ArticlePDF Available

Participant Roles in Bullying: How Can Peer Bystanders Be Utilized in Interventions?

Authors:

Abstract

This article provides a view of school bullying as a group phenomenon and practical implications stemming from this approach. The motivation for bullying perpetration often relates to one's social standing in the group. Peer bystanders are typically present when bullying takes place, often providing the perpetrators with social rewards. The more such rewards (e.g., laughing, cheering) are present and the less the victimized children are supported and defended, the more likely bullying is maintained in a classroom or a peer group. However, bystanders are not necessarily aware of the consequences of their responses when witnessing bullying, and they may not know how to support and defend vulnerable peers. In interventions aiming to reduce bullying, peer bystanders' awareness of their own role, their empathy toward victimized youth, as well as their self-efficacy related to defending those youth should be enhanced. Intervention evaluations have shown that changing bystander responses to bullying is a fruitful way to reduce bullying and victimization.
This article was downloaded by: [Turku University]
On: 23 February 2015, At: 23:01
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered
office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK
Click for updates
Theory Into Practice
Publication details, including instructions for authors and
subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/htip20
Participant Roles in Bullying: How
Can Peer Bystanders Be Utilized in
Interventions?
Christina Salmivallia
a University of Turku, Finland.
Accepted author version posted online: 31 Jul 2014.Published
online: 15 Oct 2014.
To cite this article: Christina Salmivalli (2014) Participant Roles in Bullying: How Can
Peer Bystanders Be Utilized in Interventions?, Theory Into Practice, 53:4, 286-292, DOI:
10.1080/00405841.2014.947222
To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00405841.2014.947222
PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE
Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the
“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,
our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to
the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions
and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,
and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content
should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources
of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,
proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or
howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising
out of the use of the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any
substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,
systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &
Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-
and-conditions
Downloaded by [Turku University] at 23:01 23 February 2015
Theory Into Practice, 53:286–292, 2014
Copyright © The College of Education and Human Ecology, The Ohio State University
ISSN: 0040-5841 print/1543-0421 online
DOI: 10.1080/00405841.2014.947222
Christina Salmivalli
Participant Roles in Bullying:
How Can Peer Bystanders Be
Utilized in Interventions?
This article provides a view of school bullying as
a group phenomenon and practical implications
stemming from this approach. The motivation
for bullying perpetration often relates to one’s
social standing in the group. Peer bystanders are
typically present when bullying takes place, often
providing the perpetrators with social rewards.
The more such rewards (e.g., laughing, cheering)
are present and the less the victimized children
are supported and defended, the more likely
bullying is maintained in a classroom or a peer
group. However, bystanders are not necessarily
aware of the consequences of their responses
when witnessing bullying, and they may not know
how to support and defend vulnerable peers.
In interventions aiming to reduce bullying, peer
bystanders’ awareness of their own role, their
empathy toward victimized youth, as well as their
self-efficacy related to defending those youth
should be enhanced. Intervention evaluations
have shown that changing bystander responses
to bullying is a fruitful way to reduce bullying
and victimization.
How (and Why) Do Bystanders
Reward Bullying?
‘‘ITS EASY,IT WO RK S,A ND IT makes
me feel good.” The title of an article
by Sutton and colleagues (Sutton, Smith, &
Christina Salmivalli is a professor at the University of
Turku, Finland.
Correspondence should be addressed to Professor
Christina Salmivalli, University of Turku, Psykologian
Iaitos, 20014 Turun Yliopisto, Turku, Finland. E-mail:
tiina.salmivalli@utu.fi.
Swettenham, 2001) captures something essential
about the recent view of bullying: It has a
function for the perpetrator. Bullying is a highly
prevalent phenomenon which often persists over
long periods of time, because “it works”; in other
words, it helps the perpetrator get something that
he or she wants.
To prevent and tackle bullying, one must un-
derstand what a child or an adolescent acquires,
or attempts to acquire, by bullying others. Al-
though the perpetrators of bullying are not all the
same (Peeters, Cillessen, & Scholte, 2010), for
many of them bullying seems to be motivated by
286
Downloaded by [Turku University] at 23:01 23 February 2015
Salmivalli Participant Roles in Bullying
the pursuit of visibility, power, and a high status
in the peer group (Houghton, Nathan, & Tay-
lor, 2012; Salmivalli, 2010; Sijtsema, Veenstra,
Lindenberg, & Salmivalli, 2009; Veenstra et al.,
2007). For that reason, bullies need bystanders,
or spectators. They do not want to attack their
target in a situation where there are no witnesses
around. Rather, they choose a time and place
where other peers (but no adults) are present:
in its very core, bullying is about public abuse
and ridicule of another person. Observational
studies in school playgrounds confirm that this
is the case: in most bullying situations, there is
a group of peers present (Hawkins, Pepler, &
Craig, 2001).
Rather than intervening on behalf of the
victimized peer, many bystanders reinforce the
bully’s behaviors by verbal or nonverbal cues
that are socially rewarding as they signal that
bullying is acceptable, or even funny and enter-
taining (Salmivalli, 2010; Salmivalli, Lagerspetz,
Björkqvist, Österman, & Kaukiainen, 1996). For
instance, bystanders may be laughing or cheer-
ing when the bullying is taking place. Mak-
ing others laugh is already rewarding for the
child doing the bullying, and such a response
from peers is likely to prolong the bullying
episode. Other bystanders are even more active
and assist the bully by catching the targeted
child, or by preventing him or her from es-
caping during episodes of physical aggression
or other types of humiliation. Other peers may
just silently witness what is happening, without
acknowledging that the bully might interpret such
behavior as an approval of his or her mean
acts. Luckily, some children support the victim
or try to make others stop bullying. Within
the research line that started in the 1990s, the
different ways of responding while witnessing
bullying have been labelled participant roles:
reinforcer (of the bully), assistant (of the bully),
outsider, and defender (of the victim; Salmivalli
et al., 1996; Sutton & Smith, 1999). Later on,
outsiders have also been called silent approvers
of bullying; this captures better the idea that they
are not completely noninvolved either but might,
through their inaction, be involved in maintaining
bullying.
