ArticlePDF Available

Evidence for Geomagnetic Imprinting and Magnetic Navigation in the Natal Homing of Sea Turtles

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Natal homing is a pattern of behavior in which animals migrate away from their geographic area of origin and then return to reproduce in the same location where they began life [1-3]. Although diverse long-distance migrants accomplish natal homing [1-8], little is known about how they do so. The enigma is epitomized by loggerhead sea turtles (Caretta caretta), which leave their home beaches as hatchlings and migrate across entire ocean basins before returning to nest in the same coastal area where they originated [9, 10]. One hypothesis is that turtles imprint on the unique geomagnetic signature of their natal area and use this information to return [1]. Because Earth's field changes over time, geomagnetic imprinting should cause turtles to change their nesting locations as magnetic signatures drift slightly along coastlines. To investigate, we analyzed a 19-year database of loggerhead nesting sites in the largest sea turtle rookery in North America. Here we report a strong association between the spatial distribution of turtle nests and subtle changes in Earth's magnetic field. Nesting density increased significantly in coastal areas where magnetic signatures of adjacent beach locations converged over time, whereas nesting density decreased in places where magnetic signatures diverged. These findings confirm central predictions of the geomagnetic imprinting hypothesis and provide strong evidence that such imprinting plays an important role in natal homing in sea turtles. The results give credence to initial reports of geomagnetic imprinting in salmon [11, 12] and suggest that similar mechanisms might underlie long-distance natal homing in diverse animals. Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Map Showing Inclination Isolines near Florida and Diagrams Showing Predicted Effects of Isoline Movement on Nesting Density (A) Because these isolines trend east/west whereas the coastline trends north/south, a unique inclination angle marks each area along Florida's east coast. Thus, turtles might locate natal beaches by returning to the appropriate isolines; locations to the north of the target area have steeper inclination angles, whereas locations to the south have shallower inclination angles. Black isolines bordering each color indicate increments of 0.5 and were derived from the IGRF model 11 [23] for the year 2012. The map for intensity isolines is not shown but is qualitatively similar, with different isolines of intensity existing at each area along Florida's east coast [16]. (B and C) Horizontal lines indicate three hypothetical isolines, and green dots represent nesting turtles, each of which has imprinted on the magnetic signature that marked her natal site as a hatchling. Over the past two decades, isolines near Florida have moved northward, but at variable rates. Sometimes, isolines to the south moved less than those to the north, resulting in divergence (C; upper two isolines). In these situations, the geomagnetic imprinting hypothesis predicts a decrease in nesting density, because turtles that imprinted on the fields between the isolines should return to nest over a larger area. In places where isolines converged (because those to the south moved more than those to the north), the hypothesis predicts that nesting density should increase (C; lower two isolines). Tan represents land; blue represents sea.
… 
Content may be subject to copyright.
Report
Evidence for Geomagnetic Imprinting and Magnetic
Navigation in the Natal Homing of Sea Turtles
Highlights
dSea turtle nesting density varies with slight changes in
Earth’s magnetic field
dResults imply that sea turtles locate nesting beaches using
geomagnetic cues
dTurtles likely imprint on the unique magnetic signature of
their natal beach
dSimilar mechanisms may explain natal homing in diverse
long-distance migrants
Authors
J. Roger Brothers, Kenneth J. Lohmann
Correspondence
brotherj@live.unc.edu
In Brief
How sea turtles return to nest on their
natal beaches after long migrations has
remained enigmatic. Brothers and
Lohmann report a relationship between
sea turtle nesting density and small
changes in Earth’s magnetic field.
Results imply that turtles use
geomagnetic cues to find nesting areas
and may imprint on the magnetic field of
the natal beach.
Brothers & Lohmann, 2015, Current Biology 25, 392–396
February 2, 2015 ª2015 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.12.035
Current Biology 25, 392–396, February 2, 2015 ª2015 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.12.035
Report
Evidence for Geomagnetic Imprinting
and Magnetic Navigation
in the Natal Homing of Sea Turtles
J. Roger Brothers
1,
*and Kenneth J. Lohmann
1
1
Department of Biology, University of North Carolina, CB 3280,
Chapel Hill, NC 27599, USA
Summary
Natal homing is a pattern of behavior in which animals
migrate away from their geographic area of origin and then
return to reproduce in the same location where they began
life [1–3]. Although diverse long-distance migrants accom-
plish natal homing [1–8], little is known about how they
do so. The enigma is epitomized by loggerhead sea turtles
(Caretta caretta), which leave their home beaches as hatch-
lings and migrate across entire ocean basins before return-
ing to nest in the same coastal area where they originated
[9, 10]. One hypothesis is that turtles imprint on the unique
geomagnetic signature of their natal area and use this
information to return [1]. Because Earth’s field changes
over time, geomagnetic imprinting should cause turtles to
change their nesting locations as magnetic signatures drift
slightly along coastlines. To investigate, we analyzed a
19-year database of loggerhead nesting sites in the largest
sea turtle rookery in North America. Here we report a strong
association between the spatial distribution of turtle nests
and subtle changes in Earth’s magnetic field. Nesting den-
sity increased significantly in coastal areas where magnetic
signatures of adjacent beach locations converged over time,
whereas nesting density decreased in places where mag-
netic signatures diverged. These findings confirm central
predictions of the geomagnetic imprinting hypothesis
and provide strong evidence that such imprinting plays an
important role in natal homing in sea turtles. The results
give credence to initial reports of geomagnetic imprinting
in salmon [11, 12] and suggest that similar mechanisms
might underlie long-distance natal homing in diverse
animals.
Results and Discussion
Ever since John James Audubon tied silver threads to the legs
of young songbirds and observed their return the following
year [13], evidence has accumulated that many animals return
to their natal areas after migrating to distant locations [1–8]. An
extreme example exists in loggerhead sea turtles, which leave
their natal beaches as hatchlings and traverse entire ocean
basins before returning to nest, at regular intervals, on the
same stretch of coastline where they hatched [9, 10, 14].
How sea turtles accomplish natal homing has remained an
enduring mystery of animal behavior [1, 14–16].
