Technical ReportPDF Available

Exploring the Longitudinal Relationship between Arts Engagement and Health

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

A new report, published by Arts for Health (Manchester Metropolitan University) on Thursday 12 February 2015, reveals that engaging with the arts and culture generally has a positive long-term effect on health and wellbeing. Research undertaken by Dr. Rebecca Gordon-Nesbitt has uncovered evidence, stretching back a number of decades, which shows a significant association between engaging with the arts and longer lives better lived. Under the auspices of the Cultural Value Project – initiated by the Arts and Humanities Research Council, the UK’s main academic funder in the field – Dr. Gordon-Nesbitt has compiled an evidence base comprised of fifteen longitudinal studies. These international studies collectively suggest that attending high-quality cultural events has a beneficial impact upon a range of chronic diseases over time. This includes cancer, heart disease, dementia and obesity, with an inevitable knock-on effect upon life expectancy. Many possible reasons for this positive association are speculated upon by the researchers brought together in this report – from increased social capital to psycho-neuroimmunological responses – all of which are interrogated in detail in Exploring the Longitudinal Relationship between Arts Engagement and Health. One of the most compelling potential explanations for any positive association observed between arts engagement and health comes from the field of epigenetics, specifically the idea that environmental enrichment (in this case, cultural activity) can cause certain harmful genes to be switched off, enabling health-protective effects to be communicated from one generation to the next. In an era in which arts organisations are repeatedly urged to account for themselves in economic terms and we have largely lost sight of the individual and social value of culture, it is hoped that these combined findings will be heeded by policy-makers in the arts and health. As several of the researchers included in the evidence base observe and Dr. Gordon-Nesbitt highlights in her report, there is every chance that any positive health effects attributed to arts engagement are the result of a hidden factor, most likely a socio-economic one. As such, this compelling report urgently incites further research into the inequalities that mediate our access to health and the arts. Dr. Gordon-Nesbitt launched her report at 10am on Thursday 12 February, as part of the day seminar, Chaos and Comfort: The Arts from Long-Term Impact to Social Change, at the Manchester School of Art, Cavendish Street, M15 6BR.
No caption available
… 
Content may be subject to copyright.
Exploring the Longitudinal Relationship
Between Arts Engagement and Health
Rebecca Gordon-Nesbitt
February 2015
Exploring the Longitudinal Relationship between Arts Engagement and Health
Rebecca Gordon-Nesbitt
http://longitudinalhealthbenefits.wordpress.com
Published by:
Clive Parkinson
Arts for Health
Manchester Metropolitan University
Righton Building
Cavendish Street
Manchester
M15 6BG
http://artsforhealthmmu.blogspot.com
ISBN: 978-1-900756-77-8
© Rebecca Gordon-Nesbitt and Arts for Health, 2015
All rights reserved. No part may be reproduced in any manner whatsoever
without the prior written permission from the copyright holders.
With special thanks to all those who assisted with this research.
Proofreading by 100% Proof
www.100percentproof.org
Design by Gary Nip
www.behance.net/garynip
garynip@hotmail.com
www.artsforhealth.org
ARTS FOR HEALTH
www.miriad.mmu.ac.uk
www.art.mmu.ac.uk www.ahrc.ac.uk
Research Framework 1
Foreword 3
Executive Summary 5
Introduction 6
Methodology 9
Critical Analysis of the Evidence Base 13
Discussion 30
Analysis of Possible Mechanisms
Informing the Relationship between
Arts Participation and Health
l Social Capital 36
l Cognition 40
l Occupational Health and Work Strain 42
l Psycho-Neuroimmunology, 44
Endocrine and Metabolic Effects
l Epigenetics 46
Critical Analysis of the Data Sources Relevant
to Explorations of the Long-Term Relationship
Between Arts Participation and Health
l Nordic Datasets 48
l UK-Based Datasets 50
Recommendations for Future Research 55
Concluding Remarks 57
Bibliography 59
Illustrative Table 67
Contents
1
Research Framework
This research was conducted by Rebecca Gordon-Nesbitt at Arts for Health
(Manchester Metropolitan University) under the auspices of the Cultural Value Project
initiated by the Arts and Humanities Research Council (AHRC).
Rebecca Gordon-Nesbitt holds a BSc in Biochemistry with Molecular Biology, an MA in the
History of Art, an MRes in Social Research and a PhD in Sociology. Between 1994 and
2003, she worked as an international curator of contemporary art, latterly at the Nordic
Institute for Contemporary Art, with a responsibility for stimulating visual arts exhibitions,
events, residencies and publications throughout the Nordic region, the UK and Ireland.
She maintains a close relationship with the cultural sector, serving as Researcher-in-Residence
at the Centre for Contemporary Art Derry~Londonderry during the first incarnation of UK City
of Culture and advising Common Practice – a prominent network of small arts organisations
in London and New York. Rebecca has written widely on issues affecting the cultural field,
and her study of the cultural policy of the Cuban Revolution will be published in the US in
spring 2015. She is a founder member of The Centre for Cultural Change (cambiarcultura.
org), which is dedicated to exploring the potential of research and creative practice to
precipitate socio-cultural change.
2
Founded in 1987, Arts for Health (artsforhealth.org) has consistently influenced
research and development in a rapidly evolving global field. Arts for Health is currently
working with people in recovery from substance misuse, exploring the social aspects
of addiction, and, together with European partners, developing a Recoverist Manifesto
which places recovery in a civil rights context. The current focus of Arts for Health is on
less prescriptive notions of arts/health that take inequalities and the social determinants
of health into account. Arts for Health works with NHS partners across the UK, and
has supported the development of arts and health strategy in Australia, Finland and
Lithuania. Current director, Clive Parkinson, is Co-Investigator on the Dementia and
Imagination project, organised as part of the UK’s Arts and Humanities Research
Council (AHRC), Connected Communities programme. This builds on the Treasury-
funded project, Invest to Save: Arts in Health Evaluation, which informed the first House
of Lords debate on arts and health. Clive is also a member of the National Alliance for
Arts, Health and Wellbeing, which is currently involved in the establishment of an
All-Party Parliamentary Group on Arts and Health convened by Lord Howarth of
Newport. He regularly blogs at www.artsforhealthmmu.blogspot.co.uk
In 2012, the AHRC launched the Cultural Value Project, with the aim of making
a major contribution to how we think about the value of arts and culture to individuals
and to society. The Cultural Value Project has sought to establish a framework that
will advance the way in which we talk about the value of cultural engagement and the
methods by which we evaluate that value. Its starting premise has been that we need to
begin by looking at the actual experience of culture and the arts rather than the ancillary
effects of this experience. It is understood that the cultural itself will give coherence to
the framework as a whole. The value begins there, with something fundamental and
irreducible, and all the other components in the framework might be seen, to a greater
or lesser extent, to cascade from it. In giving priority to the cultural experience itself,
the Cultural Value Project takes the lead in developing a rigorous approach to what
many see as the most important aspect of art and culture.
3
As part of the Arts and Humanities Research Council’s Cultural Value Project, exploring
the individual and social value of culture, Dr. Rebecca Gordon-Nesbitt devised a piece
of research which explored the association between the arts and health. The main
question underlying this research was: Is there a relationship between engaging in the
arts and long-term health benefits, and, if so, can we find evidence of it? Inevitably,
this entailed some meta-level work – research about research that had already been
undertaken in this area. A scoping review and wide consultation in the field generated
an evidence base, comprised of fifteen previous studies, which revealed significant,
aggregated long-term health impacts that are summarised in this report.
In itself, this would qualify as a succinct and useful piece of work; after all, we all want
evidence of impact – right? But perhaps a supplementary question might be: Who
has been seeking this evidence and to what end? In considering the first part of this
question, we find that it is the Nordic countries – specifically Finland, Norway and
Sweden – which have strategically been seeking evidence of the long-term relationship
between arts engagement and health over the past 30 years. At the same time,
in the UK, Arts Council England has repeatedly told us that no such evidence exists.
This is incorrect and misleading. The fact that attention has now turned to evaluating
the individual and social value of the arts and culture prompts concerns about the
evidence-seeking obsession of technocratic societies, in which a swathe of middle
managers, informed by statistics and reports, have replaced specialists, guided by
knowledge and intuition.
In answer to the second part of the question posed above, Nordic governments have
used the evidence (however partial and inconclusive) to inform and implement
high-quality arts programmes. This report not only scrutinises existing data; it also
makes a serious contribution to the debate about how we might understand the value
of culture and the arts within a health and wellbeing context.
Foreword
Over the past ten years, the hybrid field of arts and health seems to have been growing
incrementally, as evinced by the emergence of specialist academic journals and conferences
around the world. Many in the arts and health field intuitively understand the benefits of
arts engagement, based on years of experience and mountains of anecdotal evidence.
But, while there are certainly many successful arts and health projects active on the ground,
the relevant research networks in the UK are still fledgling.
4
1 CASE: The Culture and Sport Evidence Programme, Understanding the drivers, impact and value of engagement in culture
and sport: An over-arching summary of the research, July 2010.
The main headings in this body of work may draw your attention to the distinctions
between cultural and social engagement, or they may suggest that, in areas of
dementia, mental health, obesity and occupational health, the potential exists for
immediate cost benefits and improvements to the NHS and our working lives.
But this report poses some political challenges – challenges which, in the current
climate of deficit and reductivism, might provoke new thinking in arts/health. It does
this by considering whether engagement in the arts is a direct determinant of health
or whether other factors come into play. Time and again, socio-economy comes to the
fore as a potential mediator in the relationship between arts and health, which prompts
urgent consideration of inequalities of access to the arts and health.
In recent years, the UK cultural policy landscape has been centred on investment in
infrastructure – based on the premise of ‘if you build it, they will come’. However, early
analysis of the Taking Part survey shows that it is the educated, white middle classes
who attend arts events.1 We might extrapolate from this that being affluent, educated
and white is part of a package that encompasses access to the arts and health.
Altering this picture requires systemic change, in which providing access to creative
education from primary school upwards would seem to be a necessary prerequisite.
There are lessons to be learnt from short-lived projects such as Sure Start, Healthy
Schools and Creative Partnerships – lessons which invariably point to the benefits of
a more sustained and holistic approach throughout the life-course.
Another key concern raised by this research pertains to the quality of the arts
experience. Hitherto, attention has been focused on the quantity of culture consumed –
with questions inevitably capturing how much and how often people engage with
the arts. Much more work is needed to describe the actualities of experiencing the arts.
Whilst the arts and health inhabit two distinct policy areas, and the particularities
of each field needs to be borne in mind, both health and the arts are inherently
political. It follows that arts/health is a political movement, to which this report adds
compelling weight.
Clive Parkinson,
Director of Arts for Health
5
In the first place, this research adopted an international perspective to locate and
critically analyse those English-language studies to have explored the association
between arts engagement and health. This gave rise to an evidence base, comprised
of fifteen studies, which collectively suggest that arts engagement has a beneficial
impact upon health over time. This observation, made in relation to a range of chronic
diseases, prompted a detailed consideration of the likely physiological and molecular
biological mechanisms underlying any positive results.
Attention was then turned to the likelihood of replicating and elaborating upon these
results. This necessitated a critical appraisal of the population-based datasets
underpinning international studies and a consideration of their UK comparators.
Complemented by detailed consultation with leading researchers in the field, this part of
the project yielded a series of recommendations for future analyses of the relationship
between arts engagement and health in the UK and beyond. This takes account of
the potential for further longitudinal studies, intervention into the surveys making up
the relevant datasets and an ambitious new biomedical analysis. In this effort, it is
suggested, attention will need to be paid to persistent inequalities of access to the arts,
health and economic resources. It is to be hoped that past and future research in this
area will provide the necessary evidence for policy-makers to invest in high-quality arts
activities well beyond the clinical environment.
Executive Summary
The prevailing narrative in UK policy-making circles is that we lack evidence around how
engagement in the arts – as an audience member and/or practitioner – affects our physical and
psychological health over time. Between February and July 2014, research was conducted
which sought to address this perceived deficit in several ways.
6
Introduction
In March 2014, an evidence review by Arts Council England (ACE), entitled The Value of Arts
and Culture to People and Society, included health and wellbeing as one of its four key themes.
This drew upon recent reports generated in the UK, including those by the Department
for Culture, Media and Sport (DCMS) and the Scottish Government, to claim that ‘These
studies show that arts and cultural activities can have a positive impact on the symptoms
of conditions, for example improved cognition, physical stability, or self-esteem, and the
ability of people to manage them, for example through changes in behaviour and increased
social contact’.2 While this statement jumbles together physical and psychological impacts,
it rightly suggests that a wealth of cross-sectional studies has illustrated the beneficial
short-term effect of arts engagement upon a range of symptoms.
2 Arts Council England, The Value of Arts and Culture to People and Society: An Evidence Review (London: Arts Council England,
2014), p. 26.
3 loc cit.
4 See Dr. Sam Ladkin, ‘Against Value’, as part of the Cultural Value Project.
5 Arts Council England, op cit., p. 4.
6 John D. Carnwath and Alan S. Brown, Understanding the Value and Impacts of Cultural Experience: A Literature Review (London:
Arts Council England, 2014), p. 2.
7
The research presented here departs from prevailing discourse around the arts and
health in three main ways. In the first place, it refuses to confine itself to a consideration
of symptoms, attempting instead to address the broader social and physiological
factors underlying health conditions and the ways in which arts engagement might
mitigate this relationship. In the second place, this research programme considers the
effect upon physical and mental health of engaging in high-quality arts activities in
non-clinical settings such as galleries and museums, theatres, cinemas and concert
halls. In the third place, it addresses the fact that, in the UK, scant consideration has
been given to the ways in which health may be affected by engaging with the arts over
an extended period. While the ACE evidence review references large-scale Nordic
research showing the positive impact of longitudinal cultural engagement, it ultimately
defers to the UK to conclude that ‘there is no evidence that these improvements
are sustained in the long term, and the majority of studies have been small scale
and unable to do more than report a correlation between the intervention and these
benefits’.3 In a bid to redress the balance, this project set out to evaluate the long-
term relationship between arts participation and health by adopting an international
perspective, bringing new insights with regard to both longevity and scale and allowing
us to move beyond the language of ‘intervention’.
In considering the social determinants of health, this research programme potentially
veers into instrumental territory. Advocates of the instrumental benefits of arts and
cultural engagement are rightly criticised for their econometric understandings of the
arts and culture, their upbeat, often moralistic, slant and their lack of attention to the
actual experience of cultural production and reception.4 By contrast, this research
programme is underwritten by a thorough understanding of the subjective and
potentially ambiguous nature of creative practice.
In his introduction to the aforementioned evidence review, Sir Peter Bazalgette, Chair of
ACE, insisted that, ‘When we talk about the value of arts and culture, we should always
start with the intrinsic – how arts and culture illuminate our inner lives and enrich our
emotional world. This is what we cherish. But while we do not cherish arts and culture
because of the impact on our social wellbeing and cohesion, our physical and mental
health […] they do confer these benefits and we need to show how important this is’.5
Alongside the evidence review, ACE commissioned Wolf Brown to conduct a review of
the international literature dealing with the value and impact of culture. In his foreword
to this second review, ACE Chief Executive, Alan Davey, assumed intrinsic value to
be ‘associated with benefits to the individual (like happiness or inspiration)’.6
Taken together, these statements seem to suggest that the intrinsic impact of the arts
is felt at the (individual) psychological level whereas health benefits are registered at
the instrumental (social) level. While conceding that improvements to physical health
8
wrought by arts engagement are experienced by individuals, Wolf Brown ultimately
argue that the manifest effect is cumulative and societal, thereby exempting the health
effects of arts engagement from their focus on short-term, individualistic benefits.
Accordingly, not one of the key international studies to have explored the longitudinal
relationship between health and arts engagement (presented here) is included in
the Wolf Brown review, and Davey reiterates Bazalgette virtually verbatim to bemoan
the lack of ‘longitudinal studies of the health benefits of participation in the arts, and
comparative studies of the effects of participation in the arts as opposed to say,
participation in sport’.7
The research presented here began with a scoping review of the international
evidence concerning the association between arts engagement and health. Contrary
to expectations, this process yielded fifteen key longitudinal studies which show arts
engagement to have many beneficial effects upon human beings that cannot easily
be separated into intrinsic and instrumental. While refuting the separation between
individual/psychological and social/physical benefits, this research differentiates
cultural from more general social engagement and distinguishes participation in
the arts from that of sport. The evidence base has been compiled at:
http://longitudinalhealthbenefits.wordpress.com This digital repository includes a précis
of each study (compiled below), accompanied by links to the original research articles
where it has been possible to secure copyright clearance. Visitors to this site are invited
to provide details of any omissions, and it is hoped that the international evidence base
will continue to grow as researchers draw attention to existing work in this area and new
studies are carried out.
Critical analysis of the evidence base is followed here by a discussion of the findings
and a detailed consideration of the social and physiological mechanisms thought to
underlie any positive associations observed between arts engagement and health.
This paves the way for an interrogation of the international datasets upon which
the main epidemiological studies are based. In turn, this permits consideration of
the suitability of UK-based datasets to longitudinal studies centred on the two main
variables of arts engagement and health. Based on the foregoing considerations,
recommendations are made which, it is hoped, will provide a useful starting point for
future projects exploring the fertile territory between the arts and health.
7 loc cit.
9
Methodology
The part of the research programme presented here was comprised of three
main elements:
l Scoping review of previous research into the relationship between arts and cultural
engagement and long-term health outcomes in the UK and internationally.
l Critical analysis of the data sources relevant to explorations of the relationship
between arts and cultural engagement and long-term health outcomes in the UK
and internationally.
l Recommendations about possible future research directions.
10
Review of the Evidence Base in the UK and Internationally
This part of the project sought evidence of a longitudinal relationship between
arts/cultural participation and health. The Principal Investigator (PI) initially searched
the MEDLINE (using PubMed) and EMBASE (using Ovid SP) databases, beginning with
generic terms such as ‘art’, ‘culture’, ‘health’, ‘longitudinal’. However, as these words
are ubiquitous in the literature – with ‘culture’ appearing frequently in relation to the
cultivation of cells and ‘art’ regularly occurring in phrases such as ‘state-of-the-art’ and
serving as an acronym for Atraumatic Restorative Treatment, Anti Retroviral Treatment
and Assisted Reproductive Technologies – this generated in excess of 10,000 results
when using Ovid. Sorting the results by relevance (five stars) failed to isolate three
terms together. The use of truncated versions of the search terms – such as ‘long’
(306 results when searched with ‘art’ and ‘survival’ in Medline) – and Boolean operators
– e.g. NOT HIV (216 results in Medline) – reduced the quantity of results, but came
no closer to isolating even those studies which were known at the outset. As will be
apparent from the titles of studies forming the evidence base, there is little common
language, which precluded the use of generic search terms. The review also sought
to take account of grey literature in the field, but a search of Open Grey produced
11 irrelevant studies, reinforcing the need for other methods.
In light of the above, it was decided to conduct a hand search of material, beginning
with a nucleus of widely known studies – specifically those undertaken by Bygren et al
and Hyyppä et al. From there, the review radiated outwards, taking account of research
published by these two teams and the studies to which they referred. This scoping
process was complemented by the use of web-based search engines and facilitated
by dialogues with the main researchers in the field, either in person during a week-long
trip to the Nordic region (as in the case of Bygren, Väänänen and the team at the HUNT
Research Centre) or by email (as in the case of Hyyppä, now retired, and Kouvonen).
Additionally, reports issued by the UK’s main policy-making bodies, at Westminster and
Holyrood, were scrutinised, and experts at DCMS were consulted.
11
The longitudinal focus of the review precluded cross-sectional studies. Furthermore,
it has been noted that ‘The phenomenal success of the pharmaceutical industry
and the widespread adoption of the randomised placebo-controlled trial as the gold
standard method of assessing new treatments has, in part, been responsible for the
development of the view that the patient’s state of mind and psychological well-being
are largely irrelevant to disease outcome’.8 As this review reinstated consideration of
psychological factors in examining the health effects of arts engagement in non-clinical
settings, randomised controlled trials (RCTs) were excluded. Engagement in high-
quality arts activities was understood not as an ‘intervention’, in the medical sense,
but as a voluntary part of participants’ lives, and its effect on all tangible physical and
mental health conditions – that is to say, known and quantifiable morbidities – was
included. Definitions of cultural engagement spanned the art forms.
The scoping process generated a long list of studies from which the eventual evidence
base was culled. As several of the studies (particularly those undertaken from a social
capital perspective) only mentioned arts engagement as an incidental element of
socio-cultural interaction, the ultimate criterion for inclusion in the evidence base
became the consideration of two or more discrete cultural activities.
The scoping process turned up an initial fourteen studies adopting a longitudinal
approach. Given their finite number, it seemed appropriate to consider the specificities
of each study separately, along with their strengths and weaknesses, in a format
accessible to all those with an interest in the subject. Judgment of the quality of
studies was based on the number of respondents involved, the efficacy of follow-up,
the suitability of confounders and the reporting of confidence intervals. Qualitative
assessment was made of the strengths and weaknesses of each study and whether
potential for the null hypothesis was accommodated. Permission to make the research
articles publicly available was requested from the primary authors, and copyright
clearance was obtained from the relevant journals.
The international evidence base was launched at the annual conference of the
Faculty of Public Health of the Royal College of Physicians on 3 July 2014. Since then,
an additional study has been identified by the PI and included in the evidence base.
This paved the way for a detailed analysis of the mechanisms that are speculated
upon in cases where a positive relationship between arts engagement and health
is observed.
Critical Review of UK and International Data Sources
This part of the project interrogated the datasets that have been used by the main
research teams in this area. This included a day-long visit to the HUNT Research Centre
in Levanger, Norway, to assess the quality of the biomedical and cultural data being
collated there.
8 A.D. Watkins, ‘Perceptions, emotions and immunity: an integrated homoeostatic network’, Quarterly Journal of Medicine, 88, 1995,
p. 94.
12
In the UK, two documents were useful when considering longitudinal surveys
– the Scottish Government’s Inventory of UK Longitudinal Surveys9 and a database of
Longitudinal Data Sources compiled by DCMS.10 The former offers a brief overview of
each of the major longitudinal surveys being conducted in the UK, with an emphasis on
Scotland; the latter attends to whether longitudinal surveys contain questions pertinent
to DCMS’s areas of interest, namely: culture, media and broadcasting, creative
industries and sport (for the purposes of this analysis, culture was the relevant
category, specifically the columns headed ‘arts’ and ‘museums and galleries’).
These two overviews were cross-referenced with each other and, where possible,
with the original questionnaires and/or data sources.
Face-to-face consultation with statisticians at DCMS and representatives from ACE was
complemented by email enquiries to those researchers responsible for a range
of longitudinal studies:
l British Cohort Study 1970 (BCS70)
l English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA)
l Growing up in Scotland
l Healthy Older People in Edinburgh
l Millennium Cohort Study
l Million Women Project
l MRC Unit for Lifelong Health and Ageing
l Office for National Statistics (ONS)
l Scottish Longitudinal Study
l Understanding Society
Overall, this process revealed that some of the studies highlighted as relevant by DCMS
– such as the Families and Children Survey or the Longitudinal Study of Young People
in England – contained scant data, centred on music lessons or television/internet use.
Other data sources dismissed by DCMS – such as the English Longitudinal Study of
Ageing – were found to be more useful.
Consultation and Recommendations
The inter-related elements outlined above formed the basis of a series of
recommendations for future research. This drew on consideration of the evidence
base and extant datasets, complemented by detailed and ongoing consultation with
research teams in the Nordic countries. In its entirety, it is hoped that this report will
serve as a useful grounding in the theoretical and methodological challenges at stake.
9 http://www.scotland.gov.uk/Topics/Statistics/About/longitudinal
10 http://old.culture.gov.uk/what_we_do/research_and_statistics/8250.aspx
13
Critical Analysis of the
Evidence Base
14
Lennart Welin, Bo Larsson, Kurt Svardsudd, Bodil Tibblin and Gösta Tibblin, ‘Social
Network and Activities in Relation to Mortality from Cardiovascular Diseases, Cancer
and Other Causes: A 12 Year Follow up of the Study of Men Born in 1913 and 1923’,
Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 46, 1992, pp. 127–32.
Drawing upon research demonstrating a positive association between social
environment, illness and mortality, this team was among the first to include cultural
participation in its consideration of activities. This study took as its sample population
men born in 1913 who had been randomly selected from the Gothenburg population
register in 1963. In 1973–4, all those still living locally were invited to participate in a
health examination and questionnaire, along with any other men born in 1913 who had
moved to the area in the intervening years and another random sample of men born in
1923. Serum cholesterol and blood pressure were measured, and data were gathered
about smoking, alcohol consumption and previous incidence of heart attack and stroke.
