ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Objective: This study sought to investigate the dynamics of attentional focus and cognitive control during endurance activity from a metacognitive perspective. The study also intended to examine the situational factors which may influence cognitive strategy use by elite endurance runners. Design: Semi-structured qualitative interviews were utilised. Method: Ten elite-level endurance runners were interviewed to explore retrospectively their attentional focus and cognitive strategy use during endurance running. Results: The findings revealed that metacognitive strategies such as planning, monitoring, reviewing and evaluating, and metacognitive experiences were fundamental to cognitive control and cognitive strategy use in elite endurance runners. The findings also added to the array of active self-regulatory strategies previously reported in the literature. Conclusions: These results suggest that metacognitive processes are central to effective cognitive control in elite endurance athletes during running. The findings allowed for the development of an integrative metacognitive framework, which incorporates dimensions of attentional focus. This model may better represent the processes which underpin cognitive control and determine cognitive strategy use in elite athletes during endurance running.
Accepted Manuscript
Metacognitive processes in the self-regulation of performance in elite endurance
runners
Noel Brick, Tadhg MacIntyre, Mark Campbell
PII: S1469-0292(15)00010-2
DOI: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2015.02.003
Reference: PSYSPO 975
To appear in: Psychology of Sport & Exercise
Received Date: 14 November 2014
Revised Date: 20 January 2015
Accepted Date: 17 February 2015
Please cite this article as: Brick, N., MacIntyre, T., Campbell, M., Metacognitive processes in the self-
regulation of performance in elite endurance runners, Psychology of Sport & Exercise (2015), doi:
10.1016/j.psychsport.2015.02.003.
This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to
our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo
copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please
note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all
legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
1
1
Metacognitive processes in the self-regulation of performance in elite endurance
2
runners
3
4
5
Noel Brick, Tadhg MacIntyre,
& Mark Campbell
6
Department of Physical Education and Sport Sciences, University of Limerick, Limerick,
7
Ireland
8
9
Corresponding Author: Noel Brick
10
Address: Department of Physical Education and Sport Sciences,
11
University of Limerick, Limerick, Ireland
12
Tele: +44 28 71675366
13
E-mail: n.brick@ulster.ac.uk
14
15
16
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
1
1
2
3
4
5
6
Metacognitive processes in the self-regulation of performance in elite endurance
7
runners
8
9
10
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
2
Abstract
1
Objective: This study sought to investigate the dynamics of attentional focus and cognitive
2
control during endurance activity from a metacognitive perspective. The study also intended
3
to examine the situational factors which may influence cognitive strategy use by elite
4
endurance runners.
5
Design: Semi-structured qualitative interviews were utilised.
6
Method: Ten elite-level endurance runners were interviewed to explore retrospectively their
7
attentional focus and cognitive strategy use during endurance running.
8
Results: The findings revealed that metacognitive strategies such as planning, monitoring,
9
reviewing and evaluating, and metacognitive experiences were fundamental to cognitive
10
control and cognitive strategy use in elite endurance runners. The findings also added to the
11
array of active self-regulatory strategies previously reported in the literature.
12
Conclusions: These results suggest that metacognitive processes are central to effective
13
cognitive control in elite endurance athletes during running. The findings allowed for the
14
development of an integrative metacognitive framework, which incorporates dimensions of
15
attentional focus. This model may better represent the processes which underpin cognitive
16
control and determine cognitive strategy use in elite athletes during endurance running.
17
18
Keywords
19
Metacognition; cognitive strategy; attentional focus; self-regulation; endurance exercise
20
21
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
3
Introduction
1
The study of attentional focus in endurance activity has operated on a largely atheoretical
2
basis since its inception almost four decades ago. While subsequent research has progressed
3
our understanding of how cognitions – both deliberate and spontaneous – impact endurance
4
performance (see Brick, MacIntyre, & Campbell, 2014 for a detailed review), the need for a
5
comprehensive conceptual framework still exists. Recent proposals include a social-cognitive
6
perspective (Tenenbaum, 2001), Leventhal and Everhart’s (1979) parallel processing model
7
of pain (Brewer & Buman, 2006), and a mindfulness approach (Salmon, Hanneman, &
8
Harwood, 2010).
9
The above approaches allude to potential mechanisms to explain how specific
10
cognitions may allow endurance performers better tolerate exertional discomfort. For
11
example, Tenenbaum’s (2001) social-cognitive perspective considers the multidimensional
12
nature of effort tolerance and perceived exertion. Similarly, Brewer and Buman’s (2006)
13
application of the parallel processing model provides an insight on how attentional foci may
14
alter pain perception. Some issues remain unaddressed, however. Brewer and Buman (2006),
15
for example, expressed a need to clarify how individuals develop schemata, or cognitive
16
structures developed from previous pain experiences, to accurately evaluate exertional signals
17
during exercise. Concomitantly, we further highlight the need for a framework to illustrate
18
how endurance performers control cognitive activity to optimise performance.
19
More recently, researchers have sought to better understand mental processes in
20
athletic performance from the perspective of cognitive sport psychology (Moran, 2009,
21
2012). Theoretical approaches, such as grounded cognition recognise the interaction between
22
perception, action, the body, and the environment during goal achievement (e.g., Barsalou,
23
2008). When these interactions pose a significant challenge, such as during effortful
24
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
4
endurance running, a high level of cognitive control, or the ability to ‘regulate, coordinate,
1
and sequence thoughts and actions in accordance with internally maintained behavioural
2
goals’ (Braver, 2012; p. 106) should be important. In such situations, a focus of attention
3
which best facilitates performance may be considered an imperative to competitive success.
4
To emphasise the significance of cognitive control, much research evidence supports
5
the contention that attentional focus impacts endurance performance (e.g., Brick et al., 2014;
6
Schücker, Knopf, Strauss, & Hagemann, 2014). Amongst elite performers, task-relevant, self-
7
regulatory cognitive strategies have been shown to facilitate performance improvement,
8
while distractive thoughts may result in non-optimal pacing (e.g., Clingman & Hilliard, 1990;
9
Rushall & Shewchuk, 1989). What is less clear is when, or why endurance athletes engage
10
specific attentional strategies. It has been suggested that elite performers employ cognitive
11
strategies depending on circumstance and need (e.g., Moran, 1996). However, little is
12
understood about the determinants of cognitive strategy use amongst elite endurance athletes.
13
One framework which may help to address these conceptual issues is the
14
metacognitive approach. Metacognition has been defined as an individual’s insight into, and
15
control over their own mental processes (Flavell, 1979), and is a key sub-process of, and
16
essential to effective self-regulation (Tarricone, 2011). Efklides (2006) describes
17
metacognition as a model of cognition, acting at a meta-level, and related to cognition
18
through monitoring and control functions. Thus, meta-cognition implies two (or more)
19
processes, one concerning cognitions of external objects (i.e. object-level cognition), and a
20
second, the meta-level, concerning cognitions of object-level cognitions (Nelson, 1996).
21
Metacognitive process include metacognitive strategies (or metacognitive skills) such
22
as planning and monitoring, and metacognitive experiences (Efklides, 2006; Tarricone,
23
2011). Based on monitoring processes, metacognitive experiences allow for concurrent, or
24
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
5
‘on-line’ monitoring during task performance. They include metacognitive feelings, which
1
inform the individual about task performance in the form of a feeling, such as feelings of
2
difficulty, and tend to be implicit in nature (Efklides, 2006). Alternatively, metacognitive
3
judgements and estimates, such as judgement of solution correctness, are made by the
4
individual, and may be the result of both implicit, non-analytic processes, and explicit,
5
analytic processes (Efklides, 2006). Collectively, awareness of metacognitive experiences, in
6
conjunction with performance, forms a representation of the task, or the context (Efklides,
7
2014). In turn, these metacognitive representations provide input for conscious, deliberate
8
regulation and control of cognition via cognitive, or metacognitive strategies (Efklides,
9
2014). Applied to the current study of endurance running, metacognitive representations may
10
indicate the perceived difficulty of a running task, for example, and provide the impetus for
11
the initiation of an appropriate cognitive strategy to control attentional focus.
12
A metacognitive framework has the potential to enhance our understanding of self-
13
regulation and cognitive control during endurance activity. Precedent for this contention can
14
be found in physical activity (e.g., Settanni, Magistro, & Rabaglietti, 2012), and pain
15
management (e.g., Yoshida et al., 2012) settings, for example. Metacognition has also been
16
considered a distinguishing feature of expert performance in the sporting domain (MacIntyre,
17
Igou, Campbell, Moran, & Matthews, 2014). However, few researchers have specifically
18
employed a metacognitive perspective to investigate attentional dynamics in endurance
19
activity. Only Nietfeld (2003) highlighted the significance of metacognitive monitoring and
20
strategy use during endurance running. Consequently, the role of metacognitive processes in
21
controlling cognition during endurance performance has yet to be fully explored.
22
The primary aims of the present qualitative investigation were firstly to apply a
23
metacognitive approach to better understand the influences on, and dynamics of attentional
24
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
6
focus and cognitive control during endurance activity. The emphasis was on elite endurance
1
runners, to determine cognitive strategy use during both competition and endurance training.
2
Employing this strength-based approach, high-ability participants were deliberately recruited
3
on the basis of their expertise and experience in endurance activity, and potential for highly
4
developed cognitive abilities (e.g., MacIntyre, Moran, Collet, & Guillot, 2013; MacIntyre et
5
al., 2014). Combined with a theory-driven analysis of cognitive activity, (i.e. metacognition),
6
the convergence of these approaches (MacIntyre et al, 2013) may advance our understanding
7
of attentional focus and cognitive control during endurance running. The second key aim of
8
the study was to more clearly illustrate the situational factors which may influence the
9
attentional focus and cognitive strategy use by elite endurance runners.