Turtles derive long-distance navigational information from
the Earth’s magnetic field by detecting the intensity and
inclination angle (the angle at which field lines intersect Earth’s
surface) [17–20]. These parameters vary predictably across
the globe [21, 22]. As a result, each area of coastline is typically
marked by a different isoline of inclination and a different
isoline of intensity (Figure 1A) and thus has a unique magnetic
signature [1]. In principle, if turtles were to imprint on the incli-
nation angle and/or intensity of their natal beach, then return-
ing might be relatively simple: a turtle might need only to locate
the coast and then swim north or south until it encounters the
correct magnetic signature (Figure 1A). No evidence presently
exists, however, to support or refute this hypothesis.
An important consideration for the geomagnetic imprinting
hypothesis is that Earth’s magnetic field changes slowly over
time. Because of this field change, known as secular variation
[24], the magnetic signatures that mark natal sites often drift
slightly along the coast while turtles are gone [1, 25]. Thus, if
an adult female selects her nesting sites by seeking out the
magnetic signature on which she imprinted as a hatchling,
she will inevitably change her nesting location in accordance
with secular variation [26, 27]. Such individual changes might
result in detectable population-level shifts in nesting distribu-
tions, providing a unique opportunity to test the geomagnetic
imprinting hypothesis.
Specifically, the hypothesis predicts that when isolines of
inclination or isolines of intensity converge along the coast,
the magnetic signatures marking natal locations between
those isolines will also converge (Figure 1). Thus, returning
turtles will nest on a shorter length of coastline, and the num-
ber of nests per kilometer should increase (Figures 1B and 1C).
By contrast, when isolines diverge, magnetic signatures also
diverge, so returning turtles will nest over a longer length
of coastline and the concentration of nests should decrease
(Figures 1B and 1C). Until now, this possibility has not been
investigated.
We analyzed a 19-year (1993–2011) database of loggerhead
nesting sites for each of the 12 counties on the east (Atlantic)
coast of Florida [28], an area encompassing the largest sea tur-
tle rookery in North America. To evaluate secular variation, we
developed an objective metric (convergence index) that quan-
tifies the degree of isoline movement along the coast within
each county during 17 two-year time steps (see Experimental
Procedures). A positive convergence index indicates that iso-
lines within a particular coastal area moved closer together,
with higher values indicating greater convergence. A negative
convergence index indicates that isolines moved apart, with
more negative values indicating greater divergence. For each
county and time-step combination, we calculated two different
convergence indices, one based on the movement of inclina-
tion isolines and the other based on the movement of intensity
isolines. We then analyzed the relationship between each
convergence index and changes in nesting density.
Analyses confirmed the predictions of the geomagnetic
imprinting hypothesis. For inclination, regardless of year or
location, isoline convergence was associated with increased
nesting density, whereas isoline divergence was associated
with decreased nesting density (p = 5.34 310
24
)(Figure 2).
Moreover, a linear mixed-effects model revealed a highly sig-
nificant relationship between the magnitude of isoline move-
ments and the magnitude of changes in nesting density (p =
3.67 310
24
)(Figure 3;Table S1): the highest convergence
indices were associated with the greatest increases in nesting
*Correspondence: brotherj@live.unc.edu
density, and the lowest convergence indices were associated
with the greatest decreases in nesting density. This trend per-
sisted within each of the 12 counties on Florida’s east coast
(Figure 4;Table S2).
For intensity, there were no areas along the coast where iso-
lines converged; thus, all convergence indices were negative.
In all other regards, however, the results of the analysis were
qualitatively identical to those of the inclination analysis. A
linear mixed-effects model revealed a strong positive relation-
ship between convergence index and changes in nesting den-
sity (p = 8.2 310
24
)(Figure 3;Table S1): as convergence index
increased, so did the percent change in nesting density. This
trend persisted within all 12 counties on Florida’s east coast
(Figure 4;Table S2).
These results provide strong evidence that nesting sea tur-
tles use Earth’s magnetic field to locate their natal beaches.
Although the exact geomagnetic component (or components)
exploited by turtles cannot be determined from the analyses,
the findings are consistent with the hypothesis that nest site
selection depends at least partly on magnetic signatures con-
sisting of inclination angle, field intensity, or a combination of
the two.
In a previous study, the migratory route of salmon approach-
ing their natal river was shown to vary with subtle changes in
the Earth’s field [11]. Whereas the endpoint of the salmon
spawning migration was presumably the same regardless of
route, our findings demonstrate for the first time a relationship
between changes in Earth’s magnetic field and the locations
where long-distance migrants return to reproduce.
Sea turtles are long lived, and females undertake reproduc-
tive migrations periodically throughout their adult lives [29].
Thus, the population of turtles that migrate to a given beach
to nest each year consists of two subsets: a group of first-
time nesters, and another, typically larger group of older ‘‘re-
migrants’’ that have nested in the area during previous years.
Genetic analyses indicate that both groups display natal hom-
ing [3, 5, 9, 14]. An unresolved question, however, is whether
both reach the natal region by using the same navigational
strategy and sensory cues [26].
At least two possibilities are compatible with the data. One
is that hatchling turtles imprint on the magnetic signature of
the natal beach and retain this information into adulthood [1].
Alternatively, nesting females might somehow reach the natal
beach the first time without relying on magnetic information
(e.g., by following an experienced nester or by using nonmag-
netic cues) and then learn the magnetic signature of the beach
and use it to return during subsequent nesting migrations.
Although neither possibility can be excluded, we presently
favor the first because ‘‘socially facilitated’ migration has
never been observed in sea turtles [3, 30], and because no
nonmagnetic cue has been identified that can provide the
necessary positional information for long-distance navigation
[16]. Regardless of how the first return to the natal region is
accomplished, turtles might periodically update their knowl-
edge of the magnetic field at the nesting area each time they
visit so as to minimize navigational errors that might otherwise
accrue due to secular variation [25, 26].
Given the strong relationship between subtle changes in
Earth’s magnetic field and sea turtle nesting density, one pos-
sibility is that turtles are highly sensitive to small differences in
magnetic fields. Alternatively, however, the same relationship
can be explained if, in a typical nesting area, numerous
error-prone individuals seek out a magnetic signature but
miss the target by varying distances. Such imperfect naviga-
tion might, through a process resembling a ‘‘wisdom of the
crowd’’ phenomenon [31, 32], give rise to a nesting distribution
centered on the magnetic signature and, in effect, coupled to
it. Thus, when Earth’s field changes slightly and magnetic sig-
natures move, the population-level nesting distribution might
change even if individual turtles have relatively low magnetic
sensitivity and make considerable navigational errors.
