ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Attention can be improved by repetition of a specific task that involves an attention network (network training), or by exercise or meditation that changes the brain state (state training). We first review the concept of attention networks that link changes in orienting, alerting and executive control to brain networks. Network training through video games or computer exercises can improve aspects of attention. The extent of transfer beyond the trained task is a controversial issue. Mindfulness is a form of meditation that keeps attention focused on the current moment. Some forms of meditation have been shown to improve executive attention reduce stress and produce specific brain changes. Additional research is needed to understand the limits and mechanisms of these effects.
Enhancing
attention
through
training
Michael
I
Posner
1
,
Mary
K
Rothbart
1
and
Yi-Yuan
Tang
2
Attention
can
be
improved
by
repetition
of
a
specific
task
that
involves
an
attention
network
(network
training),
or
by
exercise
or
meditation
that
changes
the
brain
state
(state
training).
We
first
review
the
concept
of
attention
networks
that
link
changes
in
orienting,
alerting
and
executive
control
to
brain
networks.
Network
training
through
video
games
or
computer
exercises
can
improve
aspects
of
attention.
The
extent
of
transfer
beyond
the
trained
task
is
a
controversial
issue.
Mindfulness
is
a
form
of
meditation
that
keeps
attention
focused
on
the
current
moment.
Some
forms
of
meditation
have
been
shown
to
improve
executive
attention
reduce
stress
and
produce
specific
brain
changes.
Additional
research
is
needed
to
understand
the
limits
and
mechanisms
of
these
effects.
Addresses
1
University
of
Oregon,
United
States
2
Texas
Tech
University,
United
States
Corresponding
author:
Posner,
Michael
I
(mposner@uoregon.edu)
Current
Opinion
in
Behavioral
Sciences
2015,
4:15
This
review
comes
from
a
themed
issue
on
Cognitive
enhancement
Edited
by
Barbara
Sahakian
and
Arthur
Kramer
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2014.12.008
2352-1546/#
2015
Elsevier
Ltd.
All
rights
reserved.
Introduction
We
consider
two
brain
training
strategies
to
improve
attention
and
other
cognitive
functions
[1

].
Network
training
involves
practice
of
a
cognitive
task
thought
to
exercise
specific
brain
networks
related
to
attention.
State
training,
on
the
other
hand,
uses
practice
designed
to
develop
a
brain
state
that
may
influence
attention
and
other
networks
[26].
State
training
also
involves
net-
works,
but
it
is
does
not
include
cognitive
tasks
designed
specifically
to
train
a
network.
Both
aerobic
exercise
and
meditation
training
can
establish
a
state
that
appears
to
improve
cognition,
attention
and
mood
[1

].
Training
attention
networks
Since
attention
involves
a
number
of
neural
networks
with
different
functions
[7]
there
are
many
ways
to
improve
one
or
more
of
these
functions
and
thereby
improve
the
efficiency
of
the
network.
Some
of
the
cues
for
dissociations
between
attentional
functions
arise
from
studies
of
brain
injury
using
the
Attention
Network
Test
(ANT)
to
classify
patients.
The
ANT
(see
Fig.
1)
is
a
cognitive
task
built
around
the
flanker
effect
and
designed
to
provide
separate
measurement
of
the
alert-
ing,
orienting
and
executive
networks
[8].
Limitations
in
the
reliability
and
utility
of
the
ANT,
especially
for
applied
research,
have
been
discussed
[9].
While
some
efforts
have
been
made
to
improve
the
test
its
main
goal
is
to
link
behavioral
changes
in
attention
to
the
brain
net-
works
involved.
For
example,
in
one
study
110
stroke
patients
and
62
con-
trol
participants
were
given
the
ANT
[10].
Analysis
of
brain
scans
and
ANT
revealed
three
separate
groups
of
patients:
(i)
those
patients
with
thalamic
damage
showing
deficits
in
alerting,
(ii)
parietal
damage
patients
showing
deficits
in
orienting,
and
(iii)
patients
with
damage
to
white
matter
tracts
connected
to
the
anterior
cingulate
showing
deficits
in
executive
control
[10].
This
classifica-
tion
into
three
categories
fits
with
many
findings
from
imaging
research
summarized
in
Ref.
[7].
Studies
have
used
training
techniques
to
improve
atten-
tion
in
patient
groups
similar
to
those
discussed
above.
Patients
with
thalamic
damage
and
participants
who
are
sleep
deprived
have
trouble
in
obtaining
a
sufficient
level
of
alertness
to
perform
well
in
cognitive
tasks.
The
use
of
auditory
warning
signals
has
been
shown
to
provide
a
temporary
improvement
in
performance
and
training
to
use
the
signals
can
improve
both
alerting
and
orienting
functions
[11].
Patients
with
right
parietal
damage
often
have
problems
orienting
to
signals
that
go
directly
to
the
damaged
hemisphere.
Training
involving
teaching
the
patients
to
instruct
themselves
to
orient
to
the
left
during
visual
search
improved
performance
[12].
Training
in
action
video
games
has
been
shown
to
improve
orienting
network
performance
during
visual
search
and
similar
tasks
[13].
There
is
more
doubt
that
general
improvement
in
all
forms
of
attention
or
learning
occurs
with
videogame
training
[14,15

].
However,
in
a
recent
very
extensive
training
study
with
video
games
and
controls
the
authors
do
find
that
transfer
to
untrained
paradigms
in
some
forms
of
video
games
[15].
A
similar
controversy
regarding
transfer
is
found
when
training
involves
a
specific
but
general
network
such
as
working
memory
[16,17].
Many
studies
of
training
executive
attention
have
been
carried
out
in
children
[18

,19,20]
using
computerized
exercises
designed
to
improve
conflict
resolution
[19,20]
or
more
general
school
curricula
designed
to
exercise
all
aspects
of
executive
functions
[18

].
These
studies
have
often
demonstrated
in
improvement
executive
attention
[18

]
and
some
transfer
[19,20].
While
there
is
evidence
Available
online
at
www.sciencedirect.com
ScienceDirect
www.sciencedirect.com
Current
Opinion
in
Behavioral
Sciences
2015,
4:15
that
self
control
score
in
childhood
can
predict
adult
performance
[21]
there
are
no
studies
showing
that
train-
ing
the
executive
network
in
children
can
improve
adult
outcomes.
Although
most
of
the
work
in
video
games
has
been
done
with
adults
and
most
of
the
work
on
school
curricula
with
children
there
is
little
evidence
that
train-
ing
is
limited
to
any
one
age.
There
may
be
ways
to
enhance
the
success
of
network
training.
For
example,
there
is
evidence
that
training
parents
can
greatly
enhance
the
effectiveness
of
child
training,
presumably
by
getting
the
parent
to
support
home
activity
[22].
It
is
also
possible
that
brain
stimula-
tion
through
scalp
electrodes
or
trans-cortical
magnetic
stimulation
(TMS)
may
improve
plasticity
and
enhance
training
[23].
The
safety
of
the
stimulation
methods,
particularly
when
practiced
by
consumers
without
train-
ing,
remains
an
important
concern
[24].
Below
we
con-
sider
some
state
training
methods
that
can
be
used
either
individually
or
in
combination
with
network
training.
Training
brain
states
A
number
of
methods
have
been
proposed
for
training
a
brain
state
that
will
foster
attention
and
self
regulation
[1

