ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Recent research has shown that compassionate feelings for the suffering environment promote conservation of nature. We extend this notion and relate compassion for suffering humans to proenvironmental tendencies. The proposed relation should hold true as compassion elicits moral actions and judgments across different moral domains which should also be applicable to the environment. Therefore, we expect compassion for other humans to relate positively to proenvironmental tendencies. Two studies were conducted to test this assumption. Study 1 included three independent samples (final N = 2,096) and several measures of proenvironmental tendencies. Results revealed that compassion was indeed positively related to proenvironmental values, proenvironmental intentions and reported donations to nature or environmental organizations. In Study 2 we experimentally tested and found a causal path between compassion for humans and proenvironmental intentions. Implications for climate change and protection of nature are discussed.
1
Running Head: COMPASSION AND PROENVIRONMENTAL TENDENCIES
Feelings for the suffering of others and the environment:
Compassion fosters proenvironmental tendencies
Stefan Pfattheicher1, Claudia Sassenrath1,2, & Simon Schindler3
1 Ulm University, Germany, 2 Knowledge Media Research Center, Tübingen, Germany,
3 Kassel University, Germany
Address correspondence to
Stefan Pfattheicher
Universität Ulm
Abteilung Sozialpsychologie
89069 Ulm, Germany
stefan.pfattheicher@uni-ulm.de
+49-(0)731/50 31161
2
ABSTRACT
Recent research has shown that compassionate feelings for the suffering environment promote
conservation of nature. We extend this notion and relate compassion for suffering humans to
proenvironmental tendencies. The proposed relation should hold true as compassion elicits moral
actions and judgments across different moral domains which should also be applicable to the
environment. Therefore, we expect compassion for other humans to relate positively to
proenvironmental tendencies. Two studies were conducted to test this assumption. Study 1
included three independent samples (final N = 2,096) and several measures of proenvironmental
tendencies. Results revealed that compassion was indeed positively related to proenvironmental
values, proenvironmental intentions and reported donations to nature or environmental
organizations. In Study 2 we experimentally tested and found a causal path between compassion
for humans and proenvironmental intentions. Implications for climate change and protection of
nature are discussed.
Word count: 139
Keywords: compassion; conservation; environment; environmental concern; morality;
proenvironmental tendencies
3
INTRODUCTION
In recent years, other-oriented tendencies have been shown to be an important factor for
proenvironmental behavior (e.g., Berenguer, 2007, 2010; Hopper & Nielsen, 1991; Schultz, 2001,
2002; Tam, 2013). That is to say, along with other basic motivations (i.e., self-interest and justice
concerns; e.g., Griskevicius, Tybur, & Den Bergh, 2010; Parris, Hegtvedt, Watson, & Johnson,
2014), other-oriented tendencies have been considered to foster proenvironmental behavior in a
conclusive way. The present work investigates one important other-oriented tendency, that is,
compassion for other individuals, with regard to proenvironmental tendencies. It has already been
shown that specific compassion for the environment relates to proenvironmental intentions (e.g.,
Berenguer, 2007, 2010; Tam, 2013). We extend this notion and relate compassion for the suffering
of other individuals to proenvironmental tendencies. Indeed, Tam (2013) recently claimed that the
relation between compassion for humans and environmental behavior needs investigation to
acquire a better theoretical understanding of this relation. In this sense, the present research
addresses a gap in this field in that we study whether compassion for other individuals influence
proenvironmental tendencies.
Specifically, in the present work we argue that compassion for other individuals should
lead to proenvironmental tendencies as compassion guides moral actions and judgments across
different moral domains which should be also applicable to the environment (Goetz et al., 2010;
Haidt, 2003; McCullough, Kilpatrick, Emmons, & Larson, 2001). Therefore, we expect
compassion for other individuals to relate positively to proenvironmental tendencies. The rationale
for this assumption is outlined in detail below.
COMPASSION AND CONCERN FOR THE SUFFERING OF OTHERS
We define compassion in line with Lazarus (1991) who emphasized that the core theme of
compassion is being moved by another’s suffering (see also Haidt, 2003). Accordingly, the
4
essence of compassion is feelings that are elicited in response to suffering others (for a compelling
review on compassion, see Goetz, Keltner, & Simon-Thomas, 2010).
It is noteworthy that other terms that have been used in this context are “empathic concern”
or “empathy” in general, instead of compassion. In fact, empathic concern is often used
interchangeably with compassion (cf. Batson, 2009; Goetz et al., 2010; Singer & Klimeki, 2014).
Basically, empathy can be considered to be a more general construct including perspective taking,
emotional sharing, and a concern for suffering others (i.e., compassion; Decety & Cowell, 2014).
Thus, compassion constitutes one dimension of empathy, while empathy consists of additional
dimensions (Batson, 2009; Singer & Klimeki, 2014). In the present work, we used the term
compassion rather than the general term empathy in order to make explicit that the present work
focuses on feelings for the suffering of others.
As a consequence of compassion, a “prosocial action tendency” is elicited (Haidt, 2003)
especially when the suffering other deserves help and one has the resources to help (Goetz et al.,
2010). Hence, compassionate individuals follow the ultimate moral goal of preventing and
relieving the suffering of others as well as improving others’ welfare. In this regard, compassion
has been shown to be associated with a concern for humanity (Goetz et al., 2010; Sprecher & Fehr,
2005). This applies to, for instance, helping vulnerable others, volunteerism, and donations to a
common group project (Batson, O’Quin, Fultz, Vanderplas, Isen, 1983; Eisenberg, McCreath, &
Ahn, 1988; Eisenberg et al., 1989; Omoto, Malsch, & Barraza, 2009). Compassion should not be
restricted to the suffering of humans but should also apply to the suffering of other species (e.g.,
Mehrabian & Epstein, 1972; Tam, 2013). That is to say, compassion elicits moral judgments and
actions across different moral domains (Goetz et al., 2010; Haidt, 2003; McCullough et al., 2001).
Consequently, compassion is included in the spectrum of basic moral emotions (Haidt, 2003).
Regarding proenvironmental tendencies, a concern for the suffering of nature (e.g., a
suffering tree) strengthened environmental attitudes and the moral obligation to help nature.
5
(Berenguer, 2007). Recently, Tam (2013) proposed that empathy with suffering nature is one
fundamental factor in predicting proenvironmental behavior. Schultz (2001, see also Stern, Dietz,
& Kalof, 1993) suggested that biospheric environmental concerns, that is, individuals’ concern for
plants, animals, marine life, and birds predicts proenvironmental behavior. That is to say, a
concern for the environment (i.e., the other-oriented tendencies of environmental concern,
empathy and feelings for nature) promotes engagement in proenvironmental tendencies (Karpiak
& Baril, 2008; Gosling & Wiliams, 2010; Mayer & McPherson Frantz, 2004; Milfont & Sibley,
2012; Raymond, Brown, & Robinson, 2011; Schultz, 2001, 2002; Stern et al., 1993; Tam, 2013).
