Content uploaded by Timothy Levine
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Timothy Levine on Jun 01, 2015
Content may be subject to copyright.
This article was downloaded by: [University of Alabama at Birmingham]
On: 01 June 2015, At: 06:23
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered
office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK
Communication Research Reports
Publication details, including instructions for authors and
subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rcrr20
Teenagers Lie a Lot: A Further
Investigation into the Prevalence of
Lying
Timothy R. Levine
a
, Kim B. Serota
b
, Frankie Carey
c
& Doug
Messer
d
a
School of Media and Communication , Korea University , Seoul ,
Republic of Korea
b
Department of Management and Marketing , Oakland University
c
University of Miami
d
Syracuse University
Published online: 12 Jul 2013.
To cite this article: Timothy R. Levine , Kim B. Serota , Frankie Carey & Doug Messer (2013)
Teenagers Lie a Lot: A Further Investigation into the Prevalence of Lying, Communication Research
Reports, 30:3, 211-220, DOI: 10.1080/08824096.2013.806254
To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08824096.2013.806254
PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE
Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the
“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,
our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to
the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions
and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,
and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content
should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources
of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,
proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or
howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising
out of the use of the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any
substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,
systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &
Teenagers Lie a Lot: A Further
Investigation into the Prevalence
of Lying
Timothy R. Levine, Kim B. Serota, Frankie Carey, &
Doug Messer
Although it is commonly believed that lying is ubiquitous, recent findings show large,
individual differences in lying, and that the proclivity to lie varies by age. This research
surveyed 58 high school students, who were asked how often they had lied in the past
24 hr. It was predicted that high school students would report lying with greater fre-
quency than previous surveys with college student and adult samples, but that the dis-
tribution of reported lies by high school students would exhibit a strongly and
positively skewed distribution similar to that observed with college student and adult
samples. The data were consistent with both predictions. High school students in the
sample reported telling, on average, 4.1 lies in the past 24 hr—a rate that is 75% higher
than that reported by college students and 150% higher than that reported by a nation-
wide sample of adults. The data were also skewed, replicating the ‘‘few prolific liar’’ effect
previously documented in college student and adult samples.
Keywords: Deception; Lying; Prevalence Of Lies; Teenagers
Despite the large literature on lying and deceptive communication, relatively
little research has sought to answer the question of how often people lie. Instead,
most deception research takes questions of prevalence for granted, presuming that
deception is commonplace and ubiquitous.
Timothy R. Levine (PhD, Michigan State University, 1992) is a professor in the School of Media and
Communication at Korea University, Seoul, Republic of Korea. Kim B. Serota (PhD, Michigan State
University, 2011) is a visiting professor in the Department of Management and Marketing at Oakland
University. Frankie Carey is an undergraduate student at the University of Miami. Doug Messer is an
undergraduate student at Syracuse University. Correspondence: Timothy R. Levine, School of Media and
Communication, 607 Media Hall, Korea University, Seoul, Republic of Korea; E-mail: levinet111@gmail.com
Communication Research Reports
Vol. 30, No. 3, July–September 2013, pp. 211–220
ISSN 0882-4096 (print)/ISSN 1746-4099 (online) # 2013 Eastern Communication Association
DOI: 10.1080/08824096.2013.806254
Downloaded by [University of Alabama at Birmingham] at 06:23 01 June 2015
This presumption of ubiquity is problematic for at least two reasons. First, good
science requires a rich descriptive understanding of the phenomenon under study as
a prerequisite to sound theory building and experimental work (Rosin, 2001).
Clearly, knowledge of prevalence would be part of such a necessary descriptive under-
standing. A lack of such knowledge suggests a shaky foundation on which to build.
Second, the presumption of ubiquity may simply lack correspondence with existing
data. Recent research on the prevalence of deception (Serota, Levine, & Boster, 2010;
Serota, Levine, & Burns, 2012) suggests (a) large individual differences in how often
people lie, (b) that most people do not lie with great frequency, (c) that the distri-
bution of lying is not normally distributed across the population, rendering the arith-
metic mean number of lies misleading, and (d) that the prevalence of lying varies
over the lifespan of humans, making college student samples non-representative of
other age groups. What the existing data suggest is that most lies are told by a
‘‘few prolific liars’’ and that prevalence declines with age (Serota et al., 2010).
