ArticlePublisher preview availableLiterature Review

It Is Time to Rethink Central Auditory Processing Disorder Protocols for School-Aged Children

American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
American Journal of Audiology
Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the author.

Abstract and Figures

Purpose The purpose of this article is to review the literature that pertains to ongoing concerns regarding the central auditory processing construct among school-aged children and to assess whether the degree of uncertainty surrounding central auditory processing disorder (CAPD) warrants a change in current protocols. Method Methodology on this topic included a review of relevant and recent literature through electronic search tools (e.g., ComDisDome, PsycINFO, Medline, and Cochrane databases); published texts; as well as published articles from the Journal of the American Academy of Audiology; the American Journal of Audiology; the Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research; and Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools. Results This review revealed strong support for the following: (a) Current testing of CAPD is highly influenced by nonauditory factors, including memory, attention, language, and executive function; (b) the lack of agreement regarding the performance criteria for diagnosis is concerning; (c) the contribution of auditory processing abilities to language, reading, and academic and listening abilities, as assessed by current measures, is not significant; and (d) the effectiveness of auditory interventions for improving communication abilities has not been established. Conclusions Routine use of CAPD test protocols cannot be supported, and strong consideration should be given to redirecting focus on assessing overall listening abilities. Also, intervention needs to be contextualized and functional. A suggested protocol is provided for consideration. All of these issues warrant ongoing research.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
AJA
Tutorial
It Is Time to Rethink Central Auditory
Processing Disorder Protocols
for School-Aged Children
David A. DeBonis
a
Purpose: The purpose of this article is to review the
literature that pertains to ongoing concerns regarding the
central auditory processing construct among school-aged
children and to assess whether the degree of uncertainty
surrounding central auditory processing disorder (CAPD)
warrants a change in current protocols.
Method: Methodology on this topic included a review
of relevant and recent literature through electronic
search tools (e.g., ComDisDome, PsycINFO, Medline,
and Cochrane databases); published texts; as well
as published articles from the Journal of the American
Academy of Audiology;theAmerican Journal of Audiology;
the Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research;
and Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in
Schools.
Results: This review revealed strong support for the following:
(a) Current testing of CAPD is highly influenced by nonauditory
factors, including memory, attention, language, and executive
function; (b) the lack of agreement regarding the performance
criteria for diagnosis is concerning; (c) the contribution of
auditory processing abilities to language, reading, and academic
and listening abilities, as assessed by current measures, is not
significant; and (d) the effectiveness of auditory interventions for
improving communication abilities has not been established.
Conclusions: Routine use of CAPD test protocols cannot
be supported, and strong consideration should be given
to redirecting focus on assessing overall listening abilities.
Also, intervention needs to be contextualized and functional.
A suggested protocol is provided for consideration. All of
these issues warrant ongoing research.
After many years of discussion, the reality of central
auditory processing disorder (CAPD) as a diag-
nostic construct is still far from any scenario that
would put an end to the ongoing questioning of both its
existence and its value. The concept of CAPD as a unique
diagnostic entity that could be assessed and treated in
school-aged children continues to engender controversy.
Cacace and McFarland (2005) described the current status
of CAPD construct as stalled; Cowan, Rosen, and Moore
(2009) referred to the auditory processing-related research
as stagnating(p. 188); and even proponents of CAPD
have admitted that the persistent lack of evidence validat-
ing the nature of the disorder and the most appropriate
test protocol threatens its viability as a diagnostic entity
(Bellis, 2002; Dawes & Bishop, 2009; DeBonis & Moncrieff,
2008).
The purpose of this article is to review the literature
that pertains to ongoing concerns regarding the central
auditory processing construct among school-aged children
and to assess whether the degree of uncertainty surrounding
CAPD warrants a change in current protocols.
CAPD and Auditory Processing
Disorder (APD)
It may be helpful to first clarify the use of the terms
central auditory processing disorder and auditory processing
disorder. CAPD was the original diagnostic term used for
individuals who were believed to exhibit sensory process-
ing deficits that are more pronounced in the auditory mo-
dality(American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
[ASHA], 2005, p. 2). Jerger and Musiek (2000), working
with 14 scientists and clinicians, recommended use of the
latter term in an effort to avoid attachment of specific ana-
tomic loci to the disorder and to better reflect peripheral
and central contributions to the auditory difficulties. Although
ASHA (2005) continued to support the use of the CAPD
terminology, they also conceded that the terms could be used
a
The College of Saint Rose, Albany, NY
Correspondence to David A. DeBonis: debonisd@strose.edu
Editor and Associate Editor: Larry Humes
Received July 31, 2014
Revision received December 10, 2014
Accepted January 11, 2015
DOI: 10.1044/2015_AJA-14-0037
Disclosure: The author has declared that no competing interests existed at the time
of publication.
