ArticlePDF Available

Psychology of Beauty: An Overview of the Contemporary Research Lines

Authors:

Abstract

We present a synthesis of the contemporary approaches on the topic of corporal beauty, by dividing them into eight major research lines, each illustrated by a set of most representative theories and results: evolutionary, sociocultural and body image approaches, researches on the cross-cultural standards of beauty, on its factors, effects, personal benefits and media representations.
PsychologyofBeauty:AnOverviewoftheContemporaryResearchLines
«PsychologyofBeauty:AnOverviewoftheContemporaryResearchLines»
byAndreiHolman
Source:
SocialPsychology(Psihologiasocială),issue:28/2011,pages:8194,onwww.ceeol.com.
Andrei Holman1
Psychology of Beauty: An Overview
of the Contemporary Research Lines
Abstract: We present a synthesis of the contemporary approaches on the topic of corporal
beauty, by dividing them into eight major research lines, each illustrated by a set of most
representative theories and results: evolutionary, sociocultural and body image approaches,
researches on the cross-cultural standards of beauty, on its factors, effects, personal benefits and
media representations.
Keywords: beauty, research lines
Beauty is frequently conceived in contemporary psychosocial studies as physical
attractiveness. Yet, as we will show, the concept of attractiveness can include references
to other traits than the strictly physical ones, usually when dealing with contexts which
imply a hypothetical or real social interaction (or relationship) between the perceiver and
the perceived  such as the context of mate choice. Moreover, the sociocultural approaches
prefer the term beauty in order to underline the cultural dependencies of this construct,
the delimitation from the material level of the actual body. As a way of dealing with these
ambiguities which complicate the task of synthesizing the various positions, we will keep
track of the plurality of possible semantic loadings of the concept investigated in each
research line.
The research lines presented are mainly differentiated on the bases of the concept
representing the main focus of research (such as body image). Nevertheless, in some
cases, it becomes as informative to mention their particular scientific approach  such as
anthropological or cultural studies  or, in the instances where their option is more
consistently ideological, the peculiar vision which supports them (such as feminist studies).
a. The evolutionary approaches attempt to explain attractiveness as a function of the
genetic quality of the individual, defined as health status (past  developmental incidents,
current and future  disease and stress resistance) as well as reproductive potential and
quality (heterozygosity). Such biological endowment is signaled through a set of features,
evolved through sexual selection, with a high degree of cross-cultural and even cross-species
consensus in terms of their attractiveness: symmetry, averageness, and sexually dimorphic
traits. As such, the strongest support for this perspective comes from the studies which
reveal the cross-cultural homogeneity of attractiveness ratings of the same targets and the
1. Alexandru Ioan Cuza University, Iaºi, România; holman@psih.uaic.ro.
82 ANDREI HOLMAN
lack of influence of the degree of exposure to Western mass-media on these ratings (Fink and
Neave, 2005), as well from those suggesting that beauty preferences appear from the early
childhood, supposedly before any cultural influences would begin to manifest.
According to the fitness hypothesis which is at the core of the evolutionary discourse
on beauty, due to the presence of such features in healthy individuals, humans have
evolved to prefer them in their mating choices (e.g. Fink and Neave, 2005; Frederick and
Haselton, 2007). Yet, some evolutionary-inspired researches debate the importance of
these physical traits with various methodologies. Some argue, for instance, that facial
averageness is less attractive than expected (Pollard et al., 1999), or that the effect size of
averageness is smaller for the evaluations of the opposite sex than of the representatives
of the same sex (Langlois et al., 2000)  thus being less important in heterosexual choices.
One of the fundamental elements of the evolutionary perspective has also been questioned
by some authors  namely the association of attractiveness to the health status of the
individual. One the one side, research (e.g. Henderson and Anglin, 2003) support this
thesis at the level of social perception, in the sense that people with attractive faces are
considered to be healthier by their perceivers. On the other hand, as the meta-analyses on
the topic show (e.g. Langlois et al., 2000), the results of the medical investigations on
participants with various health and attractiveness levels reveal only moderate relationships
between the latter and physical health, as well as weak associations to mental health.
Moreover, other studies suggest that such a relationship to health  in a moderate degree 
exists for women, but not for men (Hume and Montgomerie, 2001), which is also the case
with the link between one of the three main criteria proposed by the evolutionary view 
sexual dimorphism  and attractiveness (Rhodes, 2006).
As a consequence of these results, other studies explored the variations of the importance
of the evolutionary criteria and their health correlates depending on the attractiveness level
of the target perceived. Their conclusion is that the link between attractiveness and health
is limited to faces below the median in attractiveness. Such individuals appearance
present strong deviations from the evolutionary criteria stated above, which are also
associated with some chromosomal disorders and, consequently, with a low health status
(Rhodes, 2006). In other words, high levels of health stress  affecting the functional state
of the body  determine low levels of attractiveness through the deviations from the geo-
metric standards that they induce, thus signaling the individuals poor health state. Never-
theless, such a signaling function of these negative geometry-related apprehensions  in
the case of unattractive people  seems less relevant for the other background referent put
forth by the evolutionary perspective  ones genetic potential. For instance, the idea that
asymmetry signals poor genetic quality and developmental instability was empirically
infirmed by biological data (Saad, 2004).
Another significant moderating variable seems to be the level of societal development,
especially in terms of the health risks that individuals are exposed to in their physical
environment. As Gangestad and Buss (1993, cited in Rhodes, 2006) show, attractiveness
in a mate is valued more in societies with high parasitism rates and poorer health. In the
Western cultures, due to the availability of medical cure and pharmaceuticals, as well as
to the better nutritional status of the population, the link between health and beauty has
been severely undermined. This raises the possibility of a biomedical underpinning of the
cross-cultural variations in beauty standards  a theme which will be reviewed in the
following pages.
Because of the contradictory results of the investigations carried out from a strict
evolutionary perspective, other studies in this field include social factors in their analyses
Access via CEEOL NL Germany
83PSYCHOLOGY OF BEAUTY: AN OVERVIEW OF THE CONTEMPORARY...
of these complex relationships. An example from this sociobiological research field is
the study carried out by Cunningham (1986) on the attractiveness of different types of
facial features in various contexts. In his two quasi-experiments on the sociobiology of
female facial beauty, he elicited attractiveness ratings of female photographs from male
participants. The results allowed the extraction of several specific facial features with a
high degree of attractiveness, grouped into three categories: neonate, mature and expressive.
