ArticlePDF Available

Benefits of Mindfulness Meditation on Emotional Intelligence, General Self-Efficacy, and Perceived Stress: Evidence from Thailand

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

This study investigated the benefits of mindfulness meditation practice on self-report emotional intelligence, general self-efficacy, and general perceived stress. The survey data was collected from 317 respondents in Thailand. The results analyzed using the partial least squares regression indicated that mindfulness meditation practice tended to associate positively with emotional intelligence. Practicing mindfulness meditation also negatively relates to general perceived stress directly and indirectly through emotional intelligence. However, the positive association between the meditation and general self-efficacy was only found to be mediated by emotional intelligence.
Content may be subject to copyright.
This article was downloaded by: [200.178.118.85]
On: 26 August 2015, At: 23:35
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered
office: 5 Howick Place, London, SW1P 1WG
Click for updates
Journal of Spirituality in Mental Health
Publication details, including instructions for authors and
subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/wspi20
Benefits of Mindfulness Meditation on
Emotional Intelligence, General Self-
Efficacy, and Perceived Stress: Evidence
from Thailand
Peerayuth Charoensukmongkola
a National Institute of Development Administration, International
College, Bangkok, Thailand
Published online: 28 Jul 2014.
To cite this article: Peerayuth Charoensukmongkol (2014) Benefits of Mindfulness Meditation on
Emotional Intelligence, General Self-Efficacy, and Perceived Stress: Evidence from Thailand, Journal
of Spirituality in Mental Health, 16:3, 171-192, DOI: 10.1080/19349637.2014.925364
To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19349637.2014.925364
PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE
Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the
“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,
our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to
the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions
and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,
and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content
should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources
of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,
proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or
howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising
out of the use of the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any
substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,
systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &
Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-
and-conditions
Downloaded by [200.178.118.85] at 23:35 26 August 2015
Journal of Spirituality in Mental Health, 16:171–192, 2014
Copyright © Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 1934-9637 print/1934-9645 online
DOI: 10.1080/19349637.2014.925364
Benefits of Mindfulness Meditation on
Emotional Intelligence, General Self-Efficacy,
and Perceived Stress: Evidence from Thailand
PEERAYUTH CHAROENSUKMONGKOL
National Institute of Development Administration, International College, Bangkok, Thailand
This study investigated the benefits of mindfulness meditation
practice on self-report emotional intelligence, general self-efficacy,
and general perceived stress. The survey data was collected from
317 respondents in Thailand. The results analyzed using the par-
tial least squares regression indicated that mindfulness meditation
practice tended to associate positively with emotional intelligence.
Practicing mindfulness meditation also negatively relates to gen-
eral perceived stress directly and indirectly through emotional
intelligence. However, the positive association between the medi-
tation and general self-efficacy was only found to be mediated by
emotional intelligence.
KEYWORDS mindfulness meditation, self-efficacy, stress,
Thailand, emotional intelligence
INTRODUCTION
Research related to the benefits of mindfulness on psychological well-being
and a variety of performance measures has been growing extensively over
the past decade. Mindfulness is widely defined as the ability to “bring one’s
complete attention to the experiences occurring in the present moment, in
a nonjudgmental or accepting way” (Baer, Smith, Hopkins, Krietemeyer, &
Toney, 2006, p. 27). Literature has shown that one particular practice that
can enhance the level of mindfulness is mindfulness meditation (MM; Brown
& Ryan, 2003; Charoensukmongkol, 2013; Jha, Krompinger, & Baime, 2007;
Address correspondence to Peerayuth Charoensukmongkol, National Institute of
Development Administration, International College, 118 Moo 3, Serithai Road, Klong-Chan,
Bangkapi, Bangkok 10240, Thailand. E-mail: peerayuth@outlook.com
171
Downloaded by [200.178.118.85] at 23:35 26 August 2015
172 P. Charoensukmongkol
Kabat-Zinn, 1990). Cahn and Polich (2006, p. 180) defined meditation as
“practices that self-regulate the body and mind, thereby affecting mental
events by engaging a specific attentional set.” The key characteristic of MM is
how practitioners learn to be consistently attentive to their own body action
(e.g., movement, breath) or internal stimuli (e.g., emotion, thought) that they
are experiencing at the present moment (Kabat-Zinn, 2005). MM also requires
practitioners to cultivate a moment-to-moment awareness of the self and
the environment (Wallace, 2006), acknowledge any stimuli nonjudgmentally
(Kabat-Zinn, 1994), and maintain equanimity in the face of any pleasant or
unpleasant sensation they are encountering during the practice (Goenka,
2002).
Although MM is rooted in Eastern cultures, research on its contribu-
tions in Eastern countries is still lacking compared to Western countries.
Therefore, the present study was conducted using the survey data collected
from respondents in Thailand who came from diverse backgrounds. The
present study aimed to test the outcomes of MM on: (a) emotional intel-
ligence, (b) general self-efficacy, and (c) general perceived stress. These
three outcomes were chosen because existing studies have shown that they
contributed significantly to better health, well-being, and occupational per-
formance (Bedini, Gladwell, Dudley, & Clancy, 2011; Joseph & Newman,
2010; Yang, Kim, & McFarland, 2011).
In particular, there are two main research questions that the present
study intended to address. The first question is whether the regular practice
of MM could explain the difference in the level of emotional intelligence,
general self-efficacy, and general perceived stress. In addition to the direct
effect, the second research question is whether the effect of MM on general
self-efficacy and general perceived stress could be mediated by emotional
intelligence. Although previous studies have provided some evidence about
the contribution of MM on various psychological outcomes, little research
tended to focus on its indirect effect. Emotional intelligence was selected as
a mediator for this study because it has been shown as a good predictor for
self-efficacy (Rathi & Rastogi, 2008) and stress (Salovey, Stroud, Woolery, &
Epel, 2002).
BENEFITS OF PRACTICING MINDFULNESS MEDITATION
Emotional Intelligence
Emotional intelligence (EQ) was defined by Salovey and Mayer (1990,
p. 189) as “the subset of social intelligence that involves the ability to mon-
itor one’s own and others’ feelings and emotions, to discriminate among
them and to use this information to guide one’s thinking and actions.” EQ
represents the ability of individuals to make a connection between emo-
tions and reasoning in a way that enables them to use emotions to guide
Downloaded by [200.178.118.85] at 23:35 26 August 2015
Benefits of Mindfulness Meditation on Psychological Well-Being 173
their actions and use reasoning to regulate their emotions (Mayer, Salovey, &
Caruso, 2000). A number of studies have found EQ to be a good predictor of
psychological well-being and work performance (Fitzgerald & Schutte, 2010;
Goleman, 1995). As a result, these contributions have made EQ trainings
become one of the major interventions to help people deal effectively with
various life- and job-related stressors.
Mayer and Salovey (1997) suggested that EQ consists of four functions.
First, appraisal and expression of emotion in the self refers to the ability to
understand one’s own deep emotions and be able to express them naturally.
Second, appraisal and expression of emotion in others refers to the ability
to perceive and understand the emotions of other people (Goleman, 1995).
Third, regulation of emotion in the self refers to the ability to control one’s
own emotion, which is crucial for an individual to recover quickly when
experiencing a negative emotion. Fourth, using emotion to facilitate decision-
making represents the ability to direct one’s own emotions to help improve
performance.
Mindfulness Meditation and Emotional Intelligence
In literature, scholars have reported a positive relationship between mindful-
ness and EQ (Baer et al., 2006; Brown & Ryan, 2003). Since the objective of
MM is to enhance the level of mindfulness, it can significantly facilitate the
development of EQ. First, regularly practicing MM can enhance the ability to
understand one’s own emotions (Brown, Ryan, & Creswell, 2007). Since the
meditation training requires practitioners to closely observe their thoughts
and feelings moment-to-moment without any judgment or interference, prac-
titioners tend to develop a higher tendency to be aware of their emotional
state and change than those who do not. This contribution is supported by
a study conducted by Feldman, Hayes, Kumar, Greeson, and Laurenceau
(2007), which found that the level of mindfulness was associated positively
with more clarity of feelings, attention to feelings, and lower distraction.
Second, people who regularly practice MM can easily develop the ability
to detect and understand the emotions of others. In particular, being mindful
allows individual to better focus their attention on how other people around
them are feeling (Brown et al., 2007), which subsequently helps them deci-
pher emotional cues of others more accurately (Krasner et al., 2009). This
contribution is supported by a study conducted by Shapiro, Schwartz, and
Bonner (1998), which found that participants who attended the MM program
tended to score higher on the overall empathy self-reported measurement.