Our findings do not imply that turtles reflexively nest at a
particular magneticsignature regardless of other environmental
ABC
Figure 1. Map Showing Inclination Isolines near Florida and Diagrams
Showing Predicted Effects of Isoline Movement on Nesting Density
(A) Because these isolines trend east/west whereas the coastline trends
north/south, a unique inclination angle marks each area along Florida’s
east coast. Thus, turtles might locate natal beaches by returning to the
appropriate isolines; locations to the north of the target area have steeper
inclination angles, whereas locations to the south have shallower inclination
angles. Black isolines bordering each color indicate increments of 0.5!and
were derived from the IGRF model 11 [23] for the year 2012. The map for in-
tensity isolines is not shown but is qualitatively similar, with different isolines
of intensity existing at each area along Florida’s east coast [16].
(B and C) Horizontal lines indicate three hypothetical isolines, and green
dots represent nesting turtles, each of which has imprinted on the magnetic
signature that marked her natal site as a hatchling. Over the past two de-
cades, isolines near Florida have moved northward, but at variable rates.
Sometimes, isolines to the south moved less than those to the north, result-
ing in divergence (C; upper two isolines). In these situations, the geomag-
netic imprinting hypothesis predicts a decrease in nesting density, because
turtles that imprinted on the fields between the isolines should return to nest
over a larger area. In places where isolines converged (because those to the
south moved more than those to the north), the hypothesis predicts that
nesting density should increase (C; lower two isolines). Tan represents
land; blue represents sea.
Figure 2. Changes in Nesting Density for Coastal Areas with Converging
and Diverging Inclination Isolines
At times and places in which isolines converged (n = 29), nesting density
increased by an average of 35%. At times and places in which isolines
diverged (n = 172), nesting density decreased by an average of 6%. The
mean changes of the two groups were significantly different (p = 5.34 3
10
24
). Error bars represent standard error of the mean.
393
conditions, or that nesting distributions will track the steady
movement of isolines along a coast no matter what. Successful
nesting requires deposition of eggs in a location suitable for in-
cubation. Factors such as beach erosion, sand quality, visual
cues, and predation are known to influence where turtles nest
on a local scale [1, 26]. Because these and other environmental
conditions also affect the likelihood that a nest will yield viable
hatchlings [26, 33], natural selection is likely to act against
turtles that choose nesting locations by relying on magnetic
cues to the exclusion of all else. Moreover, sensory cues other
than geomagnetism are likely to help guide natal homing, espe-
cially once turtles have arrived in the vicinity of the nesting area
[25, 26].
Given that geomagnetic cues appear to play an important
role in natal homing, an intriguing speculation is that, over
evolutionary time, turtles might have had difficulty locating
their natal beaches during brief periods of rapid field change,
as are thought to have occurred during some magnetic polarity
reversals [34]. During these intervals, turtles might have had a
tendency to stray into new nesting areas, which subsequent
generations could then locate reliably as the field stabilized
and geomagnetic imprinting once more became an effective
strategy for natal homing [1].
Because sea turtles nest in different environmental settings
worldwide, it is possible that different nesting populations
exploit geomagnetic cues in different ways during natal
homing [1, 16, 35]. Our analysis suggests that turtles use
geomagnetic cues to locate natal areas along continental
coastlines, the most common setting for large sea turtle
rookeries worldwide [16]. In other settings, such as on small
islands, turtles must nest in specific, restricted areas
because no alternative exists. In such situations, a clear rela-
tionship between field changes and nesting sites is unlikely
because, over time, magnetic signatures that once marked
the natal site drift offshore where nesting is impossible
[1, 26]. In these cases, turtles might use magnetic cues to
navigate to the vicinity of the island and then use odorants
or other supplemental local cues to locate the nesting beach
[16, 35, 36].
Regardless of these considerations, our results provide
the strongest evidence to date that sea turtles find their nest-
ing areas at least in part by navigating to unique magnetic
signatures along the coast. In addition, our results are
consistent with the hypothesis that turtles accomplish natal
homing largely on the basis of magnetic navigation and
geomagnetic imprinting. These findings, in combination
with recent studies on Pacific salmon [11, 12], suggest that
similar mechanisms might underlie natal homing in diverse
long-distance migrants such as fishes [2, 4], birds [37, 38],
and mammals [6].
AB
-50
0
50
100
150
-0.004 -0.002 0.000 0.002
Inclination Convergence Index
Percent change in nesting density
-50
0
50
100
150
-0.006 -0.004 -0.002 0.000
Intensity Convergence Index
Percent change in nesting density
Figure 3. Relationship between Isoline Move-
ment and Change in Nesting Density
Each data point represents values for one county
in one time step.
(A) For inclination, a significant, positive relation-
ship exists between convergence index and
change in nesting density (p = 3.67 310
24
;
n = 204) (Table S1). As the degree of isoline
convergence increased, so did the change in
nesting density; the greatest increases in nesting
were associated with the highest rates of
convergence, and the greatest decreases in
nesting were associated with the highest rates
of divergence.
(B) For intensity, a significant positive relation-
ship also exists between convergence index
and change in nesting density (p = 8.2 310
24
;
n = 204) (Table S1). The slope and intercept for
each red line were estimated with mixed-effects
models that included convergence index as a
fixed effect and a random slope and intercept
for time step.
AB
-50
0
50
100
150
-0.004 -0.002 0.000 0.002
Inclination Convergence Index
Percent change in nesting density
County
Nassau
Duval
St Johns
Flagler
Volusia
Brevard
Indian River
St Lucie
Martin
Palm Beach
Broward
Miami-Dade
-50
0
50
100
150
-0.006 -0.004 -0.002 0.000
Intensity Convergence Index
Percent change in nesting density
Figure 4. Relationship between Isoline Move-
ment and Change in Nesting Density for Individ-
ual Counties
Each data point represents values for one county
in one time step; each county is represented by
a different color. In the color key, counties are
arranged from north (top) to south (bottom). For
both the inclination analysis (A) and the intensity
analysis (B), all counties show a positive relation-
ship between convergence index and change in
nesting density (n = 17 for each county) (Table
S2). The greatest increases in nesting were asso-
ciated with the highest rates of convergence,
and the greatest decreases in nesting were asso-
ciated with the highest rates of divergence. For
inclination, this relationship was significant in
eight individual counties (p < 0.05), and the trend
was present in all. For intensity, the relation-
ship was significant in seven individual counties
(p < 0.05), and the trend was present in all.