10
Method
11
Participants
12
Elite endurance runners were purposefully sampled for the present study. Following
13
institutional ethical approval, a recruitment email was sent to prospective athletes via the
14
national endurance coach. Potential participants were also contacted via email. Inclusion
15
criteria were that runners had competed internationally at senior-level running competition
16
during their career and still ran competitively in events ranging from 3000m to ultra-distance
17
(e.g. 24-hour, 100km). The sampling procedure provided a total of 10 athletes who met these
18
criteria and were willing to participate. Considering the idiographic aims of the study (e.g.,
19
Côté, Salmela, Baria, & Russell, 1993), the sample size was considered appropriate to allow
20
individual cases to be represented in the data, and for a sufficiently intensive analysis of each
21
case to be conducted (Robinson, 2013). Employing a classification system proposed by
22
Swann, Moran, and Piggott (2015), two of the athletes were classified as successful elite, and
23
eight were classified as competitive elite. See Table 1 for participant demographics.
24
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
7
[INSERT TABLE 1 NEAR HERE]
1
Data Collection
2
Pre-Interview information. Approximately one week prior to interview, each
3
participant was emailed a pre-interview information sheet (see appendix 1). The purpose was
4
to familiarise participants with the area of research, the procedures involved, and to clarify
5
the purpose of the study (Wagstaff, Fletcher, & Hanton, 2012).
6
Qualitative interview guide. Given the limited knowledge available on
7
metacognitive activity during endurance running, a qualitative approach to data collection
8
was considered best suited to this study. A semi-structured interview guide was developed
9
based on a review of the attentional focus literature (see Brick et al., 2014), and on relevant
10
accounts from the metacognition literature (e.g., Efklides, 2006; Tarricone, 2011). The format
11
and structure of the guide derived from reviewing previous studies with an exploratory intent
12
(e.g., Sarkar & Fletcher, 2014; Wagstaff et al., 2012). Prior to the study, the interview guide
13
was piloted with three endurance athletes, and was subsequently refined for clarity and
14
content. The finalised guide (see appendix 2) consisted of six sections, and explored the
15
athletes’ mental preparation for running, their cognitive strategy use during running (both
16
competition and training), the athletes’ monitoring of attentional foci and cognitive strategy
17
effectiveness, and how they acquired, developed, and refined the cognitive strategies used.
18
Interviews. Initial exploration required the athletes to retrospectively recount their
19
attentional focus and cognitive strategy use during endurance running. Subsequently,
20
participants were provided a list of attentional foci and cognitive strategies typically used by
21
runners (see Brick et al., 2014). Participants were invited to discuss their use of both the
22
attentional foci dimensions on this list, and any other strategies they might employ. Nine of
23
the interviews were conducted face-to-face, while one interview was completed via
24
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
8
telephone. All interviews were conducted by the first author, and each participant gave
1
written informed consent prior to commencement. The interviews lasted between 55 and 98
2
minutes (M = 75.5 min, SD = 13.5). Each interview was digitally recorded and transcribed
3
verbatim for subsequent analysis. Member checking was completed by returning transcripts
4
to the interviewee within one week of interview to review for accuracy.
5
Data Analysis
6
Qualitative interview data. There are many differing methodological approaches to
7
analysing qualitative data, including grounded theory, and discourse analysis (Vaismoradi,
8
Turunen, & Bondas, 2013). Given the exploratory nature of the present study, however, the
9
most suitable approach was considered to be a content analysis (Green & Thorogood, 2004).
10
According to Elo and Kyngäs (2008), there are three phases to content analysis;
11
preparation, organising, and reporting of the data. Following transcription of the interview,
12
the first author initially immersed himself in the interview data. Because a metacognitive
13
perspective was employed to analyse the interview transcripts, a deductive approach was
14
considered the most suitable modality for initial data analysis (Elo & Kyngäs, 2008). After
15
further consideration, it was decided to analyse both manifest and latent content in the data,
16
given that some metacognitive processes may be non-conscious in nature (Efklides, 2006).
17
Units of analysis relevant to attentional focus and cognitive control during running included
18
single words, sentences and more complete paragraphs.
19
To organise the data, a categorisation matrix was developed using both a conceptual
20
framework of metacognition (Tarricone, 2011) and Efklides’ (2006) facets of metacognition.
21
The data was reviewed for content and coded for correspondence with these categories.
22
Analysis was not constrained to the categories of the conceptual framework, however. As
23
analysis of each transcript continued, and content emerged from the data, new categories and
24
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
9
subcategories were created and defined, thus following the principles of inductive content
1
analysis within a broader deductive analysis (Elo & Kyngäs, 2008).
2
To check credibility, and to enhance the trustworthiness and quality of the analysis,
3
the researchers periodically discussed the emergent categories and reached agreement
4
through constructive debate (e.g., Sarkar & Fletcher, 2014). To ensure reliability between the
5
classification of raw data and the content of the transcripts, the researchers independently
6
analysed the data using the categorisation matrix. A follow-up meeting took place to discuss
7
the consistency of analysis and refine the matrix. Finally, a second reliability check was
8
performed on the classification process. For this, an independent analyst analysed a random
9
sample (20%) of the transcripts. Following familiarisation with the classification system and
10
subsequent analysis, further refinements were made to the categorisation matrix, after which
11
greater than 80% agreement was reached with the independent analyst. With consensus
12
reached, categories were established and the results were synthesised.
13
Results
14
The findings from the interview data were organised under two broad cognitive and
15
metacognitive dimensions; Regulation of Cognition, and Metacognitive Experiences.
16
Presentation of the results will focus primarily on the dimensions that emerged from the data,
17
and specifically on the categories and subcategories that either influenced, or resulted from
18
the control of cognition during running. The findings are presented using quotations from the
19
interviews to illustrate the metacognitive processes influencing cognitive control during
20
running. The range of cognitive and metacognitive processes are presented fully in Figure 1.
21
[INSERT FIGURE 1 NEAR HERE]
22
Regulation of Cognition
23
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
10
Planning before running. Planning before running consisted of two categories, plan
1
for competition, and plan for training. The most frequently cited planning for competition
2
subcategories were plan race tactics and pacing, plan race objectives, and plan other
3
cognitive strategies, and both successful elite runners reported each of these processes. A
4
minority of athletes discussed planning for training. Though most reported planning alone,
5
some athletes planned race objectives (three athletes), and race tactics and pacing (two
6
athletes) with their coach. Tactics and pacing for longer races, such as marathons, focused
7
primarily on individuals’ own performance. For shorter races, however, athletes were also apt
8
to consider potential competitors, as one successful elite athlete recounted:
9
I’d be thinking about like who’s in the race and so for international races you could,
10
kind of, look at what races they ran previously and how they did and how their form
11
is, and, ah, then I would be checking out the route of the race and the map of the race
12
and be looking at that. And, ah, yea different like points in the race, whether it be like
13
say cross-country… you’d usually go walk the course and decide, like, if you’re going
14
to put in any tactics and where you’re going to make your moves, or if you’re going to
15
sit in and stuff.
16
Most athletes planned other cognitive strategy use (i.e. other than race objectives,
17
tactics and pacing) by themselves or with their coach, while three athletes reported planning
18
cognitive strategies with a psychologist. No athletes reported specifically planning cognitive
19
strategy use before training, however. The following quote from one of the successful elite
20
runners typifies an approach to planning cognitive strategy use before a race:
21
So, I’d have it planned before, and I haven’t really had a race where I haven’t been
22
able to think what I want, bar things were going so bad it was just like, you know,
23
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
11
your head wasn’t in it as such like. That doesn’t happen very often, but, ah, I’d have
1
planned like I’m going to think about my breathing or do the posture checks or I’m
2
going to have this song that I’ve been using in training anyway, you know.
3
While these results indicate the importance of planning, many aspects of competitive
4
running cannot be planned for. To emphasise this point, athletes indicated that many
5
cognitive strategies were implemented in reaction to situational events that occurred during
6
running. As such, the importance of monitoring relevant information and responding in an
7
appropriate manner was highlighted, and was the next category to emerge from the data.
8
Monitoring during running. Monitoring during running consisted of both internal
9
sensory monitoring, and outward monitoring. The most frequently cited internal sensory
10
monitoring subcategories were monitor bodily sensations, and monitor overall effort or feel.
11
Bodily sensations monitored during running included exertional pain and muscular fatigue,
12
breathing, thirst and nutritional needs, and body movement and form. Internal sensory
13
monitoring was typically used for informational purposes to control cognition. For example,
14
while many athletes reported awareness of exertional pain during running, this awareness was
15
primarily used as a signal to engage an appropriate cognitive strategy. During competition,
16
the purpose was to divert attentional focus from pain sensations and maintain performance. In
17
contrast, during training exertional pain was used by some athletes (40%) to monitor their
18
response to the training load. Both contexts were epitomised by one marathon runner:
19
If you are hurting, and you are in pain …it’s part of the race, you expect that anyway,
20
subconsciously you just push through it anyway. Whereas in training, it’s something
21
that you’d sometimes keep in check, because you wouldn’t want to push through that
22
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
12
to the same extent. You’ve got to keep in mind that you’ve got another couple of
1
intervals to do, or that it’s part of a long-term plan...
2
Monitoring overall effort and feel was predominantly used by athletes to gauge
3
running intensity and pacing. While eight runners reported monitoring overall effort and feel,
4
many (50%) of the athletes also recounted how they associated a feeling of effort with
5
running pace during training, and this feeling was subsequently used to gauge running
6
intensity. For example, one athlete reported about their marathon training:
7
I know what it should feel like… So if I’m doing... like a lot of my tempo runs were
8
surprisingly easy… our tempo runs are like 6:10 or 6:20 pace, and I was targeting six
9
minute miling [sic], ah, so, I feel absolutely fine at 6:10 pace like, you know.... So I
10
know that feeling, so it’s about the way I should feel during it…
11
The athletes reported using outward monitoring more often during competition than
12
training, and frequently cited subcategories were monitor split-times for pacing, monitor
13
other runners during racing, and monitor course/route/terrain. Perhaps unsurprisingly for
14
competitive athletes, monitoring other runners during racing was important for pacing and
15
tactical decisions. The need to monitor the running course and terrain was also important for
16
pacing or tactical decisions, particularly for athletes who ran cross-country or trail courses.