How come bystanders reward bullying, even if
antibullying attitudes are common among school-
aged children and youth (e.g., Rigby & Slee,
1991; Salmivalli & Voeten, 2004)—why aren’t
such attitudes expressed in public? First, although
it might be difficult (or unpleasant) to believe,
youth who bully others are often perceived as
popular by their classmates, especially during
adolescence (e.g., Caravita, DiBlasio, & Salmi-
valli, 2009; Peeters et al., 2010). Longitudinally,
youth who engage in aggressive behavior be-
come increasingly popular over time (Cillessen
& Borch, 2006). In a study by Juvonen, Graham,
and Schuster (2003), bullies were perceived as
cool and those who continued their bullying
behavior from fall to spring even became more
cool over the course of the school year. That
is probably one reason why bystanders are not
eager to intervene on behalf of the victim: They
may be worried about their own status in the
group, or about the possibility of ending up the
next victims themselves.
The second mechanism that prevents people
from expressing antibullying attitudes in public
has been referred to as “pluralistic ignorance”
(Juvonen & Galvan, 2008, pp. 233–234). When
no one challenges the behavior of the bullies,
students come to (falsely) perceive each other
as approving of it. As a consequence, each
individual member of the group might believe
that “although I don’t personally like bullying,
everyone else seems to think it is okay.” Such
a misperception of peer group norms further
prevents students from intervening.
Finally, the perpetrators of bullying are typ-
ically selective in their aggression. By choos-
ing targets who are submissive and insecure
(Schwartz et al., 1998), or in a low-power po-
sition in the group (e.g., Hodges & Perry, 1999;
Salmivalli & Isaacs, 2005), bullies can maximize
the social rewards they gain from peers while
minimizing their loss of affection (Veenstra, Lin-
denberg, Munniksma, & Dijkstra, 2010). If they
targeted peers who had many friends, or who
were highly liked by classmates at large, students
doing the bullying would be more likely to be
confronted or rejected by these friends, or by
classmates in general.
287
Downloaded by [Turku University] at 23:01 23 February 2015
Theories of Bullying and Cyberbullying
To sum up, bullies desire to be visible,
powerful, and popular. Unfortunately, research
demonstrates that it is possible for them to reach
such goals by bullying others. The peer group
rewards the perpetrators of bullying in at least
two ways: by expressing approval (or by not
expressing disapproval) during actual bullying
situations, and by providing the perpetrators with
the position of power and status in the long
run.
Bystanders Matter
How bystanders behave when witnessing bul-
lying is important from the perspective of the
individual children targeted by bullying, but also
from the perspective of the whole bullying dy-
namics of the classroom.
Research has demonstrated that the victims
who have classmates supporting and defending
them are better off than victims without defend-
ers. The defended victims are less depressed and
anxious, they have a higher self-esteem, and they
are less rejected by peers than victims without
defenders, even when the frequency of their
victimization experiences is taken into account
(in other words, their better adjustment is not
only a result of the fact that they are bullied
to a lesser extent; Sainio, Veenstra, Huitsing,
& Salmivalli, 2011). Furthermore, interviews of
adults who used to be bullied during their school
days (Teräsahjo, 1997) have revealed that the
most traumatic memories related to past bullying
are often related to the feeling that no one cared,
rather than to the attacks of the perpetrators as
such. Thus, even one bystander taking sides with
the victimized child or expressing support to him
or her can make a difference.
How bystanders respond when witnessing bul-
lying also influences the extent to which bullying
behavior takes place in a given classroom. Class-
rooms vary in how much and how often bullying
is taking place; about 7% of total variation in
bullying behavior is due to differences between
different classrooms (the rest being due to in-
dividual differences; see Salmivalli, Voeten, &
Poskiparta, 2011). Although the percentage may
seem relatively low, it means that there are some
characteristics of the classroom context (in addi-
tion to characteristics of individual children) that
explain why there is more bullying in some class-
rooms than in others. The classroom differences
can be partly explained by the degree to which
classmates tend to reinforce bullying versus sup-
port and defend victimized peers (Nocentini,
Menesini, & Salmivalli, 2013; Salmivalli et al.,
2011). More specifically, the more classmates
tend to reinforce bullies’ behavior and the less
they provide support to the victims, the more of-
ten bullying is likely to take place in a classroom.
Furthermore, some well-known individual risk
factors for victimization, such as social anxiety,
are more likely to lead to victimization in some
classrooms than in others: again, this depends
on whether it is common among classmates to
reinforce the bullies or support the victimized
peers (Kärnä, Voeten, Poskiparta, & Salmivalli,
2010). Even vulnerable at-risk children do not
necessarily end up as victimized, if the classroom
context does not support such behavior.
The important role of teachers in creating
an environment that does not support bullying
should be noted. Teachers’ efforts to intervene
in bullying, or lack of such efforts, may affect
classroom norms regarding bullying and related
behaviors. Students who perceive their teacher as
clearly disapproving of bullying are less likely
to engage in it (Saarento, Kärnä, Hodges, &
Salmivalli, 2013). Also, it has been found that
positive and supportive student–teacher relation-
ships increase students’ willingness to report
bullying (Eliot, Cornell, Gregory, & Fan, 2010).
Implications for Interventions
Targeting the Group: Why and How?
Children and adolescents facing bullying
problems as bystanders are in a controversial
situation. On one hand, they understand that
bullying is wrong and they would like to do
something to stop it—on the other hand, they
strive to secure their own status and safety in the
peer group. However, if fewer children took on
288
Downloaded by [Turku University] at 23:01 23 February 2015
Salmivalli Participant Roles in Bullying
the role of reinforcer or assistant when witnessing
bullying, and if the group refused to assign high
status for those who bully, important rewards for
bullying would be lost. Although peers are often
part of the bullying problem, they can also be part
of the solution. Therefore, the success of bullying
prevention/intervention efforts often depends on
how well peer bystanders are utilized in such
efforts.
Bystanders are likely to be easier to influence
than the active, initiative-taking bullies. The by-
standers often think that bullying is wrong; they
feel bad for the victim, and they would like to
do something to help. Converting their already
existing attitudes into behavior is a challenging
task, but it might nevertheless be a more realistic
goal than influencing an individual bully by adult
sanctions or rewards only.