A study by Brown and Kasser (2005) also found that the level of mind-
fulness tended to associate positively with a felt sense of relatedness and
interpersonal closeness.
Third, regularly practicing MM can significantly enhance the ability of
individuals to regulate and control their emotions (Cahn & Polich, 2006).
Downloaded by [200.178.118.85] at 23:35 26 August 2015
174 P. Charoensukmongkol
In particular, Feldman et al. (2007) found that people with a higher level
of mindfulness tended to recover quickly from emotional distress compared
with those with a lower level of mindfulness. Moreover, research found that
practicing MM could heighten one’s meta-cognitive ability (Zeidan, Johnson,
Diamond, David, & Goolkasian, 2010), which is considered a higher-level
cognitive ability that allows individuals to monitor and control their thought
process (Flavell, 1987). In EQ literature, this meta-cognitive ability was pro-
posed as a crucial ability for people to effectively regulate their emotions
(Mayer et al., 2000; Salovey & Mayer, 1990).
Finally, regularly practicing MM can also allow individuals to effectively
use their emotions. This is because the quality of being mindful to one’s
own emotions, both negative and positive, enables one to appropriately
focus on a task that might be performed better when a specific emotion is in
place; and to avoid performing a task that cannot be performed well under
such emotion (Averill, Chon, & Hahn, 2001). For example, George (2000)
argued that while being in positive moods is important for tasks that require
creativity, integrative thinking, and deductive reasoning, being in a negative
mood tends to make people become more effective in tasks that require
attention to detail, detection of errors and problems, and careful information
processing. Without being sufficiently mindful, on the other hand, it can be
difficult for people to focus on a task that could benefit from their currently
felt emotion.
Considering the aforementioned arguments, therefore, the first set of
hypotheses is presented:
H1: Individuals who regularly practice MM will report higher level of
emotional intelligence than those who do not.
General Self-Efficacy
Self-efficacy represents the belief of an individual that he/she can perform
well in a specific task (Bandura, 1997,1999). Studies have found that self-
efficacy tended to correlate strongly with many positive individual-level
outcomes. For example, it was reported that people with high self-efficacy
tended to have better health (Schreurs, van Emmerik, Notelaers, & De
Witte, 2010), higher achievement, and better performance (Brady-Amoon &
Fuertes, 2011). Studies found that they tended to be optimistic about life and
had more confidence that they could deal effectively with life events and
stressors (Nasurdin, Ramayah, & Chee, 2009). On the other hand, people
with low self-efficacy were reported to have more anxiety and depression
(Jimmieson, 2000). Lack of self-efficacy was also found to be associated posi-
tively with a sense of helplessness, low self-esteem, and pessimistic thoughts
(Schwarzer & Hallum, 2008). Studies conducted in organizational settings
Downloaded by [200.178.118.85] at 23:35 26 August 2015
Benefits of Mindfulness Meditation on Psychological Well-Being 175
have also found that employees with high self-efficacy tended to have better
performance than employees with low self-efficacy (Yang et al., 2011).
Generally, self-efficacy is considered to be domain-specific (Wang &
Richarde, 1988). For example, self-efficacy has been applied to some partic-
ular situation or function such as job-related efficacy or task-specific efficacy
(Lang & Lee, 2005). However, scholars have suggested that self-efficacy can
also be considered a global confidence in one’s own competence (Schwarzer
& Jerusalem, 1995). In this sense, general self-efficacy is defined as “indi-
viduals’ perception of their ability to perform across a variety of different
situations” (Judge, Erez, & Bono, 1998, p. 170). Unlike task-specific self-
efficacy, general self-efficacy is a stable trait-like belief in one’s own potential
to perform well in a wide array of tasks (DeRue & Morgeson, 2007).
Mindfulness Meditation and General Self-Efficacy
There are several reasons to support that individuals who regularly practice
MM tend to develop higher general self-efficacy than those who do not. For
example, Bandura (1997) proposed that the sources of self-efficacy involve
both cognitive and affective processes. From the social cognitive theory,
Bandura (1986) argued that individuals tend to act according to how they
interpret the realities, and this activity in turn is strongly determined by their
level of self-awareness, self-regulation, and self-control. For example, when
people are overwhelmed by a negative psychological state such as worry or
anxiety, it can be difficult for them to focus their thinking and to evaluate
their true potentials accurately. In the same manner, Gundlach, Martinko,
and Douglas (2003, p. 232) suggested, “without an awareness or willingness
to decipher and understand how one produces beliefs about his/her own
[job] capability, it will be difficult to explain, understand, or improve existing
self-efficacy levels.”
Because self-efficacy belief is influenced by the cognitive process of
how people make causal attribution between their abilities and performance
outcomes (Bandura, 1997), regularly practicing MM can allow individuals
to make more positive attribution because it helps them overcome their
pessimistic thoughts. Specifically, a study suggested that the ability to observe
the mind’s operations nonjudgmentally was associated with more realistic
perceptions (Brown et al., 2007). Being aware and having a clear mind also
enhance the ability of people to think constructively (Kabat-Zinn, 1990).
For example, Feldman et al. (2007) found that people with a high level of
mindfulness tended to have a high level of cognitive flexibility, problem
analysis, and plan rehearsal and less stagnant deliberation. Furthermore, a
study by Astin (1997) reported that participants who completed the MM
training tended to demonstrate a higher sense of control over one’s cognitive,
affective, and behavioral experience. Studies also reported that people with
a high level of mindfulness tended to possess a high capability to deal with
Downloaded by [200.178.118.85] at 23:35 26 August 2015
176 P. Charoensukmongkol
challenges and difficulties (Feldman et al., 2007). Therefore, the following
hypothesis is presented:
H2: Individuals who regularly practice MM will report a higher level of
general self-efficacy than those who do not.
Because the sources of self-efficacy involve an emotional process
(Bandura, 1997), this study argues that MM can also indirectly influence gen-
eral self-efficacy through EQ. Although emotion and cognition are normally
treated as independent entities (Zajonc, 1980), scholars have argued that they
can be highly interdependent (Storbeck & Clore, 2007). For example, stud-
ies have shown that negative emotions tended to hinder the capability of
individuals to think rationally (Herabadi, Verplanken, & Van Knippenberg,
2009; van Knippenberg, Kooij-de Bode, & van Ginkel, 2010). In this regard,
Gundlach et al. (2003) argued that emotional awareness and emotional
regulation are considered key factors that facilitate the perception of self-
efficacy because they prevent individuals from being tampered by their
negative emotion when making causal attribution between their abilities and
outcomes.
Accordingly, the lack of ability to understand and control one’s own
negative emotions can have a detrimental effect on how people develop
general self-efficacy belief. In particular, individuals who are overwhelmed
by their negative emotions are less likely to evaluate their potential opti-
mistically compared to when they are having positive emotions (Kavanagh
& Bower, 1985). In this regard, Tsai, Chen, and Liu (2007) argued that a pos-
itive mood not only makes people easily recall an outstanding performance
that they had in the past, but it also enhances their positive feelings about
their past performance, thereby allowing them to raise expectation about
their ability. Plus, their study conducted on employees and supervisors from
insurance companies in Taiwan also found a strong positive relationship
between positive mood and task-specific self-efficacy measure (Tsai et al.,
2007). Moreover, a study by Rathi and Rastogi (2008), which was conducted
on scientists from research organizations in India, found EQ to be a good
predictor for occupational self-efficacy. Therefore:
H3: EQ will mediate the positive relationship between practicing MM
and general self-efficacy.
General Perceived Stress
According to Lazarus and Folkman (1984, p. 19), individuals tend to expe-
rience stress when they perceive events they encounter as “taxing or
exceeding his or her resources and endangering his or her well-being.”
Cohen, Kamarck, and Mermelstein (1983) argued that stress can also be
Downloaded by [200.178.118.85] at 23:35 26 August 2015
Benefits of Mindfulness Meditation on Psychological Well-Being 177
conceptualized as a generalized perception. Scholars suggested that sources
of stress, or stressors, could be caused by macroevents and microevents
(Luria & Torjman, 2009). Microevents are any small situations that can hap-
pen in everyday life (e.g., missing a bus), which can have a cumulative effect
on stress level. On the other hand, macroevents are major life events (e.g.,
losing a loved one) that suddenly cause stress and can subsequently lead
individuals into a deep depression.