].
Exposure
to
nature
has
been
shown
to
be
a
useful
method
to
improve
attention
[1

],
and
aerobic
exercise
has
been
shown
in
many
studies
to
have
broad
effects
on
cognition
including
attention
[6].
Meditation
is
a
mental
method
that
has
the
most
similarities
to
the
network
training
discussed
above.
In
understanding
the
training
of
attention
it
is
important
to
consider
the
way
attention
works
during
cognitive
tasks.
When
involved
in
cognitive
tasks
it
is
common
for
the
human
mind
to
wander
to
thoughts
unrelated
to
the
task
[25]
and
mind
wandering
is
often
directed
to
the
self
[26].
In
some
cases
there
is
awareness
of
the
mind
wandering
and
in
other
cases
one
is
not
aware
until
questioned.
To
determine
the
occurrence
of
such
thinking
it
is
valuable
to
employ
experience
sampling
[27]
where
participants
are
probed
to
report
whether
or
not
their
mind
is
wandering.
In
one
such
study
parti-
cipants
carried
out
a
sustained
attention
task
[28]
while
being
scanned
with
fMRI
and
probed
as
to
whether
their
mind
had
wandered
and
whether
they
had
been
aware
of
that
wandering.
Findings
suggest
that
both
the
default
(a
mostly
midline
network
of
brain
areas
show-
ing
correlated
activity
when
the
person
is
not
engaged
2
Cognitive
enhancement
Figure
1
congruent
no cue
ALERTING = NO CUE RT – DOUBLE CUE RT
ORIENTING = CENTER CUE RT – SPATIAL CUE RT
CONFLICT = INCONGRUENT TARGET RT – CONGRUENT TARGET RT
center cue double cue spatial cue
Cue
Target
incongruent Neutral
+
+
*
+
+
+
D = 400 –
1600 ms
100 ms 400 ms RT < 1700 ms 3000 – RT – D ms
+
+
**
*
+
*
*
+
*
(b) Three target conditions
(c) Time line
(a) Four cue conditions
(d) Three subtractions
Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences
A
schema
of
the
Attention
Network
Test.
(a)
Cue
conditions,
(b)
target
conditions,
(c)
time
line
of
events.
Adapted
from
Ref.
[8].
Current
Opinion
in
Behavioral
Sciences
2015,
4:15
www.sciencedirect.com
in
a
task)
and
executive
networks
are
active
during
mind
wandering
[29].
The
activation
in
both
networks
is
greater
when
the
person
is
not
aware
of
mind
wan-
dering
prior
to
the
probe.
Meditation
is
a
method
that
works
to
resist
mind
wandering
and
produce
an
atten-
tion
focus.
Five
different
styles
of
meditation
have
been
involved
in
over
400
clinical
trials
[30],
but
one
style
of
meditation,
mindfulness
meditation
seems
to
dominant
current
stud-
ies.
Mindfulness
meditation
involves
a
set
of
mental
practices
designed
to
achieve
control
by
the
person
over
the
direction
of
attention.
This
is
done
by
either
focusing
on
a
specific
content
(e.g.
ones
breathing
or
a
word)
or
keeping
a
relaxed
state
in
which
attention
is
not
allowed
to
move
away
from
the
present
state
but
is
not
focused
on
a
particular
content.
Recent
meta-analyses
of
the
behav-
ioral
effects
of
meditation
[31],
functional
brain
systems
involved
[32,33]
and
structural
changes
in
brain
gray
and
white
matter
[34
]
have
been
generally
favorable
to
changes
in
behavior
induced
by
various
forms
of
medita-
tion
training.
The
meditative
state
is
often
accompanied
by
changes
in
measures
related
to
autonomic
activity
often
used
as
a
biomarker
for
monitoring
meditative
states
[31,32].
The
central
nervous
system
also
undergoes
changes
following
meditation
training.
Consistent
structural
changes
found
in
a
metaanalysis
[34
]
are
in
the
ACC
and
insula
parts
of
the
executive
attention
network
[7].
Many
of
the
early
meditation
studies
compared
long
time
practitioners
with
control
participants
selected
to
be
similar
to
the
meditators.
Although
many
findings
were
of
interest,
it
was
hard
to
be
sure
that
differences
were
due
to
meditation
rather
than
to
other
differences
be-
tween
those
devoted
to
the
practice
and
controls.
More
recently
longitudinal
studies
over
periods
of
a
week
to
several
months
have
compared
training
with
various
forms
of
meditation
to
either
waiting
list
or
other
controls.
These
studies
allow
random
assignment
and
can
attri-
bute
cause
to
the
training.
The
studies
have
often
found
important
differences
attributed
to
meditation
[2
5,32,33,34
,3537].
However,
meditation
training
is
itself
complex
and
it
remains
unclear
which
aspects
of
the
training
are
most
important
to
obtain
each
of
the
changes
discussed
below.
Longitudinal
studies
A
series
of
meditation
training
studies
have
compared
one
week
of
integrated
body
mind
training
(IBMT),
a
version
of
mindfulness
meditation,
with
an
active
con-
trol
group
given
relaxation
training
[3,37,38]
The
groups
are
randomly
assigned.
IBMT
uses
most
of
the
methods
of
other
mindfulness
training
including
maintaining
attention
in
the
present
without
judgment
and
use
of
instruction
to
promote
a
state
of
high
concentration
on
the
present
with
minimal
mind
wan-
dering.
Relaxation
training
involves
deliberate
relaxa-
tion
of
each
muscle
group
and
has
been
widely
practiced
as
a
part
of
cognitive
behavioral
therapy.
In
comparison
with
the
relaxation
control
group,
one
week
of
IBMT
(30
min
per
day)
produced
better
executive
attention
using
the
Attention
Network
Test,
higher
positive
mood
and
lower
negative
mood
in
self
report,
lower
secretion
of
the
stress
hormone
cortisol
following
meditation
after
a
mental
arithmetic
challenge
and
increased
immuno-reactivity
[3].
A
month
of
IBMT
in
comparison
with
a
relaxation
train-
ing
control
group
showed
reduced
cortisol
at
baseline
(before
any
cognitive
stress)
and
evidence
that
stress
was
reduced
in
a
dose
dependent
manner.
These
changes
do
not
seem
to
be
dependent
upon
specific
aspects
of
IBMT
as
they
have
been
reported
with
other
mindfulness
methods
[39].
However,
one
well
documented
form
of
training,
mindfulness
based
stress
reduction
[40]
has
not
been
found
to
improve
attention
[41].
The
reasons
for
this
difference
are
not
known.
The
IBMT
studies
also
examined
both
central
and
autonomic
nervous
system
activity
in
comparison
to
relaxation
controls.
There
was
clear
evidence
of
changes
in
activity
in
the
ventral
ACC
and
in
its
functional
connectivity
with
the
striatum
[37,38].
These
findings
fit
well
with
meta-analysis
of
meditation
effects
on
func-
tional
and
structural
activation
[4].
In
addition
two
to
four
weeks
of
IBMT
produced
increases
in
fractional
anisot-
ropy
(FA),
an
index
of
white
matter
efficiency
[38].
These
changes
were
found
in
a
band
of
white
matter
tracts
connecting
the
ACC
to
striatal
and
cortical
areas.
After
two
weeks,
the
changes
were
mainly
in
axial
diffu-
sivity
(AD),
often
taken
as
a
measure
of
axonal
density,
while
after
four
weeks
the
study
found
both
improved
AD
and
improved
RD
(radial
diffusivity,
related
to
myelination)
[38].
White
matter
is
usually
regarded
as
changing
in
develop-
ment,
but
relatively
fixed
in
adulthood.
However,
recent
reviews
have
shown
much
more
dynamism
in
adult
white
matter
that
can
be
changed
rapidly
in
response
to
events
such
as
demyelination
in
conjunction
with
multiple
scle-
rosis
[42
].
A
number
of
studies
of
network
training
have
also
found
changes
in
white
matter
with
practice
[4345,46