In the second study, which required male participants to make personality inferences and
to relate to several potential contexts of interactions with the depicted women, these
features proved to be correlated to personality attributions, altruistic inclinations and
reproductive interest.
b. The investigation of the cross-cultural standards of beauty is based on the opposite
hypothesis to evolutionism: it recognizes the universal character of some attractive
features, but makes the assumption that there is a large inter-cultural variability in this
respect. The empirical comparisons involved in such research are either:
cross-national: for instance, Bjerke and Polegato (2006) investigated the ideal self-
-image of women in terms of health and beauty, in a sample of participants from five
European cities, through their preferences for one of the two models, representing
distinct advertising beauty types. Results revealed significant cross-cultural variation
in ideal self-image in terms of health and beauty.
cross-continental: for example, Swami et al. (2006) compared participants from
Japan and Britain on the relative contribution of Body Mass Index (BMI) and Waist to
Hip Ratio (WHR) in female physical attractiveness. BMI emerged as a significant
predictor of attractiveness, but with a significant cross-cultural variation, in the sense
that Japanese participants preferred images of women with lower BMIs than Britons,
on one hand, and the latter placing a lower importance on body shape when judging
female physical attractiveness than the former. This result is interpreted by the authors
as a consequence of the cross-cultural differences in the sex roles occupied by the two
genders, representatives of the cultures with traditional sex roles manifesting stronger
preferences for traditionally attractive body shapes.
between representatives of various ethnic groups in the same country: Poran (2002)
compared Latina, Black and White women participants in what regards their conceptions
of beauty, perceptions of cultural standards of beauty, as well as their tendency to
engage in a specific social comparison process  the denial of personal disadvantage (a
self-protective psychological mechanisms revealed in the studies on minorities). Results
indicate similarities in the personal definitions of beauty, but significant variations in
the cultural standard of beauty. For instance, Black women were most likely to
mention Whiteness overtly as an important part of the cultural definition of beauty,
Latina women sometimes mentioned Whiteness, while White women rarely did.
Putting together the corpus of results from the two approaches reviewed so far 
evolutionary and cross-cultural  we can conclude about the validity of a joint model,
which would include elements from both. As weve mentioned, the evolutionary-inspired
associations between beauty and health status seem to appear only in individuals with low
levels of attractiveness. We can hypothesize that for those more endowed in terms of
physical appeal, their evaluations have less to do with the rigid evolutionary standards,
being dependent instead, as Osborn (1996) suggest, on cultural norms. In other words,
Beauty is skin deep, but ugly is to the bone, where skin deep implies the superficiality
84 ANDREI HOLMAN
and modifiability of cultural beauty, as well as the individuals possibility to conform
to its norms  in contrast to the structural, deep ugliness brought by the physical
deviance from the geometrical standards.
c. The research on the factors of beauty  other than the evolutionary ones listed
above  can be split in two major arrays on the basis of the focus of analysis:
the perceiver  the idea that beauty is in the eyes of the perceiver has been a very
popular one. Some of the studies testing this hypothesis  inspired by the social
relations model (e.g. Osborn, 1996) focused on the variations in attractiveness judgments
brought by experiences in ongoing relationships between people. For instance, Park
and Flink (1989) compared the changes in variances of attractiveness ratings statistically
explained by the perceiver and, respectively, by the target across three days of social
interaction between the two. Their results show the decrease in the variance attributable
to the target (from its high value in the first day) and the increase in the variance
attributable to the perceiver, which more than doubled as a result of the interaction
between the two (from 22% to 48%).
Another topic in the perceiver realm is the critical periods in the development of
personal tastes in appearance styles. In their study using fashion advertisements with both
male and female models, covering the interval from 1933 to 1990, Schindler and Holbrook
(1993) reveal a marked gender difference in the preferences expressed by their participants.
While men seem to have a clear developmental stage  around the age of 20s  in which
their lifelong preferences for women styles are decided, womens preferences (in general,
for both genders) seem to be built during a broader and older range of years. The
explanations of mens critical period in attractiveness assessment development, as suggested
by the authors, are both in emotional terms (nostalgia), as well as in cognitive-perceptual
ones (mere exposure).
Another investigation in the area of gender differences in judgments of attractiveness
was conducted by Marcus and Miller (2003), who had participants of both genders rate
each other on the dimension of attractiveness, while also providing metacognitions of the
evaluations they thought they would receive from the others. Comparing the two sources
of variance of the attractiveness scores, the authors found the highest levels of consensus
to appear when men judged female targets, and the highest levels of idiosyncrasy in the
situation of men rating other male targets. Regarding the metacognitions, womens were
highly associated to mens judgments of them. Overall, results suggest that women have
a high level of accuracy in self-assessment on the attractiveness scale, which could be a
consequence of the consistency of evaluations received from men, thus, in time, enhancing
their correctness of self-apprehensions.
the perceived  a similar point to the evolutionary one concerning the importance of
the spatial arrangement of the body parts in terms of symmetry is noticeable in the
literature on the ideal proportions. One of the differences between the two positions
is in terms of localization: while the influences of symmetry on human attractiveness
have mostly been studied at the face level, proportions are usually analyzed on the
overall level  of the body shape or profile.
According to this approach, the harmonic body shape  as perceived by the human eye
is a result of a series of definite numeric relationships between the sizes and positions of
various segments of the body. The most frequently applied canons of beauty have been the
85PSYCHOLOGY OF BEAUTY: AN OVERVIEW OF THE CONTEMPORARY...
ratios of unity (1:1) and the golden ratio (1,618:1), based on the number phi. This
holistic perspective on the body claims that the attractiveness of particular physical traits
is dependent on the evaluation of the body as a whole in terms of its deviations from the
ideal proportions. Support for its arguments comes from the various referents of these
geometrical relationships in art history, from the ancient Egyptian, Greek and Roman art,
to the body representations in the Renaissance period, initiated by Leonardo da Vinci.
These geometrical standards are of particular importance for aesthetic surgery  the
medical branch involved in the artificial increase of ones beauty status. For instance,
in an article published in the Aesthetic Plastic Surgery journal, Atiyeh and Hayek (2008)
present in detail these numeric principles of bodily harmony, as addressing the need to
determine the beauty of the final result as precisely as possible (p. 209). Moreover, as
the authors admit, modern plastic surgeons have frequently used the neoclassical canons
of beauty as working guides on their patients, even though studies on real Caucasian,
Asian, and African Americans revealed that such proportions do not represent the average.
In other words, the Renaissance geometrical relationships, developed as a mathematical
formalization of the ideal body, as depicted in art, have been applied in reshaping real
human bodies.
Research testing the relevance of these proportions for human attractiveness has
suggested a different set of numeric relationships as important for the two genders. They
usually represent ratios of sizes of two body parts, and their optimal state (their beauty
standard) is derived from various fundamental geometrical proportions. In womens ratings
of mens attractiveness, waist-to-chest ratio, body mass index (BMI), and waist-to-hip
ratio seem to be the most important cues, while in the evaluations of women, the set is
comprised of five such ratios: bust-to-underbust ratio, bust-to-waist ratio, bust-to-hip
ratio, waist-to-leg ratio, and an androgyny index (Atiyeh and Hayek, 2008).
