ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

The current study aims to analyze the impact of online and conventional couple quality on the explanation of dating aggression in Spain and Italy. 312 Italian and 430 Spanish university students participated in the study. Logistic regression analysis showed that conflicts increased the likelihood to be involved in psychological and physical aggression in both countries. Transgressive behavior increased the odds of being involved in physical and psychological aggression in Spain and in psychological aggression in Italy. Online intrusiveness influenced Spanish participants' involvement in physical and psychological dating aggression while online jealousy was the main predictor of both types of aggression in Italy. Results are discussed in terms of the insecurity that seems to characterize dating aggression in young adulthood.
47
Maltrattamento e abuso all’infanzia, Vol. 16, n. 3, novembre 2014, pp. 47-65
Online Intrusiveness, online jealousy and dating
aggression in young adults: a cross-national study
(Spain-Italy)
Virginia Sánchez, Noelia Muñoz*, Annalaura Nocentini,
Rosario Ortega-Ruiz, & Ersilia Menesini**
The current study aims to analyze the impact of online and conventional couple quality
on the explanation of dating aggression in Spain and Italy. 312 Italian and 430 Spanish
university students participated in the study. Logistic regression analysis showed that
conflicts increased the likelihood to be involved in psychological and physical aggres-
sion in both countries. Transgressive behavior increased the odds of being involved in
physical and psychological aggression in Spain and in psychological aggression in Ita-
ly. Online intrusiveness influenced Spanish participants’ involvement in physical and
psychological dating aggression while online jealousy was the main predictor of both
types of aggression in Italy. Results are discussed in terms of the insecurity that seems
to characterize dating aggression in young adulthood.
Key words: dating aggression, conflicts, online intrusiveness, online jealousy, young
adults.
Intrusività, gelosia e dating aggression online nei giovani adulti: uno studio cross-
culturale Spagna-Italia. Lo studio intende analizzare l’impatto della qualità della rela-
zione tra partner online e offline nella spiegazione del dating aggression in Spagna e in
Italia. 312 studenti universitari italiani e 430 spagnoli hanno partecipato allo studio.
Regressioni logistiche hanno mostrato come il conflitto aumenti la probabilità di essere
coinvolto nel dating aggression fisico e psicologico in entrambi i Paesi. Il comporta-
mento trasgressivo incrementa la probabilità di essere coinvolto nel dating aggression
fisico e psicologico in Spagna ma solo in quello psicologico in Italia. Infine
l’intrusività online influenza il dating aggression fisico e psicologico in Spagna mentre
in Italia è la gelosia online a predire entrambe le forme. I risultati enfatizzano il ruolo
dell’insicurezza nel predire la qualità delle relazioni sentimentali nella prima adultità.
Parole chiave: dating aggression, conflitti, instrusività online, gelosia online, giovane
adultità.
Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology, University of Seville,
Spain.
Granted by the Spanish Ministry of Education (FPU program-2013).
 Department of Psychology, University of Córdoba, Spain.
 Department of Psychology, University of Florence, Italy.
Indirizzare le richieste a: Virginia Sánchez, (virsan@us.es).
This research was financed by the Spanish Ministry of Education under the National
Program of Research and Innovation (I+D 2008-2011) Violencia y Cortejo Juvenil: los
riesgos del cortejo violento, la aggression sexual y el cyberbullying (PSI 2010 – 17246),
and under the National Program of Research and Innovation (I+D+i 2013-2016)
“Dat_Adolescence” (PSI2013-45118- R).
Copyright © FrancoAngeli
N.B: Copia ad uso personale. È vietata la riproduzione (totale o parziale) dell’opera con qualsiasi
mezzo effettuata e la sua messa a disposizione di terzi, sia in forma gratuita sia a pagamento.
Sánchez, Muñoz, Nocentini, Ortega-Ruiz, & Menesini
Maltrattamento e abuso all’infanzia, Vol. 16, n. 3, novembre 2014, pp. 47-65
48
1. Introduction
In Western cultures, the years of the transition to adulthood are char-
acterized by the progressive attainment of commitments related to life
plans: education, job and romantic relationships (Arnett, 2001; Shul-
man, Feldman, Blatt, Cohen, & Mahler, 2005). With regard to romantic
relationships, Shulman and Connolly (2013) have recently described
this pain of life as a transitional romantic stage defined by the task of
coordinating dyadic commitment together with individual life plans. By
negotiating the interdependence among the couple, young adults will
achieve long-terms romantic relationships. Studies conducted with ado-
lescent and young adults revealed that 90% of 18-25 year-old have had
a sentimental experience, but the diversity of these is very wide, from
those characterized by a relational instability (Garcia & Reiber, 2008) to
married couples. This diversity of romantic experiences implies that the
task is not easy, with cultural, social, family and personal factors affect-
ing this process. To this respect, the relational context of the dyad has
been considered a central factor to explain the positive or negative out-
comes of the romantic relationship (Capaldi & Kim, 2007), being more
important than the involvement in different romantic experiences (Col-
lins, Welsh, & Furman, 2009; Grover & Nangle, 2007). Studies have
showed that couples with high levels of positive quality (relationship
skills, commitment or intimacy) are involved in more stable, satisfied
and long lasting romantic relationships, while those involved in roman-
tic relationships characterized by conflicts, jealousy and intrusive be-
haviors show less satisfaction (Seiffge-Krenke & Connolly, 2010).
Moreover, longitudinal studies have described how the quality of ro-
mantic relationships in adolescence predicts the satisfaction and the
quality of romantic relationships in young adults (Madsen & Collins,
2011).
In short, the ability to negotiate and the acquisition of interpersonal
skills will promote good relationship quality and the attainment of inter-
dependence with the romantic partner. On the contrary, the presence of
conflicts, controlling behaviors and negative strategies for conflict reso-
lution should reflect poor outcomes, with the presence of aggressive be-
haviors in adolescents’ and young adults’ couples (Butzer & Kuiper,
2008; Connolly, Nocentini, Menesini, Pepler, Craig, & Williams, 2010;
Davila, Steinberg, Kachadourian, Cobb, & Fincham, 2004; La Greca &
Harrison, 2005; Marcus, 2012). To this respect, Nocentini, Pastorelli &
Copyright © FrancoAngeli
N.B: Copia ad uso personale. È vietata la riproduzione (totale o parziale) dell’opera con qualsiasi
mezzo effettuata e la sua messa a disposizione di terzi, sia in forma gratuita sia a pagamento.
Cyberdating quality and dating aggression
Maltrattamento e abuso all’infanzia, Vol. 16, n. 3, novembre 2014, p. 47-65
49
Menesini (2013) confirmed that poor levels of self-efficacy regarding
anger management predict high levels of conflicts within the couple,
and the occurrence of psychological and physical dating aggression in
both partners.
The presence of imbalance of power, controlling, and intrusive be-
haviors in the romantic relationships has also been associated with da-
ting aggression. Messinger and colleagues found that the use of escalat-
ing strategies (a combination of controlling and coercive strategies) was
related to dating aggression (perpetrated and received) in adolescents
and young females (Messinger, Rickert, Fry, Lessel, & Davidson,
2011), whilst Menesini and colleagues (Menesini, Nocentini, Ortega-
Rivera, Sánchez, & Ortega-Ruiz, 2011) concluded that the presence of
imbalance of power and conflicts were more likely in couples character-
ized by reciprocal aggression in comparison to couples not involved in
aggression. These results emphasize that behaviors and attitudes of con-
trol, intrusiveness and dominance over the romantic partner reinforce
similar responses from the other member of the dyad, contributing to an
escalation of conflict and aggression (Capaldi & Kim, 2007; Foran &
O’Leary, 2008).
To date, the impact of poor relationship quality on dating aggression
has not included the analysis of the interplay of the online context. Pre-
vious research has demonstrated that online and offline romantic rela-
tionships are specific but closely related (Caughlin & Sharabi, 2013), so
it can be expected that behaviors that people act in offline romantic rela-
tionships will be related to behaviors that they express online. Accord-
ing to this hypothesis, offline couple quality should be related with
online romantic relationship quality, and the influence of both contexts
would impact on dating aggression. Indeed, studies interested in the re-
lation between both measures of couple quality have showed that they
are linked. Ishii (2010) found that conflict management styles in online
relationships were related to the closeness and expectations for the fu-
ture of the relationship, with adults with no intentions for the future us-
ing more dominating, obliging, and avoidant strategies during argu-
ments with the partner.
One of the topics related to online behaviors among young and adult
couples that have received more attention in the last years has been cy-
berstalking (Menard & Pincus, 2012; Sheridan & Grant, 2007;
Strawhum, Adams, & Huss, 2013). These studies have been focused on
cyberstalking behaviors perpetrated and received by ex-partners, alt-
Copyright © FrancoAngeli
N.B: Copia ad uso personale. È vietata la riproduzione (totale o parziale) dell’opera con qualsiasi
mezzo effettuata e la sua messa a disposizione di terzi, sia in forma gratuita sia a pagamento.