Building on the notion that compassion elicits moral judgments and actions across different
moral domains (Goetz et al., 2010; Haidt, 2003; McCullough et al., 2001), we assume that
compassion elicits the morality-driven motivation to prevent and to relieve suffering and to
improve others’ welfare which should also include nature and the environment. As a consequence,
we expect compassion for humans to relate positively to proenvironmental tendencies. These
assumptions are tested in two empirical studies as reported below.
STUDY 1
In Study 1 we hypothesized that dispositional compassion for humans is positively related
to proenvironmental tendencies. In this study, different proenvironmental tendencies were
measured which was done in three independent samples. Specifically, we assessed: (a)
individuals’ dispositional compassionate tendencies for other humans (in all samples), (b)
proenvironmental values (in Sample 1), (c) proenvironmental intentions (in Sample 2), and (d)
whether participants report having donated money to nature or environmental organizations (in
Sample 3).
Method
Participants. Study 1 involved two samples of students from a German university (Sample
1: N = 101; Mage = 20.6; 85.1% females; Sample 2: N = 60; Mage = 22.9; 56.7% females). Sample 3
6
consists of a representative study (the LISS Panel) taken in the Netherlands (N = 1.935; Mage =
51.7; 53.4% females). The participants in Sample 1 completed the measures at a computer
laboratory at the university; Sample 2 completed a paper/pencil study; Sample 3 completed an
online survey. Unless indicated otherwise, all scale endpoints of self-report items were labelled
(1) not at all true and (7) completely true.
Compassion. We measured dispositional compassion using an emotional empathy scale
(see also Saslow et al., 2013, for this approach). In all samples, an adapted version of the
emotional empathy scale developed by Mehrabian and Epstein (1972) was used to assess
compassion (for validation of the scale and items see Keller & Pfattheicher, 2013). A sample item
of the nine-item scale reads: “It makes me sad to see a lonely stranger in a group; (MSample1 =
5.36, SDSample1 = 0.85, α = .81; MSample2 = 4.98, SDSample2 = 0.76, α = .75; MSample3 = 4.39, SDSample3
= 1.10, α = .87).
Proenvironmental values. In Sample 1, we assessed basic individual values as proposed by
Schwartz et al. (2012). The value that is particularly relevant here is “universalism–nature” which
reflects individuals’ concern for preserving the environment (M = 3.32, SD = 1.23, α = .91). A
sample item of the three-item scale reads: “Caring for nature is very important to him/her” (results
involving the other values are available on request).
Proenvironmental intentions. In Sample 2, proenvironmental intentions were assessed
using an adapted version of the environmental behavior scale (Schultz & Zelezny, 1998; Schultz,
Zelezny, & Dalrymple, 2000). The original scale relates to behavior in the past (e.g., “I often
looked for ways to reuse things”). We rephrased the items slightly in order to obtain a measure for
future proenvironmental behavior (M = 5.24, SD = 0.98, α = .83), thereby allowing the
measurement of proenvironmental intentions in experiments (as in Study 2). A sample item of the
eight-item scale reads: In future, I will look for ways to reuse things”.
7
Proenvironmental donations. In Sample 3, the representative sample, participants were
asked to indicate whether they donate money to one or more nature or environmental
organizations”. They could answer this question by stating yes (42.2%) or no (57.8%). Due to the
panel character of the LISS study, proenvironmental donations were assessed in October 2009;
compassion in this sample was assessed in February 2011.1
Social desirability. In Sample 3, social desirability was assessed via the Crowne-Marlowe
Scale (1960; alpha = .52) in May 2011. This measure was included in our analyses to rule out the
possibility that the relation between compassion and proenvironmental tendencies is merely a
result of socially desirable responding (cf. Batson, 2011). This is particularly important given that
proenvironmental behavior was self-reported. A sample item of the ten-item scale reads: “I have
never intensely disliked anyone”. The items could be answered by stating yes (1) or no (0). The
sum score (min = 0, max = 10) had a mean of 5.91 (SD = 1.96).
Results and Discussion
Compassion was shown to be significantly positively correlated with proenvironmental
values, r(99) = .28, p < .01, and proenvironmental intentions, r(58) = .27, p < .05. Compassion
could also significantly predict proenvironmental donations in a logistic regression (B = 0.17, SE =
.04, p < .001, Nagelkerke’s R² = .011), that is, the stronger a participant’s dispositional
compassion the higher the chance that they would donate to one or more nature or environmental
organizations. Including the factor of social desirability in this analysis did not change the
coefficient of compassion (B = 0.17, SE = .04, p < .001). Social desirability was not significantly
related to proenvironmental donations (B = .15, SE = .24, p = .52).
These results consistently support our hypothesis that compassion for the suffering of
others positively relates to proenvironmental tendencies beyond socially desirable responding.
Regarding the correlations, we found a medium effect size (Cohen, 1988) which corresponds to
the average effect size in social psychology estimated across 100 years (Richard et al., 2003).
8
As a next step we tested the causal relation between compassion and proenvironmental
tendencies. Therefore, in Study 2 compassion was manipulated and proenvironmental tendencies
were subsequently assessed. Before we report this study we introduce and validate an easily
applicable manipulation to induce compassion, which was then applied in Study 2.
VALIDATION STUDY
Method
Participants and procedure. Sixty-eight students from a German university (Mage = 24.92,
SD = 5.55, 71.1% females) participated in an online study.
Manipulation of compassion. Our manipulation of compassion was based on two works:
Batson et al. (1997) and Oveis, Horberg, and Keltner (2010). Oveis and colleagues (2010; see also
Oveis, Cohen, Gruber, Shiota, Haidt, & Keltner, 2009) induced compassion using fifteen
emotional pictures displaying suffering individuals. We used two of these pictures, a homeless
person leaning against the wall of a house and a diseased child, ensuring that none of the pictures
included any reference to the environment and nature. In our validation study, all participants were
presented each picture for ten seconds. Participants were randomly assigned to either the high-
compassion condition (n = 34) or the low-compassion condition (n = 34). In the high-compassion
conditions, before viewing the pictures participants read compassion promoting instructions for
how they should view the pictures (cf. Batson et al., 1997, Study 3; Batson, Chang, Orr, &
Rowland, 2002). Participants read “It is important for the study that you imagine how the pictured
persons feel. Try to feel what the persons are currently going through and how they feel. You can
let yourself be guided by your feelings.” In the low-compassion condition, participants read “It is
important for the study that you stay objective when viewing the pictures. Try not to let yourself
be guided by your feelings. That is, try to stay neutral and detached.”