Even if the change in bystanders’ behavior
would not lead (at least immediately) to changes
in the bully’s behavior, it is very likely to make
a difference in the victim’s situation. Mobilizing
the peer group to support the victim is crucial
in minimizing the adverse effects for those who
are victimized. Victimization is an attack on the
victim’s status, but also on his or her need to
belong (Hawker & Boulton, 2001), and often a
successful one. Having protective friendships or
supporters in the classroom has been shown to
buffer against further victimization, as well as
the negative influences of victimization (Boulton,
Trueman, Chau, Whitehand, & Amatya, 1999;
Hodges, Boivin, Vitaro, & Bukowski, 1999;
Sainio et al., 2011).
Raising children’s awareness of the role by-
standers have in the bullying process is impor-
tant. Many children and youth may think that
as long as they do not participate in bullying
behavior as perpetrators, they do not have any-
thing to do with the problem. Introducing the
different participant roles to children and having
them reflect on their own typical behavior when
witnessing bullying may already be helpful (“I
don’t really approve of bullying and I would like
it to stop, but admittedly, I often laugh when Lisa
is making fun of Mary”). Students may even be
provided feedback about their typical participant
roles, as observed by the teacher or by class-
mates. Students can discuss together and brain-
storm ways in which they might change their re-
sponses to bullying into more constructive ones.
Enhancing children’s empathic understanding
of the victims’ situation can strengthen their
motivation to stand up for victimized peers. Short
films of former victims telling how bullying has
influenced their schooldays, but also their lives
later on, can be helpful. Also learning-by-doing
exercises where children have a chance to un-
derstand the feelings resulting from victimization
from the target’s standpoint provide them insight
into the negative feelings associated with such a
plight. Role-play can also be used to explore the
feelings associated with other participant roles.
How does it feel to witness bullying without
doing anything on behalf of the victim? Why is it
difficult to show disapproval of bullying or take
sides with the vulnerable peers?
Students should be provided with safe strate-
gies to support the victim. This does not nec-
essarily mean heroic acts such as intervening
in the bully’s behavior directly, but more subtle
ways in which the victimized peer can be made
feel included and supported. Also, when children
decide to express their disapproval of bullying
together as a group, they are likely to be safe—
and much more influential. After identifying
strategies of countering bullying as an individual
and as a group, these strategies can be rehearsed
in exercises done together in classrooms, or in
smaller groups. When the reward structure of
the classroom changes, supporting and defending
the victim can actually become reinforced and
rewarded.
Because of the group mechanisms involved in
bullying, the teacher is usually the key person in
delivering the preventive interventions described.
This is not to say that parents are unimportant;
however, the whole group (such as the students in
a classroom, smaller peer networks within class-
rooms) is present at school together as a group,
and the teacher has the possibility to interact with
this group, whereas the parents typically know—
and interact with—their own child and perhaps
some closest friends of their child. The school is,
therefore, a unique place to work with the group
and to influence its norms.
289
Downloaded by [Turku University] at 23:01 23 February 2015
Theories of Bullying and Cyberbullying
Focusing on bystanders in bullying preven-
tion/intervention work does not mean that indi-
vidual bullies should not be targeted at all. When
bullying comes to the attention of adults, the
particular case should be handled, not together
in the classroom but by private, firm discussions
with the individuals involved. Even in such cases,
however, other peers (e.g., some prosocial class-
mates of the targeted child) can be met separately
and encouraged to think of ways in which they
could support the victimized peer who is having
a difficult time.
Can Bystanders Be Influenced by
Interventions?
The important role of bystanders in the bul-
lying process and, consequently, the potential
reductions in bullying that could be achieved by
influencing this group of children is not a new
idea; it has been discussed in the literature for
about 2 decades. It is more rare, however, to
find research where the idea has been put into an
empirical test, examining whether it is possible
to influence bystander behaviors by school-based
interventions (usually delivered by teachers), and
whether changes in bystander responses actually
lead to reductions in bullying.
Polanin, Espelage, and Pigott (2012) identi-
fied 12 studies evaluating the effects of school-
based programs that focused on increasing by-
stander intervention (e.g., Creating a Peaceful
School Learning Environment, Expect Respect,
Kiusaamista Vastaan [Against Bullying; KiVa]
antibullying program, Steps to Respect). Accord-
ing to their meta-analysis, the programs had
(on average) statistically significant effects on
bystander behaviors (intervening on behalf of the
victim), suggesting that it is, indeed, possible to
change peer responses to bullying. There was
variation between different programs, however,
and looking at their effects individually reveals
that six out of the 12 programs yielded significant
changes in the desired direction, that is, leading
to more bystander intervention. In most cases,
the effects were in the expected direction, even
if not significant.
Only one intervention study (Saarento, Boul-
ton, & Salmivalli, 2014) has tested whether
changes in bystander behaviors, in turn, lead to
reduced levels of bullying. The study was done
in the context of evaluating the effects of the
KiVa antibullying program (www.kivaprogram.
net; Salmivalli, Poskiparta, Ahtola, & Haataja,
2013) which is strongly built on the partici-
pant role approach to bullying. The universal
and indicated actions included in the KiVa pro-
gram (three different curricula of student lessons,
online antibullying games, school-based KiVa
teams tackling the cases of bullying coming to
attention together with classroom teachers) are
based on the principles of raising awareness
and empathy, and providing safe strategies to
intervene on behalf of the victimized peers rather
than rewarding the bullies.
It was found by Saarento and colleagues
(2014) that one mechanism through which KiVa
reduced bullying perpetration was by changing
students’ perceptions of how their classmates
responded to bullying as bystanders. In other
words, the KiVa program led individual children
to see more defending of victimized students
in their classroom environment, which in turn
reduced their own engagement in bullying perpe-
tration. Even though other mechanisms of KiVa
effects (e.g., changes in students’ attitudes, as
well as their perceptions of teachers) were found
as well, especially the results concerning how
perceptions of the peers’ reactions influenced
bullying perpetration lends support to the view
that bystanders are important for either maintain-
ing bullying or stopping it, and they should be
utilized in interventions.