Sánchez, Muñoz, Nocentini, Ortega-Ruiz, & Menesini
Maltrattamento e abuso all’infanzia, Vol. 16, n. 3, novembre 2014, pp. 47-65
50
hough Spitzberg and Cupach (2007) point out that these severe forms of
cyberstalking are not very frequent. The authors differentiate among se-
vere forms of cyberstalking tactics and obsessive relational intrusion, a
way of common or moderate cyberstalking. These tactics are considered
the most frequent forms of stalking that usually occur within romantic
relationships, and not necessarily causing fear to the victim. Obsessive
relational intrusion has been related directly with online jealousy and
control, as a “distorted version of courtship and romantic relationships”
(Spitzber & Cupach, p. 79), mainly oriented to maintain the intimacy
and proximity with the partner after a discussion, a breakup, or looking
for reconciliation. When it occurs, in the form of persistent and unwant-
ed email messages, SMS, or instant messages, the response of the other
partner has an important role in the perpetuation of the behavior, con-
tributing to maintain this specific communication strategy for conflict
resolution between partners (Nguyen, Spitzberg, & Lee, 2012). On the
same line, Elphinston and Noller (2011) showed that intrusive behavior
over the partner in social networks was linked to relationship dissatis-
faction and jealousy, reflecting a relational romantic dynamic laden with
insecurities and fears (Lavy, Mikulincer, & Shaver, 2010). In relation to
offline dating aggression, studies have considered the relation among
cyberstalking behaviors received or perpetrated and physical or psycho-
logical dating aggression in young adult couples, and among online
jealousy and psychological dating aggression, especially in females
(Strawhun, Adams, & Huss, 2013). No studies have analyzed the rela-
tion among intrusiveness and dating aggression in current relationships.
Starting from this literature, the present study will analyze the impact
of online quality couple (in terms of intrusiveness and online jealousy)
and conventional quality couple (in terms of conflicts, transgressive be-
havior, and imbalance of power) on physical and psychological dating
aggression in two countries, Spain and Italy. Although the studies on
dating aggression have been extended to Italy and Spain in the last years
(Menesini & Nocentini, 2008; Muñoz-Rivas, Graña, O’Leary, & Gonzá-
lez, 2007; Viejo, Sánchez, & Ortega, 2014; Nocentini, Pastorelli, &
Menesini, 2013), most research still comes from USA and Canada.
Moreover, cross-cultural research is still scarce, and mainly focused on
adolescents (see Menesini et al., 2011 for a comparison among Spain
and Italy; or Connolly et al., 2010 for a comparison among Italy and
Canada). Spain and Italy are countries with very similar cultural and so-
cial characteristics, but results of previous studies confirmed that both
Copyright © FrancoAngeli
N.B: Copia ad uso personale. È vietata la riproduzione (totale o parziale) dell’opera con qualsiasi
mezzo effettuata e la sua messa a disposizione di terzi, sia in forma gratuita sia a pagamento.
Cyberdating quality and dating aggression
Maltrattamento e abuso all’infanzia, Vol. 16, n. 3, novembre 2014, p. 47-65
51
countries present different levels of physical aggression. Although no
comparative studies have been conducted, results suggest that Spanish
adolescents are more involved in physical aggression than Italian (Vie-
jo, in press; Menesini & Nocentini, 2008) while no differences appear
for psychological forms of violence. For young adults, the studies are
still scarce and no consistent results have been presented. Studies devel-
oped in Spain, suggested that there is a decreasing trend from late ado-
lescence to the first years of adulthood (Corral, 2009; Fernández-
González, O’Leary, & Muñoz-Rivas, 2014), while in Italy prevalence
rates seem to indicate a stabilization of physical aggression in university
students (Romito & Grassi, 2007). According to literature, we expect to
find higher levels of physical dating aggression in Italian young adults
in comparison to Spanish ones. Regarding psychological aggression, we
expect to find similar rates in adolescents’ samples both in Spain and
Italy.
In relation to the impact of conventional and online quality couple in
dating aggression, results have indicated the central role of conflicts and
imbalance of power in the explanation of dating aggression across cul-
tures (Menesini et al., 2011). We do not know studies regarding intru-
siveness and online jealousy either in Spain or in Italy. Both countries
are characterized for being lower users of ICT, with Italy showing lower
levels than Spain in relation to children and adolescents users (EU-Kids
Online, 2013). A recent national report from Italy stated that children
and young adults of 6 to 25 years used internet and ICT below the aver-
age of the European countries (Istat, 2013). Similar levels have also
been found in Spain (INE, 2014). We hypothesize that the conflicts will
continue to impact on dating aggression, including also the influence on
online quality of the relationship. Considering the novelty of this study
and the absence of previous research on this topic, we do not have a
specific hypothesis for it.
2. Method
2.1 Sample
793 university students with current romantic relationships partici-
pated in the study (469 from Seville, Southern Spain; 324 from Flor-
ence, Central Italy). Participants were recruited from the faculties of
Psychology, Law, Sciences and Engineering, following Ferrer and col-
Copyright © FrancoAngeli
N.B: Copia ad uso personale. È vietata la riproduzione (totale o parziale) dell’opera con qualsiasi
mezzo effettuata e la sua messa a disposizione di terzi, sia in forma gratuita sia a pagamento.
Sánchez, Muñoz, Nocentini, Ortega-Ruiz, & Menesini
Maltrattamento e abuso all’infanzia, Vol. 16, n. 3, novembre 2014, pp. 47-65
52
leagues’ (Ferrer, Bosh, Ramis, & Navarro, 2006) considerations regard-
ing the impact of the formative curriculum in the perception and sensiti-
zation with dating aggression among university students. The distribu-
tion of students per faculty was representative of the total university
sample in each city. In both cases, there were more girls than boys
(71.6% and 66% in Italy and Spain, respectively). Sample characteris-
tics and descriptive results of the measures are shown on Table 1.
Table 1 - Sample descriptive statistics
Italy Spain Diff.
Sex Male 92(28.4%) 159(34%) ns
Female 232 (71.6%) 308(66%)
Age (M; SD) 21.14(1.85) 21.90(2.33) t (775.54)=5.113***
d=.36
Father Educa-
tion Illiterate 2 (0.7%) 17 (3.8%) X2(3)=175.024***
Elementary School 18 (5.9%) 196 (43.7%)
Middle School 76 (24.8%) 119 (26.5%)
Universitary degree 210 (68.6%) 117 (26.1%) X2(3)=236.894***
Mother Educa-
tion Illiterate 1 (0.3%) 20 (4.4%)
Elementary School 12 (3.9%) 205 (44.7%)
Middle School 75 (24.1%) 137 (29.8%)
Universitary degree 223 (71.7%) 97 (21.1%)
Mean relation-
ship length
(M; SD)
126.80 (98.61) 137.91
(108.24) ns
Conventional
Quality of
romantic rela-
tionship (M;
SD)
Conflict 2.04 (.70) 2.28 (.74) t(780)=4.436***
d=.33
Imbalance of power 1.72 (.78) 1.55 (.70) t(777)=3.022***
d=.23
Transgressive Behav-
iour 1.32 (.45) 1.43 (.57) t(760.93)=3.146***
d=.22
Online Quality
of romantic
relationship
(M; SD for
Jealousy)
Online Jealousy 1.35 (1.01) 1.14 (.89) t(604.53)=2.950***
d=.33
Intrusiveness NI 158 (51.5%) 209 (45.8%) Ns
I 149 (48.5%) 247 (54.2%)
Dating aggres-
sion Psychological NI 54 (16.8%) 70 (15%) ns
I 268 (83.2%) 396 (85%)
Physical NI 196 (60.9%) 361 (78.3%) X2(1)=28.708***
I 126 (39.1%) 100 (21.7%)
Note: NI = Not involvement; I=Involvement; d:.30 (small) .50(medium) .80 (large); **p .
001
Copyright © FrancoAngeli
N.B: Copia ad uso personale. È vietata la riproduzione (totale o parziale) dell’opera con qualsiasi
mezzo effettuata e la sua messa a disposizione di terzi, sia in forma gratuita sia a pagamento.
Cyberdating quality and dating aggression
Maltrattamento e abuso all’infanzia, Vol. 16, n. 3, novembre 2014, p. 47-65
53
2.2 Measures
Conventional Quality of dating relationships. Dating relationship
quality was measured by the Network Relationships Inventory (NRI;
Furman & Buhrmester, 1992). This measure comprises 8 items, which
assess two subscales on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from never true to
always true: (1) six items were related to conflict in the dyad (i.e., I get
upset with my boy/girlfriend); Italy Į = .82; Spain Į = .89; (2) two items
were related to imbalance of power in the dyad (i.e., How often does
someone tend to be bossy in this relationship?); Italy Į = .64; Spain Į
=.64. Transgressive behavior in the dyad was measured by an adapta-
tion of four items on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from never true to
always true (Fuligni & Eccles, 1993) (i.e., how often do you break the
rules to please your partner?) Italy Į = .72; Spain Į = .73. Previous stud-
ies using the same measures showed cross-cultural invariance (Menesini
et al., 2011).
Online quality of dating relationship. Online quality of dating rela-
tionship was measured using two subscales of Cyberdating_Q (CBD;
Sánchez, Muñoz & Ortega-Ruiz, in press; Sánchez, Muñoz, & Ortega-
Ruiz, 2014). This measure comprised 10 items, which assess two sub-
scales on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from never true to always true;
(1) six items focused on online jealousy, Italy Į = .83; Spain Į = .80
(i.e., I feel jealous if my partner posts provocative photos on her/his so-
cial network profile); and (2) four items were related to intrusiveness
(Italy Į =.72; Spain Į = .74). According to Spitzberg & Cupach (2003),
intrusiveness has been conceptualized as a specific form of cyberstalk-
ing characterized by invasive, unwanted and, undesirable behavior that
violate physical or symbolic privacy of the partner. In this study, we
will consider intrusiveness as a response to a breakup within the rela-
tionship (i.e., when I’m angry and my partner does not answer me I
leave many messages on her/his private chat).