At the same time, questions were asked about self-rated health and the frequency of
leisure activities undertaken inside and outside the home. Fourteen activities outside
the home were assayed, including attendance at the cinema, theatre, concerts,
museums and exhibitions.
A preliminary analysis of the data – with participants followed up for survival to the end
of 1982 and less detail about cultural participation – was written up in The Lancet in
1985. The present study covers a re-examination undertaken in 1980, with participants
followed up for mortality to the end of 1985 and causes of death classified as cancer,
cardiovascular or other. Each of these three mortality causes was compared to the
surviving group, and a logistic regression technique was used in a multivariate analysis
involving only those variables significantly related to the specific causes of death.
When leisure-time activity patterns were considered in relation to causes of death,
socially orientated activities were found to be a significant predictor of death from
cardiovascular disease, but much weaker than blood pressure, smoking and previous
cardiovascular illness; socio-cultural activities were found to have no impact upon
cancer-related mortality, for which age and smoking habits were predictors;
in relation to other causes of death, low levels of domestic activity were seen to be
a predictor along with poor perceived health. Residual confounders, such as low
income (common among men who declined to participate in the study), were
acknowledged as significant. In summary, it was possible to say that ‘A poor social
network and low levels of activities appear to be important predictors of various
causes of mortality, but those already ill at the baseline examination (higher scores for
perceived health or previous myocardial infarction or stroke) might be more isolated
and less active due to the illness and the illness makes them more prone to death’
(p. 130). This acknowledged the possibility of a ‘disease drift’ causing diminished
participation in social and cultural activities as a result of actual and perceived ill health,
thus skewing the results through reverse causation. In the context of this systematic
review, it is significant that cultural activities were included, but noteworthy that no
distinction was made between cultural and other leisure activities occurring outside
the house.
a
15
Lars Olov Bygren, Boinkum Benson Konlaan and Sven-Erik Johansson, ‘Attendance at
Cultural Events, Reading Books or Periodicals, and Making Music or Singing in a Choir
as Determinants for Survival: Swedish Interview Survey of Living Conditions’, British
Medical Journal, 313, 21–28 December 1996, pp. 1577–80.
As part of the 1982–3 Swedish Survey of Living Conditions, 12,982 randomly selected
individuals aged 16–74 were interviewed about their (passive) attendance at, and
(active) engagement in, both individual and social cultural activities. A total of 6,301
men and 6,374 women (97.64 percent of the original sample) were followed up to
31 December 1991, and it was found that 533 men and 314 women died during this
period. Taking survival as the main outcome measure, this study used a proportional
hazards model to estimate the risk of mortality. The impact of three independent
indices was then studied. These were: an attendance index (including cinema, theatre,
concerts, art and other exhibitions/museums, sermons and sporting events, the latter
of which was analysed separately), a reading index (books/periodicals) and a music-
making index (including choral singing). Frequency of attendance was categorised as
rarely, occasionally or often, the latter of which (at least 80 visits per year) became the
reference group. Potential confounders included were: age, gender, education level,
income, long-term disease, social networks, smoking and exercise.
Age, smoking, disease and exercise influenced survival in the expected directions.
By contrast, educational level was not found to be an important confounder with respect
to mortality, whereas income level was. For men, the possession of a social network
was a slight risk factor; for women, the opposite pattern was observed. After adjusting
for all of the confounders, it seemed that people attending cultural events occasionally
were more at risk of dying than those attending either seldom or often, those attending
least found to have a 60% higher risk of death. This led the authors to speculate
that ‘Perhaps cultural participation underlies some of the notorious social class
differences in survival’ (p. 1580). Nonetheless, no causal conclusion could be drawn.
Rather, the study acknowledged that the social element of cultural participation might
be an important determinant of survival, suggesting that ‘Perhaps cultural behaviour
is so intermingled with life as a whole that it is impossible to discern its influence’
(p. 1578). In terms of follow-up, the team recommended scrutiny of large samples
with well-controlled confounders and well-differentiated cultural activities.
b
16
The team highlighted some of the flaws in the research design, including the crudeness
of the original dataset and the frequency measures derived therefrom, which were
lacking in qualitative detail. They also acknowledged possible reverse causation,
with disease determining attendance, and it took account of residual confounding,
particularly in relation to educational level. Beyond this, the three independent indices
– attendance, reading and music-making – enabled a distinction to be made between
passive and active and between individual and collective forms of engagement.
However, in combining so many forms of attendance within each index, differentiation
between art forms and between cultural and sporting or religious events was lost.
Speculating on the possible mechanisms through which cultural participation might
improve survival rates, the authors commented that increased self-reflexivity and
vicarious emotional arousal might lead to changes in the nervous and immune systems,
via innervation of the lymphatic organs, the release of growth hormones and prolactin
or the production of neurotransmitters possessing immunological resonance, thereby
improving physical health. In relation to psychological health, it was thought that
environmental enrichment might increase glucocorticoid receptors in the hippocampal
region of the brain, implying a positive impact upon depressive diseases.
After adjusting for all
of the confounders,
it seemed that people
attending cultural events
occasionally were more
at risk of dying than those
attending either seldom or
often, those attending least
found to have a 60% higher
risk of death.
17
Boinkum Benson Konlaan, Lars Olov Bygren and Sven-Erik Johansson, ‘Visiting the
Cinema, Concerts, Museums or Art Exhibitions as Determinant of Survival: A Swedish
Fourteen-Year Cohort Follow-Up’, Scandinavian Journal of Public Health, 28, 2000,
pp. 174–78.
Four years later, the Swedish team followed up their 1996 study, this time to 31
December 1996. From the original random sample of 10,609 people (5,364 men and
5,245 women) aged 25–74 years, 916 men and 600 women found to have died in the
intervening period. The purpose of this study was to introduce differentiation between
the different types of cultural activity in which respondents had participated. In this
regard, the seven independent variables were attendance at: the cinema, theatre,
concerts, art exhibition, museum, church service and sporting event. Attendance at
each type of event was stratified into rarely, sometimes and at least once a week.
Among the potential confounders, cash buffer was used instead of income, and music-
making and reading were inserted as background variables known to have a positive
impact upon health.
Regarding the control variables, the same pattern was observed as before, with
educational level exhibiting a more pronounced positive bias in relation to survival.
Significantly, social ties were found to have a negligible effect as a confounder,
irrespective of strength/quality. When checking for the effect on life expectancy of
each of the cultural variables, a positive association was observed for cinema,
concerts, art exhibition and museum visits but not for theatre, church or sporting
attendance. The discussion drew attention to the fact that the art forms in which
a positive association was observed are all nonverbal.
As the same dataset was used as in the previous study, the same weaknesses
persisted. These included the lack of complexion offered by frequency measures when
contemplating the qualitative cultural experience and the possible negative effects
of cultural engagement. The team also confessed to not knowing which residual
confounders might remain. Notwithstanding these limitations, the art form differentiation
identified as lacking in the previous study was addressed.
The possible mechanisms through which arts participation might influence health
were elaborated upon. Consideration was given the communicative theory of art
action (which prioritises the symbolic nature of nonverbal art forms and their power
in structuring feelings), the arousal theory (which supposes that art stimulates
functions necessary to our survival as a species, including food, sex and death) and
the psychoanalytic theory (which presumes that art offers vicarious satisfaction of
sublimated desires). The neuro-immunological possibilities considered in the previous
study were reiterated, and the connection between the hypothalamus, pituitary and
adrenal glands (the HPA axis) and depression was elaborated, with cultural attendance
thought to contribute to an enriched environment, increasing neural receptors in the
hippocampus and lowering depression.
c
18
Sven-Erik Johansson, Boinkum Benson Konlaan and Lars Olov Bygren, ‘Sustaining
habits of attending cultural events and maintenance of health’, Health Promotion
International, 16, no. 3, 2001, pp. 229–34.
In 1990–1, 3,793 of the initial random sample of subjects from the 1982–3 Swedish
Survey of Living Conditions were re-interviewed using the same questionnaire. In this
study, self-reported health status was taken as the main outcome measure, categorised
as good, poor or somewhere in between, the latter two of which were grouped into
the ‘poor’ category. Control variables were: age, type of residence (rented or owned),
geographical region of domicile (metropolis, municipality, small town/rural) and
socio-economic status (via educational level). Independent variables comprised
reading, music-making and attendance at: cinemas, theatres, concerts, museums and
art exhibitions. A longitudinal transitional model was analysed using logistic regression.
The researchers acknowledged that a change in health status could affect both self-
reported health and cultural attendance (in either direction). Low self-rated health was
found to correspond with poor education, older age and low urbanisation. Music-
making and reading did not have any significant effect upon self-reported health.
Taking account of all the variables, those participants whose cultural attendance was
low at both interview dates or had decreased between the two interview dates reported
lower perceived health. The converse was also true – those participants whose cultural
attendance was high at both interview dates or had increased between the two dates
reported higher perceived health. The researchers concluded that cultural stimulation
was transient, a ‘perishable commodity’ that needed replenishing if good perceived
health was to be maintained over a long period. Again, the possibility of establishing
a causal connection between cultural participation and (self-rated) health was rejected.
In framing these results, the team lingered on tension reduction and the possible
psychological mechanisms through which this might operate. As before, attention
was paid to psychoanalytic, arousal and communicative theories. Similarly, space
was dedicated to psycho-neuroimmunological theories, including the innervation of
lymphoid organs and the release of appropriate neurotransmitters, offering protection
from infections and perhaps also autoimmune diseases. Again, the HPA axis was
looked to as a possible beneficiary of environmental enrichment in the defence
against depression.
d
19
Lars Olov Bygren, Sven-Erik Johansson, Boinkum Benson Konlaan, Andrej M Grjibovski,
Anna V Wilkinson and Michael Sjöström, ‘Attending Cultural Events and Cancer
Mortality: A Swedish Cohort Study’, Arts & Health, 1, no. 1 (March 2009), pp. 64–73.
Building on the observed association between attendance at cultural events, survival
and self-rated health, this research aimed to investigate the link between cultural
attendance and cancer-related mortality.
A cohort of 9,011 cancer-free participants from the randomly selected 1990–1 Swedish
Survey of Living Conditions was followed up to 31 December 2003. During this time,
according to the Swedish National Death Register, 290 participants had died of cancer,
630 of other causes. The main outcome measure was cancer-related mortality and the
main independent variable was cultural attendance. Participants were asked about
the regularity of their previous year’s attendance at: cinemas, theatres, art galleries,
live music concerts and museums. Frequency was categorised on a four-point scale
ranging from fewer than five to more than twenty visits per year, and a cumulative
score (a cultural participation index, or CPI) was generated across the cultural activities
assayed. Demographic co-variables subjected to a proportional hazards analysis
included: age, sex, chronic conditions, disposable income, educational attainment,
smoking, exercise and urban/non-urban residency.
Low cultural attendance was found among older, less well educated participants with
a lower disposable income, who were often current or former smokers and reported
more chronic conditions than frequent attenders. Cancer mortality was also found to be
associated with the demographic and behavioural variables in the expected directions.
After adjusting for potential confounders, it was suggested that, in urban areas only,
‘rare attendees at cultural events had higher cancer-related mortality than frequent
attendees’ (p. 68).
In the process of explaining this result, it was acknowledged that the cultural activities
assayed might not have broad appeal across all segments of society, raising the
possibility of residual confounding. In the opposite direction, it was freely admitted
that those of higher socio-economic status enjoyed healthier lifestyles and possessed
greater cancer awareness and access to treatment, suggesting that ‘the relationship
between attending cultural events and health is not causal; rather, attending cultural
events serves as a proxy variable for other cancer preventative factors’ (p. 71).
The urban bias was hard to explain, and was thought to arise as a result of lower
concentrations of particular types of cultural activities in non-urban areas. The self-
reported nature of the cancer diagnosis at baseline was also highlighted as a flaw
in the study, potentially obscuring detail about the onset of cancer and raising the
possibility of reverse causation. Nonetheless, it was suggested that ‘in urban areas,
frequently attending cultural events is a robust predictor of cancer mortality, similar to
physical activity and being a current smoker, for the vast majority of the population,
regardless of current health status, socio-economic status, and behavioural risks’
(p. 69). In interpreting this result, it was suggested that engagement in meaningful
activities (including culture) might play a part in counteracting the stress and negative
immunological responses associated with cancer.
e
20
Carin Lennartsson and Merril Silverstein, ‘Does Engagement With Life Enhance Survival
of Elderly People in Sweden? The Role of Social and Leisure Activities’, Journal of
Gerontology, 56B, no. 6, 2001, pp. S335–S342.
This study examined data from the Swedish Panel Study of Living Conditions of the
Oldest Old from 1992, which had approached 537 participants who had exceeded
the upper age limit of the Swedish Survey of Living Conditions (used in the studies
by Bygren et al), to ask them about their leisure activities. This included scope for
reporting on attendance at the cinema and cultural venues including theatres, concerts
and museums (reported as a combined category), reading books or newspapers
(two separate categories) and participating in study groups across the frequency
levels of not at all, sometimes or often. In 1996, 463 non-institutionalised respondents
were followed up for survival. Mortality between 1992 and 1996 was taken as the
main outcome measure, and a Cox proportional hazard regression model was used
to estimate the relative effects of independent variables (including age, gender and
educational level) on the logged hazard rate of mortality. Additional controls were made
for factors observed to increase mortality risk, including functional impairment,
the presence of heart or circulatory problems and tobacco use.
The focus of this study – looking at the relationship between engagement in life and
successful ageing – was on the extent to which those leisure-time activities found
to influence mortality were sedentary or active, solitary or social. Within this, socio-
cultural attendance was taken to fall into the latter category on each axis, i.e. active
and social, which contradicted studies suggesting that non-participatory attendance
is more passive than active. When adjusting for age and education, socio-cultural
activities showed a negligible association with mortality in either gender. The same
pattern was observed when adjusting for age, education, functional health, circulatory/
heart problems and current smoking. The only association that was observed to have
a positive effect on mortality was engagement in solitary–active activities, such as
gardening and engaging in hobbies (including carpentry), and this association was only
observed in men.
In interpreting these results, the researchers offered a cautionary note that reverse
causation might exist between the choice of activities and health condition. An effort
was made to compensate for this by including the multi-dimensional confounder of
functional ability, which was arrived at through the physical testing of participants.
Further, the relatively small sample size and the observational nature of this study
precluded any causal claims while admitting the possibility of residual confounding.
The relevance of this study to the present analysis lies in its rejection of socially
orientated leisure-time events (including cultural and religious attendance) as
a significant factor in mortality in older people. It also considered the subjective nature
of participation, observing that ‘It is likely that activities are consequential when they are
experienced as significant ways to engage one’s free time’ (p. S340).
f
21
Hui-Xin Wang, Anita Karp, Bengt Winblad and Laura Fratiglioni, ‘Late-Life Engagement
in Social and Leisure Activities Is Associated with a Decreased Risk of Dementia:
A Longitudinal Study from the Kungsholmen Project’, American Journal of Epidemiology,
155, no. 12, 2002, pp. 1081–87.
This study sought to explore whether social and leisure activities in later life diminished
the risk of dementia. It followed two acknowledged case control and follow-up studies
to adopt a longitudinal approach. To this end, it drew upon data from the Kungsholmen
Project, a population-based study carried out in a central area of Stockholm. In 1987–9,
information was gathered – through interviews with nurses, examinations by physicians
and assessments by psychologists – from 1,810 participants, aged over 75, about their
mental, physical, social, productive and recreational activities. Of these, 1,375 people
were determined to be cognitively intact and living outside an institution. They were
followed up in 1991–3 (during which 158 dementia cases were identified among 934
participants) and 1994–6 (at which point a further 123 dementia cases were identified
among 683 participants). The study group was taken to be those subjects who
developed dementia between the first and second follow-ups. Age, gender, education,
cognitive functioning, comorbidity (indicated by hospitalisation), depressive symptoms
and physical functioning at baseline were taken into account as potential confounders.
Type and frequency of social and leisure participation was arranged according to:
mental activity (reading books/newspapers, writing, studying, undertaking crossword
puzzles, painting or drawing); physical activity (swimming, walking or gymnastics);
social activity (attending the theatre, concerts or art exhibitions, traveling, playing cards/
games or participating in social groups or a pension organisation); productive activity
(gardening, housekeeping, cooking, working for pay after retirement, doing voluntary
work or sewing, knitting, crocheting or weaving); recreational activity (watching
television or listening to the radio). Of relevance to the present analysis is the fact that
solitary/participatory engagement – including reading, painting and drawing – was
categorised as mental activity, while attendance at the theatre, concerts or exhibitions
was considered social activity, irrespective of the potential for mental stimulation.
Within the four activity groups, no separate account was taken of different activities.
Cox proportional hazard models were used to estimate the relative risks and
corresponding 95 percent confidence intervals for the social and leisure activities
associated with development of dementia, followed by multivariate analysis for each
of the potential confounders. The results ‘suggest that stimulating activity, either
mentally or socially oriented, may protect against dementia, indicating that both social
interaction and intellectual stimulation may be relevant to preserving mental functioning
in the elderly’ (p. 1081). Conversely, physical activity was not deemed to have
a discernible effect on dementia risk.
The significance of this finding to the present analysis is that both participatory creative
activity (including painting and drawing, classified as mental activity) and cultural
attendance (understood as social activity) were found to be beneficial in protecting
against dementia. Various hypotheses were offered as to why this might be the case,
with mental stimulation thought to improve cognition and social participation thought
to increase self-efficacy. The possibility of environmental enrichment giving rise to
physiological changes in the cerebral cortex was also discussed.
g
22
Kristina Sundquist, Martin Lindström, Marianne Malmström, Sven-Erik Johansson and
Jan Sundquist, ‘Social Participation and Coronary Heart Disease: A Follow-up Study
of 6900 Women and Men in Sweden, Social Science & Medicine, 58, 2004, pp. 615–22.
This research team – which overlaps with the Bygren team by virtue of its use of the
same dataset and the involvement of Johansson – sought to explore the longitudinal
relationship between social participation and coronary heart disease.
A sample of 6,861 women and men aged 35–74 who had taken part in the 1990–1
Swedish Survey of Living Conditions was followed up for hospital admissions and
deaths due to coronary heart disease to 31 December 2000. As has been seen, the
baseline survey collected data about attendance at the cinema, theatre, concerts,
art exhibitions and museums. Eighteen such variables were used to make up a social
participation index, and respondents were grouped into low, medium and high social
participation. This evinces a reversion to social measures from which Bygren et al had
been departing. A Cox regression model was used to estimate the hazard ratio
for the different variables. Socio-economic and educational status, housing tenure
and smoking were controlled for, along with age, gender, marital status and
geographical region.
As might be expected, social participation was negatively associated with advanced
age and low socio-economic and educational levels, corresponding with a higher
risk of coronary heart disease, as did smoking. After adjustment for all the variables,
an association was found between low social participation and increased incidence
of coronary heart disease morbidity and mortality. The relevance of this study to the
present analysis is that, of the eighteen variables in the social participation index
subjected to factor analysis (with a higher coefficient corresponding to greater
importance in the index), the highest scores were seen in relation to the five cultural
factors listed above. In other words, attendance at the cinema, theatre, concerts,
art exhibitions and museums had (by far, in most cases) the most significance within
the social participation index.
h
After adjustment for all the
variables, an association
was found between low
social participation and
increased incidence of
coronary heart disease
morbidity and mortality.
23
Markku T Hyyppä, Juhani Mäki, Olli Impivaara and Arpo Aromaa, ‘Leisure-Based
Participation Predicts Survival: A Population-Based Study in Finland’, Health Promotion
International, 21, no. 1, 2006, pp. 5–12.
Crossing the Baltic Sea to Finland, this study sought to explore the relationship
between leisure participation and survival. The Mini-Finland Health Survey – a two-stage
cluster sample of 8,000 people aged 30–99, carried out in 1978–80 – was designed
to assess health status and its determinants via a comprehensive health examination,
interviews and questionnaires. Demographic questions included residential stability,
socio-economic status, marital status and relations, trusting relationships, alcohol
consumption and smoking. Health data related to mental health, self-reported chronic
diseases or disabilities and self-rated overall health. Cultural and leisure attendance
took account of: (1) clubs and voluntary societies; (2) cultural and sports attendance
(including theatre, cinema, concerts, art exhibitions and sporting events); (3) religious
engagement and (4) outdoor activities. The questionnaire also recorded more
participatory activities such as (5) studying, (6) cultural interests (reading, listening
to music) and (7) so-called hobby activities (including drama, singing, photography,
painting and handicraft). The frequency of these activities was allotted a numerical
value from never (0) to once a week or more (3), and a cumulative activity score was
achieved by multiplying the number of leisure activities engaged in by their frequency
(maximum = 21).
Since the baseline survey, the mortality of survey participants was followed up to 2002,
showing 962 subjects (632 men and 330 women) to have died. Multivariable Cox
proportional hazard models were applied to survival, controlling for relevant covariates.
This showed that ‘68.5% of the subjects with scarce leisure participation and 84.6% of
the subjects with intermediate and abundant participation were alive’ (p. 7). However,
this association was found to be lacking in healthy women.
The research team acknowledged the limitations of this study, including the lack of
sampling during a long intermediary period in which Finland experienced an economic
recession. They also accepted that reverse causation was in evidence as ‘self-
reported and self-rated health predicts survival and modifies leisure engagement’
(p. 10). Equally, participants may have had undiagnosed cancers at the time of the
baseline study, which was not captured in self-rated health measures. Nonetheless,
the researchers maintained that the ‘significant protective effect of the leisure activity
endures. Although our findings suggest causality from leisure participation towards
health, this does not rule out that leisure participation in itself may be a component
of health’ (Ibid).
In this study, cultural participation is inextricably linked to social capital, using individual
measures of social participation. This implies that it is the social side of cultural
engagement which has an impact, with even reading and listening to music linked to
social action via public libraries, and it goes some way towards explaining the gender-
specific nature of these findings. A social capital approach permits a multiplicity of
diverse leisure-time activities to be bunched together in the analysis, obviating any
differentiation between art forms and between passive and active forms of cultural
engagement. Re-interpretation of the same data with attention to art form specificity
would be pertinent in the future, as would closer inspection of the biochemical
measurements taken during the baseline survey.
i
24
Markku T Hyyppä, Juhani Mäki, Olli Impivaara and Arpo Aromaa, ‘Individual-Level
Measures of Social Capital as Predictors of All-Cause and Cardiovascular Mortality:
A Population-Based Prospective Study of Men and Women in Finland’, European
Journal of Epidemiology, 22, 2007, pp. 589–97.
Drawing upon the same dataset as the previous study, this analysis emphasised the
focus upon social capital by considering migration and trusting relationships alongside
leisure participation (the latter of which was arranged over the same seven categories
and frequency scale as before). Individual-level social capital was captured through
questions about migration (from one municipality to another), residential stability, trust in
family relations, close friends and trust in them and leisure and social participation (as
above). This time, deaths during the first five years of follow-up were excluded, to allow for
undiagnosed diseases at the time of the baseline study. In the reduced cohort, all-cause
and cardiovascular mortality (including strokes) up to November 2004 was established, with
reference to the Finnish National Registry for Cause of Death as before. Four sets of Cox
proportional hazard models were constructed around individual-level social capital, with all
non-significant confounders omitted.
This study found that ‘In men, leisure social participation only just predicted all-cause
mortality, but none of the measures of individual-level social capital predicted cardiovascular
mortality. Economic status slightly modified the effect of leisure participation in men, thus
emerging as a tentative mediator between social capital and health in men’ (p. 594).
The measures taken to be indicative of social capital were less robust. While interpersonal
trust proved to be a predictor of both all-cause and cardiovascular mortality in women,
residential stability (enabling longevity of trust networks) was rejected as a measure of
social capital.
The researchers responsible for this study assessed its strengths and limitations. As with the
Swedish studies, it was based on a robust population sample and accurate mortality data.
While reverse causation during the first five years of follow-up was avoided within this revised
research design, the problem of the long intervening period persisted.
j
25
Neda Agahi and Marti G. Parker, ‘Leisure Activities and Mortality: Does Gender Matter?’,
Journal of Aging and Health, 20, 2008, pp. 855–71.
This study relied on Swedish data from both the Level of Living Survey (used by Bygren
et al and Sundquist et al) and the Panel Study of Living Conditions of the Oldest Old
(used by Lennartson and Silverstein). The former includes participants up the age of
74; the latter follows those who have reached the age of 75. A total of 1,246 men and
women who had participated in both surveys in 1990–1 and 1992 were selected and
followed up for survival until 31 December 2003, during which period 691 individuals
died. Account was taken of the frequency of hobby activities (including handicrafts and
painting), cultural activities (including attendance at the cinema, theatre, concerns,
museums and exhibitions) dancing, playing musical instruments, and choir singing.