Numerous studies have reported that perceived stress tended to asso-
ciate positively with psychological and health-related problems. For example,
a study by Ghorbani, Krauss, Watson, and LeBreton (2008)onAmericanand
Iranian samples found that those who were high in stress perception tended
to experience higher anxiety and depression, regardless of the national cul-
ture characteristics. Bedini et al. (2011) also found that perceived stress was
negatively associated with a quality of life measure. Perceived stress not only
affects personal life, but can also have a spillover effect on work perfor-
mance. In an organization, for example, studies have found that employees
who reported having high general perceived stress tended to have low
job satisfaction and performance (Lourel, Ford, Gamassou, Guéguen, &
Hartmann, 2009).
Mindfulness Meditation and General Perceived Stress
Regularly practicing MM can prevent an individual from being preoccupied
by stressors. The key benefit of MM on alleviating stress lies in the ability of
practitioners to be attentive to their thought and emotional reactions when
experiencing stressful situations. In most part, MM is based on the Buddhist
wisdom that the emotions and sensations that we experience are transient
phenomena; they are impermanent and unstable (Kabat-Zinn, 1990). In fact,
the purpose of practicing MM does not give individuals the ability to shield
themselves from experiencing any unwanted emotion. Rather, it makes indi-
viduals understand and get along well with those unpleasant encounters
without being affected by them (Kabat-Zinn, 1990). For this reason, when
any stressor that causes unwanted sensations or emotions arises, they only
acknowledge it nonjudgmentally until that sensation or emotion fades away
on its own (Kabat-Zinn, 1994)
In literature, scholars also argued that mindfulness tends to help individ-
uals deal effectively with unpleasant feelings, particularly because it prevents
them from underengaging (e.g., experiential avoiding, through suppressing)
and overengaging (e.g., worry, rumination, overgeneralization) with emo-
tional encounters (Baer et al., 2006; Feldman et al., 2007). Moreover, studies
have shown that individuals with a higher level of mindfulness tended to
experience lower stress-related factors. For example, Christopher and Gilbert
(2010) found that mindfulness was positively related to satisfaction with life
and low depression. Therefore:
Downloaded by [200.178.118.85] at 23:35 26 August 2015
178 P. Charoensukmongkol
H4: Individuals who practice MM regularly will report a lower level of
perceived stress than those who do not.
In addition to the direct effect, the benefit of practicing MM on perceived
stress can also be mediated through EQ. Specifically, Salovey et al. (2002)
suggested that EQ enhances the cognitive ability of individuals to cope well
with stressful events. Plus, a study by Luria and Torjman (2009) showed that
individuals who processed higher cognitive resources, as measured by their
problem solving ability, demonstrated higher tendency to cope well with
stressors over time. However, as mentioned earlier, negative felt emotions
can hinder the cognitive ability of individuals to think clearly and accurately
(Gundlach et al., 2003). Thus, the ability to understand and manage negative
emotion is crucial for people to better evaluate their coping resources to deal
with stress.
Studies have shown that people with high EQ tended to come up
with more effective coping strategies to deal with stressors. For example,
Matthews et al. (2006, p. 98) argued that persons with high EQ “tend to use
strategies such as eliciting social support and disclosure of feelings, in place
of the maladaptive coping strategy of rumination.” Their study also found
that participants who had lower EQ not only demonstrated higher worry but
also engaged more in avoidance coping when they were assigned to stressful
tasks, even after controlling for the five personality factors (Matthews et al.,
2006). Finally, a study by Nikolaou and Tsaousis (2002) also found the neg-
ative correlation between EQ and occupational stress. Therefore, the final
hypothesis is presented:
H5: EQ will mediate the negative relationship between practicing MM
and perceived stress.
METHODOLOGY
Participants
The data were collected using the online self-administered questionnaire
hosted by Survey Monkey. In order to obtain the respondents, espe-
cially those who had regularly practiced MM, to complete the survey, the
announcements of the study were posted in several major religious web sites
in Thailand. These websites offer discussion boards for members/visitors to
discuss about various topics, including meditation practices. The visitors of
these web sites are diverse and not just limited to religious purposes. Visitors
of the websites were informed about the objectives and contributions of the
study and were given the link to enter the online survey. The participa-
tion was conducted in a voluntary basis with no monetary compensation.
Downloaded by [200.178.118.85] at 23:35 26 August 2015
Benefits of Mindfulness Meditation on Psychological Well-Being 179
However, the researcher promised a 10 Thai baht donation to the charity
fund for each survey completed.
Approximately two months after the announcement, a total of 317 ques-
tionnaires were completed. The final samples included 188 female (59%)
and 129 male (41%) respondents. The average age of the respondents was
35 years old (SD =10.87). For education background, 68 had below a bach-
elor’s degree (21%), 177 held a bachelor’s degree (56%), 60 held a master’s
degree (19%), and 12 held a doctoral degree (4%). For occupation, 77 were
students (24%), 156 were full-time employees (49%), and 84 were business
owners (27%). For marital status, 208 were single (66%) and 109 were mar-
ried (34%). Finally, 93 of the respondents reported having children (29%)
whereas 224 did not (71%). The average number of children was two (SD =
0.69).
For MM practice, 200 reported that they had regularly practiced MM
(63%) and 117 reported that they did not practice at all (37%). The majority
of MM practitioners reported that they meditated by observing their breath-
ing. For the intensity of meditation, the majority of them reported that they
meditated 2 hours per day, 7 days per week, and had meditated for 5 years.
Measurements
This study used established scales that have been used by other scholars.
To ensure the validity of the constructs, all questionnaire items originally in
English were translated to Thai and then back-translated to English by a pro-
fessional bilingual translator. Finally, the contents from the back-translation
were compared with the original contents to ensure that they convey the
same meanings.
Mindfulness Meditation
This variable was measured by the number of hours per day, days per week,
and the total years that participants had practiced MM. In the survey, respon-
dents were asked if they had regularly practiced MM until the present time.
Those who answered yes to this question were then asked to describe how
they meditated. Finally, they were asked to estimate the length in hours,
days, and years that they had meditated. These variables were coded in ordi-
nal scale. The number of hours ranges from 1 to 8 (1 =less than one hour;
2=about one hour ...7=about six hours; and 8=more than six hours).
The number of days ranged from 1 to 7. The number of years ranged from
1to11(1=less than one year; 2=about one year to 10 =about nine
years; 11 =more than ten years). For those who did not practice MM, these
three variables were coded as 0. These numbers were then used to construct
a single reflective latent variable.
Downloaded by [200.178.118.85] at 23:35 26 August 2015
180 P. Charoensukmongkol
Emotional Intelligence
The Wong and Law EQ Scale, which was originally developed for measuring
the EQ of respondents in the Eastern country, was used to measure the level
of EQ (Law et al., 2004; Wong & Law, 2002). The scale contains 16 items in
5-point Likert scales ranging from 1 (strongly disagree)to5(strongly agree).
Sample items include “I have good understanding of my own emotions”
(emotion appraisal of self), “I am sensitive to the feelings and emotions
of others” (emotion appraisal of other), I am quite capable of controlling
my own emotions (emotion regulation), and “I would always encourage
myself to try my best” (use of emotion). First, these indicators were used to
construct the reflective latent variables of four EQ aspects. The calculation
was performed using WarpPLS 3.0. According to Kock (2012), the score of
each latent variable is calculated based on the indicators defined by the user
as associated with the latent variable. The calculation is based on a partial
least squares (PLS) algorithm. The factor scores of each latent variable were
then used to construct the second-order latent variable for overall EQ.
General Self-Efficacy
The measurement of general self-efficacy was adopted from Schwarzer and
Jerusalem (1995). This scale was tested in 25 countries and received sup-
port for it reliability across cultures (Scholz, Doña, Sud, & Schwarzer, 2002).
It contains 10 items in a 5-point Likert scales ranging from 1 (strongly dis-
agree)to5(strongly agree). Sample items of this measure include “I can
always manage to solve difficult problems if I try hard enough,” and “I am
confident that I could deal efficiently with unexpected events.”
General Perceived Stress
Perceived stress was measured using the Cohen and Williamson’s (1988)
Perceived Stress Scale. The scale is designed to measure the degree to which
situations in one’s life are appraised as stressful. Sample items of this measure
include “In the last month, how often have you been upset because of
something that happened unexpectedly?”, “In the last month, how often have
you felt you were unable to control the important things in your life?” It
contains 14 items in a 5-point Likert scales ranging from 1 (never)to5
(always). However, four items were loaded weakly to the latent variable;
therefore they were removed from the model.