17
Thus, the information athletes gleaned via internal sensory and outward monitoring
18
appeared to play a pivotal role in cognitive control and the adoption of a suitable attentional
19
focus to cope with the demands of the running task.
20
Controlling cognition during running. The importance of cognitive control by
21
means of active self-regulatory strategies was emphasised by both the number of athletes
22
reporting active self-regulatory strategy use, and by the range of idiosyncratic strategies
23
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
13
revealed (see Figure 1). Active self-regulatory strategies recounted by each of the elite
1
endurance runners were pacing and tactical decisions, relaxation, and chunking distance or
2
time. Often, active self-regulatory strategies were used in combination, as conveyed by one
3
runner describing their cognitions over a half-marathon distance:
4
…and then you obviously want to focus on your running form really itself and making
5
sure that you’re trying to keep as relaxed as possible and just keep the same rhythm
6
ticking over, and yea, keeping the breathing just as relaxed as possible as well. Yea,
7
just try keep focused on everything you’re doing and making sure that you’re running
8
at a pace that’s being sustained for the 13 miles.
9
Active self-regulatory strategies served many distinct purposes. For example,
10
chunking distance or time was predominantly used to break down the perceived challenge of
11
longer distance runs, or intense interval sessions and maintain a present moment focus.
12
Furthermore, athletes attended to running technique when running was difficult (e.g., running
13
uphill), as part of a periodic check, or during situations when fatigue, and a deterioration of
14
movement efficiency may have been a concern. Typically, for running technique athletes
15
would focus on task-relevant cues such as maintaining an efficient running ‘form’, using their
16
arms, keeping elbows in, or hips high. Similarly, relaxation, self-talk and mantras (positive
17
and motivational), and mindfulness were primarily used when athletes experienced greater
18
exertional pain. Overall, active self-regulatory strategies were principally engaged when a
19
need optimise performance or cope with increased physical discomfort was a priority.
20
Conversely, during easier, slower paced, or longer distance runs (e.g. training or ultra-
21
distance), active distraction/switching off was a more frequent attentional focus. In such
22
circumstances, when the athlete felt comfortable, or performance was less of a concern,
23
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
14
active distraction served to relax control over cognition, and allow the athlete engage in other
1
thoughts. Using other people for distraction and conversing was also a recurrent distractive
2
strategy during training or ultra-distance running. However, during intense racing or
3
strenuous training the majority of athletes, including both successful elite runners, reported
4
attempts to avoid involuntary distraction and stay focused. Overwhelmingly, involuntary
5
distraction was associated with performance disruption, and typically avoided by engaging an
6
active self-regulatory strategy. For example, counting was expressly used by two athletes to
7
counter involuntary distraction and regain a more effective attentional focus. One competitive
8
elite athlete runner did indicate an occasional need for others to intervene (e.g. a coach
9
shouting instructions) when they became involuntarily distracted, however. The importance
10
of controlling cognitive focus and avoiding involuntary distraction during racing was
11
emphasised by one runner who recounted this experience over an 8km cross-country race:
12
I went through the 2k and the 4k on the back of the leading group. Ah, and going into
13
the third lap, I started falling off the leading group. And that… it was everything for
14
me to stay attached, and it was only for there was a person there standing at that time,
15
and suddenly I just lost a seconds concentration, and it was like, ‘don’t lose the
16
concentration, concentrate now’, and I covered the move, and…I finished second…in
17
that race. But only for that split second, it meant everything for me. It was like down
18
to, I’d say literally, two seconds worth of concentration like, ‘cause if I had fallen off
19
that group, I wouldn’t have gotten back on the group, and that would have been it...
20
Overall, the elite endurance runners in the present sample reported a diverse range of
21
cognitive strategies used to control attentional focus. These strategies were primarily acquired
22
through experience, or from discussions with significant others. The following section deals
23
with reviewing and evaluating processes that emerged from the interview data.
24
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
15
Reviewing and evaluating after running. Most athletes, including both successful
1
elite performers, reported reviewing and evaluating by self after running. A minority (40%)
2
of athletes also reported reviewing and evaluating with others, such as with a coach, or a
3
psychologist. Reviewing and evaluating by self after running included the subcategories of
4
evaluate cognitive strategies and performance, acquire cognitive strategies through
5
experience, and eliminate ineffective cognitive strategies. Subsequently, many cognitive
6
strategies were acquired through experience and further developed and refined, a processes
7
characterised by an elite athlete competing in 24-hour events:
8
Ah, but what it has involved is just the details of how to do it – little things –
9
particularly in the longer stuff where… in the first 12 or 24 hours I learned… that
10
keeping all those mental puzzles for yourself like working out pacing and things like
11
that, keep them for the race, don’t work them out beforehand…. I went into that race
12
with, you know, a radio on standby, with earphones and so on, and I never used it
13
‘cause I learned that there’s more than enough racing going on over 24 hours to keep
14
you totally mentally engaged that you don’t actually need any supplementary stuff. In
15
some ways it’s just been just, kind of, refining what I already have…
16
Furthermore, some athletes described how they eliminated ineffective cognitive
17
strategies as a result of reviewing and evaluating. These findings highlight the importance of
18
reviewing cognitive strategies and performance to develop a bespoke range of strategies for
19
future use. In addition, evaluations were often based on metacognitive experiences, and these
20
were the second broad dimension to emerge from the data.
21
Metacognitive Experiences
22
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
16
Metacognitive feelings. The categories of metacognitive feelings that emerged from
1
the data were feeling of knowing, feeling of difficulty, feeling of confidence, and feeling of
2
familiarity. Metacognitive feelings were a product of both internal sensory monitoring and
3
outward monitoring during running. The metacognitive feelings which tended to mediate
4
cognitive control were feeling of knowing, and feeling of difficulty.
5
In terms of feeling of knowing, each performer reported knowing when to apply a
6
cognitive strategy. Only one competitive elite runner reported a feeling of knowing one does
7
not know a cognitive strategy to apply, and alluded to specific competitive race scenarios
8
where they experienced direct, ‘head-to-head’ racing with other competitors. Athletes did not
9
always explicitly report a feeling of knowing, but rather described contexts where they would
10
employ particular cognitive strategies. Similarly, feeling of difficulty was strongly associated
11
with cognitive control during running, and athletes typically engaged an active self-regulatory
12
strategy when running felt hard, or an active distraction strategy when running felt easy.
13
These interactions were exemplified by a marathon, and mountain running competitor:
14
…I suppose there’s times when things are appropriate and when things are not, and
15
some of them are like your emergency strategies… and others are, sort of, a lesser
16
strategy. So like the thing where I say sometimes I would count or whatever, or think
17
of a number in my head… generally you do that at a point where… you might be
18
mildly uncomfortable, or you’re ok, or it’s fine…. But…the thing where you look at
19
your band or you just have to accept, you do the pain acceptance thought in your
20
head… that is more in a situation that’s more… emergency ‘cause you’re in a lot of
21
pain, you’re really suffering quite a bit.
22
Thus, both feeling of knowing, and feeling of difficulty specifically acted as stimuli to
23
adopt a suitable cognitive focus to cope with the subjective demands of a running task.
24
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
17
Metacognitive judgements and estimates. The main categories of metacognitive
1
judgements and estimates that emerged were estimate of solution correctness, judgements
2
about own capabilities, judgements about running performances, and estimate of effort. With
3
regard to estimate of solution correctness, the majority of athletes, including both successful
4
elites, recounted judgments of effective cognitive strategies, and judgments of ineffective
5
attentional focus. Active self-regulatory strategies were predominantly judged as effective,
6
however. Subsequently, athletes reported how these strategies benefited running
7
performance, as typified in the following quote by one competitor:
8
So, the more tight you are; your stride is short, or everything, your breathing,
9
everything. So, the minute I relax and I drop my arms, my elbows are in and my knees
10
are high, my stride automatically lengthens…. So, already I’m on a better flow…
11
Distractive thoughts were judged equally as effective or ineffective, depending on the
12
running context and circumstantial needs. Involuntary distraction was unanimously judged as
13
ineffective, however, and considered to have a negative impact on performance.
14
Although more athletes reported positive judgements about their own capabilities,
15
beliefs about own attributes, and beliefs about own limitations influenced both planned, and
16
self-regulatory pacing and tactical decisions prior to, and during running. Conversely,
17
judgements about running performances, and estimate of effort were strongly related to
18
reviewing and evaluating after running. In particular, while satisfaction with own
19
performance was often reported following races where cognitive strategies worked well,
20
dissatisfaction with own performance followed accounts of less successful races, or cognitive
21
foci that did not work well. Similarly, feeling tired because of competition or training load
22
was often associated with an adjustment to training plans, or an understanding by the athlete
23
that such feelings were an inevitable consequence of their current training cycle.
24
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
18
Discussion
1
The findings of the present investigation indicate that metacognitive processes may be
2
fundamental to effective cognitive control during running in elite endurance runners. The
3
data also supports the contention that metacognition underpins expertise in both training and
4
competitive sporting settings (MacIntyre et al., 2014). Metacognitive processes, such as
5
planning, monitoring, reviewing and evaluating, and metacognitive experiences were central
6
to the adoption and initiation of cognitive strategies during running. The present study
7
highlights the role of metacognitive monitoring and control functions to cognitive regulation
8
(Efklides, 2014) in the context of endurance running.
9
In terms of monitoring activities, the athletes in this study appeared to have
10
established, through experience, a means of prioritising sensorimotor inputs to optimise
11
running performance. Periodic monitoring of internal states (e.g. exertional pain) and the
12
outward environment (e.g. other runners) often generated metacognitive feelings, such as
13
running feeling hard, or knowing when to apply a cognitive strategy, for example. In turn,
14
these metacognitive representations exerted control over cognition (Efklides, 2006). These
15
data suggest that the present elite endurance runners predominantly attended to the
16
informational aspect of sensory stimuli, and used this information to adopt a focus of
17
attention appropriate to the context.