Hazard ratios were performed using Cox regression analyses. A range of symptoms
and diseases, functional status, age, gender, educational level (as a measure of socio-
economic position), smoking, alcohol, body mass index were included as potential
confounders.
When all the activities were analysed collectively, participation in 0-1 activities tripled
mortality risk, while participating in two activities doubled the risk relative to those taking
part in six or more activities. Within this, women exhibited a dose-response relationship
between overall participation and mortality risk. When the activities were analysed
individually, together with age and education, strong associations between hobby and
cultural activities in survival were observed, with the former particularly significant
for men and the latter for women. When health indicators were taken into account,
the association between hobby activities and survival was lost for women, but the
relationship between cultural activities and survival uniquely persisted for both men
and women. A significant relationship emerged between reading books and survival
amongst women even when controlling for cognitive status and education level.
In interpreting these results, the authors accepted the possibility of reverse causation,
with health status influencing both participation and mortality. In considering residual
confounders, they understood that the socio-cultural activities analysed might serve
as proxies for health status, but concluded that this would not explain any gender
biases observed. A further confounder might be health behaviour, which was presumed
by Bygren et al to exert an influence on the relationship between socio-cultural
engagement and health. In a bid to compensate for this, account was taken of smoking,
alcohol intake and body mass index, which did not modify the results. And, while
socio-economic position was acknowledged as a likely confounder of the relationship
between engagement and mortality, it was accepted that using education level as
a solitary, dichotomised measure may prove inadequate to the task of capturing this.
k
26
Hanna-Reetta Lajunen, Anna Keski-Rahkonen, Lea Pulkkinen, Richard J Rose,
Aila Rissanen and Jaakko Kaprio, ‘Leisure Activity Patterns and Their Associations
with Overweight: A Prospective Study among Adolescents’, Journal of Adolescence,
32, no. 5, October 2009, pp. 1089–1103.
The FinnTwin12 study included all twins born in Finland between 1983 and 1987,
collating data, through self-completion questionnaire, from 5,184 twins aged 11–12
years, followed up at age 14 and 17 years. Responses concerning leisure activities
and pubertal development, weight and height and parental education at baseline were
analysed. A wide range of leisure activities was assayed, including: television and
video viewing, computer games, listening to music, playing board games and musical
instruments, reading, arts, crafts, socialising and taking part in clubs or scouts, sports
and outdoor activities. Within this, ‘Arts were defined as drawing or painting and
crafts as handicrafts, woodwork, or building scale models’ (p. 4), and frequency was
classified as 2-3 times per week, month or year. Logistic regression models were used
to study associations between becoming overweight, individual leisure activities and
leisure activity patterns. The latter were categorised as active and sociable, active but
less sociable, passive but sociable or passive and solitary. Models were adjusted for
pubertal timing, socio-economic status and parents’ educational levels.
The study found that activity patterns did not predict the tendency to become
overweight in boys, but sports and playing an instrument reduced the risk and arts
and listening to music increased it. This finding is significant to the present analysis
because it suggests that engagement in the arts in boys was detrimental to the
maintenance of recommended weights. Among girls, few individual leisure activities
predicted becoming overweight. However, the ‘passive and solitary’ cluster carried the
greatest risk of becoming overweight in late adolescence. Contrary to Lennartsson and
Silverstein’s definition of cultural participation as active and social, it is unclear which
of the above-mentioned leisure activities mapped onto the passive and solitary cluster.
This renders the deployment of leisure activity patterns useless to the present analysis.
The research team accepted the possibility of reverse causation between social
participation and being overweight. One of the (acknowledged) weaknesses in
this study was the self-reported nature of weight and the potential bias this might
introduce, though this was not thought significant when considered in relation to leisure
participation. Data about genetic predispositions to obesity were equally lacking from
this study. In seeking to explain the findings, speculations were made about lack of
sociability precipitating changes in the autonomic nervous system or the HPA axis,
leading to feelings of loneliness and depression.
l
27
Ari Väänänen, Michael Murray, Aki Koskinen, Jussi Vahtera, Anne Kouvonen and Mika
Kivimäki, ‘Engagement in Cultural Activities and Cause-Specific Mortality: Prospective
Cohort Study’, Preventive Medicine, 49, 2009, pp. 142–47.
In 1986, the Finnish Institute of Occupational Health (an independent research institute
linked with the Ministry of Social Affairs) invited the 12,173 workers at Enzo Guzeit –
a major employer in the forestry industry, which accounted for 80–90 percent of income
in Finland at that time – to participate in a survey. This was open to employees of all
grades, from cleaners to managers, although the majority of the eventual sample (of
whom 1,681 were women and 5,864 men) was comprised of blue-collar employees.
Linking to national registers, participants with cardiovascular disease, cancer-related
and alcohol-related diseases and psychological conditions (including suicidal
behaviour) were excluded from the study. The questionnaire solicited information
about engagement in socially shared arts and cultural activities, associations and
societal action, as compared with individual engagement via reading and studying.
Frequency was self-rated from low (a few times a year) to high (daily or near daily)
with an intermediate category of once a week or twice a month. The original cohort
was re-surveyed in 1996 and 2000 and followed up for survival until 2004 (using the
National Death Registry data kept by Statistics Finland). Associations between cultural
engagement and various types of mortality were assessed using Cox proportional
hazard models. Account was taken of socio-demographic factors (including age,
marital status, educational level, social contact, smoking, alcohol consumption,
exercise), stress, diabetes and hypertension.
After adjusting for socio-demographic, biological and social factors and stress,
the risk of all-cause mortality and deaths from cardiovascular and external causes
(such as suicides, accidents and violence-related deaths) was found to be reduced for
those regularly engaging with culture. After taking account of behavioural risk factors,
this association remained for external-cause mortality (including the primary external
causes of accidents and suicides) but it was significantly diminished for cardiovascular
mortality. From this, the researchers concluded that there was a ‘robust link between
cultural activities and the reduction in deaths from external causes’. Possible reasons
given for this were previously reported links between cultural engagement, health status
and morale, combined with the fact that engagement with non-risky cultural activity
might insulate people from life-threatening situations while providing routes to better
psychological health and diminishing the risk of suicide. Within this, solitary cultural
activities seemed to be related to all-cause and cardiovascular mortality while socially
shared cultural activity generally pertained to death from external causes, but this
association was lost when adjusting for socio-economic status and behavioural risk
factors. This led to the speculation that readers might be better informed about health
risks, while mental health might be improved through socio-cultural engagement.
Arts and cultural activities were included as a catch-all category in the survey, alongside
a range of other activities from gardening to housework. In future studies, it would be
useful to have more differentiation between types of arts and cultural activities. As the
distinction between collective and solitary participation was foregrounded in this study,
it would also be beneficial to acknowledge the difference between (social) attendance
at arts events and (individual) participation in the making of artwork. This would add
complexion to the persistent debate around (passive) attendance and (active) creation.
m
28
Anne Kouvonen, Judy Anne Swift, Mia Stafford, Tom Cox, Jussi Vahtera, Ari Väänänen,
Tarja Heponiemi, Roberto De Vogli, Amanda Griffiths and Mika Kivimäki, ‘Social
Participation and Maintaining Recommended Waist Circumference: Prospective
Evidence From the English Longitudinal Study of Aging’, Journal of Aging and Health,
24, no. 2, 2012, pp. 250–68.
With four of the same personnel as the Väänänen research team, this study used data
drawn from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Since 2002, this panel survey
has assayed the health and social habits of a representative sample of Englishmen
and women aged 50 and over. In 2004–5 and 2008–9 (waves 2 and 4), a nurse
visited respondents and took various measurements including waist circumference.
The research team sought to determine whether this dependent variable (as an
indicator of obesity) was affected by respondents’ social behaviours. The sample was
comprised of 4,280 participants (2,373 women and 1,907 men) with complete data in
waves 2 and 4. Logistic regression was used to determine whether social participation
at baseline predicted waist circumference at follow-up (in two groups, according to
whether they met or exceeded recommended baseline waist measurements).
Gender-stratified models adjusted for age, ethnicity, marital status, total wealth,
longstanding limiting illness, depressive symptoms, smoking status and physical
activity. Interestingly, the discussion highlighted the ethnic homogeneity of the sample.
No association was found between social participation and waistline measurement in
women. By contrast, those men with an initial waist measurement in the recommended
range who participated in education, arts or music groups or evening classes and
in charitable associations were more likely to maintain their waist circumference,
while social participation showed no association with meeting recommended waist
measurements for those with a waistline that exceeded the recommended range at
baseline. As before, the possibility was acknowledged that obesity might determine
participation, that social participation may be comprised of, or encourage, physical
activity or that it may be a proxy for a healthier lifestyle and better access to information
and resources.
The significance of this study to the present analysis is that it unites the main Finnish
and Swedish work in an English context. Referencing Bygren et al and Lennartson and
Silverstein, this work acknowledges the possible impact of arts and cultural attendance
upon the HPA axis in particular and upon health, longevity and psychological wellbeing
in general. However, despite the claim that ‘the study differentiates between different
forms of participation’ (p. 262), no analysis was made of the specificities of arts
engagement.
n
29
Koenraad Cuypers, Karin De Ridder, Kirsti Kvaløy, Marguun Skjei Knudtsen, Steinar
Krokstad, Jostein Holmen and Turid Lingaas Holmen, ‘Leisure Time Activities in
Adolescence in the Presence of Susceptibility Genes for Obesity: Risk or Resilience
against Overweight in Adulthood? The HUNT Study’, BMC Public Health, 12,
no. 820, 2012.
Adding complexion to the studies of Lajunen et al and Kouvonen et al, this research –
conducted in Norway – distinguished between social and cultural participation when
considering adolescent obesity. It relied on data collected as part of the HUNT Study.
In 1995–7, 8,408 adolescents (13–19 years) completed a comprehensive questionnaire
about their lifestyle, health and quality of life and underwent a clinical examination
during which anthropometric measurements, including waist circumference, were taken.
In 2006–8, 1,450 of these participants were followed up as young adults (24–30 years);
they had measurements and blood taken and were genotyped. Cultural activities were
taken to imply a mental function that could be performed alone, such as reading
a book, listening to or playing music, doing homework or watching television (with
the latter understood to be biased by snacking and psychosocial problems), whereas
social activities were understood to involve friends. Each activity was awarded a score
from one to four, based on frequency (from never to four times a week), and cumulative
scores were calculated as a function of the number and frequency of cultural and
social activities, dichotomised into highly culturally active and not. In relation to
the dependent variable of obesity, account was taken of body mass index, waist
circumference, waist-hip ratio and natural development of the body over the life course.
Possible confounders – such as physical activity, socio-economic status, pubertal
timing and genetic proclivity to obesity – were also included. Linear regression models
were used to explore the association between genetic predisposition, body mass and
waist circumference, and the interaction between these scores and cultural/social
participation was assessed at follow-up.
More girls than boys were found to be engaged in cultural activities, while social
activities were equally distributed across the genders. Beyond this, participation in
cultural activities was found to have a negative association with obesity in girls in
adulthood, whereas participation in social activities was found to have a positive
association with obesity in both girls and boys. In other words, participation in social
activities increased the tendency towards obesity in girls, whereas participation in
cultural activities guarded them against being overweight. These results were amplified
when considering those participants who were at the recommended weight when
the survey began and when television was excluded as an activity. Interestingly, no
interaction was found between social and cultural activities, suggesting that they
are independent concepts in relation to fat retention. As in the Kouvonen study, this
research suggested that, rather than having a corrective effect, ‘highly culturally active
adolescents seemed to be better protected against the effect of obesity-susceptibility
genes when measured in young adulthood’ (p. 6).
Among the possible explanations given for the relationship between cultural
participation and obesity were healthy lifestyle, stress reduction and the impact
of enriched environments upon the production and metabolism of fats.
o
30
The first study in the evidence base – exploring the impact of social engagement
upon deaths through cancer, cardiovascular disease and other causes – took account
of attendance at the cinema, theatre, concerts, museums and exhibitions.11 While
any association between socio-cultural engagement and longevity was cautiously
reported, especially when compared to more directly causal relationships – such as that
observed between cardiovascular disease and blood pressure, smoking and underlying
health conditions – this paved the way for more rigorous scrutiny of cultural – as distinct
from social – impacts.
In isolating cultural from social engagement, the team comprised of Lars Olov
Bygren, Sven-Erik Johansson and Boinkum Benson Konlaan has led the field. In a
study published in the British Medical Journal, which has formed the basis of many
subsequent research programmes, this team conceded that the social element of
cultural participation might be an important determinant of survival, suggesting that
‘Perhaps cultural behaviour is so intermingled with life as a whole that it is impossible
to discern its influence’.12 Four years later, the same team found social ties to have a
negligible effect as a confounder, irrespective of their strength or quality.13 At the same
time, low (compared to regular) attendance at cultural events was shown to significantly
increase the likelihood of death. They also began to address the lack of differentiation
between art forms – which had typified their own earlier studies and those of other
Discussion
As the above summary shows, longitudinal research into the relationship between arts
engagement and long-term health outcomes has largely been centred on the Nordic
countries. Given the ready availability of data pertaining to date and cause of death in the
Nordic region it is, perhaps, inevitable that research teams initially focused upon mortality/
survival as their main dependent variable.
11 Lennart Welin, Bo Larsson, Kurt Svardsudd, Bodil Tibblin and Gösta Tibblin, ‘Social Network and Activities in Relation to Mortality
from Cardiovascular Diseases, Cancer and Other Causes: A 12 Year Follow up of the Study of Men Born in 1913 and 1923’, Journal
of Epidemiology and Community Health, 46, 1992, pp. 127–32.
12 Lars Olov Bygren, Boinkum Benson Konlaan and Sven-Erik Johansson, ‘Attendance at Cultural Events, Reading Books or
Periodicals, and Making Music or Singing in a Choir as Determinants for Survival: Swedish Interview Survey of Living Conditions’,
British Medical Journal, 313, 21–28 December 1996, p. 1578.
13 Boinkum Benson Konlaan, Lars Olov Bygren and Sven-Erik Johansson, ‘Visiting the Cinema, Concerts, Museums or Art Exhibitions
as Determinant of Survival: A Swedish Fourteen-Year Cohort Follow-Up’, Scandinavian Journal of Public Health, 28, 2000a, pp.
174–78.
31
research teams – to demonstrate a positive association between attendance at
the cinema, concerts and exhibitions and survival. Transferring this approach to
a US context, Bygren worked on a cross-sectional study to show a positive association
between self-rated health and attendance at exhibitions, dance performances, films,
popular music concerts and theatre plays, as well as establishing a directly proportional
relationship between self-rated health and the number of cultural activities attended.14
Bygren, Johansson and Konlaan have also consistently offered convincing speculations
about the possible mechanisms through which these associations might operate,
which will be discussed in greater depth in the next section. For now, it is interesting
to note that they posit cultural stimulation as a ‘perishable commodity’,15 continually
in need of replenishment, which has important consequences for our conception of
arts engagement. These combined findings suggest that it is the cultural, rather than
social, nature of arts engagement which has a part to play in alleviating life-threatening
conditions, and that regular and sustained attendance at stimulating arts events gives
way to longer lives better lived.
In differentiating between the most prevalent causes of mortality, Welin et al were
unable to find a link between socio-cultural activities and cancer-related mortality,
while Lennartson and Silverstein found no negligible effect upon mortality from socio-
cultural engagement.16 In 2007, Hyyppä et al found that leisure social participation
mildly predicted all-cause mortality but had no discernible effect upon cardiovascular
mortality.17 Building on this study two years later, Väänänen et al found an association
between external causes of mortality (such as accident and suicide) and (particularly
socially orientated) cultural participation.18 All of these studies deployed a social capital
approach, the implications of which will be discussed in the next section. In the same
year as the Väänänen et al study was published, Bygren et al orchestrated a project
focusing on cultural, rather than social, activities, and found arts engagement to have
a potentially preventative effect.19 In this regard, rare cultural attendees were found
to be suffering from higher rates of cancer-related mortality than their high-attending
counterparts in urban areas. This study claimed that cultural attendance evinced
a similar effect to physical activity and smoking as a predictor of cancer-related
mortality, irrespective of health and socio-economic status. This is significant because,
as we have seen, both the Chair and Chief Executive of Arts Council England have
decried the lack of studies distinguishing cultural from physical activity.
14 Anna V. Wilkinson, Andrew J. Waters, Lars Olov Bygren and Alvin R. Tarlov, ‘Are Variations in Rates of Attending Cultural Activities
Associated with Population Health in the United States?’, BMC Public Health, 31 August 2007.
15 Sven-Erik Johansson, Boinkum Benson Konlaan and Lars Olov Bygren, ‘Sustaining habits of attending cultural events and
maintenance of health’, Health Promotion International, 16, no. 3, 2001, p. 233.
16 Carin Lennartsson and Merril Silverstein, ‘Does Engagement With Life Enhance Survival of Elderly People in Sweden? The Role of
Social and Leisure Activities’, Journal of Gerontology, 56B, no. 6, 2001, pp. S335–S342.
17 Markku T. Hyyppä, Juhani Mäki, Olli Impivaara and Arpo Aromaa, ‘Individual-Level Measures of Social Capital as Predictors of
All-Cause and Cardiovascular Mortality: A Population-Based Prospective Study of Men and Women in Finland’, European Journal of
Epidemiology, 22, 2007, pp. 589–97.
18 Ari Väänänen, Michael Murray, Aki Koskinen, Jussi Vahtera, Anne Kouvonen and Mika Kivimäki, ‘Engagement in Cultural Activities
and Cause-Specific Mortality: Prospective Cohort Study’, Preventive Medicine, 49, 2009, pp. 142–47.
19 Lars Olov Bygren, Sven-Erik Johansson, Boinkum Benson Konlaan, Andrej M Grjibovski, Anna V Wilkinson and Michael Sjöström,
‘Attending Cultural Events and Cancer Mortality: A Swedish Cohort Study’, Arts & Health, 1, no. 1, March 2009, pp. 65–6.
32
Another area in which the long-term relationship between cultural attendance and
health has been studied is that of dementia. In this endeavour, Wang et al categorised
attendance at cultural events as social and engagement in creative activity (such as
drawing or painting) as mental, to find that both types of activity exhibited a positive
association with dementia prevention, as compared to physical activity.20
This observation is echoed in many cross-sectional dementia studies, and informs
the Dementia and Imagination programme.
With obesity looming as a major public health issue, a further area of research has
centred on the relationship between socio-cultural engagement and weight gain.
Focusing on the social aspects of participation, Lajunen et al alluded to arts
engagement increasing the likelihood of adolescent boys becoming overweight.21
Yet, while Kouvonen et al found no association between social participation and
waistline measurement in adult women, they observed a greater likelihood of
maintaining weight within the recommended range for adult men.22 Also in 2012,
Cuypers et al found that, as compared to social participation, teenage girls engaging in
cultural activities were less likely to be obese.23 This conflicting body of work seems to
reaffirm the distinction between social and cultural participation in favour of the latter.
Research carried out around successful ageing and obesity elaborates on the gendered
nature of health effects. One paper in the evidence base takes this as its main focus,
drawing on the ageing Swedish population. It is here that we encounter the most direct
claims of causality, with a dose-response relationship being reported between social
participation and survival in women. Differentiating between a range of social activities,
it was found that ‘participation in cultural activities was the only activity that was
significantly related to survival in both men and women’.24
More generally, the studies considered here are united by the tentative nature of
their claims around association (or occasionally correlation) rather than causation.
In their seminal 1996 paper, Bygren, Konlaan and Johansson suggested that cultural
participation might underlie the different survival rates observed across social classes.
Low income (which had been identified as a residual confounder by Welin et al in
1992) was taken into account and found to be significant with respect to mortality.
When subsequently considering the association between cancer-related mortality and
cultural attendance, Bygren et al asserted that ‘the relationship between attending
cultural activities and health is not causal; rather, attending cultural events serves
20 Hui-Xin Wang, Anita Karp, Bengt Winblad and Laura Fratiglioni, ‘Late-Life Engagement in Social and Leisure Activities Is
Associated with a Decreased Risk of Dementia: A Longitudinal Study from the Kungsholmen Project’, American Journal of
Epidemiology, 155, no. 12, 2002, pp. 1081–87.
21 Hanna-Reetta Lajunen, Anna Keski-Rahkonen, Lea Pulkkinen, Richard J Rose, Aila Rissanen and Jaakko Kaprio, ‘Leisure Activity
Patterns and Their Associations with Overweight: A Prospective Study among Adolescents’, Journal of Adolescence, 32, no. 5,
October 2009, pp. 1089–1103.
22 Anne Kouvonen, Judy Anne Swift, Mia Stafford, Tom Cox, Jussi Vahtera, Ari Väänänen, Tarja Heponiemi, Roberto De Vogli,
Amanda Griffiths and Mika Kivimäki, ‘Social Participation and Maintaining Recommended Waist Circumference: Prospective
Evidence From the English Longitudinal Study of Aging’, Journal of Aging and Health, 24, no. 2, 2012, pp. 250–68.
23 Koenraad Cuypers, Karin De Ridder, Kirsti Kvaløy, Marguun Skjei Knudtsen, Steinar Krokstad, Jostein Holmen and Turid Lingaas
Holmen, ‘Leisure Time Activities in Adolescence in the Presence of Susceptibility Genes for Obesity: Risk or Resilience against
Overweight in Adulthood?’, The HUNT Study’, BMC Public Health, 12, no. 820, 2012a.
24 Neda Agahi and Marti G. Parker, ‘Leisure Activities and Mortality: Does Gender Matter?’, Journal of Aging and Health, 20, 2008,
p. 865.
33
as a proxy variable for other cancer preventive factors’.25 Given that the association
between cancer-related mortality and cultural attendance could only be determined
in urban locations, the researchers concluded that arts participation might be part of
a healthy and active lifestyle. Elsewhere, it is argued that cultural participation might
be a component26 or marker27 of health, and attempts have been made to test this
hypothesis. Agahi and Parker concluded that, if arts engagement were simply
a proxy for health, this would not explain the observed gender biases. They factored
in smoking, alcohol intake and body mass index as indicators of a healthy lifestyle,
but this failed to modify the results. Similarly, Lennartson and Silverstein found that
their chosen methodology reduced ‘the chance that the effect of activity involvement
is simply a proxy for physical health’.28 Wilkinson et al hinted at the existence of
a possible missing link (such as motivation) between cultural participation and health,
which raises questions about who is motivated to access the arts. The potential factors
mediating between arts engagement and health are discussed in the next section, while
the relationship of health to economy and class remains an open question.
In a similarly cautious way, cultural engagement is presumed to have a preventative,
rather than remedial, effect. So, for example, Kouvonen et al and Cuypers et al assume
participation to have an impact upon the maintenance of recommended weights.
When the international evidence base was launched at the annual conference of the
Faculty of Public Health, it generally met with a positive response. However, caution
was advised regarding the use of statistics to prove a point at which we have already
arrived, and the merits of the null hypothesis were extolled. Indeed, a number of the
studies in the evidence base seem keen to fit their findings to a predetermined thesis,
whereas others remain more open to explanations.
Interestingly, the potentially detrimental effects of arts participation have been
acknowledged from the outset, with Bygren et al speculating that ‘Negative effects
of cultural activities could be that people lose their sense of reality and identify
with asocial models of behaviour and are themselves encouraged towards asocial
behaviour’.29 This is echoed by Hyyppä, with the sentiment that ‘It is highly probable
that not all cultural activities are beneficial for health and survival; some can even be
detrimental to health’.30 In a variation on this theme, Welin et al acknowledged that
‘We did not measure the quality of home, outside home, and social activities. It may
well be that this quality of for example social participation (social activities) is more
important than its quantity as a factor of premature mortality’.31
25 Bygren et al, 2009, op cit., p. 71.
26 Markku T Hyyppä, Juhani Mäki, Olli Impivaara and Arpo Aromaa, ‘Leisure-Based Participation Predicts Survival: A Population-
Based Study in Finland’, Health Promotion International, 21, no. 1, 2006, pp. 5–12.
27 Koenraad Cuypers, Steinar Krokstad, Turid Lingaas Holmen, Margunn Skjei Knudtsen, Lars Olov Bygren and Jostein Holmen,
‘Patterns of receptive and creative cultural activities and their association with perceived health, anxiety, depression and satisfaction
with life among adults: the HUNT study, Norway’, Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 66, 2012b, pp. 698–703.