This research seeks to answer two related questions. First, previous research reports a
negative association between age and the prevalence of lying (DePaulo, Kashy, Kirkendol,
Wyer, & Epstein, 1996; Serota et al., 2010; Serota et al., 2012). College students lie with
greater frequency than adults and younger adults lie more than older adults. This
research questions if that trend extends backward with even greater frequency among
high school students. Second, previous research reports large and nonnormally distrib-
uted individual differences in the prevalence of lying. This research investigates the dis-
tribution of lying among a sample of teenagers. In short, this research tests the hypotheses
that teenagers lie with greater frequency than college students or older adults, but that the
presence of a distribution dominated by a few prolific liars holds across age groups.
Previous Research on Lie Prevalence
Most research on deception prevalence has used one of two methodological strate-
gies: experiments or self-report. Experimental work puts people in situations where
lies might be prompted and observes the proportion of people who lie. The limitation
in such research is that the results are context bound. The alternative is self-report,
either through diaries or survey methods. The limitation is that self-report work
may result in underreporting stemming from social desirability bias.
As an example of experimental research, Levine, Kim, and Hamel (2010) put part-
icipants in situations where they either did or did not have a motive to lie. They
found that lacking motive, virtually all participants were honest. However, in various
deception motive conditions, approximately two-thirds of participants lied. Feldman,
Forrest, and Happ (2002) had participants interact with a stranger. They found that
more than 60% of participants lied in a 10-min interaction, and that lies were more
frequent for participants who were instructed to make a positive impression than
those in a control condition. Furthermore, a reanalysis by Serota et al. (2010) of
the Feldman et al. findings found that across 121 participants, 26% of the participants
accounted for 72% of the lies, whereas 49 participants told no lies at all. Together,
experimental research on deception prevalence suggests that there are situations
212 T. R. Levine et al.
Downloaded by [University of Alabama at Birmingham] at 06:23 01 June 2015
which reliably prompt deception, but not everyone lies, even in situations where
there is reason to lie.
More widely known and often cited is the self-report work on deception
prevalence. Of that body of work, a diary study by DePaulo et al. (1996) has been
the most influential. DePaulo et al. found that, on average, college students reported
telling two lies per day, and non-student adults reported one lie per day. Replications
by George and Robb (2008) and Hancock, Thom-Santelli, and Ritchie (2004) yielded
estimates for college students ranging from 0.6 to 1.6 lies per day.
Most recently, Serota et al. (2010) conducted a survey with a representative sample
of 1,000 adults in the United States. They replicated the DePaulo et al. (1996) finding
that, on average, people 18 and older tell between one and two lies per day average,
but also found that the distribution was radically skewed, thus rendering the mean
misleading. Sixty percent of adults reported telling no lies in the past 24 hr, and
50% of all reported lies were told by just 5% of the sample. Serota et al. (2010)
obtained and reanalyzed the student datasets from DePaulo et al. and George and
Robb (2008) and found similarly skewed distributions. Finally, in a third study,
Serota et al. (2010) reported college student data, finding that students reported more
lies than the representative adult sample, but that the positive skewed distribution
was again evident. Studies by Cole (2001), Ennis, Vrij, and Chance (2008), and Horan
and Booth-Butterfield (2011), which measured both age and lying frequency, indi-
cated that various individual differences may alter the rate of lying but, consistent
with Serota et al. (2010) and Serota et al. (2012), incidences of lying by young adults
are generally high.
Research Focus and Hypotheses
Although research on the prevalence of lying is scarce relative to research on topics
such as deception detection, previous research provides a coherent picture of decep-
tion prevalence in American adult and college student populations. Most striking are
the individual differences. For most people, lying is an infrequent activity. But, there
are a few prolific liars. The result is that the distribution of the frequency of lying
among liars is positively skewed, and can be modeled as a power function that holds
across college student and adult samples and across survey, diary, and experimental
methods (Serota et al., 2010). When including non-liars, the model can be further
refined using a combination of Poisson and power distributions to separate everyday
and prolific liars (Serota et al., 2012).