Dependent variables. To check whether the introduced manipulation actually produces a
difference in state compassion between the two conditions, we measured state compassion directly
9
after the presentation of the two pictures. Participants rated each item on a seven-point Likert scale
to indicate how they currently felt. We used the five items for measuring compassionate states
taken from Batson and colleagues (Batson, 1987, 1991; Batson et al., 1995): sympathetic, tender,
soft-hearted, compassionate, and moved. These items (α = .91) were averaged to compose a
compassion index. In order to present the compassion items in a meaningful context and to reduce
demand effects, we embedded the compassion items into the ten-item short form of the Positive
= .56) and Negative (α = .62) Affect Schedule (PANAS; Mackinnon et al., 1999). Given that the
item “moved” of the negative subscale of the PANAS is already part of the compassion index, it
was not considered in the negative subscale of the PANAS.
Results and Discussion
Analysis revealed a significant difference (t(66) = 2.79, p < .01; Cohen’s d = .67) in state
compassion between the high-compassion condition (M = 4.76, SD = 1.08) and the low-
compassion condition (M = 3.89, SD = 1.46). There was no significant difference (t(66) = 1.66, p
= .11) in negative affect (high-compassion condition, M = 2.69, SD = 1.05; low-compassion
condition, M = 2.29, SD = 0.94; Cohen’s d = .40) and positive affect (t(66) = 1.16, p = .25; high-
compassion condition, M = 2.61, SD = 0.72; low-compassion condition, M = 2.84, SD = 0.91;
Cohen’s d = .28).
The results indicate a successful manipulation of compassion. The aim of the study was to
develop an easily applicable and valid method to manipulate compassion. The findings of the
validation study speak to the applicability of the manipulation. We applied this manipulation in the
study reported below to test the hypothesis that state compassion leads to proenvironmental
tendencies.
10
STUDY 2
Method
Participants and procedure. Ninety-four students from a German university (Mage = 22.97,
SD = 2.58, 46.8% females) participated in this paper-pencil study.
Manipulation of compassion. We applied the manipulation of compassion as outlined in
the validation study above, resulting in a high-compassion condition and a low-compassion
condition. Participants were randomly assigned to either the high-compassion condition (n = 47)
or the low-compassion condition (n = 47). Instead of presenting each picture for precisely ten
seconds as in our (online) validation study, we asked participants to look at each picture for
approximately ten seconds.
Given the findings of the Validation Study, which suggest that our approach reflects a
successful manipulation of compassion, we did not again include the compassion state items of the
Validation Study in this study. The choice to not include the manipulation check items is in line
with research showing that even simple measurements of psychological states (e.g., manipulation
checks) can bias subsequence measurements (Kühnen, 2010).
Dependent variable. Subsequent to the manipulation of compassion, proenvironmental
intentions (α = .83) were assessed as in Study 1.
Results and Discussion
Analysis revealed a significant difference between the two conditions, t(92) = 2.70, p < .01
(Cohen’s d = .55). Participants in the high-compassion condition reported more proenvironmental
intentions (M = 5.15, SD = 0.90) compared with those in the low-compassion condition (M = 4.62,
SD = 1.01). Hence, in line with our hypothesis that compassion promotes proenvironmental
tendencies, the present results extend the findings of Study 1 by establishing a causal relation
between compassion and proenvironmental tendencies.
11
GENERAL DISCUSSION
The present work has investigated one important other-oriented tendency, namely
compassion for other humans, with regard to proenvironmental tendencies. Specifically, in two
studies we documented that compassion is positively related to proenvironmental tendencies. Most
importantly, we could establish a causal path between compassion and proenvironmental
tendencies indicating that compassion for other individuals indeed promotes proenvironmental
tendencies. These findings take into consideration a recent claim by Tam (2013) that the relation
between compassion for humans and environmental tendencies need investigation.
Overall, the present research is in line with approaches highlighting other-oriented
tendencies as crucial factors fostering proenvironmental tendencies. For instance, Berenguer
(2007; 2010) and Tam (2013) have proposed that a specific concern for the suffering of nature
fosters proenvironmental tendencies. In the present work, we extend this notion and show that
individuals report stronger proenvironmental tendencies in the high-compassion condition
compared with the low-compassion condition. Accordingly, our work extends the conceptually
close relation of a concern for nature and proenvironmental behavior.
Beyond these contributions to the field of other-oriented tendencies and proenvironmental
behavior, several other points are noteworthy. First, we document the relation between
compassion and proenvironmental tendencies in several independent samples thus acknowledging
recent claims emphasizing replication in psychology (e.g., IJzerman, Brandt, & Van Wolferen,
2013; Makel, Plucker, & Hegarty, 2012). Second, we show the relation between compassion and
proenvironmental tendencies across different measures of proenvironmental tendencies. Thus, it
seems reasonable to assume that our conclusion of compassion being related to proenvironmental
tendencies is not the result of a specific measure of proenvironmental tendencies. This speaks to
the validity of the relation. Third, we document that compassion relates to proenvironmental
12
tendencies in a representative sample which was unaffected by socially desirable response
tendencies. As such, the findings seem to hold for a general population and not only for students.
In critically reflecting on the current work, we want to acknowledge the fact that some of
the findings rely on self-report by the participants and no real observed behavior. Self-report
instruments only provide access to information about people’s beliefs about themselves. Also,
there is evidence that self-report instruments including affective response towards stimuli (such as
dispositional compassion) do not align with momentary affective experiences (Feldman Barrett,
1997; Robinson & Clore, 2002). We could handle the issue of self-reported compassion in Study 1
by inducing compassion in Study 2. Yet, proenvironmental intentions are still self-reported and it
is unclear whether participants correctly forecast their intentions and act in a more
proenvironmental way after compassion for humans is induced (cf. Wilson & Gilbert, 2003).
Nonetheless, intentions have been shown to be an important basis for the emergence of real
behavior (cf. Ajzen, 1985). Additionally, in Study 1, Sample 3, participants report whether they
donate money to one or more nature or environmental organization. In sum, while future research
can extend the present research by showing the link between compassion for humans and
proenvironmental behavior, the present contribution represents a meaningful foundation in this
regard.
We also want to address another issue. The duration for which participants viewed the
pictures inducing compassion (or not) might have differed between the Validation Study and
Study 2. Given that the Validation Study was an online study we were able to program the precise
duration the two pictures were displayed on the screen (i.e., 10 seconds). The same pictures were
used in Study 2 which was a paper/pencil study. Here, it was technically impossible to ensure that
the pictures were displayed for exactly ten seconds. Therefore, we asked participants “to look at
each picture for approximately ten seconds.” Although there is no guarantee that participants
followed the instructions, it seems likely that participants dealt with the pictures appropriately
13
because the study was conducted under controlled laboratory conditions (i.e., no external
distraction) and both the high- and the low-compassion condition included a statement
highlighting the importance of viewing the pictures as instructed (see instructions in the Method
section of the Validation Study).
