ArticlePDF Available

From Honesty–Humility to fair behavior – Benevolence or a (blind) fairness norm?

Authors:
  • Max Planck Institute for the Study of Crime Security and Law

Abstract

Recent research has identified trait Honesty-Humility as a vital determinant of a variety of cooperative, fair, or pro-social behaviors. However, it is less clear what drives the link between Honesty-Humility and the tendency to exhibit fair behavior which we first demonstrate in a metaanalysis of prior work using the dictator game. To close this gap, we test two potential explanations, namely benevolence versus an internalized (blind) fairness norm. In a fairness paradigm, we implemented an unfair situation in which participants and their hypothetical counterparts received asymmetric rewards for the same performance in a quiz. The asymmetry was either to the participants' advantage or disadvantage, manipulated experimentally. Finally, participants were empowered to redistribute all rewards at will. In line with the benevolence account, individuals high in Honesty-Humility established a fair distribution if they themselves were advantaged (thus making sure they do not exploit others) whereas they refrained from rectifying the asymmetry (by taking points from the other) if they were disadvantaged. Vice versa, the pattern cannot be accommodated by the idea of a (blind) fairness norm which would have implied that individuals high in Honesty-Humility establish an equal distribution irrespective of who is advantaged.
From Honesty-Humility to fair behavior benevolence or a (blind) fairness norm?
Benjamin E. Hilbig1, Isabel Thielmann1, Johanna Wührl2, and Ingo Zettler3
1University of Koblenz-Landau
2University of Jena
3University of Copenhagen
This article has been published in Personality and Individual Differences. This accepted
manuscript version is made available under the CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 license. This version might
not exactly replicate the official version published in the journal.
The published version of this article is available at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.02.017.
Please cite as:
Hilbig, B. E., Thielmann, I., Wührl, J. & Zettler, I. (2015). From Honesty-Humility to fair behavior
benevolence or a (blind) fairness norm? Personality and Individual Differences, 80, 91-95.
BENEVOLENCE OR (BLIND) FAIRNESS NORM 2
Abstract
Recent research has identified trait Honesty-Humility as a vital determinant of a variety
of cooperative, fair, or pro-social behaviors. However, it is less clear what drives the link
between Honesty-Humility and the tendency to exhibit fair behavior which we first demonstrate
in a meta-analysis of prior work using the dictator game. To close this gap, we test two potential
explanations, namely benevolence versus an internalized (blind) fairness norm. In a fairness
paradigm, we implemented an unfair situation in which participants and their hypothetical
counterparts received asymmetric rewards for the same performance in a quiz. The asymmetry
was either to the participants’ advantage or disadvantage, manipulated experimentally. Finally,
participants were empowered to redistribute all rewards at will. In line with the benevolence
account, individuals high in Honesty-Humility established a fair distribution if they themselves
were advantaged (thus making sure they do not exploit others) whereas they refrained from
rectifying the asymmetry (by taking points from the other) if they were disadvantaged. Vice
versa, the pattern cannot be accommodated by the idea of a (blind) fairness norm which would
have implied that individuals high in Honesty-Humility establish an equal distribution
irrespective of who is advantaged.
Keywords: Honesty-Humility, fairness, pro-social, benevolence, HEXACO model, social norm
BENEVOLENCE OR (BLIND) FAIRNESS NORM 3
Cooperation represents a key aspect of social behavior, fostering benefits of groups rather
than individual agents (cf. Sullivan, Snyder, & Sullivan, 2008). A basic ingredient of individuals’
inclination to cooperate is fairness (e.g., Arneson, 1982; Fehr & Schmidt, 1999), which in
distributive terms denotes “the concern for how money or goods are distributed among
individuals” (Kamas & Preston, 2012, p. 538). Understanding individual differences in fairness
hence provides important insights into interpersonal as well as societal functioning.
Correspondingly, many studies have investigated the relation between fairness and basic
personality traits (e.g., Ben-Ner, Kong, & Putterman, 2004; Kurzban & Houser, 2001).
In recent years, research relying on the HEXACO Model of Personality (comprising the
traits Honesty-Humility, Emotionality, Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and
Openness to Experience; e.g., Ashton & Lee, 2007; Lee & Ashton, 2004) has provided a fruitful
approach and a more fine-grained understanding of the traits influencing fairness. The HEXACO
model has emerged from lexical studies across various languages, essentially representing a
variant and extension of the classical Big Five framework (e.g., McCrae & Costa, 1985): Beyond
factors that are equivalent across the two frameworks (Extraversion, Conscientiousness, and
Openness), the HEXACO model encompasses variants of Emotionality (i.e., Neuroticism) and
Agreeableness (see Ashton, Lee, & De Vries, 2014, for a recent review). In addition and most
prominently the model proposes a sixth basic factor termed Honesty-Humility, specifically
capturing individual differences in fairness.
Essentially, Honesty-Humility represents “the tendency to be fair and genuine in dealing
with others, in the sense of cooperating with others even when one might exploit them without
suffering retaliation” (Ashton & Lee, 2007, p. 156). As such, high levels of Honesty-Humility
stand for sincerity, fairness, greed-avoidance, and modesty whereas low levels imply dishonesty,
BENEVOLENCE OR (BLIND) FAIRNESS NORM 4
unfairness, greed, and pretentiousness. In line with this theoretical conceptualization, Honesty-
Humility relates negatively to crime, delinquency (e.g., Dunlop, Morrison, Koenig, & Silcox,
2012; Van Gelder & De Vries, 2014), and counterproductive work behavior (e.g., Marcus, Lee,
& Ashton, 2007) but positively to integrity (Lee, Ashton, Morrison, Cordery, & Dunlop, 2008)
and fairness-related behaviors such pro-social distribution decisions (Ackermann, Fleiß, &
Murphy, in press; Hilbig, Glöckner, & Zettler, 2014).