Further, age appears to be a reliable predictor of frequency of lying. Older people
appear to lie less often than their younger counterparts. The age effect showed up
reliably in the difference between student and adult samples in both the DePaulo
et al. (1996) and Serota et al. (2010) studies, and age was a significant predictor of
the rate of lying in Serota et al.’s nationwide adult sample (b ¼0.18).
What is undocumented is if general trends of the nonnormal distribution and
increasing prevalence with younger samples extend to adolescent populations.
Nevertheless, we expect this trend will hold. There are good reasons to believe that
Communication Research Reports 213
Downloaded by [University of Alabama at Birmingham] at 06:23 01 June 2015
high school students will lie more frequently than college students. One reason is
simply moral development. Lying is generally socially disapproved and is a morally
questionable act (Bok, 1999). Foundational research by developmental psychologists
such as Piaget and Kohlberg indicates that morality develops with age (Crain, 1985;
Piaget, 1932). Therefore, one might expect younger people to lie more. For example,
Jensen, Arnett, Feldman, and Cauffman (2002) found that college students rated aca-
demic dishonesty as less acceptable than did high school students. A similar finding
might be expected for lying.
Adolescence is also a time when individuals seek to establish autonomy from
parents, and lying to parents may be a means to covertly establish such autonomy
(Jensen, Arnett, Feldman, & Cauffman, 2004). Jensen et al. (2004) found that high
school students lie to parents more often than college students. Therefore, there
are good reasons, and some supportive empirical data, to suggest that high school
students might lie more than older people.
Although it is expected that mean levels of lying will be statistically higher for this
sample of high school students than for previous samples of college students or
non-student adults, it is also expected that the skewed distribution observed in pre-
vious studies will be evident in data from younger participants. Serota et al. (2010)
showed that the power function for the distribution of lies was robust across previous
datasets. We expect the findings regarding the shape of the distribution to replicate
because positive skew is typical in a variety of socially undesirable behaviors, not just
deception (Serota et al., 2010).
To summarize, these research predictions can be described with two hypotheses:
H1: The average number of lies per day observed in a sample of high school students
will be higher than that observed in previous studies sampling college students
and adults.
H2: The distribution of the number of lies told by high school students will not be
normally distributed around the mean. Instead, the distribution of liars will
(a) have a strong positive skew and (b) fit a power function similar to those
described by Serota et al. (2010).
Method
Participants
The data were collected in class from 58 high school students at a suburban New York
high school. The sample was evenly split between males and females. Participants’
ages ranged from 14 to 17 years old (M ¼ 15.45, SD ¼ 0.81), with a slight majority
of the sample being 15 years of age. All the students were enrolled in a class where
students gained college credit for conducting university professor-mentored science
projects, and the data were collect as part of one such project. The data collection
was institutional review board approved at both the high school and the supervising
professor’s university. Demographic data were collected separately from the lie preva-
lence data to maintain anonymity.
214 T. R. Levine et al.
Downloaded by [University of Alabama at Birmingham] at 06:23 01 June 2015
Measurement
The instructions and survey question format was identical to that used in previous
research (Serota et al., 2010) except that the survey was done with paper and pencil,
rather than online. Participants were told that the research was about lies in everyday
communication, and they were provided with a definition of lying. Lying was defined
as intentionally misleading another person. It was explained that lies can be big or
small, that lies can be completely false statements or subtle omissions, and that lies
can be told for a variety of reasons. Participants were asked how many times they
had lied in the past 24 hr, and were provided with a grid to complete. The grid
crossed the face-to-face or mediated (writing, phone, Internet, etc.) message format
with the target of the lie (family, friends, coworkers, acquaintances, and strangers).
Participants were asked to write in how many lies they had told in each of the 10
categories, and to write in a ‘‘zero’’ if no lies of a particular type were told.