394
Experimental Procedures
Using data from Florida’s Statewide Nesting Beach Survey [28], which re-
ports the number of kilometers surveyed within each county and the corre-
sponding number of sea turtle nests counted, we calculated the loggerhead
turtle nesting density in Florida’s 12 east coast counties for each of 19 years
(1993–2011). We then calculated each county’s percent change in nesting
density for 17 two-year time steps (e.g., from 1993 to 1995, 1994 to 1996,
and so on). Because the total number of loggerhead nests on Florida’s
east coast varied from year to year [39], we estimated the change in nesting
density attributable to population fluctuations by calculating the average
change in nesting for all counties and time steps. We then calculated the dif-
ference between this average and each data point and used the resulting
value in our analyses.
Two-year time steps were used because adult female loggerheads typi-
cally return to nest on their natal beach every two to three years [29]; thus,
this time step reflects isoline movement that turtles realistically encounter
during successive reproductive migrations. Ideally, an analysis of nesting
data designed to test geomagnetic imprinting would be limited to first-
time migrants and would also use a longer time step that coincides with
the interval between hatching and first migration, but this was impractical
because no existing dataset spans a sufficient time period or distinguishes
between first-time and experienced migrants.
To assign coastal position, we used Google Earth to calculate distance
along a line parallel to the east coast of Florida (Figure S1). We then used
data from the International Geomagnetic Reference Field model 11 (IGRF-
11) [23] to calculate the distance isolines traveled along the coast over the
same two-year time steps for which we evaluated changes in nesting den-
sity. We described isoline movement as a function of coastal position (Fig-
ure S2A). The derivative of this function, with respect to position, quantifies
isoline convergence or divergence (Figure S2B). This metric, referred to as
the convergence index, was calculated at the midpoint of each county for
each time step. A convergence index was calculated for both inclination
and intensity isolines.
Over the past two decades, isolines near Florida have moved northward,
but at variable rates. At some times and places, isolines to the south moved
less than those to the north, resulting in the divergence of isolines. In such
cases, the derivative (convergence index) is negative (Figure S2). At other
times and places, isolines to the south traveled farther than those to the
north, resulting in the convergence of isolines. In these places, the derivative
(convergence index) is positive (Figure S2). In addition, the degree of isoline
convergence or divergence is proportional to the magnitude of the deriva-
tive; a more positive derivative indicates high rates of convergence, while
a more negative derivative indicates high rates of divergence.
To characterize the relationship between convergence index and change
in nesting density, we evaluated several linear models, including ordinary
least-squares regression, mixed-effects regressions with random effects
for time step, and mixed-effects regressions with random effects for county.
The random effects included in the models take into account the year-to-
year variations in nesting density along the Florida coast, as well as the
county-to-county variations. While all models revealed equivalent results,
the best-fit models for both the inclination analysis and the intensity analysis
include convergence index as a fixed effect and a random intercept and
slope for time step (Table S1). We evaluated the difference between nesting
changes for areas with converging or diverging inclination isolines using a
mixed-effects model with convergence or divergence as a fixed effect and
time step as a random effect. This last analysis was not performed for inten-
sity isolines because there were no coastal areas with converging intensity
isolines.
Supplemental Information
Supplemental Information includes two figures and two tables and can be
found with this article online at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.12.035.
Author Contributions
The study was conceived by J.R.B. and K.J.L. The data were analyzed by
J.R.B. The manuscript was written by J.R.B. and K.J.L.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Professor Michael Lavine for statistical assistance and
Dr. Catherine Lohmann, David Ernst, and Vanessa Be
´zy for comments on
manuscript drafts. This work was supported by National Science Founda-
tion grant IOS-1022005 and Air Force Office of Scientific Research grant
FA9550-14-1-0208.
Received: November 11, 2014
Revised: December 10, 2014
Accepted: December 11, 2014
Published: January 15, 2015
References
1. Lohmann, K.J., Putman, N.F., and Lohmann, C.M.F. (2008).
Geomagnetic imprinting: A unifying hypothesis of long-distance natal
homing in salmon and sea turtles. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 105,
19096–19101.
2. Rooker, J.R., Secor, D.H., De Metrio, G., Schloesser, R., Block, B.A., and
Neilson, J.D. (2008). Natal homing and connectivity in Atlantic bluefin
tuna populations. Science 322, 742–744.
3. Meylan, A.B., Bowen, B.W., and Avise, J.C. (1990). A genetic test of the
natal homing versus social facilitation models for green turtle migration.
Science 248, 724–727.
4. Feldheim, K.A., Gruber, S.H., Dibattista, J.D., Babcock, E.A., Kessel,
S.T., Hendry, A.P., Pikitch, E.K., Ashley, M.V., and Chapman, D.D.
(2014). Two decades of genetic profiling yields first evidence of natal
philopatry and long-term fidelity to parturition sites in sharks. Mol.
Ecol. 23, 110–117.
5. Bowen, B.W., Bass, A.L., Chow, S.M., Bostrom, M., Bjorndal, K.A.,
Bolten, A.B., Okuyama, T., Bolker, B.M., Epperly, S., Lacasella, E.,
et al. (2004). Natal homing in juvenile loggerhead turtles (Caretta
caretta). Mol. Ecol. 13, 3797–3808.
6. Baker, C.S., Steel, D., Calambokidis, J., Falcone, E., Gonza
´lez-Peral, U.,
Barlow, J., Burdin, A.M., Clapham, P.J., Ford, J.K.B., Gabriele, C.M.,
et al. (2013). Strong maternal fidelity and natal philopatry shape genetic
structure in North Pacific humpback whales. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 494,
291–306.
7. Wheelwright, N.T., and Mauck, R.A. (1998). Philopatry, natal dispersal,
and inbreeding avoidance in an island population of savannah spar-
rows. Ecology 79, 755–767.