18
Controlling cognition on the basis of monitoring processes might be considered a
19
form of reactive (Braver, 2012), bottom-up (Buschman & Miller, 2007), or stimulus driven
20
(e.g., Corbetta & Shulman, 2002) attentional control. Braver (2012) suggests that reactive
21
cognitive control may have the advantage of efficiency and be less demanding on cognitive
22
resources. Furthermore, linked with the parallel processing model of pain (Leventhal &
23
Everhart, 1979), metacognitive representations may explain how athletes develop schemata to
24
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
19
appraise exertional signals during running. Via metacognitive strategies and experiences,
1
these schemata may allow experienced runners to appraise pain signals more accurately
2
(Brewer & Buman, 2006) and adopt an appropriate cognitive focus as a result.
3
Alongside reactive control, evidence for proactive cognitive control (Braver, 2012)
4
also emerged. Athletes often reported employing metacognitive skills such as planning
5
cognitive strategies prior to competitive running and it is noteworthy that both successful
6
elite runners engaged in planning pre-competition. While proactive control may be more
7
demanding of cognitive resources, potentially deleterious interference from both internal and
8
external distractors may be minimised as a result (Braver, 2012). To assist proactive control,
9
some athletes also reported planning with significant others, such as coaches and
10
psychologists. This form of social metacognition may be considered as metacognition at a
11
meta-meta-level (e.g., Efkildes, 2014) and allow for communication of metacognitive
12
information (Shea et al., 2014). Discussions during instances of planning and evaluation may
13
have developed athletes’ abilities to interpret metacognitive representations, for example, and
14
moderate strategy selection and subsequent cognitive control during running.
15
The range of cognitive strategies reported by the elite endurance runners was diverse.
16
The findings add substantially to the array of active self-regulatory strategies previously
17
reported (Brick et al., 2014). Crucially, however, the present findings also add clarity as to
18
when, and why the athletes initiated specific cognitive strategies. All athletes reported
19
focusing on pacing and tactical decisions during competition, for example, which were often
20
informed by metacognitive representations resulting from outward environmental monitoring
21
activities. For example, pacing and tactical decisions during running were often preceded by
22
a metacognitive feeling of confidence, and specifically a belief in one’s ability to meet the
23
task demands. Regulating performance, based on task-relevant environmental monitoring, has
24
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
20
previously been shown to improve competitive endurance performance (e.g., Williams et al.,
1
in press). More importantly, in the present discussion, controlling action based on the
2
outcome of metacognitive processes highlights the role of metacognitive activity in
3
movement planning, guidance, and execution (e.g., Augustyn & Rosenbaum, 2008).
4
Knowing when to apply a cognitive strategy was predominantly influenced by task
5
context and demands (e.g., Efklides, 2014; Tarricone, 2011). For example, when running felt
6
hard (metacognitive feeling of difficulty), self-regulatory strategies such as relaxation,
7
positive and motivational self-talk, mindfulness, and a focus on running technique were
8
frequently initiated. Athletes also repeatedly judged these strategies as effective, and research
9
evidence reinforces the beneficial impact of these self-regulatory strategies on both
10
endurance performance (e.g., Blanchfield, Hardy, de Morree, Staino, & Marcora, 2014;
11
Rushall & Shewchuk, 1989) and cognitive function (e.g., Hasse et al., 2014). The finding that
12
athletes used mindfulness techniques, alongside other cognitive strategies during running
13
suggests that mindfulness might be considered an active self-regulatory strategy, rather than a
14
conceptual framework within the attentional focus domain (e.g., Salmon et al., 2010).
15
Reported episodes of distraction further highlighted a tendency by the elite runners to
16
adapt attentional focus based on contextual needs (e.g. Moran, 1996). Specifically, active
17
distraction predominantly occurred when running tasks were longer (e.g. long training runs,
18
or ultra-distance races), were relatively undemanding, and felt easier (metacognitive feeling
19
of difficulty). Research on mind-wandering suggests that distractive thoughts may intensify
20
in such contexts (e.g., Randall, Oswald, & Beier, 2014), and can be useful to allow relief
21
from boredom, for example (e.g., Mooneyham & Schooler, 2013). However, when optimal
22
performance was a priority, such as during shorter races, or intense training sessions, athletes
23
reported avoiding involuntary distraction. In such circumstances, distraction was often judged
24
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
21
as ineffective, and cognition assumed a form of conscious, top-down control (e.g., Buschman
1
& Miller, 2007; Corbetta & Shulman, 2002), where processing of irrelevant information was
2
attenuated in favour of a more appropriate attentional focus. It was noteworthy that one
3
competitive elite reported not knowing a cognitive strategy to employ in specific, competitive
4
racing situations, while another reported needing external assistance on occasion when they
5
became involuntarily distracted. While neither successful elite athlete reported such issues,
6
this may indicate that athletes of a lower performance standard may benefit from
7
interventions to develop metacognitive skills and optimise self-regulatory abilities.
8
Finally, while contributing to cognitive control during running, metacognitive
9
judgements and estimates also informed evaluative processes after running. Metacognitive
10
judgements and estimates allow information on progress reach the level of conscious
11
awareness (e.g., Efklides, 2014). Thus, judgements about running performances, or estimates
12
of solution correctness, for example, were critical antecedents to the conscious review of
13
running performances. As with planning, reviewing and evaluating were also performed both
14
individually, and with significant others, once more implying supra-personal cognitive
15
control, and metacognition both at a meta-, and at a meta-meta-level (e.g., Efkildes, 2014;
16
Shea et al., 2014). These metacognitive processes allowed athletes adopt and refine those
17
strategies which were effective, and eliminate those which were not.
18
The data indicate the potential utility of a metacognitive perspective to guide research
19
activity in the attentional focus domain. Figure 2 provides a framework to illustrate the
20
interactions between metacognitive process and the attentional focus dimensions suggested
21
by Brick et al. (2014). According to this framework, athletes (and significant others) may (1)
22
plan cognitive strategies, or what to monitor, prior to running. During running, monitoring
23
processes (2) may directly, or via metacognitive feelings (3), form a metacognitive
24
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
22
representation of the running task which, in turn, stimulates cognitive control and the
1
adoption of an appropriate cognitive strategy (4). For example, internal sensory monitoring
2
(e.g. increased exertional pain), and outward monitoring (e.g. of a competitor) may generate a
3
metacognitive feeling (e.g. running feels hard). Awareness of this feeling, in conjunction with
4
awareness of performance, forms a representation of the task which, in turn, stimulates the
5
initiation of an appropriate cognitive strategy (e.g. to relax), and exert control over cognition.
6
[INSERT FIGURE 2 NEAR HERE]
7
Consequently, the athlete may make explicit metacognitive judgements or estimates
8
(5) regarding the (in) effectiveness of the cognitive strategy employed (e.g. estimate of
9
solution correctness). Depending on the outcome of this metacognitive judgement, alongside
10
continued monitoring of task performance, the athlete may choose to maintain their current
11
attentional focus, or adopt an alternative cognitive strategy. Following performance,
12
metacognitive judgements and estimates may further inform review and evaluation processes
13
(6). At this point, cognitive strategies may be further refined, or eliminated and, as a result,
14
impact on metacognitive planning prior to future running activities.
15
The findings of the present study indicate that metacognitive strategies, such as
16
planning before running, and reviewing and evaluating after running influence attentional
17
focus and cognitive control during running. Further, metacognitive experiences, such as
18
metacognitive feelings, and metacognitive judgements and estimates inform cognitive
19
strategy use in elite endurance runners. This knowledge allows us to augment our
20
understanding of psychological skills (e.g., MacIntyre et al., 2014; Moran, 1996) with an
21
appreciation of when and why elite endurance runners initiate cognitive strategies during
22
running. Integrated with the dimensions of attentional focus suggested by Brick et al. (2014),
23
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
23
the present study highlights the utility of a metacognitive framework to advance our
1
understanding of attentional processes during endurance activity.
2
References
3
Augustyn, J. S., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2005). Metacognitive control of action: Preparation
4
for aiming reflects knowledge of Fitts’ Law. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12,
5
911-916. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03196785
6
Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645.
7
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639
8
Blanchfield, A. W., Hardy, J., de Morree, H. M., Staino, W., & Marcora, S. M. (2014).
9
Talking yourself out of exhaustion: The effects of self-talk on endurance performance.
10
Medicine and Science in Sport and Exercise, 46, 998-1007.
11
http://dx.doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0000000000000184
12
Braver, T. S. (2012). The variable nature of cognitive control: A dual mechanisms
13
framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 106-113.
14
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.010
15
Brewer, B. W., & Buman, M. P. (2006). Attentional focus and endurance performance:
16
Review and theoretical integration. Kinesiologica Slovenica, 12, 82-97.
17
Brick, N., MacIntyre, T., & Campbell, M. (2014). Attentional focus in endurance activity:
18
New paradigms and future directions. International Review of Sport and Exercise
19
Psychology, 7, 106-134. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1750984X.2014.885554
20
Buschman, T. J. & Miller, E. K. (2007). Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in
21
the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices, Science, 315, 1860–1862.
22
http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1138071
23
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
24
Clingman, J. M., & Hilliard, D. V. (1990). Race walkers quicken their pace by tuning in, not
1
stepping out. The Sport Psychologist, 4, 23–32.
2
Côté, J., Salmela, J. H., Baria, A., & Russell, S. J. (1993). Organizing and interpreting
3
unstructured qualitative data. The Sport Psychologist, 7, 127-137.
4
Efklides, A. (2006). Metacognition and affect: What can metacognitive experiences tell us
5
about the learning process? Educational Research Review, 1, 3-14.
6
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2005.11.001
7
Efklides, A. (2014). How does metacognition contribute to the regulation of learning? An
8
integrative approach. Psychological Topics, 23, 1-30.
9
Elo, S., & Kyngäs, H. (2008). The qualitative content analysis process. Journal of Advanced
10
Nursing, 62, 107-115. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2648.2007.04569.x
11
Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive
12
developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906-911.
13
Green, J., & Thorogood, N. (2004). Analysing qualitative data. In: D. Silverman, D. (Ed.).
14
Qualitative Methods for Health Research (pp. 173–200). London: Sage Publications.