Dating Aggression. Dating Aggression was assessed using two
scales measuring psychological and physical aggression. Following the
definition by Straus (1979) and Capaldi & Crosby (1997), we decided to
include verbal aggression, offense, threatening and control in the cate-
gory of psychological aggression. The first scale comprised nine items
for perpetrated acts, derived from the Physical Violence Scale of Con-
flict Tactics Scale-2 (Nocentini, Menesini, Pastorelli, Connolly, Pepler,
& Craig, 2011; Straus, 1979; Viejo, Sánchez, & Ortega, 2014), i.e.,
Copyright © FrancoAngeli
N.B: Copia ad uso personale. È vietata la riproduzione (totale o parziale) dell’opera con qualsiasi
mezzo effettuata e la sua messa a disposizione di terzi, sia in forma gratuita sia a pagamento.
Sánchez, Muñoz, Nocentini, Ortega-Ruiz, & Menesini
Maltrattamento e abuso all’infanzia, Vol. 16, n. 3, novembre 2014, pp. 47-65
54
pushing, grabbing or shoving, slapping, kicking, or biting (Italy Į = .83;
Spain Į = .91). The second scale was derived from the Relational Ag-
gression Scale (Crick, 1995) and consisted of six items focused on psy-
chological aggression (i.e., spreading rumors or telling mean lies to
make one’s boy/girlfriend unpopular; telling one’s boy/girlfriend she/he
won’t be liked anymore unless she/he does what she/he is being told to
do; teasing), (Italy Į = .75; Spain Į = .68). The items of the two scales
were rated on a 5- point Likert scale, ranging from never true to always
true. This measure has been previously validated in Italy (Nocentini et
al., 2011) and Spain (Viejo et al., 2014) showing scalar invariance
across countries (Menesini et al., 2011).
2.3 Plan of analyses
We conducted a binomial logistic regression across country, consid-
ering two profiles of involvement in aggressive behaviors – psychologi-
cal or physical – as dependent variables; and gender and quality of ro-
mantic relationships – conventional and online – as independent varia-
bles. All these analyses were conducted using SPSS 20. The logistic re-
gression was evaluated by R2 Nagerlkerke, and Percentage of Cases
Correctly Classified (PCCC).
3. Results
3.1 Descriptive results
Measures of physical and psychological dating aggression were re-
coded in two points variables (Not involved versus Involved). Partici-
pants were considered as involved in physical or psychological dating
aggression if they reported to perpetrate at least one of the items of the
physical or psychological aggression scales.
As shown in Table 1, no significant differences among countries
were found for psychological aggression. For physical aggression, re-
sults showed that Italian university students were significantly more in-
volved than Spanish ones.
In relation to conventional and online quality of the romantic rela-
tionships, results showed that Italian young adults presented higher lev-
els of imbalance of power and online jealousy than Spanish ones. In
contrast, Spanish participants presented higher level of conflicts and
Copyright © FrancoAngeli
N.B: Copia ad uso personale. È vietata la riproduzione (totale o parziale) dell’opera con qualsiasi
mezzo effettuata e la sua messa a disposizione di terzi, sia in forma gratuita sia a pagamento.
Cyberdating quality and dating aggression
Maltrattamento e abuso all’infanzia, Vol. 16, n. 3, novembre 2014, p. 47-65
55
transgressive behaviors than Italians. No statistical differences were
found for intrusiveness.
No gender differences were found within countries for Psychological
(Spain, Ȥ2 psychological(1) = 1.103; p = .339; Italy, Ȥ2psychological(1)
=.222; p = .742), nor for physical aggression (Spain, Ȥ2physical(1) =
.318; p = .632; Italy, Ȥ2physical (1) = 1.597; p = .209).
In relation to the quality of romantic relationships, significant differ-
ences were found for transgressive behavior (Spain t(460) = 5.838, p <.
001; Italy t(314) = 3.628, p < .001) and imbalance of power (Spain t(460) =
3.430, p < .001; Italy t(313) = 2.749, p < .001). Boys presented higher
levels of transgressive behavior and imbalance of power than girls both
in Spain and Italy. There was no significant effect of gender on conflicts
(Spain t(460) = -1.651, p = .099; Italy t(316) = .430; p = .668), online jeal-
ousy (Spain t(457) = -1.310, p = .191; Italy t(307) = -1.31, p = .191) and in-
trusiveness (Spain, Ȥ2intrusiveness(1) = .178; p = .692; Italy,
Ȥ2intrusiveness(1) = .196; p = .704).
3.2 Modeling the association between conventional and online
quality of romantic relationships, and physical and psychologi-
cal dating aggression
Binomial logistic regressions were run in order to analyze the impact
of online and conventional negative quality within the romantic couple
(conflict, imbalance of power, transgressive behavior, intrusiveness and
online jealousy) as predictors of being involved in psychological and
physical aggression.
Previous analysis confirmed significant associations among predic-
tors and the involvement in both forms of aggression, so all the predic-
tors were included in the model. Given the relevant interplay of gender
and country in the results, regressions were run separately by country,
and sex was controlled including this variable in a first step. A two-step
model was run with main effects in the first step, and interaction effects
of quality with gender in the second step. Since no interaction effects
were found, we deleted it from the final model. Table 2 shows the re-
gression coefficients and the odds ratio for the final model in both coun-
tries.
The final models showed significant associations between conven-
tional and online negative quality and being involved in psychological
and physical aggression in boys and girls. Overall, in Spain, involve-
Copyright © FrancoAngeli
N.B: Copia ad uso personale. È vietata la riproduzione (totale o parziale) dell’opera con qualsiasi
mezzo effettuata e la sua messa a disposizione di terzi, sia in forma gratuita sia a pagamento.
Sánchez, Muñoz, Nocentini, Ortega-Ruiz, & Menesini
Maltrattamento e abuso all’infanzia, Vol. 16, n. 3, novembre 2014, pp. 47-65
56
ment in psychological and physical dating aggression was predicted by
conflicts, transgressive behavior and intrusiveness. In Italy, involvement
in both aggressive behaviors was predicted by conflicts and online jeal-
ousy, and psychological dating aggression by transgressive behavior. In
both countries imbalance of power was excluded from the final model.
Table 2 - Betas and standard errors resulting from the logistical regression
analysis in both samples, Spain and Italy
Spain Italy
Psychologi-
cal aggres-
siona
Physical
aggressionb Psychologi-
cal aggres-
sionc
Physical
aggressiond
Romantic Re-
lationship
Quality
Conflict 1.344(.265)
OR =
3.834***
.984(.183)
OR=
2.676***
1.146(.333)
OR =
3.146***
.724(.192)
OR =
2.062***
Transgres-
sive beha-
viours
1.185(.441)
OR =
3.272**
.476(.214)
OR =
1.609*
1.666(.749)
OR =
5.288*
ns.
Romantic Re-
lationship
Quality Onli-
ne
Intrusive-
ness -.881 (.301)
OR =
.414**
-1.074(.288)
OR =
.342***
ns. ns.
Online Jea-
lousy ns. ns. .692(.218)
OR =
1.998***
.270(.127)
OR =
1.311*
Note: *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.
aR2 = .24; % PCCC = 86.5%; bR2 = .25; X% PCCC = 80.4%; cR2 = .23; % PCCC = 83%; dR2 =
.14; % PCCC = 66.9%
Specifically, in Spain, a unit of change in conflict and in transgres-
sive behavior increases the odds of being involved in psychological ag-
gression rather than not being involved by about 3.8 and 3.3 times, re-
spectively; for physical aggression, conflicts and transgressive behav-
iors increase the odds of being involved rather than not being involved
by about 2.7 and 1.6 times, respectively. Likewise, to be involved in in-
trusive behaviors increases the odds of being involved in psychological
Copyright © FrancoAngeli
N.B: Copia ad uso personale. È vietata la riproduzione (totale o parziale) dell’opera con qualsiasi
mezzo effettuata e la sua messa a disposizione di terzi, sia in forma gratuita sia a pagamento.
Cyberdating quality and dating aggression
Maltrattamento e abuso all’infanzia, Vol. 16, n. 3, novembre 2014, p. 47-65
57
aggression rather than not being involved by about 0.4 times; and by
about 0.3 times in physical aggression.
In Italy, a unit of change in conflict and in transgressive behavior in-
creases the odds of being involved in psychological aggression rather
than not being involved by about 3.1 and 5.2 times, respectively; in the
case of physical aggression, conflict increases the odds of being in-
volved by about 2.1 times in comparison to not involved participants.
Finally, a unit of change in online jealousy increases the odds of being
involved in psychological aggression rather than not being involved by
about 2 times, and 1.3 times with regard to the involvement in physical
aggression in comparison to not involved participants.
4. Conclusions
Studies on dating aggression in young adults are still scarce in Eu-
rope, and particularly in Italy and Spain. This work represents a first at-
tempt to explore the predictors of physical and psychological dating ag-
gression related to online and conventional quality couples of young
adults. In a digital world, where ICTs are changing the way people so-
cialize, the analysis of how online behaviors in romantic relationships
impact face to face contacts, needs to be performed in order to under-
stand more accurately the dynamics of romantic relationships. This is
especially true in different cultures, where the meanings people attribute
to online and offline behaviors within romantic relationships can be in-
terpreted differently (Seiffge-Krenke, 2008).