Results
The high school students in this sample reported, on average, telling M ¼ 4.1 lies in
the past 24 hr (SD ¼ 3.62, Mdn ¼ 3.0, mode ¼ 2.0, minimum ¼ 0.0, maximum ¼ 17.0,
and 95% confidence interval [CI] ¼ 3.15–5.06 lies per day). The mean was statistically
greater than the means reported by Serota et al. (2010) for college students (M ¼ 2.34,
SD ¼ 2.94; N ¼ 225), t(281) ¼ 3.86, p < .01; and adults (M ¼ 1.65, SD ¼ 4.45;
N ¼ 998), t(1,054) ¼ 4.11, p < .01. Thus, the data were consistent with H1. Means
and 95% CIs for the three groups are visually depicted in Figure 1.
The data were substantially positively skewed (skew ¼þ1.88, standard error of
skew ¼ 0.31) and Kolmogorov–Smirnov and Shapiro–Wilk tests for normality
showed statistically significant and substantial deviation from normality at
p < .0001, as did the visual examination of Quantile–Quantile plots. Curve fitting
found that a power function (y ¼ 4.3316
x
^
1.4647) fit the distribution.
1
Positive
skews and significant deviations from normality were also evident for face-to-face
lies, mediated lies, and lies told to all targets. In all, the top 10% of liars accounted
for 33% of the lies and 50% of the reported lies were told by 29% of the sample. Thus,
the data were consistent with H2, which predicted nonnormally distributed results. A
plot of this curve and a comparison with curve fitting of the Serota et al. (2010) adult
and college student samples are provided in Figure 2.
The participants reported telling more face-to-face lies (M ¼ 2.66) than mediated
lies (M ¼ 1.45), t(57) ¼ 3.55, p < .001. There was a nonsignificant trend toward tell-
ing more lies to friends (M ¼ 2.03) than family members (M ¼ 1.41), t(57) ¼ 1.84,
p ¼ .07 (two-tailed). By far, more lies were told to friends and family than to other
potential targets such as acquaintances or strangers (all ts > 5.0, p < .001).
Addition Data on Teenage Liars
As a check on the validity of the results reported in this study, the authors compared
teenager and adult results from the U.K. data analyzed by Serota et al. (2012). That
Communication Research Reports 215
Downloaded by [University of Alabama at Birmingham] at 06:23 01 June 2015
Figure 1 Comparison of Results With Serota, Levine, and Boster (2010) findings for college students and
adults. Note. CI = confidence interval.
Figure 2 Similar Power Functions Observed in the Data and Serota, Levine, and Boster (2010). Note. Intercept
values are inversely related to the means. The similarity of the slope values indicates that the distribution of
behaviors is similar across samples.
216 T. R. Levine et al.
Downloaded by [University of Alabama at Birmingham] at 06:23 01 June 2015
study only reports results for adults (18 years and older) but the survey, which was
conducted for the London Science Museum, obtained the same data from parti-
cipants aged 16 and 17 years (cf. Serota et al., 2012, for a complete description of
the methodology and data weighting).
Participants 16 to 17 years old reported telling M ¼ 3.82 lies per day (SD ¼ 3.73;
unweighted n ¼ 122), whereas those 18 years and older reported M ¼ 2.08 lies per
day (SD ¼ 3.57; unweighted n ¼ 2,981). The teenage sample reports telling significant
more lies than adults, t(3,006) ¼ 2.57, p ¼ .01.
Discussion
This research investigated the prevalence of lying among a sample of high school
students. It was predicted that (a) high school students lie more often than college
students or adults, but (b) the nonnormal, positive skew observed in college student
and adult lie prevalence data would also be observed in the distribution of high
school student lies. The data were consistent with both hypotheses.
Relatively speaking, high school students lie a lot. On average, the high school
students in this sample reported telling 4.10 lies in the past 24 hr. Previous research
with an identical question format found means of 2.34 and 1.65 lies per day for col-
lege students and adults, respectively. These means reflect 75% and 150% increases
over previously reported means, respectively. A comparison with similar data from
the United Kingdom provides convergent validity; U.K. teens reported telling 84%
more lies than U.K. adults.
The few prolific liar pattern was also observed in the distribution of these high
school student lies. The positive skew was six times the standard error, and a power
function curve fit the data. As with other lie prevalence datasets, most participants
reported lie frequency below the mean (71% in these data), and most lies were told
by a relative few high-frequency lairs.