15
Hasse, L., Thom, N. J., Shukla, A., Davenport, P. W., Simmons, A. N., Paulus, M. P., &
16
Johnson, D. C. (2014) Mindfulness-based training attenuates insula response to an
17
aversive interoceptive challenge. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience.
18
Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu042
19
Leventhal, H., & Everhart, D. (1979). Emotion, pain, and physical illness. In C. E. Izard
20
(Ed.). Emotions in personality and psychopathology (pp. 263-298). New York, NY:
21
Plenum.
22
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
25
MacIntyre, T., Igou, E. R., Moran, A., P., Campbell, M., & Matthews, J. (2014).
1
Metacognition and action: A new pathway to understanding social and cognitive
2
aspects of expertise in sport. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1155.
3
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01155
4
MacIntyre, T. E., Moran, A. P., Collet, C., & Guillot, A. (2013). An emerging paradigm: A
5
strength-based approach to exploring mental imagery. Frontiers in Human
6
Neuroscience, 7, 104. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00104
7
Mooneyham, B. W., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). The cost and benefits of mind-wandering: A
8
review. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 11-18.
9
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0031569
10
Moran, A. P. (1996). The psychology of concentration in sport performers: A cognitive
11
analysis. East Sussex: Psychology Press.
12
Moran, A. P. (2009). Cognitive psychology in sport: Progress and prospects. Psychology of
13
Sport and Exercise, 10, 420-426. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2009.02.010
14
Moran, A. P. (2012). Thinking in action: Some insights from cognitive sport psychology.
15
Thinking Skills and Creativity, 7, 85-92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2012.03.005
16
Nelson. T. O. (1996). Consciousness and metacognition. American Psychologist, 51, 102-
17
116.
18
Nietfeld, J. L. (2003). An examination of metacognitive strategy use and monitoring skills by
19
competitive middle distance runners. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 15, 307-
20
320. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10413200390237942
21
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
26
Randall, J. G., Oswald, F. L., & Beier, M. E. (2014). Mind-wandering, cognition and
1
performance: A theory-driven meta-analysis of attention regulation. Psychological
2
Bulletin, 140, 1411-1431. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0037428
3
Robinson, O. C. (2013). Sampling in interview-based qualitative research: A theoretical and
4
practical guide. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 11, 25-41.
5
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14780887.2013.801543
6
Rushall, B. S., & Shewchuk, M. L. (1989). Effects of thought content instructions on
7
swimming performance. Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness, 29, 326-
8
334.
9
Sarkar, M., & Fletcher, D. (2014). Ordinary magic, extraordinary performance: Psychological
10
resilience and thriving in high achievers. Sport, Exercise and Performance
11
Psychology, 3, 46-60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/spy0000003
12
Schücker, L., Knopf, C., Strauss, B., & Hagemann, N. (2014). An internal focus of attention
13
is not always as bad as its reputation: How specific aspects of internally focused
14
attention do not hinder running efficiency. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology,
15
36, 223-243. http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/jsep.2013-0200
16
Setanni, M., Magistro, D., & Rabaglietti, E. (2012). Development and preliminary validation
17
of an instrument to measure metacognition applied to physical activity during early
18
adolescence. Cognition, Brain, Behaviour. An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 67-87.
19
Shea, N., Boldt, A., Bang, D., Yeung, N., Heyes, C., & Frith, C. D. (2014). Supra-personal
20
cognitive control and metacognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 186-193.
21
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.01.006
22
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
METACOGNITIVE PROCESSES AND ENDURANCE RUNNING
27
Salmon, P., Hanneman, S., & Harwood, B. (2010). Associative/dissociative cognitive
1
strategies in sustained physical activity: Literature review and proposal for a
2
mindfulness-based conceptual model. The Sport Psychologist, 24, 127-156.
3
Swann, C., Moran, A. P., & Piggott, D. (2015). Defining elite athletes: Issues in the study of
4
expert performance in sport psychology. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 16, 3-14.
5
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.07.004
6
Tarricone, P. (2011). The taxonomy of metacognition. East Sussex: Psychology Press.
7
Tenenbaum, G. (2001). A social-cognitive perspective of perceived exertion and exertion
8
tolerance. In R. N. Singer, H. Hausenblas, & C. Janelle (Eds.). Handbook of sport
9
psychology (2
nd
ed.) (pp. 810-820). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley & Sons.
10
Vaismoradi, M., Turunen, H., & Bondas, T. (2013). Content analysis and thematic analysis:
11
Implications for conducting a qualitative descriptive study. Nursing and Health
12
Sciences, 15, 398-405. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/nhs.12048
13
Wagstaff, C. R. D., Fletcher, D., & Hanton, S. (2012). Exploring emotion abilities and
14
regulation strategies in sport organizations. Sport, Exercise, and Performance
15
Psychology, 1, 268-282. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0028814
16
Williams, E. L., Jones, H. S., Sparks, A., Marchant, D. C., Midgley, A. W., & McNaughton,
17
L. R. (in press). Competitor presence reduces internal attentional focus and improves
18
16.1 km cycling time trial performance. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport.
19
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2014.07.003
20
Yoshida, T., Nakamura, T., Kubo, C., Molton, I. R., Jensen, M. P., Arimura, T., & Hosoi, M.
21
(2012). Cognitions, metacognitions, and chronic pain. Rehabilitation Psychology, 57,
22
207-213. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0028903
23
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Table 1. Demographic variables of study sample (n = 10)
Demographic Variables
Age Mean: 35.6 ± 6.6 years
Gender 6 females, 4 males
Primary running event
Ultra-Distance (n = 2)
10km – Marathon (n = 6)
3km – 10km (n = 2)
Athlete’s highest standard
of performance Olympic Games (n = 2)
World championship level (n = 4)
European championship level (n = 3)
Commonwealth Games (n = 1)
Success at the athlete’s
highest level Infrequent success at international level (n = 3)
National titles, selected to represent nation (n = 4)
Competitive at national level, selected to represent nation (n = 3)
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Figure 1. Cognitive and metacognitive processes in the regulation of performance and
control of cognition by elite endurance runners. A frequency analysis is presented in the first
column to indicate the number of participants mentioning each subcategory. Symbols denote
either both (*) or one (#) of the successful elite athletes reported the cognitive/metacognitive
process
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Figure 2. A metacognitive framework of attentional focus and cognitive control in elite endurance runners.
4. Active Distraction
during running
4. Involuntary Distraction
during running
1. Plan before running
2. Internal Sensory
Monitoring
(during running)
4. Active Self-Regulation
during running
3. Metacognitive Feelings
Feeling of knowing
Feeling of difficulty
Feeling of confidence
Feeling of familiarity
5. Metacognitive Judgements
and Estimates
Estimate of solution correctness
Judgements about own capabilities
Judgements about running performance
Estimate of effort
6. Review and Evaluate
after running
2. Outward
Monitoring
(during running)
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Highlights
Applied a metacognitive approach to study attentional focus during endurance activity
Interviewed ten elite endurance runners about cognitive strategy use during running
A content analysis was used to interpret the data
Findings indicate metacognitive activity influences cognitive control during running
We present an integrative framework of metacognitive processes and attentional focus
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Metacognitive strategies in the self-regulation of performance in elite endurance
runners.
Pre-Interview Information
Outline of the study and the interview process
This information sheet is to provide you with a little more insight into the interview we will
be completing, and what I will be asking you to discuss. The interviews are part of a study I
am undertaking on the mental strategies used by elite runners during endurance activity. The
interview will involve thinking about past events and situations where you have employed
various mental strategies. Mental strategies might include things you think about during
competitive running, or during running training. An example of one such mental strategy is
that used by Paula Radcliffe. In her book How to Run, she reveals how she counts to 100 to
determine where she is during each mile. She explains: ‘This is something that I started doing
a long time ago as a means of focusing on where I was within each grass/road rep that was
run to time rather than marked distance. I found it helped me to judge and pace myself. As I
moved to road races, I learned that breaking each mile down worked well for me. For a half
to full marathon pace, counting three times to 100 roughly equates to a mile: this technique
helps me focus on where I am within each mile of the race and has become my technique for
anchoring my concentration. I use it to truly stay in the moment.’ This is just one example of
a mental strategy during running. You may use many others and use them in your own way.
During the interview, I will ask you to talk about the mental strategies you use. We
will discuss the mental strategies you use during competitive events, and also during running
training. It is important to note that we will only discuss mental strategies during running, and
not other types of training or event. I will ask you about how you monitor the mental
strategies you use. For example, how do you know if a mental strategy is working
effectively? Finally, I will also ask you about how you acquired the strategies you use, and
how you have developed and refined your mental strategies over the course of your career.
This interview will be digitally recorded. This recording will be used to accurately
capture and transcribe the interview. The written transcript of the interview will be sent to
you within one week of this interview. At that stage you can check the written transcript for
accuracy. You may also wish to add further detail or clarification to the interview at this
point. The recordings and transcript will only be accessed by me and two principal
investigators in this research study, and all information will be kept strictly confidential.
Insights gathered from you and other participants may be used in writing a research paper
which will be published in a reputable, peer reviewed journal. Though direct quotes from you
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
may be used in the paper, your name and any other identifying information will be kept
strictly anonymous.
The outcomes of this study may be used in many ways. The research might help you
to analyse the mental strategies you currently use and gain a better insight into your own
mental processes. The findings may also be beneficial to sport psychologists, coaches and
athletes by employing the knowledge gained through this study to improve the performance
of athletes in the early stages of their development, or individuals who experience difficulty
coping with the demands of endurance activity. Finally, the findings of the study might also
help researchers to better understand and categorise the thoughts and mental strategies elite
endurance runners use during endurance performance.
Your participation in this study is entirely voluntary. Before the interview begins, I
will ask you to sign an Informed Consent Form, demonstrating your understanding of the
study and what is involved. However, you can choose not to consent, or to withdraw consent
and stop participating in the study at any time. In the event you do choose to withdraw, all
information you provide will be permanently destroyed and omitted from the final research
paper. You may also choose to abstain from answering any questions within this interview.