8. Welch, A.J., Fleischer, R.C., James, H.F., Wiley, A.E., Ostrom, P.H.,
Adams, J., Duvall, F., Holmes, N., Hu, D., Penniman, J., and Swindle,
K.A. (2012). Population divergence and gene flow in an endangered
and highly mobile seabird. Heredity (Edinb) 109, 19–28.
9. Bowen, B.W., Kamezaki, N., Limpus, C.J., Hughes, G.R., Meylan, A.B.,
and Avise, J.C. (1994). Global phylogeography of the loggerhead turtle
(Caretta caretta) as indicated by mitochondrial DNA haplotypes.
Evolution 48, 1820–1828.
10. Bowen, B.W., Abreu-Grobois, F.A., Balazs, G.H., Kamezaki, N., Limpus,
C.J., and Ferl, R.J. (1995). Trans-Pacific migrations of the loggerhead
turtle (Caretta caretta) demonstrated with mitochondrial DNA markers.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 92, 3731–3734.
11. Putman, N.F., Lohmann, K.J., Putman, E.M., Quinn, T.P., Klimley, A.P.,
and Noakes, D.L.G. (2013). Evidence for geomagnetic imprinting as a
homing mechanism in Pacific salmon. Curr. Biol. 23, 312–316.
12. Putman, N.F., Jenkins, E.S., Michielsens, C.G.J., and Noakes, D.L.G.
(2014). Geomagnetic imprinting predicts spatio-temporal variation in
homing migration of pink and sockeye salmon. J. R. Soc. Interface 11,
20140542.
13. Audubon, J.J. (1840). Pewee flycatcher. In The Birds of America, Volume
1, (E.G. Dorsey), pp. 223–230.
14. Shamblin, B.M., Dodd, M.G., Bagley, D.A., Ehrhart, L.M., Tucker, A.D.,
Johnson, C., Carthy, R.R., Scarpino, R.A., McMichael, E., Addison,
D.S., et al. (2011). Genetic structure of the southeastern United
States loggerhead turtle nesting aggregation: evidence of additional
structure within the peninsular Florida recovery unit. Mar. Biol. 158,
571–587.
15. Carr, A.F. (1967). So Excellent a Fishe: a Natural History of Sea Turtles
(Scribner).
16. Lohmann, K.J., Lohmann, C.M.F., Brothers, J.R., and Putman, N.F.
(2013). Natal homing and imprinting in sea turtles. In The Biology of
Sea Turtles, Volume III, Wyneken, J., Lohmann, K.J., and Musick, J.A.,
eds. (CRC Press), pp. 59–77.
17. Lohmann, K.J., and Lohmann, C.M.F. (1996). Detection of magnetic field
intensity by sea turtles. Nature 380, 59–61.
395
18. Lohmann, K.J., and Lohmann, C.M.F. (1994). Detection of magnetic
inclination angle by sea turtles: a possible mechanism for determining
latitude. J. Exp. Biol. 194, 23–32.
19. Lohmann, K.J., Lohmann, C.M.F., Ehrhart, L.M., Bagley, D.A., and
Swing, T. (2004). Animal behaviour: geomagnetic map used in sea-turtle
navigation. Nature 428, 909–910.
20. Collett, T.S., and Collett, M. (2011). Animal navigation: following sign-
posts in the sea. Curr. Biol. 21, R843–R846.
21. Gould, J.L. (1982). The map sense of pigeons. Nature 296, 205–211.
22. Lohmann, K.J., Lohmann, C.M.F., and Putman, N.F. (2007). Magnetic
maps in animals: nature’s GPS. J. Exp. Biol. 210, 3697–3705.
23. Finlay, C.C., Maus, S., Beggan, C.D., Bondar, T.N., Chambodut, A.,
Chernova, T.A., Chulliat, A., Golovkov, V.P., Hamilton, B., Hamoudi,
M., et al. (2010). International Geomagnetic Reference Field: the
eleventh generation. Geophys. J. Int. 183, 1216–1230.
24. Skiles,D.D. (1985). The geomagnetic field: its nature, history, and biolog-
ical relevance. In Magnetite Biomineralization and Magnetoreception in
Organisms: A New Biomagnetism, J.L. Kirschvink, D.S. Jones, and B.J.
MacFadden, eds. (Plenum Press), pp. 43–102.
25. Putman, N.F., and Lohmann, K.J. (2008). Compatibility of magnetic
imprinting and secular variation. Curr. Biol. 18, R596–R597.
26. Lohmann, K.J., Hester, J.T., and Lohmann, C.M.F. (1999). Long-
distance navigation in sea turtles. Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 11, 1–23.
27. Freake, M.J., Muheim, R., and Phillips, J.B. (2006). Magnetic maps in
animals: a theory comes of age? Q. Rev. Biol. 81, 327–347.
28. Florida Fish and Wildlife Conservation Commission-Fish and Wildlif e
Research Institute (2013). Statewide Nesting Beach Survey program.
http://myfwc.com/research/wildlife/sea-turtles/nesting/statewide/.
29. Schroeder, B.A., Foley, A.M., and Bagley, D.A. (2003). Nesting patterns,
reproductive migrations, and adult foraging areas of loggerhead turtles.
In Loggerhead Sea Turtles, A.B. Bolton and B.E. Witherington, eds.
(Smithsonian Institution Press), pp. 114–124.
30. Plotkin, P.T., Byles, R.A., Rostal, D.C., and Owens, D.W. (1995).
Independent versus socially facilitated oceanic migrations of the olive
ridley, Lepidochelys olivacea. Mar. Biol. 122, 137–143.
31. Galton, F. (1907). Vox populi. Nature 75, 450–451.
32. Surowiecki, J. (2004). The Wisdom of the Crowds (Doubleday).
33. Putman, N.F., Bane, J.M., and Lohmann, K.J. (2010). Sea turtle nesting
distributions and oceanographic constraints on hatchling migration.
Proc. Biol. Sci. 277, 3631–3637.
34. Sagnotti, L., Scardia, G., Giaccio, B., Liddicoat, J.C., Nomade, S.,
Renne, P.R., and Sprain, C.J. (2014). Extremely rapid directional change
during Matuyama-Brunhes geomagnetic polarity reversal. Geophys. J.
Int. 199, 1110–1124.
35. Lohmann, K.J., Luschi, P., and Hays, G.C. (2008). Goal navigation and
island-finding in sea turtles. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 356, 83–95.