In particular, several studies (Hilbig, Thielmann, Hepp, Klein, & Zettler, 2015; Hilbig,
Zettler, Leist, & Heydasch, 2013; Hilbig & Zettler, 2009; Thielmann & Hilbig, 2014; Thielmann,
Hilbig, & Niedtfeld, 2014) have linked Honesty-Humility to fair behavior in the dictator game
(for a recent review see Zhao & Smillie, in press). In this paradigm, an individual is asked to
divide an endowment at will between herself and an unknown other. Across studies, individuals
high in Honesty-Humility gave more, thus refraining from abusing their power to exploit the
other. Summarizing these findings in a meta-analysis (N = 1231), the sample-size weighted
average correlation (cf. Field, 2001) between Honesty-Humility and dictator game giving was r
= .29 and thus a medium-sized effect (Cohen, 1988).
Although these findings clearly demonstrate that individuals high in Honesty-Humility
behave more pro-socially, the conclusions are merely relative. That is, such a positive association
does not necessarily imply that individuals high in Honesty-Humility are also more inclined
toward fair behavior in absolute terms in the sense of sharing equally which most people (i.e.,
more than 80%) perceive to be the fair solution in the dictator game (Mellers, Haselhuhn,
Tetlock, Silva, & Isen, 2010). To test this more directly, we re-analyzed the data referred to
above, testing whether individuals high in Honesty-Humility are actually more likely to share
equally in this game. Indeed, amongst individuals high in Honesty-Humility (i.e., those with
BENEVOLENCE OR (BLIND) FAIRNESS NORM 5
values larger or equal to the median), 64.4% chose the equal split, whereas only 34.6% of their
counterparts low in Honesty-Humility did so, OR = 3.42, 95% CI [2.71, 4.33], χ²(1) = 109.48, p
< .001.
1
This finding clearly indicates that high levels of Honesty-Humility are not only related
to more pro-sociality or non-exploitation in relative terms, but also to more fairness in absolute
terms.
However, although extant evidence linking Honesty-Humility to fair behavior is
consistent, it remains unclear why individuals with high levels of Honesty-Humility are more
inclined towards fair behavior. Specifically, the empirical picture allows for two alternative
explanations. First, the HEXACO theory suggests that this pattern is essentially due to
benevolence. That is, Honesty-Humility is conceptualized in terms of non-exploitation, implying
that individuals high in Honesty-Humility should be motivated to avoid having an edge over
others (i.e., receiving more for similar input compared to others), but not mind the opposite. In
simple terms, they may share equally in the dictator game due to their benevolent nature
placing high importance on justice for others, without insisting on justice for themselves.
Alternatively, extant evidence could also be accounted for by an internalized (“blind”)
fairness norm (i.e., a general preference for equality) in individuals high in Honesty-Humility. In
terms of equity sensitivity (Huseman, Hatfield, & Miles, 1985, 1987), individuals high in
Honesty-Humility might generally prefer equivalent input-outcome-ratios both for others and
themselves and thus insist that no-one is under- or over-rewarded. In simple terms, they may
share equally in the dictator game because this is the social fairness norm (Mellers et al., 2010),
thus placing high importance on justice for both others and themselves.
1
Regressing the dictator game allocation (equal split: yes vs. no) on the (continuous) Honesty-Humility
scores in a logistic regression analysis yielded similar results, OR = 2.90, 95% CI [2.36, 3.55], p < .001.
BENEVOLENCE OR (BLIND) FAIRNESS NORM 6
The purpose of the present study was to disentangle these two explanations. The
benevolence account implies a mechanism that may be summarized as “I need to ensure I do not
get more than others; I should not take advantage of others” in individuals high in Honesty-
Humility. The fairness norm account, by contrast, implies “I need to ensure we all get the same
(for similar input)”. So, to tease the two apart, we used a fairness paradigm in which participants
were asked to re-distribute points that they and a hypothetical other earned through exerting
(cognitive) effort in a quiz. Establishing an unfair situation, there were asymmetric rewards in
the form of different gains (points earned) for the same number of correct solutions. Thus, the
same effort and success in the quiz came with different outcomes. This asymmetry was either in
favor of the participant (advantage-self) or in favor of the hypothetical other (advantage-other).
Finally, participants could redistribute all points at will between themselves and the other. That
is, participants were empowered to give points to or take points from the other and thus to
increase or reduce the asymmetry (or leave it unaltered).
This fairness paradigm allows for disentangling benevolence from an internalized
fairness norm as the two make incompatible predictions: According to benevolence and the
HEXACO theory individuals high in Honesty-Humility should use their redistribution-power
to reduce asymmetry only if they themselves are in the advantageous position. That is, if
participants get more points than the other for the same performance in the quiz, those high in
Honesty-Humility should give points to the other to reduce this unfairness. However, if
participants actually get fewer points in the quiz than the other, those high in Honesty-Humility
should refrain from taking points from the other due to their unwillingness to abuse their power
over the final distribution of points. Participants’ final share should therefore be lower for
individuals with high levels of Honesty-Humility, irrespective of the direction of asymmetry. In
BENEVOLENCE OR (BLIND) FAIRNESS NORM 7
statistical terms, the benevolence account would thus essentially imply a main effect of Honesty-
Humility on the final share.
By contrast, according to an internalized fairness norm, individuals high in Honesty-
Humility should be motivated to establish fairness in general, that is, an equal split (e.g.,
Kahneman, Knetsch, & Thaler, 1986; Van Dijk & Vermunt, 2000). Hence, they should not only
give points to the other if they themselves are profiting from the asymmetry, but also take points
from the other if the other is in the advantageous position. In other words, the fairness norm
account predicts that the final distribution is always fair for individuals high in Honesty-Humility
no matter who was initially advantaged. In turn, the fairness norm account would imply an
interaction between Honesty-Humility and the direction of asymmetry (advantage-self vs.
advantage-other) in predicting the final share. Specifically, it would predict that individuals high
in Honesty-Humility decrease their final share if they are initially advantaged but also increase
their final share if they are initially disadvantaged.
Method
Materials
To measure Honesty-Humility, we used the German version (Moshagen, Hilbig, &
Zettler, 2014) of the 60-item HEXACO Personality Inventory Revised (HEXACO-60; Ashton &
Lee, 2009). For each of the six HEXACO dimensions, the inventory contains 10 items to be
answered on a five-point Likert-type scale. Sample items for Honesty-Humility are “Having a lot
of money is not especially important to me” and “I would never accept a bribe, even if it were
very large”. In our study, Cronbach’s α for Honesty-Humility was .79.