Other studies have revealed the importance of specific physical features. For instance,
Grundl et al. (2008) investigated, in a research published in the Aesthetic Plastic Surgery
journal, the features that create an attractive eye, by manipulating specifically the eye axis
of the female targets judged by German participants of both genders. The results of this
study show significant age differences in the eye axis preferences, in the sense that young
subjects regarded the oblique type of eye axis as more attractive, whereas older subjects
showed a preference for the more horizontal eyes.
Various situational factors have also been taken into account in the area of the
perceived. Jones et al. (2006) analyze gaze direction and emotional expression, revealing
significant interactions of these two factors with physical attractiveness. Expression seems
to determine different attractiveness ratings for faces with direct and averted gaze, in the
sense that for judgments of faces with direct gaze, preferences were stronger for smiling
faces than for faces with neutral expressions, while the opposite result was found for faces
with averted gaze. These results are interpreted by the authors in a social interaction
frame, suggesting that individuals base their attractiveness ratings on the inferences drawn
from the facial cues regarding the others interest in the perceiver.
A target-related factor investigated in a study published in the International Journal of
Cosmetic Science by Mulhern et al. (2003) is the use of cosmetics by Caucasian women,
aiming to assess the contribution of each type of facial cosmetics in overall attractiveness,
as judged by Caucasian participants of both genders, as well as their interaction effects.
Results show significant between-genders differences, eye make-up proving to be the
stronger predictor of attractiveness when evaluated by women, while the combination of
foundation with eye make-up having the strongest impact on men preferences. Overall, the
86 ANDREI HOLMAN
effects of make-up in the attractiveness evaluations of women seem to be comparable with
those of the more traditional criteria put forth by evolutionary approach  the structural
geometric facial features. Other examples of attractiveness factors in the perceived targets
control, whose importance was assessed using a similar research design, are body dynamics
(Riggio et al., 1991) or posture (Osborn, 1996).
An interesting idea put forth in this last study is the connection between these
appearance enhancing actions and the cultural standards of beauty. Osborn (1996) observes
that the effectiveness of these beautification practices is assessed through a set of cultural
criteria. The term conceived for this relationship between the individual space of action
and the societal appearance norms is the social role theory of beauty. As we will see,
the theme of cultural operant conditioning in what regards appearance  the cultivation
and reinforcement of specific beautification behaviors, in agreement with the prevalent
norms  has also been extensively researched in the sociocultural studies.
d. The effects of beauty on the perceiver are investigated at various levels:
the neural activity level, revealing the brain regions which respond to attractiveness.
For instance, Chatarjee et al. (2009) report a study using functional MRI (fMRI) in
order to investigate the neural activity associated with the apprehension of faces.
Participants were required to perform two successive judgment tasks  facial beauty
rating and facial identity recognition  on artificial (computer-generated) faces, which
allowed the precise control of various geometric parameters. Results reveal that despite
the differences in neural correlates between the two tasks, there was a specific brain
region  the ventral occipital region  which remained responsive to facial beauty in
both situations. As the authors conclude, this area is activated automatically by beauty
and may serve as a neural trigger for the mental effects of beautiful faces.
the cognitive processing level  including, for instance, studies on the processing
advantages of attractive targets; in their three computer-based experiments on this topic,
Becker et al. (2005) found that physical attractiveness leads to more efficient matching for
female faces, but not for male faces, in the sense that attractive women are less confusable
with one another. The significantly weaker effect discovered in the case of male
attractive faces processing is interpreted by the authors in a sexual selection frame, as
revealing different socio-sexual functions that attractiveness has for men and women.
In the same vein of sexually selective cognition, Maner et al. (2003) examined, across
five experiments, the cognitive biases induced by beauty. Their results indicate a differential
selective processing of attractive men and women by the two genders, in the sense that the
two cognitive phenomena investigated  biased estimates and recognition of attractive
targets  appear less in the case of male targets, as well as being less manifested by female
perceivers. The other between-subjects variable included in the research design  ones
involvement in a stable relationship  also had a significant interaction effect with gender,
in the sense that being committed to a relationship reduced womens, but not mens,
attention to attractive targets of the opposite sex. At a more general level, these differences
indicate a consistent tendency to focus on attractive women, which manifests not only in
men, but also in women. This result is in line with other studies in the field, and can be
read in two frames, either as revealing the intense social comparison processes that
women engage in as competitors in the sexual market (the evolutionistic account),
either as a sign of the self-objectification perspective that women have interiorized,
87PSYCHOLOGY OF BEAUTY: AN OVERVIEW OF THE CONTEMPORARY...
adapting to and striving for the decorative role that the patriarchal society has limited them
to (the feminist account).
Another relevant study in this area is the investigation carried out by Olson and
Marshuetz (2005). Their results reveal another cognitive effect of beautiful faces, similar
to the ones specific to the studies on implicit attitudes, namely the priming of positive
words by attractive upright faces, while no priming effect emerged for unattractive faces
or for other sorts of attractive stimuli. Another important result of this experimental
investigation was that attractiveness can be perceived from minimal amounts of visual
information: even when the faces were presented under severely impoverished viewing
conditions (induced by masking or rapid presentation), participants could judge correctly
their attractiveness level.
the emotional level, investigating the effects of attractive people on ones mood and
body satisfaction, usually introducing the perceivers body image as a mediator of
these influences. For example, Pinhas et al. (1999) found that women were more
depressed and angrier as a consequence of exposure to pictures in fashion magazines.
Other studies focus on the interaction of physical attractiveness with other personal
traits. Gutierres et al. (1999) investigated in their experimental research the effects of
exposure to physically attractive and dominant same-sex individuals on self-evaluations,
measured on the dimension of mate-value (desirability as marriage partner). Results
show a different between  genders pattern: womens self-assessments of their mate
value are negatively influenced by exposure to highly physically attractive women,
while mens self-assessments were more affected by the social dominance of the men
to whom they were exposed. These two threats to self-esteem mirror and are
determined by their high importance in the perceived attractiveness of the opposite
gender; in his investigation on 37 cultures, Buss (1994) discovered the universal
character of the preferred characteristics in ones ideal mate, the patterns for each
gender being related to the threats mentioned before: for men, beauty and youth are
the essential qualities, while for women it is men social status which mostly determine
their attractiveness.
Other studies also reveal mediators of these effects; for instance, Choi et al. (2008)
notice that the mechanism of how women are negatively affected by beautiful female
models is still unclear, and propose a third-person effect framework as a possible such
mechanism. Overall, their results show that lower body satisfaction is specific to women
who believe that men (especially close male friends) would be more influenced, in their
female beauty evaluations, by the images of fashion models they were exposed to.
the social inferences level (stereotypes) includes the vast person perception research
on the personality traits which are ascribed to beautiful targets by perceivers, in
various interpersonal settings (e.g. Ellis et al., 1983). This phenomenon can be traced
back to the antic assumption that character is immanent in appearance  as Aristotle
formulated it, Beauty is a greater recommendation than any letter of introduction 
or to the theories of physiognomy regarding the connection between beauty and virtue,
respectively between beauty and vice.