References
Boulton, M., Trueman, M., Chau, C., Whitehand, C.,
& Amatya, K. (1999). Concurrent and longitudinal
links between friendship and peer victimization:
Implications for befriending interventions. Journal
of Adolescence, 22, 461–466.
Caravita, S., Di Blasio, P., & Salmivalli, C. (2009).
Unique and interactive effects of empathy and
social status on involvement in bullying. Social
Development, 18, 140–163.
290
Downloaded by [Turku University] at 23:01 23 February 2015
Salmivalli Participant Roles in Bullying
Cillessen, A. H. N., & Borch, C. (2006). Developmen-
tal trajectories of adolescent popularity: A growth
curve modeling analysis. Journal of Adolescence,
29, 935–959.
Eliot, M., Cornell, D., Gregory, A., & Fan, X. (2010).
Supportive school climate and student willingness
to seek help for bullying and threats of violence.
Journal of School Psychology, 48, 533–553.
Hawker, D., & Boulton, M. (2001). Subtypes of peer
harassment and their correlates. In J. Juvonen &
S. Graham (Eds.), Peer harassment in school:
The plight of the vulnerable and the victimized
(pp. 378–397). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Hawkins, D. L., Pepler, D. J., & Craig, W. M. (2001).
Naturalistic observations of peer interventions in
bullying. Social Development, 10, 512–527.
Hodges, E. V. E., Boivin, M., Vitaro, F., & Bukowski,
W. (1999). The power of friendship: Protection
against an escalating cycle of peer victimization.
Developmental Psychology, 35, 94–101.
Hodges, E. V. E., & Perry, D. G. (1999). Personal
and interpersonal antecedents and consequences of
victimization by peers. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 76, 677–685.
Houghton, S. J., Nathan, E., & Taylor, M. (2012).
To bully or not to bully, that is not the question:
Western Australian early adolescents’ in search of
a reputation. Journal of Adolescent Research, 27,
498–522.
Juvonen, J., & Galvan, A. (2008). Peer influence in
involuntary social groups. Lessons from research
on bullying. In M. Prinstein & K. Dodge (Eds.),
Understanding peer influence in children and ado-
lescents (pp. 225–244). New York, NY: Guilford.
Juvonen, J., Graham. S., & Schuster, M. (2003).
Bullying among young adolescents: The strong,
weak, and troubled. Pediatrics, 112, 1231–1237.
Kärnä, A., Voeten, M., Poskiparta, E., & Salmivalli,
C. (2010). Vulnerable children in varying class-
room contexts: Bystanders’ behaviors moderate the
effects of risk factors on victimization. Merrill-
Palmer Quarterly, 56, 261–282.
Nocentini, A., Menesini, E., & Salmivalli, C. (2013).
Level and change of bullying after high school tran-
sition: A multilevel growth curve analysis. Journal
of Adolescence,36, 495–505.
Peeters, M., Cillessen, A., & Scholte, R. (2010). Clue-
less or powerful? Identifying subtypes of bullies in
adolescence . Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39,
1041–1052.
Polanin, J., Espelage, D., & Pigott, T. (2012). A meta-
analysis of school-based bullying prevention pro-
grams’ effects on bystander intervention behavior.
School Psychology Review, 41, 47–65.
Rigby, K., & Slee, P. T. (1991). Bullying among
Australian school children: Reported behavior and
attitudes toward victims. Journal of Social Psychol-
ogy, 131, 615–627.
Saarento, S., Boulton, A., & Salmivalli, C. (2014).
Reducing bullying and victimization: Student- and
classroom-level mechanisms of change. Journal of
Abnormal Child Psychology [online]. doi: 10.1007/
s10802-013-9841-x.
Saarento, S., Kärnä, A., Hodges, E. V. E., & Salmivalli,
C. (2013). Student-, classroom-, and school-level
risk factors for victimization. Journal of School
Psychology, 51, 421–434.
Sainio, M., Veenstra, R., Huitsing, G., & Salmivalli,
C. (2011). Victims and their defenders: A dyadic
approach. International Journal of Behavioral De-
velopment, 35, 144–151.
Salmivalli, C. (2010). Bullying and the peer group: A
review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 15, 112–
120.
Salmivalli, C., & Isaacs, J. (2005). Prospective re-
lations among victimization, rejection, friendless-
ness, and children’s self- and peer-perceptions.
Child Development, 76, 1161–1171.
Salmivalli, C., Lagerspetz, K., Björkqvist, K., Öster-
man, K., & Kaukiainen, A. (1996). Bullying as a
group process: Participant roles and their relations
to social status within the group. Aggressive Be-
havior, 22, 1–15.
Salmivalli, C., Poskiparta, E., Ahtola, A., & Haataja,
A. (2013). The implementation and effectiveness
of the KiVa antibullying program in Finland. Eu-
ropean Psychologist, 18, 79–88.
Salmivalli, C., & Voeten, M. (2004). Connections
between attitudes, group norms, and behaviors
associated with bullying in schools. International
Journal of Behavioral Development, 28, 246–258.
Salmivalli, C., Voeten, M., & Poskiparta, E. (2011).
Bystanders matter: Associations between defend-
ing, reinforcing, and the frequency of bullying in
classrooms. Journal of Clinical Child and Adoles-
cent Psychology, 40, 668–676.
Schwartz, D., Dodge, K., Hubbard, J., Cillessen,
A., Lemerise, E., & Bateman, H. (1998). Social-
cognitive and behavioral correlates of aggression
and victimization in boys’ play groups. Journal of
Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 431–440.
Sijtsema, J., Veenstra, R., Lindenberg, S., & Salmivalli,
C. (2009). An empirical test of bullies’ status goals:
Assessing direct goals, aggression, and prestige.
Aggressive Behavior, 35, 57–67.
291
Downloaded by [Turku University] at 23:01 23 February 2015
Theories of Bullying and Cyberbullying
Sutton, J., & Smith, P. K. (1999). Bullying as a
group process: An adaptation of the participant role
approach. Aggressive Behavior, 25, 97–111.
Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (2001).
‘It’s easy, it works, and it makes me feel good’:
A response to Arsenio and Lemerise. Social De-
velopment, 10, 74–78.