American Journal of Audiology Vol. 24 124136 June 2015 Copyright © 2015 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association124
... Besides these peripheral function problems, we consider the cognitive system to be the cause of LiD. Recent studies have reported that fundamental LiD is associated with cognitive weakness or poor attention [3,4,[8][9][10][11][12][13][14][15] and speech-language processing mechanisms beyond the central auditory nervous system [4]. In these studies, children or adults with and without LiD conducted cognitive tasks. ...
... Although participants with a history of ASD or ADHD diagnosis were strictly excluded from this study, adults with LiD might have both attention traits and show their difficulties in both questionnaires. The poor attention problems in adults with LiD noted in previous studies [3,4,[8][9][10][11][12][13][14][15] might be related to these traits of developmental disorders. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Some individuals have a normal audiogram but have listening difficulties (LiD). As many studies have investigated the relationship between listening and developmental disorders, the traits of developmental disorders might explain the symptoms of LiD. In this study, we examined the traits of developmental disorders of adults with LiD to help clarify the cause of LiD symptoms. Methods: In total, 60 adults with LiD and 57 adults without LiD were included. Participants completed a questionnaire for the autism spectrum quotient (AQ) test, the Adult Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Self-Rating Scale (A-ADHD), the Adolescent/Adult Sensory Profile (SP), and the severity of subjective LiD in daily life. Results: Before analysis, we excluded participants with LiD who were already diagnosed or met the criteria for autism spectrum disorder (ASD) or ADHD, and the results of the remaining 30 participants (50.0%) with LiD were analyzed. Adults with LiD showed higher scores than those without LiD in the AQ. Attention switching in the AQ and attention ability in the A-ADHD scale were correlated with the severity of LiD symptoms in everyday life. The AQ scores were also significantly correlated with subscales of the SP. Conclusions: Adults with LiD showed greater autistic traits than those without LiD; therefore, LiD symptoms are possibly related to autistic symptoms. Furthermore, adults with LiD might have attention disorder traits of both ASD and ADHD and sensory processing problems. These findings suggest that the attention problems in adults with LiD noted in previous studies might be related to these traits of developmental disorders.
... The perceptual skills and behaviour of children with APD vary, as the deficit can be in any area of auditory processing (e.g., recruitment, coding, memory, and time processing) (Smoski et al., 1992). It is observed that children with APD also show deficits in language, PA, and cognitive functions (De Bonis, 2015). A child diagnosed with APD may have a listening divergence (De Wit et al., 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this minireview is to explore the relationship between auditory (AP) and phonological processing and to probe potential ‘cascade effects’ on literacy development. The overall purpose of this study is to specify auditory deficits in language and literacy outcomes to inform intervention. Important underpinnings of language and literacy development and characteristics of AP, phonological processing, and phonological awareness in children with literacy disorders in light of auditory processing skills. Children with language and literacy impairments experience difficulties processing temporal and/or spectral changes in acoustic stimuli resulting from atypical neural synchronization. On a behavioural level, studies have revealed a relationship between temporal processing skills (e.g., rise time discrimination, frequency modulation sensitivity) and literacy development. While research remains inconclusive on intervention efficacy centred on auditory processing, this review serves as the stepping stone for investigating intervention methods focused specifically on temporal processing. Frameworks associated with literacy deficits and interventions may benefit from auditory modality-specific assessment and interventions.
... Auditory processing disorder (APD) refers to a deficit in the neural processing of auditory information that can result in higher-order disorders related to learning, attention, memory, cognitive, communicative, or language-related skills [1,2]. APD affects an individual's ability to adequately acquire auditory skills (i.e., detection, discrimination, identification, and comprehension of speech), especially in complex noisy listening environments [1,3]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Identification of auditory processing disorders is achieved using questionnaires along with linguistic, non-linguistic, and auditory processing tests. Notably, the questionnaires "Children's Auditory Performance Scale" (CHAPS) and "Auditory Processing Domain Questionnaire" (APDQ) are widely recognized and used. The current study investigated the psychometric properties of the CHAPS and APDQ in Greek Cypriot children. Methods: The CHAPS and APDQ questionnaires were completed by parents of 40 Greek Cypriot children, 16 typically developing (TD) children, and 24 children with a history of Speech Sound Disorders (SSDs). Results: There were significant differences between the two groups on both questionnaires. Cronbach's alpha was calculated at α = 0.922 for the CHAPS total score and α = 0.926 for the APDQ total score. The Receiver Operating Curve (ROC) analysis provided a cutoff point equal to −0.30 (AUC 0.849, p < 0.001) for CHAPS and a cutoff point equal to 90.00 (AUC 0.820, p < 0.001) for APDQ. Significant positive Spearman ρ correlations were observed between the CHAPS and APDQ (ρ = 0.639, p = 0.001). Conclusions: The CHAPS and APDQ can identify distinct auditory processing characteristics between in children with SSDs and TD children.