Other studies on the whats beautiful is good phenomenon reveal its limits and
variations; for example, Dermer and Thiel (1975), in their study on the inferences drawn
by women about attractive female targets, show that attributions favorability is not
consistent along all the personality dimensions. More specifically, their results reveal
88 ANDREI HOLMAN
various socially undesirable personal characteristics which were ascribed to targets of high
attractiveness, such as vanity, egotism, materialism, snobbism or likelihood of a marital
disaster. Generally, as Eagly et al. (1991) conclude on the basis of their meta-analysis,
the differences in subjects perception of attractive and unattractive targets were largest
for indexes of social competence; intermediate for potency, adjustment, and intellectual
competence; and near zero for integrity and concern for others.
the attitudinal level  studies focused on the development of sexist attitudes and
behaviors have revealed their influences from the exposure to media images in sexually
objectifying frames. Generally, individuals of both genders exposed to such content
have a higher acceptance of rape myths, sexual harassment, sex-role stereotypes,
interpersonal violence, and adversarial beliefs about relationships compared to those in
control conditions (Machia and Lamb, 1999).
e. The personal benefits of beauty  mostly represent consequences of the advantages
in social perception outcomes stated above. Such self-fulfilling prophecies come in many
forms: psychological (mental health, sense of personal competence, life satisfaction, etc.
Sullivan, 2001), social (e.g. help from strangers  Patzer, 1985, cited in Sullivan, 2001),
economical (financial earnings  beauty bonus  Umberson and Hughes, 1987). There
are virtually no areas of social interaction which are left uncontaminated by physical attrac-
tiveness, in which the beautiful would not receive certain privileges, most of the time
outside the conscious awareness of their victims. Moreover, attractiveness brings an
advantage in terms of the accuracy of personality traits ascribed by people with whom
beautiful individuals interact, as compared to the less attractive ones, because it motivates
the perceivers to initiate a closer social contact, to engage in a more in-depth social
interaction with the perceived. In other words, people do judge a book by its cover, but
a beautiful cover prompts a closer reading (Lorenzo et al., 2010).
There are also distinctive and significant social consequences assigned by society to
those at the other end of the beauty continuum. We might say that these negative effects
of being ugly are one of the few forms of social discrimination which have escaped the
censorship of political correctness. As Berry (2007) remarks, unattractive and ordi-
nary-looking people are fair game for insults and discrimination (p. 2). An illustrative
example that the author offers of this still legitimate prejudice and its consequences is the
case of short men in the US, who are more likely to drop out of school, drink heavily,
date sparsely, and become emotionally depressed and physically ill. They have less
opportunity than tall men of marrying and having children. Their salaries are far less than
those of tall men (p. 60). As I can be noticed, the drawbacks of not attaining the
appearance standards mirror the advantages received by those at the opposite end of the
beauty continuum.
One of the hidden purposes of this looksism would be, as the author suggests, a
commercial one, namely the persuasion of those less gifted in the attractiveness domain to
invest their earnings in the solutions provided by beauty industry, in order to avoid
discrimination and receive access to the forbidden privileges of the beautiful.
f. Body image  for instance, Jung and Lennon (2003) investigated the fluctuations of
womens body image after exposure to attractive images, taking into account participants
appearance self-schemas (cognitive representations of organized information about the
self in relation to appearance). Their results show that only in women who were schematic
on appearance such exposure led to lower body image and mood. Similarly, Wiederman and
89PSYCHOLOGY OF BEAUTY: AN OVERVIEW OF THE CONTEMPORARY...
Hurst (1997) studied the relationships between womens sexual self-schema and physical
attractiveness, body size and shape, and body image. Bulhmann et al. (2009) examined the
mental associations of beauty in individuals with Body Dysmorphic Disorder in three
samples: individuals diagnosed with, individuals with subclinical BDD symptoms, and
healthy control participants. Using the Implicit Association Test, the authors revealed that
BDD participants manifested a stronger stereotype about physical attractiveness  its
association to competence , indicating a higher degree of inferential reactivity (of
contamination of personality inferences with beauty evaluations), which might reflect a
cognitive mechanism responsible for their disorder.
Ones body image is also investigated in many studies in this area as a mediator of the
effects of exposure to the beauty ideals in the mass-media. These effects are analyzed on
a behavioral level  such as eating disorders  or at a psychological one  which includes
many of the dimensions listed above: body dissatisfaction, negative self-appraisals, low
self-esteem, etc.
g. Socio-cultural approaches, preoccupied with the societal roles of beauty definitions
and norms, usually emphasize the ubiquitous influences of beauty in all social aspects 
the social aesthetics research field , as well as the opposite relationships, the socially
constructed character of attractiveness criteria and norms. As Rhodes (2006) notices, this
idea of the beauty standards being arbitrary cultural conventions already has a respectable
history, being formulated by the first time by Darwin, after observing large cultural
differences in beautification practices.
For instance, the study carried out by Robertson (2001) on the evaluation criteria used
in the Miss Nipon contests in the first half of the XX-th century illustrates an idea at the
core of the feminist perspective  personal is political  but in a more literal fashion 
without any reference to the gender relationships. Instead, the author emphasizes the
political intentions which were guiding the selection of Miss Nipon, which were the
improvements in the Japanese race through sports and a more Western-like clothing
style (less body restrictive). As such, the main criterion for this selection was the girls
height, as well as other eugenics  oriented dimensions, such as their reproductive
potential, proportionate health or purity of their spirit and blood.
A similar case is the cultural construction of obesity, a topic on which representatives of
this approach have remarked the variability and arbitrariness of the medical (supposedly
scientific) standards, suggesting their dependency on a political war against fatness. As the
media portrayals of beauty use increasingly thinner models, body weight has received moral
connotations, obesity  as defined by the current standards  being associated with lack of
self-control, self-discipline and generally, an inferior moral status. Moreover, the correlation
between body weight and socio-economic status in contemporary society seems to manifest a
historical reversal: in the Western societies of today, it is the thin people who are the rich ones.
The gradual lowering of the obesity thresholds seems contradictory even with the
biomedical results, which show that people who are moderately overweight have a higher
life expectancy than people who are not overweight. One of the possible hidden causes of
this phenomenon is the financial interests of insurance companies, since these it allows
them to deny coverage or to increase premiums for heavier people (Berry, 2007), using
the medical legitimization of the risks of obesity-related illnesses.