You may do so by answering ‘no comment’, and I will move on with the next question. If at
any stage during the investigation you have any queries, you are encouraged to ask questions
or raise concerns at any time about the nature of the study or the methods I am using.
Because I will be asking you to think back over past events, you may not be able to
recall your mental strategy use straight away. Please take your time and don’t worry about
pausing to think during the interview. As I will also be asking you to recall your mental
strategy use in both training, and in competitive events, again, please take your time to
accurately recall your mental strategy use in each. Finally, at various stages during the
interview I will be asking you to rate on a scale how frequently you use various mental
strategies, or how effective you find various mental strategies. Again, take your time to
carefully consider your responses to each.
If at any point I ask a question that you do not understand, please ask me to clarify
and explain further. Thank you once again for your participation in this study and I look
forward to meeting with you next week.
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Metacognitive strategies in the self-regulation of performance in elite endurance
runners interview guide.
Participant Number:
Name:
Age:
Gender:
Address:
Telephone:
Email:
Main event:
Years running competitively:
International representation:
Year of first international representation:
Major Achievements:
Interview date:
Interview start time:
Interview finish time:
Duration of interview
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Metacognitive strategies in the self-regulation of performance in elite endurance
runners interview guide.
Part one – Outline of the study and the interview process (Not digitally recorded)
Hi. I am conducting interviews on the mental strategies used by elite runners during
endurance activity. The interview will involve thinking about past events and situations
where you have employed various mental strategies. Mental strategies might include things
you think about during competitive running, or during training. An example of one such
mental strategy is that used by Paula Radcliffe. In her book How to Run, she reveals how she
counts to 100 to determine where she is during each mile. She explains: ‘This is something
that I started doing a long time ago as a means of focusing on where I was within each
grass/road rep that was run to time rather than marked distance. I found it helped me to
judge and pace myself. As I moved to road races, I learned that breaking each mile down
worked well for me. For a half to full marathon pace, counting three times to 100 roughly
equates to a mile: this technique helps me focus on where I am within each mile of the race
and has become my technique for anchoring my concentration. I use it to truly stay in the
moment.’ This is just one example of a mental strategy during running. You may use many
others and use them in your own way.
During the interview, I will ask you to talk about the mental strategies you use. We
will discuss the mental strategies you use during competitive events, and during running
training. It is important to note that we will only discuss mental strategies during running, and
not other types of training or event. I will ask you about how you monitor the mental
strategies you use. For example, how do you know if a mental strategy is working
effectively? Finally, I will also ask you about how you acquired the strategies you use, and
how you have developed and refined your mental strategies over the course of your career.
This interview will be digitally recorded. This recording will be used to accurately
capture and transcribe the interview. The written transcript of the interview will be sent to
you within one week of this interview. At that stage you can check the written transcript for
accuracy. You may also wish to add further detail or clarification to the interview at this
point. The recordings and transcript will only be accessed by me and two principal
investigators in this research study, and all information will be kept strictly confidential.
Insights gathered from you and other participants may be used in writing a research paper
which will be published in a reputable, peer reviewed journal. Though direct quotes from you
may be used in the paper, your name and any other identifying information will be kept
strictly anonymous.
The outcomes of this study may be used in many ways. The research might help you
to analyse the mental strategies you currently use and gain a better insight into your own
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
mental processes. The findings may also be beneficial to sport psychologists, coaches and
athletes by employing the knowledge gained through this study to improve the performance
of athletes in the early stages of their development, or individuals who experience difficulty
coping with the demands of endurance activity. Finally, the findings of the study might also
help researchers to better understand and categorise the thoughts and mental strategies elite
endurance runners use during endurance performance.
Your participation in this study is entirely voluntary. Before the interview begins, I
will ask you to sign an Informed Consent Form, demonstrating your understanding of the
study and what is involved. However, you can choose not to consent, or to withdraw consent
and stop participating in the study at any time. In the event you do choose to withdraw, all
information you provide will be permanently destroyed and omitted from the final research
paper. You may also choose to abstain from answering any questions within this interview.
You may do so by answering ‘no comment’, and I will move on with the next question. If at
any stage during the investigation you have any queries, you are encouraged to ask questions
or raise concerns at any time about the nature of the study or the methods I am using.
Because I will be asking you to think back over past events, you may not be able to
recall your mental strategy use straight away. Please take your time and don’t worry about
pausing to think during the interview. As I will also be asking you to recall your mental
strategy use in both training, and in competitive events, again, please take your time to
accurately recall your mental strategy use in each. Finally, at various stages during the
interview I will be asking you to rate on a scale how frequently you use various mental
strategies, or how effective you find various mental strategies. Again, take your time to
carefully consider your responses to each.
If at any point I ask a question that you do not understand, please ask me to clarify
and explain further. Thank you once again for your participation in this study. Are you happy
with everything I’ve explained so far? If so, could I ask you to give your written informed
consent to take part in this study (see informed consent sheet), and we will begin the
interview.
[Hand participant Informed Consent Form to sign]
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Part Two – The interview (Digitally recorded)
Section One – General Questions
We will start with some general questions about your running career to date and general
mental preparation for running.
Could you please tell me briefly about your running history and your achievements to
date?
o Probe: When did you first start running?
o Probe: What international events have you competed in as a senior athlete?
Does mental preparation play an important role in your running? If yes, could you tell me
briefly about your general mental preparation for running?
o Prompt: General mental preparation, not specifically mental strategies.
o Prompt: Do you practice imagery/goal setting/relaxation, etc.?
In my study I am investigating the mental strategies experienced, elite endurance runners use
during performance.
For you – what do you understand by mental strategies during running?
Do you use mental strategies during running? If yes, what mental strategies do you use?
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Section Two –
Specific questions on mental strategy use during running
I am now going to focus a little more specifically on your mental strategy use during running.
Did you use any mental strategies in your most recent running event? If yes, could you tell
me about the mental strategies you used?
o Prompt: Starting with the beginning of the event, right through to completion.
o Probe: What mental strategy did you use at stage X of the event?
Could you describe the mental strategies you have used in running events prior to that?
o Prompt: Not the very beginning of your career, but thinking back a number of years.
o Probe: How were your mental strategies different then, compared with now?
Could you describe the mental strategies you would have used at the very beginning of
your running career (i.e., when you first started running)?
o Probe: How were your mental strategies different then, compared with now?
Thank you. I will return to some of the points you’ve mentioned later in the interview. For
now, could you please read the following list of mental strategies typically used by runners.
[Hand List 1 to the participant]
Do you use any of the mental strategies listed here? If yes, could you elaborate on how
you use each of those mental strategies? Please use specific examples where possible.
o Prompt: How do you focus on pacing, compartmentalise distance/time, etc.?
Do the mental strategies you use affect your performance in any way? If yes, could you tell
me how the mental strategies you use affect your performance?
o Prompt (only if required): What about pacing, or feelings of effort?
o Probe: Is your performance noticeably different when you use/don’t use those mental
strategies?
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Section Three – Specific questions on mental strategy use during competitive running:
I am now going to focus on the mental strategies you use during competitive running events.
Do you use different mental strategies during different competitive running events?
o Prompt: For example, during a short race v’s a long race, or a road/trail/track race.
o Probe: Why do you use different mental strategies in different competitive events?
o Probe: Do the mental strategies you use change over the course of a season?
Could you tell me how you choose a mental strategy to use during competitive running?
o Probe: Do you consciously decide on mental strategies to use?
o Probe: Do you use different mental strategies at different times in the same event?
o Probe: Do you plan beforehand mental strategies to use during competitive running?
o Probe: Do you choose a mental strategy to use in reaction to events that happen
during competitive running?
Are there other situational factors, apart from those you’ve just discussed, which affect
your mental strategy use during competitive running? If yes, could you tell me about any
that come to mind? Please give specific situations/examples where possible.
o Prompts: Competitors, terrain, conditions, weather, event importance, stage of race.
o Probe: Have you tried different mental strategies in those situations before?
Are there other mental strategies you would use during competitive running that are not
listed here (see list 1)? If yes, please tell me about them, giving specific examples.
Could you now please rate each of the following types of mental strategy in terms of how
frequently you use each category during competitive running? If you also use other mental
strategies during competitive running, please include these at the end of the list.
[Hand Rating Scale 1 to the participant]
Ratings are based on a 1-5 scale where: 1 = Never; 2 = Rarely; 3 = Sometimes; 4 = Often; 5
= Almost always
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Section Four – Specific questions on mental strategy use during running training
I am now going to focus on the mental strategies you use during running training.
Do you use different mental strategies during different types of running training session?
o Prompt: Intervals, Tempo, Long distance, or easy recovery training runs.
o Probe: Why do you use different mental strategies in different
training sessions
?
o Probe: Do the mental strategies you use in training change in the lead up to
competition?
Could you tell me how you choose a mental strategy to use during running training?
o Probe: Do you consciously decide on mental strategies to use?
o Probe: Do you use different mental strategies at different times in the same session?
o Probe: Do you plan beforehand mental strategies to use during running training?
o Probe: Do you choose a mental strategy to use in reaction to events that happen
during running training?
Are there other situational factors, apart from those you’ve just discussed, which affect
your mental strategy use during running training? If yes, could you tell me about any that
come to mind? Please give specific situations/examples where possible.
o Prompt: Intensity of session, terrain, conditions, weather, proximity to competition.
o Probe: Have you tried different mental strategies in those situations before?
Are there other mental strategies you would use during running training that are not listed
here (see list 1)? If yes, please tell me about them, giving specific examples.
Could you now please rate each of the following types of mental strategy in terms of how
frequently you use each category during running training? If you also use other mental
strategies during running training, please include these at the end of the list.
[Hand Rating Scale 2 to the participant]
Ratings are based on a 1-5 scale where: 1 = Never; 2 = Rarely; 3 = Sometimes; 4 = Often; 5
= Almost always
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Section Five – Specific questions on monitoring and effectiveness of mental strategies.
I am now going to ask you about how you monitor the effectiveness of the various mental
strategies you use. For example, I am interested in finding out about how you know if a
mental strategy is working for you, or not.