Technical Report
Full-text available
Executive summary 1. The aim of this report is to review whether A-Weighting accurately represents the hearing of the whole UK population, to explore the consequences if it does not, and to make recommendations for future research to address the problems identified. 2. The approach taken was to first research the construction of A-Weighting from its root in early research on loudness perception and the development of the equal loudness contours. We then identified three groups of interest, who may not be well represented by A-Weighting. These are people with age-related hearing loss, neurodivergent people, and noise-sensitive people. These groups were chosen because they each represent a different kind of hearing difference, and they are all a substantial size. 3. A detailed literature review was conducted to examine the available evidence of hearing differences in the three groups and to discuss how these relate to A-Weighting and its uses. We reviewed 540 papers and cite 182 in this report. This dataset was then analysed to identify significant research gaps and suggest future research to fill them. 4. A-Weighting was found to be a significant way in which the assumption of normal hearing is embedded in a very wide range of acoustic assessment and legislation. This is because A-Weighting was developed from the equal loudness contours, and the contours are produced from measurements on a relatively small sample of otologically normal people-18-25-year-olds with no hearing impairment. This is important because A-Weighting extends far beyond its original use to convert sound levels into loudness estimates and is now found in most assessments and control of noise worldwide, from product design to environmental noise. 5. People with age-related hearing loss represent 7.9% of the UK population. Age-related hearing loss is caused by degradation of the hair cells' transducer mechanism in the inner ear. Among other effects, this typically causes problems with masking, where it becomes harder to pick a target speech sound out of a noisy background. Because background noise is usually quantified using A-Weighting, and A-Weighting models normal hearing, this may mean that the enhanced need for reduced background interference is often not met for this group. 6. Neurodivergent people (who include autistic people, those with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), dyslexia, dyspraxia, dysgraphia, dyscalculia, and Tourette's) make up about 10% of the UK population. Evidence was found of auditory processing differences for autistic people, and for people with ADHD and dyslexia. The auditory processing differences occur at a low level in the brain and can result in difficulty separating out sound signals, especially when trying to listen to someone speak against a background of competing speech. It is likely that enhanced background noise targets would improve the experience of this group, but it is currently difficult to translate the laboratory findings on auditory processing into new noise assessment methods. There is too little evidence for people with dyspraxia, dyscalculia, dysgraphia and Tourette's to base a conclusion on. 7. People who are noise-sensitive represent 17-50% of the population. In this report, this group included people who are more sensitive to general noise, people with misophonia and people with hyperacusis. For this group, the widespread use of A-Weighted sound level to predict noise annoyance is likely to significantly underpredict their heightened response. Hyperacusis is very common in autistic people, and it is particularly likely that A-Weighted levels do not represent the experience of this group. 8. It is not possible to simply adapt the A-Weighting curve to better fit any of the groups at this time. Partly this is because of lack of data on how to adjust, but mostly it is because most of the differences in sound perception are not equivalent to a simple transformation in loudness. The exception is for people with hyperacusis, where more detailed data on their loudness responses might enable a new weighting to be derived. 9. The overarching research gap identified is a lack of translational research. There is a need for research that builds on existing laboratory studies of hearing differences and translates them into guidelines for practitioners to assess sound and design spaces suitable for people with atypical hearing. 10. We also identify nine specific research gaps and projects to address them: a. Acoustic guidelines for spaces for people with a hearing impairment b. Acoustic design guidelines for people with auditory processing problems c. Accurate estimate of the numbers of noise sensitive people in the UK d. Loudness for people with age-related hearing impairment e. Loudness functions for autistic people with hyperacusis f. Equal loudness contours for autistic people with hyperacusis g. Demographic factors as a modulating effect on neurodivergent hearing h. Loudness functions for people with hyperacusis i. Psychoacoustic factors in misophonia Of these, we suggest that the first three are the most important. 11. This report represents an initial exercise in a much larger project for acoustics to respond to the aural diversity movement-the idea that a wide range of different kinds of hearing differences exist, in contrast to the assumed binary division of normal hearing and impaired hearing. This project could be repeated with other groups of people, such as those with noise-induced hearing loss, Meniere's disease, or many other conditions.