The feminist perspective, most often employed in this array, claims that beauty ideals
and practices are tools for the oppression of women, reducing them to an inferior,
unnatural and decorative role (e.g. Jeffrey, 2005; Wolf, 1991). Although initially reserved
90 ANDREI HOLMAN
for the analysis of feminine beauty, the feminist perspective has also included masculine
beauty, drawing attention to the increasingly stronger cultural forces directed at men in
this respect. The specificity of the feminist writings  which are reserved a larger space in
the following section, dealing with cosmetic surgery  is the inclusion of patriarchy as an
essential referent in the cultural exploration of beauty and, generally, of the roles and
meanings of the body. Even though there are differences within the various feminist
positions, the concept of oppression of women in more or less a general one. This
oppression is carried out, among other instruments, through contemporary beauty stan-
dards and practices, which have a series of masked roles and consequences: to assign
women an inferior status, to legitimize it through the cultural focus on superficial,
unimportant characteristics  such as appearance  and to limit womens involvement in
actions which would contribute to their real social ascension. Thus, they create an artificial
space of competition in which they would invest their efforts, while raising the beauty
standards high enough in order to make sure that most of them would fail to attain them.
This, in turn, ensures the diminishing of womens self-esteem, a necessary and final piece
of the puzzle which would guarantee their interiorization of the lower social status. Being
submitted to the continuous scrutiny of their appearance by men, women learn to evaluate
themselves first and best, which leads to their own self-objectification, treating their
bodies as exterior objects (Jeffreys, 2005).
For instance, Scott (1997, p. 12) puts forth the following four central themes of the
feminist studies on beauty:
1. Beauty is fundamentally feminine.
2. Beauty is imperative for women.
3. Beauty is paramount among womens qualities.
4. Womens beauty requires substantial modification of the natural appearance.
h. Media studies on the representations of beauty  apart from the theme of
self-evaluation effects of mass-media portrayals of beauty above (e.g. Pinhas et al.,
1999), other studies point out, for instance, variations in the beauty representations across
cultures. In this respect, Frith et al. (2004) analyzed advertisements collected from
womens fashion and beauty magazines in Singapore, Taiwan, and the United States,
taking into account models clothing style, beauty type, product categories. Their general
conclusion is that overall, Western models were assigned a seductive role in adverts more
often than Asian ones, being more used in pictorials body oriented. Yet, there are also
studies (e.g. OBarr, 1994, Griffin et al., 1994) which conclude on the increasing
homogenization of such representations across cultures.
A connected area of research is the analysis of the specific definitions of beauty in
various communication spaces. Smith et al. (1999) examined the personality characteristics
of physically attractive characters in U.S. films across a random sample from 5 decades
of top-grossing films. They found a consistent beauty-and-goodness stereotype, stable
across time periods, character sex, and characters centrality to the plot, in the sense that
in the Hollywood movie industry, the beautiful characters are endowed with more positive
personality traits than the unattractive ones.
Englis et al. (1994) compared the emphasis placed on different ideals of beauty across
two US-based communications media (print  fashion magazines vs. music television) and
formats (advertising vs. entertainment). The results reveal a multiplicity of beauty ideals,
each strongly associated with contextual cues, but also a set of ideals more prevalent than
others, namely the sensual/exotic, trendy, and classic/feminine forms of beauty. In a
91PSYCHOLOGY OF BEAUTY: AN OVERVIEW OF THE CONTEMPORARY...
similar vein, Sypeck et al. (2003) analyzed the trends in print medias depictions of the
ideal of feminine beauty between 1959 and 1999, by measuring the body size and the
proportion of the body depicted on the cover of four American fashion magazines. They
found that while the first indicator decreased  in the sense that the models were
increasingly thin , the second increased, in other words the depiction of the entire body
of the models became more and more frequent across time. Garner et al. (1980) and
Wiseman et al. (1992) revealed a similar consistent trend towards thinness in their
examinations of the evolution of Playboy magazine centerfolds and Miss America Pageant
contestants, together covering the period from 1959 to 1988. The same phenomenon
appears in the fashion world: while in the 1950s models weighted 8% less than the
average American woman, in the 1990s the difference went up to 23% (Berry, 2007).
Such data indicate the higher importance of and cultural pressure from the thin ideal for
women in contemporary society.
As the socio-cultural approach has suggested, men have also been included in the
target audience of beauty industry, the pressure on their remodeling of appearance
according to its norms being on the rise. An example of the instruments of this pressure
comes from the results of the study cited by Berry (2007) on the evolution of the muscle
size of the GI Joe dolls from 1982 to mid-1990s; the muscles that the doll was gradually
endowed with have reached unrealistic proportions during this time interval  as compared
to the actual possibilities of the human body. Of course, the evolution of the Barbie dolls
has also included unrealistic modifications, each moving it closer to a material prototype
of current beauty standards.
Gottschall et al. (2008) investigated the gendered attractiveness emphasis (the difference
of attractiveness between the two genders) in a large sample of traditional folktales from
13 diverse cultural areas. The research aimed to empirically compare the hypothesis of the two
confronting perspectives on this topic: the feminist, constructivist one, according to which
the greater cultural emphasis on female attractiveness is a specific Western phenomenon,
and the evolutionary account, which suggests that is it a universal one. The results of their
analysis reveal that across culture areas, information on physical attractiveness was much
more likely to be conveyed for female characters, thus providing support for the evolutionary
hypothesis. The authors conclude that the beauty myth  the term used by feminism
when referring to the Western societal definitions of beauty and their misogynistic
character  is no myth, since it appears virtually in all the cultures.
A similar result was found in cross-cultural studies on the portrayals of the two
genders in advertisement productions (Saad, 2004), women being placed in decorative
roles, generally depicted as younger and more attractive than men.
Psihologia frumuseþii: o privire sinteticã
asupra abordãrilor contemporane
Rezumat: Articolul prezintã o sintezã a abordãrilor contemporane ale temei frumuseþii corporale,
clasificate în opt linii majore de cercetare, fiecare fiind ilustratã prin cele mai reprezentative teorii
ºi rezultate: abordãrile evoluþioniste, socio-culturale ºi centrate pe imaginea corporalã, cercetãrile
asupra standardelor transculturale ale frumuseþii, asupra factorilor, efectelor, beneficiilor personale
ºi reprezentãrilor sale în media.
Cuvinte-cheie: frumuseþe, linii de cercetare
92 ANDREI HOLMAN
La psychologie de la beauté: Une synthèse
des approches contemporaines
Résumé: Larticle présente une synthèse des approches contemporaines sur le thème de beauté
corporelle, classifiées en huit lignes principales de recherches, chacune illustrée avec un group de
théories et résultats significatives: les approches évolutionniste, socio-culturel et centrée sur
limage corporelle, les recherches sur les standards cross-culturel de beauté, sur ses facteurs,
effets, avantages personnels et représentations dans les media.
Mots-clés: beauté, lignes de recherches
References
Atiyeh, B., Hayek, E. (2008). Numeric Expression of Aesthetics and Beauty. Aesthetic Plastic
Surgery, 32, 209-216.