Teräsahjo, T. (1997). Long-term consequences of vic-
timization as an example of a phenomenological
research approach. (Unpublished master’s thesis).
University of Turku, Finland.
Veenstra, R., Lindenberg, S., Munniksma, A., &
Dijkstra, J. (2010). The complex relation between
bullying, victimization, acceptance, and rejection:
Giving special attention to status, affection, and sex
differences. Child Development, 81, 480–486.
Veenstra, R., Lindenberg, S., Zijlstra, B. J. H., De Win-
ter, A. F., Verhulst, F. C., & Ormel, J. (2007). The
dyadic nature of bullying and victimization: Testing
a dual perspective theory. Child Development, 78,
1843–1854.
292
Downloaded by [Turku University] at 23:01 23 February 2015
... Иногда в рамках социально-психологического подхода к ШБ обсуждается потребность в принадлежности как мотив поведения агрессоров: преследование другого может быть вариантом фрустрационной агрессии, способом справиться с чужой популярностью и попыткой обеспечить себе психологическую безопасность в условиях появления предполагаемого конкурента за статус и социальную нишу (Underwood, Ehrenreich, 2014). Однако агрессоры обычно избирательны и преследуют того, кто никем не защищен и имеет невысокий статус в группе, выстраивая конфигурацию с минимумом риска и максимумом социального вознаграждения (Salmivalli, 2014), что не поддерживает гипотезу о ШБ как реакции на угрозу утраты социального статуса. В рамках этого подхода интервенции -например, «Meaningful Roles» 3 -направлены на интенсивную работу со свидетелями, на развитие у них осознанного отношения к собственной позиции и влиянию на отношения в классе, на изменение системы социальных вознаграждений и поддержку просоциального поведения (Salmivalli, 2014;Ellis et al., 2016). ...
... Однако агрессоры обычно избирательны и преследуют того, кто никем не защищен и имеет невысокий статус в группе, выстраивая конфигурацию с минимумом риска и максимумом социального вознаграждения (Salmivalli, 2014), что не поддерживает гипотезу о ШБ как реакции на угрозу утраты социального статуса. В рамках этого подхода интервенции -например, «Meaningful Roles» 3 -направлены на интенсивную работу со свидетелями, на развитие у них осознанного отношения к собственной позиции и влиянию на отношения в классе, на изменение системы социальных вознаграждений и поддержку просоциального поведения (Salmivalli, 2014;Ellis et al., 2016). ...
... В настоящее время развивается комплексный социально-экологический подход (Hong, Espelage, 2012;Espelage, 2014), рассматривающий ШБ в обширном социальном контексте, включающем индивидуальные особенности детей (Sutton et al., 1999;Fekkes et al., 2006;Gini, Pozzoli, 2009), характеристики семьи (Cross, Barnes, 2014;Espelage et al., 2014), школьный климат (Bosworth, Judkins, 2014;Новикова, Реан, 2019), поведение учителей (Yoon, Bauman, 2014;Yoon et al., 2016;Стратийчук, Чиркина, 2019), отношения между сверстниками (Salmivalli, 2010(Salmivalli, , 2014Sainio et al., 2011), а также связи между системами, характеристики культуры, социальные нормы, законодательство и др. . Такой подход дает возможность выявлять факторы защиты и риска, моделировать условия с разной вероятностью появления ШБ, однако целостная социально-экологическая модель почти недоступна для эмпирической проверки и, как правило, в эмпирических исследованиях сильно редуцируется. ...
Article
Full-text available
School bullying has long been the subject of numerous empirical studies and applied interventions, due to its high prevalence and serious negative consequences. However, the theoretical understanding of this phenomenon, its predictors and dynamics, is not so rich. The paper examines the most established and influential theories developed by domestic and foreign scientists explaining the causes and dynamics of school bullying. The views on bullying as a result of social learning and a consequence of cognitive representations, a way to increase status and achieve popularity, a group process, the result of synergy of factors within the various social systems, as well as a form of adaptation are discussed. These theories are used to varying degrees to develop anti-bullying interventions, such as prevention programs, technologies to stop bullying, and educational activities. Based on the works on the effectiveness of various interventions, the paper problematizes the issues of the reasons for joining school bullying of children and adults, as well as the rapid return to bullying in schools after the end of anti-bullying activities. The author's concept of bullying is proposed as a communal coping strategy of the school community, where the rejection and displacement of one or more children who find themselves in the role of a victim to the periphery of a group of participants in the educational process, performs the functions of coping with stress and helps to reduce emotional stress to all other members of the community. It seems that this view of school bullying provides answers to the questions raised about the spread and stability of bullying and can be used as a basis for the development of programs addressed to the school community.
... Although some studies show differences in the impact of different forms of bullying, most bullying, regardless of whether it is performed offline or online, can impact the recipient negatively (Jadambaa et al., 2020). One of the most striking aspects of youth bullying is that it typically occurs in a group context (Salmivalli, 2014), involving not only the bully and the victim but also bystanders. Importantly, moral disengagement mechanisms are invoked not only by perpetrators but also by victims and bystanders to alleviate negative self-image following bullying incidents. ...
... Bystanders are frequently present during bullying episodes and have become the focus of much recent research as their involvement in these episodes provides an opportunity to either reduce or escalate bullying (Salmivalli, 2014). However, bystanders are not a unitary group. ...
... Some defenders respond constructively by comforting the victim, telling the bully to stop, or reporting the bullying; other defenders act aggressively towards the bully and this can escalate the bullying. Passive bystanders simply observe the bullying Salmivalli, 2014). The manifestation of moral disengagement mechanisms thus varies in accordance with the different bystander responses. ...
Article
Moral disengagement, a term coined by Bandura, explains how people can hurt others and behave immorally, contradicting their beliefs and values and yet live peaceably with themselves. He developed a conceptual model of moral disengagement based on the Social Cognitive Theory of moral agency. By activating moral disengagement mechanisms, people can disregard their moral standards and behave immorally without any self‐censure. This paper synthesises the extensive research on moral disengagement in a youth bullying context to demonstrate the conceptual strength of moral disengagement theory for research and interventions on bullying. It advances the understanding of the contextual basis of moral disengagement by examining its diverse role in bullying episodes that includes bullying perpetration, bullying victimisation and bullying bystanders. The analysis of the different components of moral disengagement representing the loci of the disengagement mechanisms sheds light on how the disengagement processes function in the youth bullying context. Its role as both a moderator and mediator further advances its divergent functioning in the youth bullying context. Finally, implications of the review for advancing knowledge on the moral basis of youth bullying are discussed with suggestions for the development of interventions to attenuate the use of moral disengagement.