Article
Objectives To examine the effects of distractor sounds presented to the contralateral ear on speech intelligibility in patients with listening difficulties without apparent peripheral pathology and in control participants. Design This study examined and analyzed 15 control participants (age range, 22 to 30 years) without any complaints of listening difficulties and 15 patients (age range, 15 to 33 years) diagnosed as having listening difficulties without apparent peripheral pathology in the outpatient clinic of the Department of Otolaryngology-Head and Neck Surgery, Tohoku University Hospital. Speech intelligibility for 50 Japanese monosyllables presented to the right ear was examined under the following three different conditions: “without contralateral sound,” “with continuous white noise in the contralateral ear,” and “with music stimuli in the contralateral ear.” Results The results indicated the following: (1) speech intelligibility was significantly worse in the patient group with contralateral music stimuli and noise stimuli; (2) speech intelligibility was significantly worse with contralateral music stimuli than with contralateral noise stimuli in the patient group; (3) there was no significant difference in speech intelligibility among three contralateral masking conditions (without contra-stimuli, with contra-noise, and with contra-music) in the control group, although average and median values of speech intelligibility tended to be worse with contralateral music stimuli than without contralateral stimuli. Conclusions Significantly larger masking effects due to a contralateral distractor sound observed in patients with listening difficulties without apparent peripheral pathology may suggest the possible involvement of masking mechanisms other than the energetic masking mechanism occurring in the periphery in these patients. In addition, it was also shown that the masking effect is more pronounced with real environmental sounds, that is, music with lyrics, than with continuous steady noise, which is often used as a masker for speech-in-noise testing in clinical trials. In other words, it should be noted that a speech-in-noise test using such steady noise may underestimate the degree of listening problems of patients with listening difficulties in their daily lives, and a speech-in-noise test using a masker such as music and/or speech sounds could make listening problems more obvious in patients with listening difficulties.
Preprint
OBJECTIVES: Listening difficulty refers to difficulty hearing speech despite normal pure-tone audiometry. It is as prevalent as clinical hearing loss among adults, but incidence, causes and treatment remain poorly understood in children. We hypothesized that four medical risk factors would be associated with listening difficulty in children. METHODS: A prospective, case-control study was conducted in a tertiary care childrens hospital. Children (6-13 years old) with clinically normal hearing divided into listening difficulty (n=68) and typically developing (n=84) groups based on a validated caregiver report. All children were native English users without reported conditions restricting participation. Testing included extended high frequency (EHF) audiometry, speech and spatial perception, and cognitive function. Caregiver reports, electronic medical records, and testing ascertained risk of prematurity, head injury, otitis media and EHF hearing loss. Logistic regression, chi-square, correlation, and odds ratios determined associations of listening difficulty with risk factors. RESULTS: Prevalence and risk of prematurity (18%, OR 3.39 [95% CI, 1.1-10.2]), head injury (21%, 3.37 [1.2-9.3]), and high frequency hearing loss (32%, 2.42 [1.1-5.5]) were significantly greater for children with listening difficulty than typically developing children. Ventilation tubes were no more common in the listening difficulty group (25%, 1.14 [0.5-2.4]). EHF hearing loss was associated with prematurity and tubes. Prematurity, tubes, and EHF loss were significantly related to poorer competing speech perception and dichotic listening. CONCLUSIONS: Children with a history of prematurity, head injury or EHF loss were at increased risk of listening difficulties. Early intervention to boost communication skills could potentially improve poorer long-term outcomes.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose Auditory processing disorder (APD) has been studied in both research and clinic settings, but the relation between the two has not been addressed. In a longitudinal research study (SICLiD), we found that children with clinically normal audiometry who had caregiver-reported listening difficulties (LiD), with or without clinically assessed APD, performed poorly on both listening and cognitive tests. Specific questions asked here were, for the children with LiD, what other neurodevelopmental clinical conditions were identified, what interventions were used by different clinical providers, and how clinical practice was predicted by research results. Methods Study setting was a large, research-led, tertiary pediatric hospital. Electronic medical records of 74 children aged 6-13 years, recruited into SICLiD and assigned to an LiD group based on a validated and reliable caregiver report (ECLiPS), were independently reviewed. Focus was on clinical assessments and interventions following appointments provided in the Hospital Divisions of Audiology, Occupational Therapy, Psychology (Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics), and Speech-Language Pathology (SLP), prior to participation in SICLiD. Descriptive statistics on clinical encounters, identified conditions, and interventions were compared with quantitative, standardized performance on SICLiD assessments of listening and cognitive function. SICLiD z-scores were compared for participants with and without each clinical condition using univariate and logistic prediction analyses. Results Most (86%) of the children with LiD had been evaluated by at least one clinical service. Overall, 24 assessment categories related to LiD, including APD, were identified. Most common conditions were attention (32%), language (28%), hearing (18%), anxiety (16%), and autism spectrum (6%) disorders. Performance on SICLiD measures varied significantly between providers, conditions, and interventions. Significant relationships between SICLiD and clinical conditions were mostly caregiver-reported items from the ECLiPS or the Children’s Communication Checklist (CCC-2). Other significant correlations were scarce, but included the SCAN composite score, which predicted clinical language and attention, but not other auditory abilities or APD. SICLiD data combined with caregiver reports provided reliable predictions of all clinical conditions except APD. Conclusions The variety of disciplines, assessments, conditions and interventions revealed here supports previous studies showing that LiD and APD are multifaceted problems of neurodevelopment. Comparisons between clinical- and research-based assessments suggest a diagnostic path that prioritizes caregiver reports and selected psychometric tests for screening and diagnostic purposes.