BENEVOLENCE OR (BLIND) FAIRNESS NORM 8
As introduced above, individuals’ redistribution decision in a fairness paradigm
2
(that is,
the final distribution of points in this paradigm) served as the main dependent variable to
differentiate between benevolence and internalized fairness norm. The fairness paradigm was
designed to measure participants’ willingness to rectify an existing asymmetry in input-outcome-
ratio, depending on the direction and the degree of the asymmetry. In the paradigm, participants
are coupled with an unknown other.
3
Both earn points by taking a quiz (15 multiple choice tasks
with four response options each) on verbal and mathematical skills (similar to the 10-Minute-
Test; cf. Hilbig, Heydasch, & Zettler, 2014). As an exemplary task consider: “Which of the
following fractions is smallest: 2/4, 2/5, 1/3, or 3/8?”. For each correct answer in the quiz,
participants (and the other) earned points. To ensure that solving the tasks was strenuous to some
extent thus eliciting a feeling of earning the points we speeded the quiz with a limit of five
minutes. Supporting the suitability of this time limit, participants in an independent lab-based
pre-study (N = 63, 30 female, aged 18-27, M = 20.4, SD = 1.8 years) solved M = 8.8 (59%) tasks
(SD = 2.8) correctly and only three participants (< 5% of the sample) were able to solve 14 or all
15 tasks, despite substantial monetary incentives (1.00€ for each task solved, thus up to 15.00€ in
five minutes). Also, in this pre-study, performance in the quiz was unrelated to the six HEXACO
factors (as assessed via the German HEXACO-60), all |r| < .15.
2
The fairness paradigm is structurally similar to a variant of the dictator game in which dictators earn
their endowment (Cherry, Frykblom, & Shogren, 2002). However, contrary to the standard dictator game,
both individuals earn an endowment and, more importantly there is asymmetry in the earnings (different
payoff for the same effort or success).
3
Note that the paradigm was completely hypothetical in nature, meaning that participants were asked to
imagine both the other person and the points earned and to be distributed. Thus the points did not convert
to actual monetary stakes.
BENEVOLENCE OR (BLIND) FAIRNESS NORM 9
Following the quiz, participants learned how many tasks they had solved correctly and
how many points this earned them. They also learned about the number of correct solutions
given by the hypothetical other which was always the same as the participants’. As sketched
above, this performance was then translated into hypothetical payoffs (points) asymmetrically.
Specifically, participants received either more (advantage-self) or less (advantage-other) points
than the other for the same number of correct responses. In addition, we varied the degree of
asymmetry, such that one individual’s points were either doubled (moderate asymmetry) or
quadrupled (severe asymmetry) as compared to her counterpart’s points. For illustration, Table 1
displays example payoffs (assuming 10 correct solutions) for the four experimental conditions to
which participants were randomly assigned.
Finally, participants were given the power to redistribute all the points between
themselves and the other. That is, they could reduce the asymmetry by either giving points to the
other or taking points from the other. As dependent measure, we used participants’ relative
outcome, that is, the ratio of (i) a participant’s own points after the redistribution to (ii) the sum
of all points earned by both individuals. The relative outcome hence mirrors a participant’s final
share. For the sake of simplicity, we subtracted 0.5 from this proportion so that a value of 0
mirrors establishment of an equal split (i.e., no asymmetry left after redistribution). Any value
larger than 0 indicates that participants gained beyond the equal split through their redistribution
choice; any value smaller than 0 indicates the opposite (the other came off better than implied by
the equal split). Note that, by implication, the same value of the relative outcome implies
different redistribution choices across the two conditions. For example, a value of 0 indicates
giving in the advantage-self condition, but taking in the advantage-other condition.
BENEVOLENCE OR (BLIND) FAIRNESS NORM 10
Procedure and Participants
The study was run online in close adherence to common standards for Internet-based
experimenting (Reips, 2002). After providing informed consent and demographic information,
participants completed the HEXACO-60. Next, they worked on the quiz in which they (and the
other) earned the points. After the quiz, participants received information on their own and the
other’s earnings and decided on the final distribution of points by giving, taking, or leaving the
points untouched. As a manipulation check, we finally asked participants about the strenuousness
of the quiz and further assessed the seriousness of participation.
Participants were recruited from a community sample via mailing lists and social
networks. Out of 185 participants who started the study, N = 146 (82 female, aged 18-62, M =
25.3, SD = 8.4) completed all tasks and were thus considered in the analyses.
4
The majority
(72.6%) of participants were students, 15.8% were employees. Participants were almost equally
distributed across the experimental conditions, with n = 72 in the advantage-self (n = 36
moderate and n = 36 severe) and n = 74 in the advantage-other (n = 36 moderate and n = 38
severe) condition.
Results
As intended by the chosen time limit and supporting the “earning character” of the
fairness paradigm participants perceived the quiz to be moderately strenuous (M = 2.89, SD =
0.83, on a 5-point Likert-type scale). On average, they solved 8 out of 15 tasks (SD = 3.0) which
4
As indicated by a post-hoc power analysis using G*Power (Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, 2007),
this sample size yielded a high statistical power to detect small-to-medium sized ( = .10) main effects (in
case of two predictors in total, 1-β = .99) as well as small-to-medium sized interaction effects (1-β = .93;
significant increase in explained variance when adding an interaction term) in a multiple linear regression.
BENEVOLENCE OR (BLIND) FAIRNESS NORM 11
is highly similar to the performance observed in the incentivized pilot study summarized above.
In turn, participants and others earned a total of 67.7 points (SD = 28.5). In the redistribution of
these points, 42% of participants took points from the other (on average 19.2 points, SD = 13.2),
31% gave points to the other (on average 14.8 points, SD = 8.9), and 27% left the distribution
untouched. A majority of 51% of participants established an equal split, 32% advantaged
themselves, and 17% advantaged the other. Correspondingly, after the redistribution, participants
had 35.7 points on average (SD = 18.6) whereas the other had 31.9 points on average (SD =
17.9).
