ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Abstract and Figures

In the introduction to this theme issue, Honing et al. suggest that the origins of musicality-the capacity that makes it possible for us to perceive, appreciate and produce music-can be pursued productively by searching for components of musicality in other species. Recent studies have highlighted that the behavioural relevance of stimuli to animals and the relation of experimental procedures to their natural behaviour can have a large impact on the type of results that can be obtained for a given species. Through reviewing laboratory findings on animal auditory perception and behaviour, as well as relevant findings on natural behaviour, we provide evidence that both traditional laboratory studies and studies relating to natural behaviour are needed to answer the problem of musicality. Traditional laboratory studies use synthetic stimuli that provide more control than more naturalistic studies, and are in many ways suitable to test the perceptual abilities of animals. However, naturalistic studies are essential to inform us as to what might constitute relevant stimuli and parameters to test with laboratory studies, or why we may or may not expect certain stimulus manipulations to be relevant. These two approaches are both vital in the comparative study of musicality.
Content may be subject to copyright.
rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org
Review
Cite this article: Hoeschele M, Merchant H,
Kikuchi Y, Hattori Y, ten Cate C. 2015 Searching
for the origins of musicality across species. Phil.
Trans. R. Soc. B 370: 20140094.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2014.0094
One contribution of 12 to a theme issue
‘Biology, cognition and origins of musicality’.
Subject Areas:
behaviour, cognition, neuroscience, evolution,
ecology
Keywords:
musicality, music perception, evolution of
music, animal models, comparative studies
Author for correspondence:
Marisa Hoeschele
e-mail: marisa.hoeschele@univie.ac.at
Searching for the origins of musicality
across species
Marisa Hoeschele1, Hugo Merchant2, Yukiko Kikuchi3, Yuko Hattori4
and Carel ten Cate5,6
1
Department of Cognitive Biology, Vienna, Austria
2
Instituto de Neurobiologia, UNAM, Campus Juriquilla, Santiago de Quere
´taro, Mexico
3
Institute of Neuroscience, Newcastle University Medical School, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK
4
Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan
5
Institute of Biology, and
6
Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands
In the introduction to this theme issue, Honing et al. suggest that the origins
of musicality—the capacity that makes it possible for us to perceive, appreci-
ate and produce music—can be pursued productively by searching for
components of musicality in other species. Recent studies have highlighted
that the behavioural relevance of stimuli to animals and the relation of exper-
imental procedures to their natural behaviour can have a large impact on the
type of results that can be obtained for a given species. Through reviewing
laboratory findings on animal auditory perception and behaviour, as well as
relevant findings on natural behaviour, we provide evidence that both tra-
ditional laboratory studies and studies relating to natural behaviour are
needed to answer the problem of musicality. Traditional laboratory studies
use synthetic stimuli that provide more control than more naturalistic
studies, and are in many ways suitable to test the perceptual abilities of ani-
mals. However, naturalistic studies are essential to inform us as to what
might constitute relevant stimuli and parameters to test with laboratory
studies, or why we may or may not expect certain stimulus manipulations
to be relevant. These two approaches are both vital in the comparative
study of musicality.
1. Introduction
Honing et al. [1] suggest that the origins of musicality—the capacity that makes
it possible for us to perceive, appreciate and produce music—can be pursued
productively by searching for components of musicality in other species.
Perhaps the most obvious starting point in this endeavour is the examination
of animal responses to music. In 1984, Porter & Neuringer [2] were the first
to conduct an experiment from this perspective by training pigeons (Columba
livia) to discriminate the music of different composers. The authors used an
operant paradigm, where pigeons received a food reward after pecking one
of two discs during presentation of excerpts from several Bach pieces for
organ and Stravinsky’s Rite of spring. Pigeons were trained to respond to the
left disc during Bach, and a right disc during Stravinsky excerpts. With time,
the pigeons learned this discrimination. Once the pigeons were making few
errors, they were presented with novel excerpts from the same composers,
and similar excerpts from other composers. The pigeons generalized to all of
these novel stimuli through their responses to the two choice discs in a way
that mirrored that of human participants.
A more recent study was performed using a similar operant paradigm with
carp (Cyprinus carpio) using blues and classical stimuli and found comparable
results [3]; after initial training with a small set of blues and classical music
stimuli, carp were able to correctly classify stimuli from these genres that
they had never heard before. How can we interpret the fact that distantly
&2015 The Authors. Published by the Royal Society under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution
License http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/, which permits unrestricted use, provided the original
author and source are credited.
related animal species have human-like boundaries for the
categorization of such complex auditory stimuli? Moreover,
what can we learn from such studies?
2. Key problems in studying biomusicology
The genre classification performance of pigeons and carp has
an analogue in studies on visual categorization. In their semi-
nal study, Hernnstein & Loveland [4] successfully trained
pigeons to discriminate photos that contained humans from
photos that did not, with all photos exhibiting considerable
variability. Subsequent debate has centred on whether the
pigeons were detecting humans or simply using local fea-
tures (e.g. large flesh-coloured area) to solve the task [5].
Similarly, pigeons and carp in the music categorization
tasks may use specific local features (e.g. presence of absence
of a specific frequency) rather than use global or abstract fea-
tures to solve the task. One way to determine what features
are controlling the behaviour in each species would be to pre-
sent altered stimuli that are missing some of the features from
the rich training stimuli or present some features of the rich
stimuli in isolation.
A second, and equally important issue, is motivational.
Aside from the music categorization abilities demonstrated in
pigeons and carp, do they have any music preferences? We
know that chimpanzees prefer at least some types of music to
silence, as they spent more time close to a speaker when
music was being played than when it was not [6]. But another
primate species not as closely related to humans, the cotton-top
tamarins (Saguinus oedipus), showed the opposite result [7].
Several other studies have looked at animal musical prefer-
ences using similar place preference paradigms. Chiandetti
& Vallortigara [8] put newborn chicks (Gallus gallus)inan
environment with consonant music playing on one side, and
dissonant music on the other. They found that the chicks
spent more time on the side with consonant music. When
McDermott & Hauser [9] presented cotton-top tamarins with
consonant stimuli in one arm of a Y-maze and dissonant
stimuli in the other arm, the animals showed no preference.
Western adults confronted with similar contingencies spent
more time listening to consonant than to dissonant sounds.
Although such a preference paradigm suggests that a feature
such as consonance and dissonance is relevant to a given
species, it tells us little about the mechanisms underlying any
preferences. The human preference for consonance over disso-
nance is at least partially based on the physical properties of
sound and is evident across many cultures [10]. It is equally
important to ascertain thefactors contributing to music-related
preferences in non-human species.
Another problem is the selection of appropriate species
for study. Taking a traditional laboratory approach, it is poss-
ible to take virtually any species with sound-sensing capacity
and rudimentary learning capacities and measure its physi-
ology and train it to discriminate different sounds. But
which species are relevant for biomusicology? One approach
is to study species based on shared ancestry. Species that are
closely related to humans are likely to share some of our abil-
ities, and are therefore good models for experiments that
would be difficult to conduct in humans. For traits that are
not shared among closely related species, it is easier to pin-
point the differences in underlying mechanisms. Species
that are more distantly related sometimes share traits without
sharing a common ancestor with those traits. By examining the
evolutionary convergence between these non-related species,
we can identify biological constraints or mechanisms required
for that trait and the selection pressures giving rise to it. For
example, some of the traits that are considered highly relevant
for biomusicology to date are vocal learning and entrainment.
Vocal learning involves the ability to produce vocalizations
based on auditory input, and entrainment involves the ability
to synchronize movements with an external stimulus (usually
sound). Both traits are uncommon in non-human species but
shared across some unrelated species, so their study could pro-
vide clues to their nature and possible interaction [11,12]. Thus,
both approaches, examining closely related and distantly
related species, can be quite useful for probing the biological
basis of musicality.
Thus, we need to consider the perceptual abilities of ani-
mals, their natural preferences, as well their similarity to
humans in terms of phylogeny or shared traits. This task
necessitates the combined fruits of traditional laboratory
studies with artificial stimuli and more naturalistic studies.
3. Traditional laboratory studies
There is much debate about the relative utility of naturalistic
and artificial laboratory studies. Proponents of naturalistic
studies argue that training animals to perform ‘unnatural’
behaviours, or using stimuli that differ markedly from those
in their natural environment, does not constitute an appropri-
ate comparison for human behaviours that emerge without
training. They also point out that artificial stimuli can result
in underestimation of animal abilities. Proponents of labora-
tory research note that experimental control and systematic
comparisons across species may reveal underlying abilities
and potential that are not apparent in natural behaviour. As
a result, laboratory studies can shed light on the presence
of cognitive abilities that support such behaviour as well as
their biological basis. Both sides have valid insights about the
limitations of the other approach, but they fail to appreciate
its strengths and the utility of combined approaches. For
example, much has been gained from studying behavioural
and neural processes in various species in response to artificial
auditory patterns presented in laboratory settings.
1
(a) Rhythm
The processing of auditory rhythms—both the underlying
pulse or beat and the organization of the beats into repetitive
groups—relies on basic features of the auditory system. The
use of operant conditioning procedures has revealed greater
temporal sensitivity in birds than in humans [13] when evalu-
ating perceptual differences among brief stimuli. In another
study [14], pigeons successfully learned to differentiate two
metrical patterns (8/4 versus 3/4) and to transfer the discrimi-
nation to different tempos, but their learning did not generalize
to metrical patterns in a different timbre. In addition, they had
difficulty differentiating rhythmic sequences from random
sequences. European starlings, however, learned to differen-
tiate rhythmic from non-rhythmic sequences and showed a
broader range of generalization [15].