Becker, D., Kenrick, D., Guerin, S., Maner, J. (2005). Concentrating on Beauty: Sexual
Selection and Sociospatial Memory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(12),
1643-1652.
Berry, B. (2007). Beauty bias: Discrimination and Social Power. Praeger Publishers.
Bjerke, R., Polegato, R. (2006). How Well Do Advertising Images of Health and Beauty Travel
Across Cultures? A Self-Concept Perspective. Psychology & Marketing, 23(10), 865-884.
Buhlmann, U., Teachman, B., Naumann, E., Fehlinger, T., Rief, W. (2009). The meaning of
beauty: Implicit and explicit self-esteem and attractiveness beliefs in body dysmorphic
disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 694-702.
Buss, D. M. (1994). The evolution of desire. New York, Basic Book.
Chatterjee, A., Thomas, A., Smith, S., Aguirre, G. (2009). The Neural Response to Facial
Attractiveness. Neuropsychology, 23(2), 135-143.
Choi, Y., Leshner, G., Choi, J. (2008). Third-Person Effects of Idealized Body Image in
Magazine Advertisements. American Behavioral Scientist, 52(2), 147-164.
Cunningham, M. (1986). Measuring the Physical in Physical Attractiveness: Quasi-Experiments
on the Sociobiology of Female Facial Beauty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
50(5), 925-935.
Dermer, M., Thiel, B. (1975). When Beauty May Fail. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 31(6), 1168-1176.
Eagly, A., Ashmore, R., Makhijani, M., Longo, L. (1991). What Is Beautiful Is Good, But...:
A Meta-Analytic Review of Research on the Physical Attractiveness Stereotype. Psychological
Bulletin, L10(1), 109-128.
Ellis, R., Olson, J., Zanna, M. (1983). Stereotypic personality inferences following objective
versus subjective judgments of beauty. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 15(1).
Englis, B., Solomon, M., Ashmore, R. (1994). Beauty Before the Eyes of Beholders: The
Cultural Encoding of Beauty Types in Magazine Advertising and Music Television.
Journal of Advertising, XXIII (2).
Fink, B., Neave, N. (2005). The biology of facial beauty. International Journal of Cosmetic
Science, 27, 317-325.
Frederick, D., Haselton, M. (2007). Why Is Muscularity Sexy? Tests of the Fitness Indicator
Hypothesis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(8), 1167-1183.
Frith, K., Cheng, H., Shaw, P. (2004). Race and Beauty: A Comparison of Asian and Western
Models in Womens Magazine Advertisements. Sex Roles, 50(1-2).
Garner, D.M., Garfinkel, P.E., Schwartz, D., Thompson, M. (1980). Cultural expectations of
thinness in women. Psychological Reports, 47, 483-491.
93PSYCHOLOGY OF BEAUTY: AN OVERVIEW OF THE CONTEMPORARY...
Gottschall, J. (2008), The Beauty Myth Is No Myth. Human Nature, 19, 174-188.
Griffin, M., Viswanath, K., Schwartz, D. (1994). Gender advertising in the U.S. and India:
Exporting cultural stereotypes. Media, Culture & Society, 16, 487-507.
Grundl, M., Klein, S., Horczakiwskyj, R., Feser, D. (2008). The Jaguars Eye as a New
Beauty Trend? Age-Related Effects in Judging the Attractiveness of the Oblique Eye
Axis. Aesthetic Plastic Surgery, 32(6), 915-919.
Gutierres, S., Kenrick, D., Partch, J. (1999). Beauty, Dominance, and the Mating Game: Contrast
Effects in Self-Assessment Reflect Gender Differences in Mate Selection. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(9), 1126-1134.
Henderson J.J.A., Anglin J.M. (2003). Facial attractiveness predicts longevity. Evol Hum Behav,
24, 351-356.
Hume DK, Montgomerie R. (2001). Facial attractiveness signals different aspects of quality in
women and men. Evol Hum Behav, 22, 93-112.
Jeffrey, S. (2005). Beauty and Misogyny: Harmful cultural practices in the west, Routledge.
Jones, B., De Bruine, L., Little, A., Conway, C., Feinberg, D. (2006). Integrating Gaze
Direction and Expression in Preferences for Attractive Faces. Psychological Science,
17(7), 588-591.
Jung, J., Lennon, S.J. (2003). Body image, appearance self-schema, and media images. Family
and Consumer Sciences Research Journal, 32(1), 27-51.
Langlois, J., Kalakanis, L., Rubenstein, A., Larson, A., Hallam, M., Smoot, M. (2000). Maxims
or Myths of Beauty? A Meta-Analytic and Theoretical Review. Psychological Bulletin,
126(3), 390-423.
Lorenzo, G.L., Biesanz, J.C., Human, L.J. (2010). What is beautiful is good and more accurately
understood: Physical attractiveness and accuracy in first impressions of personality.
Psychological Science, 21, 1777-1782.
Machia, M., Lamb, S. (2009). Sexualized Innocence. Effects of Magazine Ads Portraying Adult
Women as Sexy Little Girls. Journal of Media Psychology, 21(1), 15-24.
Maner, J., Kenrick, D., Becker, V., Delton, A., Hofer, B., Wilbur, C., Neuberg, S. (2003).
Sexually Selective Cognition: Beauty Captures the Mind of the Beholder. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 85 (6), 1107-1120.
Marcus, D., Miller, R. (2003). Sex Differences in Judgments of Physical Attractiveness: A
Social Relations Analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(3), 325-335.
Mulhern, R., Fieldman, G., Hussey, T. (2003). Do cosmetics enhance female Caucasian facial
attractiveness? International Journal of Cosmetic Science, 25, 199-205.
OBarr, W. (1994). Culture and the ad: Exploring otherness in the world of advertising. Boulder,
CO: Westview.
Olson, I., Marshuetz, C. (2005). Facial Attractiveness Is Appraised in a Glance. Emotion, 5(4),
498-502.
Osborn, D. (1996). Beauty is as Beauty Does? Makeup and Posture Effects on Physical Attractiveness
Judgments. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 26(1), 31-51.
Park, B., Flink, C. (1989). A social relations analysis of agreement in liking judgments. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 506-518.
Pinhas, L., Toner, B., Ali, A. (1999). The Effects of the Ideal of Female Beauty on Mood and
Body Satisfaction. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 25(2), 223-226.
Pollard, J., Shepperd, J. (1999). Average Faces Are Average Faces. Current Psychology, 18(1),
98-103.
Poran, M. (2002). Denying diversity: perceptions of beauty and social comparison processes
among Latina, Black, and White women. Sex Roles, 47(1-2), 65-81.
Rhodes, G. (2006). The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty. Annual Review of Psychology,
57, 199-226.