].
The
finding
of
an
early
change
in
AD
followed
later
by
RD
is
similar
to
the
order
of
changes
found
in
early
develop-
ment
and
raises
the
question
of
whether
examination
of
behavioral
changes
that
occur
with
learning
in
adults
may
provide
added
insight
into
the
slower
changes
manifest
during
child
development
[38].
It
is
only
possible
to
speculate
on
exactly
how
meditation
and
other
forms
of
learning
might
work
to
alter
white
matter,
but
a
recent
Enhancing
attention
Posner,
Rothbart
and
Tang
3
www.sciencedirect.com
Current
Opinion
in
Behavioral
Sciences
2015,
4:15
paper
points
to
frontal
theta
as
a
possible
important
cause
of
these
effects
[47].
Clinical
studies
Many
physical
and
mental
disorders
involve
deficits
of
attention
[48],
so
an
important
goal
of
training
attention
is
to
treat
disorders.
There
are
correlations
between
the
executive
attention
network
and
self
control
or
self
regu-
lation
in
children
and
adults
[49
].
Since
addictions
have
been
related
to
deficits
in
self
regulation
[50]
they
are
a
natural
target
for
such
studies.
One
such
study
has
involved
smoking
addiction
[51

],
but
unlike
most
such
studies
participants
were
not
com-
mitted
to
quit
smoking,
but
were
recruited
for
stress
reduction.
Smokers
and
non
smokers
were
randomly
assigned
to
two
weeks
of
IBMT
or
two
weeks
of
relaxa-
tion
training.
The
IBMT
group
showed
a
60%
reduction
in
smoking
as
measured
by
a
carbon
monoxide
meter,
while
the
control
group
showed
no
significant
reduction.
Prior
to
training
smokers
showed
lower
activity
in
the
anterior
cingulate
than
non
smokers,
but
the
training
produced
an
increase
in
ACC
activation.
The
authors
suggest
that
self
regulation
was
reduced
in
smokers
prior
to
training,
but
increased
after
IBMT
training.
Although
there
is
increased
interest
in
using
training
methods
to
improve
several
clinical
conditions
[48,49
],
research
will
be
needed
to
understand
more
fully
how
this
can
best
be
brought
about.
Future
issues
Training
brain
networks
and
changing
brain
states
have
both
proven
effective
in
some
studies
as
a
means
of
improving
attention.
For
network
training,
the
degree
of
transfer
to
remote
tasks
remains
an
important
issue.
The
claims
that
training
in
a
single
task
improves
vast
areas
of
cognition
seems
to
be
unjustified.
However,
effort
to
train
a
set
of
tasks
that
might
together
make
a
more
general
improvement
in
attention
and
cognition
remains
a
possibility.
State
training
does
not
involve
a
single
task
so
the
issue
of
how
general
the
effects
are
is
rather
different
than
for
network
training.
Some
forms
of
meditation
have
been
shown
to
improve
attention
[3,37],
but
others
may
not.
For
state
training
there
are
remaining
issues
about
which
methods
are
most
effective,
their
mechanisms
and
how
long
they
last
following
training.
Executive
attention
appears
to
be
important
in
achieving
ability
for
self
control.
State
training
through
meditation
has
been
used
to
reduce
addiction
by
increasing
self
regu-
lation.
Other
disorders
involving
attention
may
also
be
improved
by
training,
but
more
research
on
this
is
needed.
Conflict
of
interest
statement
Nothing
declared.
Acknowledgements
This
research
was
supported
by
the
Office
of
Naval
Research
grant
to
the
University
of
Oregon
and
by
the
National
Institute
of
Health
grant
HD060563
to
Georgia
State
University.
References
1.