1
For definitions of musical terms, please see the glossary, table 1.
rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 370: 20140094
2
The use of neurophysiological (e.g. functional magnetic res-
onance imaging, fMRI) measures has revealed that the neural
substrates of sequencing and timing behaviours overlap with
those related to human music perception and performance (see
[16] for a review) and that the motor corticobasal ganglia–
thalamocortical circuit (mCBGT) plays an important role
[1719]. Not only are trained rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta)
capable of interval timing and motor sequencing tasks per-
formed by humans [2024], but they also show similar neural
activationin mCBGT in both sequential [25,26], and single inter-
val timing tasks [27– 29]. These results suggest that the role of
mCBGT in auditory rhythm processing is shared across these
two species of primates. Uncovering findings such as these
was only possible with the use of artificial laboratory probing
of the limits of the perceptual systems of these two species.
(b) Timbre
Timbre perception has not received much attention in the
animal literature, but laboratory studies have begun to provide
some insights. As with temporal intervals, avian species seem
to detect more fine-grained timbral differences than humans do
[30– 32]. Timbre is considered a surface [33] feature, because
humans recognize the same musical patterns, regardless of
the timbre of presentation (e.g. vocal, piano, flute). This ability
to generalize across timbres is reportedly present from the new-
born period [34]. In one study, humans’ responses to chords
readily generalized across timbres, but that was not the case
with black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus) [35]. Zebra
finches exhibit generalization across timbres [36,37]. When
trained to discriminate between two words produced by
male (or female) speakers, they showed generalization across
speaker gender (i.e. fundamental frequencyand spectral differ-
ences). Clearly, more research is needed to clarify the nature
and extent of timbre generalization across species.
(c) Pitch
The pitch of a sound is typically based on the fundamental
frequency of that sound [38], although human listeners can
perceive the pitch of a sound in which the fundamental fre-
quency is missing [39]. The ability to perceive the so-called
missing fundamental is present in infants as young as three
months of age [40] and is also demonstrable in cats (Felis
catus [41]), rhesus monkeys [42] and starlings [43]. The
assumption is that this ability is shared across species, but
its generality has not been established empirically.
Listeners sometimes evaluate fundamental frequency or
pitch in an absolute manner, as when musicians with absolute
pitch correctly name the pitch class ofmusical notes (e.g. 440 Hz
as A) [44] or non-musicians distinguish the original pitch level
of highly familiar recorded music from versions that have been
shifted by one or two semitones [45]. In general, birds are
superior to mammals at detecting absolute pitch ([46– 48],
but see [49]). In most cases, however, human listeners focus
on relations among pitches rather than absolute pitch levels
while listening to music. In musical contexts, moreover,
human listeners exhibit octave generalization, perceiving the
similarity of notes that are one or more octaves apart [50,51].
The evidence foroctave generalizationin non-human species
is both limited and controversial. Blackwell & Schlosberg [52]
claimed that rats (Rattus norvegicus) generalized from training
stimuli in one octave to test stimuli in another octave. However,
there are alternative explanations of the findings, because the
stimuli may have contained harmonics that provided common
cues across octaves [50]. Suggestive evidence for octave general-
izationcomes from a bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus)that
mimicked sounds outside of her vocal range by reproducing
them an octave apart from the original [53]. Interestingly,
rhesus monkeys trained to differentiate melodies in a same
different task responded to octave-transposed melodies as
‘same’ for Western tonal, but not atonal melodies [54].
To date, there is no evidence of octave generalization in
avian species. Cynx [55] trained starlings to discriminate
between two tones, and then tested whether they generalized
this discrimination to the octave. They did not. The failure of
human listeners to exhibit octave generalization on the same
task [56] called the starling findings into question. In a similar
operant training task, humans exhibited octave generalization
[56], but an adaptation of the task for black-capped chickadees
revealed no octave generalization [57]. The available evidence
is consistent with the absence of octave generalization in birds,
but more laboratory research is needed with a wider range of
species before the question can be resolved definitively.
With regards to relative pitch, several studies have found
that non-human animals could be trained to discriminate
among chords (i.e. simultaneous combinations of tones):
European starlings [58], Java sparrows (Lonchura oryzivora;
[59]), Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata; [60]), pigeons [61]
and black-capped chickadees [35,62]. All of these studies
ensured the animals were not simply memorizing the absol-
ute properties of the sounds by presenting novel stimuli with
identical or similar relative pitch properties but different
absolute pitches. All species were able to transfer what they
learned to these novel stimuli.
Table 1. Glossary of relevant musical terms.
term definition
beat the underlying pulse, or unit of time, in music
entrainment the ability to perceive a beat in music and align
bodily movement with it
melody a sequence of tones defined by its pitch
patterning and rhythm
meter the recurring pattern of stressed and unstressed
beats in music
musicality the capacity that underlies the human ability to
perceive, appreciate, and produce music
pitch a perceptual attribute related to the fundamental
frequency that enables comparisons of sounds
as higher or lower
prosody rhythm, loudness, pitch, and tempo of speech
rhythm a non-random repetitive temporal auditory
pattern
timbre the quality of musical sound that distinguishes
different sound sources such as voices and
specific musical instruments
vocal learning long-term modification of vocal production by
imitation
rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 370: 20140094
3
Evidence of relative pitch processing with sounds presented
sequentially rather than simultaneously is less promising. Star-
lings, brown-headed cowbirds (Molothrus ater), and northern
mockingbirds (Mimus polyglottos) were trained to discrimi-
nate ascending from descending note patterns [63–67].
However, they failed to generalize these patterns to novel
pitch levels when the altered patterns were outside the training
range, although they could quickly learn to do so. In general, it
appeared that the birds encoded both absolute and relative
pitch information in discriminating the patterns but depended
more on absolute information. Another set of studies trained
zebra finches and black-capped chickadees to discriminate
sets of pitches based on either their pitch ratios (i.e. relative
pitch) or their absolute frequencies. Both species learned the dis-
crimination more quickly when there was a simple relative
pitch rule that they could use, although the discrimination
was quite difficult for the birds in comparison with learning a
simple absolute pitch rule. In short, these birds can engage in
relative pitch processing although they rely primarily on the
absolute pitch of sounds [68,69]. In one study, a bottlenose dol-
phin learned to respond to descending pitch contours, and after
extensive training, generalized that response to descending
pitch contours regardless of the component pitches [70].
In general, non-human species recognize the relative pitch
patterns of single chords more readily than those of note
sequences. Three factors may be implicated. First, the com-
ponent frequencies of chords give rise to qualities such as
sensory consonance and dissonance [71] that contribute to
their distinctiveness. Second, chords, as single events, pose
fewer memory demands than sequences of notes. Third,
there are suggestions that harmonic (simultaneous) pitch
ratios are processed at early stages of the auditory cortical
pathway in rhesus macaques [72]. As a result, the pitch
ratios of chords or simultaneously presented notes may be
processed more automatically and therefore compared more
readily than the pitch ratios of melodic sequences.
(d) High-order acoustic patterns
Building on the foundations of the auditory system and
interval timing is the perception of grouping. Gestalt psychol-
ogists noted long ago that a group of visual or auditory
elements has qualities that are more than the sum of its parts.
A repeated series of tones of equal frequency and amplitude,
with one tone having longer duration than the others, is per-
ceived as an iambic pattern in which the long sound marks
the end of a sound unit [73]. A repeated series of tones in
which one tone has higher frequency or amplitude than the
others is perceived as a trochaic pattern in which the higher
pitched or louder sound marks the beginning of the sound
unit [73]. This type of patterning is common in music as well
as speech and young infants seem to spontaneously recognize
trochaic patterns [74]. Rats seem to group tones according to
trochaic, but not iambic rules [75], which indicates that such
grouping abilities are not exclusive to human listeners. Further
research is needed to explore the nature of auditory grouping
abilities across species.
The ability to perceive higher-order temporal patterns in a
stream of sounds is relevant to speech as well as music per-
ception. The perception of speech prosody, or the melody of
speech, is relevant to music perception. In many languages,
for example, statements end with a falling terminal pitch con-
tour, and yes/no questions end with a rising terminal pitch
contour [76]. Different languages also have different prosodic
patterns [77]. Tamarins [78], rats [79,80] and Java sparrows
[81] have shown the ability to discriminate between spoken
sentences in different languages and to generalize this discrimi-
nation to novel sentences. Zebra finches use pitch, duration and
amplitude to discriminate prosodic patterns, and they can gen-
eralize specific prosodic patterns of speech syllables to novel
syllables [36]. Further exploration of non-human species’ sensi-
tivity to melodic aspects of speech may be a fruitful approach to
the study of some aspects of musicality.
(e) Criticisms of laboratory studies
When non-human animals are trained to discriminate audi-
tory patterns, they typically take a lot longer to learn the
task than their human counterparts. A critic may ask, for
example, whether a bird trained for hundreds of trials to dis-
criminate chords can really be compared with a human who
discriminates the chords without training or with minimal
training. That situation does not negate the value of compari-
sons of music perception in human and non-human listeners.
Although human listeners may require little training for
specific tasks, they have had years of exposure to music
and have a wealth of implicit musical knowledge. Moreover,
the ability of non-human listeners to perform certain tasks,
even after extensive training, can provide insights into the
mechanisms underlying that ability.
Consider the studies of interval timing in rhesus maca-
ques and humans. As the interaction between the auditory
stimulus and required motor output becomes more complex,
monkeys’ performance lags increasingly behind that of
humans. In one study, monkeys and humans were required
to tap on a push-button to produce six isochronous intervals
in a sequence. An auditory stimulus was present to guide
tapping during the first three taps but not the last three,
which required internal timing based on the preceding audi-
tory stimulus or taps [20,82]. Although monkeys produced
rhythmic movements with appropriate tempo matching,
their movements lagged by approximately 250 ms after each
auditory stimulus, even after long periods of training (close
to a year; [20]). In contrast, humans easily perform the same
task, with no training, showing stimulus movement asynchro-
nies approaching zero or with negative values [20,83]. Such
differences in two closely related species make it possible to
predict that the mCBGT may have subtle, but critical differ-
ences that evolved in order to process complex auditory
information and use it in a predictive fashion to control the
temporal and sequential organization of movement, as recently
stated in the gradual audiomotor evolution hypothesis [84].