28 Lennartson and Silverstein, op cit., p. 341.
29 Bygren et al, 1996, op cit., p. 1578.
30 Markku T. Hyyppä, Healthy Ties, Social Capital, Population Health and Survival (Dordrecht: Springer, 2010), p. 51.
31 Welin et al, op cit., p. 131.
34
One of the persistent ambiguities within this body of research concerns the way in
which arts engagement is defined, and the extent to which it is considered active or
passive, individual or social. So, for example, a summary of (longitudinal and cross-
sectional) research in Sweden and Norway distinguishes between the ‘recreational/
receptive (e.g. visiting museums, concerts, spectator in cultural events and so on)
and creative (club meetings, singing, painting, and various physically challenging
cultural activities)’.32 While Bygren et al spearheaded a focus upon attendance, later
studies have reintroduced considerations of participatory creative activity, with Wang
et al pointing to the beneficial mental stimuli afforded by practising the arts. Similarly,
Lennartson and Silverstein’s subdivision of social engagement into passive–proactive,
sedentary–active, solitary–social and solitary–active showed positive results for the
latter, which included (creative) hobbies.
Perhaps surprisingly, a cross-sectional study of self-rated health for DCMS, drawing
on a UK-based dataset (wave 2 of Understanding Society – to be considered in a later
section), finds that:
Interestingly, attendance at arts events has an effect on health but
participation in arts does not. Attending the arts is associated with
a 5% increase in the likelihood of reporting good health. Within the
audience variables film (cinema), exhibitions and plays and dramas all
had significant positive impacts and music audience was positive and
significant at the 10% level. The participation variables were a mix of
positive and negative effects, which were all insignificant except art
participation and this explains why the overall participation variable was
found to be insignificant. Participation in art was actually found to have
a negative impact on health, although this may be explained to some
extent by reverse causality; that is, unhealthy people may be more likely
to engage in arts.33
The likelihood of ‘reverse causality’ – that is, the possibility that health has an impact
on arts participation, rather than the other way around – will be returned to, and it
will be seen that ill health is generally thought likely to diminish, rather than increase,
engagement. In exploring the active–passive distinction, a study conducted at the
HUNT Research Centre in Norway attempted to distinguish between receptive and
creative cultural activities in relation to self-rated health, depression and life satisfaction.
This showed both receptive and creative cultural activity to have a positive association
with all the health indices for both genders, which was only slightly stronger for
receptive cultural activities and particularly pronounced for men.34 This reinforced
the gender differentiation noted elsewhere, and found participation to be strongly
associated with socio-economy. It is clear that future research needs to bear in mind
the distinction between attendance at cultural events and participation in creative
activity while continuing to address the broadest range of factors (confounders) that
might otherwise influence health.
32 Koenraad Frans Cuypers, Margunn Skjei Knudtsen, Maria Sandgrenc, Steinar Krokstad, Britt Maj Wikström and Töres Theorell,
‘Cultural activities and public health: research in Norway and Sweden: An overview’, Arts & Health, Vol. 3, No. 1, March 2011, p. 7.
33 Daniel Fujiwara, Laura Kudrna and Paul Dolan, Quantifying the Social Impacts of Culture and Sport (London: Department of
Culture, Media and Sport, 2014a), p. 17, emphasis in original.
34 Cuypers et al., 2012b, op cit.
35 35 Hanna-Liisa Liikanen, Art and Culture for Well-being (Helsinki: Publications of the Ministry of Education and Culture, Finland 2010).
So, where does this leave us when considering the relationship between arts participation and health?
The initial results are encouraging. Evangelists working at the intersection between the arts and health
can go forward, armed with slightly more evidence than theologians – evidence that has been considered
sufficient for Nordic governments to implement arts and health programmes at a local, regional and
national level.35 There is still much work to be done, certainly in the UK, where several longitudinal,
population-based surveys are ripe for analysis. This will be considered more fully below. For now, let us
turn to a consideration of the possible mechanisms through which research teams envisage that arts
engagement might evince a positive association with health.
The initial results are
encouraging. Evangelists
working at the intersection
between the arts and
health can go forward,
armed with slightly more
evidence than theologians
– evidence that has been
considered sufficient for
Nordic governments to
implement arts and health
programmes at a local,
regional and national level.
36
Social Capital
In the 1970s, a raft of research investigating the relationship between health and
socio-cultural factors was published in the American Journal of Epidemiology and
elsewhere. In 1976, Cassel drew attention to a ‘category of environmental factors
capable of producing profound effects on host susceptibility to environmental disease
agents’, which included ‘the presence of other members of the same species,
or more generally, certain aspects of the social environment’.36 This paved the way
for consideration of psychosocial factors as determinants of health. At the end of
the decade, Berkman and Syme asserted that social isolation increased the risk of
all-cause mortality, irrespective of socio-economic status, self-rated health, smoking,
drinking and obesity.37 Beyond this, it was argued that social support guarded
against a range of chronic diseases38 and self-reported symptoms, both physical and
psychological.39 The mechanisms that were looked to in mediating these associations
included the diminution of life stress and the activation of nervous, hormonal and
immunological systems, all of which will be discussed in greater depth shortly.
Analysis of Possible
Mechanisms Informing
the Relationship
between Arts
Participation
and Health
36 John Cassel, ‘The Contribution of the Social Environment to Host Resistance’, American Journal of Epidemiology, Vol. 104, No. 2,
1976, p. 108.
37 Lisa F. Berkman and S. Leonard Syme, ‘Social Networks, Host Resistance and Mortality: A Nine-Year Follow-Up of Alameda
County Residents’, American Journal of Epidemiology, Vol. 109, No. 2, 1979, pp. 186–204.
38 James S. House, Cynthia Robbins and Helen L. Metzner, ‘The Association of Social Relationships and Activities with Mortality:
Prospective Evidence from the Tecumseh Community Health Study’, American Journal of Epidemiology, Vol. 116, No. 1, 1982,
pp. 123–40; Victor J. Schoenbach, Lisa Fredman and David G. Kleinbaum, ‘Social Ties and Mortality in Evans County, Georgia’,
American Journal of Epidemiology, Vol. 123, No. 4, 1986, pp. 577–91; George A. Kaplan, Jukka T. Salonen, Richard D. Cohen,
Richard J. Brand, S. Leonard Syme and Pekka Puska, ‘Social Connections and Mortality from all Causes and from Cardiovascular
Disease: Prospective Evidence from Eastern Finland’, American Journal of Epidemiology, Vol. 128, No. 2, 1988, pp. 370–80.
39 Lisa F. Berkman ‘The Role of Social Relations in Health Promotion’, Psychosomatic Medicine, 57, 1995, pp. 245–54; Teresa E.
Seeman, ‘Social Ties and Health: The Benefits of Social Integration’, Annals of Epidemiology, Vol. 6, No. 5, September 1996, pp.
442–51.
37
40 Robert Putnam, Bowling Alone: The Collapse and Revival of American Community (London: Simon & Schuster, 2001).
41 Frances E. Baum and A.M. Ziersch, ‘Social capital’, Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 57, 2003, pp. 320–3.
42 Simon Szreter and Michael Woolcock, ‘Health by association? Social capital, social theory, and the political economy of public
health’, International Journal of Epidemiology, 33, 2004, pp. 650–67.
43 Á. Skrabski, M. Kopp and I. Kawachi, ‘Social capital in a changing society: cross sectional associations with middle aged female
and male mortality rates’, Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 57, 2003, pp. 114–9; Nan Lin, ‘Inequality in Social Capital’,
Contemporary Sociology, Vol. 29, No. 6, Nov 2000, pp. 785–95.
44 Tarja Nieminen, Ritva Prättälä, Tuija Martelin, Tommi Härkänen, Markku T Hyyppä, Erkki Alanen and Seppo Koskinen, ‘Social
capital, health behaviours and health: a population-based associational study’, BMC Public Health, 13, 613, 2013, 405.
45 Hyyppä, 2010, op cit.
46 Frances E. Baum, Robert A. Bush, Carolyn C. Modra, Charlie J. Murray, Eva M. Cox, Kathy M. Alexander and Robert C. Potter,
‘Epidemiology of participation: an Australian community study’, Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 54, 2000, p. 414.
By the beginning of the new millennium, social capital was increasingly
being invoked to explain the perceived diminution of social cohesion.40
Contemporary commentators acknowledge the multi-faceted and mutable
nature of social capital,41 and attempt to reconcile the social, political and
ideological connotations of this problematic construct.42 With the
interaction between social networks and mortality most pronounced in
middle class white men,43 attention has been paid to the unequal
distribution of social capital across class, ethnic and gender lines.
Given its complex nature, a reliable unitary measure for social capital has proven
elusive, necessitating a composite approach, made up of ‘social support, social
participation and networks, and trust and reciprocity’.44 Markku T Hyyppä –
a Finnish neuroscientist with two studies in the evidence base – distinguishes
between the structural and cognitive dimensions of social capital, with the former
regarded as an objective, measurable entity (made up of connections, networks and
modes of participation) and the latter taken to be subjective and slippery (centred
on interpersonal and personal trust and reciprocity), often resulting in it being
downplayed in measurement frameworks.45 There is much scope for unforeseen
confounders; so, for example, it has been found that ‘Levels of participation in social
and civic community life in an urban setting are significantly influenced by individual
socioeconomic status, health and other demographic characteristics’.46
Unsurprisingly, research into the relationship between social capital and health
has consistently produced ambiguous results.
The first of the studies in the evidence base to include a longitudinal consideration of
arts engagement came out of research into social capital. In the context of the present
analysis, it is significant that, although cultural activities were included as a factor
of social participation, no distinction was made between cultural and other leisure
activities occurring outside the house. More generally, this body of work suggests that
the artistic specificities of engagement are subordinate to the social milieu in which
engagement occurs, on the basis that:
38
Cultural attendance and events are socially-related, and cultural
experiences are gained in interaction with other people. Consequently,
if attending cultural events, making art, visiting museums, and the
multitude of other forms of cultural participation have causal and positive
influences on population health, such influences may depend on the
social nature of cultural capital, which has either been totally missed or not
been measured or controlled for in the handful of epidemiological surveys
in existence up to date.47
Such an approach permits a multiplicity of diverse leisure-time activities to be bunched
together in the analysis, obviating differentiation between art forms.
Hyyppä and Mäki have consistently focused upon the social dimension of cultural
participation in an attempt to explicate the longer life expectancies observed in
the Swedish-speaking minority population in Finland.48 Their second study in the
evidence base finds that, ‘In men, leisure social participation only just predicted all-
cause mortality, but none of the measures of individual-level social capital predicted
cardiovascular mortality. Economic status slightly modified the effect of leisure
participation in men, thus emerging as a tentative mediator between social capital
and health in men’.49 Despite this finding, there is a tendency to underplay socio-
economic inequalities within social capital research. By contrast, it has been observed
that ‘cultural engagement levels are highest in the highest household income groups
in Scotland and decline to be lowest in the lowest household income groups. Similarly,
adult participation in cultural and sporting activities varies by area deprivation, with
participation increasing as area deprivation decreases’.50 This has implications for
public health in general and for health (and cultural) inequalities in particular.
We have seen that Bygren et al have dismissed the relationship between social ties
and longevity. Departing from this team to reconsider cultural engagement as a factor
in social participation, Sundquist et al drew up a participation index with eighteen
variables.51 It is noteworthy that those variables with the greatest significance were
found to be the five forms of cultural attendance included in the index: cinema, theatre,
concerts, art exhibitions and museums.
47 Hyyppä, 2010, op cit., p. 53.
48 Markku T. Hyyppä and Juhani Mäki, ‘Why do Swedish-speaking Finns have longer active life? An area for social capital research’,
Health Promotion International, Vol. 16, No. 1, 2001a, pp. 55–64; Markku T. Hyyppä and Juhani Mäki, ‘Individual-Level Relationships
between Social Capital and Self-Rated Health in a Bilingual Community’, Preventive Medicine, 32, 2001b, pp. 148–55; Markku T.
Hyyppä and Juhani Mäki, ‘Social participation and health in a community rich in stock of social capital’, Health Education Research,
Vol. 18, No. 6, 2003, pp. 770–9.
49 Hyyppä et al, 2007, op cit., p. 594.
50 Clare Leadbetter and Niamh O’Connor, Healthy Attendance? The Impact of Cultural Engagement and Sports Participation on Health
and Satisfaction with Life in Scotland (Edinburgh: Scottish Government, 2013), p. 7.
51 Kristina Sundquist, Martin Lindström, Marianne Malmström, Sven-Erik Johansson and Jan Sundquist, ‘Social Participation and
Coronary Heart Disease: A Follow-up Study of 6900 Women and Men in Sweden’, Social Science & Medicine, 58, 2004, pp. 615–22.
39
Contemplating Bygren et al’s work, Hyyppä insisted that ‘it may be too early to argue
that attendance at cultural events per se has beneficial effects on survival, without
taking into account (individual-level) social capital that is latently included in all cultural
participation’.52 However, he conceded that ‘the Swedish surveys were well-controlled,
and what is important, they were adjusted for long-lasting diseases and other health-
related factors that are known to strongly influence on [sic] one’s health and survival.
Adjusting for several conventional health-related factors, the authors were able to show
a strong link between cultural activity and survival’.53 A report by the Swedish National
Institute of Public Health noted that:
One difficulty highlighted by several researchers concerning research
in this area is to determine whether it is the artistic experience in itself
or the social context within which it takes place that has a positive impact
on health. Experiments comparing cultural participation to participation
in physical activity under the same social circumstances indicate, however,
that social stimulus alone cannot explain the health effects of participating
in cultural activities.54
Taking account of the studies comprising the evidence base, it seems likely that ‘there
is a general, rather than cause-specific, effect of social support on health status’.55
If, as Bourdieu suggested, both social and cultural capital may be regarded as proxies
for access to resources within class society,56 there is likely to be a proportional
relationship between access to the arts and to health information and services. In this
sense, considerations of social capital may help us to understand who has access to
these vital goods and services, but it tells us little about how arts participation might
affect health. Instead, Bygren et al consistently emphasise cognitive and physiological
explanations which are useful to unravel here, delving into cross-sectional studies and
RCTs where necessary.
52 Hyyppä, 2010, op cit., pp. 50–1.
53 Ibid, p. 51.
54 Swedish National Institute of Public Health, Kultur för hälsa [Culture for Health], (Stockholm: Swedish National Institute of Public
Health, 2005), p. 17.
55 Kristina Orth-Gomér and Jeffrey V. Johnson, ‘Social Network Interaction and Mortality: A Six-Year Follow-Up Study of a Random
Sample of the Swedish Population’, Journal of Chronic Disease, Vol. 40, No. 10, 1987, p. 956.
56 Pierre Bourdieu, ‘Cultural Reproduction and Social Reproduction’ in Knowledge, Education, and Social Change: Papers in the
Sociology of Education (London: Tavistock, 1973), pp. 71–112.
40
Cognition
Characterised by their openness to possible explanations for any positive association
observed between arts participation and health, Bygren et al have alluded to several
cognitive theories. These include: the communicative theory of art action (which
prioritises the symbolic nature of nonverbal art forms and their power in structuring
feelings), the arousal theory (which supposes that art stimulates functions necessary
to our survival as a species, akin to food and sex) and the psychoanalytic theory
(which presumes that art offers vicarious satisfaction of sublimated desires).
In humans, arts engagement has been shown to have a cognitive effect, and studies
are increasingly being dedicated to unravelling the neural basis for this. Different parts
of the brain have been found to respond to the rhythm, tone and timbral complexity of
music,57 which becomes differentiated during free improvisation.58 In a study of people
with dementia, singing and listening to music were found to improve mood, orientation,
remote episodic memory and general cognition, while singing has been seen to
enhance short-term and working memory.59
Delving deeper into physiological explanations for these observations, the connection
between the hypothalamus, pituitary and adrenal glands (the HPA axis) has been
looked to as a possible mediator in psychosomatic mechanisms since the 1940s,
and Bygren et al have consistently implicated this axis as a possible factor in the
defence against depression. Studies of brain function repeatedly implicate excess
production of glucocorticoids (cortisol in humans – produced when the body is placed
under stress) in the suppression of the hypothalamus and pituitary gland60 and
deterioration of the hippocampus (which deals with memory and spatial navigation).61
The HPA axis is also implicated in bipolar disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity
disorder (ADHD) and major depressive disorder. By contrast, environmental enrichment
in rodents has been found to increase the number of glucocorticoid receptors in the
hippocampus, augmenting the plasticity of neurons, improving cognitive functions,
such as learning and memory,62 and increasing willingness to explore.63
57 Vinoo Alluri, Petri Toiviainen, Iiro P. Jääskeläinen, Enrico Glerean, Mikko Sams and Elvira Brattico, ‘Large-scale brain networks
emerge from dynamic processing of musical timbre, key and rhythm’, NeuroImage, 59, 2012, pp. 3677–89.
58 Aaron L. Berkowitz and Daniel Ansaric, ‘Generation of novel motor sequences: The neural correlates of musical improvisation’,
NeuroImage, 41, 2008, pp. 535–43.
59 Teppo Särkämö, Mari Tervaniemi, Sari Laitinen, Ava Numminen, Merja Kurki, Julene K. Johnson and Pekka Rantanen, ‘Cognitive,
Emotional, and Social Benefits of Regular Musical Activities in Early Dementia: Randomized Controlled Study’, The Gerontologist,
2014, pp. 634–50.
60 Carina Hibberd, Joyce L.W. Yau and Jonathan R. Seckl, ‘Glucocorticoids and the ageing hippocampus’, Journal of Anatomy, 197,
2000, pp. 553–62.
61 Tommy Olsson, Abdul K. Mohammed, Lucy F. Donaldson and Jonathan R. Seckl, ‘Transcription factor AP-2 gene expression in
adult rat hippocampal regions: effects of environmental manipulations’, Neuroscience Letters, 189, 1995, pp. 113–16.
62 Sigbritt Rasmuson, Tommy Olsson, Bengt G. Henriksson, Paul A.T. Kelly, Megan C. Holmes, Jonathan R. Seckl and Abdul
H. Mohammed, ‘Environmental enrichment selectively increases 5-HT1A receptor mRNA expression and binding in the rat
hippocampus’, Molecular Brain Research, 53, 1998, pp. 285–90.
63 Aurelia Zimmermann, Markus Stauffacher, Wolfgang Langhans and Hanno Würbel, ‘Enrichment-dependent differences in novelty
exploration in rats can be explained by habituation’, Behavioural Brain Research, 121, 2001, pp. 11–20.
41
64 Örjan De Manzano, Simon Cervenka, Anke Karabanov, Lars Farde and Fredrik Ullén, ‘Thinking Outside a Less Intact Box:
Thalamic Dopamine D2 Receptor Densities Are Negatively Related to Psychometric Creativity in Healthy Individuals’, PLOS one,
May 2010, Vol. 5, No. 5, e10670, p. 1.
65 Örjan De Manzano, Simon Cervenka, Aurelija Jucaite, Oscar Hellenäs, Lars Farde and Fredrik Ullén, ‘Individual differences in the
proneness to have flow experiences are linked to dopamine D2-receptor availability in the dorsal striatum’, NeuroImage, 67, 2013,
p. 1.
66 K. Asakawa, ‘ Flow experience and autotelic personality in Japanese college students: how do they experience challenges in
everyday life?’, Journal of Happiness Studies, 5, 2004, pp. 123–154; K. Asakawa, ‘Flow experience, culture, and well-being: how
do autotelic Japanese college students feel, behave, and think in their daily lives?’, Journal of Happiness Studies, 11, 2010, pp.
205–223; I. Ishimura and M.Kodama, ‘Dimensions of flow experience in Japanese college students: relation between flow experience
and mental health’, Journal of Health Psychology, 13, 2006, pp. 23–34.
67 Bygren et al, 2009, op cit., p. 65; emphasis added.
Extrapolating the connection between creativity and cognition, a Swedish team has
found that creativity increases as the density of D2 dopamine receptors in the thalamus
decreases.64 A lower density of receptors in an area of the brain associated with
schizophrenia and bipolar disorder facilitates greater flexibility and originality.
The same team found the density of D2 receptors in the striatal region of the brain to be
positively associated with ‘flow’, which is understood as a ‘psychological state of high
but subjectively effortless attention, low self-awareness, sense of control and enjoyment
that can occur during the performance of tasks that are challenging, but matched in
difficulty to the skill level of the person’.65 Several studies have
established links between proneness to flow and self-esteem,
life satisfaction and psychological wellbeing.66
These findings are reflected in the evidence base, with Bygren
et al speculating that ‘The cognitive engagement and effort
that results from attending cultural events may stimulate
[immunological responses] in sensitive individuals’.67 Wang
et al and Cuypers et al also understand cultural activities to
require a mental function. In investigating deaths through
accident and suicide, Väänänen et al presume that increased
socio-cultural participation gives way to better mental health.
This takes us to another site for scrutinising the long-term
effect of arts participation – that of occupational health.
In humans, arts
engagement has
been shown to
have a cognitive
effect, and studies
are increasingly
being dedicated
to unravelling the
neural basis for this.
42
Occupational Health and Work Strain
After Cassel, Welin et al outline two ‘types of social processes of importance in disease
aetiology. The first is dominated by deleterious or stress factors which enhance
vulnerability to disease. The second consists of protective factors which buffer the
organism from the effects of noxious stimuli’.68 In relation to the first factor, work
strain – typically engendered through a composite of high psychological effort and
low recognition/reward, enduring into retirement and equally applicable to un/under-
employment – is ‘thought to suppress the immune system and thus render an organism
more susceptible to disease, especially those diseases closely linked with immune
functioning, such as malignancy, infection, autoimmune disease, and allergy’.69
In relation to the second factor, Bygren et al observe that ‘Cultural activity might be
thought to increase resistance to a broad spectrum of diseases or be the impetus to
start dealing with problems’.70 It is useful to consider the relationship between these
two factors more closely.
In 2005, a short-term study of Canadian emergency services employees found that
‘higher levels of frequency in cultural leisure significantly predicted greater physical
health’.71 Cultural leisure was taken to embrace concerts, ballet, theatre and museums,
and thought to be a palliative means of coping with stress. Drawing upon this research,
Bygren’s US collaboration delved further into the possible molecular biological
explanations for stress and its relationship to diseases including cancer.72 This lingered
upon the oxidation of DNA to form 8-hydroxydeoxyguanosine (8-OH-dG) – a biomarker
for cancer – which is caused by stress, particularly in women.73 More recently, (broadly
defined) creative activity undertaken outside of work has been found to hasten recovery
from work strain and enhance work-related performance.74
68 Lennart Welin, Kurt Svärdsudd, S. Ander-Peciva, Gösta Tibblin, Bodil Tibblin, Bo Larsson and L. Wilhemsen, ‘Prospective Study of
Social Influences on Mortality: The Study of Men Born in 1913 and 1923’, The Lancet, Vol. 325, No. 8434, 20 April 1985, p. 917.
69 Alison Fife, Pamela J. Beasley and Debra L. Fertig, ‘Psychoneuroimmunology and cancer: Historical perspectives and current
research’, Advances in Neuroimmunology, Vol. 6, 1996, p. 183.
70 Bygren et al, 1996, op cit., p. 1577.
71 Yoshitaka Iwasaki, Roger C. Mannell, Bryan J.A. Smale and Janice Butcher, ‘Contributions of Leisure Participation in Predicting
Stress Coping and Health among Police and Emergency Response Services Workers’, Journal of Health Psychology, Vol 10, No. 1,
2005, p. 94.
72 Wilkinson et al, 2007, op cit.
73 Masahiro Irie, Shinya Asami, Shoji Nagata, Masato Ikeda, Masakazu Miyata and Hiroshi Kasai, ‘Psychosocial Factors as a
Potential Trigger of Oxidative DNA Damage in Human Leukocytes’, Japanese Journal of Cancer Research, 92, March 2001, pp.
367–376; Masahiro Irie, Shinya Asami, Shoji Nagata, Masato Ikeda, Masakazu Miyata and Hiroshi Kasai, ‘Psychosocial Mediation
of a Type of Oxidative DNA Damage, 8-Hydroxydeoxyguanosine, in Peripheral Blood Leucocytes of Non-Smoking and Non-Drinking
Workers’, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 71, 2002, pp. 90–6.
74 Kevin J.Eschleman, Jamie Madsen, Gene Alarcon and Alex Barelka, ‘Benefiting from creative activity: The positive relationships
between creative activity, recovery experiences, and performance-related outcomes’, Journal of Occupational and Organizational
Psychology, early view online, 14 April 2014.
43
75 Töres Theorell, Walter Osika, Constanze Leineweber, Linda L. Magnusson Hanson, Eva Bojner Horwitz and Hugo Westerlund, ‘Is
cultural activity at work related to mental health in employees?’, International Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health, 86,
2013, p. 282.
Considering cultural activities within the workplace, a team around Töres Theorell at the Stress Research
Institute of Stockholm University has speculated that health effects ‘could arise (1) because such activities
may strengthen cohesiveness between employees and between management and employees resulting
in improved psychosocial work environment or (2) because of direct effects of the cultural activities
themselves’.75 Investigating which of these possibilities was more likely over time, they team found that
emotional exhaustion decreased as workplace cultural activities increased, with the latter predictive of
the former over a two-year period, independent of other psychosocial factors. In this, the quality and
regularity of activities appeared to be significant, and it was suggested that the need for such activities
became heightened during periods of high national unemployment.