For the relative outcome (i.e., the final proportion of participant’s points), Table 2
provides the means and standard deviations for all four experimental conditions. As can be seen
(in a column-wise comparison), the relative outcome was larger when participants themselves
profited from the asymmetry (M = 0.12, SD = 0.13) as compared to the condition in which they
were disadvantaged (M = -0.05, SD = 0.15). In particular, in the advantage-self condition,
participants were less willing to shift towards the equal split. This main effect was confirmed by
a 2 (advantage: self vs. other) by 2 (degree of asymmetry: moderate vs. severe) ANOVA with
F(1, 142) = 47.49, p < .001, ηp2 = .25. In contrast, the severity of asymmetry (row-wise
comparison in Table 2) did not influence participants relative outcome, F(1, 142) < 1, p = .658,
ηp2 < .01. That is, participants were as willing to establish the equal distribution if the asymmetry
was moderate (M = 0.04, SD = 0.14) as compared to when the asymmetry was severe (M = 0.03,
SD = 0.18). Finally, there was no interaction between the direction and the degree of asymmetry
(F < 1). For this reason and given that our main question refers to the effect of who is
advantaged (i.e., self vs. other), not how severely someone is advantaged, on the redistribution
decision we discarded the severity-factor in the following analyses.
BENEVOLENCE OR (BLIND) FAIRNESS NORM 12
To test the main hypotheses, we regressed the direction of asymmetry (dummy coded),
Honesty-Humility, and their interaction on the relative outcome. First off, this analysis revealed a
(positive) main effect of the direction of asymmetry, indicating a larger relative outcome in the
advantage-self condition than in the advantage-other condition, β = .52, p < .001. Furthermore
in line with the benevolence account we found a (negative) main effect of Honesty-Humility
on the relative outcome, β = -.24, p = .013. That is, high levels of Honesty-Humility were
generally associated with smaller values in the relative outcome, irrespective of the direction of
asymmetry. However, in contrast to the predictions of the fairness norm account, there was no
interaction between Honesty-Humility and the direction of asymmetry, β = -.06, p = .529.
5
The
results were comparable when controlling for the remaining HEXACO dimensions in a multiple
regression analysis.
6
As can be seen in Figure 1, individuals low in Honesty-Humility left the
asymmetry untouched whenever they benefited from it but strongly reduced the asymmetry (took
points from the other) whenever it disadvantaged them. By contrast, their counterparts high in
Honesty-Humility reduced the asymmetry especially whenever they originally profited from it
(gave points to the other), but did not establish a full equal split whenever they were initially
disadvantaged.
Discussion
Fairness represents a vital aspect of interpersonal and societal functioning. Regarding
individual differences in fairness and traits driving fair behavior, recent developments point to
5
Moreover, this interaction did not depend on the severity of asymmetry as indicated by a non-significant
three-way-interaction between Honesty-Humility, direction, and severity of asymmetry, β = .02, p = .895.
6
The corresponding regression coefficients were β = .00 (Emotionality), β = .08 (Extraversion), β = -.08
(Agreeableness), β = .11 (Conscientiousness), and β = -.09 (Openness), all p > .19.
BENEVOLENCE OR (BLIND) FAIRNESS NORM 13
Honesty-Humility as a vital basic trait dimension (cf. Ashton et al., 2014). In line with its
theoretical conceptualization, past research has consistently linked Honesty-Humility to fairness-
related and pro-social behaviors. Notably as indicated by our re-analysis of existent findings on
economic behavior Honesty-Humility is not only associated with pro-sociality in relative
terms, but also with an inclination to establish fairness (i.e., equality) in absolute terms.
However, different explanations may account for why these individuals show such a strong
tendency to establish fairness. First, the HEXACO theory primarily suggests that benevolence
drives these effects in that individuals high in Honesty-Humility avoid exploiting others.
However, prior evidence cannot rule out an internalized (blind) fairness norm (i.e., a general
preference for equality) in these individuals.
To tease the two explanations apart, the present study assessed participants’ willingness to
rectify an initial asymmetry in rewards, that is, unfair outcomes following a demanding task.
Specifically, it was tested whether individuals differ in their willingness to redistribute outcomes
conditional upon (a) whether they themselves or the other profited from the asymmetric
distribution of rewards and (b) their individual level of Honesty-Humility. In line with the
benevolence account, our results show that individuals high in Honesty-Humility reduce an
asymmetry and establish an equal split if and only if they themselves are the ones profiting from
the unfairness. If, in turn, it is to their disadvantage, they do not alter the asymmetric distribution
as strongly and mostly refrain from taking points from the other (even though this would actually
result in a more fair distribution). These results corroborate the non-exploitation aspect of
Honesty-Humility as the motivating force behind fair behavior, but are incompatible with the
alternative notion of a general (blind) fairness norm. Of course, we cannot claim that
benevolence is the only explanation that might account for the observed pattern of results.
BENEVOLENCE OR (BLIND) FAIRNESS NORM 14
However, among the two accounts that we derived from the extant literature, the data exclusively
supports one (benevolence) and contradicts the other (blind fairness norm). In turn, our findings
support the conceptualization of Honesty-Humility in the HEXACO model, demonstrating that
individuals with high factor levels do not take advantage of their power even if they themselves
are in a disadvantageous position.
Extending prior research, our study is the first specifically designed to test why individuals
with high levels in Honesty-Humility are inclined towards fair behavior. We hence provide a
first toehold towards explaining the consistently observed link between Honesty-Humility and
fairness-related behaviors. However, as a limitation, our findings are based on a hypothetical
scenario without actual stakes, thus potentially limiting the conclusiveness of results due to
socially desirable responding. Arguably, fairness is socially desirable (Hoffman, McCabe,
Shachat, & Smith, 1994) as are high levels of Honesty-Humility (Hilbig, Glöckner, et al., 2014).
Nonetheless, as indicated by recent research, the relation between Honesty-Humility and non-
exploitation seems to be equivalent in hypothetical scenarios vs. real decisions with actual stakes
(Hilbig et al., 2015).
In conclusion, the present study provides first evidence that the willingness to establish
fairness shown by individuals high in Honesty-Humility can be traced back to benevolence and a
reluctance to take advantage of others’ inferiority, but not to a general preference for equality in
terms of adherence to a “blind” fairness norm. Given that Honesty-Humility explains a variety of
socially relevant behaviors such as crime, delinquency, and cooperation (e.g., Hilbig, Glöckner,
et al., 2014; Van Gelder & De Vries, 2014), it is important to understand these behavioral
tendencies, shedding light on the “black box” between Honesty-Humility and pro-social
behavior.