36. Luschi, P., Benhamou, S., Girard, C., Ciccione, S., Roos, D., Sudre, J.,
and Benvenuti, S. (2007). Marine turtles use geomagnetic cues during
open-sea homing. Curr. Biol. 17, 126–133.
37. Alerstam, T. (2006). Conflicting evidence about long-distance animal
navigation. Science 313, 791–794.
38. Wiltschko, R., and Wiltschko, W. (2003). Avian navigation: from histori-
cal to modern concepts. Anim. Behav. 65, 257–272.
39. Witherington, B., Kubilis, P., Brost, B., and Meylan, A. (2009).
Decreasing annual nest counts in a globally important loggerhead sea
turtle population. Ecol. Appl. 19, 30–54.
396
... This adaptation could reflect behavioural plasticity in response to climate change [33,43,44]. Another possible influence may include Earth's shifting magnetic field, affecting the natal homing and geomagnetic spatial orientation during navigation imprinted at birth [45]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: The conservation of loggerhead sea turtles (Caretta caretta) in the central Mediterranean benefits from an in-depth understanding of its population genetic structure and diversity. Methods: This study, therefore, investigates C. caretta in Maltese waters by genetically analysing 63 specimens collected through strandings and in-water sampling, using mitochondrial DNA control region and microsatellites. Additionally, the two nests detected in Malta in 2023 were analysed for the same markers. Results: Mitochondrial data identified 10 haplotypes, with mixed stock analyses tracing 87.5% of the specimens to Mediterranean origins, primarily from Libyan rookeries, with contributions from Lebanon, Israel and Turkey. Three Atlantic haplotypes were identified in six specimens, with CC-A17.1 linking central Mediterranean foraging individuals to rookeries in Cape Verde. Five of these six Atlantic haplotype records were from recently sampled individuals (2022–2023), possibly indicating a recent eastward expansion of Atlantic haplotypes into the Mediterranean. Bayesian clustering (K = 2) of microsatellite data using haplotypes as priori revealed similar proportions for clusters across most specimens, except for three specimens with Atlantic haplotypes CC-A1.1 and CC-A1.3, which exhibited distinct patterns. The two nests examined here displayed Mediterranean haplotypes, with nuclear DNA matching the predominant Mediterranean profiles found in foraging individuals, suggesting that local clutches originated from Mediterranean parents. Conclusions: Increasing nesting activity on Maltese beaches and this archipelago’s geographical position highlight the need for ongoing genetic monitoring to track changes in genetic diversity and develop conservation strategies that support the effective protection of this species and its habitats.
... Sea turtles are however generally limited in their nestsite choice, as they display natal homing, returning to the geographic area where they hatched to reproduce (Brothers and Lohmann 2015;Lohmann and Lohmann 2019;Levasseur et al. 2019). Additionally, some species exhibit high nest-site fidelity, laying consecutive clutches on the same beach (Kamel and Mrosovsky 2004;Heredero Saura et al. 2022), within the same location of the beach Mrosovsky 2005, 2006;Patrício et al. 2018;Shimada et al. 2021), even across nesting seasons (Heredero Saura et al. 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
Understanding spatial heterogeneity in reproductive success among at-risk populations facing localised threats is key for conservation. Sea turtle populations often concentrate at one nesting site, diverting conservation efforts from adjacent smaller rookeries. Poilão Island, Bijagós Archipelago, Guinea-Bissau, is a notable rookery for green turtles Chelonia mydas within the João Vieira-Poilão Marine National Park, surrounded by three islands (Cavalos, Meio and João Vieira), with lower nesting activity. Poilão’s nesting suitability may decrease due to turtle population growth and sea level rise, exacerbating already high nest density. As the potential usage of secondary sites may arise, we assessed green turtle clutch survival and related threats in Poilão and its neighbouring islands. High nest density on Poilão leads to high clutch destruction by later turtles, resulting in surplus eggs on the beach surface and consequently low clutch predation (4.0%, n = 69, 2000). Here, the overall mean hatching success estimated was 67.9 ± 36.7% (n = 631, 2015–2022), contrasting with a significantly lower value on Meio in 2019 (11.9 ± 23.6%, n = 21), where clutch predation was high (83.7%, n = 98). Moderate to high clutch predation was also observed on Cavalos (36.0%, n = 64) and João Vieira (76.0%, n = 175). Cavalos and Meio likely face higher clutch flooding compared to Poilão. These findings, alongside observations of turtle exchanges between islands, may suggest a source-sink dynamic, where low reproductive output sink habitats (neighbouring islands) are utilized by migrants from Poilão (source), which currently offers the best conditions for clutch survival.
... We do not know how large an area the signature represents to the animal (a localized area or a broad region), or whether the animal imagines itself to be anywhere at all. We do not know how sensitivity to magnetic fields is distributed within animal populations, i.e., whether population-level responses reflect the sensitivity of most individuals, or of just some particularly accurate individuals, or, instead, represent wisdom-of-the-crowd effects in which weakly sensitive individuals collectively appear to display higher sensitivity 11 . In the absence of such information, we favor a more conservative approach in which experiments reveal responses of animals, and data, rather than assumptions, shape discussion. ...
... It may be generalizable to other marine or terrestrial species for which individuals disperse from known or assigned point sources. Hence, its adaptability can be queried for various species, in particular migratory fish (Thorrold et al., 2001), seabirds (Putman, 2020), butterflies (Mouritsen, 2018), marine turtles (Brothers and Lohmann, 2015) or bats (Baerwald et al., 2021), that accomplish natal homing, spawning site fidelity or return migrations. ...