Control Variables
Five demographic variables including age, gender, education, marital status,
and the number of children were used as control variables. Studies found that
Downloaded by [200.178.118.85] at 23:35 26 August 2015
Benefits of Mindfulness Meditation on Psychological Well-Being 181
these demographic factors tend to determine the level of outcome variables
used in this study (e,g., Gbadamosi and Ross (2012). Gender was measured
as a dummy variable where Female was coded 0 and Male was coded 1. Age
was measured in years. Education was measured ordinally. Marital status was
measured as a dummy variable where single was coded 0 and married was
coded 1. The number of children was measured in ordinal values.
Estimation Technique
The model was analyzed using partial least squares (PLS) performed in
WarpPLS 3.0 (Kock, 2012). PLS is a technique combining principal com-
ponent analysis, path analysis, and a set of regressions to generate estimates
of standardized regression coefficients for the model’s paths and factor load-
ings for the measurement items (Chin, 1998a). Like the covariance-based
SEM method, PLS allows multiple hypotheses to be tested simultaneously
while also enabling single-item and multi-item measurement and the use of
formative and reflective scales. It also permits the simultaneous assessment
of both measurement and structural models. However, PLS provides more
flexibility than covariance-based SEM techniques as it does not require the
data to be normally distributed (Kline, 2005). Another advantage of PLS is
that it allows for smaller sample sizes compared to other SEM techniques, as
scholars recommend that at least 30 to 100 observations are adequate for the
analysis (Chin, 1998b).
RESULTS
Model Validity and Reliability
Before estimating the model, a series of analyses were performed. Table 1
reports the correlations among variables in the model along with some valid-
ity and reliability indicators. First, the construct reliabilities were evaluated
using Cronbach’s alphas (α). The results showed that all values exceeded
the widely recommended minimum value of 0.7 (Fornell & Larcker, 1981).
Second, the convergent validity of each item was evaluated using factor load-
ings. The results indicated that the loadings were greater than 0.5, which is
satisfactory as recommended by Hair, Black, Babin, and Anderson (2009).
Third, the test for discriminant validity, which requires that a construct should
share more variance with its measures than it shares with other constructs in
a given model (Hulland, 1999), was also evaluated. This was performed by
comparing the average variance extracted (AVE) to the squared correlation
coefficient. As suggested by Fornell and Larcker (1981), the square root of
the AVE must be greater than correlations between the constructs in order for
discriminant validity to exist. The result indicated that the AVE for each con-
struct meets this requirement for all scales. Finally, the test for the possible
Downloaded by [200.178.118.85] at 23:35 26 August 2015
182 P. Charoensukmongkol
TABLE 1 Correlations Matrix
Variable α123456789
1. Mindfulness
meditation
.76 (.821)
2. Emotional
intelligence
.72 .323∗∗ (.739)
3. Perceived
self-efficacy
.92 .271∗∗ .682∗∗ (.758)
4. Perceived
stress
.83 .353∗∗ .442∗∗ .477∗∗ (.637)
5. Age .106 .110 .095 .201∗∗ (1)
6. Sex .126.080 .179∗∗ .062 .105 (1)
7. Education .104 .115.042 .014 .268∗∗ .195∗∗ (1)
8. Marital status .003 .063 .157∗∗ .065 .495∗∗ .092 .073 (1)
9. Number of
children
.067 .049 .114.064 .522∗∗ .032 .023 .685∗∗ (1)
Note. Average variance extracted of latent variables are shown in the parentheses.
p<.05. ∗∗p<.01.
presence of multicollinearity among the indicators was also performed using
the variance inflation factor (VIF) statistics. The VIFs ranged from 1.05 to
2.09, which is considerably below the critical value of 3.3 as suggested by
Petter, Straub, and Rai (2007).
Test of Hypotheses
Before testing the hypotheses, the comparison of means of key outcomes
variable between MM practitioners and nonpractitioners, regardless of how
long they had practiced, was performed using a t-test. The results are
reported in Table 2. Apparently, those who practiced MM had significantly
higher mean scores of EQ and general self-efficacy, and lower mean score of
general perceived stress, than those who did not practice at all. These results
provided some preliminary evidence about the benefits of MM practice.
Next, analysis results from the structural model after controlling for all
demographic variables are presented in Figure 1. The standardized coeffi-
cient and t-values were calculated using a bootstrap resampling procedure
with 100 subsamples (Efron, Rogosa, & Tibshirani, 2004).
TABLE 2 Comparison of Means
Emotional
intelligence
General
self-efficacy
General perceived
stress
MtMtM t
Meditators .251 6.166∗∗∗ .198 4.769∗∗∗ .235 5.734∗∗∗
Non-meditators .428 .339 .401
p<.1. ∗∗p<.05. ∗∗∗p<.01.
Downloaded by [200.178.118.85] at 23:35 26 August 2015
Benefits of Mindfulness Meditation on Psychological Well-Being 183
–.402***
.66***
.047
.332***
–.199***
R2 = 0.13
R2 = 0.30
R2 = 0.50
General
Self-Efficacy
General
Perceived Stress
Emotional
Intelligence
Mindfulness
Meditation
Control variables:
Age
Gender
Education
Marital status
Number of children
FIGURE 1 PLS Results
Note. Standardized coefficients are reported. Control variables pointing to each dependent
variable are not shown.
p<.1. ∗∗p<.05. ∗∗∗p<.01.
First, for the association between MM and EQ, the result showed the
positive relationship between the two variables, suggesting that practicing
MM can be linked to higher EQ. This relationship was also statistically
significant (β=.332; p<.01). Therefore, Hypothesis 1 was supported.
For the direct relationship between MM and general self-efficacy,
although the result indicated a positive association between the two vari-
ables, it was not statistically significant (β=.047; p=.149). Thus,
Hypothesis 2 was not supported. However, it appeared that the effect of MM
on general self-efficacy could be mediated through EQ, as the link between
EQ and general self-efficacy was statistically significant (β=.66; p<.01).
To test the mediating effect, the method proposed by Preacher and Hayes
(2004) was performed. The analysis revealed a strong indirect link between
MM and general self-efficacy, which was mediated by EQ. This indirect link
was statistically significant (β=.219; p<.01). Therefore, Hypothesis 3 was
supported.
For the direct relationship between MM and general perceived stress, the
result revealed the negative relationship between the variables, which was
statistically significant (β=−.199; p<.01). Thus, Hypothesis 4 was sup-
ported. Moreover, there was a negative association between EQ and general
perceived stress that was statistically significant (β=−.402; p<.01). The
result from of the test of the mediating effect also found that the effect of MM
Downloaded by [200.178.118.85] at 23:35 26 August 2015
184 P. Charoensukmongkol
on general perceived stress was also strongly mediated by EQ. This indirect
link was statistically significant (β=−.133; p<.01). Therefore, Hypothesis
5 was supported.
Next, the significant relationships between control variables and the
main dependent variables were reported as the following. First, age was
negatively associated with general perceived stress (β=−.311; p<.01).
For gender, males tended to report higher general self-efficacy (β=.125;
p<.01) than females. For education, the results only revealed its positive
relationship with general self-efficacy (β=.062; p=.08). For marital status,
married persons tended to report lower general self-efficacy (β=−.107; p=
.03) than single persons. Lastly, there was no significant relationship between
the number of children and any of the dependent variables.
For the model fit indicators, three indices are provided by WarpPLS:
average path coefficient (APC), average R-squared (ARS), and average vari-
ance inflation factor (AVIF). The p-values for APC and ARS are calculated
through a complex process that involves resampling estimations coupled
with Bonferroni-like corrections (Rosenthal & Rosnow, 1991). For the model
fit indices to be satisfactory, the p-values for the APC and ARS must be lower
than 5%, whereas the AVIF must be lower than 5. From the analysis, the APC
is equal to .13 the ARS is equal to .308, and the AVIF is equal to 1.475. Both
APC and ARS are statistically significant at .1%.
Finally, to check if the results from the analysis were affected by out-
liers, the author performed the analysis using ranked data and compared
the results with the original results. In particular, WarpPLS 3.0 allows users
to select an option to conduct the analyses with only ranked data, whereby
all the data are automatically ranked prior to the SEM analysis. When data
are ranked, typically the value distances that typify outliers are significantly
reduced, effectively eliminating outliers without any decrease in sample size
(Kock, 2012). The results show that all the path coefficients as well as the
p-values from the analysis using ranked data did not change from the original
findings, suggesting that outliers are not a major problem in the analysis.
DISCUSSION
General Findings
This study contributes to existing research related to benefits of MM. This
study, which focused on diverse groups of respondents from Thailand, tested
the direct and indirect effect of practicing MM on EQ, general self-efficacy,
and general perceived stress. In particular, evidence was found about the
direct effects of practicing MM on EQ and general perceived stress. On the
other hand, the indirect effects of MM were found, through EQ, for general
self-efficacy and also for general perceived stress.