We have proposed that compassion includes a moral concern which should be applicable to
nature and the environment. Beyond that, others processes are possible, which is in line with
recent theorizing emphasizing a multiple process perspective (Bullock, Green, & Ha, 2010;
Fiedler, Schott, & Meiser, 2011; Hayes, 2013). Thus, it seems reasonable to assume that several
routes can lead to specific behavior. Hence, it could be possible that compassion, defined as a
reaction to negative, baleful experiences others are undergoing, implies some sort of negativity
bias also affecting proenvironmental tendencies (Keller & Pfattheicher, 2013). Specifically, if
compassion implies the processing of the negative experiences of other entities then
compassionate individuals might be more likely to consider the potential negative impact that their
non-proenvironmental behavior might have on other entities. Hence, a heightened awareness of
potentially negative developments for others might also contribute to the effect that compassion
has on proenvironmental tendencies.
It is also plausible that “incidental affect” or “incidental emotion” can explain the results.
Specifically, it has been shown that emotional states in one situation can carry over to another
unrelated situation and impact judgment and decision-making (e.g., Bodenhausen, 1993). There is
research on compassion showing carry-over effects of compassion from one situation to another
unrelated one (Condon & DeSteno, 2011; Oveis et al., 2010). Therefore, it is well possible in the
present case that compassion for humans might carry over to the unrelated situation of a concern
for the environment which would result in strengthened proenvironmental tendencies.
We have argued that compassion elicits moral judgments and actions across different
moral domains (Goetz et al., 2010; Haidt, 2003; McCullough, Kilpatrick, Emmons, & Larson,
14
2001). From this basis we assumed that compassion elicits the morality-driven motivation to
prevent and to relieve suffering which should also be applicable to nature and the environment.
Building on these considerations, it seems possible that political orientations could moderate the
association between compassion for humans and proenvironmental tendencies. Research by
Graham, Haidt, and Nosek (2009) on the basis of moral foundations theory (e.g., Haidt & Graham,
2007; Haidt & Joseph, 2007) shows that liberals endorse a stronger harm/care and
fairness/reciprocity sensitivity compared to conservatives. Conservatives display three other sets
of moral intuitions more strongly than liberals (i.e., ingroup/loyalty, authority/respect, and
purity/sanctity). In this regard one could predict that the compassion-for-individuals and
compassion-for-nature link is more pronounced in liberals given that these individuals are
particularly harm sensitive (which corresponds to compassion for suffering humans) as well as
caring sensitive (which corresponds to a concern for the environment and proenvironmental
tendencies). Unfortunately, political orientations were not measured in our studies but it seems
likely that the majority of participants possess liberal attitudes (many are students at a German
university). Thus, the composition of the sample might have provided a basis for the association
between compassion for humans and proenvironmental tendencies. Still, it is an interesting open
question and future research can test political orientations as a moderator.
One could also discuss whether seeing nature suffer is a precondition for the relation of
compassion and proenvironmental tendencies. Indeed, in Study 2, it was not explicitly stated that
nature was suffering. Nonetheless, we found that compassion for other humans promotes
proenvironmental intentions. On this basis one could assume that compassion leads to
proenvironmental tendencies without the suffering of nature being explicitly salient. This is in line
with the conceptualization of compassion as a basic moral emotion (Haidt, 2003) and congruent
with the notion that compassion elicits moral actions and judgments across different moral
domains including proenvironmental tendencies as shown in the present studies.
15
In addition, it would be relevant for future research to investigate what subjects/objects can
be the target of compassion. Research by Epley and colleagues (Epley, Akalis, Waytz, &
Cacioppo, 2008; Epley, Waytz, & Cacioppo, 2007) has shown that individuals attribute human
mental characteristics and emotions to non-human animals and gadgets (i.e.,
anthropomorphization). This notion is compatible with the findings by Tam (2013) and Berenguer
(2007, 2010) which show that compassion applies to a wide spectrum of targets including
compassion for suffering trees and animals. However, one may speculate that compassion is
restricted to “living” targets (e.g., trees and animals) and not “non-living” targets such as the air or
stones. In general, it is an open question whether compassion promotes proenvironmental
tendencies as an ultimate goal, that is, whether compassionate individuals anthropomorphize
nature and benefit nature as an agent (e.g., Mother Earth; Ahn, Kim, & Aggarwal, 2014; Tam,
Lee, & Chao, 2013; Sacci, Riva, & Brambilla, 2013), or whether compassion promotes
proenvironmental tendencies as an instrumental goal, that is, whether compassionate individuals
benefit nature in order to benefit the lives of others.
As a final step, we want to discuss the applied value of the present contribution. That is,
the present work is also relevant in terms of implications for climate change and protection of
nature. So far, research has shown that specific compassion for the environment fosters
proenvironmental tendencies (e.g., Berenguer, 2007, 2010; Tam, 2013). Building on the present
work we provide evidence that feelings for the suffering of humans (i.e., compassion) also
promotes proenvironmental tendencies. Thus, speakers (e.g., in front of the United Nations) or
climate change campaigns (e.g., Greenpeace) that aim to mobilize people to protect nature may
also include suffering humans in their demonstrations to elicit compassion which in turn may
mobilize people to protect nature.
Furthermore, the majority of people are likely aware that damaging the environment is bad
and acting proenvironmentally is desirable (Gifford, 2014). However, irrespective of the
16
knowledge people possess, proenvironmental tendencies can still be strengthened. The present
contribution offers an affective perspective on how to strengthen proenvironmental tendencies. We
suggest that individuals’ proenvironmental motivation to protect nature can be increased by
inducing the emotion of compassion. That is to say, we suggest increasing moral behavior in the
domain of nature protection by activating the moral emotion of compassion.
To conclude, the present work has highlighted that other-oriented tendencies such as
compassion for others effectively strengthen tendencies related to the conservation of nature. As
such, the findings of the present work are important in the context of climate change because they
suggest that general other-oriented tendencies such as compassion can successfully be used to
promote proenvironmental behavior.
17
FOOTNOTES
1 Of note, the LISS study also included two items related to proenvironmental tendencies. We did
not include the items of (a) whether individuals “performed voluntary work for an
organization for environmental protection, peace, or animal rights” because it was not
restricted to proenvironmental tendencies, (b) “I am involved with one or more nature or
environment organizations.” because prevalence was fairly low and therefore variance was
restricted (only 16.4% report that they are involved with one or more nature or environment
organizations).
18
REFERENCES
Ahn, H. K., Kim, H. J., & Aggarwal, P. (2014). Helping fellow beings anthropomorphized social
causes and the role of anticipatory guilt. Psychological Science, 25, 224-229.
Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to action: A theory of planned behavior. In J. Kuhl & J.
Beckman (Eds.), Action control: From cognitions to behaviors (pp. 11-39). New York:
Springer.
Batson, C. D. (1987). Prosocial motivation: Is it ever truly altruistic? In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 20, pp. 65-122). New York: Academic
Press.
Batson, C. D. (1991). The altruism question: Toward a social-psychological answer. Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Batson, C. D. (2009). These things called empathy. In J. Decety & W. Ickes (Eds.), The social
neuroscience of empathy (pp. 16-31). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Batson, C. D. (2011). Altruism in humans. New York: Oxford University Press.
Batson, C. D., Batson, J. G., Todd, R. M., Brummett, B. H., Shaw, L. L., & Aldeguer, C. M.
(1995). Empathy and the collective good: Caring for one of the others in a social dilemma.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 619-631.
Batson, C. D., Chang, J., Orr, R., & Rowland, J. (2002). Empathy, attitudes, and action: Can
feeling for a member of a stigmatized group motivate one to help the group? Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1656-1666.
Batson, C. D., O’Quin, K., Fultz, J., Vanderplas, M., & Isen, A. M. (1983). Influence of self-
reported distress and empathy on egoistic versus altruistic motivation to help. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 706-718.
Batson, C. D., Polycarpou, M. P., Harmon-Jones, E., Imhoff, H. J., Mitchener, E. C., Bednar, L.
L., … Highberger, L. (1997). Empathy and attitudes: Can feeling for a member of a
stigmatized group improve feelings toward the group? Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 72, 105-118.
19
Berenguer, J. (2007). The effect of empathy in proenvironmental attitudes and
behaviors. Environment and Behavior, 39, 269-283.
Berenguer, J. (2010). The effect of empathy in environmental moral reasoning. Environment and
Behavior, 42, 110-134.
Bodenhausen, G. V. (1993). Emotion, arousal and stereotypic judgment: A heuristic model of
affect and stereotyping. In D. Mackie & D. Hamilton (Eds.), Affect, cognition and
stereotyping: Interactive processes in intergroup perception (pp. 13-37). San Diego, CA:
Academic Press.
Bullock, J. G., Green, D. P., & Ha, S. E. (2010). Yes, but what’s the mechanism? (Don’t expect an
easy answer). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 550-558.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Condon, P., & DeSteno, D. (2011). Compassion for one reduces punishment for another. Journal
of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 698-701.
Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent of
psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 349-354.
Decety, J., & Cowell, J. M. (2014). Friends or foes: Is empathy necessary for moral behavior?
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 525-537.
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Miller, P. A., Fultz, J., Shell, R., Mathy, R. M., & Reno, R. R. (1989).
Relation of sympathy and personal distress to prosocial behavior: A multimethod study.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 55-66.
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Murphy, B., Karbon, M., Maszk, P., Smith, M., Suh, K. (1994).
The relations of emotionality and regulation to dispositional and situational empathy-related
responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 776-797.
Eisenberg, N., McCreath, H., & Ahn, R. (1988). Vicarious emotional responsiveness and prosocial
behavior: Their interrelations in young children. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
14, 298-311.
20
Epley, N., Akalis, S., Waytz, A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). Creating social connection through
inferential reproduction loneliness and perceived agency in gadgets, gods, and greyhounds.
Psychological Science, 19, 114-120.
Epley, N., Waytz, A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2007). On seeing human: A three-factor theory of
anthropomorphism. Psychological Review, 114, 864-886.
Feldman Barrett, L. (1997). The relationships among momentary emotion experience, personality
descriptions, and retrospective ratings of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 23, 1100-1110.
Fiedler, K., Schott, M., & Meiser, T. (2011). What mediation analysis can (not) do. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 1231-1236.
Gifford, R. (2014). Environmental psychology matters. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 541-
579.
Goetz, J., Keltner, D., & Simon-Thomas, E. (2010). Compassion: An evolutionary analysis and
empirical review. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 351-374.
Gosling, E., & Williams, K. J. (2010). Connectedness to nature, place attachment and conservation
behaviour: Testing connectedness theory among farmers. Journal of Environmental
Psychology, 30, 298-304.
Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of
moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 1029-1046.
Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., & Den Bergh, B. V. (2010). Going green to be seen: Status,
reputation, and conspicuous conservation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98,
392-404.
Haidt, J. (2003). The moral emotions. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.),
Handbook of Affective Sciences (pp. 852-870). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2007). When morality opposes justice: Conservatives have moral
intuitions that liberals may not recognize. Social Justice Research, 20, 98-116.
21
Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (2007). The moral mind: How 5 sets of innate intuitions guide the
development of many culture-specific virtues, and perhaps even modules. In P. Carruthers, S.
Laurence, & S. Stich (Eds.), The Innate Mind (Vol. 3, pp. 367-391). New York: Oxford.
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis.
New York: The Guilford Press.
Hopper, J. R., & Nielsen, J. M. (1991). Recycling as altruistic behavior normative and behavioral
strategies to expand participation in a community recycling program. Environment and
Behavior, 23, 195-220.
IJzerman, H., Brandt, M. J., & Van Wolferen, J. (2013). Rejoice! In replication. European Journal
of Personality, 127, 128-129.
Karpiak, C. P., & Baril, G. L. (2008). Moral reasoning and concern for the environment. Journal
of Environmental Psychology, 28, 203-208.
Keller, J., & Pfattheicher, S. (2013). The Compassion-hostility-paradox: The interplay of vigilant,
prevention-focused self-regulation, compassion and hostility. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 39, 1518-1529.
Kühnen, U. (2010). Manipulation-checks as manipulation: Another look at the ease of retrieval
heuristic. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 47-58.
Lazarus, R. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.
Mackinnon, A., Jorm, A. F., Christensen, H., Korten, A. E., Jacomb, P. A., & Rodgers, B. (1999).
A short form of the positive and negative affect schedule: Evaluation of factorial validity and
invariance across demographic variables in a community sample. Personality and Individual
Differences, 27, 405-416.
Makel, M. C., Plucker, J. A., & Hegarty, B. (2012). Replications in psychology research: How
often do they really occur? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 537-542.
Mayer, F. S., & Frantz, C. M. (2004). The connectedness to nature scale: A measure of
individuals’ feeling in community with nature. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24,
503-515.
22
McCullough, M. E., Kilpatrick, S. D., Emmons, R. A., & Larson, D. B. (2001). Is gratitude a
moral affect? Psychological Bulletin, 127, 249-266.
Mehrabian, A., & Epstein, N. (1972). A measure of emotional empathy. Journal of Personality,
40, 525-543.