Tang
YY,
Posner
MI:
Attention
training
and
attention
state
training.
Trends
Cogn
Sci
2009,
13:222-227.
This
paper
classifies
attention
training
methods
into
those
designed
to
alter
particular
brain
networks
and
those
designed
to
change
brain
state.
2.
Tang
YY,
Rothbart
MK,
Posner
MI:
Neural
correlates
of
establishing,
maintaining
and
switching
brain
states.
Trends
Cogn
Sci
2012,
16:330-337.
3.
Tang
YY,
Ma
Y,
Wang
J,
Fan
Y,
Feng
S,
Lu
Q,
Yu
D,
Rothbart
MK,
Fan
M,
Posner
MI:
Short-term
meditation
training
improves
attention
and
self
regulation.
Proc
Natl
Acad
Sci
U
S
A
2007,
104:17152-17156.
4.
Holzel
BK,
Lazar
SK,
Gard
T,
Schuman-Olivier
Z,
Vago
DR,
Ott
U:
How
does
mindfulness
meditation
work?
Proposing
mechanisms
of
action
from
a
conceptual
and
neural
perspective.
Perspect
Psychol
Sci
2011,
6:537-559.
5.
Tang
YY,
Posner
MI,
Rothbart
MK:
Meditation
improves
self-
regulation
over
the
life
span.
Ann
N
Y
Acad
Sci
2014,
1307:104-
111.
6.
Hillman
CH,
Erickson
KI,
Kramer
AF:
Be
smart,
exercise
your
heart:
exercise
effects
on
brain
and
cognition.
Nat
Rev
Neurosci
2008,
9:58-65.
7.
Petersen
SE,
Posner
MI:
The
attention
system
of
the
human
brain:
20
years
after.
Ann
Rev
Neurosci
2012,
35:71-89.
8.
Fan
J,
McCandliss
BD,
Sommer
T,
Raz
A,
Posner
MI:
Testing
the
efficiency
and
independence
of
attentional
networks.
J
Cognit
Neurosci
2002,
3:340-347.
9.
Redick
TS,
Engle
RW:
Working
memory
capacity
and
attention
network
test
performance.
Appl
Cognit
Psychol
2006,
20:713-
721.
10.
Rinne
P,
Hassan
M,
Goniotakis
D,
Chohan
K,
Sharma
P,
Langdon
D,
Soto
D,
Bentley
P:
Triple
dissociation
of
attention
networks
in
stroke
according
to
lesion
location.
Neurology
2013,
81:812-820.
11.
Thimm
M,
Fink
GR,
Ku
¨st
J,
Karbe
H,
Sturm
W:
Impact
of
alertness
training
on
spatial
neglect:
a
behavioural
and
fMRI
study.
Neuropsychology
2006,
44:1230-1246.
12.
Van
Kessel
ME,
Geurts
ACH,
Brouwer
WH,
Fasotti
L:
Visual
scanning
training
for
neglect
after
stroke
with
and
without
a
computerized
lane
tracking
dual
task.
Front
Hum
Neurosci
2013,
7:358.
13.
Green
CS,
Bavelier
D:
Action
video
games
modify
visual
selective
attention.
Nature
2003,
423:534-537.
14.
Boot
WR,
Kramer
AF,
Simons
DJ,
Fabiani
M,
Gratton
G:
The
effects
of
video
game
playing
on
attention,
memory,
and
executive
control.
Acta
Psychol
2008,
129:387-398.
15.

Baniqued
P,
Kranz
MB,
Voss
MW,
Lee
H,
Casman
JD,
Severson
J,
Kramer
AF:
Cognitive
training
with
casual
video
games:
points
to
consider.
Front
Psychol
2014,
4
Article
1010.
Extensive
treatment
of
the
cognitive
effects
of
video
game
training.
16.
Redick
TS,
Shipstead
Z,
Harrison
TL,
Fried
DE,
Hamrick
DZ,
Kane
MJ,
Engle
RW:
No
evidence
of
intelligence
improvement
after
working
memory
training:
a
randomized,
placebo-
controlled
study.
J
Exp
Psychol
General
2013,
142:359-379.
17.
Harrison
TL,
Shipstead
Z,
Hicks
KL,
Hambrick
DZ,
Redick
TS,
Engle
RW:
Working
memory
training
may
increase
working
memory
capacity
but
not
fluid
intelligence.
Psychol
Sci
2013,
24:2409-2419.
4
Cognitive
enhancement
Current
Opinion
in
Behavioral
Sciences
2015,
4:15
www.sciencedirect.com
18.

