... Social perceptions of faces influence a variety of important social outcomes (e.g., Fiske et al., 2007;Little et al., 2011;Vernon et al., 2014;Todorov et al., 2015) and are made rapidly (e.g., Todorov et al., 2005;Willis and Todorov, 2006;Engell et al., 2007;Carre et al., 2009;Olivola and Todorov, 2010), even, on some dimensions, when irrelevant to the task at hand (Ritchie et al., 2017). Complementing work on the role of social and physical dominance in leadership emergence and effectiveness (e.g., Rule and Ambady, 2008;Wong et al., 2011;Blaker et al., 2013;Hamstra, 2014;Pillemer et al., 2014;Rule and Tskhay, 2014;, see Watkins, 2018 for a recent review), first impressions of dominance and competence can guide leadership choice based on facial cues alone (Todorov et al., 2005;Ballew and Todorov, 2007;Little et al., 2007;Antonakis and Dalgas, 2009;Re et al., 2012Re et al., , 2013Olivola et al., 2014;Re and Perrett, 2014). Preferences for such traits in leaders may function to accrue fitness benefits for group members, if dominant, prestigious and/or intelligent leaders have the necessary leverage to represent or protect their group in exchanges with out-groups (see, e.g., Van Vugt et al., 2007;MacDonald et al., 2012;Spisak et al., 2012; for discussion), or to maintain cohesion, resolve conflict and/or enforce punishment within groups (see Watkins, 2018 for discussion). ...