Do you monitor the effectiveness of the mental strategies you use? If yes, could you tell me
how you do this?
o Prompt (only if required): For example, monitor pace/feelings of exertion, etc.
o Probe: How do you know a mental strategy is working for you?
o Probe: Do you monitor throughout the run – to completion?
o Probe: Do you evaluate your mental strategies post-run (competition and training)?
For you, do different mental strategies have different performance effects? If yes, please
elaborate on how you feel different mental strategies affect your performance.
o Probe: Do you use this knowledge to choose a mental strategy to use?
Do you change or modify a mental strategy if one is not working? If yes, could you tell me
how do you do this? Please give specific examples where possible.
o Probe: How do you know a mental strategy is not working for you?
o Probe: Do you consciously make a decision to modify the mental strategy used?
Are there any other aspects to how you monitor the effectiveness of your mental strategies
that we have not discussed here?
Could you now please rate each of the following types of mental strategy in terms of how
effective you find each category during competitive running or training? If you also use other
mental strategies during running, please include these at the end of the list and rate each.
[Hand Rating Scale 3 to the participant]
Ratings are based on a 1-5 scale where: 1 = Very ineffective; 2 = Ineffective; 3 = Average; 4
= Effective; 5 = Very effective
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Section Six – Specific questions on the acquisition, development and refinement of the
mental strategies used.
In this final section, I’m going to probe a little more into how you acquired, developed, and
refined the mental strategies you use. We discussed in Section 2 how your mental strategy use
has changed during your career – I would now like to delve deeper into this.
How did you acquire the mental strategies you use?
o Probe: Why did you acquire those mental strategies?
o Probe: When did you acquire those mental strategies?
Have you attempted to develop and refine the mental strategies you use? If yes, could you
tell me how you have done this? Please give specific examples where possible.
o Probe: If you haven’t developed or refined your mental strategies, thinking about it
now, how might you develop or refine those mental strategies?
o Probe: Why did you develop and refine your mental strategies?
Are there mental strategies you have tried before that didn’t work? If yes, what were they?
o Probe: How did you know that mental strategy was not working for you?
o Probe: Did you decide to change that mental strategy? If yes, what did you change?
Could you now please tick to indicate which of the following methods you have used to
acquire, and secondly to develop and refine the mental strategies you use during running.
You may wish to make some additional comments to clarify if necessary.
[Hand List 2 to the participant]
Do you consider your mental strategy use a strength, or a weakness? Please elaborate.
o Probe: What else do you consider as your main strengths/weaknesses as a runner?
Are there any other aspects to how you acquired, developed and refined your mental
strategies that we have not discussed here? If yes, please tell me about them.
MANUS CRIP T
ACCEP TED
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Conclusion to the interview
Are there any other mental strategies or aspects of attentional focus you would like to
discuss that we have not covered in the interview?
Concluding remarks and questions on the interview
How do you think this interview went?
Do you feel we fully explored your mental strategy use during running?
Did I lead or influence your responses in any way?
Have you any comments or suggestions about the interview itself?
Thank you for taking the time to complete this interview. Your comments and experiences will
be of great value in my study and will contribute to the overall success of this project.
Do you have any further points you would like to add to this discussion, or any questions you
would like to ask at this point?
In the next week I will send you a copy of the transcript for this interview. I would ask you to
read through it to ensure it is an entirely accurate record of everything we have discussed
today. If you wish to further add to any of your comments, or further clarify anything, please
feel free to do so at this stage.
Again, I would like to assure you that all comments raised will be treated with the strictest
confidentiality and no individual contributor will be referred to by name in the discussion
and presentation of the results of this interview. Thank you for your time, comments, and
interest in this research.
[Conclusion to the interview. Stop digital recorder.]
... Metacognitive experience is a critical component in controlling cognitive activities and achieving learning goals or problem-solving. Metacognitive experiences allow for concurrent or 'on-line' monitoring during task performance (Brick et al., 2015). They include metacognitive feelings, which 2 inform the individual about task performance in the form of a feeling, such as feelings of 3 difficulty, and tend to be implicit in nature (Brick et al., 2015;Efklides, 2006). ...
... Metacognitive experiences allow for concurrent or 'on-line' monitoring during task performance (Brick et al., 2015). They include metacognitive feelings, which 2 inform the individual about task performance in the form of a feeling, such as feelings of 3 difficulty, and tend to be implicit in nature (Brick et al., 2015;Efklides, 2006). According to Schraw and Moshman, the three main components of metacognitive experience are planning, evaluation, and monitoring. ...
... Metacognitive functions influence lifestyle behaviours. For instance, recreational endurance runners reported less engagement in metacognitive processes during PA than did elite athletes, who practiced metacognitive planning, monitoring, reviewing, and evaluating of performance [25,26]. The role of metacognition in lifestyle behaviours extends to diet and sleep behaviours as well. ...
Article
Full-text available
Our cognitive capacities like working memory and attention are known to systematically vary over time with our physical activity levels, dietary choices, and sleep patterns. However, whether our metacognitive capacities––such as our strategic use and optimization of cognitive resources––show a similar relationship with these key lifestyle factors remains unknown. Here we addressed this question in healthy young adults by examining if physical activity, diet, and sleep patterns were predictive of self-reported metacognitive status. Participants completed a set of validated surveys assessing these lifestyle factors over the past week to month, as well as three measures of metacognition. Using multiple regression and exploratory factor analyses we identified four clusters of metacognitive processes that are sensitive to lifestyle behaviours. Specifically, knowledge of and offline regulation of cognition is linked with physical activity, on-line cognitive regulation is related to diet, and metacognitive worry is associated with sleep behaviours. These findings suggest that lifestyle behaviours do not just affect objective cognitive functioning, but also the meta-level processes we use to monitor our cognitive performance and exert strategic control over our cognitive resources.
... According to Brick and colleagues, metacognitive experiences are based on metacognitive monitoring (i.e., attentional focus and cognitive control) of feelings, judgments, and estimates of effort (Brick et al., 2015). The present study extends these findings as participants who reported more accurately assessing their current effort or emotional state during competition had better self-regulation, and more balanced mentalizing overall. ...
... In this study, there was a significant influence between imagery training on the service jump accuracy of UPI Bandung volleyball athletes in the men's category. Mental imagery training includes 5 forms of imagery training , namely, motivational specific , motivational general mastery, motivational general arousal, cognitive specific, cognitive general so that the training runs effectively (Brick et al., 2015) . Motivational specific is the act of motivating athletes by involving themselves to win a match. ...
Article
Full-text available
Problems: Problems in study This is Athletes often pay little attention to their psychological training methods, because they think that service is not that important, therefore imagery training is very important and is the solution in this research. Purpose: Internal goal study This is to know influence exercise imagery against accuracy service jump volleyball athlete son. Methods: Research This use method quantitative with approach experiment one group pre-test post-test design. Result: Research results show that exercise imagery give influence to accuracy volleyball athlete's service jump son by 26%. Conclusion: The implementation of appropriate and consistent imagery training has been proven effective in enhancing the accuracy of jump serves among male volleyball athletes. It is therefore recommended that volleyball coaches incorporate sustained imagery training into their training programs.
... Numerous psychologists argue that athletes use metacognition prior to and during performance. Examples concern strategic control of attention, or mental techniques in managing pain or fatigue (Moran et al., 2019;Brick et al., 2015;MacIntyre et al., 2014;Brick et al., 2020;Brick et al., 2019). This is interesting. ...
Article
Full-text available
Philosophers frequently refer to ‘flow’ when discussing skill and expertise. This is especially clear when considering the ‘flow argument’ advocated by the Automatic view. Although the Automatic view is a minority position today, many philosophers and scientists endorse the flow argument or central parts of it. The present paper argues that the argument is implausible in the light of current psychological research. It proceeds in two steps. First, the paper argues that the psychological research does not suggest that flow is the only type of psychological state characteristic of optimal expert performance. Experts undergo different states, notably the state of clutch. Second, the paper argues that the psychological research does not suggest that flow states are devoid of conscious supervision in the form of conscious control, self-awareness, or task-related thoughts. Experts execute multiple conscious operations involved in attention control, motivational self-talk, and certain psychological mindsets while being in flow. These considerations oppose the two premises of the flow argument. Moreover, they correct misunderstandings of flow in the broader philosophical and scientific literature.
... This relationship better represents the control of cognitive processes that underlie and determine the use of complex cognitive strategies (Brick et al., 2015). They assume that metacognitive strategies increase the level of flexibility and vigour sufficient to be compatible with the prevailing concept of self-regulation (Amini et al., 2020). ...
Article
Predictably, millions of dollars get invested in sport science by the federation for impeccable performance of an athlete. However, there is a gap which exist to understand the nature of cognitive-motion nexus in sports with regard to performance. Hence, this systematic review synthesized evidence on the influence of metacognition, grit and self-efficacy on performance in sport domain using PRISMA guidelines. A search of Web of Science, PubMed, Google Scholar, PsycINFO and Scopus databases reaped 26 eligible studies. Findings were classified into three domains namely metacognitive inferences, feedback for grit and role of self-efficacy. Grit is positively correlated with metacognition and self-efficacy. We conclude by encouraging the sports system to foster the skills of these constructs.