Even if humans and monkeys had comparable experience
with the stimuli in such experimental tasks, which they do
not, both have very different interpretations of the exper-
imental context and the experimenter’s intentions, even
where efforts have been made to minimize differences in
training requirements and outcomes [35,49,57,62].
(f ) Conclusions
Overall, the aforementioned evidence indicates the enormous
potential of laboratory studies of some components of musi-
cality with non-human species. Operant conditioning studies
have the potential to reveal skills that are not part of an
animal’s natural repertoire. Animals’ performance in these
tasks is deeply rooted in the limitations and adaptive
rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 370: 20140094
4
plasticity of their nervous system [85]. By using these animals
as models, we can gain information about the neural activity
(e.g. through electrophysiological recordings) as well as
manipulations (e.g. pharmacology, electric-stimulation, opto-
genetics) that can alter brain mechanisms and corresponding
behaviour, facilitating our understanding of the neural
underpinnings of musicality in humans.
4. Importance of natural behaviour
Laboratory experiments with artificial stimuli have been helpful
in revealing perceptual skills and perceptual–motor coordi-
nation in non-human species. It is possible, however, that
their use may lead to underestimates of ability. One alternative
or supplementary approach is to use biologically relevant
stimuli in laboratory studies. Another is to study music-like
features in the natural behaviour (e.g. vocalizations) of animals.
(a) Incorporating naturalistic stimuli into
experimental work
As noted, laboratory research with artificial stimuli revealed
that birds focus on absolute aspects of pitch rather than relative
pitch [6369], but evidence from field studies suggests other-
wise. For example, fieldwork with black-capped chickadees
has shown that they produce a simple two-note tonal song
that can be sung at different absolute pitches, but maintains
its relative pitch ratio [86]. Moreover, this relative pitch ratio
is produced more accurately by dominant males [87], and accu-
rately produced song pitch ratios are preferred by females [88].
These findings prompted laboratory research on this issue with
chickadees [89]. Chickadees were trained to discriminate pitch
ratios presented at different absolute frequencies, and made
use of this relevant song pitch ratio. An experimental group
was trained to respond to the pitch ratio from chickadee
song and not to respond to two non-chickadee-song pitch
ratios. A control group was trained to respond to a non-
chickadee-song pitch ratio and not respond to two different
non-chickadee-song pitch ratios. The chickadees that were
required to identify the pitch ratio of their song learned the
task more quickly than the control group, suggesting that it
was easier for the chickadees to learn to discriminate the natu-
ral song pitch ratio than other pitch ratios [89]. A related study
showed that starlingsthat were trained to discriminate conspe-
cific vocalizations were able to maintain that discrimination
even when the songs were transposed (i.e. pitches changed,
but pitch relations preserved) [90], raising the possibility that
absolute pitch processing has priority over relative pitch
processing only with stimuli lacking in ecological validity.
There are parallels in the realm of rhythm perception.
Although pigeons have difficulty with rhythm perception
tasks involving artificial stimuli [14], the natural coo voca-
lizations of pigeons and doves, neither of which are vocal
learners, are rhythmic. The collared dove produces a coo that
consists of five elements of different duration: three notes separ-
ated by two silences [91]. Playback experiments in the field
show that replacing the second or third note of the coo by
silence caused little change in the behavioural response to the
coo. When the removed note was not replaced bysilence, short-
ening the duration of the coo, or when the pauses before and
after the second note were reversed, the response was signifi-
cantly reduced. This suggested a sensitivity to the overall
rhythmic structure of the coos [92]. Although rhythm percep-
tion in doves may be closely tied to properties of their natural
coos, it is important to explore sensitivity to rhythms in patterns
that share at least some properties with natural vocalizations. In
another study, zebra finches were trained to discriminate con-
specific songs and subsequently tested with novel versions
that altered amplitude, fundamental frequency or duration
[93]. Although performance decreased substantially with
changes in amplitude or fundamental frequency, it was main-
tained with duration changes of over 25%, well beyond zebra
finches’ reported sensitivity to temporal changes [13]. The
implication is that the rhythmic patterning is particularly
important for pattern classification in zebra finches.
(b) Music-like features in natural behaviour
The two best known features of musicality found in distantly
related species are vocal learning and entrainment (see glos-
sary, table 1). There are suggestions that the two abilities are
related [94]. To date, the species that have been shown to
exhibit both vocal learning and entrainment are distantly
related to humans. Figure 1 shows the relatedness of various
vertebrate species, indicating which have vocal learning and
entrainment abilities.
The greatest focus has been on vocal learning, with much
greater concern for its relevance to language acquisition [95]
than to musicality. However, vocal learning is also relevant
to music production. For example, consider the extensive
research of Nicolai [96,97] on vocal learning in the bullfinch
(Pyrrhula pyrrhula), a songbird. Although bullfinches normally
learn their species-specific songs from conspecifics, they were
trained to sing folk melodies whistled to them. One bullfinch
learned a 45-note tune from a human tutor and sang it in
transposition (i.e. at a different pitch level), indicating excep-
tional vocal learning and relative pitch processing skills, also
incorporating appropriate rhythm. Other bullfinches alter-
nated parts with the human tutor, as in antiphonal singing,
indicating that they anticipated as well as followed the notes
of a learned melody. Experiments such as these build on the
natural abilities of animals, as revealed by field research,
productively extending them to controlled contexts.
Snowball, the sulfur-crested cockatoo (Cacatua galerita
eleonora) whose dance video became a YouTube sensation,
helped renew scientific interest in entrainment in non-
human species. Systematic study revealed that Snowball
could synchronize his movements to the beat of music and
adjust his rate of movement to changes in tempo [98], challen-
ging the notion that entrainment is uniquely human. The
authors suggested, moreover, that such entrainment might
be evident in other species of vocal learners. In fact, a study
of YouTube videos featuring animal ‘dancing’ provided con-
firmation of entrainment to music in vocal learning species
but not in other species (e.g. dogs [12]). Another consistent
finding was successful training of a budgerigar (Melopsittacus
undulates) to tap along with a beat [99].
That only vocal learners have the capacity for entrainment
seems reasonable, given that the three avian groups in which
vocal learning has evolved independently have similar func-
tional neural pathways that are not shared with non-vocal
learners, and are comparable to humans [100]. That is, they
have a direct connection between auditory perception areas
and motor areas [101]. Entrainment may necessitate this
kind of neural architecture [94]. At the same time, Schachner
rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 370: 20140094
5
et al. [12] found evidence for entrainment in only one of the
avian vocal learning subgroups, the parrot species, and not
in songbirds. The only non-parrot species in which entrain-
ment has been detected to date is elephants. Although
elephants show evidence of vocal learning [11] their vocal
learning mechanism is unknown, but is likely to differ from
that of parrots. A compelling recent study showed that a Cali-
fornia sea lion (Zalophus californianus) could also be trained to
synchronize with a beat, and then spontaneously generalized
to music [102]. This species is not thought to be a vocal lear-
ner, although some other pinnipeds are vocal learners [11]. It
is possible that sea lions have vocal learning abilities that are
as yet unknown. However, it could also be that the ability to
synchronize with a beat only requires part of what is required
for vocal learning, or even that entrainment abilities can occur
without any of the components for vocal learning. Another
recent study showed that a chimpanzee, one of the closest
non-vocal learning relatives of humans, spontaneously
entrained to a beat while completing a motor tapping task
[103]. Clearly, the proposed connection between vocal learn-
ing and entrainment [94] requires further research with
species that are not vocal learners.
If entrainment is defined more broadly, it could include
many non-vocal learning species such as several species of
fireflies synchronizing flashing with one another [104], and
several species of frogs [105] and katydids [106] synchroniz-
ing their chorusing. Identifying a pulse and locking in
phase with it is a simpler task than detecting and entraining
to a beat within a stream of music, where the beat is not
always marked with an acoustic event, and other acoustic
events are present between beats (see [94] for review). Under-
standing the range of natural abilities related to entrainment
could clarify what is relevant for musicality.
There are otherpotentially productive means of studying the
precursors of musicality in non-human species. One approach is
to search for music-like features in animal vocalizations. For
example, Araya-Salas [107] examined whether the pitch ratios
created by adjacent notes of the song of nightingale wrens
(Microcerculus philomela) conform to harmonic pitch ratios.
From 243 comparisons, only six were significantly close to
harmonic pitch ratios, suggesting no consistent use of har-
monic pitch ratios. Another approach builds on the studies by
Hartshorne [108] and others in seeking ‘aesthetic’ features in
natural birdsong that might have arousing or emotive conse-
quences, as music does for human listeners. This notion was
met with considerable scepticism, but Rothenberg et al. [109]
posed a similar question with thrush nightingales (Luscinia lusci-
nia). According to their analysis,the songs of thrush nightingales
have similar patterns of tension and resolution to those of music,
which createexpectation and anticipation in human listeners. Ifa
certain level of familiarity and novelty is valued across species
that produce complex songs, this could lead to insights into
the origins of our motivation for music.
Another route to discovering music-like behaviours in
non-human species is to make predictions from the natural
behaviour of humans. For example, humans generalize across
timbres, recognizing a melody, regardless of the instrument on
which it is played. In most cases, it makes sense not to generalize
across timbres. Different spectral information can change the
meaning ofvocalizations not only in human speech with differ-
ent vowels, but also in animal vocalizations [110]. A study
species that may be more fruitful for timbre generalization
research would be a species that mimics the vocalizations of
other species. For satin bowerbirds (Ptilonorhynchus violaceus)
female preference for mates and male mate success may
depend on the accuracy with which males imitate heterospecific
vocalizations [111]. If the mimetic accuracy is what is important,
and not simply how well-learned a song is(as has been shown to
be important in other species, [112]), female bowerbirds would
need to assess the original heterospecific vocalization, and the
conspecific imitation, in a way that is similar to a human evalu-
ating a singer’s performance in comparison with a pianist. She
would need to be able to distinguish the two,but also generalize
between them in the sense that she is aware that they are meant
to be the same thing. In short, reflecting on natural human and
non-human behaviours that are musically relevant can provide
ideas about species and abilities that offer promising directions
for comparative study.