... For outsider behavior, up to now one of the most popular explanations is based on Hazler's arguments (1996aHazler's arguments ( , 1996b, which was often taken up in discussions (see e.g. Gini et al., 2008;Hawkins et al., 2001;O'Connell et al., 1999;Salmivalli, 2014;Shultz et al., 2014). A sequence of different psychological patterns suggests a lack of skills or abilities and favorize "fear" as the predominant explanation for outsider behavior. ...
... However, knowledge is also expanded through role differentiation and stress profiles: The often-described image of a social "silent" group (cf. Gini et al., 2008;Pronk et al., 2013;Salmivalli, 2014) is extended. For about a third of the outsider group, "silent" can be translated into a psychological detachment. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Evaluations evidence that up to now outsiders can’t be targeted effectively to tackle bullying in schools. Neither promoting skills and abilities nor reducing fear aspects result in convincing improvements. Borrowing from the field of developmental psychology and supported by Darley and Latané’s findings on helping behavior, Eisenberg’s research proves personal distress as a source of freezing when empathetic instead of sympathetic responses take over. A linkage between observable outsider behavior and processual insights of bystanding is postulated and examined. 326 8th and 9th graders responded to the Participant Role Questionnaire, sociometric and related measures including the moral disengagement scale and underwent physiological testing to assess stress levels triggered by a 20-minute movie clip on bullying. The results extend existing knowledge on the behavioral level: For one in three, sociometric indexes imply a social detachment from class interactions by general outsiders. However, if outsiders are reported to be close to a bullying episode (situational outsiders), personal distress proves to be a central source to explain their behavior. Implications point to changes in how preventive actions should be adapted.
... El principal objetivo del presente estudio es avanzar en la comprensión de la figura upstander en la prevención del acoso y el ciberacoso (Salmivalli, 2014). En concreto, este trabajo forma parte de un estudio más amplio que busca identificar las motivaciones, las barreras y los facilitadores que contribuyen al desarrollo de acciones solidarias en defensa de las víctimas a partir del análisis de las diferentes estrategias utilizadas por personas upstanders ante situaciones de ciberacoso. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Estrategias para el posicionamiento activo de los bystanders ante el ciberacoso en defensa de las víctimas. Proposición de un modelo de intervención de seis categorías que amplía el modelo existente de las 6 Ds.
... Victimization; empathy; defending tendency; prosocial behavior; school bullying; left-behind children process, most school bullying happens in a school or classroom context in the presence of bystanders [5]. Defending others in school bullying situations is a manifestation of prosocial behavior that includes actively stopping the bullying behavior, reporting the bullying to teachers and seeking help from others, and comforting the child who was bullied [6]. ...
... Numerous empirical studies have contributed to the understanding of factors connected to students' bystander behaviors (e.g., Ettekal et al., 2015;Lambe et al., 2019;Salmivalli, 2014). Nevertheless, a crucial gap remains concerning the scarcity of longitudinal studies. ...
Article
Full-text available
Numerous empirical studies have contributed to the understanding of factors connected to stu-dents' bystander behaviors in peer victimization situations. Nevertheless, a crucial gap remains concerning the scarcity of longitudinal studies. Drawing on social cognitive theory, the present study examined whether moral disengagement and defender self-efficacy predicted bystander behaviors a year later. Participants were 1346 Swedish adolescents (M ageT1 = 13.6 years, M ageT2 = 14.6 years) who answered a web-based self-report questionnaire in seventh and eighth grades. Random intercept models showed that higher levels of moral disengagement in seventh grade were associated with more pro-aggressive bystanding in eighth grade (Est = 0.19, p < .001), with interaction analyses revealing that this effect was particularly pronounced in students with high defender self-efficacy (Est = 0.05, p < .01). The results also revealed that higher levels of defender self-efficacy in seventh grade were associated with more defending (Est = 0.18, p < .001) and less passive bystanding (Est = − 0.11, p < .001) in eighth grade. Interaction analyses further demonstrated that the negative association between defender self-efficacy and passive bystanding was significant only at low levels of moral disengagement (Est = 0.09, p < .001). Our findings suggest that moral disengagement is more strongly related to the inhibitive form of moral agency among bystanders, whereas defender self-efficacy is more strongly related to proactive moral agency. Thus, interventions aiming to reduce pro-aggressive bystanding and promote defending need to consider both moral disengagement and defender self-efficacy.
... Menyadari urgensi dari masalah ini, berbagai pihak seperti pemerintah, lembaga pendidikan, orang tua, dan masyarakat luas perlu bekerja sama untuk menciptakan lingkungan sekolah yang aman dan mendukung. Implementasi program pencegahan bullying yang efektif memerlukan pendekatan holistik yang melibatkan kebijakan yang jelas, pendidikan karakter, dukungan psikologis, serta partisipasi aktif dari seluruh komunitas sekolah [6], [7]. Pencegahan bullying dan penanganan kekerasan di sekolah memerlukan keterlibatan semua pihak terkait, termasuk pemerintah, sekolah, orang tua, dan masyarakat. ...