Results have showed that Italian university students present higher
levels of physical aggression than Spanish, while no differences were
found for psychological aggression. For Spain, results on physical da-
ting aggression are similar to those of Corral (2009) and Fernández-
González and colleagues (2014). Both papers showed a pattern of phys-
ical dating aggression decreasing with age. The authors explained this
trend as a consequence of a developmental pattern where youngsters
mature in terms of personality, but also in terms of social and romantic
relationships (Moffit, 2006). In contrast, results from Italy are not con-
clusive. Nocentini, Menesini & Pastorelli (2010) found a significant lin-
ear decrease of physical aggression from 16 to 18 years, while Romito
& Grassi (2007) found similar rates of physical aggression in compari-
son to adolescent years, as we found in this study. Cohort effects and
different design methods (transversal and longitudinal) could influence
Copyright © FrancoAngeli
N.B: Copia ad uso personale. È vietata la riproduzione (totale o parziale) dell’opera con qualsiasi
mezzo effettuata e la sua messa a disposizione di terzi, sia in forma gratuita sia a pagamento.
Sánchez, Muñoz, Nocentini, Ortega-Ruiz, & Menesini
Maltrattamento e abuso all’infanzia, Vol. 16, n. 3, novembre 2014, pp. 47-65
58
the results, indicating that studies on physical dating aggression in Italy
need further assessments. As expected, no differences were found be-
tween Italy and Spain in relation to psychological dating aggression.
More than 80% of university students affirmed to perpetrate one or
more of these behaviors against their partners, stressing the normative
use of these behaviors among young couples (Foshee et al., 2009). Since
very scarce evidence exists regarding the prevalence of psychological
aggression in young adults, studies to come should clarify this trend in
the results and cultural correlated factors.
Logistic regressions showed that conflicts within the couple in-
creased the probability of being involved in psychological and physical
aggression in both countries. These results are consistent with previous
cross-cultural studies (Menesini et al., 2011) and reflect the conflictual
and contextual nature of dating aggression also in the first years of
adulthood. In contrast, imbalance of power did not show any effect on
dating aggression, reflecting an important level of symmetry in power
between the two partners. Previous studies on dating aggression in ado-
lescence have repeatedly showed that the low levels of imbalance of
power are related to the fact that dating aggression is mainly reciprocal
or mutual (Archer, 2000; Johnson, 1995; Menesini et al., 2011), where
males and females present the same levels of aggression and victimiza-
tion. This mutual violence regards couples where both members fight
for the control of the relationship, and there is not a clearly dominating
partner. Higher levels of reciprocal violence in comparison to pure or
unidirectional roles have also been presented in recent studies on dating
aggression in young adults (Fernández-Gonzáles et al., 2014; Marcus,
2012). Even if we have not controlled reciprocal involvement in this
study, results seem to point to this direction. Futures studies could con-
firm this result.
Transgressive behavior has been described as an important predictor
of psychological and physical aggression in Spain, and of psychological
aggression in Italy. This result is very interesting because it could reflect
a certain kind of reactivity in dating aggression among young adults.
Transgressive behavior can be defined as a feeling of being under the
pressure of the partner (Fuligni & Eccles, 1993) with people affirming
to have difficulties to negotiate their needs with their partners’ needs.
Together with conflicts and with the absence of imbalance of power, the
results should indicate that the perception of being submitted or domi-
nated by the partner will deal on conflicts and, finally, on dating aggres-
Copyright © FrancoAngeli
N.B: Copia ad uso personale. È vietata la riproduzione (totale o parziale) dell’opera con qualsiasi
mezzo effettuata e la sua messa a disposizione di terzi, sia in forma gratuita sia a pagamento.
Cyberdating quality and dating aggression
Maltrattamento e abuso all’infanzia, Vol. 16, n. 3, novembre 2014, p. 47-65
59
sion (Werkele & Wolfe, 1999), resulting in a more insecure and impul-
sive aggression. The inclusion of emotional variables in futures studies,
as attachment style, emotion regulation and self-esteem, could help to
test this hypothesis.
The predictor effect of online quality relationships on physical and
psychological aggression in young adults has underlined the importance
of this context in young romantic couples. Intrusiveness was the main
online predictor of physical and psychological aggression in Spain
while online jealousy was the main factor in Italy. It is difficult to un-
derstand why these clear differences appeared in the two countries since
no previous study exists on this topic. Overall, the profile continues to
delineate the insecurity as the main characteristic of the romantic cou-
ples in both countries, as previous studies about cyberstalking, insecure
attachment and online jealousy have shown (see Fox & Garber, 2014;
Nitzburg & Farber, 2013; Strawhun, Adams, & Huss, 2013). Starting
from this pattern of insecurity, Spanish participants seem to externalize
more their insecurity, invading the online privacy of their partners fol-
lowing an argument or a break-up. These online obsessive behaviors
could increase the levels of conflicts and lead to psychological and
physical aggression. Italian young adults, in contrast, present a more in-
ternalized profile of online insecurity, often displayed with feelings of
jealousy, mistrustfulness and fear about their partners’ online behavior.
These feelings could contribute to an escalation of conflicts and discus-
sions that would lead, consequently, to psychological and physical ag-
gression. Future studies, including other measures, as rumination and
other externalized behaviors together with cultural factors, could clarify
these two different profiles found in both countries.
Even if the impact of conventional and online quality has been
demonstrated, the impact of the different predictors was different. Con-
ventional quality presented more predictive power than online quality in
both countries, Spain and Italy. These results would suggest that online
interactions have not the same relevance as face to face interactions in
romantic relationships. Cauglin & Sharabi (2013) have concluded with a
similar consideration. In their study, young adults affirmed that face to
face communication was more important than online interactions. When
the latter was used as an instrument or resource for the former, the lev-
els of satisfaction and intimacy with the partner increased. To this re-
spect, it could be interesting to test if online interaction could have the
same impact in adolescents’ couples or in couples at an early stage of
Copyright © FrancoAngeli
N.B: Copia ad uso personale. È vietata la riproduzione (totale o parziale) dell’opera con qualsiasi
mezzo effettuata e la sua messa a disposizione di terzi, sia in forma gratuita sia a pagamento.
Sánchez, Muñoz, Nocentini, Ortega-Ruiz, & Menesini
Maltrattamento e abuso all’infanzia, Vol. 16, n. 3, novembre 2014, pp. 47-65
60
the relationship. Future studies can test if the stability of the romantic
relationship together with the age of the participant could vary in rela-
tion to the weight of the online and conventional predictors of dating
aggression.
This study has been a first attempt to explore the impact of online in-
teraction on dating aggression in young adults. Since the research in this
area is still scarce, this study represents one of the first contributions
that leaves many open questions on the relation among online and off-
line context in the explanation of dating aggression. The cross-cultural
design has allowed to find similar trends in both countries, but also cul-
tural differences that need to be explored deeply in future studies. The
study presents, also, limitations that must be addressed. First of all, the
measures of online quality were partial and very specific. Since the top-
ic of online quality is emergent, researchers ought to make efforts to de-
sign more comprehensive measures of online quality in romantic rela-
tionships. A second limitation is that we have not included positive
measures of conventional and online quality. The analyses would pro-
vide keys for the design of prevention programs, not only oriented to
minimize the negative aspects of the couples but also maximizing the
positive ones. Finally, another limitation is that we have not controlled
the time that participants used ICT and internet together with other per-
sonal measures already mentioned and other forms of electronic aggres-
sion. Their inclusion in futures studies would illuminate the interplay of
personal and relational factors in online contexts and their relation to
dating aggression.
References
Archer, J. (2000). Sex differences in aggression between heterosexual partners:
A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 651-680, doi:
10.1037//0033-2909.126.5.651.
Arnett, J. J (2001). Conceptions of the transition to adulthood: Perspectives
from adolescence to midlife. Journal of Adult Development, 8, 133-143,
doi: 10.1023/a:1026450103225.
Butzer, B., & Kuiper, N. A. (2008). Humor use in romantic relationships: The
effects of relationship satisfaction and pleasant versus conflict situations.
The Journal of psychology, 142, 3, 245-260, doi: 10.3200/JRLP.142.3.245-
260.
Copyright © FrancoAngeli
N.B: Copia ad uso personale. È vietata la riproduzione (totale o parziale) dell’opera con qualsiasi
mezzo effettuata e la sua messa a disposizione di terzi, sia in forma gratuita sia a pagamento.
Cyberdating quality and dating aggression
Maltrattamento e abuso all’infanzia, Vol. 16, n. 3, novembre 2014, p. 47-65
61
Capaldi, D. M., & Crosby, L. (1997). Observed and reported psychological and
physical aggression in young, at-risk couples. Social Development, 6, 2,
184-206, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.1997.tb00101.x.
Capaldi, D. M., & Kim, H. K. (2007). Typological approaches to violence in
couples: A critique and alternative conceptual approach. Clinical Psycholo-
gy Review, 27, 3, 253-265, doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.09.001.
Caughlin, J. P., & Sharabi, L. L. (2013). A communicative interdependence
perspective of close relationships: The connections between mediated and
unmediated interactions matter. Journal of Communication, 63, 5, 873-893,
doi: 10.1111/jcom.12046.
