ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Serenity has been defined as a positive emotion that reflects feelings of inner peace and confidence. The development of serenity constitutes a valuable resource that enables children to cope with life stressors and to establish positive relationships with others. The present study examined serenity and possible gender group differences in Argentinean children. The second goal was to investigate the relation between serenity and prosocial and aggressive behaviors. Participants were 615 children aged 9 to 13 years (276 boys, 339 girls), from 11 public primary schools in the city of Buenos Aires, Argentina. Gender analyses showed no differences between boys and girls in serenity scores. As expected, serenity proved to be positively related to prosocial behaviors and negatively related to physical and verbal aggression. These results indicate that children who describe themselves as more serene are also prone to give help and comfort to others. Moreover, serene children appear to be less aggressive towards classmates. Evidence suggests that peer relations can be enhanced by the utilization of serenity skills in the school context.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Journal of Latino/Latin American Studies
2014, 6(1), 31-39
Serenity and Its Relationship to Prosocial and Aggressive
Behaviors in Argentinean Children
Serenity has been defined as a positive emotion that reflects feelings of inner peace and confidence.
The development of serenity constitutes a valuable resource that enables children to cope with life
stressors and to establish positive relationships with others. The present study examined serenity and
possible gender group differences in Argentinean children. The second goal was to investigate the
relation between serenity and prosocial and aggressive behaviors. Participants were 615 children
aged 9 to 13 years (276 boys, 339 girls), from 11 public primary schools in the city of Buenos Aires,
Argentina. Gender analyses showed no differences between boys and girls in serenity scores. As
expected, serenity proved to be positively related to prosocial behaviors and negatively related to
physical and verbal aggression. These results indicate that children who describe themselves as
more serene are also prone to give help and comfort to others. Moreover, serene children appear to
be less aggressive towards classmates. Evidence suggests that peer relations can be enhanced by the
utilization of serenity skills in the school context.
Keywords: Serenity, Prosocial behaviors, Aggression, Children
Serenity has been defined as a positive
emotion that reflects feelings of inner peace
and confidence, and that implies the harmony
of body and mind (Fredrickson & Levenson,
1998; Fredrickson, Mancuso, Branigan, &
Tugade, 2000). Serenity is not always being
happy; rather it is being able to maintain an
inner calm despite negative life events
(Roberts & Whall, 1996).
Within the positive psychology
framework, Fredrickson and colleagues
(Fredrickson, 2003; Fredrickson et al., 2000;
Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998) proposed that
experiencing positive emotions can lead to
states of mind and to modes of behavior that
prepare an individual for later difficult times.
It has been observed that positive emotions,
compared to neutral or negative states, can
promote a broader thought-action repertoire
by stimulating the individual to pursue a
wider range of thoughts and actions.
According to Fredrickson and Branigan
(2005), broadened thought-action repertoires
play an essential role in the development of
enduring personal resources (e.g., health,
physical skills, executive control, resilience,
creativity, and social support networks). With
reference to serenity, research shows that
experiencing this emotion can reduce
physiological arousal (e.g., decrease
cardiovascular acceleration) after enduring a
negative event (Fredrickson et al. 2000;
Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998). Moreover, it
has been observed that the elicitation of
serenity and contentment by watching a film
clip broadens the scope of attention in a visual
processing task and increases thought-action
repertoires (Fredrickson & Branigan, 2005).
The study of serenity as a positive
emotion is a relatively new area of research,
and empirical studies concerning its relation
with other psychological variables are scarce.
However, since the emergent assessment of
positive emotions with psychometric
questionnaires and scales, serenity has begun
Marina I. Cuello
Universidad Católica Argentina
CIIPME-CONICET
Laura B. Oros
CIPCA, Universidad Adventista del Plata
CIIPME-CONICET
Marina I. Cuello, Department of Psychology, Universidad
Católica Argentina; Centro Interdisciplinario de
Investigaciones en Psicología Matemática y Experimental
(CIIPME) - Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas
y Técnicas (CONICET). Correspondence concerning this
paper should be addressed to Marina I. Cuello, Centro
Interdisciplinario de Investigaciones en Psicología
Matemática y Experimental, Teniente Gral. Juan D. Perón
2158- Buenos Aires Argentina. 54(11) 4953-1477/3541. E-
mail: marikarak@yahoo.com; marina.cuello@conicet.gov.ar
Laura B. Oros, Centro de Investigación en Psicología y
Ciencias Afines (CIPCA), Universidad Adventist a del Plata;
CIIPME-CONICET.