Diamond
A,
Lee
K:
Interventions
shown
to
aid
executive
function
development
in
children
412
years
old.
Science
2011,
333:959-964.
An
overview
of
methods
of
training
attention
and
executive
function
in
children.
19.
Rueda
MR,
Rothbart
MK,
McCandliss
B,
Saccamanno
L,
Posner
MI:
Training,
maturation
and
genetic
influences
on
the
development
of
executive
attention.
Proc
Natl
Acad
Sci
U
S
A
2005,
102:14931-14936.
20.
Rueda
MR,
Checa
P,
Combita
LM:
Enhanced
efficiency
of
the
executive
attention
network
after
training
in
preschool
children:
immediate
and
after
two
month
effects.
Dev
Cognit
Neurosci
2012,
2(Supplement
1):S192-S204.
21.
Moffitt
TE,
Arseneault
L,
Belsky
D,
Dickson
N,
Hancox
RJ,
Harrington
HL,
Houts
R,
Poulton
R,
Roberts
BW,
Ross
S,
Sears
MR,
Thomson
WM,
Caspi
A:
A
gradient
of
childhood
self-control
predicts
health,
wealth
and
public
safety.
Proc
Natl
Acad
Sci
U
S
A
2011,
108:72693-72698.
22.
Neville
HJ,
Stevens
C,
Pakulak
E,
Bell
TA,
Fanning
J,
Klein
S,
Isbell
E:
Family-based
training
program
improves
brain
function,
cognition,
and
behavior
in
lower
socioeconomic
status
preschoolers.
Proc
Natl
Acad
Sci
U
S
A
2013,
110:
12138-12143.
23.
Utz
KS,
Dimova
V,
Oppenla
¨nder
K,
Kerkhoff
G:
Electrified
minds:
transcranial
direct
current
stimulation
(tDCS)
and
Galvanic
Vestibular
Stimulation
(GVS)
as
methods
of
non-invasive
brain
stimulation
in
neuropsychology—a
review
of
current
data
and
future
implications.
Neuropsychology
2010,
48:2789-2810.
24.
Kadosh
RC,
Levy
N,
O’Shea
J,
Shea
N,
Savulescu
J:
The
neuroethics
of
noninvasive
brain
stimulation.
Curr
Biol
2012,
22:R108-R111.
25.
Smallwood
J,
Schooler
JW:
The
restless
mind.
Psychol
Bull
2006,
132:946-958.
26.
Epernay
M:
The
McLandress
Dimension.
New
York:
Houghton
Mifflin;
1963,
.
27.
Kahneman
D,
Krueger
AB,
Schkade
DA,
Schwarz
N,
Stone
AA:
A
survey
method
for
characterizing
daily
life
experience:
the
day
reconstruction
method.
Science
2004,
306:1776-1780.
28.
Robertson
IH,
Manly
T,
Andrade
J,
Baddeley
BT,
Yend
J:
Oops!
Performance
correlates
of
everyday
attention
failure
in
traumatic
brain
injured
and
normal
subjects.
Neuropsychology
1997,
35:747-758.
29.
Christoff
K,
Gordon
AM,
Smallwood
J,
Schooler
JW:
Experience
sampling
during
fMRI
reveals
default
network
and
executive
system
network
contributions
to
mind
wandering.
Proc
Natl
Acad
Sci
U
S
A
2009,
106:8719-8724.
30.
Ospina
MB,
Bond
K,
Karkhaneh
M,
Buscemi
N,
Dryden
DM,
Barnes
V,
Carlson
LE,
Dusek
JA,
Shannahoff-Khalsa
D:
Clinical
trials
of
meditation
practices
in
health
care:
characteristics
and
quality.
J
Altern
Complement
Med
2008,
14:1199-1213.
31.
Sedlmeier
P,
Eberth
J,
Schwarz
M,
Zimmermann
D,
Haarig
F,
Jaeger
S,
Kunz
S:
The
psychological
effects
of
meditation:
a
meta-analysis.
Psychol
Bull
2012,
138:1139-1171.
32.
Cahn
BR,
Polich
J:
Meditation
states
and
traits.
EEG,
ERP
and
neuroimaging
studies.
Psychol
Bull
2008,
132:180-211.
33.
Vago
DA,
Silbersweig
DA:
Self
awareness,
self
regulation
and
self
transcendence
(S-ART):
a
framework
for
understanding
the
neurobiological
mechanisms
of
mindfulness.
Front
Hum
Neurosci
2012,
6:1-30.
34.
Fox
KCR,
Nijrboer
S,
Dixon
ML,
Floman
JL,
Ellamil
M,
Rumak
SP,
Sedlmeier
P,
Christoff
K:
Is
meditation
associated
with
altered
brain
structure?
A
systematic
review
and
meta-analysis
of
morphometric
neuroimaging
in
meditation
practitioners.
Neurosci
Biobehav
Rev
2014,
43:48-73.
This
study
examines
evidence
for
structural
changes
due
to
meditation.
35.
Tomasino
B,
Fregona
S,
Skrap
M,
Fabbro
F:
Meditation-related
activations
are
modulated
by
the
practices
needed
to
obtain
it
and
by
the
expertise:
an
ALE
meta-analysis
study.
Front
Hum
Neurosci
2013,
6:346.
36.
Lutz
A,
Slagter
HA,
Dunne
JD,
Davidson
RJ:
Attention
regulation
and
monitoring
in
meditation.
Trends
Cognit
Sci
2008,
12:
163-169.
37.
Tang
YY,
Ma
Y,
Fan
Y,
Feng
H,
Wang
J,
Feng
S,
Lu
Q,
Hu
B,
Lin
Y,
Li
J,
Zhang
Y,
Wang
Y,
Zhou
L,
Fan
M:
Central
and
autonomic
nervous
system
interaction
is
altered
by
short
term
meditation.
Proc
Natl
Acad
Sci
U
S
A
2009,
106:8865-8870.
38.
Tang
YY,
Lu
Q,
Fan
M,
Yang
Y,
Posner
MI:
Mechanisms
of
white
matter
changes
induced
by
meditation.
Proc
Natl
Acad
Sci
U
S
A
2012,
109:10570-10574.
39.
Allen
M,
Dietz
M,
Blair
KS,
van
Beek
M,
Rees
G,
Vestergaard-
Poulsen
P,
Lutz
A,
Roepstorff
A:
Cognitive-affective
neural
plasticity
following
active
controlled
mindfulness
intervention.
J
Neurosci
2012,
32:15601-15610.
40.
MacCoon
DG,
MacLean
KA,
Davidson
RJ,
Saron
CD,
Lutz
A:
No
sustained
attention
differences
in
a
longitudinal
randomized
trial
comparing
mindfulness
based
stress
reduction
versus
active
control.
PLOS
ONE
2014,
9:e97551.
41.
MacCoon
DG,
Imel
ZE,
Rosenkranz
MA,
Sheftel
JG,
Weng
HY,
Sullivan
JC,
Bonus
KA,
Stoney
CM,
Salomons
TV,
Davidson
RJ,
Lutz
A:
The
validation
of
an
active
control
intervention
for
Mindfulness
Based
Stress
Reduction
(MBSR).
Behav
Res
Ther
2012,
50:3.
42.
Beirowski
B:
Concepts
for
regulation
of
axon
integrity
by
enwrapping
glia.
Front
Cell
Neurosci
2013,
7:256.
An
important
paper
showing
the
dynamic
nature
of
changes
in
white
matter
in
adults
under
some
circumstances.
43.
Takeuchi
H,
Sekiguchi
A,
Taki
Y,
Yokoyama
S,
Yomogida
Y,
Komuro
N,
Yamanouchi
T,
Suzuki
S,
Kawashima
R:
Training
of
working
memory
impacts
structural
connectivity.
J
Neurosci
2010,
30:3297-3303.
44.
Bengtsson
SL,
Nagy
Z,
Skare
S,
Forsman
L,
Forssberg
H,
Ullen
F:
Extensive
piano
practicing
has
regionally
specific
effects
on
white
matter
development.
Nat
Neurosci
2005,
8:1148-1150.
45.
Hu
YZ,
Geng
FJ,
Tao
L,
Hu
N,
Du
FL,
Fu
KA,
Chen
FY:
Enhanced
white.
Matter
tracts
integrity
in
children
with
abacus
training.
Hum
Brain
Map
2011,
32:10-21.
46.

Zatorre
RJ,
Fields
RD,
Johansen-Berg
H:
Plasticity
in
gray
and
white:
neuroimaging
changes
in
brain
structure
during
learning.
Nat
Neurosci
2012,
15:528-536.
An
excellent
review
of
white
matter
changes
due
to
learning.
47.
Posner
MI,
Tang
YY,
Lynch
G:
Mechanisms
of
white
matter
change
induced
by
meditation
training.
Front
Psychol
2014,
5
Article
1220.
48.
Tang
YY,
Posner
MI,
Rothbart
MK:
Meditation
improves
self-regulation
over
the
life
span.
In
advances
in
meditation
research:
neuroscience
and
clinical
applications.
N
Y
Acad
Sci
2014,
1307:104-111.
49.
Rothbart
MK,
Rueda
MR:
The
development
of
effortful
control.
In
Developing
Attention
in
the
Human
Brain:
A
Tribute
to
Michael
I.
Posner
2005.
Edited
by
Mayr
U,
Awh
E,
Keele
SW.
Washington,
DC:
American
Psychological
Association;
2005:167-188.
50.
Volkow
ND,
Baler
R:
Addiction:
a
disease
of
self
control
in
neurosciences
and
the
human
person:
new
perspectives
of
human
activities.
Pontif
Acad
Sci
Scr
Varia
2013,
121:1-6.
51.