(c) Integrating natural and artificial studies
Naturalistic studies have revealed important abilities and
questions related to the biological basis of music such as
vocal learning and entrainment. They have also suggested
new directions for laboratory research.
Laboratory studies often reveal abilities that are not used
by non-human species under natural conditions. Knowledge
of the underlying capacity for those abilities can contribute
to an understanding of their evolutionary, developmental
and physiological foundations. The capacity for a particular
ability, even if it is unrealized in nature, may arise from the
evolutionary history of the species. Identifying the
birds
non-primate mammals
New World monkeys
Old World monkeys
non-human apes
humans
elephants
bats
sea lions
cetaceans
songbirds
vocal learning
entrainment
parrots
hummingbirds
reptiles
amphibians
Figure 1. Species with vocal learning and entrainment abilities and their relationship in a phylogenetic tree.
rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 370: 20140094
6
requirements of such abilities and their evolutionary pressures
may be facilitated by studying the limits of these abilities.
There is increasing research on aspects of musicality in var-
ious non-human species, but it is rare to find naturalistic studies
of musically relevant abilities and studies of the limits of those
abilities inthe same species. A rare but productive example of a
combined approach involvesthe chickadee, which has been the
subject of extensive field and laboratory research. This blend of
research methods made it possible to understand the relative
pitch processing skills of this species [69,86–89]. Comparably
important insights might arise from increased field research
with species that have received extensive experimental study
and increased laboratory research with species whose natural
behaviours have been well documented.
5. Conclusion
At present, there is limited laboratory research on the com-
ponents of musicality in non-human species although there is
increasing interest in this domain, so considerable expansion
of this research direction is likely. As noted, traditional labora-
tory studies and naturalistic studies can provide equally
important and complementary insights into musically rele-
vant skills. One example noted earlier was finer pitch
[46– 49] and temporal [13] resolution in birds than in mam-
mals, which emerged from laboratory studies, and vocal
learning and entrainment abilities in some bird and mammal
species [11– 12,94], which emerged from naturalistic studies.
Currently, vocal learning and entrainment are the principal
focus of research on musically related behaviours and their
underpinnings in non-human species. There are other poten-
tially productive questions that could be pursued. For
example, what kinds of behaviour require relative pitch
preferences like those present in chickadees [89]? What kinds
of behaviour require timbre generalization like that observed
in zebra finches [36– 37]? Why is auditory grouping relevant
in some species? One way forward is to search for relevant natu-
ral behaviours in less studied species and to examine the natural
behaviours of species commonly studied in the laboratory.
Acknowledgements. We thank everyonewho attended the ‘What makesus
musical animals? Cognition, biology and the origins of musicality’
workshop for the stimulating meeting that led to this special issue,
especially Henkjan Honing for organizing the meeting and the other
attendees that participated in the discussion relating directly to this
paper on natural versus artificial studies of animal music percep-
tion, including Peter Tyack and Aniruddh Patel. We thank Sandra
Trehub for her very detailed revisions as editor, and the anonymous
reviewers for their insights on paper organization. We thank also
Daniel L. Bowling for help drawing the figure.
Author contributions. M.H. wrote the majority of the initial draft and revi-
sion of the paper while integrating several paragraphs from C.t.C.
and H.M. This paper is based on a review talk on ‘Animal music per-
ception’ by M.H., an introduction from C.t.C to the ‘What makes us
musical animals? Cognition, biology and the origins of musicality’
workshop, and a discussion involving all authors. All authors con-
tributed to the ideas and framework contained within the paper
and edited the manuscript.
Funding statement. M.H. was supported by a European Research Council
advanced grant (no. 230604 ‘SOMACCA’) awarded to W. Tecumseh
Fitch at the University of Vienna during the initial submission of this
manuscript and is currently supported by a Banting Postdoctoral Fel-
lowship awarded by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research
Council of Canada hosted by the University of Vienna. H.M. is sup-
ported by PAPIIT IN201214-25 and CONACYT 151223. Y.K. is
currently supported by a contract from the National Institutes of
Health, BBSRC (BB/J009849/1) and Wellcome Trust (WT102961/Z/
13/Z) grants to C. Petkov at Newcastle University. Y.H. is supported
by a grant-in-aid for Young Scientists (B) (26730074) from the Japan
Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS).
Competing interests. The authors have no competing interests.
References
1. Honing H, ten Cate C, Peretz I, Trehub SE. 2015
Without it no music: cognition, biology and
evolution of musicality. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 370,
20140088. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2014.0088)
2. Porter D, Neuringer A. 1984 Music discriminations
by pigeons. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. B 10, 138– 148.
(doi:10.1037/0097-7403.10.2.138)
3. Chase AR. 2001 Music discriminations by carp
(Cyprinus carpio). Anim. Learn. Behav. 29,
336353. (doi:10.3758/BF03192900)
4. Herrnstein RJ, Loveland DH. 1964 Complex visual
concept in a pigeon. Science 146, 549551.
(doi:10.1126/science.146.3643.549)
5. Weisman RG, Spetch ML. 2010 Determining
when birds perceive correspondence between pictures
and objects: a critique. Comp. Cogn. Behav. Rev. 5,
117–131. (doi:10.3819/ccbr.2010.50006)
6. Mingle ME, Eppley TM, Campbell MW, Hall K, Horner
V, de Waal FBM. 2014 Chimpanzees prefer African and
Indian music over silence. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. B 40,
502–505. (doi:10.1037/xan0000032)
7. McDermott J, Hauser MD. 2007 Nonhuman primates
prefer slow tempos but dislike music overall.
Cognition 104, 654 668. (doi:10.1016/j.cognition.
2006.07.011)
8. Chiandetti C, Vallortigara G. 2011 Chicks like
consonant music. Psychol. Sci. 22, 12701273.
(doi:10.1177/0956797611418244)
9. McDermott J, Hauser M. 2004 Are consonant
intervals music to their ears? Spontaneous acoustic
preferences in a nonhuman primate. Cognition 94,
B11B21. (doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.04.004)
10. Cook ND, Fujisawa TX. 2006 The psychophysics of
harmony perception: harmony is a three-tone
phenomenon. Empirical Mus. Rev. 1, 106126.
11. Tyack PL. 2008 Convergence of calls as animals form
social bonds, active compensation for noisy
communication channels, and the evolution of vocal
learning in mammals. J. Comp. Psychol. 122,
319331. (doi:10.1037/a0013087)
12. Schachner A, Brady TF, Pepperberg IM, Hauser MD.
2009 Spontaneous motor entrainment to music in
multiple vocal mimicking species. Curr. Biol. 19,
831866. (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.061)
13. Dooling RJ, Leek MR, Gleich O, Dent ML. 2002
Auditory temporal resolution in birds: discrimination
of harmonic complexes. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 112,
748759. (doi:10.1121/1.1494447)
14. Hagmann CE, Cook RG. 2010 Testing meter, rhythm,
and tempo discriminations in pigeons. Behav.
Process 85, 99– 110. (doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2010.
06.015)
15. Hulse SH, Humpal J, Cynx J. 1984 Discrimination
and generalization of rhythmic and arrhythmic
sound patterns by European starlings (Sturnus
vulgaris). Music Percept. 1, 442– 464. (doi:10.2307/
40285272)
16. Janata P, Grafton ST. 2003 Swinging in the brain:
shared neural substrates for behaviors related to
sequencing and music. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 682– 687.
(doi:10.1038/nn1081)
17. Grahn JA, Brett M. 2007 Rhythm and beat
perception in motor areas of the brain. J. Cogn.
Neurosci. 19, 893– 906. (doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.
5.893)
18. Grahn JA. 2009 The role of the basal ganglia in beat
perception. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1169, 35– 45.
(doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04553.x)
19. Zatorre RJ, Chen JL, Penhune VB. 2007 When the
brain plays music: auditory– motor interactions in
music perception and production. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.
8, 547558. (doi:10.1038/nrn2152)
20. Zarco W, Merchant H, Prado L, Mendez JC. 2009
Subsecond timing in primates: comparison of
interval production between human subjects and
rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 370: 20140094
7
rhesus monkeys. J. Neurophysiol. 106, 31913202.
(doi:10.1152/jn.00066.2009)
21. Mendez JC, Prado L, Mendoza G, Merchant H. 2010
Temporal and spatial categorization in human and
non-human primates. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 5, 50.
22. Merchant H, Battaglia-Mayer A, Georgopoulos AP.
2003 Interception of real and apparent motion
targets: psychophysics in humans and monkeys.
Exp. Brain Res. 152, 106– 112. (doi:10.1007/
s00221-003-1514-5)
23. Merchant H, Zarco W, Pe
´rez O, Prado L, Bartolo R.
2011 Measuring time with different neural
chronometers during a synchronization-continuation
task. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 19 784 –19 789.
(doi:10.1073/pnas.1112933108)
24. Hikosaka O, Rand M, Nakamura K, Miyachi S,
Kitaguchi K, Sakai K, Shimo Y. 2002 Long-term
retention of motor skill in macaque monkeys and
humans. Exp. Brain Res. 147, 494– 504. (doi:10.
1007/s00221-002-1258-7)
25. Hikosaka O, Nakamura K, Sakai K, Nakahara H. 2002
Central mechanisms of motor skill learning. Curr.
Opin. Neurobiol. 12, 217222. (doi:10.1016/S0959-
4388(02)00307-0)
26. Tanji J. 2001 Sequential organization of multiple
movements: involvement of cortical motor areas.
Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 631– 651. (doi:10.1146/
annurev.neuro.24.1.631)
27. Macar F, Coull J, Vidal F. 2006 The supplementary
motor area in motor and perceptual time
processing: fMRI studies. Cogn. Process 7, 8994.