Downloaded by [200.178.118.85] at 23:35 26 August 2015
Benefits of Mindfulness Meditation on Psychological Well-Being 185
First, the results indicated that regularly practice of MM tended to be
a good predictor of EQ and general perceived stress. In particular, those
who had regularly practiced MM tended to report higher EQ and lower
general perceived stress than those who did not. These results are consis-
tent with past research conducted in the Western countries on the benefits
of MM (Feldman et al., 2007). In addition to the direct effect, the present
study found that regularly practicing MM may allow people to experience
less stress perception indirectly through EQ. Because people who had reg-
ularly practiced MM were frequently mindful to their inner self more than
those who did not, they tended to be better in deciphering how and why
their emotion was triggered by a specific stimuli and how it should be man-
aged. This, in turn, allowed them to adjust their emotion more quickly or
to direct their attention to other appropriate activities that suit their currently
felt emotion more effectively whenever they experienced stress. These bene-
fits were therefore considered important factors that may be associated with
less stress perception. Furthermore, this study offers new evidence that MM
practice was also associated with higher general self-efficacy. However, the
results suggested that the linkage was only achieved indirectly through EQ.
This finding is consistent with the argument that emotions can exert a strong
influence on how people make causal reasoning about their capabilities to
achieve challenge tasks (Gundlach et al., 2003).
Practical Implications
Given all the findings, this research provides implications that can help
people improve their well-being. Considering the hectic lifestyle of the pop-
ulation in developing countries such as in Thailand (Floro & Pichetpongsa,
2010), it is inevitable that people will be exposed to various life- and work-
related stressors that are harmful to their health and psychological well-being
(Taylor, 2009). This study suggests that MM can be considered an interven-
tion that can significantly help people deal effectively with those stressors.
Importantly, practicing MM could be associated with the ability of people to
maintain peace of mind despite experiencing unfavorable situations in their
work and life. Moreover, the indirect benefit of practicing MM on general
self-efficacy suggested that MM intervention may also help people improve
their ability to perform challenge tasks, as the clarity of mind and the stability
of emotion can promote more optimistic thinking and enhance their belief
that they can effectively overcome any difficulty and obstacle.
The strong associations between MM practice and key psychology out-
comes that were found in this paper also offer important implication to
organizations. Because having good mental health can help people develop
inner strength to cope well with stressors, the author suggests that organi-
zations should consider MM training as an intervention to help employee
develop the capability to deal effectively with stress at work. Offering MM
Downloaded by [200.178.118.85] at 23:35 26 August 2015
186 P. Charoensukmongkol
training can also help employees improve their focus and alertness (Kabat-
Zinn, 2005), thereby allowing them to concentrate better on their tasks at
hand.
Limitations
There are several limitations in this study that need to be addressed. The
major limitation of this study is its cross-sectional research design. In partic-
ular, using a cross-sectional design make it difficult for researchers to infer
causality between constructs (Maxwell & Cole, 2007). Moreover, there is a
possibility for the bidirectional relationships between the variables in the
study. For example, it could be possible that less perceived stress could
mediate an indirect effect of MM on EQ. Using a cross-sectional study make
it difficult to check for the bidirectional causality in the model. Because of
these limitations, it is important for future research to use an experimental
design to test the causal association between MM practice and the outcome
variables over time. Future research that uses longitudinal design will pro-
vide more solid evidence whether MM practice can lead to improvement in
these outcome variables.
Second, the level of mindfulness of the respondents was not measured
in this study. However, based on the results from previous research that
found that the intensity of MM practice tended to be associated positively
with mindfulness measures (Baer et al., 2008), the present study assumed that
the measure of MM practice (the numbers of hours per day, days per week,
and years of practice) would associate positively with the level of mindful-
ness. However, it is possible that the measure of the MM practice may not
accurately capture the level of mindfulness that participants developed from
the practice. Thus, future research should also incorporate a mindfulness
measure in the studies.
Another limitation is the self-report measurements of the key variables,
which may not be the accurate measurement of the constructs. Some scholars
may claim that self-report measurements may have also inflated the relation-
ships among them because of common method variance (van Beek, Hu,
Schaufeli, Taris, & Schreurs, 2012). However, Spector (2006) argues that self-
reports may not necessarily inflate associations between variables and do not
necessarily lead to significant results. The second limitation of this study is
the sample selection, which only targeted the visitors of the religion websites.
This sample selection method can limit the ability to generalize the findings
to the larger population of the country. Therefore, future research focusing
on different groups of samples from other sources is needed. In addition,
because this study employed the data collected from a diverse group of
respondents, general self-efficacy can only be investigated. Future research
should target a specific group of respondents (e.g., by similar occupation
Downloaded by [200.178.118.85] at 23:35 26 August 2015
Benefits of Mindfulness Meditation on Psychological Well-Being 187
or the same workplace) to test whether practicing MM can associate with
specific self-efficacy measures.
CONCLUSION
In summary, this study offered some evidence that practicing MM can asso-
ciate strongly with higher EQ, higher general self-efficacy, and lower general
perceived stress. Having good mental health is crucial for people to live hap-
pily and to be more effective at work. Therefore, the author concludes that
MM could be an essential practice that people should learn so that they can
apply the technique to help them improve their psychological well-being.
FUNDING
This research received financial support from the International College of
National Institute of Development Administration.
REFERENCES
Astin, J. A. (1997). Stress reduction through mindfulness meditation: Effects on
psychological symptomatology, sense of control, and spiritual experiences.
Psychother Psychosom,66, 97–106. doi:10.1159/000289116
Averill, J. R., Chon, K. K., & Hahn, D. W. (2001). Emotions and creativity, East
and West. Asian Journal of Social Psychology,4(3), 165–183. doi:10.1111/1467-
839x.00084
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-
report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment,13(1),
27–45. doi:10.1177/1073191105283504
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Lykins, E., Button, D., Krietemeyer, J., Sauer, S., ...Williams,
J. M. G. (2008). Construct validity of the five facet mindfulness question-
naire in meditating and nonmeditating samples. Assessment,15(3), 329–342.
doi:10.1177/1073191107313003
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive
theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York, NY: Freeman.
Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspective. Asian Journal
of Social Psychology,2(1), 21–41. doi:10.1111/1467-839x.00024
Bedini, L. A., Gladwell, N. J., Dudley, W. N., & Clancy, E. J. (2011). Mediation analysis
of leisure, perceived stress, and quality of life in informal caregivers. Journal of
Leisure Research,43(2), 153–175.
Brady-Amoon, P., & Fuertes, J. N. (2011). Self-efficacy, self-rated abilities, adjust-
ment, and academic performance. Journal of Counseling & Development,89(4),
431–438. doi:10.1002/j.1556-6676.2011.tb02840.x
Downloaded by [200.178.118.85] at 23:35 26 August 2015
188 P. Charoensukmongkol
Brown, K. W., & Kasser, T. (2005). Are psychological and ecological well-being
compatible? The role of values, mindfulness, and lifestyle. Social Indicators
Research,74(2), 349–368. doi:10.1007/s11205-004-8207-8
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness
and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,84(4), 822–848. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822
Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., & Creswell, J. D. (2007). Mindfulness: Theoretical founda-
tions and evidence for its salutary effects. Psychological Inquiry,18(4), 211–237.
doi:10.1080/10478400701598298
Cahn, B. R., & Polich, J. (2006). Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and
neuroimaging studies. Psychological Bulletin,132(2), 180–211.
Charoensukmongkol, P. (2013). The contributions of mindfulness meditation on
burnout, coping strategy, and job satisfaction: Evidence from Thailand. Journal
of Management & Organization,19(5), 544–558. doi:10.1017/jmo.2014.8
Chin, W. W. (1998a). The partial least squares approach to structural equation mod-
eling. In G. A. Marcoulides (Ed.), Modern methods for business research (pp.
295–336). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Chin, W. W. (1998b). Issues and opinion on structural equation modeling. MIS
Quarterly,22(1), vii–xvi.
Christopher, M. S., & Gilbert, B. D. (2010). Incremental validity of components of
mindfulness in the prediction of satisfaction with life and depression. Current
Psychology,29(1), 10–23. doi:10.1007/s12144-009-9067-9
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived
stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior,24(4), 385–396.
Cohen, S., & Williamson, G. M. (1988). Perceived stress in a probability sample of
the United States. In S. Spacapan & S. Oskamp (Eds.), The social psychology of
health (pp. 31–61). London, England: Sage.