Tang
Y,
Tang
R,
Posner
MI:
Brief
meditation
training
induces
smoking
reduction.
Proc
Natl
Acad
Sci
U
S
A
2013,
110:
13971-13975.
Evidence
that
a
brief
training
period
can
reduce
smoking
even
when
there
is
no
intention
on
the
part
of
the
participant
to
do
so.
Enhancing
attention
Posner,
Rothbart
and
Tang
5
www.sciencedirect.com
Current
Opinion
in
Behavioral
Sciences
2015,
4:15
... Network and state training are two primary approaches for enhancing sustained attention [12]. Network training involves repetitive engagement in a cognitive task, which activates neural networks associated with attention. ...
... Network training involves repetitive engagement in a cognitive task, which activates neural networks associated with attention. In contrast, state training aims to induce a specific brain state believed to impact attention and other networks without focusing on particular cognitive tasks [13][14][15][16][17]. Physical activity and meditation are state training approaches suggested to enhance attention [12,18]. ...
... Numerous studies recommend various methods for attention enhancement. They range from direct training, such as attention state training (AST), to indirect training through video games [26][27][28][29]. However, in recent times, the direction of research has begun to shift. ...
... Attention is a pivotal pathway to healing ourselves and the world, as it is a sensory function that improves mental health and sustainable behavior [8][9][10][11][12][13][14][15][16][17][18][19]. In the past, attention enhancement was often attributed to training interventions [26][27][28][29]. However, recent research has demonstrated efforts to stimulate attention instantaneously [30][31][32]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The climate change crisis is negatively impacting the mental health of people worldwide. Attention is a pivotal pathway to healing ourselves and the world, as it is a sensory process that enhances mental health and promotes sustainable behavior. Despite architecture’s potential to captivate all six human senses immediately, there is still a significant gap in research. Thus, this study aimed to identify architectural features that stimulate attention through the six human senses: visual, touch, auditory, olfaction, taste, and emotion. This review article was conducted by searching data from Scopus in February 2024, identifying 4844 related publications. After data screening following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines, 32 relevant and accessible research works were identified. Finally, data were extracted from the selected articles and analyzed using thematic analysis to explore their relevance to all six senses. The results reveal that the architectural features predominantly eliciting attention are mainly related to visual sensory stimuli. Closely following are the architectural features perceived through the emotional sense. The architecture that promotes attention is minimally associated with touch, auditory, and olfaction senses. Lastly, no architectural features were found to influence attention perceived through the sense of taste. Nevertheless, this study merely synthesizes data from previous research studies. Future research endeavors should validate this study’s findings for broader implications empirically.
... Although pharmacological treatment is often the first-line option for managing the symptoms of inattention in many conditions including ADHD and autism, concerns surrounding long-term use, side effects, and compliance in children warrant the need to explore nonpharmacologic approaches (16,17). Numerous studies suggest that cognitive abilities during development can be improved through various types of training and practice (18)(19)(20)(21)(22)(23), pointing to the value of multimodal approaches to support cognitive development during childhood and adolescence (24). ...
Article
Full-text available
Accessibility to developmental interventions for children and adolescents could be increased through virtual, at-home delivery of training programs. Virtual childhood training programs and their effects on cognitive outcomes have not been well studied. To that end, this study examined the effects of the at-home Brain Balance ® (BB) program on the cognitive task performance of children and adolescents with baseline developmental and attentional difficulties. The study included students with reported diagnosis of ADHD, autism, anxiety, sensory processing disorder, or dyslexia, and students with no reported diagnosis (aged 4-17 years). The at-home BB program is delivered through (1) a computer-based format utilizing multimodal program activities previously studied in-center (multisensory stimulation, gross motor, coordination, balance, and nutritional recommendations); and (2) the BB app (visual motor, auditory and visual processing, and rhythm and timing training)-creating a comprehensive program experience delivered remotely. Cognitive performance was measured by six cognitive tasks from Creyos Health before and after 3 months of participation in the at-home BB program (N = 316) or in-center BB program (N = 4,232), compared to controls. Results showed that overall cognitive assessment scores (including attention, response inhibition, and working memory) improved after participation in either the at-home or the in-center program, compared to controls. Importantly, significant improvements over the controls were observed for two tasks involving attention and inhibitory control, in both programs. Further, two analyses support that the effects on cognitive performance from either delivery format, in-center or at home, are comparable in magnitude. This research: (1) presents new findings demonstrating improved cognitive performance after completing the at-home BB program; (2) replicates previous findings of cognitive improvements after completing the BB program; and (3) suggests that the cognitive effects of virtual at-home BB training are similar to those observed for in-center BB training. Overall, the results demonstrate the effectiveness of the at-home BB program in improving cognitive functioning in pediatric populations with preexisting developmental and attentional difficulties. Virtual delivery and ease of use, provide at-home programs the potential to reduce barriers of access to much-needed developmental and cognitive support, for individuals who may otherwise lack access to high-quality, evidence-based developmental programs.
... Studies demonstrate that metacognitive monitoring can be developed and improved through training (Baird, Mrazek, Phillips, & Schooler, 2014). For instance, attentional processes can be developed and enhanced through the repeated practice of attention-based tasks (Posner et al., 2015). Metacognitive training such as mindfulness techniques is integral to both Cognitive Behavior Therapy (CBT; Dobson, 2013) and Metacognitive Therapy (MCT; Normann & Morina, 2018) and facilitates improved control over maladaptive thoughts and emotions (Wells, 2011(Wells, , 2019Hagen et al., 2017). ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
This paper investigates the computational mechanisms underlying a type of metacognitive monitoring known as detached mindfulness, a particularly effective therapeutic technique within cognitive psychology. While research strongly supports the capacity of detached mindfulness to reduce depression and anxiety, its cognitive and computational underpinnings remain largely unexplained. We employ a computational model of metacognitive skill to articulate the mechanisms through which a detached perception of affect reduces emotional reactivity.
... Studies demonstrate that metacognitive monitoring can be developed and improved through training (Baird, Mrazek, Phillips, & Schooler, 2014). For instance, attentional processes can be developed and enhanced through the repeated practice of attention-based tasks (Posner et al., 2015). Metacognitive training such as mindfulness techniques is integral to both Cognitive Behavior Therapy (CBT; Dobson, 2013) and Metacognitive Therapy (MCT; Normann & Morina, 2018) and facilitates improved control over maladaptive thoughts and emotions (Wells, 2011(Wells, , 2019Hagen et al., 2017). ...
Article