Article
Bullying dan kekerasan di lingkungan sekolah merupakan masalah serius yang dapat mempengaruhi kesehatan mental dan fisik siswa. SMK Al-Had Nusantara menyadari pentingnya menciptakan lingkungan belajar yang aman dan kondusif bagi seluruh siswa. Tujuan untuk mendeskripsikan program pengabdian masyarakat yang dilaksanakan untuk mencegah bullying dan menangani kekerasan di lingkungan sekolah. Program ini dilaksanakan melalui pendekatan partisipatif yang melibatkan seluruh komponen sekolah, termasuk siswa, guru, dan orang tua. Kegiatan yang dilakukan meliputi sosialisasi dan edukasi seperti melakukan pelatihan dan seminar tentang pengenalan jenis bullying, dampaknya, serta cara pencegahannya. Pembentukan tim Anti-Bullying dengan tim khusus yang terdiri dari guru dan siswa untuk memonitor dan menangani kasus bullying. Konseling dan dukungan Psikologis dengan penyediaan layanan konseling bagi korban bullying dan pelaku. Pembuatan Kebijakan Sekolah dilakukan pengembangan dan implementasi kebijakan anti-bullying yang jelas dan tegas, termasuk prosedur pelaporan dan sanksi bagi pelaku. Program ini berhasil meningkatkan kesadaran dan pemahaman mengenai bullying di kalangan siswa dan staf sekolah. Siswa merasa lebih aman dan didukung untuk melaporkan insiden kekerasan. Pendekatan komprehensif dalam pencegahan bullying dan penanganan kekerasan di sekolah dapat menciptakan lingkungan belajar yang lebih aman dan harmonis. Program ini dapat dijadikan model bagi sekolah lain dalam upaya menciptakan lingkungan pendidikan yang bebas dari kekerasan.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Numerous factors influence bystander behaviors during school bullying, with the school climate being a crucial and external environmental factor. However, empirical evidence on this relationship remains limited. Drawing on moral disengagement theory, the present research employs both variable- (structural equation modeling, network analysis modeling, and a situational simulation experiment) and person-centered (qualitative comparative analysis, latent profile analysis, and virtual simulation modeling) perspectives to systematically investigate the influence of school climate on bystander behavior and its underlying mechanisms. Method: This research included three empirical studies with Chinese primary school students (total N = 923). The survey assessed perceptions of school climate, bystander behavior, moral disengagement, and demographic information. Results: School relationships and fairness of rules are key factors through which the school climate affects bystander behavior. Rather than directly intervening in bullying situations, students tend to employ indirect strategies. School climate—via moral disengagement—had an indirect effect on bystander behavior. While “bystander effect” can occur when many students are present, a larger number of students witnessing bullying incidents is a crucial factor for teachers to intervene promptly and effectively. Conclusion: These findings deepen our current understanding of moral disengagement theory in the context of school bullying and indicate that cultivating positive school relationships and maintaining fair rules may enhance bystander intervention.
Article
Full-text available
Wnioski płynące z wieloletniej tradycji badań nad przemocą rówieśniczą wskazują jednoznacznie na szereg negatywnych konsekwencji obejmujących zdrowie psychiczne dzieci i nastolatków. Liczne badania potwierdziły, że stan zdrowia osób doświadczających bullyingu jest znacząco gorszy w porównaniu z ich rówieśnikami i rówieśniczkami niemającymi podobnych doświadczeń. Należy jednak zwrócić uwagę, że konsekwencje te mogą mieć charakter długoterminowy. Badania podłużne wykazały destrukcyjny wpływ przemocy rówieśniczej na zdrowie psychiczne w życiu dorosłym. Ofiary znęcania się rówieśniczego borykały się z zaburzeniami depresyjnymi, lękowymi, jak również z trudnościami we wchodzeniu i podtrzymywaniu satysfakcjonujących relacji społecznych. Trudności te obejmowały również funkcjonowanie w rolach rodzinnych i zawodowych. W artykule ukazano negatywne znaczenie retrospektywnego czynnika psychologicznego, jakim jest doświadczenie szkolnej przemocy rówieśniczej w roli ofiary dla dobrostanu psychicznego w życiu dorosłym. Główną osią analizy uzyskanych wyników w ramach dwudziestu indywidualnych pogłębionych wywiadów z młodymi dorosłymi jest pojęcie zdrowia psychicznego jako złożonego procesu ciągłego reagowania na wyzwania związane z sytuacjami stresowymi. W tym kontekście bullying był nie tylko silnym stresorem, lecz także czynnikiem negatywnie wpływającym na kształtujące się w okresie adolescencji zasoby, które pozwalają utrzymywać zdrowie psychiczne, czyli stabilną samoocenę, wysoko rozwinięte umiejętności interpersonalne, sieć wsparcia społecznego rozumianą jako relacje przyjacielskie czy wewnętrzne umiejscowienie kontroli. Zdecydowana większość uczestników i uczestniczek badania wskazywała na chroniczne konsekwencje bullyingu dla zdrowia i życia społecznego, szczególnie w odniesieniu do lęku i depresji.
Article
Full-text available
The present study aimed to examine the heterogeneity of Chinese adolescents’ defender behaviors and outsider behaviors in school bullying and their predictors. A total of 1335 adolescents (59.78% were boys, Mage = 14.22, SD = 1.28) completed questionnaires concerning defender and outsider behaviors, moral disengagement, defender self-efficacy, and empathy. The results showed that: (1) There were four subtypes of defender behaviors and outsider behaviors in school bullying: comprehensive defender behaviors (23.30%), comforting defender behaviors (30.71%), typical outsider behaviors (12.88%), and mixed behaviors (33.11%). (2) Compared with the comprehensive defender behaviors, adolescents with lower level of defender self-efficacy were more likely to engage in comforting defender behaviors. Compared with the comprehensive defender behaviors, adolescents with higher level of moral disengagement as well as lower level of defender self-efficacy and affective empathy were more likely to perform typical outsider behaviors. Meanwhile, adolescents with higher level of moral disengagement as well as lower level of defender self-efficacy and cognitive empathy were more likely to engage in mixed behaviors rather than comprehensive defender behaviors. These findings provided important implications for developing intervention programs targeting school bullying.