Milfont, T. L., & Sibley, C. G. (2012). The big five personality traits and environmental
engagement: Associations at the individual and societal level. Journal of Environmental
Psychology, 32, 187-195.
Oveis, C., Cohen, A. B., Gruber, J., Shiota, M. N., Haidt, J., & Keltner, D. (2009). Resting
respiratory sinus arrhythmia is associated with tonic positive emotionality. Emotion, 9, 265-
270.
Oveis, C., Horberg, E. J., & Keltner, D. (2010). Compassion, pride, and social intuitions of self-
other similarity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 618-630.
Parris, C. L., Hegtvedt, K. A., Watson, L. A., & Johnson, C. (2014). Justice for all? Factors
affecting perceptions of environmental and ecological injustice. Social Justice Research, 27,
67-98.
Raymond, C. M., Brown, G., & Robinson, G. M. (2011). The influence of place attachment, and
moral and normative concerns on the conservation of native vegetation: A test of two
behavioural models. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 31, 323-335.
Richard, F. D., Bond Jr, C. F., & Stokes-Zoota, J. J. (2003). One hundred years of social
psychology quantitatively described. Review of General Psychology, 7, 331-363.
Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Belief and feeling: Evidence for an accessibility model of
emotional self-report. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 934-960.
Sacchi, S., Riva, P., & Brambilla, M. (2013). When mother earth rises up. Social Psychology, 44,
271-277.
Saslow, L. R., Willer, R., Feinberg, M., Piff, P. K., Clark, K., Keltner, D., & Saturn, S. R. (2013).
My brother’s keeper? Compassion predicts generosity more among less religious individuals.
Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4, 31-38.
23
Schultz, P. W. (2001). The structure of environmental concern: Concern for self, other people, and
the biosphere. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 21, 327-339.
Schultz, P. W. (2002). New environmental theories: Empathizing with nature: The effects of
perspective taking on concern for environmental issues. Journal of Social Issues, 56, 391-406.
Schultz, P. W., & Zelezny, L. C. (1998). Values and proenvironmental behavior: A five-country
survey. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 29, 540-558.
Schultz, P. W., Zelezny, L., & Dalrymple, N. J. (2000). A multinational perspective on the relation
between Judeo-Christian religious beliefs and attitudes of environmental concern.
Environment and Behavior, 32, 576-591.
Schwartz, S. H., Cieciuch, J., Vecchione, M., Davidov, E., Fischer, R., Beierlein, C., ... Konty, M.
(2012). Refining the theory of basic individual values. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 103, 663-688.
Singer, T., & Klimecki, O. M. (2014). Empathy and compassion. Current Biology, R875-R878.
Sprecher, S., & Fehr, B. (2005). Compassionate love for close others and humanity. Journal of
Social and Personal Relationships, 22, 629-651.
Stern, P. C., Dietz, T., & Kalof, L. (1993). Value orientations, gender, and environmental concern.
Environment and Behavior, 25, 322-348.
Tam, K. P. (2013). Dispositional empathy with nature. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 35,
92-104.
Tam, K. P., Lee, S. L., & Chao, M. M. (2013). Saving Mr. Nature: Anthropomorphism enhances
connectedness to and protectiveness toward nature. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 49, 514-521.
Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2003). Affective Forecasting. Advances in Experimental Social
Psychology, 35, 345-411.
... Therefore, understanding the neurocognitive mechanisms underlying individuals' pro-environmental behavior is crucial for creating successful interventions and policies. On the psychological level, proenvironmental behavior was linked to prosociality and future orientation ( Weber, 2017;Pfattheicher, Sassenrath, & Schindler, 2016). Consistent with this, previous neuroscientific studies revealed that pro-environmental behavior is related to activation in brain regions that are also involved in prosocial and future-oriented decisionmaking, including prefrontal and parietal cortices (Guizar Rosales, Baumgartner, & Knoch, 2022;Langenbach, Savic, Baumgartner, Wyss, & Knoch, 2022;Bellucci, Camilleri, Eickhoff, & Krueger, 2020;Baumgartner, Langenbach, Gianotti, Müri, & Knoch, 2019;Strombach et al., 2015;Wesley & Bickel, 2014;Rilling & Sanfey, 2011;Carter, Meyer, & Huettel, 2010;Van Overwalle, 2009;McClure, Laibson, Loewenstein, & Cohen, 2004). ...
... Prosociality was hypothesized to foster pro-environmental behavior because acting pro-environmentally requires people to weigh their selfish interests against preserving natural resources, which primarily benefits others and in particular future generations (Gladwin, Krause, & Kennelly, 1995). This notion is supported by empirical evidence showing a positive correlation between self-reported pro-environmental attitudes and compassion, with the latter being related to prosociality (Pfattheicher et al., 2016;Batson & Shaw, 1991;Eisenberg & Miller, 1987). In line with this, we recently showed that pro-environmental decisions positively correlate with prosocial decisions in experimental tasks with real-world consequences (Le Houcq Corbi, Koch, Hölzel, & Soutschek, 2024). ...
... In this respect, it is important to emphasize that TPJ activation moderated the attitude-action gap also in the CO 2 emission task where the proenvironmental option did not involve a monetary consequence for others. Our results are thus consistent with theoretical accounts positing a relationship between pro-environmental and prosocial choices (Pfattheicher et al., 2016). The current findings moreover provide insights into the neural mechanisms underlying the attitude-behavior gap in pro-environmental decision-making. ...
Article
Full-text available
Previous evidence suggests a link between pro-environmental decisions and prosociality, but it remains unknown whether pro-environmental and prosocial decisions rely also on common neural mechanisms. Here, we tested the hypothesis that the neural correlates of pro-environmental decisions overlap with brain regions involved in prosociality, including the TPJ. To test this hypothesis, we used fMRI on 35 healthy participants performing pro-environmental and prosocial decision tasks as well as a control task for future-oriented decisions. As expected, pro-environmental and prosocial decision-making showed overlapping neural activation in regions belonging to the mentalizing network, including the TPJ. In addition, the TPJ moderated the attitude–behavior gap: Increasing TPJ activation was associated with lower attitude–behavior gaps. Taken together, our findings provide insight into the neurocognitive processes of pro-environmental decision-making by suggesting that pro-environmental decisions share neural correlates with prosocial decisions and by elucidating the role of the TPJ in the attitude–behavior gap.
... Influential in the healing experiences people have with nature (Anderson et al., 2018), awe has been shown to mediate the relationship between nature exposure (i.e., viewing nature images) and improved mood (Joye & Bolderdijk, 2015). Similar to awe, compassion motivates a more-than-self orientation (Keltner & Haidt, 2003;Shiota et al., 2007) and proecological orientations (Lumber et al., 2017;Pfattheicher et al., 2016;Yang et al., 2018). Compassion involves appraisal and understanding of others' undeserved suffering (Goetz et al., 2010) and is related to wider ecological concerns (Pfattheicher et al., 2016). ...