Objective: Attention regulation is an essential ability in daily life that affects learning and work efficiency and is closely related to mental health. The effectiveness of brain-computer interface (BCI) systems in attention regulation has been proven, but most of these systems rely on bulky and expensive equipment and are still in the experimental stage. This study proposes a wearable BCI system for real-time attention regulation and cognitive monitoring. Methods: The BCI system integrates a wearable singlechannel electroencephalogram (EEG) headband with wireless data streaming for real-time analysis. Twenty healthy subjects participated in the long-term attention regulation experiment and were evenly divided into an experimental group and a control group based on the presence of realtime neurofeedback. The neurofeedback is represented by output value of attention, which calculated from singlechannel EEG data. Before and after the regulation sessions, baseline assessments were conducted for each subject, incorporating multi-channel EEG data analysis and cognitive behavioral evaluations, to verify the effectiveness of system for attention regulation. Results: The online experimental results indicate that the average attention level in the experimental group increased from 0.625 to 0.812, while no significant improvement was observed in the control group. Further comparative analysis revealed the reasons for the enhancement of attention regulation ability in terms of both brain network patterns and cognitive performance. Significance: The proposed wearable BCI system is effective at improving attention regulation ability and could be a promising tool for assisting people with attention disorders.
Article
Full-text available
Early childhood marks a pivotal period in the maturation of executive function, the cognitive ability to consciously regulate actions and thoughts. Mindfulness-based interventions have shown promise in bolstering executive function in children. This study used the functional near-infrared spectroscopy technique to explore the impact of mindfulness-based training on young children. Brain imaging data were collected from 68 children (41 boys, aged 61.8 ± 10.7 months) who were randomly assigned to either an intervention group (N = 37, aged 60.03 ± 11.14 months) or a control group (N = 31, aged 59.99 ± 10.89 months). Multivariate and multiscale sample entropy analyses were used. The results showed that: (1) brain complexity was reduced in the intervention group after receiving the mindfulness-based intervention in all three executive function tasks (ps < 0.05), indicating a more efficient neural processing mechanism after the intervention; (2) difference comparisons between the intervention and control groups showed significant differences in relevant brain regions during cognitive shifting (left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and medial prefrontal cortex) and working memory tasks (left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex), which corroborates with improved behavioral results in the intervention group (Z = −3.674, P < 0.001 for cognitive shifting; Z = 2.594, P < 0.01 for working memory). These findings improve our understanding of early brain development in young children and highlight the neural mechanisms by which mindfulness-based interventions affect executive function. Implications for early intervention to promote young children’s brain development are also addressed.
Article
Objective This systematic review, performed in accordance with the PRISMA guidelines, seeks to summarize the interventions that have been developed in order to improve executive functioning and attention in children born prematurely. Methods The PICOS framework helped guide the structure and relevant terms selected for the study. Electronic systematic searches of the databases PubMed (NLM), Ovid Medline, Ovid All EBM Reviews, Ovid Embase, and Ovid PsycINFO were completed in March 2022. This review focuses on interventions that target attention and executive functioning in prematurely born children between birth and 12 years old, with outcome measures assessed between 3 and 12 years old, even if the age range in the study can exceed our own parameters. Data extraction included sample characteristics, country of recruitment, type of intervention, description of the intervention group and control group, outcome measures, and overall results. An assessment of the quality of methodology of studies was performed through an adaptation of the Downs and Black checklist for both randomized and nonrandomized studies in healthcare interventions. An assessment of the risk of bias was also presented using the Cochrane risk of bias tool for randomized trials 2.0. Results A total of 517 premature children received an intervention at some point between birth and early adolescence. Eleven different interventions were assessed in 17 studies, with rating of the quality of methodology and outcomes ranging from lower quality studies (44% quality rating) to robust studies (96% quality rating) in terms of reporting standards, external and internal validity, and power. Five of those studies focused on interventions administered in the neonatal intensive care unit or shortly postdischarge (e.g., the Mother–Infant Transaction Program and the Newborn Individualized Developmental Care and Assessment Program, documented in two articles each [11%] or the Infant Behavioral Assessment and Intervention Program assessed in one study [about 5%]), while 12 articles reported on interventions administered between the ages of 1.5–12 years old [mostly computerized cognitive training programs such as Cogmed (23%) and BrainGame Brian (17%)]. Of the 17 articles examined, 12 (70%) showed positive short-term outcomes postintervention and 3 (17%) demonstrated positive long-term results with small to large effect sizes (0.23–2.3). Among included studies, 50% showed an overall high risk of bias, 21.4% showed some concerns, and 28.6% were low risk of bias. Conclusions Due to the heterogeneity of the programs reviewed, the presented findings should be interpreted as descriptive results. A careful and individualized selection from the various available interventions should be made based on the target population (i.e., age at intervention administration and outcome testing) before implementing these program protocols in clinical settings.
Article
Full-text available
Training can induce changes in specific brain networks and changes in brain state. In both cases it has been found that the efficiency of white matter as measured by diffusion tensor imaging is increased, often after only a few hours of training. In this paper we consider a plausible molecular mechanism for how state change produced by meditation might lead to white matter change. According to this hypothesis frontal theta induced by meditation produces a molecular cascade that increases myelin and improves connectivity.
Article
Full-text available
Background Mindfulness Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) is a secular form of meditation training. The vast majority of the extant literature investigating the health effects of mindfulness interventions relies on wait-list control comparisons. Previous studies have found that meditation training over several months is associated with improvements in cognitive control and attention. Methodology/Principal Findings We used a visual continuous performance task (CPT) to test the effects of eight weeks of mindfulness training on sustained attention by comparing MBSR to the Health Enhancement Program (HEP), a structurally equivalent, active control condition in a randomized, longitudinal design (ClinicalTrials.gov, NCT01301105) focusing on a non-clinical population typical of MBSR participants. Researchers were blind to group assignment. 63 community participants were randomized to either MBSR (n = 31) or HEP (n = 32). CPT analyses were conducted on 29 MBSR participants and 25 HEP participants. We predicted that MBSR would improve visual discrimination ability and sustained attention over time on the CPT compared to HEP, with more home practice associated with greater improvements. Our hypotheses were not confirmed but we did find some evidence for improved visual discrimination similar to effects in partial replication of other research. Our study had sufficient power to demonstrate that intervention groups do not differ in their improvement over time in sustained attention performance. One of our primary predictions concerning the effects of intervention on attentional fatigue was significant but not interpretable. Conclusions Attentional sensitivity is not affected by mindfulness practice as taught in MBSR, but it is unclear whether mindfulness might positively affect another aspect of attention, vigilance. These results also highlight the relevant procedural modifications required by future research to correctly investigate the role of sustained attention in similar samples. Trial Registration ClinicalTrials.gov, NCT01301105
Article
Full-text available