(doi:10.1007/s10339-005-0025-7)
28. Mita A, Mushiake H, Shima K, Matsuzaka Y, Tanji J.
2009 Interval time coding by neurons in the
presupplementary and supplementary motor areas.
Nat. Neurosci. 12, 502– 507. (doi:10.1038/nn.2272)
29. Merchant H, Pe
´rez O, Zarco W, Ga
´mez J. 2013 Interval
tuning in the primate medial premotor cortex as a
general timing mechanism. J. Neurosci. 33, 9082– 9096.
(doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5513-12.2013)
30. Amagai S, Dooling RJ, Shamma S, Kidd TL, Lohr B.
2000 Detection of modulation in spectral envelopes
and linear-rippled noises by budgerigars
(Melopsittacus undulatus). J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 105,
20292035. (doi:10.1121/1.426736)
31. Lohr B, Dooling RJ. 1998 Detection of changes in
timbre and harmonicity in complex sounds by zebra
finches (Taeniopygia guttata) and budgerigars
(Melopsittacus undulatus). J. Comp. Psychol. 112,
3647. (doi:10.1037/0735-7036.112.1.36)
32. Cynx J, Williams H, Nottebohm F. 1990 Timbre
discrimination in zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata)
song syllables. J. Comp. Psychol. 104, 303– 308.
(doi:10.1037/0735-7036.104.4.303)
33. Warker JA, Halpern AR. 2005 Musical stem
completion: humming that note. Am. J. Psychol.
118, 567585.
34. Ha
´den GP, Stefanics G, Vestergaard MD, Denham SL,
Sziller I, Winkler I. 2009 Timbre-independent extraction
of pitch in newborn infants. Psychophysiology 46,69
74. (doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00749.x)
35. Hoeschele M, Cook RG, Guillette LM, Hahn AH,
Sturdy CB. 2014 Timbre influences chord
discrimination in black-capped chickadees but not
humans. J. Comp. Psychol. 128, 387– 401. (doi:10.
1037/a0037159)
36. Spierings MJ, ten Cate C. 2014 Zebra finches are
sensitive to prosodic features of human speech.
Proc. R. Soc. B 281, 20140480. (doi:10.1098/rspb.
2014.0480)
37. Ohms VR, Gill A, Van Heijningen CAA, Beckers GJL, ten
Cate C. 2010 Zebra finches exhibit speaker-independent
phonetic perception of human speech. Proc. R. Soc. B
277, 10031009. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.1788)
38. Dowling WJ, Harwood DL. 1986 Music cognition.
Orlando, FL: Academic Press Inc.
39. Oxenham AJ. 2012 Pitch perception. J. Neurosci. 32,
13 33513 338. (doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3815-12.2012)
40. Lau BK, Werner LA. 2012 Perception of missing
fundamental pitch by 3- and 4-month-old human
infants. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 132, 38743882.
(doi:10.1121/1.4763991)
41. Heffner H, Whitfield IC. 1976 Perception of the
missing fundamental by cats. J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
59, 915919. (doi:10.1121/1.380951)
42. Tomlinson RWW. 1988 Perception of the missing
fundamental in nonhuman primates. J. Acoust. Soc.
Am. 84, 560565. (doi:10.1121/1.396833)
43. Cynx J, Shapiro M. 1986 Perception of missing
fundamental by a species of songbird (Sturnus
vulgaris). J. Comp. Psychol. 100, 356360. (doi:10.
1037/0735-7036.100.4.356)
44. Ward WD. 1999 Absolute pitch. In The psychology of
music (ed. D Deutsch), pp. 265 298, 2nd edn.
San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
45. Schellenberg EG, Trehub SE. 2003 Good pitch
memory is widespread. Psychol. Sci. 14, 262– 266.
(doi:10.1111/1467-9280.03432)
46. Weisman R, Njegovan M, Sturdy CB, Phillmore L,
Coyle J, Mewhort D. 1998 Frequency-range
discriminations: special and general abilities in
zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) and humans
(Homo sapiens). J. Comp. Psychol. 112, 244– 258.
(doi:10.1037/0735-7036.112.3.244)
47. Weisman RG, Njegovan MG, Williams MT, Cohen JS,
Sturdy CB. 2004 A behavior analysis of absolute
pitch: sex, experience, and species. Behav. Process.
66, 289307. (doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2004.03.010)
48. Friedrich A, Zentall T, Weisman R. 2007 Absolute
pitch: frequency-range discriminations in pigeons
(Columba livia)– comparisons with zebra finches
(Taeniopygia guttata) and humans (Homo sapiens).
J. Comp. Psychol. 121, 95– 105. (doi:10.1037/0735-
7036.121.1.95)
49. Weisman RG, Balkwill L-L, Hoeschele M, Moscicki
MK, Bloomfield LL, Sturdy CB. 2010 Absolute
pitch in boreal chickadees and humans: exceptions
that test a phylogenetic rule. Learn. Motiv. 41,
156173. (doi:10.1016/j.lmot.2010.04.002)
50. Burns EM. 1999 Intervals, scales, and tuning. In The
psychology of music (ed. D Deutsch), pp. 215 264,
2nd edn. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
51. Patel AD. 2003 Language, music, syntax and the brain.
Nat. Neurosci. 6, 674– 681. (doi:10.1038/nn1082)
52. Blackwell HR, Schlosberg H. 1943 Octave
generalization, pitch discrimination, and loudness
thresholds in the white rat. J. Exp. Psychol. 33,
407419. (doi:10.1037/h0057863)
53. Richards DG, Wolz JP, Herman LM. 1975 Vocal
mimicry of computer-generated sounds and vocal
labeling of objects by a bottlenosed dolphin,
Tursiops truncates.J. Comp. Psychol. 98, 1028.
(doi:10.1037/0735-7036.98.1.10)
54. Wright AA, Rivera JJ, Hulse SH, Shyan M, Neiworth
JJ. 2000 Music perception and octave generalization
in rhesus monkeys. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 129,
291307. (doi:10.1037/0096-3445.129.3.291)
55. Cynx J. 1993 Auditory frequency generalization and a
failure to find octave generalization in a songbird, the
European starling (Sturnus vulgaris). J. Comp. Psychol.
107, 140– 146. (doi:10.1037/0735-7036.107.2.140)
56. Hoeschele M, Weisman RG, Sturdy CB. 2012 Pitch
chroma discrimination, generalization, and transfer
tests of octave equivalence in humans. Attent.
Percept. Psychophys. 74, 1742 1760. (doi:10.3758/
s13414-012-0364-2)
57. Hoeschele M, Weisman RG, Guillette LM, Hahn AH,
Sturdy CB. 2013 Chickadees fail standardized
operant tests for octave equivalence. Anim. Cogn.
16, 599609. (doi:10.1007/s10071-013-0597-z)
58. Hulse SH, Bernard DJ, Braaten RF. 1995 Auditory
discrimination of chord-based spectral structures by
European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). J. Exp. Psychol.
124, 409423. (doi:10.1037/0096-3445.124.4.409)
59. Watanabe S, Uozumi M, Tanaka N. 2005
Discrimination of consonance and dissonance in
Java sparrows. Behav. Process 70, 203208.
(doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2005.06.001)
60. Izumi A. 2000 Japanese monkeys perceive sensory
consonance of chords. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 108,
30733078. (doi:10.1121/1.1323461)
61. Brooks DI, Cook RG. 2010 Chord discrimination by
pigeons. Music Percept. 27, 183– 196. (doi:10.1525/
mp.2010.27.3.183)
62. Hoeschele M, Cook RG, Guillette LM, Brooks DI,
Sturdy CB. 2012 Black-capped chickadee (Poecile
atricapillus) and human (Homo sapiens) chord
discrimination. J. Comp. Psychol. 126, 57– 67.
(doi:10.1037/a0024627)
63. Hulse SH, Cynx J. 1985 Relative pitch perception is
constrained by absolute pitch in songbirds (Mimus,
Molothrus, and Sturnus). J. Comp. Psychol. 99,
176196. (doi:10.1037/0735-7036.99.2.176)
64. Hulse SH, Cynx J. 1986 Interval and contour in serial
pitch perception by a passerine bird, the European
starling (Sturnus vulgaris). J. Comp. Psychol. 100,
215228. (doi:10.1037/0735-7036.100.3.215)
65. Hulse SH, Cynx J, Humpal J. 1984 Absolute and
relative pitch discrimination in serial pitch
perception by birds. J. Exp. Psychol. 113, 38– 54.
(doi:10.1037/0096-3445.113.1.38)
66. Page SC, Hulse SH, Cynx J. 1989 Relative pitch
perception in the European starling (Sturnus
vulgaris): further evidence for an elusive
phenomenon. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. B 15,
137146. (doi:10.1037/0097-7403.15.2.137)
67. MacDougall-Shackleton SA, Hulse SH. 1996
Concurrent absolute and relative pitch processing by
European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). J. Comp.
rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 370: 20140094
8
Psychol. 110, 139– 146. (doi:10.1037/0735-7036.
110.2.139)
68. Weisman RG, Njegovan M, Ito S. 1994 Frequency
ratio discrimination by zebra finches (Taeniopygia
guttata) and humans (Homo sapiens). J. Comp.
Psychol. 108, 363– 372. (doi:10.1037/0735-7036.
108.4.363)
69. Njegovan M, Weisman R. 1997 Pitch discrimination
in field- and isolation-reared black-capped
chickadees (Parus atricapillus). J. Comp. Psychol.
111, 294301. (doi:10.1037/0735-7036.111.3.294)
70. Ralston JV, Herman LM. 1995 Perception and
generalization of frequency contours by a bottlenose
dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). J. Comp. Psychol. 109,
268–277. (doi:10.1037/0735-7036.109.3.268)
71. Helmholtz HLF. 1877 On the sensations of tone as a
physiological basis for the theory of music.
New York, NY: Dover Publications.