DeRue, D. S., & Morgeson, F. P. (2007). Stability and change in person–team and
person–role fit over time: The effects of growth satisfaction, performance, and
general self-efficacy. Journal of Applied Psychology,92(5), 1242–1253.
Efron, B., Rogosa, D., & Tibshirani, R. (2004). Resampling methods of estimation.
In N. J. Smelser & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), International encyclopedia of the social &
behavioral sciences (pp. 13216–13220). New York, NY: Elsevier.
Feldman, G., Hayes, A., Kumar, S., Greeson, J., & Laurenceau, J.-P. (2007).
Mindfulness and emotion regulation: The development and initial vali-
dation of the Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale-Revised (CAMS-
R). Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment,29(3), 177–190.
doi:10.1007/s10862-006-9035-8
Fitzgerald, S., & Schutte, N. S. (2010). Increasing transformational leadership through
enhancing self-efficacy. Journal of Management Development,29(5), 495–505.
Flavell, J. H. (1987). Speculations about the nature and development of
metacognition. In F. E. Weinert & R. Kluwe (Eds.), Metacognition, motivation,
and understanding (pp. 21–29). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Floro, M. S., & Pichetpongsa, A. (2010). Gender, work intensity, and well-being
of Thai home-based workers. Feminist Economics,16 (3), 5–44. doi:10.1080/
13545701.2010.499657
Downloaded by [200.178.118.85] at 23:35 26 August 2015
Benefits of Mindfulness Meditation on Psychological Well-Being 189
Fornell, C., & Larcker, D. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models with unob-
servable variables and measurement error. Journal of Marketing Research,
18(1), 39–50.
Gbadamosi, G., & Ross, C. (2012). Perceived stress and performance appraisal dis-
comfort: The moderating effects of core self-evaluations and gender. Public
Personnel Management,41(4), 637–659.
George, J. M. (2000). Emotions and leadership: The role of emotional intelligence.
Human Relations,53(8), 1027–1055. doi:10.1177/0018726700538001
Ghorbani, N., Krauss, S. W., Watson, P. J., & LeBreton, D. (2008). Relationship of
perceived stress with depression: Complete mediation by perceived control and
anxiety in Iran and the United States. International Journal of Psychology,43(6),
958–968. doi:10.1080/00207590701295264
Goenka, S. N. (2002). Meditation now: Inner peace through inner wisdom. Seattle,
WA: Vipassana Research Publications.
Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence. New York, NY: Bantam Books.
Gundlach, M. J., Martinko, M. J., & Douglas, S. C. (2003). Emotional intelligence,
causal reasoning, and the self-efficacy development process. International
Journal of Organizational Analysis (2003),11(3), 229–246.
Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., & Anderson, R. E. (2009). Multivariate data
analysis (7th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Herabadi, A. G., Verplanken, B., & Van Knippenberg, A. (2009). Consumption expe-
rience of impulse buying in Indonesia: Emotional arousal and hedonistic consid-
erations. Asian Journal of Social Psychology,12(1), 20–31. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
839X.2008.01266.x
Hulland, J. (1999). Use of partial least squares (PLS) in strategic management
research. Strategic Management Journal,20(2), 195–204.
Jha, A., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. (2007). Mindfulness training modifies sub-
systems of attention. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience,7(2),
109–119. doi:10.3758/cabn.7.2.109
Jimmieson, N. L. (2000). Employee reactions to behavioural control under conditions
of stress: The moderating role of self-efficacy. Work & Stress,14(3), 262–280.
doi:10.1080/02678370010015343
Joseph, D. L., & Newman, D. A. (2010). Emotional intelligence: An integrative meta-
analysis and cascading model. Journal of Applied Psychology,95(1), 54–78.
Judge, T. A., Erez, A., & Bono, J. E. (1998). The power of being positive: The rela-
tion between positive self-concept and job performance. Human Performance,
11(2/3), 167–187. doi:10.1080/08959285.1998.9668030
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your body and
mind to face stress, pain, and illness. New York, NY: Delacorte Press.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Mindfulness meditation for everyday life.NewYork,NY:
Hyperion.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2005). Wherever you go, there you are: Mindfulness meditation in
everyday life. New York, NY: Hyperion.
Kavanagh, D. J., & Bower, G. H. (1985). Mood and self-efficacy: Impact of joy
and sadness on perceived capabilities. Cognitive Therapy and Research,9(5),
507–525. doi:10.1007/bf01173005
Kline, R. B. (2005). Principle and practice of structural equation modeling (2 ed.).
New York, NY: The Guiford Press.
Downloaded by [200.178.118.85] at 23:35 26 August 2015
190 P. Charoensukmongkol
Kock, N. (2012). WarpPLS 3.0 user manual. Laredo, TX: ScriptWarp Systems.
Krasner, M. S., Epstein, R. M., Beckman, H., Suchman, A. L., Chapman, B., Mooney,
C. J., & Quill, T. E. (2009). Association of an educational program in mind-
ful communication with burnout, empathy, and attitudes among primary care
physicians. JAMA: The Journal of the American Medical Association,302(12),
1284–1293. doi:10.1001/jama.2009.1384
Lang, J. C., & Lee, C. H. (2005). Identity accumulation, others’ acceptance, job-search
self-efficacy, and stress. Journal of Organizational Behavior,26(3), 293–312.
doi:10.1002/job.309
Law, K. S., Wong, C.-S., & Song, L. J. (2004). The construct and criterion validity of
emotional intelligence and its potential utility for management studies. Journal
of Applied Psychology,89(3), 483–496. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.89.3.483
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping.NewYork,NY:
Springer.
Lourel, M., Ford, M. T., Gamassou, C. E., Guéguen, N., & Hartmann, A. (2009).
Negative and positive spillover between work and home: Relationship to per-
ceived stress and job satisfaction. Journal of Managerial Psychology,24(5),
438–449.
Luria, G., & Torjman, A. (2009). Resources and coping with stressful events. Journal
of Organizational Behavior,30(6), 685–707. doi:10.1002/job.551
Matthews, G., Emo, A. K., Funke, G., Zeidner, M., Roberts, R. D., Costa, Jr., P. T., &
Schulze, R. (2006). emotional intelligence, personality, and task-induced stress.
Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied,12(2), 96–107. doi:10.1037/1076-
898x.12.2.96
Maxwell, S. E., & Cole, D. A. (2007). Bias in cross-sectional analyses of
longitudinal mediation. Psychological Methods,12(1), 23–44. doi:10.1080/
00273171.2011.606716
Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence? In P. Salovey & D.
Sluyter (Eds.), Emotional development and emotional intelligence: Implications
for educators (pp. 3–34). New York, NY: Basic Books.
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2000). Models of emotional intelligence.
In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp. 392–420). Cambridge,
England: Cambridge University Press.
Nasurdin, A. M., Ramayah, T., & Chee, B. Y. (2009). The impacts of structure, climate
and self-efficacy on stress: A Malaysian survey. Asian Academy of Management
Journal,14(1), 59–79.
Nikolaou, I., & Tsaousis, I. (2002). Emotional intelligence in the workplace: Exploring
its effects on occupational stress and organizational commitment. International
Journal of Organizational Analysis,10(4), 327–342.
Petter, S., Straub, D., & Rai, A. (2007). Specifying formative constructs in information
systems research. MIS Quarterly,31(4), 623–656.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect
effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, &
Computers,36(4), 717–731.
Rathi, N., & Rastogi, R. (2008). Effect of emotional intelligence on occupational
self-efficacy. ICFAI Journal of Organizational Behavior ,7(2), 46–56.
Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. L. (1991). Essentials of behavioral research: Methods and
data analysis. Boston, MA: McGraw Hill.
Downloaded by [200.178.118.85] at 23:35 26 August 2015
Benefits of Mindfulness Meditation on Psychological Well-Being 191
Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. M. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition
and Personality,9(3), 185–211.
Salovey, P., Stroud, L. R., Woolery, A., & Epel, E. S. (2002). Perceived emo-
tional intelligence, stress reactivity, and symptom reports: Further explo-
rations using the trait meta-mood scale. Psychology & Health,17(5), 611–627.
doi:10.1080/08870440290025812
Scholz, U., Doña, B. G., Sud, S., & Schwarzer, R. (2002). Is general self-efficacy a
universal construct? Psychometric findings from 25 countries. European Journal
of Psychological Assessment,18(3), 242–251. doi:10.1027//1015-5759.18.3.242
Schreurs, B., van Emmerik, H., Notelaers, G., & De Witte, H. (2010). Job insecurity
and employee health: The buffering potential of job control and job self-efficacy.