In exploring the relationship between psychosocial factors and disease, it is helpful to look a little more
closely at the relationship between stress and susceptibility.
The quality and regularity
of activities appeared
to be significant, and it
was suggested that the
need for such activities
became heightened during
periods of high national
unemployment.
44
Psycho-Neuroimmunology, Endocrine and Metabolic Effects
Towards the end of the 20th century, it was posited that, rather than being autonomous,
self-regulating entities, the nervous, immune and endocrine systems function in
a reciprocal way in response to environmental and psychological stimuli. In a study
of the impact of psychosocial and behavioural factors upon cancer, it was found that
distress negatively influenced three mechanisms central to carcinogenesis, including
the ability of cells to repair damaged DNA.76 The significance of studies of this kind
is that, in making a connection between stress, distress and chronic morbidity via
psycho-immunoneurological pathways, they pave the way for an exploration of factors
(including arts engagement) which might reduce the negative side of this equation.
It has long been asserted that ‘verbally expressing experiences by writing or talking
improves physical health, enhances immune function, and is associated with fewer
medical visits’.77 To take just one example, the lymphocyte count of HIV patients has
been found to increase following repeated half-hour bouts of emotional writing, with
positive changes in health correlating with the use of positive words, presumed to be
a result of raised levels of insight.78
In several of the longitudinal studies conducted by Bygren et al, space has been
dedicated to psycho-neuroimmunology, taking account of the innervation of lymphoid
organs and release of neurotransmitters precipitated by engagement with nonverbal
art forms including music and visual art. In rodents, it has been found that ‘music
can effectively reverse adverse effects of stress on the number and capacities of
lymphocytes that are required for an optimal immunological response against cancer’.79
This is complemented by research into the beneficial effects of music upon human
immunity.80 In specific relation to visual art, it has been observed that ‘works of art
arouse effects which were not observed after a comparable amount of conversation
about daily events’.81
76 J.K. Kiecolt-Glaser and R. Glaser, ‘Psychoneuroimmunology and Cancer: Fact or Fiction?’, European Journal of Cancer, Vol. 35,
No. 11, 1999, pp. 1603–7.
77 Diane S. Berry and James W. Pennebaker, ‘Nonverbal and Verbal Emotional Expression and Health’, Psychotherapy and
Psychosomatics, 59, 1993, p. 11.
78 Keith J.Petrie, Iris Fontanilla, Mark G. Thomas, Roger J. Booth and James W. Pennebaker, ‘Effect of Written Emotional Expression
on Immune Function in Patients With Human Immunodeficiency Virus Infection: A Randomized Trial’, Psychosomatic Medicine, 66,
2004, pp. 272–5.
79 María J.Núñez, Paula Mañá, David Liñares, María P. Riveiro, José Balboa, Juan Suárez-Quintanilla, Mónica Maracchi, Manuel Rey
Méndez, José M. López and Manuel Freire-Garabal, ‘Music, immunity and cancer’, Life Sciences, 71, 2002, p. 1047.
80 Rollin McCraty, Mike Atkinson, Glen Rein and Alan D. Watkins, ‘Music Enhances the Effect of Positive Emotional States on Salivary
IgA’, Stress Medicine, Vol. 12, 1996, pp. 167–75.
81 Britt-Mai Wikström, Töres Theorell, S. Sandström, ‘Medical Health and Emotional Effects of Art Stimulation in Old Age: A
Controlled Intervention Study concerning the Effects of Visual Stimulation Provided in the Form of Pictures’, Psychotherapy and
Psychosomatics, Vol. 60, Nos. 3-4, 1993, p. 202.
45
82 Boinkum Benson Konlaan, N Björby, Lars Olov Bygren, Gösta Weissglas, Lena Karlsson, Maria Widmark, ‘Attendance at cultural
events and physical exercise and health: a randomized controlled study’, Public Health, Vol. 114, No. 5, Sept 2000b, pp. 316–9.
83 Lars Olov Bygren, Gösta Weissglas, Britt-Mai Wikström, Boinkum Benson Konlaan, Grjibovski, Andrej Grijbovski, Ann-Birth
Karlsson, Sven-Olov Andersson and Michael Sjöström, ‘Cultural Participation and Health: A Randomized Controlled Trial Among
Medical Care Staff’, Psychosomatic Medicine, 71, 2009b, pp. 469–73.
Bygren has been part of two relevant RCTs, excluded from the evidence base due to their short-term
design. The first of these sought to differentiate the biomedical and social effects of light physical activity
and cultural attendance (at either a film, concert, play or art exhibition once a week for two months)
from the more general effects of participating in group activity. As distinct from those engaging in
exercise, those taking part in cultural activities showed reduced blood pressure and reduced levels of
adrenocorticotropical hormone (part of the HPA axis implicated in stress).82
In a second RCT led by Bygren, mainly female employees of a local government officers’ union in the
health service of northern Sweden were offered free access to films, concerts, art exhibitions (preceded
by an expert introduction where necessary) or singing in a choir once a week for eight weeks. Physical
health, social functioning and vitality improved in the group exposed to cultural stimuli. While the effects
were less pronounced than in longitudinal studies, they were most noticeable among those participants
who visited art exhibitions.83 Notwithstanding the limitations of this study, including its short timeframe,
it seems to provide additional evidence of the positive health effects of cultural participation in general
and visual art forms in particular.
Physical health, social
functioning and vitality
improved in the group
exposed to cultural stimuli.
While the effects were
less pronounced than
in longitudinal studies,
they were most noticeable
among those participants
who visited art exhibitions.
46
Epigenetics
Recent research has shown that one’s environment has a part to play in determining
which genes are switched on or off in the body at any given time. The signals
controlling this process are thought to be carried not within the coding part of the
genome but within the surrounding material (epigenome). As Bygren explains in a
recent singly authored article, ‘The molecular and physiological processes following
an environmental exposure induce epigenetic changes that cause the genome, with
its DNA sequence, to change expression patterns, potentially inducing physiological
changes that result in disease or protection from disease’.84 In other words, short-term
exposure to a range of external factors can cause long-term changes to the phenotype
(the body’s make-up) without affecting the genotype (the genetic code), bypassing
conventional genetics.
One possible mechanism for this is the methylation of cytosine (one of the four
nucleotides making up the DNA code), as observed when the body is placed under
stress. The introduction of a simple hydrocarbon at certain points within the DNA helix
impedes the transcription of messenger RNA and consequent translation of proteins.
Methylation may also act in tandem with modification of the histone proteins, which
arrange the genetic material into its characteristic chromosomal shape and provide
a spool around which the DNA winds itself during transcription.
Epigenetic markers have been found in a range of conditions
from prostate85 and breast86 cancers to ADHD.87
84 Lars Olov Bygren, ‘Intergenerational Health Responses to Adverse and Enriched Environments’, Annual Review of Public Health,
34, 2013, p. 50.
85 Rene Cortese, Andrew Kwan, Emilie Lalonde, Olga Bryzgunova, et al, ‘Epigenetic markers of prostate cancer in plasma circulating
DNA’, Human Molecular Genetics, Vol. 21, No. 16, 2012, pp. 3619–31.
86 James M. Flanagan, Marta Munoz-Alegre, Stephen Henderson, Thomas Tang, et al, ‘Gene-body hypermethylation of ATM in
peripheral blood DNA of bilateral breast cancer patients’, Human Molecular Genetics, Vol. 18, No. 7, 2009, pp. 1332–42.
87 Jonathan Mill and Arturas Petronis, ‘Pre- and peri-natal environmental risks for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): the
potential role of epigenetic processes in mediating susceptibility’, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Vol. 49, No. 10, 2008,
pp. 1020–30.
The relationship
between environment
and epigenetic
modification suggests
great potential for
arts engagement in
mitigating a range of
acute morbidities.
47
While a certain amount of reprogramming and repair of the DNA happens when a baby
is conceived, it is now thought that complex conditions, triggered by environmental
factors, create epigenetic markers that are passed through the generations.88 So, for
example, it seems that intergenerational transmission of a predisposition to alcoholism
is linked to paternal exposure to alcohol prior to conception.89 Similar epigenetic
mechanisms, stimulated by adverse environmental conditions, are implicated in the
intergenerational transmission of major psychoses, such as depressive disorder,90
schizophrenia91 and bipolar disorder,92 through the methylation of DNA in the frontal
cortex of the brain.93
As Bygren’s explanation at the start of this subsection suggests, the converse is also
true, and exposure to enriched environments can have a positive epigenetic effect.
So, for example, the aforementioned neurological enhancements observed in rodents
is transmissible through the generations.94 If we conceive cultural activity as a form
of environmental enrichment, this has obvious significance in relation to the
present analysis.
The relationship between environment and epigenetic modification suggests great
potential for arts engagement in mitigating a range of acute morbidities.
With demonstrable intragenerational effects, cultural participation may yet prove to
moderate the epigenetic transfer of disease susceptibility through the generations.
Taking this supposition together with the suspected connection between epigenetic
mechanisms and major psychoses (known to worsen through generations), we might
begin to explain the role of cultural participation in ameliorating mental disorders,
as observed in multifarious arts and health organisations.
If further evidence of these beneficial effects is needed, we shall doubtless have
recourse to national and international datasets. Let us turn now to a consideration
of their suitability to the task.
88 M.E. Pembrey, Lars-Olov Bygren, G. Kaati, S. Edvinsson, K. Northstone, et al, ‘Sex-specific, male-line transgenerational responses
in humans’, European Journal of Human Genetics, 14, 2006, pp. 159–66
89 J.G. Knezovich and M Ramsay, ‘The effect of preconception paternal alcohol exposure on epigenetic remodeling of the H19 and
Rasgrf1 imprinting control regions in mouse off-spring’, Frontiers in Genetics, 2012, 3, pp. 10–15.
90 Jonathan Mill and Arturas Petronis, ‘Molecular studies of major depressive disorder: the epigenetic perspective’, Molecular
Psychiatry, 12, 2007, 799–814.
91 Chieko Kato, Arturas Petronis, Yuji Okazaki, Mamoru Tochigi et al, ‘Molecular genetic studies of schizophrenia: challenges and
insights’, Neuroscience Research, 43, 2002, pp. 295–304; Arturas Petronis, ‘The Origin of Schizophrenia: Genetic Thesis, Epigenetic
Antithesis, and Resolving Synthesis’, Biological Psychiatry, 55, 2004, pp. 965–70.
92 Arturas Petronis, ‘Epigenetics and Bipolar Disorder: New Opportunities and Challenges’, American Journal of Medical Genetics,
123C, 2003, pp. 65–75.
93 Jonathan Mill, Thomas Tang, Zachary Kaminsky, Tarang Khare, et al, ‘Epigenomic Profiling Reveals DNA-Methylation Changes
Associated with Major Psychosis’, The American Journal of Human Genetics, 82, March 2008, pp. 696–711.
94 Naoko Kuzumaki, Daigo Ikegami, Rie Tamura, Nana Hareyama, et al, ‘Hippocampal Epigenetic Modification at the Brain-Derived
Neurotrophic Factor Gene Induced by an Enriched Environment’, Hippocampus, 21, 2011, pp. 127–32; Richelle Mychasiuka, Saif
Zahira, Nichole Schmoldb, Slava Ilnytskyyc, et al, ‘Parental enrichment and offspring development: Modifications to brain, behavior
and the epigenome’, Behavioural Brain Research, 228, 2012, pp. 294– 298.
48
Nordic Datasets
In consulting the studies that constitute the evidence base, it quickly becomes clear
that research teams benefited from population data collated over time, largely in the
Nordic countries. This is no coincidence, as Hyyppä explains:
For epidemiological surveys, it is a major strength that all Nordic countries
have for a long time kept comprehensive population registers with unique
personal identification numbers for each citizen. The personal identification
system is one of the best tools for practicing [sic] scientific epidemiology
as it can be utilized for linking data derived from different nationwide
sources. Comparative and follow-up data sets with, for example, morbidity
and mortality rates are available for investigators, and some of the
registered data are available even for laymen on the websites of Statistics
Finland and Statistics Sweden.95
Critical Analysis of the
Data Sources Relevant
to Explorations
of the Long-Term
Relationship between
Arts Participation
and Health
95 Hyyppä, 2010, op cit., p. vi.
49
96 See: http://snd.gu.se/en/catalogue/study/389
97 Steinar Krokstad, Arnulf Langhammer, K. Hveem, TL. Holmen, K. Midthjell, TR. Stene, G. Bratberg, Jon Heggland and Jostein
Holmen, ‘Cohort Profile: The HUNT Study, Norway’, International Journal of Epidemiology, 42, 2013, pp. 968–77.
Since 2005–6, Hyyppä has relied upon the Mini-Finland Health Survey, carried out in
1978–80, which asked a sample of 8,000 people aged between 30 and 99 questions
about their attendance at a range of cultural events. Linkage to morbidity and mortality
registers has permitted participants to be followed up for disease onset, mortality and
cause of death over the intervening years.
A decade earlier, Bygren et al pioneered use of the Swedish Survey of Living
Conditions, which was established in 1968 in a bid to explain the inequality and
exclusion that persisted at the heart of the country’s social democracy.96
Annually surveying around 7,600 adults up to the age of 74, a module covering
social relations, including questions on arts and cultural participation, is added
every eight years, roughly doubling the sample size. The same survey was used by
Sundquist et al, while Lennartson and Silverstein used the Swedish Panel Study of
Living Conditions of the Oldest Old, which captures participants who have exceeded
the age of 74. This way of working facilitates a posteriori analysis, as opposed to
a pre-emptive longitudinal design, which means that respondents have been blinded
to researchers’ as-yet-unformulated interests.
The final study in the evidence base draws upon the HUNT Study, centred on Nord-
Trøndelag County in central Norway.97 This is a population-based survey conducted
in 1984–6, 1995–7, 2006–8 and currently preparing for its fourth wave. At each wave,
respondents have completed a comprehensive questionnaire about their lifestyle,
health and quality of life and undergone a clinical examination during which
anthropometric measurements, blood and DNA samples were taken. At wave 2,
two questions about cultural participation were added, concerning frequency of
attendance at cultural events and participation in creative or social activities. At the
same time, adolescents (aged 13–19 years) were added to the study and asked more
detailed questions about their participation in a range of social and cultural activities.
Data from the HUNT Study, accumulated within a particular region of Norway,
map onto the national picture and could, arguably, be extrapolated to a northern
European context more generally. As will be discussed in the next section, there is
ample scope to design future biomedical studies of cultural attendance and health
drawing on this impressive resource.
50
UK-Based Datasets
In the ACE evidence review mentioned in the introduction, it was noted that ‘there
is huge untapped potential in existing data sets to explore relationships between
arts engagement and participation and a whole host of other personal and societal
outcomes, which would make an important contribution to the UK-based evidence
about the social and economic contribution of the arts’.98 As a necessary prelude to
recommendations for future research, let us take a look at the most appropriate of
these in turn.
British Cohort Study 1970 (BCS70)
BCS70 follows the lives of more than 17,000 people born in England, Scotland
and Wales during a single week in 1970. Typical of birth cohort studies, this adopts
a life course approach, seeking to ‘understand influences of early-life exposures
and development on later disease outcome and the processes occurring in the
intervening years of life that link them’.99 Since the original birth survey, there have
been seven ‘sweeps’ of cohort members at ages 5, 10, 16, 26, 30, 34, 38 and 42.
The DCMS overview mentioned in the methodology notes the relevance to studies of
arts engagement of the sweeps conducted at ages 10, 16 and 34. When consulted in
relation to the present investigation, researchers responsible for the survey pointed to
those conducted at ages 16 and 42, at which fairly extensive questions were asked with
regard to cultural participation; at the same time, the researchers affirm that physical
and mental health have been key concerns of the study over its duration.
Referring to the relevant questionnaires, it becomes clear that the 16-year-old
participants were asked how often they took part in any of 47 different leisure activities.
These included: listening to or making music, sewing, knitting, drawing, painting, writing
and visiting museums/galleries, the theatre or cinema. At the same time, the teenage
participants were asked about the frequency with which various forms of physical and
psychological discomfort were experienced, and their cognitive aptitudes were tested.
When respondents reached the age of 42, leisure questions were the first to appear
in the questionnaire, spanning similar categories as before, followed by questions
about exercise and diet. At question 31, the full Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Wellbeing
Scale (WEMWBS) was inserted. The existence of BCS70 enables associations between
mental wellbeing and arts participation to be studied on a much larger scale.
English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA)
ELSA is drawn from people who took part in the Health Survey for England (HSE,
an annual, nationally representative cross-sectional household survey) in 1998, 1999
or 2001, who were born before March 1952. Since 2002–3, participants aged 50+ have
been followed up with a face-to-face interview, self-completion questionnaire every two
years and a nurse’s visit every other wave.
98 Arts Council England, 2014, op cit., p. 41..
99 Chris Power, Diana Kuh and Susan Morton, ‘From Developmental Origins of Adult Disease to Life Course Research on Adult
Disease and Aging: Insights from Birth Cohort Studies’, Annual Review of Public Health, 34, 2013, p. 8.
51
While DCMS deems ELSA irrelevant to studies of participation in the arts or museums/
galleries, the frequency of attendance at: cinemas; art galleries/museums; theatres/
concerts/operas has been assayed. At the same time, participants have been asked
whether they would like to undertake any of these activities more often. Considerable
data about disease, disability and self-rated health have been gathered alongside a
consideration of life satisfaction, wellbeing, self-perception and social networks. Wave
6 (to be released in autumn 2014) solicited information about participation in education,
arts or music groups or evening classes. Overall, this dataset permits analysis of
creative engagement in relation to a number of health variables over time.
National Child Development Study (NCDS)
NCDS follows the lives of 17,000 people born in England, Scotland and Wales in
a single week of 1958. Since birth, all cohort members have taken part in further
sweeps at ages 7, 11, 16, 23, 33, 42, 46, 50 and 55. In 2003 (age 45), 9,000 cohort
members also participated in a special biomedical survey, in a bid to elucidate how
development, environments and lifestyles affected health.
DCMS identifies sweeps 2, 4 and 5 as being relevant to arts research, with sweep
5 additionally capturing museums and gallery data. More specifically, sweep 5 asked
about the activities of children aged between six and ten, including whether they had
access to a musical instrument, read for pleasure, were encouraged to develop and
maintain hobbies, engaged in the music/arts/dance/drama or visited a museum/
musical/theatrical performance. While the biomedical survey of 2003 promises detailed
health data, participants were only asked about their children’s arts engagement
activities, rather than their own, which limits its relevance to this study.
Taking Part
In 2005, a report commissioned by ACE analysed survey data concerning arts
participation and health, generated by the Office for National Statistics (ONS) in
2001, 2002 and 2003. Taking self-rated health as the outcome measure and excluding
participants who reported a long-term illness, this showed that,
‘Allowing for age and other socio-demographic characteristics, better health was more
likely to be reported by people who:
l Attended performing arts events
l Attended non-performing arts or cultural events or venues
l Participated in dance activities
l Accessed artforms through CDs, mini discs, tapes or records
l Listened to the arts through the radio
l Viewed the arts on television, videos or DVDs’ 100
100 Joy Windsor, Your health and the arts: a study of the association between arts engagement and health (London: Arts Council
England, 2005), p.10.
52
Consulted for this study, ONS claimed not to be exploring cultural participation any
further. To fill the gap, DCMS partnered with ACE, English Heritage and Sport England
to devise the annual Taking Part survey, implemented by TNS BMRB. The survey
includes a wide range of questions about attendance at arts and cultural events and
participation in creative activities. It also includes questions about self-rated health,
enduring illness and its debilitating effects. In addition to this, respondents are asked to
rate their happiness on a ten-point scale from extremely unhappy to extremely happy,
and three questions are posed about subjective wellbeing as follows:
l Overall, how satisfied are you with your life nowadays?
l Overall, to what extent do you feel that the things you do in your life
are worthwhile?
l On a scale where nought is ‘not at all anxious’ and 10 is ‘completely
anxious’, overall, how anxious did you feel yesterday?
In 2010, multivariate analysis of the 2007–8 wave was undertaken by researchers from
TNS BMRB, in a bid to determine the predictors of cultural engagement, which were
found to be: age, education, health and ethnicity alongside professional status and
prosperity.101 In the same year, the Culture and Sport Evidence (CASE) programme –
a three-year programme led by DCMS, with the Taking Part partners plus the Museums,
Libraries and Archives Council – analysed data in Taking Part and the British Household
Panel Survey (mentioned in the next subsection). This study undertook to calculate
the monetary value of short-term improvements in subjective wellbeing resulting from
participation in culture and sport (with consideration of long-term value confined to
sport). In 2013, Fujiwara attempted an analysis of cross-sectional Taking Part data from
2005–11, with a focus on self-rated health and subjective wellbeing (centred on the
happiness question).103 This found being an audience to the arts to have a monetary
value of around £2,000 per person per year and the value of participating in the arts to
be comparable to sport if health was controlled for (£1,500 per person per year).
Initially intended to be cross-sectional, Taking Part became longitudinal in 2013–4,104
creating scope for assessments of the impact of arts engagement upon illness and
subjective wellbeing over time.
101 Geoff Inglis and Joel Williams, Models of sporting and cultural activity: Analysis of the Taking Part Survey, August 2010.
102 CASE: The Culture and Sport Evidence Programme, Understanding the value of engagement in culture and sport: Summary
Report, July 2010.
103 Daniel Fujiwara, Museums and happiness: the value of participating in museums and the arts, commissioned by the Happy
Museum, April 2013a.
104 TNS BMRB, Taking Part Survey 2012/13 Longitudinal Development Report, 2012.
53
Understanding Society/United Kingdom Household Longitudinal Study (UKHLS)
In 1991, the British Household Panel Survey (BHPS) embarked upon a longitudinal
study of British households (initially around 5,500 households and 10,300 individuals,
growing to just over 8,000 households). At wave 18, BHPS participants were asked if
they would consider joining a new, wider-ranging survey called Understanding Society,
and almost 6,700 participants did so from wave 2 of 2010–11.105
Since 2009, Understanding Society has annually captured important information
about the social and economic circumstances and attitudes of people living in
40,000 UK households, approximately half of whom also take part in a detailed health
questionnaire. DCMS points to wave 2 uniquely soliciting data about arts participation
and museum/gallery attendance. Indeed, questions pertaining to leisure, culture
and sport, adapted from Taking Part, were included, in a bid to ascertain whether,
at any point during the preceding year, respondents participated in creative activity
or attended cultural events and, if so, how often. Also at wave 2, a questionnaire was
inserted for use by nurses conducting health checks. This sought to gather data on
medical history and prescribed medicines; blood pressure and a blood sample were
also taken and waist circumference and lung function measured.
While the DCMS overview only goes as far as wave 4, the same cultural questions
were also included in the survey during wave 5, but the Understanding Society team
advises that these questions will not be carried again in their current form.
Taking account of the above, there is scope for cross-sectional analysis of wave 2 data,
with any of the biometric variables taken as dependent and cultural attendance as
independent, adjusting for a variety of socio-demographic confounders. In April 2014,
DCMS published two cross-sectional analyses of wave 2 data – commissioned from
a team centred on the London School of Economics – a report on the financial benefits
of cultural and sporting engagement (including health) and an analysis of the impact
of such engagement upon subjective wellbeing. The first of these found that ‘Those
engaging with the arts as an audience member were 5.4% more likely to report good
health’, which translates as a saving to the NHS.106 The second analysis, which took
life satisfaction as an indicator of subjective wellbeing, noted that, after adjusting for
a range of potential confounders, ‘Arts engagement was found to be associated with
higher wellbeing. This is valued at £1,084 per person per year, or £90 per person per
month’.107 Leadbetter and O’Connor suggest that ‘Further research could be carried
out using longitudinal data from Understanding Society to explore the direction of
causality’.108 So, for example, wave 2 data could be compared with data from wave
5 to establish the health value of continued cultural participation (akin to Johansson et
al, 2001). Additionally, there is much more scope for analysis of physical health data –
including waist circumference – in relation to arts participation.
105 https://www.iser.essex.ac.uk/bhps
106 Fujiwara et al, 2014a, op cit., p. 9.
107 Daniel Fujiwara, Laura Kudrna and Paul Dolan, Quantifying and Valuing the Wellbeing Impacts of Culture and Sport (London:
Department of Culture, Media and Sport, 2014b), p. 9.
108 Leadbetter and O’Connor, op cit., p. 17.
54
Scottish Household Survey
In a report commissioned by the Scottish Government, called Healthy Attendance?