72. Kikuchi Y, Horwitz B, Mishkin M, Rauschecker JP.
2014 Processing of harmonics in the lateral belt of
macaque auditory cortex. Front. Neurosci. 8, 204.
(doi:10.3389/fnins.2014.00204)
73. Woodrow H. 1909 A quantitative study of rhythm:
the effect of variations in intensity, rate and
duration. Arch. Psychol. 14, 1 66.
74. Bion R, Benavides-Varela S, Nespor M. 2011
Acoustic markers of prominence influence infants’
and adults’ segmentation of speech sequences.
Lang. Speech 54, 123140. (doi:10.1177/
0023830910388018)
75. de la Mora DM, Nespor M, Toro JM. 2013 Do
humans and nonhuman animals share the grouping
principles of the iambictrochaic law? Attent.
Percept. Psychophys. 75, 92– 100. (doi:10.3758/
s13414-012-0371-3)
76. Cruttenden A. 1981 Falls and rises: meanings and
universals. J. Linguist. 17, 77– 91. (doi:10.1017/
S0022226700006782)
77. Cutler A, Dahan D, van Donselaar W. 1997 Prosody
in the comprehension of spoken language: a
literature review. Lang. Speech 40, 140201.
78. Tincoff R, Hauser M, Tsao F, Spaepen G, Ramus F,
Mehler J. 2005 The role of speech rhythm in
language discrimination: further tests with a non-
human primate. Dev. Sci. 8, 26– 35. (doi:10.1111/j.
1467-7687.2005.00390.x)
79. Toro JM, Trobalon JB, Sebastian-Galles N. 2003 The
use of prosodic cues in language discrimination
tasks by rats. Anim. Cogn. 6, 131– 136. (doi:10.
1007/s10071-003-0172-0)
80. Toro JM, Trobalon JB, Sebastian-Galles N. 2005 Effects
of backward speech and speaker variability in
language discrimination by rats. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim.
B31, 95100. (doi:10.1037/0097-7403.31.1.95)
81. Naoi N, Watanabe S, Maekawa K, Hibiya J. 2012
Prosody discrimination by songbirds (Padda
oryzivora). PLoS ONE 7, e47446. (doi:10.1371/
journal.pone.0047446)
82. Donnet S, Bartolo R, Fernandes JM, Cunha JPS,
Prado L, Merchant H. 2014 Monkeys time their
pauses of movement and not their movement-
kinematics during a synchronization-continuation
rhythmic task. J. Neurophysiol. 111, 2138– 2149.
(doi:10.1152/jn.00802.2013)
83. Repp BH. 2005 Sensorimotor synchronization: a
review of the tapping literature. Psychon. Bull. Rev.
12, 969992. (doi:10.3758/BF03206433)
84. Merchant H, Honing H. 2014 Are non-human
primates capable of rhythmic entrainment?
Evidence for the gradual audiomotor evolution
hypothesis. Front. Neurosci. 7, 274. (doi:10.3389/
fnins.2013.00274)
85. Mendoza G, Merchant H. 2014 Motor system
evolution and the emergence of high cognitive
functions. Prog. Neurobiol.122C, 73– 93.
86. Weisman R, Ratcliffe L, Johnsrude I, Hurly TA. 1990
Absolute and relative pitch production in the song
of the black-capped chickadee. Condor 92,
118124. (doi:10.2307/1368390)
87. Christie PJ, Mennill DJ, Ratcliffe LM. 2004 Pitch
shifts and song structure indicate male quality in
the dawn chorus of black-capped chickadees. Behav.
Ecol. Sociobiol. 55, 341348. (doi:10.1007/s00265-
003-0711-3)
88. Weisman R, Ratcliffe L. 2004 Relative pitch and the
song of black-capped chickadees. Am. Sci. 92,
532539. (doi:10.1511/2004.6.532)
89. Hoeschele M, Guillette LM, Sturdy CB. 2012 Biological
relevance of acoustic signal affects discrimination
performance in a songbird. Anim. Cogn. 15, 677– 688.
(doi:10.1007/s10071-012-0496-8)
90. Bregman MR, Patel AD, Gentner TQ. 2012 Stimulus-
dependent flexibility in non-human auditory pitch
processing. Cognition 122, 51– 60. (doi:10.1016/j.
cognition.2011.08.008)
91. Ballintijn MR, ten Cate C. 1999 Variation in number
of elements in the perch-coo vocalization of the
collared dove (Streptopelia decaocto) and what it
may tell about the sender. Behaviour 136,
847864. (doi:10.1163/156853999501603)
92. Slabbekoorn H, ten Cate C. 1999 Collared dove
responses to playback: slaves to the rhythm.
Ethology 105, 377391. (doi:10.1046/j.1439-0310.
1999.00420.x)
93. Nagel KI, McLendon HM, Doupe AJ. 2010
Differential influence of frequency, timing, and
intensity cues in a complex acoustic categorization
task. J. Neurophysiol. 104, 1426– 1437. (doi:10.
1152/jn.00028.2010)
94. Patel AD, Iversen JR, Bregman MR, Schulz I. 2009
Studying synchronization to a musical beat in
nonhuman animals. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1169,
459469. (doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04581.x)
95. Doupe AJ, Kuhl PK. 1999 Birdsong and human speech:
common themes and mechanisms. Annu. Rev. Neurosci.
22, 567 631. (doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.22.1.567)
96. Gu¨ttinger HR, Turner T, Dobmeyer S, Nicolai J. 2002
Melodiewahrnehmung und Wiedergabe beim
Gimpel: Untersuchungen an liederpfeifenden und
Kanariengesang imitierenden Gimpeln (Pyrrhula
pyrrhula). J. Ornithol. 143, 303318. (doi:10.1007/
BF02465481)
97. Nicolai J, Gundacker C, Teeselink K, Gu¨ttinger HR.
2014 Human melody singing by bullfinches
(Pyrrhula pyrrula) gives hints about a cognitive note
sequence processing. Anim. Cogn. 17, 143– 155.
(doi:10.1007/s10071-013-0647-6)
98. Patel AD, Iversen JR, Bregman MR, Schulz I. 2009
Experimental evidence for synchronization to a
musical beat in a nonhuman animal. Curr. Biol. 19,
827830. (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.038)
99. Hasegawa A, Okanoya K, Hasegawa T, Seki Y. 2011
Rhythmic synchronization tapping to an audio–
visual metronome in budgerigars. Sci. Rep. 1, 120.
(doi:10.1038/srep00120)
100. Jarvis ED. 2004 Learned birdsong and the
neurobiology of human language. Ann. N.Y. Acad.
Sci. 1016, 749777. (doi:10.1196/annals.1298.038)
101. Feenders G, Liedvogel M, Rivas M, Zapka M, Horita
H, Hara E, Jarvis ED. 2008 Molecular mapping of
movement-associated areas in the avian brain: a
motor theory for vocal learning origin. PLoS ONE 3,
e1768. (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0001768)
102. Cook P, Rouse A, Wilson M, Reichmuth C. 2013 A
California sea lion (Zalophus californianus) can keep
the beat: motor entrainment to rhythmic auditory
stimuli in a non vocal mimic. J. Comp. Psychol. 127,
412427. (doi:10.1037/a0032345)
103. Hattori Y, Tomonaga M, Matsuzawa T. 2013
Spontaneous synchronized tapping to an auditory
rhythm in a chimpanzee. Sci. Rep. 3, 1566. (doi:10.
1038/srep01566)
104. Buck J. 1988 Synchronous rhythmic flashing in
fireflies. II. Quart. Rev. Biol. 63, 265– 289. (doi:10.
1086/415929)
105. Gerhardt HC, Huber F. 2002 Acoustic communication
in insects and anurans. Chicago, IL: University of
Chicago Press.
106. Greenfield MD, Schul J. 2008 Mechanisms and
evolution of synchronous chorusing: emergent
properties and adaptive functions in Neoconocephalus
katydids (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae). J. Comp. Psychol.
122, 289– 297. (doi:10.1037/0735-7036.122.3.289)
107. Araya-Salas M. 2012 Is birdsong music? Evaluating
harmonic intervals in songs of a neotropical
songbird. Anim. Behav. 84, 309– 313. (doi:10.1016/
j.anbehav.2012.04.038)
108. Hartshorne C. 1973 Born to sing; an interpretation
and world survey of bird song. Bloomington, IN:
Indiana University Press.
109. Rothenberg D, Roeske TC, Voss HU, Naguib M,
Tchernichovski O. 2014 Investigation of musicality in
birdsong. Hear. Res. 308, 7183. (doi:10.1016/j.
heares.2013.08.016)
110. Templeton CN, Greene E, Davis K. 2005 Allometry of
alarm calls: black-capped chickadees encode
information about predator size. Science 308,
19341937. (doi:10.1126/science.1108841)
111. Coleman SW, Patricelli GL, Coyle B, Siani J, Borgia G.
2007 Female preferences drive the evolution of
mimetic accuracy in male sexual displays. Biol. Lett.
3, 463466. (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2007.0234)
112. Nowicki SW, Searcy A, Peters S. 2002 Quality of
song learning affects female response to male bird
song. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 269, 19491954.
(doi:10.1098/rspb.2002.2124)
rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 370: 20140094
9
... Musicality is a stable aspect of our biology; it can be defined as a natural and spontaneously developed trait based on and limited by biology and cognition [68]. However, musicality is not exclusively human, as its basic components can be considered shared with non-human animals [69]. This has led to the emergence of "biomusicology", with interdisciplinary and comparative research on the effects of music on non-human animals. ...
... From a biomusicology perspective, there are marked similarities between humans and non-human animals in anatomical structures, biochemical signals and their receptors, genes, and behavioral responses that, in humans, have been associated with musicality [69]. Therefore, non-human animals may perceive the basic components of music similarly to humans. ...