Riggio, R., Widaman, K., Tucker, J., Salinas, C. (1991). Beauty is More Than Skin Deep:
Components of Attractiveness. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 12.
94 ANDREI HOLMAN
Robertson, J. (2001). Japans First Cyborg? Miss Nippon, Eugenics and Wartime Technologies
of Beauty, Body and Blood. Body & Society, 7(1), 1-34.
Saad, G. (2004). Applying Evolutionary Psychology in Understanding the Representation of
Women in Advertisements. Psychology & Marketing, 21(8), 593-612.
Schindler, R., Holbrook, M. (1993). Critical Periods in the Development of Mens and Womens
Tastes in Personal Appearance. Psychology & Marketing, 10(6), 549-564.
Scott, B.A. (1997). Beauty myth beliefs: Theory, measurement, and the use of a new construct.
(Doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota, 1997). Dissertation Abstracts Inter-
national: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 58, 459.
Smith, S., McIntosh, W., Bazzini, D. (1999). Are the Beautiful Good in Hollywood? An
Investigation of the Beauty-and-Goodness Stereotype on Film. Basic and Applied Social
Psychology, 27(1), 69-80.
Sullivan, D.A. (2001). Cosmetic Surgery: The Cutting Edge of Commercial Medicine in America.
Rutgers University Press.
Swami, V., Caprario, C., Tovee, M., Furnham, A. (2006). Female Physical Attractiveness in
Britain and Japan: A Cross-Cultural Study. European Journal of Personality, 20, 69-81.
Sypeck, M., Gray, J., Ahrens, A. (2003). No Longer Just a Pretty Face: Fashion Magazines
Depictions of Ideal Female Beauty from 1959 to 1999. International Journal of Eating
Disorders, 36, 3, 342-347.
Umberson, D., Michael H. (1987). The Impact of Physical Attractiveness in Achievement and
Psychological Well-Being. Social Psychology Quarterly, 50, 227-236.
Wiederrnan, M., Hurst, S. (1997). Physical attractiveness, body image, and womens sexual
self-schema, Psychology of Women Quaterly, 21, 567-580.
Wiseman, C.V., Gray, J.J., Mosimann, J.E., Ahrens, A.H. (1992). Cultural expectations of
thinness in women: An update. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 11, 85-89.
Wolf, N. (1991). The Beauty Myth: How Images of Beauty Are Used Against Women. HarperCollins.
... Debate over what constitutes beauty, Particularly beauty of the human body, has raged since philosophy began (9). The old adage beauty lies in the eye of the beholder (4,21,22). Stipulated that the individual judgements were paramount and needed to be regarded since the assessment of facial attractiveness is very complex (21). Aristotle puts just once benchmark offers a beautiful tragedy and it is when it says, tragedy should not be too long that it does not have enough memory to record, Nor too short, the seriousness out (23). ...
... The harmonic body shop -as perceived by the human eye-is a result of a series of definite numeric relationships between the sizes and positions of various segments of the body (22). ...
... Some theorist believe that feminization of cosmetic surgery is likely to be short-lived historically. Statistics and tends are also confirmed so that is indicated a gradual increase in men and Non-whites (7) Geometrical standards are of particular importance for aesthetic-the medical branch involved in the artificial increase of one's, beauty status (22). ...
... 9 The old age beauty lies in the eye of the beholder. 4,24,25 They stipulated that the individual judgements were paramount and needed to be regarded since the assessment of facial attractiveness is very complex. 25 Aristotle put just once a benchmark offering a beautiful tragedy that should not be neither too long and not to have enough memory to record, nor to be too short. ...
... 28 The harmonic body shop as perceived by the human eye is a result of a series of definite numeric relationships between the sizes and positions of various segments of the body. 24 Furthermore, geometric patterns and the numbers associated with them gave symbolic role to this system with holy concepts that permanent arch type or as jung, draw primordial role in the proper pattern with the artistic language, until being standard for human. 29 The golden ratio also known as the divine proportion, is considered by many to be the key to the secret of aesthetics, attraction and human beauty. ...
Article
Full-text available
BACKGROUND Beauty is a universal phenomenon and debate over what constitutes beauty particularly beauty to human body, has raged since philosophy began. The beauty of individual features depends on “ideal” proportions, and it is suggested that expressing beauty in terms of geometry is possible. Assessment of some used parameters in facial surgeries and harmony of various facial features are essential to surgeon, who requires facial analysis. One of these parameters, is nasolabial angle, in patients undergoing rhinoplasty. This study based on theoretical definitions of beauty and proportions performed the search for the application of this numerical proportions in modern cosmetic surgery. METHODS Twenty-three samples [16 (69.5%) female and 7 (30.5%)] male] were enrolled from patients who underwent rhinoplasty, by a single surgeon. The nasolabial angle was measured in these patients from their lateral profile photographs with adobe Photoshop, before and after surgery. RESULTS Ideal post-operative angle was 111.54±26.5 degrees from this study and 18.8◦ increase in male and 14.68◦ increase in female were seen. There was no significant difference between men and women. CONCLUSION Our results showed that an ideal proportion can be very useful and practical to assess patient’s preoperative expectations and to evaluate the results after surgery and satisfaction of cosmetic surgery process.
Article
Full-text available
This paper aims to analyse the elements of gender ideology and neo-liberal ideology in a corpus composed of three Italian and two Serbian as a foreign language textbooks. The focus of the study is centered on the representation of activities related to the purchase of clothes and accessories, analysed from the perspective of Critical Discourse Analysis. The results indicate that neoliberal values are sustained through the idea that women are naturally predisposed to be clothes and fashion consumers. On a broader scale, the results implicate that the educational materials taken into examination lack a critical pedagogy approach, and that they do not employ the critical and the transformative potential of education, instead they are limited to the reproduction of dominant ideologies.
Article
This paper aims to analyse the elements of gender ideology and neo-liberal ideology in a corpus composed of three Italian and two Serbian as a foreign language textbooks. The focus of the study is centered on the representation of activities related to the purchase of clothes and accessories, analysed from the perspective of Critical Discourse Analysis. The results indicate that neoliberal values are sustained through the idea that women are naturally predisposed to be clothes and fashion consumers. On a broader scale, the results implicate that the educational materials taken into examination lack a critical pedagogy approach, and that they do not employ the critical and the transformative potential of education, instead they are limited to the reproduction of dominant ideologies.
Article
Full-text available
This study examined the complex interrelationships among various static and dynamic components of physical attractiveness and their contributions to overall ratings of attractiveness and to the favorability of initial impressions. Two separate groups of undergraduate students completed a self-report measure of social and communication skills and were videotaped and photographed during initial encounters in a laboratory setting. The videotape segments or photographs were then shown to independent groups of judges who made multiple ratings of subjects' physical attractiveness, initial likability, and attractiveness as a dating partner. A series of structural equation models were fitted to the data. The model that provided the best fit to the data indicated that facial beauty and dynamic components of attractiveness were the most important contributors to overall evaluations of attractiveness, and that overall attractiveness in turn affected the positivity of initial impressions and desirability as a potential dating partner.