Work & Stress,24(1), 56–72. doi:10.1080/02678371003718733
Schwarzer, R., & Hallum, S. (2008). Perceived teacher self-efficacy as a predic-
tor of job stress and burnout: Mediation analyses. Applied Psychology: An
International Review,57 , 152–171. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.2008.00359.x
Schwarzer, R., & Jerusalem, M. (1995). Generalized Self-Efficacy Scale. In J.
Weinman, S. Wright, & M. Johnsston (Eds.), Measures in health psychology:
A user’s portfolio. causal and control beliefs (pp. 35–37). Windsor, England:
NFER-NELSON.
Shapiro, S. L., Schwartz, G. E., & Bonner, G. (1998). Effects of mindfulness-based
stress reduction on medical and premedical students. Journal of Behavioral
Medicine,21(6), 581–599. doi:10.1023/a:1018700829825
Spector, P. E. (2006). Method variance in organizational research: Truth or urban
legend? Organizational Research Methods,9(2), 221–232.
Storbeck, J., & Clore, G. L. (2007). On the interdependence of cognition and emotion.
Cognition & Emotion,21(6), 1212–1237. doi:10.1080/02699930701438020
Taylor, M. P. (2009). The applied economics of health: Introduction and overview.
Applied Economics,41(4), 413–415. doi:10.1080/00036840902757241
Tsai, W.-C., Chen, C.-C., & Liu, H.-L. (2007). Test of a model linking employee
positive moods and task performance. Journal of Applied Psychology,92(6),
1570–1583.
van Beek, I., Hu, Q., Schaufeli, W. B., Taris, T. W., & Schreurs, B. H. J. (2012). For fun,
love, or money: What drives workaholic, engaged, and burned-out employees
at work? Applied Psychology,61(1), 30–55. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.2011.00454.x
van Knippenberg, D., Kooij-de Bode, H. J. M., & van Ginkel, W. P. (2010). The
interactive effects of mood and trait negative affect in group decision making.
Organization Science,21(3), 731–744. doi:10.1287/orsc.1090.0461
Wallace, B. A. (2006). The attention revolution: Unlocking the power of the focused
mind.Boston, MA: Wisdom Publications.
Wang, A. Y., & Richarde, R. S. (1988). Global versus task-specific measures of self-
efficacy. The Psychological Record,38(4), 533–541.
Wong, C.-S., & Law, K. S. (2002). The effects of leader and follower emotional
intelligence on performance and attitude: An exploratory study. The Leadership
Quarterly,13(3), 243–274. doi:10.1016/s1048-9843(02)00099-1
Downloaded by [200.178.118.85] at 23:35 26 August 2015
192 P. Charoensukmongkol
Yang, B., Kim, Y., & McFarland, R. G. (2011). Individual differences and sales
performance: A distal-proximal mediation model of self-efficacy, conscientious-
ness, and extraversion. Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management,31(4),
371–382.
Zajonc, R. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American
Psychological Association,35(2), 151–175.
Zeidan, F., Johnson, S. K., Diamond, B. J., David, Z., & Goolkasian, P. (2010).
Mindfulness meditation improves cognition: Evidence of brief mental training.
Consciousness and Cognition,19(2), 597–605. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2010.03.014
Downloaded by [200.178.118.85] at 23:35 26 August 2015
... 10,[13][14][15][16][17] Mindfulness practices were found to help manage negative emotions experienced by children and adolescents. [18][19][20][21] Regular practice of mindfulness enhances the ability to understand one's emotions. 21 MBI is also a potential way to address attentional problems in adolescents and to improve awareness of daily routine activities. ...
... [18][19][20][21] Regular practice of mindfulness enhances the ability to understand one's emotions. 21 MBI is also a potential way to address attentional problems in adolescents and to improve awareness of daily routine activities. ...
... Contrary to our initial hypothesis and some previous findings (Charoensukmongkol, 2014), a negative correlation was observed between emotional intelligence and acceptance action. This suggests that individuals with higher emotional intelligence may be less inclined to engage in acceptance-based coping behaviours. ...
... Additionally, our study builds upon prior research by incorporating insights from various scholars. Carmeli et al. (2009) andCharoensukmongkol (2014) highlighted the positive association between emotional intelligence and wellbeing indicators, while Kong and Zhao (2013) emphasized its significance in predicting overall satisfaction among young adults. Singh and Sharma (2012) revealed a positive correlation between EI and general wellbeing, suggesting lower stress levels and better stress management among individuals with higher EI. ...
Research
Full-text available
Emotional intelligence helps navigate challenges effectively, while general wellbeing reflects overall life satisfaction. By cultivating emotional intelligence, individuals can better navigate their internal experiences and interpersonal interactions, contributing to improved general wellbeing. Further, Acceptance and action encourages individuals to accept their internal experiences and take committed action aligned with their values. This study investigates the intricate interplay between emotional intelligence, general wellbeing, and acceptance action among young adults aged 18 to 35 years. Results reveal positive correlations between emotional intelligence and general wellbeing (r= 0.63, p<.01), there is slightly negative correlation between general wellbeing and acceptance action (r=-0.068, p<.01). However, there is a negative correlation surfaced between emotional intelligence and acceptance action (r=-0.61, p<.01). These findings highlight the necessity of comprehending these dynamics to foster mental health and resilience in young adults. Future research should delve into the underlying mechanisms driving these relationships and develop targeted interventions to promote psychological wellbeing and adaptive coping strategies in this population. Emotional Intelligence Emotional Intelligence (EI) refers to a set of abilities that allow individuals to understand, assess, and regulate emotions in themselves and others. It involves being aware of one's emotions, empathizing with others, managing emotions effectively, and using emotional information to navigate social situations and achieve personal and professional goals.
... Counselors are also exposed to their clients' traumatic experiences and intense feelings (Li et al., 2023;Newton et al., 2020). Research indicates that elevated levels of selfefficacy can protect against burnout (Bardhoshi & Um, 2021;Charoensukmongkol, 2014;Ender et al., 2019). Mindfulness and clinical supervision are also protective factors against burnout (Latorre et al., 2023;Li et al., 2023;Lohani & Sharma, 2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
Play therapists experience various ethical and legal challenges when working with children and families. However, research has not previously assessed play therapists’ confidence in navigating ethical and legal challenges. In this study, we examined general self-efficacy using the General Self-Efficacy Scale and ethical and legal self-efficacy using the Ethical and Legal Issues in Counseling Self-Efficacy Scale. We conducted statistical analyses that examined relationships among demographic factors, general self-efficacy, and ethical and legal self-efficacy. We obtained usable responses from 144 participants who were members of the Association for Play Therapy. The results of this study identified age and years of experience as the most related factors for both general and ethical and legal self-efficacy. Correlational analysis identified suicide, violence, abuse, and neglect self-efficacy as the area participants were most confident in. In contrast, counselor development and wellness was the area in which participants had the lowest levels of ethical and legal self-efficacy. Implications for play therapists, play therapy educators, supervisors, and future research opportunities are explored.
... It is defined as a constellation of emotional self-perceptions located at the lower levels of personality hierarchies (Petrides et al., 2007). Some studies have revealed that individuals with high trait emotional intelligence exhibit greater non-judgmental attention to the present moment (Charoensukmongkol, 2014;Wang & Kong, 2014) and engage in less rumination compared to those with low trait emotional intelligence (Ramos et al., 2007;Salguero et al., 2013). A study conducted by Zanella et al. (2022) investigating the connection between tEI and several Emotion Regulation strategies revealed a negative relationship between tEI and certain maladaptive emotion regulation strategies, specifically suppression and self-blame. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives Acceptance is an adaptive emotion regulation strategy characterized by an open and non-judgmental attitude toward mental and sensory experiences. While a few studies have investigated the neural correlates of acceptance in task-based fMRI studies, a gap remains in the scientific literature regarding dispositional use of acceptance, and how this is reflected at a structural level. Therefore, the aim of the present study was to investigate the neural and psychological differences between infrequent acceptance users (i.e., low accepters) and frequent users (i.e., high accepters). Another question was whether high and low accepters differ in personality traits and emotional intelligence. Method For the first time, we applied a data fusion unsupervised machine learning approach (mCCA-jICA) to the gray matter (GM) and white matter (WM) of high accepters (n = 50), and low accepters (n = 78) to possibly detect joint GM-WM differences in both modalities. Results Our results show that two covarying GM-WM networks separate high from low accepters. The first network showed decreased GM-WM concentration in a fronto-temporal-parietal circuit largely overlapping with the default mode network in high accepters compared to low accepters. The second network showed increased GM-WM concentration in portions of the orbito-frontal, temporal, and parietal areas, which may correspond to a central executive network, also in high accepters compared to low accepters. At the psychological level, the high accepters displayed higher openness to experience compared to low accepters. Conclusions Overall, our findings suggest that high accepters compared to low accepters differ in neural and psychological mechanisms. These findings confirm and extend previous studies on the relevance of acceptance as a strategy associated with well-being.