The Impact of Cultural Engagement and Sports Participation on Health and Satisfaction
with Life in Scotland, Leadbetter and O’Connor note that:
In Scotland, questions on participation in culture and sport have been
included in the Scottish Household Survey since 2007. Questions on
life satisfaction and self-assessed health were added in 2009. This means
that, for the first time at a population level, data is available to statistically
explore the relationship between taking part in cultural and sporting
activities, attending cultural places and key quality of life measures
in Scotland.109
This survey captures attendance at cultural events or places of culture, including
cinemas, libraries and live music events, and participation in activities such as reading
for pleasure, dancing and crafts. Drawing on previous research (including Cuypers,
2011), Leadbetter and O’Connor included the control variables of age, economic status,
income, area deprivation, education qualification, disability/or long standing illness
and smoking in their analysis; life satisfaction and self-assessed health were also used
as independent variables, in a bid to account for any connection between health and
overall satisfaction with life. They found that, ‘after controlling for other factors including
socio-economic factors, participation in culture and sport are independently and
significantly associated with good health and high life satisfaction’.110 More specifically,
‘Significant associations were found between health and attendance at cinema,
art exhibitions, craft exhibitions, street art and theatre. There is also a relationship
between attendance at individual cultural places and high life satisfaction, with
significant associations found for attendance at museums, cinema, historical places
and ballet/dance’.111 While its cross-sectional design precludes any determination of
causality, this validates associations observed elsewhere and paves the way for further
research. At the same time, attention must be paid to inequalities of access to the arts
and culture, alongside issues of access to health resources.
109 Ibid, p. 5
110 Ibid, p. 14.
111 Ibid, p. 15.
55
This research has permitted identification of the key studies to have been conducted
in relation to the longitudinal association between arts participation and health. While it
is clearly untrue that ‘We lack longitudinal studies of the health benefits of participation
in arts and culture’,112 it might be argued that ‘More prospective studies on large
populations are needed to answer questions on causality’.113 Hyyppä categorically
states that:
[…] cross-sectional surveys and studies cannot establish the direction
of the causal link between social capital and population health.
Prospective longitudinal studies with repeated measurements of both
social capital [or arts engagement] and population health outcomes
are urgently needed to solve the fundamental problem of the direction
of causality. Such long-term surveys, however, are very costly and
technically demanding, and what is the most important issue from an
epidemiological perspective, they require huge data sources and
opportunities (and rights) for researchers to link personal data from
several different information sources.114
Even if all these conditions are met, establishing a causal link between arts engagement
and health will be incredibly challenging.
As will be clear from the evidence base, the two greatest obstacles to attributing
causality are reverse causation and residual confounders. In the first case, it is
generally assumed (by all but Fujiwara et al) that people with poor health tend to
take a diminished part in cultural activities, thereby skewing the results. Interestingly,
researchers on the HUNT Study have given consideration to the biases that might arise
through non-participation in surveys, and found socio-economic status and disease
to be the two main reasons for non-response.115 In the second case, ‘there is the
unavoidable problem of possible unknown or latent confounders’.116 We have seen the
wide range of potential health-affecting factors that have been included in the studies
in the evidence base – from age and gender to income and residential status. In the
attempt to establish causality, this effort will need to be substantiated and broadened.
Recommendations
for Future Research
112 Arts Council England, op cit., p. 4; Carnwath and Brown, op cit., p. 2.
113 Cuypers et al, 2011, op cit., p. 22.
114 Hyyppä, 2010, op cit., p. 70.
115 Arnulf Langhammer, Steinar Krokstad, Pål Romundstad, Jon Heggland and Jostein Holmen, ‘The HUNT study: participation is
associated with survival and depends on socioeconomic status, diseases and symptoms’, BMC Medical Research Methodology,
2012, 12, 143.
116 Hyyppä, 2010, op cit., p. ix.
56
Most of the datasets underlying the international evidence base are available for
re-examination. Beginning with the study conducted in Gothenburg in 1963 and 1973,
used by Welin et al, data gathered around attendance at the cinema, theatre, concerts,
museums and exhibitions could be revisited in relation to actual and perceived ill
health. Analyses of data from the Mini-Finland Survey that paid attention to art form
specificity would be pertinent to the present study, as would closer inspection of the
biochemical measurements taken during the baseline survey. Data from the HUNT
Study remain under-analysed due to the commitment of funds to conducting the
periodic survey and maintaining the impressive biomedical facility required to store the
samples gathered. Scope exists to replicate the studies of Bygren et al, with respect to
cultural participation and all-cause mortality from wave 2 onwards. Data from Young-
HUNT 1 could also be followed up with respect to continued cultural engagement and
psychological health, medication and hospital admissions into adulthood.
The relevance of the various UK data sources has been discussed above.
The longitudinal departure of the Taking Part survey opens up the potential to test
Bygren et al’s theory about cultural stimulation being a ‘perishable commodity’,
while data gathered around cultural participation within Understanding Society could
be analysed in relation to physical health metrics over time. The universal weakness of
UK, as compared to Nordic, datasets is their inability to be cross-referenced to other
databases regarding morbidity and mortality, but birth cohort studies might provide
a way around this. So, BCS70 at ages 16 and 42 could be analysed with respect to
cultural participation and mental wellbeing, while the biomedical data gathered as
part of NCDS in 2003 could be analysed together with data on cultural participation
from several years on either side. Equally, ELSA could be used to analyse participation
against a range of physical and psychological health measures over time. In this
endeavour, attention will need to be paid to the widest range of possible confounders.
Beyond the analysis of extant data, there is scope for intervention into the questions
making up the surveys, and researchers at both the HUNT Study and DCMS (pertaining
to Taking Part) indicated their willingness to consider this. Questions relating to cultural
participation tend to be centred on frequency – how often a particular activity was
undertaken – sometimes also encompassing barriers to participation. If we are to
understand the association between arts engagement and health – and any mediating
mechanisms – much more consideration needs to be given to the qualitative nature of
participation.117 Focus groups and semi-structured interviews go some way towards
addressing this, potentially elucidating the human value of culture in relation to stress,
problem-solving, cognition and critical thinking. In this regard, the Quality Metrics Pilot,
funded by ACE in Manchester, warrants close scrutiny.118
Perhaps the most exciting area of future development is that of molecular biology.
Genetic data from the HUNT Study could be analysed to determine whether cultural
engagement has an association with stress markers such as oxidised DNA.
Beyond this, an ambitious new study could be designed that would take account
of epigenetic phenomena (such as the methylation of DNA or blood RNA),
their relationship to health conditions (such as schizophrenia) and extent to which
this is mitigated by cultural participation through the generations.
117 See Theo Stickley (ed), Qualitative Research in Arts and Mental Health (Ross-on-Wye: PCCS Books, 2012).
118 http://www.artscouncil.org.uk/what-we-do/our-priorities-2011-15/quality-metrics/quality-metrics-pilot/?dm_
i=QNP,2ICCR,GBX71V,951MM,1
57
Concluding Remarks
The majority of studies in the evidence base emphasise the public health implications
of their findings. Between 2002 and 2004, the Swedish National Institute of Public
Health conducted a review of the national and international literature around cultural
participation and health, in their broadest definitions.119 Acknowledging the distinct
remit of the National Council for Cultural Affairs, the report pointed to the role of culture
in fulfilling public health objectives. This followed the inclusion of cultural activities
and health in the 2001 Swedish Governmental Commission for Public Health, and was
followed by a governmental commission on culture, which included a section on the
societal implementation of cultural activities and health.120 As we have seen, various
levels of government in the Nordic countries have chosen to heed this evidence and
increase their support of the arts.
Contrary to more obviously instrumental attempts to harness the arts and culture
to the economy, through their presumed second-order contribution to tourism and
entrepreneurialism, health effects are inseparable from the act of engaging, becoming
amplified when engagement is increased and sustained. Perhaps, then, it is only when
governments attempt to account for individual and social benefits in economic terms
and attempt to prescribe or proscribe certain forms of engagement that we can speak
of instrumental approaches in relation to the arts and health.
119 Swedish National Institute of Public Health, op cit.
120 Cuypers et al, 2011, op cit.
Several of the studies in the evidence base acknowledge the intrinsic value of the arts and
culture. Beyond this, it suggested that engagement with the arts variously permits people
to lose themselves in creative activity and gain perspective upon their individual condition
as part of a societal whole. As a consciously chosen leisure activity, engagement in the arts
is generally shown to have a positive impact upon the body’s physiology, in turn improving
health and quality of life.
58
Responsibility for public health in the UK rests with Public Health England and Wales,
the Scottish Public Health Network, the Public Health Agency for Northern Ireland and
the local authorities. Feedback received during the annual conference of the Faculty
of Public Health suggests that there is willingness, in the public health milieu, to think
laterally about the beneficial effect of accessing high-quality arts. This ethos is being
enacted by, amongst others, Dudley Public Health.121
By contrast, at the local authority level, there is a tendency
to instrumentalise the potential public health benefits of arts
engagement. So, for example, a report produced by the
Chief Culture and Leisure Officers Association on behalf
of the National Leisure and Culture Forum in March 2014,
suggests that ‘Local authorities will also need to work more
closely with the health sector to monitor clinical outcomes
to establish the interventions and programmes that are
the most successful in terms of improving and maintaining
health’.122 As will hopefully be clear, this tendency towards
clinical outcomes and interventions contradicts the
evidence demonstrating better individual and societal health
through engagement with the arts far beyond the clinical
environment.
From a cultural perspective, population-level research of the kind represented in the
evidence base potentially exempts arts organisations from continually having to justify
their value to the public purse. Added to this, the fact that the quality of arts projects
is taken to be paramount in manifesting health effects may ultimately serve to focus
attention away from quantitative measurements of cultural value.
121 Director of Public Health, Arts & Health: Dudley Annual Report, 2013.
122 Iain Varah and Mark Taylor, Chief Culture and Leisure Officers Association, The role of culture and leisure in improving health and
wellbeing, 2014, p. 3.
As a consciously
chosen leisure activity,
engagement in the
arts is generally shown
to have a positive
impact upon the body’s
physiology, in turn
improving health and
quality of life.
59
Bibliography
Agahi, Neda and Marti G. Parker, ‘Leisure Activities and Mortality: Does Gender Matter?’, Journal
of Aging and Health, 20, 2008, pp. 855–71.
Alluri, Vinoo, Petri Toiviainen, Iiro P. Jääskeläinen, Enrico Glerean, Mikko Sams and Elvira Brattico,
‘Large-scale brain networks emerge from dynamic processing of musical timbre, key and rhythm’,
NeuroImage, 59, 2012, pp. 3677–89.
Arts Council England, The Value of Arts and Culture to People and Society: An Evidence
Review (London: Arts Council England, 2014).
Asakawa, K., ‘ Flow experience and autotelic personality in Japanese college students: how do
they experience challenges in everyday life?’ Journal of Happiness Studies, 5, 2004, pp. 123–54.
Asakawa, K., ‘Flow experience, culture, and well-being: how do autotelic Japanese college
students feel, behave, and think in their daily lives?’ Journal of Happiness Studies, 11, 2010, pp.
205–23.
Baum, Frances E., Robert A. Bush, Carolyn C. Modra, Charlie J. Murray, Eva M. Cox, Kathy M.
Alexander and Robert C. Potter, ‘Epidemiology of participation: an Australian community study’,
Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 54, 2000, pp. 414–23.
Baum, Frances E. and A.M. Ziersch, ‘Social capital’, Journal of Epidemiology and Community
Health, 57, 2003, pp. 320–3.
Berkman, Lisa F. and S. Leonard Syme, ‘Social Networks, Host Resistance and Mortality: A Nine-
Year Follow-Up of Alameda County Residents’, American Journal of Epidemiology, Vol. 109, No. 2,
1979, pp. 186–204.
Berkman, Lisa F. ‘The Role of Social Relations in Health Promotion’, Psychosomatic Medicine, 57,
1995, pp. 245–54.
Berkowitz, Aaron L. and Daniel Ansaric, ‘Generation of novel motor sequences: The neural
correlates of musical improvisation’, NeuroImage, 41, 2008, pp. 535–43.
Berry, Diane S. and James W. Pennebaker, ‘Nonverbal and Verbal Emotional Expression and
Health’, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 59, 1993, pp. 11–9.
60
Bourdieu, Pierre, ‘Cultural Reproduction and Social Reproduction’ in Knowledge, Education, and
Social Change: Papers in the Sociology of Education (London: Tavistock, 1973), pp. 71–112.
Bygren, Lars Olov, Boinkum Benson Konlaan and Sven-Erik Johansson, ‘Attendance at Cultural
Events, Reading Books or Periodicals, and Making Music or Singing in a Choir as Determinants
for Survival: Swedish Interview Survey of Living Conditions’, British Medical Journal, 313, 21–28
December 1996, pp. 1577–80.
Bygren, Lars Olov, Sven-Erik Johansson, Boinkum Benson Konlaan, Andrej M Grjibovski, Anna
V Wilkinson and Michael Sjöström, ‘Attending Cultural Events and Cancer Mortality: A Swedish
Cohort Study’, Arts & Health, 1, no. 1, March 2009, pp. 64–73.
Bygren, Lars Olov, Gösta Weissglas, Britt-Mai Wikström, Boinkum Benson Konlaan, Grjibovski,
Andrej Grijbovski, Ann-Birth Karlsson, Sven-Olov Andersson and Michael Sjöström, ‘Cultural
Participation and Health: A Randomized Controlled Trial Among Medical Care Staff’,
Psychosomatic Medicine, 71, 2009, pp. 469–73.
Bygren, Lars Olov, ‘Intergenerational Health Responses to Adverse and Enriched Environments’,
Annual Review of Public Health, 34, 2013, pp. 49–60.
Carnwath, John D. and Alan S. Brown, Understanding the Value and Impacts of Cultural
Experience: A Literature Review (London: Arts Council England, 2014).
CASE: The Culture and Sport Evidence Programme, Understanding the drivers, impact and value
of engagement in culture and sport: An over-arching summary of the research, July 2010.
CASE: The Culture and Sport Evidence Programme, Understanding the value of engagement in
culture and sport: Summary Report, July 2010.
Cassel, John, ‘The Contribution of the Social Environment to Host Resistance’, American Journal
of Epidemiology, Vol. 104, No. 2, 1976, pp. 107–23.
Cortese, Rene, Andrew Kwan, Emilie Lalonde, Olga Bryzgunova, et al, ‘Epigenetic markers of
prostate cancer in plasma circulating DNA’, Human Molecular Genetics, Vol. 21, No. 16, 2012, pp.
3619–31.
Cuypers, Koenraad Frans, Margunn Skjei Knudtsen, Maria Sandgrenc, Steinar Krokstad, Britt
Maj Wikström and Töres Theorell, ‘Cultural activities and public health: research in Norway and
Sweden: An overview’, Arts & Health, Vol. 3, No. 1, March 2011, pp. 6–26.
Cuypers, Koenraad, Karin De Ridder, Kirsti Kvaløy, Marguun Skjei Knudtsen, Steinar Krokstad,
Jostein Holmen and Turid Lingaas Holmen, ‘Leisure Time Activities in Adolescence in the
Presence of Susceptibility Genes for Obesity: Risk or Resilience against Overweight in
Adulthood?’ The HUNT Study’, BMC Public Health 12, no. 820, 2012.
Cuypers, Koenraad, Steinar Krokstad, Turid Lingaas Holmen, Margunn Skjei Knudtsen, Lars
Olov Bygren and Jostein Holmen, ‘Patterns of receptive and creative cultural activities and their
association with perceived health, anxiety, depression and satisfaction with life among adults: the
HUNT study, Norway’, Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 66, 2012, pp. 698–703.
61
De Manzano, Örjan, Simon Cervenka, Anke Karabanov, Lars Farde and Fredrik Ullén, ‘Thinking
Outside a Less Intact Box: Thalamic Dopamine D2 Receptor Densities Are Negatively Related to
Psychometric Creativity in Healthy Individuals’, PLOS one, May 2010, Vol. 5, No. 5, e10670.
De Manzano, Örjan, Simon Cervenka, Aurelija Jucaite, Oscar Hellenäs, Lars Farde and Fredrik
Ullén, ‘Individual differences in the proneness to have flow experiences are linked to dopamine
D2-receptor availability in the dorsal striatum’, NeuroImage, 67, 2013, pp. 1–6.
Director of Public Health, Arts & Health: Dudley Annual Report, 2013.
Eschleman, Kevin J., Jamie Madsen, Gene Alarcon and Alex Barelka, ‘Benefiting from
creative activity: The positive relationships between creative activity, recovery experiences,
and performance-related outcomes’, Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology,
September 2014, Vol. 87, Issue 3, pp. 579–98.
Fife, Alison, Pamela J. Beasley and Debra L. Fertig, ‘Psychoneuroimmunology and cancer:
Historical perspectives and current research’, Advances in Neuroimmunology, Vol. 6, 1996, pp.
179–90.
Flanagan, James M., Marta Munoz-Alegre, Stephen Henderson, Thomas Tang, et al, ‘Gene-body
hypermethylation of ATM in peripheral blood DNA of bilateral breast cancer patients’, Human
Molecular Genetics, Vol. 18, No. 7, 2009, pp. 1332–42.
Fujiwara, Daniel, Museums and happiness: the value of participating in museums and the arts,
commissioned by the Happy Museum, April 2013.
Fujiwara, Daniel, Laura Kudrna and Paul Dolan, Quantifying the Social Impacts of Culture and
Sport (London: Department of Culture, Media and Sport, 2014).
Hibberd, Carina, Joyce L.W. Yau and Jonathan R. Seckl, ‘Glucocorticoids and the ageing
hippocampus’, Journal of Anatomy, 197, 2000, pp. 553–62.
House, James S., Cynthia Robbins and Helen L. Metzner, ‘The Association of Social Relationships
and Activities with Mortality: Prospective Evidence from the Tecumseh Community Health Study’,
American Journal of Epidemiology, Vol. 116, No. 1, 1982, pp. 123–40.
Hyyppä, Markku T. and Juhani Mäki, ‘Why do Swedish-speaking Finns have longer active life? An
area for social capital research’, Health Promotion International, Vol. 16, No. 1, 2001, pp. 55–64.
Hyyppä, Markku T. and Juhani Mäki, ‘Individual-Level Relationships between Social Capital and
Self-Rated Health in a Bilingual Community’, Preventive Medicine, 32, 2001, pp. 148–55.
Hyyppä, Markku T. and Juhani Mäki, ‘Social participation and health in a community rich in stock
of social capital’, Health Education Research, Vol. 18, No. 6, 2003, pp. 770–9.
Hyyppä, Markku T., Juhani Mäki, Olli Impivaara and Arpo Aromaa, ‘Leisure-Based Participation
Predicts Survival: A Population-Based Study in Finland’, Health Promotion International, 21, no. 1,
2006, pp. 5–12.
62
Hyyppä, Markku T., Juhani Mäki, Olli Impivaara and Arpo Aromaa, ‘Individual-Level Measures
of Social Capital as Predictors of All-Cause and Cardiovascular Mortality: A Population-Based
Prospective Study of Men and Women in Finland’, European Journal of Epidemiology, 22, 2007,
pp. 589–97.
Hyyppä, Markku T., Healthy Ties, Social Capital, Population Health and Survival (Dordrecht:
Springer, 2010).
Inglis, Geoff and Joel Williams, Models of sporting and cultural activity: Analysis of the Taking Part
Survey, August 2010.
Irie, Masahiro, Shinya Asami, Shoji Nagata, Masato Ikeda, Masakazu Miyata and Hiroshi Kasai,
‘Psychosocial Factors as a Potential Trigger of Oxidative DNA Damage in Human Leukocytes’,
Japanese Journal of Cancer Research, 92, March 2001, pp. 367–76.
Irie, Masahiro, Shinya Asami, Shoji Nagata, Masato Ikeda, Masakazu Miyata and Hiroshi Kasai,
‘Psychosocial Mediation of a Type of Oxidative DNA Damage, 8-Hydroxydeoxyguanosine, in
Peripheral Blood Leucocytes of Non-Smoking and Non-Drinking Workers’, Psychotherapy and
Psychosomatics, 71, 2002, pp. 90–6.
Ishimura, I. and M.Kodama, ‘Dimensions of flow experience in Japanese college students:
relation between flow experience and mental health’, Journal of Health Psychology, 13, 2006, pp.
23–34.
Iwasaki, Yoshitaka, Roger C. Mannell, Bryan J.A. Smale and Janice Butcher, ‘Contributions of
Leisure Participation in Predicting Stress Coping and Health among Police and Emergency
Response Services Workers’, Journal of Health Psychology, Vol. 10, No. 1, 2005, pp. 79–99.
Johansson, Sven-Erik, Boinkum Benson Konlaan and Lars Olov Bygren, ‘Sustaining habits of
attending cultural events and maintenance of health’, Health Promotion International, 16, no. 3,
2001, pp. 229–34.
Kaplan, George A., Jukka T. Salonen, Richard D. Cohen, Richard J. Brand, S. Leonard Syme
and Pekka Puska, ‘Social Connections and Mortality from all Causes and from Cardiovascular
Disease: Prospective Evidence from Eastern Finland’, American Journal of Epidemiology, Vol.
128, No. 2, 1988, pp. 370–80.
Kato, Chieko, Arturas Petronis, Yuji Okazaki, Mamoru Tochigi et al, ‘Molecular genetic studies of
schizophrenia: challenges and insights’, Neuroscience Research, 43, 2002, pp. 295–304.
Kiecolt-Glaser, J.K. and R. Glaser, ‘Psychoneuroimmunology and Cancer: Fact or Fiction?’,
European Journal of Cancer, Vol. 35, No. 11, 1999, pp. 1603–7.
Knezovich, J.G. and M Ramsay, ‘The effect of preconception paternal alcohol exposure on
epigenetic remodeling of the H19 and Rasgrf1 imprinting control regions in mouse off-spring’,
Frontiers in Genetics, 2012, 3, pp. 10–15.
Konlaan, Boinkum Benson, Lars Olov Bygren and Sven-Erik Johansson, ‘Visiting the Cinema,
Concerts, Museums or Art Exhibitions as Determinant of Survival: A Swedish Fourteen-Year
Cohort Follow-Up’, Scandinavian Journal of Public Health, 28, 2000, pp. 174–8.
63
Konlaan, Boinkum Benson, N Björby, Lars Olov Bygren, Gösta Weissglas, Lena Karlsson,
Maria Widmark, ‘Attendance at cultural events and physical exercise and health: a randomized
controlled study’, Public Health, Vol. 114, No. 5, Sept 2000, pp. 316–9.
Koskinen, Jussi Vahtera, Anne Kouvonen and Mika Kivimäki, ‘Engagement in Cultural Activities
and Cause-Specific Mortality: Prospective Cohort Study’, Preventive Medicine, 49, 2009, pp.
142–7.
Kouvonen, Anne, Judy Anne Swift, Mia Stafford, Tom Cox, Jussi Vahtera, Ari Väänänen,
Heponiemi, Tarja, Roberto De Vogli, Amanda Griffiths and Mika Kivimäki, ‘Social Participation
and Maintaining Recommended Waist Circumference: Prospective Evidence From the English
Longitudinal Study of Aging’, Journal of Aging and Health, 24, no. 2, 2012, pp. 250–68.
Krokstad, Steinar, Arnulf Langhammer, K. Hveem, TL. Holmen, K. Midthjell, TR. Stene, G.
Bratberg, Jon Heggland and Jostein Holmen ‘Cohort Profile: The HUNT Study, Norway’,
International Journal of Epidemiology, 42, 2013, pp. 968–77.
Kuzumaki, Naoko, Daigo Ikegami, Rie Tamura, Nana Hareyama, et al, ‘Hippocampal
Epigenetic Modification at the Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor Gene Induced by an Enriched
Environment’, Hippocampus, 21, 2011, pp. 127–32.
Lajunen, Hanna-Reetta, Anna Keski-Rahkonen, Lea Pulkkinen, Richard J Rose, Aila
Rissanen and Jaakko Kaprio, ‘Leisure Activity Patterns and Their Associations with Overweight:
A Prospective Study among Adolescents’, Journal of Adolescence, 32, no. 5, October 2009, pp.
1089–1103.
Langhammer, Arnulf, Steinar Krokstad, Pål Romundstad, Jon Heggland and Jostein Holmen, ‘The
HUNT study: participation is associated with survival and depends on socioeconomic status,
diseases and symptoms’, BMC Medical Research Methodology, 2012, 12, 143.
Leadbetter, Clare and Niamh O’Connor, Healthy Attendance? The Impact of Cultural Engagement
and Sports Participation on Health and Satisfaction with Life in Scotland (Edinburgh: Scottish
Government, 2013).