Article
Full-text available
Physical attractiveness has been linked to mental health, intelligence, ability and performance. Most of the studies on attractiveness have been experimental in nature and focused on perceptions of mental health and achievement rather than actual mental health and achievement. Operating within a status characteristics framework, we analyze the impact of attractiveness on the actual achievement and mental health of individuals in a national sample. We find consistently significant and monotonic relationships of attractiveness with four measures of achievement and eight measures of psychological well-being. Based on these analyses, we conclude that survey research findings corroborate experimental findings on attractiveness; that one's attractiveness does impinge on achievement and psychological well-being; and that status characteristics theory can be used to explain the effects of attractiveness on well-being and achievement.
Article
Full-text available
Although researchers have conceptualized beauty as unidimensional, modern-day cultural definitions of beauty are multidimensional. This paper focuses on two forms of mass media that play an important role in transmitting information about multiple and diverse cultural ideals of beauty—fashion magazine advertising and music videos shown on music television. We examine the overall prevalence of different beauty ideals and how these are distributed across specific vehicles within each medium. We also compare the emphasis placed on different ideals of beauty across these two communications media (print vs. television) and formats (advertising vs. entertainment). The implications of the findings for understanding the cultural construction of beauty ideals and for understanding how advertising and other mass media communications contribute to them are discussed. Several extensions of the present research are proposed.
Article
Physically attractive individuals are often viewed more favorably than unattractive people on dimensions that are weakly related or unrelated to physical looks, such as intelligence, sociability, and morality. Our study investigated the role of U.S. films in this "beauty-and-goodness" stereotype. In Study 1, we established that attractive characters were portrayed more favorably than unattractive characters on multiple dimensions (e.g., intelligence, friendliness) across a random sample from 5 decades of top-grossing films. The link between beauty and positive characteristics was stable across time periods, character sex, and characters' centrality to the plot. Study 2 established that exposure to highly stereotyped films can elicit stronger beauty-and-goodness stereotyping. Participants watching a highly biased film subsequently showed greater favoritism toward an attractive graduate school candidate (compared with ratings of an unattractive candidate) than participants viewing a less biased film.
Article
This review demonstrates that the physical attractiveness stereotype established by studies of person perception is not as strong or general as suggested by the often-used summary phrase what is beautiful is good. Although subjects in these studies ascribed more favorable personality traits and more successful life outcomes to attractive than unattractive targets, the average magnitude of this beauty-is-good effect was moderate, and the strength of the effect varied considerably from study to study. Consistent with our implicit personality theory framework, a substantial portion of this variation was explained by the specific content of the inferences that subjects were asked to make: The differences in subjects' perception of attractive and unattractive targets were largest for indexes of social competence; intermediate for potency, adjustment, and intellectual competence; and near zero for integrity and concern for others. The strength of the physical attractiveness stereotype also varied as a function of other attributes of the studies, including the presence of individuating information.
Article
An investigation of current American society's depiction of the ideal female body was performed. Body measurements of Playboy magazine centerfolds and Miss America contestants for 1979-1988 indicated body weight 13-19% below expected weight for women in that age group. Miss America contestants showed a significant decrease in expected weight between 1979 and 1988. Comparisons were made with an earlier study which had demonstrated that body measurements of both groups had decreased during the period 1959-1978. Diet-for-weight-loss and exercise articles in six women's magazines were tabulated for 1959-1988. A significant increase in both diet articles and exercise articles occurred during this period. These findings suggest that the overvaluation of thinness continues and thinness is now sought through both dieting and exercise.
Article
Cosmetic surgery is big business. With demand rising, this commercial medical practice has become a modern body custom. To explain the emergence and growth of this demand, Deborah A. Sullivan looks beyond the cultural imperatives of appearance and examines the market dynamics inherent in the business and politics of cosmetic surgery. In so doing, she also considers the effect of commercialization on the medical profession. After reviewing prevailing beauty trends, Sullivan looks at the social, psychological and economic rewards and penalties resulting from the way we look. Following an historical overview of the technological advances that made cosmetic surgery possible she explores the relationship between improved surgical techniques and the resulting increased demand. The book also examines the ensuing conflict within the profession over recognition of commercial cosmetic surgery as a specialty. Among the topics covered are sensitive areas such as physician advertising, unregulated practice and ambulatory surgery, and the consequences of commercialism on medical judgement. Finally, it reveals how physicians and their professional organizations have shaped the ways in which cosmetic surgery is presented in advertisements and women's magazines that would promote patient demand.
Book
Should western beauty practices, ranging from lipstick to labiaplasty, be included within the United Nations understandings of harmful traditional/cultural practices? By examining the role of common beauty practices in damaging the health of women, creating sexual difference, and enforcing female deference, this book argues that they should. In the 1970s feminists criticized pervasive beauty regimes such as dieting and depilation, but some 'new' feminists argue that beauty practices are no longer oppressive now that women can 'choose' them. However, in the last two decades the brutality of western beauty practices seems to have become much more severe, requiring the breaking of skin, spilling of blood and rearrangement or amputation of body parts. Beauty and Misogyny seeks to make sense of why beauty practices are not only just as persistent, but in many ways more extreme. It examines the pervasive use of makeup, the misogyny of fashion and high-heeled shoes, and looks at the role of pornography in the creation of increasingly popular beauty practices such as breast implants, genital waxing and surgical alteration of the labia. It looks at the cosmetic surgery and body piercing/cutting industries as being forms of self-mutilation by proxy, in which the surgeons and piercers serve as proxies to harm women's bodies, and concludes by considering how a culture of resistance to these practices can be created. This essential work will appeal to students and teachers of feminist psychology, gender studies, cultural studies, and feminist sociology at both undergraduate and postgraduate levels, and to anyone with an interest in feminism, women and beauty, and women's health.
Article
In June 1931, on the eve of the invasion of Manchuria, the Japanese mass media announced the winner of the first Miss Nippon contest. Applicants were limited to rank amateurs whose photographs, and not bodies, were judged by a panel of mostly elderly men who regarded the contest as `culture work'. A second contest was held in 1934. One of the main objectives of the Miss Nippon contests was to locate and record photographically, young women whose allegedly `pure blood' and ideal bodily proportions demonstrated the success of eugenics and social hygiene programs launched decades earlier. Japan's imperialist aggression in Asia and the Pacific forms the backdrop to this study on the centrality of popular cultural events and media in the state's attempt to improve the `Japanese race' through technologies of eugenics and social hygiene. The article explores the politics of blood, body and beauty, three interrelated subjects, framed by the Miss Nippon contests, that were common to the colonial project and the discourse of eugenics alike.