Article
Full-text available
Simple Summary For humans, music is a powerful tool of emotional communication, conveying affective states and modulating physiological states in ways that can influence well-being. Understanding that emotionality is not an exclusively human trait, as it is also recognized in non-human species, it is natural to assume that the intrinsic power of music to modulate the psychophysiological state may be trans-specific. In this way, music can be a powerful tool for enriching the environment and improving the welfare of captive animals, especially farm animals. As there is very limited information on non-human animals, the aim is to review what is known from a human comparative perspective, arguments that support its use, and the potential to use music in non-human species. Abstract The effects of sound stimulation as a sensorial environmental enrichment for captive animals have been studied. When appropriately implemented for farm animals, it can improve welfare, health, and productivity. Furthermore, there are indications that music can induce positive emotions in non-human animals, similar to humans. Emotion is a functional state of the organism involving both physiological processes, mediated by neuroendocrine regulation, and changes in behavior, affecting various aspects, including contextual perception and welfare. As there is very limited information on non-human animals, the objective of this review is to highlight what is known about these processes from human biological and comparative perspectives and stimulate future research on using music to improve animal welfare.
... Meter perception has also been explored in studies with nonhuman animals (see Hoeschele et al., 2015). In an experiment with rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) measuring auditory event-related potentials (ERPs), the authors concluded that the animals were able to detect the temporal regularity in a sequence (Honing et al., 2012). ...
... Further experiments using discrimination training procedures will be necessary in order to understand rats' capabilities instead of what they naturally tend to do when presented with these kinds of stimuli. In humans, beat perception and meter induction remain key phenomena for rhythm processing and there is evidence that some of their components might be already present in other species (e.g., Bouwer et al., 2021;Fitch, 2013;Hoeschele et al., 2015;Ravignani et al., 2019). More work is needed to identify the cues, if any, that other species might use to infer metrical structures over regular sequences. ...
Article
Full-text available
Meter induction is a key process for rhythm perception. However, while some nonhuman animals readily detect temporal regularities and perceive beats in auditory sequences, there is no consistent evidence that they extract metrical structures. In the present experiment, we familiarized rats (Rattus norvegicus) to auditory rhythmic sequences that evoked a duple or a triple meter. We then tested their recognition of these familiar sequences when pitted against novel sequences that evoked no meter (isotonic), evoked a different meter (either duple or triple), or were nonmetrical (nonisochronous). The animals only discriminated isochronous from nonisochronous sequences. However, we found no evidence for meter induction, as the animals did not discriminate familiar from isotonic sequences or from sequences with a different meter. The findings suggest that, under a familiarization paradigm, the natural tendency of the animals is to focus on temporal rather than melodic changes to recognize rhythmic sequences.
... To grant ecological validity to our observations (Hoeschele et al. 2015), stimuli were synthesized relying on a database of real orchestral sounds using Ableton Live 10 (Berlin, Germany Since the paradigm was also designed to test number representation (Gennari et al. 2023), tones were presented in series of 4 and 12 identical notes played at a constant rate (inter-tone-interval: 180, 60, and 20 ms for L, M, and S sounds, respectively) and with a fixed inter-sequenceinterval of 1 s ( Figure S1A). Orthogonal to tone identity, tone duration, inter-tone-interval, and total sequence duration were varied and controlled to prevent confounds between number and nonnumerical magnitudes (Gennari et al. 2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
At the physical level, the experience of pitch has a single determinant: the repetition rate of a waveform in the acoustic signal. Yet, psychologists describe pitch as composed of two perceptual dimensions, height and chroma. Chroma accounts for octave equivalence, whereby sounds with fundamental frequencies at a 1:2 ratio are perceived as sharing the same pitch. A current controversy debates whether chroma is a basic perceptual property dependent on biological constraints or a higher‐order cognitive construct shaped by culture. Here, we used high‐density electroencephalography (EEG) and time‐resolved multivariate pattern analyses to characterize pitch processing in humans at 3 months of age. We found that, when exposed to repetitive sequences of orchestral tones, infants encode two separate pitch‐related dimensions automatically and with divergent dynamics. Namely, our classifiers isolated height‐specific information from the neural signal rapidly after the onset of the auditory sequences. Beyond approximately 600 ms, the performance of pitch height decoders fell to chance level and did not recover. In contrast, neural patterns displaying octave equivalence were retrieved later in the trial, over multiple time windows throughout the unfolding of the auditory sequence, and after sequence offset. Overall, this study reveals that very early in human development, the pitch of naturally rich tones is processed over two distinct encoding stages, capturing not only their absolute height but also their relative position in the octave. We speculate that separate encoding mechanisms reflect distinct functional roles carried by the two dimensions.
... Musicality (described by Hoeschele and al. [6], as "the capacity that makes it possible for us to perceive, appreciate, and produce music", p. 1) can be studied in non-human animals at different levels, described by several authors (e.g. [7,8]): (1) the cognitive or discriminative level, i.e., the ability to distinguish between various types of music (such as classical, jazz, and rock and roll) or different composers (such as Bach, Mozart, and ...
Article
Full-text available
Music may be one of the oldest forms of art, and its appreciation is thought to be universal among humans. Music could also represent a useful tool to improve captive animals’ welfare, especially if individuals can choose the music they prefer. The ability to discriminate between different kinds of music or composers has been demonstrated in numerous non-human species. However, a reinforcing effect of music was found in only a few species, particularly in vocal learner birds such as Passeriformes and Psittaciformes. In this study, we explored whether cockatiels could learn to use a touchscreen to choose between two different pieces of music: first between rock and roll and calm music, and then between consonant and dissonant music. Some birds showed individual preferences for either rock and roll or calm music, but no preferences were found for consonance or dissonance. These results are in line with the hypothesis that vocal learners would be sensitive to music. Our findings offer new prospects for the study of musicality in non-humans and its potential applications for promoting welfare in captive animals: interacting with a touchscreen would allow them to have some control over their environment and to choose their preferred type of music as a form of environmental enrichment.
... In some cases, birds have been observed to prefer certain types of music over others. For example, European starlings were found to discriminate between different classical music composers, indicating a sophisticated level of melodic perception (Hoeschele et al., 2015). ...
... One way to understand the evolutionary origins and function of octave equivalence is through comparative research with non-human animal species. Comparative studies with model species allow for control of potential effects of enculturation in a way that is not possible in studies with human participants [12]. But perhaps even more importantly, cross-species comparative studies can be used to assess whether octave equivalence perception is a common trait among non-human animals and therefore has ancient evolutionary origins or whether it is limited to a specific clade or unrelated groups that share a particular ecological niche. ...
Article
Full-text available
Octave equivalence describes the perception that two notes separated by a doubling in frequency have a similar quality. In humans, octave equivalence is important to both music and language learning and is found cross-culturally. Cross-species studies comparing human and non-human animals can help illuminate the necessary pre-conditions to developing octave equivalence. Here, we tested whether rats (Rattus norvegicus) perceive octave equivalence using a standardized cross-species paradigm. This allowed us to disentangle concurring hypotheses regarding the evolutionary roots of this phenomenon. One hypothesis is that octave equivalence is directly connected to vocal learning, but this hypothesis is only partially supported by data. According to another hypothesis, the harmonic structure of mammalian vocalizations may be more important. If rats perceive octave equivalence, this would support the importance of vocal harmonic structure. If rats do not perceive octave equivalence, this would suggest that octave equivalence evolved independently in several mammalian clades due to a more complex interplay of different factors such as—but not exclusively—the ability to vocally learn. Evidence from our study suggests that rats do perceive octave equivalence, thereby suggesting that the harmonic vocal structure found in mammals may be a key pre-requisite for octave equivalence. Stage 1 approved protocol: the study reported here was originally accepted as a Registered Report and the study design was approved in Stage 1. We hereby confirm that the completed experiment(s) have been executed and analysed in the manner originally approved with any unforeseen changes in those approved methods and analyses clearly noted. The approved Stage 1 protocol can be found at: https://osf.io/gvf7c/?view_only=76dc1840f31c4f9ab59eb93cbadb98b7.
... Special attention must be paid to comparative approaches among species. 63 Vervet monkeys, gibbons, and songbirds are phylogenetically distant from humans. Convergent evolution may have occurred between humans and other species; however, determining the ecological background is often a difficult task. ...
Article
Various selection pressures have shaped human uniqueness, for instance, music. When and why did musical universality and diversity emerge? Our hypothesis is that “music” initially originated from manipulative calls with limited musical elements. Thereafter, vocalizations became more complex and flexible along with a greater degree of social learning. Finally, constructed musical instruments and the language faculty resulted in diverse and context-specific music. Music precursors correspond to vocal communication among nonhuman primates, songbirds, and cetaceans. To place this scenario in hominin history, a three-phase scheme for music evolution is presented herein. We emphasize (1) the evolution of sociality and life history in australopithecines, (2) the evolution of cognitive and learning abilities in early/middle Homo, and (3) cultural evolution, primarily in Homo sapiens. Human musical capacity and products should be due to the hominin-specific combination of several biosocial features, including bipedalism, stable pair bonding, alloparenting, expanded brain size, and sexual selection.
Preprint
Full-text available
Music is a complex learned behavior that is ubiquitous among humans, and many musical patterns are shared across geography and cultures ("music universals"). Knowing whether these universals are specific to humans or shared with other animals is important to understand how production-related factors (motor biases and constraints) or cognitive factors (learning) contribute to the emergence of these acoustic patterns. Bird song is often described as an animal analogue of human music, and some studies of individual avian species highlight acoustic similarities between bird song and music. However, expansive and comparative approaches are necessary to identify universal patterns within bird song, reveal mechanisms associated with these patterns , and draw parallels to music universals. Here, we adopt such an approach and analyze the prevalence of acoustic patterns (sequences) across ~300 species of passerines, spanning both oscines (songbirds; vocal learners) and their sister clade, suboscines (passerines that produce songs that are not learned), as well as within a global corpus of human vocal music. This approach allowed us to directly test hypotheses that phonation mechanisms or vocal learning shape the emergence of universal patterns. We first document acoustic patterns that were widely shared across passerines and similar to music universals (e.g., small pitch intervals), highlighting the role of shared vocal production mechanisms in these patterns. Consistent with a contribution of vocal learning, we observed patterns (e.g., alternation in durations) there were more similar between oscines and humans than between suboscines and humans . Interestingly, we also discovered patterns (e.g., pitch alternation) that were inconsistent with a contribution of vocal learning and were more similar between suboscines and humans than between oscines and humans. This research provides the broadest evidence of shared universals in vocal performance across birds and humans and highlights convergent mechanisms shaping communication patterns.