BENEVOLENCE OR (BLIND) FAIRNESS NORM 15
Acknowledgements
The work reported herein was supported by a grant to the first author from the Baden-
Württemberg Foundation (Germany).
BENEVOLENCE OR (BLIND) FAIRNESS NORM 16
References
Ackermann, K. A., Fleiß, J., & Murphy, R. O. (in press). Reciprocity as an individual difference.
Journal of Conflict Resolution. doi:10.1177/0022002714541854
Arneson, R. J. (1982). The principle of fairness and free-rider problems. Ethics, 92(4).
Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2007). Empirical, theoretical, and practical advantages of the
HEXACO model of personality structure. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11(2),
150166. doi:10.1177/1088868306294907
Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2009). The HEXACO-60: A short measure of the major dimensions
of personality. Journal of Personality Assessment, 91(4), 340345.
doi:10.1080/00223890902935878
Ashton, M. C., Lee, K., & De Vries, R. E. (2014). The HEXACO Honesty-Humility,
Agreeableness, and Emotionality factors: A review of research and theory. Personality and
Social Psychology Review, 18(2), 139152. doi:10.1177/1088868314523838
Ben-Ner, A., Kong, F., & Putterman, L. (2004). Share and share alike? Gender-pairing,
personality, and cognitive ability as determinants of giving. Journal of Economic
Psychology, 25(5), 581589. doi:10.1016/S0167-4870(03)00065-5
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (rev. ed.). Hillsdale, NJ
England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Dunlop, P. D., Morrison, D. L., Koenig, J., & Silcox, B. (2012). Comparing the Eysenck and
HEXACO models of personality in the prediction of adult delinquency. European Journal
of Personality, 26(3), 194202. doi:10.1002/per.824
BENEVOLENCE OR (BLIND) FAIRNESS NORM 17
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical
power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior
Research Methods, 39(2), 175191. doi:10.3758/BF03193146
Fehr, E., & Schmidt, K. M. (1999). A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. Quarterly
Journal of Economics, 114(3), 817868. doi:10.1162/003355399556151
Field, A. P. (2001). Meta-analysis of correlation coefficients: A Monte Carlo comparison of
fixed- and random-effects methods. Psychological Methods, 6(2), 161180.
Hilbig, B. E., Glöckner, A., & Zettler, I. (2014). Personality and pro-social behavior: Linking
basic traits and social value orientations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
107(3), 529539. doi:10.1037/a0036074
Hilbig, B. E., Heydasch, T., & Zettler, I. (2014). To boast or not to boast: Testing the humility
aspect of the Honesty-Humility factor. Personality and Individual Differences, 69, 1216.
Hilbig, B. E., Thielmann, I., Hepp, J., Klein, S., & Zettler, I. (2015). From personality to
altruistic behavior (and back): Evidence from a double-blind dictator game. Journal of
Research in Personality, 55, 4650. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2014.12.004
Hilbig, B. E., & Zettler, I. (2009). Pillars of cooperation: Honesty-Humility, social value
orientations, and economic behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(3), 516519.
doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2009.01.003
Hilbig, B. E., Zettler, I., Leist, F., & Heydasch, T. (2013). It takes two: HonestyHumility and
Agreeableness differentially predict active versus reactive cooperation. Personality and
Individual Differences, 54(5), 598603. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.11.008
Hoffman, E., McCabe, K. A., Shachat, K., & Smith, V. (1994). Preferences, property rights, and
anonymity in bargaining games. Games and Economic Behavior, 7(3), 346380.
BENEVOLENCE OR (BLIND) FAIRNESS NORM 18
Huseman, R. C., Hatfield, J. D., & Miles, E. W. (1985). Test for individual perceptions of job
equity: Some preliminary findings. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 61(3, Pt 2), 10551064.
doi:10.2466/pms.1985.61.3f.1055
Huseman, R. C., Hatfield, J. D., & Miles, E. W. (1987). A new perspective on equity theory: The
equity sensitivity construct. The Academy of Management Review, 12(2), 222234.
doi:10.2307/258531
Kahneman, D., Knetsch, J. L., & Thaler, R. H. (1986). Fairness and the assumptions of
economics. Journal of Business, 59(4), S285S300.
Kamas, L., & Preston, A. (2012). Distributive and reciprocal fairness: What can we learn from
the heterogeneity of social preferences? Journal of Economic Psychology, 33(3), 538553.
doi:10.1016/j.joep.2011.12.003
Kurzban, R., & Houser, D. (2001). Individual differences in cooperation in a circular public
goods game. European Journal of Personality, 15(1,SpecIssue), S37S52.
doi:10.1002/per.420
Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2004). Psychometric properties of the HEXACO personality
inventory. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39(2), 329358.
doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr3902_8
Lee, K., Ashton, M. C., Morrison, D. L., Cordery, J., & Dunlop, P. D. (2008). Predicting
integrity with the HEXACO personality model: Use of self- and observer reports. Journal of
Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 81(1), 147167.
doi:10.1348/096317907X195175
BENEVOLENCE OR (BLIND) FAIRNESS NORM 19
Marcus, B., Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2007). Personality dimensions explaining relationships
between integrity tests and counterproductive behavior: Big five, or one in addition?
Personnel Psychology, 60(1), 134. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.2007.00063.x
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1985). Updating Norman’s “adequacy taxonomy”: Intelligence
and personality dimensions in natural language and in questionnaires. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 49(3), 710721. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.49.3.710
Mellers, B. A., Haselhuhn, M. P., Tetlock, P. E., Silva, J. C., & Isen, A. M. (2010). Predicting
behavior in economic games by looking through the eyes of the players. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General, 139(4), 743755. doi:10.1037/a0020280
Moshagen, M., Hilbig, B. E., & Zettler, I. (2014). Faktorstruktur, psychometrische Eigenschaften
und Messinvarianz der deutschen Version des 60-Item HEXACO Persönlichkeitsinventars.
Diagnostica, 60(2), 8697.
Reips, U.-D. (2002). Standards for Internet-based experimenting. Experimental Psychology,
49(4), 243256. doi:10.1026//1618-3169.49.4.243
Sullivan, B. A., Snyder, M., & Sullivan, J. L. (2008). The centrality of cooperation in the
functioning of individuals and groups. In B. A. Sullivan, M. Snyder, & J. L. Sullivan (Eds.),
Cooperation: The political psychology of effective human interaction. (pp. 116). Malden:
Blackwell Publishing.