Collins, W. A., Welsh, D. P., & Furman, W. (2009). Adolescent romantic rela-
tionships. Annual Review of psychology, 60, 631-652, doi:
10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163459.
Connolly, J., Nocentini, A., Menesini, E., Pepler, D., Craig, W., & Williams, T.
S. (2010). Adolescent dating aggression in Canada and Italy: A cross-
national comparison. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 34,
2, 98-105, doi: 10.1177/0165025409360291.
Corral, S. (2009). Estudio de la violencia en el noviazgo en jóvenes universitar-
ios/as: cronicidad, severidad y mutualidad de las conductas violentas.
Psicopatología Clínica Legal y Forense, 9, 29-48.
Crick, N. R. (1995). Relational aggression: The role of intent attributions,
provocation type, and feelings of distress. Development and
Psychopathology, 7, 313-322, doi: 10.1017/S0954579400006520.
Davila, J., Steinberg, S. J., Kachadourian, L., Cobb, R., & Fincham, F. (2004).
Romantic involvement and depressive symptoms in early and late adoles-
cence: The role of a preoccupied relational style. Personal Relationships,
11, 2, 161-178, doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2004.00076.x.
Elphinston, R. A., & Noller, P. (2011). Time to face it! Facebook inclusion and
the implications for romantic jealousy and relationships satisfaction. Cy-
berpsychology Behavior and Social Networking, 14, 11, 631-635, doi:
10.1089/cyber.2010.0318.
EU Kids Online. (2013). Second annual progress report. EU Kids Online, De-
liverable D1.5B. London, UK: The London School of Economics and Polit-
ical Science.
Fernández-González, L., O’Leary, K. D. & Muñoz-Rivas, J. M. (2014). Age-
related changes in dating aggression in Spanish High School Students.
Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 29, 6, 1135-1152, doi:
10.1177/0886260513506057.
Ferrer, V. A., Bosh, E., Ramis, M. C., & Navarro, C. (2006). Las creencias y
actitudes sobre la violencia contra las mujeres en la pareja: determinantes
socio demográficos, familiares y formativos. Anales de Psicología, 22, 2,
251-259.
Copyright © FrancoAngeli
N.B: Copia ad uso personale. È vietata la riproduzione (totale o parziale) dell’opera con qualsiasi
mezzo effettuata e la sua messa a disposizione di terzi, sia in forma gratuita sia a pagamento.
Sánchez, Muñoz, Nocentini, Ortega-Ruiz, & Menesini
Maltrattamento e abuso all’infanzia, Vol. 16, n. 3, novembre 2014, pp. 47-65
62
Foran, H. M., & O’Leary, K. D. (2008). Alcohol and intimate partner violence:
A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 7, 1222-1234, doi:
10.1016/j.cpr.2008.05.001.
Foshee, V. A., Benefield, T., Suchindran, C., Ennett, S. T., Bauman, K. E.,
Karriker-Jaffe, K. J., McNaughton, H. L., & Mathias, J. (2009). The devel-
opment of four types of adolescent dating abuse and selected demographic
correlates. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 19, 3, 380-400, doi:
10.1111/j.1532-7795.2009.00593.x.
Fox, J., & Garber, K. M. (2014). Social networking sites in romantic relation-
ships: Attachment, uncertainty, and partner surveillance on facebook. Cy-
berpsychology behavior and social networking, 17, 1, 3-7, doi:
10.1089/cyber.2012.0667.
Fuligni, A. J., & Eccles, J. S. (1993). Perceived parent-child relationships and
early adolescents’ orientation toward peers. Developmental Psychology, 29,
4, 622-632, doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.29.4.622.
Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1992). Age and sex differences in perceptions
of networks of personal relationships. Child Development, 63, 1, 103-115,
doi: 10.2307/1130905.
Garcia, J. R., & Reiber, C. (2008). Hook-up behavior: A biopsychosocial per-
spective. Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Psychology, 2, 4,
192-208, doi: 10.1037/h0099345.
Grover, R. L., & Nangle, D. W. (2007). Introduction to the special section on
adolescent romantic competence: Development and adjustment implica-
tions. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 36, 4, 485-490,
doi: 10.1080/15374410701649342.
Instituto Nacional de Estadística (2014). España en Cifras 2014.
Ishii, K. (2010). Conflict management in online relationships. Cyberpsycholo-
gy, Behavior, and Social Networking, 13, 4, 365-370, doi:
10.1089/cyber.2009.0272.
Istituto Nazionale di Statitistica (2013). Noi Italia. 100 statistiche per capire il
paese in cui viviamo.
Johnson, M. P. (1995). Patriarchical terrorism and common couple violence:
Two forms of violence among women. Journal of Marriage and the Fami-
ly, 57, 283-294, doi: 10.2307/353683.
La Greca, A. M., & Harrison, H. M. (2005). Adolescent peer relations, friend-
ships, and romantic relationships: Do they predict social anxiety and de-
pression? Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 34, 1, 49-
61, doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3401_5.
Lavy, S., Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2010). Autonomy-proximity imbal-
ance: An attachment theory perspective on intrusiveness in romantic rela-
tionships. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 5, 552-556, doi:
10.1016/j.paid.2009.12.004.
Copyright © FrancoAngeli
N.B: Copia ad uso personale. È vietata la riproduzione (totale o parziale) dell’opera con qualsiasi
mezzo effettuata e la sua messa a disposizione di terzi, sia in forma gratuita sia a pagamento.
Cyberdating quality and dating aggression
Maltrattamento e abuso all’infanzia, Vol. 16, n. 3, novembre 2014, p. 47-65
63
Madsen, S. D., & Collins, W. A. (2011). The salience of adolescent romantic
experiences for romantic relationship qualities in young adulthood. Journal
of Research on Adolescence, 21, 4, 789-801, doi: 10.1111/j.1532-
7795.2011.00737.x.
Marcus, R. F. (2012). Patterns of intimate partner violence in Young adult cou-
ples: nonviolent, unilaterally violent, and mutually violent couples. Vio-
lence and victims, 27, 3, 299-314, doi: 10.1891/0886-6708.27.3.299.
Ménard, K. S., & Pincus, A. L. (2012). Predicting overt and cyber stalking per-
petration by male and female college students. Journal of Interpersonal
Violence, 27, 11, 2183-2207, doi: 10.1177/0886260511432144.
Menesini, E., & Nocentini, A. (2008). Comportamenti aggressivi nelle prime
esperienze sentimentali in adolescenza. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia, 2,
407-434.
Menesini, E., Nocentini, A., Ortega-Rivera, F. J., Sánchez, V., & Ortega, R.
(2011). Reciprocal involvement in adolescent dating aggression: An Italian-
Spanish study. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 8, 4, 437-
451, doi: 10.1080/17405629.2010.549011.
Messinger, A. M., Rickert, V. I., Fry, D. A., Lessel, H., & Davidson, L. L.
(2012). Revisiting the role of communication in adolescent intimate partner
violence. Journal of Interpersonal violence, 27, 14, 2920-2935, doi:
10.1177/0886260512438276.
Moffit, T. E. (2006). Life-Course persistent versus adolescence-limited antiso-
cial behavior. In D. Ciccheti & D. J. Cohen (Ed), Developmental Psycho-
pathology: Risk, disorder, and adaptation (Vol. 3, pp. 570-598). NJ: John
Wiley & Sons.
Muñoz-Rivas, M. J., Graña, J. L., O’Leary, K. D., & González, M. P. (2007).
Aggression in adolescent dating relationships: prevalence, justification, and
health consequences. Journal of Adolescent Health, 40, 4, 298-304, doi:
10.1016/j.jadohealth.2006.11.137.
Nguyen, L. K., Spitzberg, B. H., & Lee, C. M. (2012). Coping with obsessive
relational intrusion and stalking: The role of social support and coping
strategies. Violence and Victims, 27, 3, 414-433, doi:
http://dx.doi.org/10.1891/0886-6708.27.3.414.
Nitzburg, G. C., & Farber, B. A. (2013). Putting Up Emotional (Facebook)
walls? Attachment status and emerging adults’ experiences of social net-
working sites. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 69, 11, 1183-1190, doi:
10.1002/jclp.22045.
Nocentini, A., Menesini, E., Pastorelli, C., Connolly, J., Pepler, D., & Craig,
W. (2011). Physical dating aggression in adolescence. Cultural and gender
invariance. European Psychologist, 16, 4, 278-287, doi: 10.1027/1016-
9040/a000045.
Copyright © FrancoAngeli
N.B: Copia ad uso personale. È vietata la riproduzione (totale o parziale) dell’opera con qualsiasi
mezzo effettuata e la sua messa a disposizione di terzi, sia in forma gratuita sia a pagamento.
Sánchez, Muñoz, Nocentini, Ortega-Ruiz, & Menesini
Maltrattamento e abuso all’infanzia, Vol. 16, n. 3, novembre 2014, pp. 47-65
64
Nocentini, A., Menesini, E., & Pastorelli, C. (2010). Physical dating aggression
growth during adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 3,
353-565, doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9371-8.
Nocentini, A., Pastorelli, C., & Menesini, E. (2013). Self-efficacy in anger
management and dating aggression in Italian young adults. International
Journal of Conflict and Violence, 7, 2, 274-285, doi: 0070-ijcv-2013267.