Article
Full-text available
Bullying is a widespread problem in schools and communities around the world, and nationwide initiatives to prevent and reduce it have begun to emerge in European countries. In Finland, the effects of the national antibullying program, KiVa, have been evaluated first in a randomized controlled trial (2007–2009) and then during nationwide rollout across schools (since 2009). The KiVa program has been found to reduce bullying and victimization, while it increases empathy toward victimized peers and self-efficacy to defend them. Moreover, the program has produced reductions in reinforcement of the bullies’ behavior. Finally, KiVa has been found to increase school liking and school motivation, whereas it has led to significant reductions in anxiety, depression, and negative peer perceptions among children and youth. The uptake of the program by Finnish schools has been remarkable, as 90% of the comprehensive schools are at present implementing KiVa. The paper describes the development of the KiVa program, evaluation of its effects, and its implementation across Finnish schools. Challenges in sustainability and high-level implementation of the program are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2013 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
This longitudinal study examines the mediating mechanisms by which the KiVa antibullying program, based on the Participant Role approach, reduces bullying and victimization among elementary school students. Both student-level mechanisms leading to reduced perpetration of bullying and classroom-level mechanisms leading to reductions in bullying and victimization are considered. Analyses are based on a sample of 7,491 students (49.5 % boys) nested within 421 classrooms within 77 schools. At the beginning of program implementation, the children were in Grades 4, 5, and 6 (mean age 11.3 years). Multilevel structural equation modeling was used to analyze whether changes in the hypothesized mediators accounted for later reductions in the outcomes. At the student level, antibullying attitudes and perceptions regarding peers' defending behaviors and teacher attitudes toward bullying mediated the effects of KiVa on self-reported bullying perpetration. The effects on peer-reported bullying were only mediated by antibullying attitudes. At the classroom level, the program effects on both self- and peer-reported bullying were mediated by students' collective perceptions of teacher attitudes toward bullying. Also, perceived reinforcing behaviors predicted bullying but did not emerge as a significant mediator. Finally, bullying mediated the effects of the classroom-level factors on victimization. These findings enhance knowledge of the psychosocial developmental processes contributing to bullying and victimization and shed light on the key mechanisms by which school bullying can successfully be counteracted.
Article
Full-text available
We examined the connections between attitudes, group norms, and students’ behaviour in bullying situations (bullying others, assisting the bully, reinforcing the bully, defending the victim, or staying outside bullying situations). The participants were 1220 elementary school children (600 girls and 620 boys) from 48 school classes from Grades four, five, and six, i.e., 9–10, 10–11, and 11–12 years of age. Whereas attitudes did predict behaviour at the student level in most cases (although the effects were moderate after controlling for gender), the group norms could be used in explaining variance at the classroom level, especially in the upper grades. The class context (even if not classroom norms specifically) had more effect on girls’ than on boys’ bullying-related behaviours.
Article
Full-text available
We examined whether the bystanders’ behaviors in bullying situations influence vulnerable students’ risk for victimization. The sample consisted of 6,980 primary school children from Grades 3–5, who were nested within 378 classrooms in 77 schools. These students filled out Internet-based questionnaires in their schools’ computer labs. The results from multilevel models indicated that the associations between victimization and its two risk factors—social anxiety and peer rejection—were strongest in classrooms that were high in reinforcing bullying and low in defending the victims. This suggests that bystanders’ behaviors in bullying situations moderate the effects of individual and interpersonal risk factors for victimization. Influencing these behaviors might be an effective way to protect vulnerable children from victimization.
Article
Bullying was investigated as a group process, a social phenomenon taking place in a school setting among 573 Finnish sixth-grade children (286 girls, 287 boys) aged 12–13 years. Different Participant Roles taken by individual children in the bullying process were examined and related to a) self-estimated behavior in bullying situations, b) social acceptance and social rejection, and c) belongingness to one of the five sociometric status groups (popular, rejected, neglected, controversial, and average). The Participant Roles assigned to the subject were Victim, Bully, Reinforcer of the bully, Assistant of the bully, Defender of the victim, and Outsider. There were significant sex differences in the distribution of Participant Roles. Boys were more frequently in the roles of Bully, Reinforcer and Assistant, while the most frequent roles of the girls were those of Defender and Outsider. The subjects were moderately well aware of their Participant Roles, although they underestimated their participation in active bullying behavior and emphasized that they acted as Defenders and Outsiders. The sociometric status of the children was found to be connected to their Participant Roles. © 1996 Wiley-Liss, Inc.
Article
Twenty-eight early adolescent boys and girls suspended from school for bullying provided accounts of the importance of reputation in their daily lives, specifically how they initiated, promoted, and then maintained their reputation through bullying. Overall, bullying was a deliberate choice perpetrated to attain a nonconforming reputation and was initially promoted through visibility of physical bullying. These actions became more covert, particularly among girls, during the promotion phase. Sex differences were most marked in the maintenance phase. Although both boys and girls used cyber bullying to deliberately induce a sense of apprehension and fear, boys also deliberately damaged their victim's houses and gardens outside of school hours to induce a greater sense of fear and hence maintain their nonconforming reputation.
Article
This study examined factors that influence a student's willingness to seek help for a threat of violence. The sample consisted of 542 middle school students who completed an anonymous survey that asked students how likely they would be to seek help in response to being bullied or threatened. The survey also included measures of type of bullying, attitudes toward aggressive behavior, and perceptions of teacher tolerance for bullying. Stepwise multiple regression analyses indicated that willingness to seek help is lower in higher grade levels and among males. Students who hold aggressive attitudes and perceive the school climate to be tolerant of bullying were less likely to report a willingness to seek help. Implications for improving student willingness to seek help are discussed.
Article
The purpose of this study was to simultaneously investigate student-, classroom-, and school-level risk factors for victimization. Both peer nominations and students' self-reports of victimization were utilized. The sample consisted of 6731 Finnish elementary school students (3386 girls and 3345 boys) nested in 358 classrooms in 74 schools. The participants were from Grades 3, 4, and 5 (mean age 11years). The results of multilevel analyses indicated that there was considerable variability in, and distinctive risk factors associated with, both peer- and self-reported victimization at all the three levels investigated. Social anxiety and peer rejection synergistically predicted victimization at the student level. At the classroom level, negative social outcome expectations of defending the victim were associated with an increased risk of a student being bullied. Victimization was also common in classrooms and schools where students perceived their teachers to have less disapproving attitudes toward bullying. Furthermore, the effects of the student-level predictors were found to vary across classrooms, and classroom size moderated the effects of social anxiety and peer rejection on victimization. By identifying the risk factors at the multiple levels, and looking into cross-level interactions among these factors, research can help to target interventions at the key ecological factors contributing to victimization, making it possible to maximize the effectiveness of interventions.