Thielmann, I., & Hilbig, B. E. (2014). Trust in me, trust in you: A social projection account of
the link between personality, cooperativeness, and trustworthiness expectations. Journal of
Research in Personality, 50(3), 6165. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2014.03.006
BENEVOLENCE OR (BLIND) FAIRNESS NORM 20
Thielmann, I., Hilbig, B. E., & Niedtfeld, I. (2014). Willing to give but not to forgive: Borderline
personality features and cooperative behavior. Journal of Personality Disorders, 28(6),
778795. doi:10.1521/pedi_2014_28_135
Van Dijk, E., & Vermunt, R. (2000). Strategy and fairness in social decision making: Sometimes
it pays to be powerless. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36(1), 125.
doi:10.1006/jesp.1999.1392
Van Gelder, J.-L., & De Vries, R. E. (2014). Rational misbehavior? Evaluating an integrated
dual-process model of criminal decision making. Journal of Quantitative Criminology,
30(1), 127. doi:10.1007/s10940-012-9192-8
Zhao, K., & Smillie, L. D. (in press). The role of interpersonal traits in social decision making:
Exploring sources of behavioral heterogeneity in economic games. Personality and Social
Psychology Review. doi:10.1177/1088868314553709
BENEVOLENCE OR (BLIND) FAIRNESS NORM 21
Tables
Table 1. Exemplary Initial Allocation of Points for a Participant with 10 Correct Solutions in the
Quiz, Separated by Experimental Condition.
Table 2. Means and Standard Deviations of the Final Proportion of Participants Points on All
Points (i.e., Relative Outcome) Separated for the Four Experimental Conditions.
Advantage-self
Advantage-other
Degree of asymmetry
SD
M
Moderate
0.12
-0.03
Severe
0.15
-0.06
Note. A value of 0 indicates the equal split; values larger than 0 indicate that the final distribution
was beneficial for the participant relative to the other (and vice versa for values below 0).
Advantage-self
Advantage-other
Degree of asymmetry
Own points
Others’ points
Own points
Others’ points
Moderate
40
20
20
40
Severe
80
20
20
80
BENEVOLENCE OR (BLIND) FAIRNESS NORM 22
Figures
Figure 1. Relation between relative outcome and Honesty-Humility (centered on the sample
mean) separated for advantage-self (left) and advantage-other condition (right). The dotted line
indicates the equal split distribution; the dashed line indicates the initial distribution prior to the
redistribution choice. Confidence bands represent the 95% confidence intervals.
... For instance, the basic personality trait Honesty-Humility from the HEXACO Model of Personality-defined as "the tendency to be fair and genuine when dealing with others" (Ashton & Lee, 2007 p. 156)-has been found to consistently predict a range of pro-and antisocial behaviors, such as cooperation, dishonesty, and generosity (for meta-analytical evidence, see e.g., Zettler et al., 2020). But while the relation between Honesty-Humility and pro-and antisocial behavior is well-established, the question of why Honesty-Humility predicts such behavior has been investigated to a far lesser extent (e.g., Hilbig et al., 2015;Pfattheicher & Böhm, 2018;Ścigała et al., 2020b;Thielmann et al., 2021). ...
... Taken together, our findings suggest that one of the reasons why individuals with higher levels of Honesty-Humility engage in more prosocial and less antisocial behavior might be fairness/justice concerns, rather than care/harm, loyalty/betrayal, authority/subversion, and sanctity/degradation concerns. Indeed, such a possibility is in line with the theoretical understanding of Honesty-Humility as a trait primarily focused on fairness (e.g., Ashton & Lee, 2007;Ashton & Lee, 2009;Hilbig et al., 2015), as well as with research showing that in contexts when fairness is at odds with other moral concerns such as care and loyalty, high-Honesty-Humility individuals tend to act in line with the former (e.g., Ścigała et al., 2019;Thielmann et al., 2021). Other research, however, points out that under certain circumstances, individuals high in Honesty-Humility rather prefer loyalty over fairness/honesty (Ścigała et al., 2020a). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Does the basic trait Honesty-Humility predict the type of moral concerns people express in language? We explore whether Honesty-Humility relates to the expression of five moral concerns in language—namely, care/harm, justice/fairness, loyalty/betrayal, authority/subversion, and sanctity/degradation—as conceptualized by the Moral Foundations Theory. Using Natural Language Processing, we screened 16,497 (un)ethical justifications—i.e., reasons for behaving (un)ethically—for the presence of the five moral concerns (N = 901). We found that Honesty-Humility related positively to justice/fairness concerns, but it did not relate to care/harm, loyalty/betrayal, authority/subversion, and sanctity/degradation concerns. Our findings thus suggest that justice/fairness concerns might serve as one of the mechanisms relating Honesty-Humility to anti- and prosocial behavior.
... However, the effect of meta-agency on corruption is likely to vary at distinct levels of benevolence and internalization. First, benevolence is a norm (Hilbig et al., 2015) that manifests in ethnic contexts as a form of ethnic capital that increases resources of ethnic members (Zoogah, 2018;. To be benevolent to ethnic members individuals may engage in actions that seem immoral or unethical, particularly from a Western perspective (de Sardan, 1999) but which are morally accepted within the cultural context and consistent with ethnic norms (Kuba et al., 2005;Ekeh, 1975;van den Berghe, 1973). ...
Article
Full-text available
There is increased interest in deviant behavior in the workplace. However, research is lacking on the moral economy of such behavior. Moral economy is particularly important in contexts where syncretic forces impinge on deviant behavior. Consequently, we use moral economy reasoning to examine the relationship between ethnic obligation and deviant behavior in the African context. In Study 1, data (N = 27, 148) show an inverted U-shape effect of meta-agency and deviant behavior. In Study 2, difference-in-difference (DID) analysis of data (N = 25,387 in 2005 and N = 51,587 in 2013) shows significant DID effect of meta-agency. Primary data from Study 3 (N = 229) shows a U-shaped effect of agency and deviant behavior. In Study 4 (N = 333), a replication shows support for the U-shape effect in Study 3. The four studies thus show complex patterns that reflect U-shape and inverted U-shape relationships between ethnic obligation and deviant behavior as indicated by bribery, corruption, and unethicality. Robustness checks support the curvilinear relationships suggesting that the effects are not due to chance. We discuss implications for research and practice.