Romito, P., & Grassi, M. (2007). Does violence affect one gender more than
the other? The mental health impact of violence among male and female
university students. Social Science & Medicine, 65, 6, 1222-1234, doi:
10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.05.017.
Sánchez, V., Muñoz, N., & Ortega-Ruiz, R. (in press). “Cyberdating Q_A”: an
instrument to assess the quality of adolescent dating relationships in social
networks. Article accepted for publication in Computers in Human Behav-
ior.
Sánchez, V., Muñoz, N., & Ortega-Ruiz, R. (2014). Quality of romantic rela-
tionship online and offline in university students. 14th Biennial Conference
of the European Association for Research on Adolescence, Cesme, Turkey.
Seiffge-Krenke, I. (2008). Why is a cross-cultural perspective on romantic ex-
periences in adolescence clearly needed? Newsletter from ISSBD, 1, 53, 19-
20.
Seiffge-Krenke, I., & Connolly, J. (2010) Adolescent romantic relationships
across the globe: Involvement, conflict management, and linkages to par-
ents and peer relationships. International Journal of Behavioral Develop-
ment, 34, 2, 97-97, doi:10.1177/0165025409360289.
Sheridan, L. P., & Grant, T. (2007). Is cyberstalking different? Psychology,
Crime & Law, 13, 6, 627-640, doi: 10.1080/10683160701340528.
Shulman, S., & Connolly, J. (2013). The challenge of romantic relationships in
emerging adulthood reconceptualization of the field. Emerging Adulthood,
1, 1, 27-39, doi: 10.1177/2167696812467330.
Shulman, S., Feldman, B., Blatt, S., Cohen, O., & Mahler, A. (2005). Emerging
adulthood. Journal of Adolescent Research, 20, 577-603, doi:
10.1177/0743558405274913.
Spitzberg, B. H., & Cupach, W. R. (2007). The state of the art of stalking: Tak-
ing stock of the emerging literature. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 12,
1, 64-86, doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2006.05.001.
Straus, M. (1979). Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence: The Conflict
Tactics (CT) Scales. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 41, 75-88, doi:
10.2307/351733.
Strawhun, J., Adams, N., & Huss, M. T. (2013). The assessment of cyberstalk-
ing: An expanded examination including social networking, attachment,
jealousy, and anger in relation to violence and abuse. Violence and Victims,
28, 4, 715-730, doi: 10.1891/0886-6708.11-00145.
Copyright © FrancoAngeli
N.B: Copia ad uso personale. È vietata la riproduzione (totale o parziale) dell’opera con qualsiasi
mezzo effettuata e la sua messa a disposizione di terzi, sia in forma gratuita sia a pagamento.
Cyberdating quality and dating aggression
Maltrattamento e abuso all’infanzia, Vol. 16, n. 3, novembre 2014, p. 47-65
65
Viejo, C., Sánchez, V., & Ortega-Ruiz, R. (2014). Violencia física en la pareja
adolescente: la potencialidad interpretativa de un modelo bifactorial. Anales
de Psicología, 30, 1, 171-179, doi: 10.6018/analesps.30.1.141341.
Viejo, C. (in press). Violencia física en las relaciones sentimentales adolescen-
tes: ¿cortejo rudo o violencia de género? Infancia y Aprendizaje.
Wekerle, C., & Wolfe, D. A. (1999). Dating violence in mid-adolescence: The-
ory, significance, and emerging prevention initiatives. Clinical Psychology
Review, 19, 4, 435- 456, doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(98)00091-9.
Pervenuto luglio 2014
Accettato settembre 2014
Copyright © FrancoAngeli
N.B: Copia ad uso personale. È vietata la riproduzione (totale o parziale) dell’opera con qualsiasi
mezzo effettuata e la sua messa a disposizione di terzi, sia in forma gratuita sia a pagamento.
... Indeed, Sánchez-Jiménez et al. (2015) refer to this type of online dating aggression as online intrusiveness and consider it a specific dimension with unique characteristics. According to these authors, what makes online intrusiveness a differential form of control in adolescence is the fact that these behaviours occur in specific contexts, such as after an argument with a partner who then refuses to maintain contact (e.g., by not responding to or by blocking the other party), and are motivated by a desire to maintain intimacy or reconcile using aggressive conflict resolution strategies (Sánchez-Jiménez et al., 2014;Spitzberg & Rhea, 1999). This characteristic would make online intrusiveness a more contextual and less planned type of aggression than online monitoring or control. ...
... For its part, online intrusiveness aggression has been less studied and is identified only in the 'Cyberdating Q_A' instrument (Sánchez-Jiménez et al., 2015). Prevalence rates are similar to those reported for online control, i.e., between 50% and 70% depending on the country (Sánchez-Jiménez et al., 2014), although the lack of previous evidence precludes any conclusions being drawn in this sense, and further research is required. ...
... Regarding gender, the evidence seems to indicate that girls are more commonly involved as aggressors in online control than boys (Cava, Martínez-Ferrer, et al., 2020;Rodríguez-deArriba et al., 2023), whereas boys tend to perpetrate more online intrusiveness than girls (Sánchez-Jiménez et al., 2014. According to these studies, girls tend to perpetrate online dating aggression in a more premeditated and even indirect way, as is the case with online control and monitoring, whereas boys tend to have more explosive aggressive reactions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Technological advances have led to dating aggression being perpetrated online, with online control and online intrusiveness (repeated attempts to gain access to one’s partner after an argument) being two of the most prevalent forms. Previous studies have made headway in terms of identifying the correlates of online control, but longitudinal studies are still scarce. Furthermore, the predictors of online intrusiveness are yet unknown. The present study aimed to explore the cognitive and emotional predictors of these two types of online dating aggression. The role of moral disengagement (MD), anger dysregulation and online jealousy were analysed. A total of 467 Spanish adolescents aged between 12 and 19 years (M = 14.03, SD = 1.41, 57.2% girls) with recent romantic experience participated in a longitudinal study with two time points (W1, W2) separated by a four-month interval. The results revealed that, although all variables were correlated, predictors varied in accordance with the specific form of online aggression analysed. Online control aggression at W2 was predicted by previous involvement in online control and online intrusiveness aggression, high levels of anger dysregulation (W1) and online jealousy (W1). In contrast, online intrusiveness aggression at W2 was predicted by prior involvement in the same form of online aggression and poor anger regulation (W1). Moral disengagement lost its predictive value when emotional variables were incorporated. The results highlight the multidimensional nature of online dating aggression in adolescent couples and suggest that, although the strong link between online control and intrusiveness should be taken into consideration, the two forms of aggression should be explored independently.
... More numerous studies have analyzed the relation between negative online relationship dynamics and different indicators of romantic relationship quality in young adults. For instance, online jealousy -understood as an emotional reaction prompted by visualizing online relationship content -and concerns and suspicions about a partner's interest in someone else (Utz & Beukeboom, 2011) have been associated with low levels of relationship satisfaction (Elphinston & Noller, 2011), and predict involvement in psychological (Strawhun, Adams, & Huss, 2013) and physical abuse (Sánchez, Muñoz, Nocentini, Ortega-Ruiz, & Menesini, 2014). On the other hand, research on online intrusive behavior, which is understood as repeated, obsessive attempts to initiate contact and communicate with a partner after a break-up or fight, has revealed that people who exhibit these behaviors in online mode do so offline too (Spitzberg & Hoobler, 2002;Strawhun et al., 2013), and has linked it to low relationship quality (Lavy, Mikulincer, Shaver, & Gillath, 2009) as well as intimate partner abuse (Sánchez et al., 2014;Strawhun et al., 2013). ...
... For instance, online jealousy -understood as an emotional reaction prompted by visualizing online relationship content -and concerns and suspicions about a partner's interest in someone else (Utz & Beukeboom, 2011) have been associated with low levels of relationship satisfaction (Elphinston & Noller, 2011), and predict involvement in psychological (Strawhun, Adams, & Huss, 2013) and physical abuse (Sánchez, Muñoz, Nocentini, Ortega-Ruiz, & Menesini, 2014). On the other hand, research on online intrusive behavior, which is understood as repeated, obsessive attempts to initiate contact and communicate with a partner after a break-up or fight, has revealed that people who exhibit these behaviors in online mode do so offline too (Spitzberg & Hoobler, 2002;Strawhun et al., 2013), and has linked it to low relationship quality (Lavy, Mikulincer, Shaver, & Gillath, 2009) as well as intimate partner abuse (Sánchez et al., 2014;Strawhun et al., 2013). Regarding online monitoring or control, that is, surveillance and tracking of the partner's online activity (Tokunaga, 2011), results have been inconclusive. ...
... In Spain, research on this subject remains nascent. Many studies have analyzed cyberbullying in young couples (Durán-Segura & Martínez-Pecino, 2015) in relation to offline violence (Sánchez et al., 2014), but we know of no study that analyzed how online couple quality contributes to relationship satisfaction in a college population. In this study, we understand relationship satisfaction as an indicator of relationship quality, and as characterized by intimacy, communication, and a desire to stay in the relationship (Madsen & Collins, 2011). ...
