction is also emphasized
by other authors. It has long been known that cocaine-addicted
rats will forego cocaine if offered alternative goods, such as sugar
or saccharin. Zernig et al.’s (8) article “Dyadic Social Interaction
as an Alternative Reward to Cocaine” presents data demonstrat-
ing that, in certain experimental conditions, rats will also forego
cocaine for the opportunity of same-sex snuggling. Overall, these
findings further show that drug choices are not determined by the
ability of a drug to directly activate and/or sensitize the reinforcing
and incentive salience neuronal pathways in the brain. This con-
clusion is consistent with the general evolutionary view of drug
use advanced by Hagen et al. (9). Their article notes that the most
currently widely used drugs, like cannabis, cocaine, and nicotine,
are originally plant chemical defenses that evolved to deter con-
sumption. To adapt, animals are likely to have evolved internal
protective mechanisms allowing them to use drugs in a controlled
manner.
In “The Shame of Addiction,” Flanagan (10) argues that the
first-personal experience of shame – typically a social and moral
emotion – is central to understanding human addiction and the
motivation addicts have to heal, but that shame can be distin-
guished from blame, allowing addiction to be conceived as an
aspect of personal agency without returning to the moral model.
In the final article of this e-book, Heyman et al. (11) present data
suggesting that years spent in school is a key predictor of illicit
drug use, after controlling for IQ and impulsivity, suggesting not
only a potential social cause of addiction but also, equally, a social
solution.
The e-book also contains articles exploring the classification of
addiction and its potential status as a natural kind, the role and
extent of pleasure in explanations of addiction, and various more
unusual forms of addiction, such as workaholism, which we nat-
urally characterize as involving a need for control, as opposed to
involving a loss of control.
www.frontiersin.org February 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 20 | 1
Pickard et al. Alternative models of addiction
Considered as a whole, the articles in this volume demonstrate
that we can conceptualize addiction as choice,while avoiding both
the scylla of moralization and the charybda of brain disease. By
doing so, they emphasize the need to give equal attention and
weight to the historical, contextual, and biological factors that are
significant in addiction, to move forward in understanding and
responding to the problem. Addiction as choice may thus offer a
unifying and integrative framework for future research in the field.
REFERENCES
1. Heyman G. Addiction and choice: theory and new data. Front Psychiatry (2013)
4:31. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00031
2. Satel S, Lilienfeld SO. Addiction and the brain-disease fallacy. Front Psychiatry
(2014) 4:141. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00141
3. Ainslie G. Intertemporal bargaining in addiction. Front Psychiatry (2013) 4:63.
doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00063
4. Dill B, Holton R. The addict in us all. Front Psychiatry (2014) 5:139. doi:10.
3389/fpsyt.2014.00139
5. Henden E, Melberg HO, Rogeberg O. Addiction: choice or compulsion? Front
Psychiatry (2013) 4:77. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00077
6. Levy N. Addiction is not a brain disease (and it matters). Front Psychiatry (2013)
4:24. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00024
7. Wakefield JC, Schmitz MF. How many people have alcohol use disorders? Using
the harmful dysfunction analysis to reconcile prevalence estimates in two com-
munity surveys. Front Psychiatry (2014) 5:10. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00010
8. Zernig G, Kummer KK, Prast JM. Dyadic social interaction as an alternative
reward to cocaine. Front Psychiatry (2013) 4:100. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00100
9. Hagen EH, Roulette CH, Sullivan RJ.Explaining human recreational use of ‘pes-
ticides’: the neurotoxin regulation model of substance use vs. the hijack model
and implications for age and sex differences in drug consumption. Front Psychi-
atry (2013) 4:142. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00142
10. Flanagan O. The shame of addiction. Front Psychiatry (2013) 4:120. doi:10.3389/
fpsyt.2013.00120
11. Heyman G, Dunn BJ, Mignone J. Disentangling the correlates of drug use in a
clinic and community sample: a regression analysis of the associations between
drug use, years-of-school, impulsivity, IQ, working memory, and psychiatric
symptoms. Front Psychiatry (2014) 5:70. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00070
Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare that the researchwas conducted
in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed
as a potential conflict of interest.
Received: 09 January 2015; accepted: 31 January 2015; published online: 13 February
2015.
Citation: Pickard H, Ahmed SH and Foddy B (2015) Alternative models of addiction.
Front. Psychiatry 6:20. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00020
This article was submitted to Addictive Disorders and Behavioral Dyscontrol, a section
of the journal Frontiers in Psychiatry.
Copyright © 2015 Pickard, Ahmed and Foddy. This is an open-access article distr ibuted
under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, dis-
tribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s)
or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in
accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is
permitted which does not comply with these terms.
Frontiers in Psychiatry | Addictive Disorders and Behavioral Dyscontrol February 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 20 | 2