... One possibility is that the relationship between perceived control and benevolence is the sole reason studies have seen a positive correlation between trust and interactivity in learning systems. While incorporating user feedback into a system may or may not communicate ability on behalf of said system, it does communicate a certain sense of fairness within the human-agent team, similar to that in human teams [29,61], an element which is closely related to benevolence [22]. On the other hand, perceived control could exhibit a stronger relationship with general trust and competence given a larger dataset. ...
... Perspectives from personality psychology may offer some insight into the disparate results between Ranehill et al. (2018) and Strojny et al. (2021). Previous research has demonstrated a link between personality factors and altruistic giving (e.g., Honesty-Humility trait, agreeableness; Hilbig et al., 2015). By using a within person design, it is possible that three months of HC use in Ranehill et al. (2018) experiment was insufficient to produce within-subject change in dictator game behavior. ...
Article
Full-text available
Emerging evidence suggests that hormonal contraceptives (HCs) impact psychological outcomes through alterations in neurophysiology. In this review, we first introduce a theoretical framework for HCs as disruptors of steroid hormone modulation of socially competitive attitudes and behaviors. Then, we comprehensively examine prior research comparing HC users and non-users in outcomes related to competition for reproductive, social, and financial resources. Synthesis of 46 studies (n = 16,290) led to several key conclusions: HC users do not show the same menstrual cycle-related fluctuations in self-perceived attractiveness and some intrasexual competition seen in naturally cycling women and, further, may show relatively reduced status- or achievement-oriented competitive motivation. However, there a lack of consistent or compelling evidence that HC users and non-users differ in competitive behavior or attitudes for mates or financial resources. These conclusions are tentative given the notable methodological limitations of the studies reviewed. Implications and recommendations for future research are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction In this longitudinal study, we examine the potential costs and benefits of humility for well-being and civic trust among immigrants in a pluralistic democracy. Methods With data from 14,864 immigrant participants from a nationwide random sample in New Zealand, we used multilevel modeling to examine the associations of general humility (i.e., honesty-humility modesty) with well-being (life satisfaction and meaning) and civic trust (trust in police) over time in contexts with varying levels of ethnic deprivation and perceived religious discrimination. We hypothesized that (a) humility would correlate positively with well-being and civic trust (Hypothesis 1), (b) these associations would be attenuated in the contexts where perceptions of ethnic deprivation and religious discrimination are high (Hypothesis 2), and (c) these interaction effects would become more pronounced when cultural identities are salient (Hypothesis 3). Results Multilevel modeling revealed partial support for these hypotheses. Although humility correlated positively with well-being and trust in police over time, the two-way and three-way interactions did not yield substantial support for Hypotheses 2 and 3, respectively. The context of religious discrimination did, however, marginally attenuate the positive association between humility and trust in police. Discussion Collectively, these results demonstrate that humility is associated with multiple benefits to well-being and civic trust and has few—if any—potential drawbacks.
Article
Full-text available
-This study investigates relationships between spiritual capital and organisational resilience of bank employees in Nigeria. The sample consisted of one hundred and ninety-eight (198) respondents from ten (10) purposively selected area offices of banks in Anambra State SouthEast geographical zone of Nigeria. The questionnaire was in 5 Likert type ordinal scales and the Spearman's Rank Order Correlation Coefficient Statistical Technique in the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) was used for data analyses. It was revealed that all facets of spiritual capital showed positive and significant relationship with both adaptability and flexibility orientations. Although other researchers dwelt more on how spiritual leaders motivate others, formulate models of trust, tolerance, courage, and forgiveness to strengthen and develop spiritual culture, but this study has established further that spiritual leaders could not only motivate subordinates for positive organisatonal outcomes but promote organisational resilience through its adaptive and flexibility capacity. When properly managed it promotes strong commitment of employees and shareholders for sharing wealth, disseminating knowledge, enhancing well-being of customers, thus engendering progress and collective well-being. Firms high in spiritual capital meet relatedness needs of employees inherent in Maslow's model in terms of reciprocity, as organisational members expect atmosphere rich in interpersonal relationships and emotional support. Managers of banks should embrace these spiritual capital facets used in this study in their order of importance to promote adaptability and flexibility orientations and enhance organisations resilience, improve its reputation, healthy corporate culture, comradeship, teamwork, and motivated workforce, encourage discipline and justice and provide unique socially beneficial services for its members and customers.
Article
Full-text available
Fairness is a key expectation in social interactions. Its violation leads to adverse reactions, including non-cooperation and dishonesty. The present study aimed to examine how (1) fair (unfair) treatment may drive cooperation (defection) and honesty (self-serving dishonesty), (2) dishonesty primes further moral disengagement and reduced cooperation, and (3) dishonesty weakens (substitutes) the effect of fairness on cooperation. The prisoner’s dilemma (Experiment 1 and 2) and die-rolling task (Experiment 2) were employed for capturing cooperation and dishonest behaviors, respectively. To manipulate perceived unfairness, participants were randomly assigned to play the prisoner’s dilemma game, where players either choose more cooperation (fair condition) or defection (unfair condition). Results of Experiment 1 (n = 102) suggested that participants perceive higher unfairness and behave less cooperatively when the other player primarily chooses defection. Results of Exp. 2 (n = 240) (a) confirmed Exp. 1 results, (b) showed that players in the unfair condition also show more self-serving dishonest behavior, and (c) that dishonest behavior weakens the effect of fairness on cooperation. Together, these results extended previous work by highlighting the self-serving lies when the opponent is fair trigger higher cooperation, presumably as a means to alleviate self-reflective moral emotions or restore justice.
Article
Full-text available
Individual differences in morality and honesty are inherently connected such that individuals more likely to engage in immoral behavior will typically be less likely to admit to it. Indeed, this is a central prediction of the Honesty-Humility (HH) factor of personality, recently proposed as part of the HEXACO Model of Personality (Honesty-Humility [H], Emotionality [E], Extraversion [X], Agreeablenes [A], Conscientiousness [C], and Openness to Experience [O]). However, this very link between morality and honesty necessarily limits the conclusiveness of typical investigations—especially those relying on self-report data for both constructs. Herein, we apply an indirect questioning technique to overcome such limitations and provide evidence for individual differences in trait honesty and morality in line with the HH factor of personality. Specifically, results show that individuals low in (self-reported) HH are indeed more likely to engage in immoral behavior although less likely to be upfront about it when directly questioned.