Article
Full-text available
Recent studies suggest that the online and offline behaviors young people display in romantic relationships are closely related. However, the differential effects of the dimensions of couple quality in the online context have not yet been explored in depth. The aim of this study was to explore online couple quality in young-adult relationships, and its association with romantic relationship satisfaction, also looking at effects of gender, age, and length of the relationship. 431 university students currently in a romantic relationship (68.2% females; mean age = 21.57) participated in this study. They completed different self-report measures to tap the online quality of their romantic relationships (online intimacy, control, jealousy, intrusiveness, cyberdating practices, and communication strategies) and level of satisfaction with those relationships. Results showed that participants more often reported online intimacy ( Mmen = 2.49; Mwomen = 2.38) than the negative scales of online quality (mean ranged from .43 to 1.50), and all the online quality scales decreased with age (correlations ranged from –.12 to –.30) and relationship length (correlations ranged from –.02 to –.20). Linear regression analyses indicated that online intimacy ( b = .32, p = .001) and intrusiveness ( b = .11, p = .035) were positively related to relationship satisfaction, while cyberdating practices ( b = –.20, p = .001) and communication strategies ( b = –.34, p = .001) were negatively correlated with relationship satisfaction. Moreover, gender and relationship length moderated some of these associations. Results indicate that while online quality and relationship satisfaction are related, the impact of different online quality dimensions on relationship satisfaction differs depending on a participant’s sex, age, and relationship length.
... More numerous studies have analyzed the relation between negative online relationship dynamics and different indicators of romantic relationship quality in young adults. For instance, online jealousy-understood as an emotional reaction prompted by visualizing online relationship content-and concerns and suspicions about a partner's interest in someone else ( Utz & Beukeboom, 2011) have been associated with low levels of relationship satisfaction ( Elphinston & Noller, 2011), and predict involvement in psychological ( Strawhun, Adams, & Huss, 2013) and physical abuse ( Sánchez, Muñoz, Nocentini, Ortega-Ruiz, & Menesini, 2014). On the other hand, research on online intrusive behavior, which is understood as repeated, obsessive attempts to initiate contact and communicate with a partner after a break-up or fight, has revealed that people who exhibit these behaviors in online mode do so offline too ( Spitzberg & Hoobler, 2002;Strawhun et al., 2013), and has linked it to low relationship quality ( Lavy, Mikulincer, Shaver, & Gillath, 2009) as well as intimate partner abuse ( Sánchez et al., 2014;Strawhun et al., 2013). ...
... For instance, online jealousy-understood as an emotional reaction prompted by visualizing online relationship content-and concerns and suspicions about a partner's interest in someone else ( Utz & Beukeboom, 2011) have been associated with low levels of relationship satisfaction ( Elphinston & Noller, 2011), and predict involvement in psychological ( Strawhun, Adams, & Huss, 2013) and physical abuse ( Sánchez, Muñoz, Nocentini, Ortega-Ruiz, & Menesini, 2014). On the other hand, research on online intrusive behavior, which is understood as repeated, obsessive attempts to initiate contact and communicate with a partner after a break-up or fight, has revealed that people who exhibit these behaviors in online mode do so offline too ( Spitzberg & Hoobler, 2002;Strawhun et al., 2013), and has linked it to low relationship quality ( Lavy, Mikulincer, Shaver, & Gillath, 2009) as well as intimate partner abuse ( Sánchez et al., 2014;Strawhun et al., 2013). Regarding online monitoring or control, that is, surveillance and tracking of the partner's online activity ( Tokunaga, 2011), results have been inconclusive. ...
Article
Full-text available
El presente estudio supone una primera aproximación descriptiva al estudio de la calidad de las relaciones sentimentales adolescentes y a la presencia de comportamientos violentos en estas primeras relaciones. Se entrevistó a 446 adolescentes de Educación Secundaria Obligatoria y Bachillerato (47.50% chicos, 52.50% chicas, edad media 16.08 años) con relación a la satisfacción con sus relaciones de pareja, sus expectativas, grado de comunicación, presencia de conflictos, poder, comportamiento transgresivo y violencia. Los resultados descriptivos han mostrado que las relaciones de pareja durante la adolescencia son muy importantes para los chicos y chicas dado que el 90% de éstos afirmaron haber tenido alguna experiencia sentimental. Los adolescentes afirmaron estar muy satisfechos con sus relaciones sentimentales, siendo las chicas y los participantes de mayor edad los que más satisfacción y expectativas mostraron. Con relación a los problemas de violencia, los resultados han mostrado una implicación alta, aunque ocasional, de chicos y chicas con independencia de la edad y del comportamiento analizado: agresión y victimización.
... Research shows that American, Irish, and Indian participants express more behavioural and emotional jealousy than Thai participants (Croucher et al., 2012). Furthermore, studies suggest that online intrusion and the manifestation of jealousy through the partner's behaviour in social networks is the main predictor of both types of aggression (physical and psychological), observed in both Italians and Spaniards (Sánchez et al., 2014). Caridade et al. (2020) in a sample of 173 Portuguese adolescents and young adults, found that cyber dating abuse (CDA) usually appears in the context of jealousy, which also explains control CDA and CDA direct aggression. ...
... Furthermore, this article adds to the prior studies that have been performed on non-American samples (e.g. German; Degen and Kleeberg-Niepage, 2021;German and Danish;Degen and Kleeberg-Niepage, 2022;Spain and Spain-Italy;Sánchez et al., 2014Sánchez et al., , 2017 and enables the further validation of the results obtained in prior studies that utilized U.S. Western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic (WEIRD) samples to counteract this U.S.-centric sampling bias in psychological research recently recognized by researchers (e.g. Cheon et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
The current article presents the results of interviews with 30 Polish never-married singles (14 women and 16 men) aged 20–43 years. Four themes were identified: (1) ambiguous perception of the usefulness of dating services as a means to search for a romantic partner, (2) acquiring skills in using dating services, (3) personal difficulties and failures in using dating services, and (4) online dating as self-obligation. The participants positioned themselves with regard to dating technology through ambiguous opinions and beliefs about the usefulness of dating services. They perceived using dating services to be a task that requires skills in self-commodification and self-branding and a good opportunity to gain practice in dating; they experienced various difficulties related to using dating platforms, and sometimes they reported a feeling of self-obligation to use such services. Single adults related to dating technology in various modes in the context of their singlehood and relationship desires.
... In addition, findings have also indicated that jealousy is related to the quality of the relationship (Barelds & Barelds-Dijkstra, 2007). Align with this, jealousy is associated with a greater number of conflicts (Harris & Darby, 2010;Perles, San Mart ın, & Canto, 2016;Perles, San Mart ın, Canto, & Moreno, 2011), and aggressions (Ashton, Graham-Kevan, & Archer, 2008;Harris & Darby, 2010;S anchez, Muñoz, Nocentini, Ortega-Ru ız, & Menesini, 2014). ...
Article
Full-text available
Nowadays, control over one's partner is easily maintained through social networks, such as Facebook. The aim of this study was to analyze the factors associated with Facebook-jealousy. We examined a total sample of 1144 individuals distributed as follows: data from Spain (n = 393), Colombia (n = 600), and Ecuador (n = 151 individuals), with ages ranging from 14 to 42 years old. All participants held nationality from one of the respective countries, were currently or had been enrolled in a relationship, and both the participant and his/her/their partner also had a Facebook account. Participants completed an online survey with self-reported measures to evaluate: self-esteem, partner conflicts and their strategies to cope with them, romantic jealousy, and Facebook jealousy. Results show that the propensity to experience jealousy in the relationship and low self-esteem are related with more Facebook jealousy across the three countries. For both, Spain and Colombia, strategies to cope with partner conflicts are also associated with Facebook jealousy, in particular lower levels of constructive strategies and higher dominance are associated with greater Facebook jealousy. In short, Facebook jealousy represents another way to manifest jealousy that is influenced by both personal and relationship variables.
Article
The present study focuses on understanding the online aggression in adolescent couples from a gender-sensitive perspective. Specifically, the aims were: (1) to analyze the direct relationship between online jealousy and online control among adolescent boys and girls and (2) to explore the moderating role of moral disengagement and socio-emotional competence in the association between online jealousy and online control by gender. The sample comprised 1,160 high school students (52.7% girls) aged between 12 and 17 years ( M = 14.25, SD = 1.35). The study design was cross-sectional. The moderation analysis revealed that online jealousy was directly related to the perpetration of online control. Moral disengagement strengthened the relationship between online jealousy and online control among both boys and girls, while socio-emotional competence weakened it, but only among girls. The results are discussed considering the gender differences found and the practical implications for programs designed to promote healthy dating relationships.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Understanding the specific risk profile for distinct forms of dating aggression (DA) is very informative to define cross-cutting interventions. The study aims to evaluate whether specific profiles of risk defined using a person-oriented approach predicted physical, sexual, and psychological DA after 6 months. Methods Eight hundred sixty-six Spanish adolescents were interviewed at two time points (50.5% male; average age = 15.04). Latent profile analysis at T1 was used to delineate profiles of individual and relational risk. Results A three-class model best represents the data: a “normative” class ( N = 768; 88%); a “highly aggressive” class characterized by acceptance of violent norms, bullying behaviors, and anger dysregulation ( N = 13, 1.5%); a “jealous-conflictual” class characterized by cognitive and emotional jealousy, negative couple quality, and anger dysregulation ( N = 85, 10%). Controlling for age, sex, and longitudinal stability, physical DA was predicted significantly by the “highly aggressive” profile ( β = .11; p < .05), psychological DA by the “jealous-conflictual” profile ( β = .16; p < .01), and sexual DA by the “jealous-conflictual” ( β = .20; p < .001) and “highly aggressive” profile as a trend ( β = .08; p = .071). Conclusions Specific risk profiles differentially predict risk for physical, sexual, and psychological DA perpetration. A general aggressive pattern predicts physical DA and sexual DA weakly, whereas psychological and sexual DA are associated with a couple of risks, where the dimension of jealousy, control, and conflict characterizes the dynamic between partners. Policy Implications Findings suggested that physical DA, and at a lower level sexual DA, should be prevented using cross-cutting strategies on general aggression. Psychological and sexual DA might require more contextually based interventions.