Numerous studies have begun to address how the brain's grey and white matter may be shaped by meditation. This research is yet to be integrated, however, and two fundamental questions remain: Is meditation associated with altered brain structure? If so, what is the magnitude of these differences? To address these questions, we reviewed and meta-analyzed 123 brain morphology differences from 21 neuroimaging studies examining ∼300 meditation practitioners. Anatomical likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analysis found eight brain regions consistently altered in meditators, including areas key to meta-awareness (frontopolar cortex/BA 10), exteroceptive and interoceptive body awareness (sensory cortices and insula), memory consolidation and reconsolidation (hippocampus), self and emotion regulation (anterior and mid cingulate; orbitofrontal cortex), and intra- and interhemispheric communication (superior longitudinal fasciculus; corpus callosum). Effect size meta-analysis (calculating 132 effect sizes from 16 studies) suggests a global ‘medium’ effect size (Cohen's d¯ = 0.46; r¯ = .19). Publication bias and methodological limitations are strong concerns, however; further research using rigorous methods is required to definitively link meditation practice to altered brain morphology.
Article
Full-text available
Brain training programs have proliferated in recent years, with claims that video games or computer-based tasks can broadly enhance cognitive function. However, benefits are commonly seen only in trained tasks. Assessing generalized improvement and practicality of laboratory exercises complicates interpretation and application of findings. In this study, we addressed these issues by using active control groups, training tasks that more closely resemble real-world demands and multiple tests to determine transfer of training. We examined whether casual video games can broadly improve cognition, and selected training games from a study of the relationship between game performance and cognitive abilities. A total of 209 young adults were randomized into a working memory–reasoning group, an adaptive working memory–reasoning group, an active control game group, and a no-contact control group. Before and after 15 h of training, participants completed tests of reasoning, working memory, attention, episodic memory, perceptual speed, and self-report measures of executive function, game experience, perceived improvement, knowledge of brain training research, and game play outside the laboratory. Participants improved on the training games, but transfer to untrained tasks was limited. No group showed gains in reasoning, working memory, episodic memory, or perceptual speed, but the working memory–reasoning groups improved in divided attention, with better performance in an attention-demanding game, a decreased attentional blink and smaller trail-making costs. Perceived improvements did not differ across training groups and those with low reasoning ability at baseline showed larger gains. Although there are important caveats, our study sheds light on the mixed effects in the training and transfer literature and offers a novel and potentially practical training approach. Still, more research is needed to determine the real-world benefits of computer programs such as casual games.
Article
Full-text available
Long axons and their enwrapping glia (EG; Schwann cells (SCs) and oligodendrocytes (OLGs)) form a unique compound structure that serves as conduit for transport of electric and chemical information in the nervous system. The peculiar cytoarchitecture over an enormous length as well as its substantial energetic requirements make this conduit particularly susceptible to detrimental alterations. Degeneration of long axons independent of neuronal cell bodies is observed comparatively early in a range of neurodegenerative conditions as a consequence of abnormalities in SCs and OLGs . This leads to the most relevant disease symptoms and highlights the critical role that these glia have for axon integrity, but the underlying mechanisms remain elusive. The quest to understand why and how axons degenerate is now a crucial frontier in disease-oriented research. This challenge is most likely to lead to significant progress if the inextricable link between axons and their flanking glia in pathological situations is recognized. In this review I compile recent advances in our understanding of the molecular programs governing axon degeneration, and mechanisms of EG's non-cell autonomous impact on axon-integrity. A particular focus is placed on emerging evidence suggesting that EG nurture long axons by virtue of their intimate association, release of trophic substances, and neurometabolic coupling. The correction of defects in these functions has the potential to stabilize axons in a variety of neuronal diseases in the peripheral nervous system and central nervous system (PNS and CNS).
Article
Full-text available
Working memory is a critical element of complex cognition, particularly under conditions of distraction and interference. Measures of working memory capacity correlate positively with many measures of real-world cognition, including fluid intelligence. There have been numerous attempts to use training procedures to increase working memory capacity and thereby performance on the real-world tasks that rely on working memory capacity. In the study reported here, we demonstrated that training on complex working memory span tasks leads to improvement on similar tasks with different materials but that such training does not generalize to measures of fluid intelligence.
Article
Full-text available
The use of meditation to improve emotion and attention regulation has a long history in Asia and there are many practitioners in Western countries. Much of the evidence on the effectiveness of meditation is either anecdotal or a comparison of long-term meditators with controls matched in age and health. Recently, it has been possible to establish changes in self-regulation in undergraduate students after only 5 days of meditation practice, allowing randomized trials comparing effects of meditation with other self-control methods such as relaxation training. Early studies took place in Chinese universities; however, similar effects have been obtained with U.S. undergraduates, and with Chinese children aged 4.5 years and older Chinese participants aged 65 years. Studies using neuroimaging techniques have shown that meditation improves activation and connectivity in brain areas related to self-regulation, and these findings may provide an opportunity to examine remediation of mental disorders in a new light.
Article
Full-text available
More than 5 million deaths a year are attributable to tobacco smoking, but attempts to help people either quit or reduce their smoking often fail, perhaps in part because the intention to quit activates brain networks related to craving. We recruited participants interested in general stress reduction and randomly assigned them to meditation training or a relaxation training control. Among smokers, 2 wk of meditation training (5 h in total) produced a significant reduction in smoking of 60%; no reduction was found in the relaxation control. Resting-state brain scans showed increased activity for the meditation group in the anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex, brain areas related to self-control. These results suggest that brief meditation training improves self-control capacity and reduces smoking.
Article
To determine whether behavioral dissociations and interactions occur between the attentional functions-alerting, orienting, and conflict resolution-depending upon stroke location and to determine the approximate proportion of patients who can be classified into 1 of these 3 anatomical networks. We recruited 110 anatomically unselected acute stroke patients and 62 age-matched controls. Subjects underwent the attention network test (ANT), which provides a measure of each attention type, and their performance related to lesion anatomy on MRI using a voxel-lesion mapping approach. Patients as a whole performed poorer than controls, but there were no group differences in the size of attentional effects. Specific deficits in 1 of the 3 ANT-tested functions were found in the following lesion locations: alerting deficiency with bilateral anteromedial thalamus and upper brainstem (17% of patients); orienting impairment with right pulvinar and right temporoparietal cortex (15%); conflict resolution with bilateral prefrontal and premotor areas (23%). Lesions to right frontoparietal regions also modified interactions among the 3 types of attention. More than half of all stroke patients can be expected to have a lesion location classifiable into 1 of the 3 principal attention networks. Our results have potential implications for therapy personalization in focal brain diseases including stroke.