... These practices can effectively be used to inculcate the habit of cyber mindfulness to deal with the daily life routine which is surrounded by compulsive internet usage. Meditation positively impacts emotional intelligence (Charoensukmongkol, 2014). Emotional intelligence is found to positively affect mindfulness and mindfulness positively affects cyber security (Tolga et al., 2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
Technology has changed people's lifestyles and it has also drawn people's attention into the virtual world. People are becoming marionettes of the internet and technology in numerous facets of their lives, from social contact to professional work, playing games online to purchasing goods online, etc. People are starting to debilitate sight of the reality that technology is taking over their minds, rendering them incapable of making wise decisions. The control of the human mind is one of the finest ways to optimise production in the current environment of fast technological advancement. Cyber mindfulness plays a pivotal role in alleviating unhealthy practices, creating awareness, and making people conscious of their thoughts, actions, feelings and emotions in cyberspace. Cyber mindfulness could potentially be attained through resurrecting knowledge of yoga, meditation, and other practices used in ancient India. This article envisages the importance of cyber mindfulness in the era of technology and internet and comprises the knowledge of ancient practices in achieving cyber mindfulness. The article also entails the multitudinous benefits of cyber mindfulness in addition to the methods of enhancing cyber mindfulness.
... Actually, gratitude could work in a similar way to emotional intelligence. Decades of study have revealed the role of emotional intelligence effects over various relevant variables including general health, socio-emotional outcomes, and life satisfaction (Charoensukmongkol, 2014;Jacobs et al., 2016). Despite the extensive research on gratitude and emotional intelligence separately, scarce studies (Geng, 2018;Chang et al., 2022) investigated the relationship of these different constructs with subjective well-being (SWB). ...
Article
Full-text available
Gratitude has proved to be an enhancer of subjective well-being in previous studies. However, studies that linked the relation between emotional intelligence and its facets when interacting with gratitude, are still limited. In this sense, this study examined the mediating roles of emotional intelligence between gratitude and subjective well-being indicators, by introducing the general factor of emotional intelligence. The first approach to data analysis was to examine the descriptive statistics; the second approach consisted of an Exploratory Structural Equation Modelling, applying also a bifactor analysis. Data was collected from 406 Spanish students, through an online survey that includes the gratitude, trait meta mood scale, satisfaction with life and subjective happiness scale. The mean age of participants was 20.27 years (SD = 4.68), whereas 79.5% were females. The results provided preliminary evidence of the mediation role of the general factor of emotional intelligence between gratitude and subjective well-being, which provided a meaningful insight about the role of trait emotional intelligence. These findings suggested that gratitude promotes emotional intelligence, allowing to an increase in subjective well-being. Nonetheless, there is a need for further research to achieve a better understanding of the role of the emotional intelligence facets between gratitude and subjective well-being.
Article
The addition of an empowerment perspective to treatment of female trauma survivors with co-occurring mental health and substance use concerns has been found to be beneficial. Meditation is a mental health intervention that has the potential for increasing feelings of empowerment amongst this population. This qualitative study explores the experiences of female trauma survivors with co-occurring disorders engaging in a meditation intervention and its impact on their feelings of empowerment. Grounded theory approaches were used to analyse in-depth interviews with participants (N = 32). Results from analysis of the data show the process through which meditation allowed the women to gain empowerment in order to develop skills to heal on their own. This study provides evidence of the use of meditation as a viable strength-based intervention for social workers in order to increase feelings of empowerment amongst female trauma survivors of interpersonal violence with co-occurring disorders. Future research should expand understanding of this empowerment pathway by exploring the impact of meditation within other populations with high levels of trauma.
Article
Full-text available
Provides a nontechnical introduction to the partial least squares (PLS) approach. As a logical base for comparison, the PLS approach for structural path estimation is contrasted to the covariance-based approach. In so doing, a set of considerations are then provided with the goal of helping the reader understand the conditions under which it might be reasonable or even more appropriate to employ this technique. This chapter builds up from various simple 2 latent variable models to a more complex one. The formal PLS model is provided along with a discussion of the properties of its estimates. An empirical example is provided as a basis for highlighting the various analytic considerations when using PLS and the set of tests that one can employ is assessing the validity of a PLS-based model. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
The study examines the relationships between perceived stress and performance appraisal discomfort with core self-evaluations and gender as moderating variables. It also asks the question to what extent do these variables predict perceived stress. The study is a cross-sectional survey that used structured questionnaires to obtain data from a sample of 300 managers in Gaborone, Botswana. Usable data was obtained from 167 managers. A negative and insignificant association was found between performance appraisal discomfort and perceived stress. Neither core self-evaluations nor gender significantly moderated the relationship between performance appraisal discomfort and perceived stress. Women earned less income and their perceived stress was significantly higher than men's. Also gender, core selfevaluations and then performance appraisal discomfort (in that order of strength) were significant predictors of perceived stress accounting for 12 percent of its variance. The findings also suggest that HR practitioners need to consider individual characteristics more than organizational policy in effective implementation of performance appraisals and stress reduction strategies and as a consequence worker-related interventions may be particularly pertinent. Finally, it presents data for a relatively under-explored area of behavioral research focusing on appraisers' (as opposed to appraisee) discomfort and core self-evaluations as a moderator variable of perceived stress.
Article
Due to the prediction of a significant increase in older adults as well as an expected decrease in availability of professional homecare, the number of informal caregivers in the United States is expected grow exponentially by the year 2050. Unfortunately, caregivers will likely continue to experience negative health consequences due to caregiver burden. In this study, mediation analysis was used to investigate the relationship among three leisure variables, perceived stress, and quality of life of informal family caregivers. Results showed that the effect of two of the leisure variables mediated quality of life through perceived stress. Recommendations address discussion of alternative recreation programming perspectives for informal caregivers as well as further research directions to expand these findings.
Article
The statistical tests used in the analysis of structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error are examined. A drawback of the commonly applied chi square test, in addition to the known problems related to sample size and power, is that it may indicate an increasing correspondence between the hypothesized model and the observed data as both the measurement properties and the relationship between constructs decline. Further, and contrary to common assertion, the risk of making a Type II error can be substantial even when the sample size is large. Moreover, the present testing methods are unable to assess a model's explanatory power. To overcome these problems, the authors develop and apply a testing system based on measures of shared variance within the structural model, measurement model, and overall model.
Chapter
In this article, a brief description of the bootstrap method for data analysis given. The use of the bootstrap for estimating standard errors and confidence intervals is reviewed and illustrated with an application to growth curves for educational testing data.Most scientists occasionally or frequently face problems of data analysis: what data should I collect? Having collected my data, what does it say? Having seen what it says, how far can I trust the conclusions? Statistics is the mathematical science that deals with such questions. Some statistical methods have become so familiar in the scientific literature, especially linear regression, hypothesis testing, standard errors, and confidence intervals, that they seem to date back to biblical times. In fact most of the 'classical' methods were developed between 1920 and 1950, by scientists like R. A. Fisher, Jerzy Neyman and Harold Hotelling who were senior colleagues to statisticians still active today.The 1980s produced a rising curve of new statistical theory and methods, based on the power of electronic computation. Today's data analyst can afford to expend more computation on a single problem than the world's yearly total of statistical computation in the 1920s. How can such computational wealth be spent wisely, in a way that genuinely adds to the classical methodology without merely elaborating it? Answering this question has become a dominant theme of modern statistical theory.
Article
Two studies were conducted in order to reconcile contextualist accounts of self-efficacy with recent evidence supporting its global nature. A correlational analysis (Study 1) indicated that Tipton and Worthington’s (1984) Generalized Self-Efficacy (GSE) Scale was inversely related to Rotter’s Internal-External Locus of Control Scale and Beck’s Hopelessness Scale. In contrast, no discernible pattern of intercorrelations was obtained across six task-specific tests of self-efficacy. A single bipolar factor emerged, however, when the GSE Scale was factor analyzed with task-specific ratings of self-efficacy. Study 2 demonstrated that task-specific, but not GSE scores, were sensitive to actual performance on cognitive tasks. These results were interpreted as evidence for the hypothesis that global and task-specific measures assess relatively distinct aspects of the construct of self-efficacy.