Article
Full-text available
Species Distribution Models (SDM) are useful tools providing results that can be extrapolated to anticipate species range shifts, under climate change scenarios. SDM studies integrating spatial constraints are significantly lacking in the marine environment, leading to optimistic predictions. This is particularly true for anadromous species in which marine distributions can be driven by their affinity to their natal rivers. Acipenser sturio is a critically endangered anadromous fish for which two stocked populations are currently maintained in the Gironde-Garonne-Dordogne (France) and Elbe (Germany) river systems. Benefiting from bycatch reports of A. sturio, we applied a SDM process that explicitly considers distance to home when evaluating habitat suitability. More precisely, we included the variable 'distance to mouth of the natal river system' into SDM inputs to test and characterize its influence on the marine distribution of A. sturio. We used this model to obtain the marine distribution under current climatic conditions with the two source populations and under population recovery scenarios (functional populations hypothesized to exist in ten currently unoccupied river systems). We projected the model under future conditions with two climatic scenarios (RCP 4.5 and 8.5) and three time slices over the 2023-2099 period. Constrained-ranges of both existing and hypothetical populations are projected to expand in the future. We observed an overall increase of habitat suitability, with new suitable sectors localized further from natal river mouths. By informing on the suitable marine surface that each hypothetical population holds and adds to the existing ones, our approach aims at informing about the feasibility of species recovery and marine habitats protection strategies. Our findings highlight the need for including dispersal information in marine SDM. The application of our dispersal-constrained approach may be considered for other less-well-known species for which dispersal point sources are identifiable, such as other diadromous species in different study areas.
... 12 Controllers of the spatially relevant behavioral aspects of migration termination, however, are not well known. Some long-distance migrants, such as Pacific salmon or sea turtles, imprint natal habitats using geomagnetic [13][14][15][16] or olfactory 17 cues, returning to natal sites to terminate migratory movements and transition life history stages. Social environment plays a large role in migration termination in low site fidelity nomadic migrants, e.g., the pine siskin. ...
Article
Full-text available
Eastern North American migratory monarch butterflies exhibit migratory behavioral states in fall and spring characterized by sun-dependent oriented flight. However, it is unclear how monarchs transition between these behavioral states at their overwintering site. Using a modified Mouritsen-Frost flight simulator, we confirm individual directionality and compass-based orientation (leading to group orientation) in fall migrants, and also uncover sustained flight propensity and direction-based flight reinforcement as distinctly migratory behavioral traits. By testing monarchs at their Mexican overwintering sites, we show that overwintering monarchs show reduced propensity for sustained flight and lose individual directionality, leading to the loss of group-level orientation. Overwintering fliers orient axially in a time-of-day dependent manner, which may indicate local versus long-distance directional heading. These results support a model of migratory flight behavior in which modular, state-dependent switches for flight propensity and orientation control are highly dynamic and are controlled in season- and location-dependent manners.
Article
Full-text available
Growing evidence indicates that migratory animals exploit the magnetic field of the Earth for navigation, both as a compass to determine direction and as a map to determine geographical position¹. It has long been proposed that, to navigate using a magnetic map, animals must learn the magnetic coordinates of the destination2,3, yet the pivotal hypothesis that animals can learn magnetic signatures of geographical areas has, to our knowledge, yet to be tested. Here we report that an iconic navigating species, the loggerhead turtle (Caretta caretta), can learn such information. When fed repeatedly in magnetic fields replicating those that exist in particular oceanic locations, juvenile turtles learned to distinguish magnetic fields in which they encountered food from magnetic fields that exist elsewhere, an ability that might underlie foraging site fidelity. Conditioned responses in this new magnetic map assay were unaffected by radiofrequency oscillating magnetic fields, a treatment expected to disrupt radical-pair-based chemical magnetoreception4, 5–6, suggesting that the magnetic map sense of the turtle does not rely on this mechanism. By contrast, orientation behaviour that required use of the magnetic compass was disrupted by radiofrequency oscillating magnetic fields. The findings provide evidence that two different mechanisms of magnetoreception underlie the magnetic map and magnetic compass in sea turtles.
Chapter
Quantum phenomena can be identified in different forms in organisms, ranging from the exploitation of light photons for information processes or energy conversion over bioluminescence to the ubiquitous energy conversions from state differences of protons and electrons. These overwhelming diversity and ubiquitous presence of quantum processes at all levels of biological life provides strong hints for quantum-based foundations of biological evolution. A quantum-based interpretation of biological processes facilitates—scale independent—comparisons of biological phenomena over all realms of organic life. However, the final textbook of quantum biological evolution is still not written, so it is only possible to formulate preliminary hypotheses.
Chapter
Full-text available
One of the most remarkable and mysterious elements of sea turtle biology is the ability of turtles to return to nest in the same geographic area from which they originated. This behavior, often referred to as natal homing, is particularly astonishing because many sea turtles migrate long distances away from their home areas before returning. Explaining how turtles leave a beach as hatchlings and then, years later, locate the same area of coastline after traveling immense distances through the open sea has posed a daunting challenge for biologists who have long struggled to explain the phenomenon without invoking magic.
Article
Full-text available
We report a palaeomagnetic investigation of the last full geomagnetic field reversal, the Matuyama-Brunhes (M-B) transition, as preserved in a continuous sequence of exposed lacustrine sediments in the Apennines of Central Italy. The palaeomagnetic record provides the most direct evidence for the tempo of transitional field behaviour yet obtained for the M-B transition. 40Ar/39Ar dating of tephra layers bracketing the M-B transition provides high-accuracy age constraints and indicates a mean sediment accumulation rate of about 0.2 mm yr–1 during the transition. Two relative palaeointensity (RPI) minima are present in the M-B transition. During the terminus of the upper RPI minimum, a directional change of about 180 ° occurred at an extremely fast rate, estimated to be less than 2 ◦ per year, with no intermediate virtual geomagnetic poles (VGPs) documented during the transit from the southern to northern hemisphere. Thus, the entry into the Brunhes Normal Chron as represented by the palaeomagnetic directions and VGPs developed in a time interval comparable to the duration of an average human life, which is an order of magnitude more rapid than suggested by current models. The reported investigation therefore provides high-resolution integrated palaeomagnetic and radioisotopic data that document the fine details of the anatomy and tempo of the M-B transition in Central Italy that in turn are crucial for a better understanding of Earth’s magnetic field, and for the development of more sophisticated models that are able to describe its global structure and behaviour.