Preprint
Full-text available
Synchronizing movements to music is one of the hallmarks of human culture whose evolutionary and neurobiological origins remain unknown. The ability to synchronize movements requires 1) detecting a steady rhythmic pulse, or beat, out of a stream of complex sounds, 2) projecting this rhythmic pattern forward in time to predict future input, and 3) timing motor commands in anticipation of predicted future beats. Here, we demonstrate that the macaque is capable of synchronizing taps to a subjective beat in real music, and even spontaneously chooses to do so over alternative strategies. This contradicts the influential 'vocal learning hypothesis' that musical beat synchronization is only possible in species with complex vocalizations such as humans and some songbirds. We propose an alternative view of musical beat perception and synchronization ability as a continuum onto which a wider range of species can be mapped depending on their ability to perform and coordinate the general abilities listed above through association with reward.
Article
Full-text available
Musicality can be defined as a natural, spontaneously developing trait based on and constrained by biology and cognition. Music, by contrast, can be defined as a social and cultural construct based on that very musicality. One critical challenge is to delineate the constituent elements of musicality. What biological and cognitive mechanisms are essential for perceiving, appreciating and making music? Progress in understanding the evolution of music cognition depends upon adequate characterization of the constituent mechanisms of musicality and the extent to which they are present in non-human species. We argue for the importance of identifying these mechanisms and delineating their functions and developmental course, as well as suggesting effective means of studying them in human and non-human animals. It is virtually impossible to underpin the evolutionary role of musicality as a whole, but a multicomponent perspective on musicality that emphasizes its constituent capacities, development and neural cognitive specificity is an excellent starting point for a research programme aimed at illuminating the origins and evolution of musical behaviour as an autonomous trait. © 2015 The Author(s) Published by the Royal Society. All rights reserved.
Book
First published in 1973, Born to Sing is a monumental undertaking, one of the most comprehensive, totally entertaining studies of bird vocalizations ever available. It offers a global survey of modes of singing, encompassing more than 5,000 species of singing birds, with special analyses of nearly 200 species with highly developed songs. For the professional ornithologist, informed birdwatchers, biologists, psychologists, philosophers, and musicologists with a strong interest in nature.
Conference Paper
This study looked at how people store and retrieve tonal music explicitly and implicitly using a production task. Participants completed an implicit task (tune stem completion) followed by an explicit task (cued recall). The tasks were identical except for the instructions at test time. They listened to tunes and were then presented with tune stems from previously heard tunes and novel tunes. For the implicit task, they were asked to sing a note they thought would come next musically. For the explicit task, they were asked to sing the note they remembered as coming next. Experiment I found that people correctly completed significantly more old stems than new stems. Experiment 2 investigated the characteristics of music that fuel retrieval by varying a surface feature of the tune (same timbre or different timbre) from study to test and the encoding task (semantic or nonsemantic). Although we did not find that implicit and explicit memory for music were significantly dissociated for levels of processing, we did find that surface features of music affect semantic judgments and subsequent explicit retrieval.
Article
The chapter discusses the possible origins and bases of scales including those aspects of scales that are universal across musical cultures. It also addresses the perception of the basic unit of melodies and scales, the musical interval. Natural intervals are define as intervals that show maximum sensory consonance and harmony, have influenced the evolution of the scales of many musical cultures, but the standards of intonation for a given culture are the learned interval categories of the scales of that culture. Based on the results of musical interval adjustment and identification experiments, and on measurements of intonation in performance, the intonation standard for Western music appears to be a version of the equitempered scale that is slightly compressed for small intervals, and stretched for wide intervals, including the octave. The perception of musical intervals shares a number of commonalities with the perception of phonemes in speech, most notably categorical-like perception, and an equivalence of spacing, in sensation units, of categories along the respective continua. However, the perception of melodic musical intervals appears to be the only example of ideal categorical perception in which discrimination is totally dependent on identification. Therefore this chapter concludes that, rather than speech being “special” as ofttimes proclaimed by experimental psychologists it seems that music is truly special.
Chapter
This chapter provides an overview of absolute pitch. The ultimate in musical endowment is commonly regarded by musicians to be the possession of “absolute pitch” (AP), also called “perfect pitch” or “positive pitch.” It is also defined as the ability to identify the frequency or musical name of a specific tone, or, conversely, the ability to produce some designated frequency, frequency level, or musical pitch without comparing the tone with any objective reference tone. There are two major theories of why some persons have AP: (1) heredity, on the one hand, and some combination of learning, unlearning, and (2) imprinting (early) on the other. Therefore some possessors support the cause that AP is a special innate ability that one either inherits or not, that those who do inherit the trait demonstrates pitch-naming ability as soon as an appropriate situation arises, regardless of their early musical training, and that those who are not so genetically blessed can never attain the degree of excellence in identifying pitch displayed by the chosen few, no matter how much instruction they are given or how diligently they practice naming tones. Finally the development of AP depends on some more or less fortuitous set of circumstances whereby the individual is reinforced for trying to put labels on pitches.
Article
A single adult female bottlenose dolphin was tested in a series of perceptual studies. On each trial, 4 sine-wave tones were presented that contained a falling frequency contour or some other contour. There were several frequency-transposed exemplars of each contour type in each experiment. The dolphin discriminated contours at a level significantly greater than chance in all experiments. In the 1st 2 experiments, the dolphin demonstrated only modest transfer to novel stimuli and a sensitivity to the absolute frequency of stimuli. In the 3rd experiment, there was no effect of the absolute frequency of stimuli; in the 4th experiment, the dolphin successfully transferred the discrimination to novel stimuli drawn from the octave above the previously heard range. These results demonstrate dolphins' capability to perceive frequency contours, which may underlie the recognition of conspecific whistles.
Article
European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) were trained to discriminate two complex harmonic structures modeled after musical chords in a 2-alternative choice task. Musical chords provide rich acoustic structures with which to study relative pitch perception and perceptual invariance in nonhuman animals. The starlings learned the chord discrimination and transferred the discrimination to chords with different root frequencies, thus showing perceptual invariance for the chords. Further transfer tests showed that correlates of chord structure were indeed controlling discrimination performance. The proposition that the starlings were responding primarily to a sensory dimension of consonance and dissonance in the acoustic structures provides a good account of the data. The harmonic principles that govern consonance and dissonance may be important for starling auditory communication and, perhaps, auditory communication of other songbirds. From the standpoint of human music cognition, the data add to previous observations suggesting that the idea of musical universals may be extended to species other than humans.
Article
Black-capped Chickadee (Parus atricapillus) song consists of two notes, termed fee and bee. Frequency measures at three key points (at the start and end of fee, and at the start of bee) were obtained from the songs of a large sample of chickadees (n = 151) in the wild. In this sample, 19 birds produced songs shifted downward in frequency as well as their normal songs. Analysis of normal song revealed that fee declines in frequency in a glissando of nearly pure tone, then continues at greatly reduced amplitude at the start of bee; whereas bee, also a nearly pure tone, is always lower in frequency than either the start or end of fee. The absolute pitches (frequencies) of these measures vary substantially among birds, but much less within individuals. In contrast, pitch intervals (ratios of higher to lower frequencies) for frequency changes among the three measures are highly invariant among birds. Moreover, chickadees with normal and frequency-shifted songs maintain virtually the same pitch intervals in both. This analysis suggests that the absolute and relative pitch constancies in chickadee song production may provide information for individual and species recognition, respectively.
Article
European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) learned to discriminate patterns of 2000-Hz tones organized into rhythmic as compared with random, arrhythmic temporal structures, and the perceptual processes underlying the discrimination were then analyzed. Two rhythmic patterns were constructed, for different birds, according to a linear rule in which tones and intertone intervals of equal duration alternated or according to a hierarchical rule in which two subpatterns alternated. The arrhythmic pattern was a sequence of tone and intertone intervals each of random duration. The birds were required to peck at one key in the presence of a rhythmic pattern and at another key in the presence of the arrhythmic pattern to obtain food reward. All birds learned the rhythmic–arrhythmic discrimination, and discrimination accuracy was the same for both the linear and hierarchical rhythmic structures. In a series of transfer tests that followed, discrimination performance was tested when the temporal structure of the rhythmic stimulus patterns was transformed and when their pitch was shifted up or down an octave. For temporal transformations, performance was well maintained under (a) a log transformation which, from test to test, changed the absolute duration of tones and intertone intervals but kept their ratios constant (a simple tempo transformation); (b) an additive transformation which kept either tone duration or intertone interval constant from test to test while their counterparts changed in duration; and (c) a pattern interchange in which, for the appropriate birds, the linear was substituted for the hierarchical pattern, or the hierarchical was substituted for the linear pattern. Performance deteriorated (but remained above chance), however, when rhythmic patterns were degraded by holding tone (or intertone) duration constant while intertone (or tone) durations varied randomly within a test session. Performance was also well maintained when the baseline temporal patterns were shifted an octave in pitch, but the data do not necessarily force the conclusion that the birds showed true octave generalization. The results suggest the birds solved the rhythmic–arrhythmic discrimination on the basis of a nominal, qualitative pattern attribute, rhythmicity. Patterns are high in rhythmicity if pattern components are of constant duration within a test session, and if they reoccur periodically. Rhythmicity deteriorates as pattern components vary in duration within a test session and reoccur at varying time intervals. The results also show that the human capacity for discrimination among complex temporal patterns of sound is shared with at least one other species and may, therefore, represent a perceptual process that is—within limits yet to be determined—phylogenetically general.