Article
Актуальність дослідження: Соціально-когнітивну парадигму розробив на основі теорії соціального навчання А. Бандури Б. Ціммерман, який підкреслював особливу роль саморегуляції в загальній соціально-когнітивній теорії поведінки. Б. Циммерман висунув положення про те, що регулятивні навички є джерелом суб'єктивності («personal agency»), яка лежить в основі самовідчуття людини. Автор описує структуру системи саморегуляції, вплив на неї соціального та екологічного контексту, зрештою, дисфункції саморегуляції та напрями її розвитку (цит. за Олефір, 2016). У цьому підході саморегуляція визначається як «самогенеровані думки, почуття та дії, які плануються та циклічно адаптуються для досягнення особистих цілей» (Zimmerman, 2000, стор. 14), і підкреслюється важливість самостійного зворотного зв’язку. петлі, які виникають на різних етапах саморегульованого навчання (Zimmerman, 2000). Результати. У статті проаналізовано основні теоретичні позиції соціокогнітивної теорії та її можливості у поясненні саморегуляції спортивної діяльності. Висновки. Ціність соціокогнітивної теорії у поясненні саморегуляції спортивної діяльності полягає у тому, що вона у ній розглядається як сукупність дій, що спрямовані на регуляцію думок, почуттів та поведінки в цілому з урахуванням низки середовищних чинників, які у межах спорту відрізняються амбівалентністю, суперечливістю та різноманіттям. Ключова думка цієї теорії про те, що спортсмени переслідуватимуть таку лінію поведінки та такі стратегії і тактики діяльності, що приносять їм самовдоволення і почуття власної значущості, але вони утримуються від таких шляхів поведінки, які призводять до самоосуду знайшла підтвердження у теорії самоденетрімнації, яка є традиційною для пояснення спортивної саморегуляції.
Article
Full-text available
The paper addresses issues related to the accuracy of metacognitive monitoring and the effectiveness of control. Based on the enriched model of metacognition (Efklides, 2008), the facets and levels of functioning of metacognition are presented as well as the processes underlying each facet at the various levels of metacognition. Review of current research on monitoring (i.e., metacognitive experiences and metacognitive knowledge) suggests that monitoring can be inaccurate but factors such as prior knowledge, feedback, and attending to task context and response features can increase accuracy. Control, on the other hand, can be triggered by cognition, affect and metacognition. Research evidence on the relations between monitoring and control suggests that monitoring accuracy can support more effective control decisions but not always. Moreover, control can be ineffective due to lack of resources. What is of interest is that control decisions are often influenced by motivational considerations rather than objective task difficulty and through effects of affect on metacognitive monitoring. This implies that metacognition should be viewed within a broader theoretical framework of self-regulation such as the Metacognitive and Affective model of Self-regulated Learning (Efklides, 2011). The implications of the model are discussed as well as the challenges for future research on metacognition.
Article
Full-text available
For over a century, psychologists have investigated the mental processes of expert performers – people who display exceptional knowledge and/or skills in specific fields of human achievement. Since the 1960s, expertise researchers have made considerable progress in understanding the cognitive and neural mechanisms that underlie such exceptional performance. Whereas the first modern studies of expertise were conducted in relatively formal knowledge domains such as chess, more recent investigations have explored elite performance in dynamic perceptual-motor activities such as sport. Unfortunately, although these studies have led to the identification of certain domain-free generalizations about expert-novice differences, they shed little light on an important issue: namely, experts’ metacognitive activities or their insights into, and regulation of, their own mental processes. In an effort to rectify this oversight, the present paper argues that metacognitive processes and inferences play an important if neglected role in expertise. In particular, we suggest that metacognition (including such processes as “meta-attention,” “meta-imagery” and “meta-memory,” as well as social aspects of this construct) provides a window on the genesis of expert performance. Following a critique of the standard empirical approach to expertise, we explore some research on “metacognition” and “metacognitive inference” among experts in sport. After that, we provide a brief evaluation of the relationship between psychological skills training and metacognition and comment on the measurement of metacognitive processes. Finally, we summarize our conclusions and outline some potentially new directions for research on metacognition in action.
Article
Full-text available
The current meta-analysis accumulates empirical findings on the phenomenon of mind-wandering, integrating and interpreting findings in light of psychological theories of cognitive resource allocation. Cognitive resource theory emphasizes both individual differences in attentional resources and task demands together to predict variance in task performance. This theory motivated our conceptual and meta-analysis framework by introducing moderators indicative of task-demand to predict who is more likely to mind-wander under what conditions, and to predict when mind-wandering and task-related thought are more (or less) predictive of task performance. Predictions were tested via a random-effects meta-analysis of correlations obtained from normal adult samples (k =� 88) based on measurement of specified episodes of off-task and/or on-task thought frequency and task performance. Results demonstrated that people with fewer cognitive resources tend to engage in more mind-wandering, whereas those with more cognitive resources are more likely to engage in task-related thought. Addressing predictions of resource theory, we found that greater time-on-task—although not greater task complexity—tended to strengthen the negative relation between cognitive resources and mind-wandering. Additionally, increases in mind-wandering were generally associated with decreases in task performance, whereas increases in task-related thought were associated with increased performance. Further supporting resource theory, the negative relation between mind-wandering and performance was more pronounced for more complex tasks, though not longer tasks. Complementarily, the positive association between task-related thought and performance was stronger for more complex tasks and for longer tasks. We conclude by discussing implications and future research directions for mind-wandering as a construct of interest in psychological research.
Article
Full-text available
Objectives Whilst the presence of a competitor has been found to improve performance, the mechanisms influencing the change in selected work rates during direct competition have been suggested but not specifically assessed. The aim was to investigate the physiological and psychological influences of a visual avatar competitor during a 16.1-km cycling time trial performance, using trained, competitive cyclists. Design Randomised cross-over design. Method Fifteen male cyclists completed four 16.1 km cycling time trials on a cycle ergometer, performing two with a visual display of themselves as a simulated avatar (FAM and SELF), one with no visual display (DO), and one with themselves and an opponent as simulated avatars (COMP). Participants were informed the competitive avatar was a similar ability cyclist but it was actually a representation of their fastest previous performance. Results Increased performance times were evident during COMP (27.8 ± 2.0 min) compared to SELF (28.7 ± 1.9 min) and DO (28.4 ± 2.3 min). Greater power output, speed and heart rate were apparent during COMP trial than SELF (p < 0.05) and DO (p ≤ 0.06). There were no differences between SELF and DO. RPE was unchanged across all conditions. Internal attentional focus was significantly reduced during COMP trial (p < 0.05), suggesting reduced focused on internal sensations during an increase in performance. Conclusions Competitive cyclists performed significantly faster during a 16.1-km competitive trial than when performing maximally, without a competitor. The improvement in performance was elicited due to a greater external distraction, deterring perceived exertion.
Article
Full-text available
Objectives There has been considerable inconsistency and confusion in the definition of elite/expert athletes in sport psychology research, which has implications for studies conducted in this area and for the field as a whole. This study aimed to: (i) critically evaluate the ways in which recent research in sport psychology has defined elite/expert athletes; (ii) explore the rationale for using such athletes; and (iii) evaluate the conclusions that research in this field draws about the nature of expertise. Design Conventional systematic review principles were employed to conduct a rigorous search and synthesise findings. Methods A comprehensive literature search of SPORTDiscus, PsycINFO, PsycARTICLES and Academic Search Complete was completed in September, 2013 which yielded 91 empirical studies published between 2010 and 2013. The primarily qualitative findings were analysed thematically. Results Eight ways of defining elite/expert athletes were identified, ranging from Olympic champions to regional level competitors and those with as little as two years of experience in their sport. Three types of rationale were evident in these studies (i.e., “necessity”, “exploratory” and “superior”); while findings also indicated that some elite athletes are psychologically idiosyncratic and perhaps even dysfunctional in their behaviour. Finally, only 19 of the 91 included studies provided conclusions about the nature of expertise in sport. Conclusions This study suggests that the definitions of elite athletes vary on a continuum of validity, and the findings are translated into a taxonomy for classifying expert samples in sport psychology research in future. Recommendations are provided for researchers in this area.
Article
Full-text available
Although resilience has been referred to as "ordinary magic" (Masten, 2001, p. 227) that is more common than once thought, the majority of research in this area has sampled individuals who have been required to react to potentially traumatic events outside of their control. The findings of this work, however, are not easily applicable to those who actively seek to engage with challenging situations that present opportunities for them to raise their performance level. The purpose of this study was, therefore, to identify and explore resilient qualities that enable high achievers to thrive and perform at extraordinary levels. Thirteen high achievers (9 male and 4 female) from 11 professions were interviewed in the United Kingdom, and interpretative phenome-nological analysis was used to identify resilient qualities that enabled the participants to thrive in pressured environments. Results revealed 6 superordinate themes that characterized resilience and thriving: positive and proactive personality, experience and learning, sense of control, flexibility and adaptability, balance and perspective, and perceived social support. The data highlights the multifaceted nature of resilience comprising a constellation of personal qualities that enable high achievers to excel in demanding contexts. The themes are discussed in relation to previous research findings and in terms of their implications for practicing psychologists. It is anticipated that these themes will provide practitioners with an insight into the distinct features of resilience and thriving in high achievers and help individuals to attain success and well-being in their careers.
Book
This is from a book - please do not request, as I do not have pdfs - it should be available from libraries Judith
Article
We review evidence for partially segregated networks of brain areas that carry out different attentional functions. One system, which includes parts of the intraparietal cortex and superior frontal cortex, is involved in preparing and applying goal-directed (top-down) selection for stimuli and responses. This system is also modulated by the detection of stimuli. The other system, which includes the temporoparietal cortex and inferior frontal cortex, and is largely lateralized to the right hemisphere, is not involved in top-down selection. Instead, this system is specialized for the detection of behaviourally relevant stimuli, particularly when they are salient or unexpected. This ventral frontoparietal network works as a 'circuit breaker' for the dorsal system, directing attention to salient events. Both attentional systems interact during normal vision, and both are disrupted in unilateral spatial neglect.
Article
We reviewed and summarize the extant literature on associative/dissociative cognitive strategies used by athletes and others in circumstances necessitating periods of sustained attention. This review covers studies published since a prior publication by Masters and Ogles (1998), and, in keeping with their approach, offers a methodological critique of the literature. We conclude that the distinction between associative and dissociative strategies has outlived its usefulness since initially proposed in an earlier era of ground-breaking research by Morgan and Pollock (1977) that was influenced to some extent by psychodynamic thinking. In recent years there has been an evolutionary shift in concepts of sustained attention toward mindfulness-moment-by-moment attention-that has had a significant impact on conceptual models and clinical practice in diverse areas including stress management, psychotherapy, and athletic performance. We propose that future research on cognitive activity in sustained performance settings be embedded in a mindfulness-based conceptual model.