... Similar to awe, compassion motivates a more-than-self orientation (Keltner & Haidt, 2003;Shiota et al., 2007) and proecological orientations (Lumber et al., 2017;Pfattheicher et al., 2016;Yang et al., 2018). Compassion involves appraisal and understanding of others' undeserved suffering (Goetz et al., 2010) and is related to wider ecological concerns (Pfattheicher et al., 2016). High NR scorers tend to endorse compassion-based causes such as humanitarianism, vegetarianism, and environmental activism (Nisbet et al., 2009). ...
... Awe may be a key experience that supports a feeling of connection and oneness with nature (Joye & Bolderdijk, 2015;Piff et al., 2015;Yang et al., 2018). Compassion is linked to connection with nature (Lumber et al., 2017), our recognition of our similarities to and emotional bonds with nonhuman animals (Hawkins & Williams, 2017), and other-oriented proenvironmental tendencies (Pfattheicher et al., 2016). Similar to awe (Keltner & Haidt, 2003) and compassion (Verhaeghen, 2019), NR encompasses one's sense of being meaningfully connected to something larger than oneself (Howell et al., 2011;Mayer & Frantz, 2004;Mayer et al., 2009;Naess, 1987;Nisbet et al., 2009). ...
Article
Full-text available
The current study sought to explore the psychological pathways from nature connection to well-being. We investigated the potential mediating role of mindfulness on the relationship between nature relatedness and self-transcendent emotions of awe and compassion. Decades of past research show that nature relatedness can predict aspects of psychological well-being, such as happiness and vitality. Yet, little is known on the role that nature relatedness has with self-transcendent emotions. Further, we are only beginning to empirically understand the mediating role mindfulness plays in the association between nature relatedness and health. We conducted a cross-sectional survey of college students’ (N = 93) subjective sense of nature relatedness, their tendencies toward engaging in certain forms of mindfulness, and their dispositional awe and compassion. Nature relatedness was positively and moderately associated with both compassion and awe. Of varied forms of mindfulness, only observing was significantly associated with nature relatedness. The findings from this study suggest that the association between nature relatedness and awe may be partly explained by strength in mindfulness observational skills. Mindfulness did not help to explain the link between nature relatedness and compassion. With this work, we aim to support furthering our understanding of mediators in the association between nature relatedness and mental health.
... Analogously, adaptive emotion regulation strategies facilitate adequate engagement with information in intergroup conflict (Halperin, 2014a) and climate change debates (Panno et al., 2015). In addition, increases in compassion are positively linked to stronger beliefs in anthropogenic climate change (Lu & Schuldt, 2015), pro-environmental intentions (Pfattheicher et al., 2016), and the willingness to make lifestyle changes to avert climate change (Dickinson et al., 2016). ...
... Third, meditation can increase compassion for others (Birnie et al., 2010) and for nature (Pfattheicher et al., 2016), allowing people to recognize and care about the fact that-while they may not yet be affected by climate change-others already are or will be soon. Increases in compassion are positively linked to a stronger belief in anthropogenic climate change, pro-environmental intentions (Pfattheicher et al., 2016), and the willingness to make lifestyle changes to avert climate change (Dickinson et al., 2016). ...
... Third, meditation can increase compassion for others (Birnie et al., 2010) and for nature (Pfattheicher et al., 2016), allowing people to recognize and care about the fact that-while they may not yet be affected by climate change-others already are or will be soon. Increases in compassion are positively linked to a stronger belief in anthropogenic climate change, pro-environmental intentions (Pfattheicher et al., 2016), and the willingness to make lifestyle changes to avert climate change (Dickinson et al., 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives Climate change is one of the most pressing issues of our time. However, current policies are insufficient to keep climate change at manageable levels. In the United States, this is partly because attitudes and opinions on climate change have become increasingly divided along political party lines, making effective policymaking difficult. The existing literature has focused chiefly on cognitive (i.e., informational) approaches to increasing climate change awareness, which are often counterproductive due to patterns of biased information processing. We examined whether meditation can reduce affective polarization and the partisan divide on climate change. Method Using a preregistered survey experiment with 500 US respondents, we investigated whether a short (10-min) meditation practice would render partisans less polarized and more pro-environmental. Participants completed either a mindfulness meditation practice designed to cultivate awareness of thoughts and sensations, a loving-kindness meditation practice designed to cultivate unconditional love and compassion, or no meditation. Results Self-identified Republicans who completed the loving-kindness meditation subsequently reported significantly lower polarization scores compared to Republicans who did not meditate. Surprisingly, self-identified Democrats who completed the mindfulness meditation exhibited higher anthropocentrism than Democrats who did not meditate. This suggests that mindfulness meditation may, in some cases, reduce some aspects of pro-environmentalism. Conclusions Our results suggest that a short loving-kindness meditation can reduce polarization among Republicans. A form of mindfulness that cultivates awareness of thoughts and sensations but not interpersonal connectedness can have unintended side effects by fostering an anthropocentric worldview among Democrats. Preregistration This study was preregistered (https://osf.io/nza78).
... Moreover, compassion training has been shown to promote prosocial behaviour (Leiberg et al., 2011). Corroborating this reasoning, research has observed a positive role of compassion for others in the environmental domain for behaviour intentions (Pfattheicher et al., 2015) and consumption behaviours (Geiger & Keller, 2018). ...
... State Compassion: We employed the state compassion measure by embedding the four adjectives sympathetic, tender, compassionate, and moved into the short version of the PANAS (Mackinnon et al., 1999) measured on the above-mentioned 7-point scale from 0 (not at all) to 6 (very strongly). The German version was based on Pfattheicher et al. (2015), see also Geiger and Keller (2018). Internal consistency of the scale measured with McDonald's omega was found to be good (ω = 0.82). ...
... Instead, we emphasize that virtual body ownership can more effectively elicit distinct emotions, such as compassion, when individuals embody nature directly in iVR. This emotion is crucial in the context of pro-environmental behaviour (see also Pfattheicher et al., 2015;Geiger & Keller, 2018), as it further promotes nature connectedness. We stress that future research should focus on the question of how to use affordances and design features of iVR to elicit compassion. ...
... The growing focus on emotions in ethical decision making noted in the previous section has had some impact on more recent developments in ethics pedagogy. In sustainability-oriented ethics education, this is mirrored in references to developing empathy (Karpiak and Baril 2008) or to compassion (Pfattheicher et al. 2016). One way in which this has happened is in the integration of emotional content into ethics cases. ...