Article
Full-text available
Animals navigate using a variety of sensory cues, but how each is weighted during different phases of movement (e.g. dispersal, foraging, homing) is controversial. Here, we examine the geomagnetic and olfactory imprinting hypotheses of natal homing with datasets that recorded variation in the migratory routes of sockeye (Oncorhynchus nerka) and pink (Oncorhynchus gorbuscha) salmon returning from the Pacific Ocean to the Fraser River, British Columbia. Drift of the magnetic field (i.e. geomagnetic imprinting) uniquely accounted for 23.2% and 44.0% of the variation in migration routes for sockeye and pink salmon, respectively. Ocean circulation (i.e. olfactory imprinting) predicted 6.1% and 0.1% of the variation in sockeye and pink migration routes, respectively. Sea surface temperature (a variable influencing salmon distribution but not navigation, directly) accounted for 13.0% of the variation in sockeye migration but was unrelated to pink migration. These findings suggest that geomagnetic navigation plays an important role in long-distance homing in salmon and that consideration of navigation mechanisms can aid in the management of migratory fishes by better predicting movement patterns. Finally, given the diversity of animals that use the Earth's magnetic field for navigation, geomagnetic drift may provide a unifying explanation for spatio-temporal variation in the movement patterns of many species.
Article
Full-text available
We quantified the relative influence of maternal fidelity to feeding grounds and natal fidelity to breeding grounds on the population structure of humpback whales Megaptera novae -angliae based on an ocean-wide survey of mitochondrial (mt) DNA diversity in the North Pacific. For 2193 biopsy samples collected from whales in 10 feeding regions and 8 breeding regions dur-ing the winter and summer of 2004 to 2006, we first used microsatellite genotyping (average, 9.5 loci) to identify replicate samples. From sequences of the mtDNA control region (500 bp) we identified 28 unique haplotypes from 30 variable sites. Haplotype frequencies differed markedly among feeding regions (overall F ST = 0.121, Φ ST = 0.178, p < 0.0001), supporting previous evidence of strong maternal fidelity. Haplotype frequencies also differed markedly among breeding regions (overall F ST = 0.093, Φ ST = 0.106, p < 0.0001), providing evidence of strong natal fidelity. Although sex-biased dispersal was not evident, differentiation of microsatellite allele frequencies was weak compared to differentiation of mtDNA haplotypes, suggesting male-biased gene flow. Feeding and breeding regions showed significant differences in haplotype frequencies, even for regions known to be strongly connected by patterns of individual migration. Thus, the influence of migra-tory fidelity seems to operate somewhat independently on feeding and breeding grounds over an evolutionary time scale. This results in a complex population structure and the potential to define multiple units to conserve in either seasonal habitat.
Article
Restriction-site analyses of mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA) from the loggerhead sea turtle (Caretta caretta) reveal substantial phylogeographic structure among major nesting populations in the Atlantic, Indian, and Pacific oceans and the Mediterranean sea. Based on 176 samples from eight nesting populations, most breeding colonies were distinguished from other assayed nesting locations by diagnostic and often fixed restriction-site differences, indicating a strong propensity for natal homing by nesting females. Phylogenetic analyses revealed two distinctive matrilines in the loggerhead turtle that differ by a mean estimated sequence divergence p = 0.009, a value similar in magnitude to the deepest intraspecific mtDNA node (p = 0.007) reported in a global survey of the green sea turtle Chelonia mydas. In contrast to the green turtle, where a fundamental phylogenetic split distinguished turtles in the Atlantic Ocean and the Mediterranean Sea from those in the Indian and Pacific oceans, genotypes representing the two primary loggerhead mtDNA lineages were observed in both Atlantic-Mediterranean and Indian-Pacific samples. We attribute this aspect of phylogeographic structure in Caretta caretta to recent interoceanic gene flow, probably mediated by the ability of this temperate-adapted species to utilize habitats around southern Africa. These results demonstrate how differences in the ecology and geographic ranges of marine turtle species can influence their comparative global population structures.
Chapter
In the past decade, it has become apparent that the importance of magnetic interactions is not confined to the world of physics but extends into the realm of physiology and whole organism biology. That this should be so follows from two elementary observations: (1) by virtue of their magnetic moments and electrical charges, the atoms and ions which make up an organism can magnetically interact with the organism’s environment, and (2) the environment of practically every organism includes a highly ordered and stable (relative to the lifetime of the organism) geomagnetic field which contains both spatial and temporal information of potential value to the organism.
Article
Restriction-site analyses of mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA) from the loggerhead sea turtle (Caretta caretta) reveal substantial phylogeographic structure among major nesting populations in the Atlantic, Indian, and Pacific oceans and the Mediterranean sea. Based on 176 samples from eight nesting populations, most breeding colonies were distinguished from other assayed nesting locations by diagnostic and often fixed restriction-site differences, indicating a strong propensity for natal homing by nesting females. Phylogenetic analyses revealed two distinctive matrilines in the loggerhead turtle that differ by a mean estimated sequence divergence p = 0.009, a value similar in magnitude to the deepest intraspecific mtDNA node (p = 0.007) reported in a global survey of the green sea turtle Chelonia mydas. In contrast to the green turtle, where a fundamental phylogenetic split distinguished turtles in the Atlantic Ocean and the Mediterranean Sea from those in the Indian and Pacific oceans, genotypes representing the two primary loggerhead mtDNA lineages were observed in both Atlantic-Mediterranean and Indian-Pacific samples. We attribute this aspect of phylogeographic structure in Caretta caretta to recent interoceanic gene flow, probably mediated by the ability of this temperate-adapted species to utilize habitats around southern Africa. These results demonstrate how differences in the ecology and geographic ranges of marine turtle species can influence their comparative global population structures.
Article
Monitoring trends in loggerhead turtle popu-lations is critical to assessing population status and to developing and assessing conservation strategies. Presently, the most reliable estimates of sea turtle population size come from counts on nesting beaches. In addition to providing population estimates, nesting beach data also provide information on how reproductive effort is focused spatially and temporally. Several mea-sured parameters are key to describing repro-ductive effort and to estimating the number of nesting females from nest count data. Among these parameters are clutch frequency, remigra-tion interval, and nesting site fidelity (collec-tively referred to here as nesting patterns). These three key measures are intimately linked and have great bearing on the accuracy of the simple calculations used to derive nesting population estimates from numbers of nests. Although numerous authors have reported clutch frequency and remigration interval values for loggerheads at nesting beaches around the world, information on nesting site fidelity is frequent in the literature, perhaps because the difficulty of obtaining this measure. Effective conservation programs for turtles require more than an understan nesting patterns and nesting populatio With regard to the adult life stage, info on migratory routes and foraging areas needed. The purpose of this chapter is line the loggerhead reproductive data for guiding conservation progr authors review available data on log nesting patterns, reproductive migra adult foraging areas, and they cies in understanding these aspect head life history.