Lennartsson, Carin and Merril Silverstein, ‘Does Engagement With Life Enhance Survival of
Elderly People in Sweden? The Role of Social and Leisure Activities’, Journal of Gerontology, 56B,
no. 6, 2001, pp. S335–42.
Liikanen, Hanna-Liisa, Art and Culture for Well-being (Helsinki: Publications of the Ministry of
Education and Culture, Finland 2010).
Lin, Nan, ‘Inequality in Social Capital’, Contemporary Sociology, Vol. 29, No. 6, Nov 2000, pp.
785–95.
McCraty, Rollin, Mike Atkinson, Glen Rein and Alan D. Watkins, ‘Music Enhances the Effect of
Positive Emotional States on Salivary IgA’, Stress Medicine, Vol. 12, 1996, pp. 167–75.
Mill, Jonathan and Arturas Petronis, ‘Molecular studies of major depressive disorder: the
epigenetic perspective’, Molecular Psychiatry, 12, 2007, 799–814.
64
Mill, Jonathan and Arturas Petronis, ‘Pre- and peri-natal environmental risks for attention-
deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): the potential role of epigenetic processes in mediating
susceptibility’, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Vol. 49, No. 10, 2008, pp. 1020–30.
Mill, Jonathan, Thomas Tang, Zachary Kaminsky, Tarang Khare, et al, ‘Epigenomic Profiling
Reveals DNA-Methylation Changes Associated with Major Psychosis’, The American Journal of
Human Genetics, 82, March 2008, pp. 696–711.
Mychasiuka, Richelle, Saif Zahira, Nichole Schmoldb, Slava Ilnytskyyc, et al, ‘Parental enrichment
and offspring development: Modifications to brain, behavior and the epigenome’, Behavioural
Brain Research, 228, 2012, pp. 294–8.
Nieminen, Tarja, Ritva Prättälä, Tuija Martelin, Tommi Härkänen, Markku T Hyyppä, Erkki Alanen
and Seppo Koskinen, ‘Social capital, health behaviours and health: a population-based
associational study’, BMC Public Health, 13, 613, 2013.
Núñez, María J., Paula Mañá, David Liñares, María P. Riveiro, José Balboa, Juan Suárez-
Quintanilla, Mónica Maracchi, Manuel Rey Méndez, José M. López and Manuel Freire-Garabal,
‘Music, immunity and cancer’, Life Sciences, 71, 2002, pp. 1047–57.
Olsson, Tommy, Abdul K. Mohammed, Lucy F. Donaldson and Jonathan R. Seckl, ‘Transcription
factor AP-2 gene expression in adult rat hippocampal regions: effects of environmental
manipulations’, Neuroscience Letters, 189, 1995, pp. 113–6.
Orth-Gomér, Kristina and Jeffrey V. Johnson, ‘Social Network Interaction and Mortality: A Six-Year
Follow-Up Study of a Random Sample of the Swedish Population’, Journal of Chronic Disease,
Vol. 40, No. 10, 1987, pp. 949–57.
Pembrey, M.E., Lars-Olov Bygren, G. Kaati, S. Edvinsson, K. Northstone, et al, ‘Sex-specific,
male-line transgenerational responses in humans’, European Journal of Human Genetics, 14,
2006, pp. 159–66
Petrie, Keith J., Iris Fontanilla, Mark G. Thomas, Roger J. Booth and James W. Pennebaker, ‘Effect
of Written Emotional Expression on Immune Function in Patients With Human Immunodeficiency
Virus Infection: A Randomized Trial’, Psychosomatic Medicine, 66, 2004, pp. 272–5.
Petronis, Arturas, ‘Epigenetics and Bipolar Disorder: New Opportunities and Challenges’,
American Journal of Medical Genetics, 123C, 2003, pp. 65–75.
Petronis, Arturas, ‘The Origin of Schizophrenia: Genetic Thesis, Epigenetic Antithesis, and
Resolving Synthesis’, Biological Psychiatry, 55, 2004, pp. 965–70.
Putnam, Robert, Bowling Alone: The Collapse and Revival of American Community
(London: Simon & Schuster, 2001).
Power, Chris, Diana Kuh and Susan Morton, ‘From Developmental Origins of Adult Disease to Life
Course Research on Adult Disease and Aging: Insights from Birth Cohort Studies’, Annual Review
of Public Health, 34, 2013, pp. 7–28.
65
Rasmuson, Sigbritt, Tommy Olsson, Bengt G. Henriksson, Paul A.T. Kelly, Megan C. Holmes,
Jonathan R. Seckl and Abdul H. Mohammed, ‘Environmental enrichment selectively increases
5-HT1A receptor mRNA expression and binding in the rat hippocampus’, Molecular Brain
Research, 53, 1998, pp. 285–90.
Särkämö, Teppo, Mari Tervaniemi, Sari Laitinen, Ava Numminen, Merja Kurki, Julene K. Johnson
and Pekka Rantanen, ‘Cognitive, Emotional, and Social Benefits of Regular Musical Activities in
Early Dementia: Randomized Controlled Study’, The Gerontologist, 2014, pp. 634–50.
Schoenbach, Victor J., Lisa Fredman and David G. Kleinbaum, ‘Social Ties and Mortality in Evans
County, Georgia’, American Journal of Epidemiology, Vol. 123, No. 4, 1986, pp. 577–91.
Seeman, Teresa E., ‘Social Ties and Health: The Benefits of Social Integration’, Annals of
Epidemiology, Vol. 6, No. 5, September 1996, pp. 442–51.
Skrabski, Á., M. Kopp and I. Kawachi, ‘Social capital in a changing society: cross sectional
associations with middle aged female and male mortality rates’, Journal of Epidemiology and
Community Health, 57, 2003, pp. 114–9.
Stickley, Theo (ed), Qualitative Research in Arts and Mental Health (Ross-on-Wye: PCCS Books,
2012).
Sundquist, Kristina, Martin Lindström, Marianne Malmström, Sven-Erik Johansson and Jan
Sundquist, ‘Social Participation and Coronary Heart Disease: A Follow-up Study of 6900 Women
and Men in Sweden’, Social Science & Medicine, 58, 2004, pp. 615–22.
Szreter, Simon and Michael Woolcock, ‘Health by association? Social capital, social theory,
and the political economy of public health’, International Journal of Epidemiology, 33, 2004, pp.
650–67.
Swedish National Institute of Public Health, Kultur för hälsa [Culture for Health], (Stockholm:
Swedish National Institute of Public Health, 2005).
Theorell, Töres, Walter Osika, Constanze Leineweber, Linda L. Magnusson Hanson, Eva Bojner
Horwitz and Hugo Westerlund, ‘Is cultural activity at work related to mental health in employees?’,
International Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health, 86, 2013, pp. 281–8.
TNS BMRB, Taking Part Survey 2012/13 Longitudinal Development Report, 2012.
Väänänen, Ari, Michael Murray, Aki Koskinen, Jussi Vahtera, Anne Kouvonen and Mika Kivimäki,
‘Engagement in Cultural Activities and Cause-Specific Mortality: Prospective Cohort Study’,
Preventive Medicine, 49, 2009, pp. 142–7.
Varah, Iain and Mark Taylor, Chief Culture and Leisure Officers Association, The role of culture and
leisure in improving health and wellbeing, 2014.
Wang, Hui-Xin, Anita Karp, Bengt Winblad and Laura Fratiglioni, ‘Late-Life Engagement in Social
and Leisure Activities Is Associated with a Decreased Risk of Dementia: A Longitudinal Study
from the Kungsholmen Project’, American Journal of Epidemiology, 155, no. 12, 2002, pp. 1081–7.
Watkins, A.D., ‘Perceptions, emotions and immunity: an integrated homoeostatic network’,
Quarterly Journal of Medicine, 88, 1995, pp. 283–94.
66
Welin, Lennart, Kurt Svärdsudd, S. Ander-Peciva, Gösta Tibblin, Bodil Tibblin, Bo Larsson and L.
Wilhemsen, ‘Prospective Study of Social Influences on Mortality: The Study of Men Born in 1913
and 1923’, The Lancet, Vol. 325, No. 8434, 20 April 1985, pp. 915–8.
Welin, Lennart, Bo Larsson, Kurt Svardsudd, Bodil Tibblin and Gösta Tibblin, ‘Social Network
and Activities in Relation to Mortality from Cardiovascular Diseases, Cancer and Other Causes:
A 12 Year Follow up of the Study of Men Born in 1913 and 1923’, Journal of Epidemiology and
Community Health, 46, 1992, pp. 127–32.
Wikström, Britt-Mai, Töres Theorell, S. Sandström, ‘Medical Health and Emotional Effects of
Art Stimulation in Old Age: A Controlled Intervention Study concerning the Effects of Visual
Stimulation Provided in the Form of Pictures’, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Vol. 60, Nos.
3-4, 1993, pp. 195–206.
Wilkinson, Anna V., Andrew J. Waters, Lars Olov Bygren and Alvin R. Tarlov, ‘Are Variations in
Rates of Attending Cultural Activities Associated with Population Health in the United States?’,
BMC Public Health, 31 August 2007.
Windsor, Joy, Your health and the arts: a study of the association between arts engagement and
health (London: Arts Council England, 2005).
Zimmermann, Aurelia, Markus Stauffacher, Wolfgang Langhans and Hanno Würbel, ‘Enrichment-
dependent differences in novelty exploration in rats can be explained by habituation’, Behavioural
Brain Research, 121, 2001, pp. 11–20.
67
Authors, Year,
Country
Dataset,
Population
Sample Size,
Age
Outcome
Measure Confounders
Cultural
Activities Results
Welin et al,
1992, Sweden
Men born in
Gothenburg in
1913 (selected
in 1963 and
1973), n = 769
60-year-olds; 220
50-year-olds
Mortality from
cardio-vascular
diseases, cancer
and other causes
to 1985
Smoking, alcohol
consumption,
previous stroke
or heart attack,
marital status,
household size,
income
Reading, cinema,
theatre, concerts,
museums/
galleries
Middle-aged men with a good
‘social network’ may be partly
protected against non-cancer
mortality
Bygren, Konlaan
and Johansson,
1996, Sweden
Swedish Survey
of Living
Conditions
1982–3, n =
15,198 (12,675)
participants aged
16–74 years
Survival to 31
December 1991
Age, gender,
education level,
income, long-term
disease, social
network, smoking,
physical exercise
Cinema, theatre,
concerts, live
music, art/other
exhibitions,
museums,
reading, music-
making, singing
in a choir
Attending cultural events
at least once a week has a
positive effect upon survival
Konlaan, Bygren
and Johansson,
2000, Sweden
Swedish Survey
of Living
Conditions 1982–
3, n = 10,609
aged 25–74
Survival to 31
December 1996
Age, gender, cash
buffer, educational
level, long-term
disease, smoking,
physical exercise
Cinema, theatre,
concerts, live
music, art
exhibitions,
museums,
music-making,
reading
‘Attendance at cultural events
may have a beneficial effect on
longevity’ (p. 174)
Johansson,
Konlaan and
Bygren, 2001,
Sweden
Swedish Survey
of Living
Conditions 1982–
3 and 1990–1,
n = 3,793 aged
25–74
Self-reported
health
Baseline health
status, type
of residence,
geographical
region of domicile,
socio-economic
status (level of
education)
Baseline health
status, type
of residence,
geographical
region of
domicile,
socio-economic
status (level of
education)
‘Those who became culturally
less active between the first
and second occasion, or those
who were culturally inactive
on both occasions, ran a
65% excess risk of impaired
perceived health compared
with those who were culturally
active on both occasions’ (p.
229)
Bygren et al,
2009, Sweden
Swedish Survey
of Living
Conditions 1990–
1, n = 9,011
aged 25–74
Cancer incidence
in Swedish public
death register
to 31 December
2003
Age, gender,
chronic conditions,
disposable
income,
educational
attainment,
smoking status,
leisure time
physical activity,
urban/non-urban
residency
Cinema, theatre,
live music, art
gallery, museum
Rare and moderate cultural
attendees were 3.23 and 2.92
(respectively) times more likely
to die of cancer than regular
attendees in urban areas
Lennartsson
and Silverstein,
2001, Sweden
Swedish Panel
Study of Living
Conditions
of the Oldest
Old 1992 n =
537 (463 non-
institutionalised)
aged 75+
Survival to 1996 Age, gender,
educational
level, functional
impairment,
presence of heart
or circulatory
problems, tobacco
use
Cinema, cultural
events, reading
books or
newspapers,
hobbies
Solitary–active participation
(e.g. gardening, hobbies)
reduce mortality risk,
particularly in men
Illustrative Table
68
Authors, Year,
Country
Dataset,
Population
Sample Size,
Age
Outcome
Measure Confounders
Cultural
Activities Results
Wang et al,
2002, Sweden
Kungsholmen
Project 1987–9,
n = 1,810 aged
75+
Onset of
dementia
between first
follow-up
(1991–3), and
second follow-up
(1994–6)
Age, gender,
education,
cognitive
functioning,
comorbidity,
depressive
symptoms,
physical
functioning at
baseline
Theatre,
concerts, art
exhibitions
(social), painting,
drawing (mental),
sewing, knitting,
crocheting,
weaving
(productive)
‘Engagement in mental, social,
or productive activities was
inversely related to dementia
incidence’ (p. 1081)
Sundquist et al,
2004, Sweden
Swedish Annual
Level-of-Living
Survey 1990–1, n
= 6,861, 35–74
years
Coronary heart
disease morbidity
or mortality to 31
December 2000
Socio-economic
and educational
status, housing
tenure, smoking,
age, gender,
marital status,
geographical
region
Cinema, theatre,
concerts, art
exhibitions and
museums, choir
An association found between
low social participation
and increased incidence
of coronary heart disease
morbidity and mortality.
Attendance at the cinema,
theatre, concerts, art
exhibitions and museums had
(by far, in most cases) the
most significance within the
social participation index
Hyyppä, Mäki,
Impivaara and
Aromaa, 2006,
Finland
Mini-Finland
Health Survey
1978–80, n =
5,087, 30–59
years
Survival during
20 years of
follow-up (first
three years
excluded)
Residential
stability, socio-
economic status,
marital status and
relations, trusting
relationships,
alcohol
consumption,
smoking; mental
health, self-
reported chronic
diseases or
disabilities, self-
rated overall health
Theatre, cinema,
concerts, art
exhibitions,
reading, listening
to music,
drama, singing,
photography,
painting and
handicraft
‘Leisure participation predicts
survival in middle-aged
Finnish men and its effect is
independent of demographic
features, of health status and
of several other health-related
factors’ (p. 5)
Hyyppä, Mäki,
Impivaara and
Aromaa, 2007,
Finland
Mini-Finland
Health Survey
1978–80, n =
7,217, 30–99
years
Survival during
24 years of
follow-up (first
five excluded)
with attention to
all-cause and
cardiovascular
mortality
(including
strokes) up to
November 2004
Residential
stability, socio-
economic status,
marital status and
relations, trusting
relationships,
alcohol
consumption,
smoking; mental
health, self-
reported chronic
diseases or
disabilities, self-
rated overall health
Theatre, cinema,
concerts, art
exhibitions,
reading, listening
to music,
drama, singing,
photography,
painting and
handicraft
Leisure participation is
associated with reduced
all-cause mortality in women
and men (related to economic
status in the latter case)
69
Authors, Year,
Country
Dataset,
Population
Sample Size,
Age
Outcome
Measure Confounders
Cultural
Activities Results
Agahi and
Parker, 2008
Swedish Annual
Level-of-Living
Survey 1990–1
and Swedish
Panel Study of
Living Conditions
of the Oldest Old
1992, n = 1,246
men and women
aged 65 to 95
Survival to 31
December 2003
A range of
symptoms
and diseases,
functional status,
age, gender,
educational level
(as a measure of
socio-economic
position), smoking,
alcohol, body
mass index
Reading books,
hobby activities
(e.g. knitting,
sewing, carpentry
or painting),
cultural activities
(going to the
cinema, theatre,
concerts,
museums or
exhibitions),
dancing,
playing musical
instruments, and
choir singing
Women demonstrated a dose-
response relationship between
overall participation and
survival; men did not. ‘Gender-
specific analyses revealed
that participation in cultural
activities was the only activity
that was significantly related
to survival in both men and
women’ (p. 865)
Lajunen et al,
2009, Finland
FinnTwin12 study
all twins born in
Finland 1983–7
n = 5,184 twins
aged 11–12
years
Becoming
overweight
during follow-
up at 14 and 17
years
Pubertal timing,
socio-economic
status of family
Television and
video viewing,
computer
games, listening
to music,
playing musical
instruments,
reading, arts
(drawing
or painting,
handicrafts,
woodwork,
building scale
models)
Engagement in the arts in
boys was detrimental to the
maintenance of recommended
weights. Among girls, few
individual leisure activities
predicted becoming
overweight. However, the
‘passive and solitary’ cluster
carried the greatest risk of
becoming overweight in late
adolescence
Väänänen et al,
2009, Finland
Still Working
survey
(conducted by
Finnish Institute
of Occupational
Health) 1986, n
= 7,922, working
age
Survival 1986–
2004
Socio-
demographic
factors, socio-
economic status,
work stress, social
characteristics,
diabetes,
hypertension
High cultural engagement
independently associated with
decreased all-cause mortality
and external causes of death
(with solitary activities related
to the former and socially
shared cultural activities to the
latter)
Kouvonen et al,
2012, UK
English
Longitudinal
Study of Ageing
waves 2 and 4, n
= 4,280 age 50+
Waist
circumference at
follow-up
Gender, age,
ethnicity,
marital status,
total wealth,
longstanding
limiting illness,
depressive
symptoms,
smoking status
and physical
activity
Arts or music
group
No association was found
between social participation
and waistline measurement
in women. Men with an initial
waist measurement in the
recommended range who
participated in education, arts
or music groups or evening
classes and in charitable
associations were more
likely to maintain their waist
circumference
Cuypers et al,
2012, Norway
HUNT Study
1995–7, n =
8,408 13–19
years, followed
up 2006–8, n
= 1,450 24–30
years
Obesity (body
mass index, waist
circumference,
waist-hip ratio
and natural
development of
the body over the
life course)
Physical activity,
socio-economic
status, pubertal
timing and genetic
proclivity to
obesity
Reading a book,
listening to or
playing music,
doing homework,
watching
television
Participation in cultural
activities guarded girls
against being overweight.
This was amplified when
considering those who were
at the recommended weight at
baseline and when television
was excluded as an activity
© Rebecca Gordon-Nesbitt and Arts for Health, 2015
http://longitudinalhealthbenefits.wordpress.com
www.artsforhealth.org
ARTS FOR HEALTH
www.miriad.mmu.ac.uk
www.art.mmu.ac.uk www.ahrc.ac.uk
... Psychological explanations offered for the positive effects of cultural engagement and leisure on physical and mental health and cognition have included: affectenhancement, stress-reduction, and social contact (Fancourt & Steptoe, 2018); needs gratification, arousal, communication, and enriched environment (Konlaan, Bygren, & Johansson, 2000); recovery, autonomy, mastery, meaning and affiliation (Newman, Tay, & Diener, 2014); and preparation for future events (Bygren, Konlaan, & Johansson, 1996). In a review of arts engagement and health-related research, Gordon-Nesbitt (2015) categorized the principal explanations as involving effects of increased social capital, improved cognition, protection from strain, and enriched environment. ...
... Relevant empirical studies have usually investigated participatory art activities rather than receptive art activities or have not distinguished between the two types. When receptive activities have been studied, they have usually been studied as a single entity or restricted to attendance at cultural events (e.g., theater, concert, opera, cinema; see Fancourt & Steptoe, 2018;Gordon-Nesbitt, 2015). Studies have found positive effects of cultural engagement on a range of outcomes, including prosociality ( Van de Vyver & Abrams, 2018), cognitive function (Fancourt & Steptoe, 2018), self-reported health (Johansson, Konlaan, & Bygren, 2001), physiology (Konlaan, Björby, et al., 2000), stress reduction (Clow & Fredhoi, 2006), protection against obesity (Cuypers et al., 2012) and dementia (Wang, Karp, Winblad, & Fratiglioni, 2002), cancerrelated mortality (Bygren et al., 2009), coronary-related morbidity and mortality (Sundquist, Lindström, Malmström, Johansson, & Sundquist, 2004), and survival (Bygren et al., 1996;Väänänen et al., 2009). ...
... The aforementioned four studies of cultural engagement and well-being were cross-sectional in design, but longitudinal studies of cultural engagement do exist for other types of outcome variable (e.g., prosociality- Van de Vyver & Abrams, 2018). Gordon-Nesbitt (2015) identified 14 studies stemming from Scandinavia that demonstrate the longitudinal impact of cultural participation and cultural attendance on health outcomes (including some psychological outcomes) in nonclinical settings over long periods of This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. ...
Article
Full-text available
Evidence about the impact of art on well-being is confined to studies of participatory arts and receptive arts that involve attending cultural events. This investigation examined the impact of art on well-being by framing people's engagement with art as encounters with artistic imagination. These encounters include traditional forms of cultural activity, such as a gallery or theater visit, but also encompass everyday activities, such as watching a screen drama or reading fiction. Three studies examined how such encounters affect emotional well-being, life satisfaction, meaning in life, and mental well-being. A survey study (N = 544) found that participants on average spent over 4 hr engaged with art the previous day. This study and an experience-sampling study (N = 50), in which participants completed a questionnaire via their smartphones twice daily for 10 days (854 responses), revealed that individuals' variety of encounters with art and accompanying elevating emotional experiences were associated with well-being. Live arts engagement was positively associated with all aspects of well-being, and visual and literary arts with greater meaning in life, whereas screen arts, audio arts, and sports spectating (for comparison) were not positively associated. A third study using (live) arts attendance and well-being data (n = 27,918) from 2 waves (3-year interval) of a large longitudinal panel survey showed that frequency of attendance predicted subsequent well-being, whereas arts participation did not. Overall, the evidence indicates that encounters with artistic imagination contribute to people's well-being, with effects varying according to the art form and the type of well-being assessed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).
... This section examines data from 12 qualitative studies of singing that were The qualitative studies included in the review point toward positive, personal, and social experiences associated with singing. This is the case for healthy adults who take part in open access group singing (Bailey & Davidson, 2002;Baker & Ballantyne, 2013;Creech et al., 2014;Joseph & Southcott, 2014a, 2014b, 2015Judd & Pooley, 2014;Lally, 2009;Li & Southcott, 2012;Skingley, Martin, & Clift, 2015) and for more clinically oriented interventions targeted at specific conditions, including stress in pregnancy (Carolan, Barry, Gamble, Turner, & Mascarenas, 2012) and stroke (Tamplin, Baker, Jones, Way, & Lee, 2013). ...
... By drawing attention to biological, psychological, and social processes involved in singing, this chapter has highlighted the need for interdisciplinary research to understand the connections between these domains. A potential challenge for researchers in the future is to map and synthesize the plethora of theories that, implicitly or explicitly, underpin research on singing and wellbeing (Fancourt et al., 2014;Fitzgerald & Callard, 2015;Gordon-Nesbitt, 2015). This requires a critical, flexible, and dynamic understanding of the wellbeing impacts of singing. ...
... Creative arts in this context may have a significant role to play because of the increasingly understood link with emotional expression, personal growth and nurturing of healthy interrelationships (e.g. Gordon-Nesbitt, 2015;Gordon-Nesbitt, 2017). ...
... Bulmer recommend that emotional intelligence concepts are explicitly included in nurse-training curricula but they acknowledge unanswered questions relating to how it might be conceptualized, taught and measured. Here, the arts may play a significant role as evidence provides a better understanding of the link between arts engagement, emotional expression and fostering compassion and resilience (Clift, Morrison, Vella-Burrows, & Hancox, 2011;Crossick & Kaszynska, 2016;Daykin, 2017;Gordon-Nesbitt, 2015). ...
Article
In an era of global environmental deterioration and income inequity, public health faces many challenges, including the growing number of individuals, especially older people, with chronic diseases. Dementia is increasingly being seen not just as a biomedical problem to solve but as a public and community challenge to address more broadly. Concepts like prevention, brain health, and quality of life/well-being are receiving more attention. The engagement of community in addressing these challenges is being seen as critical to successful social adaptation. Arts programs are reinvigorating cultural responses to the growing number of older people with cognitive challenges. The humanities offer ways of understanding the power of words and stories in public discourse and a critical lens though which to view political and economic influences. In this paper, we report on a panel held in London on the occasion of the conference at the Royal Society for Public Health in March, 2017, in which the authors presented. Key issues discussed included problem framing, the nature of evidence, the politics of power and influence, and the development of effective interventions. In this paper, we review the rejection of two policies, one on dementia and one on the arts and humanities in public health, by the American Public Health Association; the emergence of policies in the UK; and some of the state of the art practices, particularly in training, again focusing on the UK.