As previously observed, the existence of the strong positive skew renders the mean
misleading. In the Serota et al. (2010) adult nation sample, although the average was
1.65 lies per day, 60% of the sample reported no lies at all. The mode was also zero in
the Serota et al. (2010) college student data. Although the college student mean was
2.34 lies per day, 29% of the students reported telling no lies in the past 24 hr. In these
high school data, only 3 of 58 participants (5%) reported telling no lies at all. The
mode (26% of the sample) was two lies per day, and 52% of the sample reported tell-
ing one to three lies. Clearly, the mean does not reflect the average participant. How-
ever, whereas most adults report no lies in the past 24 hr, most teenagers do report a
few lies, albeit fewer than suggested by the mean.
Interesting questions raised by these findings include why high school students
report lying more than college students and adults and why the prevalence of decep-
tion is nonnormally distributed. It is suspected that there is more than one answer to
the first question. As speculated previously, part of the answer likely involves cogni-
tive, emotional, and moral maturity. Because lying is socially disapproved, there are
long-term social sanctions for becoming known as a liar. Younger people may more
Communication Research Reports 217
Downloaded by [University of Alabama at Birmingham] at 06:23 01 June 2015
often opt for the short-term advantage that can be gained through deception, whereas
older people may see the long-term benefits of avoiding socially disapproved
behavior.
It was also speculated that adolescents lie to parents as a means of establishing
autonomy. Some evidence consistent with this speculation was obtained. Students
reported telling an average of 1.41 lies per day to family members (34% of their total
lies). Presumably, many of those lies were told to parents. However, lies to parents are
insufficient to explain teenage lying because teens also lied frequently to friends (2.03
lies per day; 50% of their total lies). Notably, teenagers reported telling more lies to
people they know than to strangers. This is in direct opposition to the Ennis et al.
(2008) study of individual differences, which found that adult students, 18 to 44 years
(M ¼ 23.1 years), told more lies to strangers. This is fertile ground for future explo-
ration, but the teenagers’ greater tendency to test credulity with friends and family is
consistent with developmental psychologists’ views on moral growth and learning.
Also in need of explanation is the robust observation of nonnormally distributed
individual differences. At a macrolevel, lying must be infrequent otherwise it would
not be effective in achieving deception, and if it were ineffective, there would be little
point in lying. If everyone lied about everything, it would make no sense to believe
others, and people would not be fooled. So, for lying to function, most people must
believe others and that belief must be functional. This provides the few prolific liars
with dupes to fool, but precludes lying from becoming ubiquitous. If lying were the
rule, rather than the exception, then little would be gained from either lying or honest
communication because little trust in others’ words would be warranted.
Lie prevalence data has important implications for deception detection experi-
ments. Deception detection experiments report that people are only slightly better
than chance at correctly distinguishing truths from lies (Bond & DePaulo, 2006).
However, in most experiments leading to this conclusion, (a) participants judge an
equal number of truths and lies, (b) participants are truth-biased and guess truth
more often than lie, and (c) accuracy is calculated as an average across truths and lies
(Levine, Park, & McCornack, 1999). As a consequence, the truth–lie base rate makes
a critical difference (Levine, Kim, Park, & Hughes, 2006). If most people lie
infrequently and if most people tend to believe others most of the time, then detec-
tion accuracy outside the lab will be underestimated by deception detection experi-
ments that present equal proportions of truths and lies.
As with all self-report data on sensitive topics, concern exists with the accuracy of
reporting. Presumably, the anonymity of the data collection helps in obtaining accu-
rate responses as does the carefully created instructions (for a more detailed descrip-
tion and discussion, see Serota et al., 2010). Nevertheless, these levels of reported lies
may be underestimates. It is unlikely, however, that social desirability biases explain
the key findings that (a) younger people lie more often than older people, and (b)
lying is nonnormally distributed in the population.
Social desirability may affect responses on surveys, but it also affects behavior. If
older people find lying less socially desirable than teenagers, then they will report
fewer lies, but they are also likely, in actuality, to tell fewer lies.