Thesis
The dissertation explores how individuals perceive and respond to moral models – individuals who engage in acts of moral virtue – in relation to pro-environmental issues, examining whether these responses encourage pro-environmental behaviors in group contexts. Through six experimental studies (Ntot = 1670), the research investigates how exposure to different moral models can elicit positive emotional reactions as moral elevation and foster intentions toward pro-environmental behaviors, including support for climate-related policies. The first chapters of the dissertation offer a comprehensive review of the literature on environmental psychology with a focus on social identity, moral emotions and moral models. The first three studies analyze how ingroup and outgroup moral models within different social categories influence moral elevation and prosocial intentions. Study 4 compares the effects of prosocial and pro-environmental moral models, exploring how attainability and relatability of models’ behaviors impact participants’ responses. The last two studies aim to replicate earlier findings from studies 1-3 in the pro-environmental domain, focusing on nationality and sports fandom as social categories. Finally, findings are synthesized with a meta-analytical approach, and limitations, future research directions, theoretical and practical implications are discussed. Findings from these studies provide insight into some psychological mechanisms that could drive environmental actions, such as volunteering for conservation efforts, highlighting the roles of moral models, elevation and social identity in promoting sustainable practices.
Article
The paper is published in an Open Access format and availabe here: https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/13504622.2025.2459340
Article
Purpose This paper aims to outline the role of self-transcendent emotions (STEs) such as compassion, gratitude and elevation in the context of sustainable tourism. It proposes a research agenda for further investigation of these emotions as facilitators of value co-creation in sustainable tourism. Design/methodology/approach Drawing on the literature on the psychology of STEs, sustainable tourist behaviour and value co-creation, this paper pinpoints potential developments and challenges associated with STEs in the domain of sustainable tourism by following a service-dominant logic approach. Findings STEs are likely to have a profound impact on sustainable tourism leading to value co-creation and mitigating the risk of value co-destruction and value no-creation. They can facilitate value co-creation by shifting the focus of actors from their own selves towards others in the tourism ecosystem, shifting the focus from extrinsic to intrinsic motivations and increasing moral values. Moreover, STEs are expected to be contiguous and reciprocal, fostering value co-creation in sustainable tourism practices. However, extreme levels of STEs can lead to value co-destruction and value no-creation. Originality/value This paper highlights the role of STEs as facilitators of value co-creation in sustainable tourism, a facet that has been overlooked in the existing literature. Further investigation is necessary in different contexts such as transformative and regenerative tourism; societal or individual crises and failures and the applications of technologies such as the Metaverse and artificial intelligence.
Article
Full-text available
Gratitude is conceptualized as a moral affect that is analogous to other moral emotions such as empathy and guilt. Gratitude has 3 functions that can be conceptualized as morally relevant: (a) a moral barometer function (i.e., it is a response to the perception that one has been the beneficiary of another person's moral actions); (b) a moral motive function (i.e., it motivates the grateful person to behave prosocially toward the benefactor and other people); and (c) a moral reinforcer function (i.e., when expressed, it encourages benefactors to behave morally in the future). The personality and social factors that are associated with gratitude are also consistent with a conceptualization of gratitude as an affect that is relevant to people's cognitions and behaviors in the moral domain.
Article
Full-text available
This review organizes a variety of phenomena related to emotional self-report. In doing so, the authors offer an accessibility model that specifies the types of factors that contribute to emotional self-reports under different reporting conditions. One important distinction is between emotion, which is episodic, experiential, and contextual, and beliefs about emotion, which are semantic, conceptual, and decontextualized. This distinction is important in understanding the discrepancies that often occur when people are asked to report on feelings they are currently experiencing versus those that they are not currently experiencing. The accessibility model provides an organizing framework for understanding self-reports of emotion and suggests some new directions for research.
Article
Assessed sympathy and personal distress with facial and physiological indexes (heart rate) as well as self-report indexes and examined the relations of these various indexes to prosocial behavior for children and adults in an easy escape condition. Heart rate deceleration during exposure to the needy others was associated with increased willingness to help. In addition, adults' reports of sympathy, as well as facial sadness and concerned attention, were positively related to their intention to assist. For children, there was some indication that report of positive affect and facial distress were negatively related to prosocial intentions and behavior, whereas facial concern was positively related to the indexes of prosocial behavior. These findings are interpreted as providing additional, convergent support for the notion that sympathy and personal distress are differentially related to prosocial behavior. Over the years, numerous philosophers (e.g., Blum, 1980) and psychologists (e.g., Barnett, 1987; Feshbach, 1978; Hoffman, 1984; Staub, 1978) have argued that empathy and sympathy, denned primarily in affective terms, are important motivators of altruistic behavior. In general, it has been asserted that people who experience emotional reactions consistent with the state of another and who feel other-oriented concern for the other are relatively likely to be motivated to alleviate the other's need or distress.
Article
This book is the third of a three-volume set on the innate mind. It provides an assessment of nativist thought and definitive reference point for future inquiry. Nativists have long been interested in a variety of foundational topics relating to the study of cognitive development and the historical opposition between nativism and empiricism. Among the issues here are questions about what it is for something to be innate in the first place; how innateness is related to such things as heritability, genetic information, and theories of cognitive development; the status of arguments both for and against nativism; and how best to understand the role of genes in development and inheritance. These issues are all explored in one way or another in this book. But the book also looks to the future. Alongside state-of-the-art discussions of such established nativist concerns as language, number, spatial cognition, and social cognition, this book examines nativist work in a variety of areas where detailed nativist exploration is relatively new, including cultural learning, creativity, economic choice, culture, and morality. The expansion of nativist theorizing into all these new areas shows both the power and the promise of nativist approaches, and points the way to the future.
Article
This book takes a hard-science look at the possibility that we humans have the capacity to care for others for their sakes (altruism) rather than simply for our own (egoism). The look is based not on armchair speculation, dramatic cases, or after-the-fact interviews, but on an extensive series of theory-testing laboratory experiments conducted over the past 35 years. Part I details the theory of altruistic motivation that has been the focus of this experimental research. The theory centers on the empathy-altruism hypothesis, which claims that other-oriented feelings of sympathy and compassion for a person in need (empathic concern) produce motivation with the ultimate goal of having that need removed. Antecedents and consequences of empathy-induced altruistic motivation are specified, making the theory empirically testable. Part II offers a comprehensive summary of the research designed to test the empathy-altruism hypothesis, giving particular attention to recent challenges. Overall, the research provides remarkably strong and consistent support for this hypothesis, forcing a tentative conclusion that empathy-induced altruism is within the human repertoire. Part III considers the theoretical and practical implications of this conclusion, suggesting that empathy-induced altruism is a far more pervasive and powerful force in human affairs than has been recognized. Failure to appreciate its importance has handicapped attempts to understand why we humans act as we do and wherein our happiness lies. This failure has also handicapped efforts to promote better interpersonal relations and create a more caring, humane society.