Article
Full-text available
There is large individual variation in altruistic behavior, spurring recurring calls for an integration of behavioral economics and personality research. However, the empirical picture has remained inconsistent. To overcome the limitations of prior work, we consider (and compare) both the classic five-factor and the HEXACO models of personality structure, use a double-blind dictator game to strictly rule out spurious effects of social desirability, and extend the research focus to include the recipient side. Results show that (i) Honesty-Humility is the primary factor to predict dictators' altruistic behavior which (ii) predicts recipients' fairness perceptions (in combination with their prior expectations) which (iii) predict recipients' (zero-acquaintance) observer judgments of dictators' trait Honesty-Humility which are (iv) associated with dictators' true (self-reported) Honesty-Humility scores.
Article
Full-text available
Im letzten Jahrzehnt wurden in lexikalischen Untersuchungen über diverse Sprachen und Kulturen hinweg sechs grundlegende Persönlichkeitseigenschaften identifiziert, welche im HEXACO-Modell der Persönlichkeit abgebildet werden. Die vorliegende Untersuchung behandelt eine umfangreiche psychometrische Evaluation der deutschen Version des 60-Item HEXACO-Persönlichkeitsinventars. Die sechsfaktorielle Struktur wurde anhand einer großen Stichprobe (N = 3240) bestätigt. Die resultierenden Faktoren verfügten über zufriedenstellende Reliabilitäten und waren praktisch unkorreliert. Überdies konnte Messinvarianz über das Geschlecht belegt werden. Anhand einer weiteren Stichprobe (N = 1012) wurde gezeigt, dass die 60-Item Version zu keinem bedeutsamen Informationsverlust gegenüber der 96-Item Version führt. Die Bestimmung von Korrelationen zu den Big-Five Persönlichkeitsfaktoren mittels einer dritten Stichprobe (N = 235) erbrachte durchweg erwartungskonforme Ergebnisse. Letztlich konnten anhand einer vierten Stichprobe (N = 31) hohe Retest-Reliabilitäten über einen Zeitraum von 7 Monaten belegt werden. Somit eignet sich die deutschsprachige Version des HEXACO-60 Persönlichkeitsinventars zur reliablen, validen und verhältnismäßig ökonomischen Erfassung der sechs Faktoren des HEXACO-Modells der Persönlichkeit.
Article
Full-text available
Concerning the dispositional determinants of prosocial behavior and cooperation, work based on the classic 5 personality factors, and especially Agreeableness, has turned out somewhat inconsistent. A clearer picture has emerged from consideration of the HEXACO model of personality-though supported entirely by hypothetical behavior as criterion, so far. Thus, in 2 studies and a reanalysis, we investigated "actual behavior" in the form of individually and socially consequential distribution decisions. As expected, HEXACO Honesty-Humility consistently predicted prosocial behavior, including a theory-consistent pattern on the facet level. Importantly, this pattern might explain why five-factor Agreeableness has only sometimes been found to account for prosocial behavior. Indeed, further results indicate that five-factor Agreeableness comprises some aspects that are predictive of prosocial behavior-aspects well covered by HEXACO Honesty-Humility-but also others that play no role for this criterion. As such, the links between five-factor Agreeableness and prosocial behavior are well-covered by HEXACO Honesty-Humility, but not vice versa. Taken together, these findings hint that especially HEXACO Honesty-Humility (and certain aspects of five-factor Agreeableness) account for prosocial behavior-thus explaining previous inconsistencies and providing a more nuanced understanding of the links between basic personality and prosocial or cooperative behavior. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2014 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Equity theory proposes that individuals who perceive themselves as either underrewarded or overrewarded will experience distress, and that this distress leads to efforts to restore equity. This paper describes a new construct, equity sensitivity, and proposes that reactions to equity/inequity are a function of an individual's preferences for different outcome/input ratios. The construct is delineated through a series of propositions, and implications for equity research in organizations are discussed.
Article
Economic games are well-established experimental paradigms for modeling social decision making. A large body of literature has pointed to the heterogeneity of behavior within many of these games, which might be partly explained by broad interpersonal trait dispositions. Using the Big Five and HEXACO (Honesty-Humility, Emotionality, eXtraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Openness to Experience) personality frameworks, we review the role of personality in two main classes of economic games: social dilemmas and bargaining games. This reveals an emerging role for Big Five agreeableness in promoting cooperative, egalitarian, and altruistic behaviors across several games, consistent with its core characteristic of maintaining harmonious interpersonal relations. The role for extraversion is less clear, which may reflect the divergent effects of its underlying agentic and affiliative motivational components. In addition, HEXACO honesty-humility and agreeableness may capture distinct aspects of prosocial behavior outside the bounds of the Five-Factor Model. Important considerations and directions for future studies are discussed within the emerging personality-economics interface. © 2014 by the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.
Article
For 880 persons in management and professional positions mean satisfaction for those classified as Benevolent, Equity Sensitive, and Entitled when under-equitably or over-rewarded showed main effects of sensitivity to equity and perceptions of equity, but no interactions. Further work is suggested.
Article
Interest in the HEXACO Model of Personality has consistently grown in the past decade – with a particular focus on the newly proposed sixth basic factor: Honesty–Humility. In essence, this factor was proposed as the driving force behind cooperative, honest behavior and the basis for a modest, unassuming nature. However, whereas recent research has accumulated substantial evidence for the prediction that Honesty–Humility drives non-exploitative behavior, there is little conclusive evidence for the proposition that it also signals humility. We tested this conjecture in a web-based study – assessing modest behavior in terms of people’s over- vs. under-representation of their actual abilities. As hypothesized, the Honesty–Humility factor scale and the Modesty facet scale of the HEXACO Personality Inventory-Revised predicted the degree to which participants’ self-appraisals matched their actual general mental ability in the expected direction: those low in Honesty–Humility and/or Modesty overstated their ability.