... With its potential to help individuals express themselves, gain coping skills, improve interpersonal skills, resolve conflicts and problems, reduce stress, manage behaviour, increase self-esteem and self-confidence (Davies et al., 2016;Davies and Clift, 2022;Mollaoglu and Yanmis, 2022;Shukla et al., 2022), arts engagement is proposed as a non-pharmacological therapeutic approach with significant effect in alleviating chronic stress and depression and in providing emotional, cognitive and social coping resources that support biological regulatory systems (Beerse et al., 2020). Moreover, it also has a positive impact on social capital by helping people in reducing loneliness (Gordon-Nesbitt, 2015;Roe et al., 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
In recent years, arts engagement has been proposed as a non-pharmacological approach to reduce cognitive decline and increase well-being and quality of life in specific populations such as the elderly or patients with severe disease. The aim of this systematic review was to assess the effects of receptive or active arts engagement on reducing cognitive decline and improving quality of life and well-being in healthy populations, with a particular focus on the role of arts engagement in the long term. A comprehensive search strategy was conducted across four databases from February to March 2023. Ten studies with a total of 7,874 participants were incorporated in accordance with the PRISMA guidelines. Active and receptive arts engagement was found to be an effective approach to reduce cognitive decline and improve well-being and quality of life in healthy populations. The role of the positive effects of arts engagement could be determined by the combination of several factors such as exposure to cultural activities and the group effect. There is limited evidence of the protective effects of active arts engagement over a long period of time. Given the increasing demand for preventive programmes to reduce the negative effects of population ageing, more research on arts engagement should be conducted to identify its mechanisms and long-term effects.
... As A Social Glue discussed, an evidence review centred on the Nordic countries suggests that cultural attendance paves the way to longer lives better lived. 9 Acknowledging that 'access to health care only accounts for around 10% of a population's health, with the rest being shaped by socio-economic factors', 10 the GM ICP will transform population health, 11 focusing investment on tackling the wider determinants of health. 12 In 2020, the Institute of Health Equity (IHE), led by Professor Sir Michael Marmot, published an update on the 2010 Marmot Review of health inequalities in England, which included a parallel report dedicated to GM. 13 The IHE followed this with a detailed analysis of how GM could become a Marmot city region by tackling inequalities across the life course, published as Build Back Fairer in Greater Manchester: Health Equity and Dignified Lives. ...
... pidemiologisissa tutkimuksissa on saatu positiivisia tuloksia yhteyksistä kulttuurisen osallistumisen, kulttuurin ja taiteen harrastamisen sekä eliniän odotteen, koetun terveyden ja elämään tyytyväisyyden välillä. Tutkimuksissa ei kuitenkaan ole voitu osoittaa, että juuri taiteeseen ja kulttuuriin osallistuminen tuottaa havaitut terveysvaikutukset.(Gordon-Nesbitt 2015;Laitinen 2017a; 2017d, 29-30;Leadbetter & O'Connor 2013.) McCarthy ym. (2004) ovat todenneet, että iso osa taiteeseen ja kulttuuriin liitetyistä hyödyistä on saavutettavissa vain pidempiaikaisen harrastuneisuuden kautta. He nostavat tällaisen pidempiaikaisen sitoutumisen aikaansaamisessa avainasemaan lasten ja nuorten varhaisen tutustutt ...
Technical Report
Full-text available
Suomessa valtion lisäksi kunnilla on tärkeä rooli kulttuuripolitiikassa. Kunnat voivat kuitenkin pääosin toteuttaa kulttuuripalvelut haluamallaan laajuudella ja tavalla. Vaikka taiteen ja kulttuurin yhteiskunnallinen merkitys on tunnustettu laajalti ovat julkisen talouden ongelmat luoneet painetta etsiä säästökohteita myös kulttuurialalta. Samalla monet yhteiskunnalliset muutostrendit, kuten globalisaatiokehitys, etninen ja kulttuurinen monimuotoistuminen, tieto- ja informaatioteknologinen kehitys, väestön ikääntyminen sekä muutokset vapaa-ajan vieton ja yhteiskunnallisen osallistumisen tavoissa pakottavat miettimään, kuinka taide- ja kulttuurikentällä vastataan näihin haasteisiin; millaisia uusia tapoja järjestää ja välittää kulttuuripalveluja tarvitaan tai minkälaista taidetta ja kulttuuria julkisen vallan tulee rahoittaa. Kulttuuripolitiikan eri tasoilla ja tahoilla tarvitaan monenlaista kulttuuri- ja taidetoimialaan liittyvää tietoa päätöksenteon tueksi. Viime aikoina kulttuurin vaikutuksista talouteen ja hyvinvointiin on tullut yhä keskeisempi osa kulttuuripoliittista keskustelua ja taiteen ja kulttuurin rahoituksen perustelua. Taide ja kulttuuri on nähty muun muassa keinona säästää sosiaali- ja terveyspalveluiden resursseja. Tulevaisuudessa kulttuurihyvinvointiin liittyvien kysymysten ohella tiedontarvetta kunnissa kasvattaa kulttuuritoimialan roolin vahvistuminen kunnan toiminnassa sote- ja maakuntauudistuksen seurauksena. Kulttuurin ja taiteen vaikutuksia on tutkittu monilla eri tieteenaloilla ja eri näkökulmista. Tutkimuksissa on raportoitu erilaisia taiteen ja kulttuurin yksilökohtaisia, yhteisöihin tai organisaatioihin kohdistuvia sekä yhteiskunnallisia hyötyjä, jotka voivat liittyä terveyteen ja hyvinvointiin, yhteisöllisyyteen ja oppimiseen, talouteen tai ekologisesti, sosiaalisesti ja kulttuurisesti kestävään kehitykseen. Vaikka taiteen ja kulttuurin yhteiskunnalliset ja yksilökohtaiset vaikutukset hyväksytään yleisesti, niiden todentaminen on osoittautunut hankalaksi. Tässä tutkimuksessa tarjotaan tietoa Itä-Suomen kunnille alueen kulttuurisektorin kehittämisen tueksi. Tällaista tietoa ei ole aiemmin ollut saatavilla Itä-Suomesta. Raportista selviää, mitä mieltä Itä-Suomen asukkaat ovat kuntansa kulttuuripalveluista, kuinka he suhtautuvat kulttuurin ja sen vaikutuksiin sekä millä tavoin he haluaisivat olla vaikuttamassa tulevaisuuden kulttuuripalveluiden kehittämiseen. Tutkimus perustuu Itä-Suomen alueella syyskuussa 2021 toteutettuun kyselyyn, johon vastasi kaikkiaan 1524 alueen asukasta. Maakunnittain vastaajat jakautuivat seuraavasti: Etelä-Savo (270 vastaajaa), Etelä-Karjala (271 vastaajaa), Pohjois-Savo (602 vastaajaa) ja Pohjois-Karjala (381 vastaajaa). Kysely välitettiin Suomen Onlinetutkimus Oy:n kuluttajapaneelin Itä-Suomessa asuville jäsenille. Tulosten perusteella maakuntien välillä ei ole suuria eroja. Itä-Suomen asukkaat pitävät nykyistä kaupunkinsa kulttuuritarjontaa keskimäärin melko tärkeänä ja ovat siihen tyytyväisiä. Erityisen tärkeitä ovat helposti lähestyttävät palvelut, kuten kirjastot, elokuvateatterit ja kansalaisopistot. Covid19-pandemia näkyy kulttuurin kuluttamisessa. Käynnit kulttuurikohteissa ja -tilaisuuksissa ovat vähentyneet pandemian seurauksena todella paljon. Yleinen asenneilmapiiri kulttuuria kohtaan on myönteinen. Taidetta ja kulttuuria pidetään sekä itselle että kaikille muillekin kuuluvana asiana. Monet kokevat tärkeäksi, että suomalaisilla on asuinpaikasta ja omista kyvyistä riippumatta tasa-arvoinen mahdollisuus harrastaa kulttuuria. Kyselyyn vastanneet ymmärtävät myös kulttuurin merkityksen kuntalaisten hyvinvoinnille ja sen katsotaan lisäävään moniarvoisuutta ja tasa-arvoa yhteiskunnassa. Lisäksi taiteen-, kulttuurin- ja luovien alojen koetaan olevan varsin tärkeitä kuntien vetovoimalle. Vastaajat pitävätkin kulttuurin ja taiteen tasapuolista saavutettavuutta ja sen turvaamista julkisella rahoituksella tärkeänä. Kuntien kulttuuripolitiikan osalta koetaan varsin tärkeäksi, että kunnissa tuetaan monipuolisesti erilaisia kulttuurin ja sen harrastamisen muotoja. Tutkimuksen vastaajat jaettiin ryhmiin koetun elämänlaadun sekä kulttuurikohteissa käynnin aktiivisuuden mukaan. Tulosten mukaan aktiivisia kulttuurin kuluttajia on 36 prosenttia vastaajista ja ei-aktiivisia 64 prosenttia. Aktiiviset kulttuurikohteissa tai -tilaisuuksissa kävijät ovat tutkimuksen tulosten mukaan hieman useammin miehiä kuin naisia, pääosin 30–40-vuotiaita, korkeasti koulutettuja ja hyvin toimeen tulevia. Elämänlaatunsa he arvioivat useimmiten vähintään hyväksi. Aktiiviset kulttuurin kuluttajat harrastavat myös kulttuuriharrastuksia aktiivisemmin. He ovat ei-aktiivisia tyytyväisempiä kotikuntansa kulttuuritarjontaan ja suhtautuvat positiivisemmin sen vaikuttavuuteen. He olivat myös halukkaampia osallistumaan kulttuuritarjonnan kehittämiseen. Vastaavaan tulokseen päädyttiin ryhmiteltäessä vastaajat koetun elämänlaadun mukaan. Tulosten mukaan hyväksi elämänlaatunsa kokevilla (68 % vastaajista) on keskimääräistä useampia erilaisia kulttuurikäyntejä ja -harrastuksia ja käynti- ja harrastuskertojen määrät ovat suuremmat. He myös kokevat muita vahvemmin saavansa kulttuurista positiivisia vaikutuksia. Lisäksi hyvän elämänlaadun ryhmään kuuluvat pitivät oman kunnan kulttuuripalveluita ja -tarjontaa tärkeämpänä ja olivat niihin myös tyytyväisempiä kuin muut. Kehittämiskohteina nousivat esille osallisuuden ja vaikutusmahdollisuuksien lisääminen, saavutettavuussekä tilojen ja resurssien kehittäminen. Mielekkäimpänä vaikuttamisen keinona koetaan erilaiset sähköiset kanavat, mutta uusiakin kokeiluja voisi tehdä, esimerkiksi osallistuvaa budjetointia tai jalkautumista erilaisiin harrastustoimintoihin. Parhaita keinoja lisätä kuntalaisten osallisuuden tunnetta ja osallistumista kulttuuritarjonnan kehittämiseen ovat kyselyn perusteella kunnan omistamien tilojen luovuttaminen maksutta tapahtumien ja tilaisuuksien järjestämiseen, vapaa-aikatoimen (ml. kulttuuritoimi) toimintaresurssien turvaaminen sekä erilaisten kuntalaisten toimintaryhmien ja pienimuotoisten asuinympäristöjen viihtyvyyttä lisääviä hankkeiden tukeminen. Saavutettavuuden edistämisen osalta kulttuuripalveluiden tulisi huomioida toiminnassaan esteettömyys, liikenneyhteydet sekä eri kohderyhmät. Kulttuuri kuuluu kaikille, ja pandemia on erityisesti osoittanut sen, että virtuaalipalveluilla on nostetta. Digitaalisuutta lisäämällä kulttuurin saavutettavuutta voitaisiin parantaa tai tehdä se saavutettaviksi sellaisille ryhmille, joilla ei ole mahdollisuutta päästä kulttuurin äärelle. Itäsuomalaiset kokevat, että kulttuurin resurssit tulee turvata myös jatkossa ja he ymmärtävät myös kulttuurin merkityksen hyvinvoinnille ja kuntien vetovoimalle. Kulttuurialalla tuleekin miettiä, miten nykyiset resurssit voitaisiin hyödyntää entistä päämäärätietoisemmin ja pitkäjänteisemmin. Kulttuurilta ja luovilta aloilta täytyy saada pois puuhastelun leima ja alkaa nähdä se elinkeino- ja yritystoimintana. Toisaalta tulisi myös nykyistä laajemmin ymmärtää, mihin kaikkeen kulttuuriin sijoitetulla rahalla voidaan vaikuttaa. Niukkojen resurssien jaossa korostuu kyky tehdä yhteistyötä eri sektoreiden välillä. Viime aikoina on ollut paljon esillä kulttuurihyvinvointi, mutta soteuudistuksen myötä yhteistyön tiivistäminen ja uusien avauksien löytäminen yli kuntarajojen sekä esimerkiksi sivistystoimen ja matkailualan kanssa on tärkeää.
... Traditionally, arts activities have tended to be taken up by higher income groups, those who have received more education and reside in asset-rich areas. Cohort studies have explored the connections between arts engagement and health inequalities at population level (Gordon-Nesbitt, 2015). A recent large survey in the US examined activities such as attending live music and arts events, taking part in performances and playing a musical instrument. ...
Article
Creativity, health and wellbeing (CHW) has emerged as a multidisciplinary field of research, policy and practice over the last 20 years. Its beginnings can be traced from the establishment of art therapies in the post war period and from the growth of community arts in the 1960s, which fostered connections between arts professionals, researchers, educators and policy advocates seeking to respond to local challenges (White, 2009). Subsequently the CHW field has grown through evidence building, advocacy and sector development and there is now a wider recognition of the contribution of arts and cultural engagement to a wide range of policy objectives. For example, policies such as social prescribing view arts spaces, activities and resources as community assets that can be used to improve health, to support people living with long-term conditions and to reduce pressure on health services. Nevertheless, the successful integration of arts and creativity into policy and practice is some way off, partly because of ongoing theoretical, methodological and political challenges (Daykin, 2020).
... 11 Miles 2007, 278. 12 Clift and Camic 2016Gordon-Nesbitt 2015;Bidwell 2014;Lelchuk Staricoff 2004. 13 Lehikoinen & Pässilä 2016; Berhoin Antal 2009; Heinsius & Lehikoinen 2013; Pässilä 2012. ...
Article
Full-text available
People in care institutions have limited opportunities to benefit from art and cultural services and the wellbeing they contribute to. This is one reason why the reach of these services needs to be extended to the social welfare and healthcare sectors. This could be done by expanding the currently employed percent for art principle. It would serve to create more equal opportunities for all people to participate in the arts and culture. This ArtsEqual policy brief is addressed to the Finnish Ministry of Education and Culture, the Finnish Ministry of Social Affairs and Health, the Finnish Ministry of Finance, managers of change in the (SOTE) social welfare and healthcare sector and regional government reform process, municipalities and political decision makers. It offers them latest research information and critical perspectives on the realization of cultural rights and cultural welfare, to support the extending of the percent for art principle in accordance with the program of the current government.
... For the Inquiry, this list was extended in three main ways. Research conducted in the Nordic countries suggests that attendance at cultural events contributes to longer lives better lived (Gordon-Nesbitt, 2015). With this in mind, consideration of the field in which the arts act upon our health and wellbeing was taken to include cultural venues such as concert halls, galleries, heritage sites, libraries, museums and theatres. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: The United Kingdom All-Party Parliamentary Group on Arts, Health and Wellbeing was formed in 2014 and, the following year, initiated an Inquiry into the relationship between arts engagement, health and wellbeing. This led to a substantial report being launched in Parliament in July 2017. Methods: The Inquiry comprised 16 round-table discussions, a series of expert meetings and a lengthy period of desk-based research. The latter applied a realist method in seeking to reconcile policy, practice and evidence. Consideration of the social determinants of health formed the theoretical framework. Results: Evidence was found of a beneficial relationship between arts engagement, health and wellbeing across the life course. Conclusions: Arts engagement can mitigate the social determinants of health by influencing perinatal mental health and child cognitive development; shaping educational and employment opportunities and compensating for work-related stress; building individual resilience and enhancing communities. Further research is needed in this area.
Article
Full-text available
The change of the century saw the emergence of a series of discourses that conceptualised different aspects related with culture as key elements in the future of urban realities. The fact that these notions have become encompassed within the celebrated label of “the creative city” leads us to think that they form a self-evident model, fully assimilated and of general value. However, the review of the process through which a reasonably cohesive and accepted framework was constructed unveils the complex nature of the creative city. This article introduces the idea of the creative city as an “approach”, in the sense of an epistemological and methodological focus that is distinguishable, yet neither rigid nor closed. An understanding of this type is useful for assessing the validity and the imbalances of the creative city in the midst of an epoch of problematic transition, in which culture and the city are alternatively defined as spaces of conflict or spaces of hope.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose of the Study: During aging, musical activities can help maintain physical and mental health and cognitive abilities, but their rehabilitative use has not been systematically explored in persons with dementia (PWDs). Our aim was to determine the efficacy of a novel music intervention based on coaching the caregivers of PWDs to use either singing or music listening regularly as a part of everyday care. Eighty-nine PWD-caregiver dyads were randomized to a 10-week singing coaching group (n = 30), a 10-week music listening coaching group (n = 29), or a usual care control group (n = 30). The coaching sessions consisted primarily of singing/listening familiar songs coupled occasionally with vocal exercises and rhythmic movements (singing group) and reminiscence and discussions (music listening group). In addition, the intervention included regular musical exercises at home. All PWDs underwent an extensive neuropsychological assessment, which included cognitive tests, as well as mood and quality of life (QOL) scales, before and after the intervention period and 6 months later. In addition, the psychological well-being of family members was repeatedly assessed with questionnaires. Compared with usual care, both singing and music listening improved mood, orientation, and remote episodic memory and to a lesser extent, also attention and executive function and general cognition. Singing also enhanced short-term and working memory and caregiver well-being, whereas music listening had a positive effect on QOL. Regular musical leisure activities can have long-term cognitive, emotional, and social benefits in mild/moderate dementia and could therefore be utilized in dementia care and rehabilitation.
Article
Full-text available
Social capital is associated with health behaviours and health. Our objective was to explore how different dimensions of social capital and health-related behaviours are associated, and whether health behaviours mediate this association between social capital and self-rated health and psychological well-being. We used data from the Health 2000 Survey (n=8028) of the adult population in Finland. The response rate varied between 87% (interview) and 77% (the last self-administered questionnaire). Due to item non-response, missing values were replaced using multiple imputation. The associations between three dimensions of social capital (social support, social participation and networks, trust and reciprocity) and five health behaviours (smoking, alcohol use, physical activity, vegetable consumption, sleep) were examined by using logistic regression and controlling for age, gender, education, income and living arrangements. The possible mediating role of health behaviours in the association between social capital and self-rated health and psychological well-being was also analysed with a logistic regression model. Social participation and networks were associated with all of the health behaviours. High levels of trust and reciprocity were associated with non-smoking and adequate duration of sleep, and high levels of social support with adequate duration of sleep and daily consumption of vegetables. Social support and trust and reciprocity were independently associated with self-rated health and psychological well-being. Part of the association between social participation and networks and health was explained by physical activity. Irrespective of their social status, people with higher levels of social capital -- especially in terms of social participation and networks -- engage in healthier behaviours and feel healthier both physically and psychologically.
Article
Full-text available
Background Environment, health behavior, and genetic background are important in the development of obesity. Adolescents spend substantial part of daily leisure time on cultural and social activities, but knowledge about the effects of participation in such activities on weight is limited. Methods A number of 1450 adolescents from the Norwegian HUNT study (1995–97) were followed-up in 2006–08 as young adults. Phenotypic data on lifestyle and anthropometric measures were assessed using questionnaires and standardized clinical examinations. Genotypic information on 12 established obesity-susceptibility loci were available for analyses. Generalized estimating equations were used to examine the associations between cultural and social activities in adolescence and adiposity measures in young adulthood. In addition, interaction effects of a genetic predisposition score by leisure time activities were tested. Results In girls, participation in cultural activities was negatively associated with waist circumference (WC) (B = −0.04, 95%CI: -0.08 to −0.00) and with waist-hip ratio (WHR) (B = −0.058, 95%CI: -0.11 to −0.01). However, participation in social activities was positively associated with WC (B = 0.040, CI: 0.00 to 0.08) in girls and with BMI (B = 0.027, CI: 0.00 to 0.05) in boys. The effect of the obesity-susceptibility genetic variants on anthropometric measures was lower in adolescents with high participation in cultural activities compared to adolescents with low participation. Conclusion This study suggests that the effects of cultural activities on body fat are different from the effects of participation in social activities. The protective influence of cultural activities in female adolescents against overweight in adulthood and their moderating effect on obesity-susceptibility genes suggest that even cultural activities may be useful in public health strategies against obesity.
Book
Social capital is a widely acknowledged candidate for implementing beneficial democratic processes and promoting public health. Healthy ties. Social capital, population health and survival traces the path from the conceptualization to the implementation of social capital. To provide empirical proof of the effects of social capital on public health is a serious challenge and the main focus of the book. In the Nordic countries, personal identification codes linking data from various sources, nation-wide population registers, nationally representative and re-tested health surveys, and the long tradition of epidemiology submit to serve well the research into social capital and public health. Up-to-date longitudinal data on social capital and health outcomes are carefully described and reviewed in this book. In Finland, the Swedish-speaking minority is very long-lived and has better health as compared with the Finnish-speaking majority. Well aware of the rule of thumb that minorities do worse than their respective majorities in terms of well-being and health, the author presents this exceptional phenomenon as an excellent area for social capital and public health research. Healthy ties. Social capital, population health and survival should inspire scholars, researchers, teachers and advanced students in social epidemiology and public health, and lead to new interventions in promoting health. © Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2010. All rights reserved.
Article
Employees have limited personal time to engage in activities that enable them to recover from a demanding work environment and perform at a high level. To evaluate the importance of non‐work creative activity, we conducted two studies that examine the relationships between non‐work creative activity, recovery experiences, and performance‐related behaviours at work. Study 1 included employees who provided self‐rated performance‐related outcomes, whereas Study 2 included employees with other‐rated (co‐workers and subordinates) performance‐related outcomes. Creative activity was positively associated with recovery experiences (i.e., mastery, control, and relaxation) and performance‐related outcomes (i.e., job creativity and extra‐role behaviours). The mediating effects of recovery experiences were examined to better understand the underlying processes involved in the relationship between creative activity and performance‐related outcomes. Creative activity was found to have both indirect effects and direct effects on performance‐related outcomes, but the effects varied by the type of performance‐related outcome. The results indicate that organizations may benefit from encouraging employees to consider creative activities in their efforts to recover from work. Practitioner points Organizations should increase employee awareness of the benefits of creative activity on recovery. Many companies already provide information to employees regarding the importance of specific activities (e.g., eating habits, exercise) on physical health. Information on activities that influence recovery – a psychological health consequence – should be included in the informational resource provided to employees. Organizations may consider professional development opportunities for employees that involve creative activities while away from work. Creative activities are likely to provide valuable experiences of mastery and control, but may also provide employees experiences of discovery that uniquely influence performance‐related outcomes. Opportunities used by large organizations, such as Z appos I nc., include employees bringing their artwork to work to decorate their offices. Other options include memberships to art studios, creative writing resources, and access to musical instruments.
Article
Maturation of long-running birth cohort studies has fostered a life course approach to adult health, function, and disease and related to conceptual frameworks. Using broad concepts of human development including physical, cognitive, and emotional function, birth cohorts provide insights into the processes across the life course and between generations that link to adult outcomes. We discuss findings on the determinants and health consequences of lifetime trajectories of body size, cognitive and emotional function, and socioeconomic position. Findings from the studies suggest that, for some adult health outcomes, explanations will be incomplete unless exposures and processes from across the life course are taken into account. New birth cohort studies are poised to delineate further the nature and timing of life course relationships in contemporary generations of children.
Article
Health consequences of relative or absolute poverty constitute a definitive area of study in social medicine. As demonstrated in the extreme example of the Dutch Hunger Winter from 1944 to 1945, prenatal hunger can lead to adult schizophrenia and depression. A Norwegian study showed how childhood poverty resulted in a heightened risk of myocardial infarction in adulthood. In England, a study of extended impaired prenatal nutrition indicated three different types of increased cardiovascular risk at older ages. Current animal and human studies link both adverse and enriched environmental exposures to intergenerational transmission. We do not fully understand the molecular mechanisms for it; however, studies that follow up epigenetic marks within a generation combined with exploration of gametic epigenetic inheritance may help explain the prevalence of certain conditions such as cardiovascular disease, schizophrenia, and alcoholism, which have complex etiologies. Insights from these studies will be of great public health importance. Expected final online publication date for the Annual Review of Public Health Volume 34 is March 17, 2013. Please see http://www.annualreviews.org/catalog/pubdates.aspx for revised estimates.