Article
This study examined the prevalence of cyber-aggression in Spanish adolescent couples as well as the common and differential predictors for cyber-aggression and psychological aggression using a short-term longitudinal study. Over a 6-month period, six hundred and thirty-two (632) Spanish adolescents with romantic relationship experience from seven schools were randomly selected to participate in the study (51% male; average age = 15.03). The results revealed a prevalence of cyber-aggression of 13% and that 68.3% of adolescents engaged in psychological aggression. Girls were significantly more involved than boys in both forms. The analysis of predictors for cyber and psychological aggression showed that these two forms of aggression shared a common factor, negative couple quality. Furthermore, cognitive empathy predicted cyber-aggression whereas anger regulation and jealousy predicted psychological aggression. These results highlighted the need to consider the particular characteristics of each setting, face-to-face and online, for designing future prevention programs.
Article
Nonostante il crescente interesse pubblico rispetto al coinvolgimento di adolescenti in comportamenti di sexting, la ricerca su questo tema è agli albori e gli studi sono principalmente di natura esplorativa. La presente review mira a delineare un quadro delle evidenze esistenti in letteratura riguardo il sexting in adolescenza. I risultati mostrano variabilità nella diffusione del fenomeno tra i diversi paesi e l’associazione con variabili demografiche. Emerge anche che il sexting in adolescenza è associato a diversi comportamenti a rischio. È inoltre analizzato il ruolo giocato da fattori individuali, sociali e culturali. Infine il sexting sembra assumere funzioni diverse in relazione ai significati e alle motivazioni: per gli adolescenti esso può essere una nuova forma di esplorazione sessuale o può condurre a conseguenze negative come il grooming e la pornografia.
Article
Full-text available
An examination of the influence of self-efficacy regarding anger management on psychological and physical dating aggression using an agentic perspective of individual functioning. The investigation applied both the individual perspective (Study 1) and the interactional perspective (Study 2). The sample comprised 470 Italian young adults (223 females) (mean age across genders = 19.10; ds = 1.30) in study 1, and 62 couples in study 2 (mean age for males = 22.34; ds = 2.59; mean age for females = 19.58; ds = 1.50). The first study found that individuals' efficacy regarding anger management affect dysfunctional behaviors toward the partner via couple conflict. The second study found that one partner's efficacy regarding anger management affected couple conflict, which in turn affected the other partner's psychological aggression. Results are discussed within an agentic framework of human development, where young adult partners are proactive agents of their own and their partners' behaviors, contributing actively to their intimate relationship adjustment rather than just re-acting to their partners' behaviors.
Article
Full-text available
Although theories of romantic stage development suggest that youth in the period of emerging adulthood are fully capable of commitment to an intimate romantic relationship, recent research suggests that the relationships of many young people are quite different. Marriage and other forms of deep commitment are delayed while many youth engage in short-term casual encounters or in noncommitted relationships. In this article, we suggest that these data pose a challenge to stage theories that can be reconciled by considering the developmental life tasks that emerging adults must simultaneously resolve. We propose a transitional emerging adult romantic stage, coordinating romance and life plans, in which young people strive to integrate their career paths and life plans with those of a romantic partner. Resolution of this stage provides the grounding for long-term commitment to a life partner. This proposal is discussed within the perspective of life cycle and evolutionary life history theories. © 2013 Society for the Study of Emerging Adulthood and SAGE Publications.
Article
Full-text available
Hook-up behavior consists of sexual activity between uncommitted individuals. “Hooking up” has become pervasive among young adults, particularly on college campuses in the United States, despite inherent emotional, physical, social and health risks. A total of 507 undergraduate students were surveyed to assess the prevalence of, and motivations for, hook-up behavior. Of those students, 64% reported having ever engaged in a hook-up. Of those who had hooked up, 51% had done so with the intention of initiating a traditional romantic relationship; there were no sex differences. Contrary to established notions of uncommitted sexual activity as an evolved behavioral strategy, results are discussed from a synergistic biopsychosocial perspective that situates this complex sexual behavior at the intersection of evolution, basic biology, psychology, and novel cultural factors. A decreasing average age at menarche and increasing age of mother at first birth has created a window of sexually mature but pre-reproductive life that is historically unprecedented in its duration. We suggest this time window, paired with evolved neural correlates of sexual and reproductive behavior, greater acceptance of the social sexualization of youth, and characteristics of the modern collegiate environment, results in the increasing prevalence of hook-ups. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2014 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
National and international studies have pointed out Conflict Tactics Scale (CTS, Straus, 1979, 1996) as one of the most widely used measures for assessing the strategies used in situations of conflict within young couples. Nevertheless, there is not any conclusive result about its structure. Especially the physical dating violence scale has undergone several structural analyses providing monofactorial and bifactorial structures. The aim of this study was focusing on the validation of structural models using confirmatory factorial analysis (CFA) of CTS within adolescent couples, considering the differences between boys and girls and between aggressors and victims. 3258 adolescents, aged 15-21, were selected using a stratified random sample and interviewed using an adaptation of the CTS questionnaire. The results pointed out that it is not possible to identify a single model fit, but boys and girls, aggressor and victims, have the same pattern: a bifactorial model which establishes different but correlated moderate aggressive behaviors and severe aggressive behaviors. These results are discussed in terms of the potential value of this two-factor structure for understanding the phenomenon.
Article
Full-text available
Prevention programs for dating partner aggression should be based on knowledge about when such aggression starts and how it changes. Given the very few studies regarding such trends, changes in physical, psychological, and sexual aggression against dating partners were examined in 14- to 20-year-old Spanish high school students (N = 2,016). Overall, psychological aggression increased linearly, whereas physical aggression had a negative quadratic association, peaking at 16 to 17 years for males and females. Sexual aggression was infrequent, but it spiked at age 16 for males. Although physical aggression diminished in late adolescence, injuries as a consequence of such aggression increased linearly for females, and they were also significantly higher for females (14.9%) than for males (3%) at ages 18 to 20 years. The findings support the hypothesis that dating physical aggression for males and females peaks during middle-to-late adolescence and shows a similar developmental pattern to other antisocial and criminal behaviors. Prevention of dating aggression, escalation of such aggression, and prevention of injury should consider developmental trends in dating aggression.
Article
This study examined the links between children's perceptions of the manner in which they and their parents adjust their relationships during early adolescence and early adolescents' orientation toward parents and peers. A sample of 1,771 children completed self-report questionnaires during the spring of their 6th and 7th grades. As predicted, early adolescents who believed their parents asserted and did not relax their power and restrictiveness were higher in an extreme form of peer orientation. Also as predicted, those who perceived few opportunities to be involved in decision making, as well as no increase in these opportunities, were higher in both extreme peer orientation and peer advice seeking. Discussion focuses on the importance for parent-child relationships to adjust to early adolescents' changing developmental needs, as well as the implications of early adolescent peer orientation for later development.
Article
This article argues that there are two distinct forms of couple violence taking place within families in the United States and other Western countries. A review of evidence from large-sample survey research and from qualitative and quantitative data gathered from women's shelters suggests that some families suffer from occasional outbursts of violence from either husbands or wives (common couple violence), while other families are terrorized by systematic male violence (patriarchal terrorism). It is argued that the distinction between common couple violence and patriarchal terrorism is important because it has implications for the implementation of public policy, the development of educational programs and intervention strategies, and the development of theories of interpersonal violence.
Article
In response to calls to recognize that both face‐to‐face (FtF) interaction and technologically mediated communication (TMC) often occur in the same relationships, we introduce a communicative interdependence perspective with a central tenet that relational closeness is associated with interconnections among modes of communication. We examined this perspective with a focus group study (N = 17) and a more extensive survey (N = 317). Consistent with the communicative interdependence predictions, relational closeness was associated positively with integration between FtF and TMC and negatively to difficulties transitioning between modes. Also, discussing topics only via technologically mediated channels was inversely associated with closeness and satisfaction, whereas discussing topics only in person was positively related to closeness and satisfaction.
Article
Because the first antistalking statute was enacted in California in 1990, stalking research has been expanded immensely, yet been largely confined to exploring traditional pursuit tactics. This study instead examined the prevalence and correlates of cyberstalking behaviors while examining the phenomenon in a more inclusive manner than previous studies focusing on cyberstalking by including social networking avenues. In addition to a measure assessing cyberstalking-related behaviors, questionnaires assessing pathological aspects of personality, including attachment style, interpersonal jealousy, interpersonal violence, and anger were also provided to participants. Results indicate that, given preliminary evidence, cyberstalking-related behaviors are related to past measures of traditional stalking and cyberstalking, although prior attachment, jealousy, and violence issues within relationships are significant predictors of cyberstalking-related behaviors. In addition, unexpected gender differences emerged. For example, women admitted greater frequencies of cyberstalking perpetration than males, signaling that further research on frequency and motivation for cyberstalking among the sexes is necessary.