218 T. R. Levine et al.
Downloaded by [University of Alabama at Birmingham] at 06:23 01 June 2015
In conclusion, teenagers lie a lot (relative to college students and old adults). However,
a few teens lie a l ot more than most teens. These results provide initial evidence that the
negative correlation between deception prevalence and age extends backward to high
school students. The finding that most lies are told by a few prolific liars is as evident
among high school students as it is among college students and adults.
Note
[1] To fit a power function, it is necessary to exclude those participants who told no lies, as a
power curve requires all positive values of x. Therefore, the prevalence curves are plotted
for liars only. In this research, there are a small number of high school students who reported
telling one lie; relative to the mean of 4.1 lies, this is nearly the equivalent of telling no lies at
all. To improve the goodness of fit, those telling one lie were treated as non-liars. For the
reported power function, r
2
¼ .900, indicating a strong fit. More important, if those who told
one lie are included, the data still show a pattern, with the majority of lies being told by a few
prolific liars; however, the goodness of fit is weaker, r
2
¼ .426.
References
Bok, S. (1999). Lying: Moral choice in public and private life. New York, NY: Vintage.
Bond, C. F.Jr., & DePaulo, B. M. (2006). Accuracy of deception judgments. Review of Personality
and Social Psychology, 10, 214–234.
Cole, T. (2001). Lying to the one you love: The use of deception in romantic relationships. Journal
of Social and Personal Relationships, 18, 107–129.
Crain, W. C. (1985). Theories of development: Concepts and applications. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice Hall.
DePaulo, B. M., Kashy, D. A., Kirkendol, S. E., Wyer, M. M., & Epstein, J. A. (1996). Lying in
everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 979–995.
Ennis, E., Vrij, A., & Chance, C. (2008). Indvidual differences and lying in everyday life. Journal of
Social and Personal Relationships, 25, 105–118.
Feldman, R. S., Forrest, J. A., & Happ, B. R. (2002). Self-presentation and verbal deception: Do
self-presenters lie more? Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 24, 163–170.
George, J. F., & Robb, A. (2008). Deception and computer-mediated communication in daily life.
Communication Reports, 21, 92–103.
Hancock, J. T., Thom-Santelli, J., & Ritchie, T. (2004). Deception and design: The impact of
communication technology on lying behavior. CHI Letters, 6, 129–134.
Horan, S. M., & Booth-Butterfield, M. (2011). Is it worth lying for? Physiological and emotional
implications of recalling deceptive affection. Human Communication Research, 37, 78–106.
Jensen, L., Arnett, J., Feldman, S., & Cauffman, E. (2002). It’s wrong, but everybody does it:
Academic dishonesty among high school and college students. Contemporary Educational
Psychology, 27, 209–228.
Jensen, L., Arnett, J., Feldman, S., & Cauffman, E. (2004). The right to do wrong: Lying to parents
among adolescents and emerging adults. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 33, 101–112.
Levine, T. R., Kim, R. K., & Hamel, L. M. (2010). People lie for a reason: An experimental test of the
principle of veracity. Communication Research Reports, 24, 271–285.
Levine, T. R., Kim, R. K., Park, H. S., & Hughes, M. (2006). Deception detection accuracy is a
predictable linear function of message veracity base-rate: A formal test of Park and Levine’s
probability model. Communication Monographs, 73, 243–260.
Communication Research Reports 219
Downloaded by [University of Alabama at Birmingham] at 06:23 01 June 2015
Levine, T. R., Park, H. S., & McCornack, S. A. (1999). Accuracy in detecting truths and lies:
Documenting the ‘‘veracity effect.’’. Communication Monographs, 66, 125–144.
Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. London, England: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner,
& Co.
Rozin, P. (2001). Social psychology and science: Some lessons from Solomon Asch. Personality and
Social Psychology Review, 5, 2–14.
Serota, K. B., Levine, T. R., & Boster, F. J. (2010). The prevalence of lying in America: Three studies
of self-reported lies. Human Communication Research, 36, 2–25.
Serota, K. B., Levine, T. R., & Burns, A. (2012, November). A few prolific liars: Variation in the
prevalence of lying. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Communication
Association, Orlando, FL.
220 T. R. Levine et al.
Downloaded by [University of Alabama at Birmingham] at 06:23 01 June 2015