Article
Full-text available
Objectives Assessing listening difficulties and associated complaints can be challenging. Often, measures of peripheral auditory functions are within normal ranges, making clinicians feel unsure about proper management strategies. The range and nature of observed or experienced difficulties might be better captured using a qualitative measure. The Evaluation of Children’s Listening and Processing Skills (ECLiPS) questionnaire was designed to broadly profile the auditory and cognitive problems often present in children with listening difficulties. This 38-item questionnaire was initially standardized in British children aged 6 to 11 years, was subsequently modified for use with North-American children, and was recently translated into Flemish–Dutch. This study aimed to compare typical scores of the Flemish version with the UK and US versions, and to evaluate and compare its psychometric quality based on Rasch analysis. Design We selected 112 Flemish children aged 6 to 11 years with verified normal hearing and typical development, and asked two caregivers of every child to fill out the ECLiPS. Data from two comparator samples were analyzed, including responses for 71 North-American children and 650 British children. Typical values for ECLiPS factors and aggregates were determined as a function of age and gender, and meaningful differences across samples were analyzed. Rasch analyses were performed to evaluate whether ECLiPS response categories work as intended, and whether item scores fit a linear equal interval measurement scale that works the same way for everyone. Item and person metrics were derived, including separation and reliability indices. We investigated whether items function similarly across linguistically and culturally different samples. Results ECLiPS scores were relatively invariant to age. Girls obtained higher scores compared with boys, mainly for items related to memory and attention, and pragmatic and social skills. Across ECLiPS versions, the most pronounced differences were found for items probing social skills. With respect to its psychometric quality, ECLiPS response categories work as intended, and ECLiPS items were found to fit the Rasch measurement scale. Cultural differences in responses were noted for some items, belonging to different factors. Item separation and reliability indices generally pointed toward sufficient variation in item difficulty. In general, person separation (and reliability) metrics, quantifying the instrument’s ability to distinguish between poor and strong performers (in a reproducible manner), were low. This is expected from samples of typically developing children with homogeneous and high levels of listening ability. Conclusions Across the languages assessed here, the ECLiPS caregiver questionnaire was verified to be a psychometrically valid qualitative measure to assess listening and processing skills, which can be used to support the assessment and management of elementary school children referred with LiD.
Article
Full-text available
The observed listening performances of 64 children with central auditory processing (CAP) disorders were obtained from teachers using the Children’s Auditory Processing Performance Scale. The data obtained indicated that the listening performances of these children vary greatly, depending upon the listening conditions and the listening functions being rated. The results also provide evidence that children with CAP disorders have difficulties not only in stressful or competing listening conditions, but also in quiet or ideal listening conditions.
Chapter
This publication is the opening number of a series which the Psychometric Society proposes to issue. It reports the first large experimental inquiry, carried out by the methods of factor analysis described by Thurstone in The Vectors of the Mind 1. The work was made possible by financial grants from the Social Science Research Committee of the University of Chicago, the American Council of Education, and the Carnegie Corporation of New York. The results are eminently worthy of the assistance so generously accorded. Thurstone’s previous theoretical account, lucid and comprehensive as it is, is intelligible only to those who have a knowledge of matrix algebra. Hence his methods have become known to British educationists chiefly from the monograph published by W. P. Alexander8. This enquiry has provoked a good deal of criticism, particularly from Professor Spearman’s school ; and differs, as a matter of fact, from Thurstone’s later expositions. Hence it is of the greatest value to have a full and simple illustration of his methods, based on a concrete inquiry, from Professor Thurstone himself.