JOURNAL OF LATINO/LATIN AMERICAN STUDIES, 2014, 6(1), 31-39 32
to be measured in a more accurate manner
(Oros, 2011; Regner, 2009; Roberts & Aspy,
1993; Schmidt, 2008). Peterson and Park
(2004), for example, showed that adults
scoring high on a measure of serenity also
score high in a scale of spirituality and in a
scale of forgiveness. Other work carried out
by Kreitzer and colleagues (Kreitzer, Gross,
Waleekhachonloet, Reilly-Spong, & Byrd,
2009) in an adult clinical population, found
that those patients with high levels of serenity
also presented high levels of positive affect,
mindful awareness, and quality of life. On the
other hand, high levels of serenity were
inversely related to negative affect, anxiety,
depression, perceived stressors, and health-
related distress.
Serenity can be perceived as a very
effective tool for coping with stress or other
life challenges. It is believed that the capacity
to be relaxed when facing difficult
circumstances facilitates reflection,
predisposes one to take action, and can
increase receptivity and creativity (Nadeau,
2001; Roberts & Cunningham, 1990).
Moreover, psychological research shows that
positive emotional experiences can facilitate
the development of beliefs, attitudes, and
constructive behaviors to manage stress
(Connors, Toscova, & Tonigan, 1999). For
these reasons, serenity is considered to have a
significant role in adaptive social functioning
in childhood, as it can inhibit aggressive
behaviors and facilitate positive responses in
interpersonal relations. For example, a study
carried out in Argentina by Richaud de Minzi
and Oros (2009) found that serene children
presented a lower tendency to solve conflicts
aggressively and were more inclined to react
in an assertive manner, compared to less
serene children. In other research, Schmidt
(2008) studied the correlation between
positive emotions and coping strategies in
Argentinean adolescents. This author found
that tranquility was a good predictor of
functional coping; those adolescents with
higher scores in tranquility obtained lower
scores on emotional discharge and
somatization. In other words, when facing a
conflict, participants who generally
experienced sentiments of peace and comfort
tended to remain in control, thus being able to
think about different alternatives to solve their
problems in a more appropriate way.
Children can face several stressful events
in different periods of their lives, and when
they do, many of them use inadequate or
ineffective coping strategies (Ryan-Wenger,
1992; Trianes Torres, 2002). For example,
they may respond aggressively or be
incapable of expressing their emotions in
appropriate ways. Evidence suggests that
aggressive behaviors can negatively affect
children’s general functioning, inhibit their
learning, and damage relationships with
family, teachers, and peers (Del Barrio &
Carrasco, 2009; Levinson, 2006; Marín
Sanchez, 2002). Also, aggressive behaviors
can produce serious psychological
consequences both in victims and aggressors
(Crick & Grotpeter, 1995; Nansel, Craig,
Overpeck, Saluja, & Ruan, 2004).
Numerous studies have found that the
personal variables most related to children’s
aggressive behavior include impulsivity,
emotional instability, difficult temperament,
attention deficit, hyperactivity, poor skills
related to conflict management, and lack of
social skills (Ayala Velázquez, Pedroza
Cabrera, Morales Chainé, Chaparro Caso-
López, & Barragán Torres, 2002; Caprara &
Pastorelli, 1993; Del Barrio & Carrasco,
2009; Del Barrio & Roa, 2006; Dorado Mesa
& Jané Ballabriga, 2001; Farrington, 2005;
Justicia et al., 2006). Interestingly, gender is
another personal feature associated with
children’s aggression; most studies have
shown that, as a group, boys exhibit
significantly higher levels of verbal and
physical aggression than girls (Card, Stucky,
Sawalani, & Little, 2008; Carlo, Raffaelli,
Laible, & Meyer, 1999; Crick & Grotpeter,
1995; Galen & Underwood, 1997; Samper,
Aparici, & Mestre, 2006).
In contrast, research show that a
controlled emotionality, empathy, and
prosocial behaviors serve as protective factors
against aggressive impulses (Mestre, Samper,
& Frías, 2002; Samper et al., 2006). It is
believed that prosocial behaviors (i.e.,
voluntary acts intended to benefit or help
others) constitute a protective factor against
aggressiveness, by favoring social adaptation
and social skills (Lemos, 2009). In addition,
there is growing recognition that the
development of empathy (i.e., the capability
to understand and share other people’s
emotions) is a fundamental pre-requisite for
the emergence of prosocial behaviors (Carlo,
Mestre, Samper, Tur, & Armenta, 2010;
Eisenberg & Fabes, 1998; Mestre, Frías, &
Samper, 2004; Roberts & Strayer, 1996).
Some investigations suggest that individual
differences in prosocial behaviors could be
33 CUELLO & OROS/SERENITY IN ARGENTINEAN CHILDREN
attributable to differences in emotionality and
impulsivity. In Spain, Mestre, Samper, and
Frías (2002, 2004) found that emotional
instability and impulsiveness constitute the
main aggressiveness predictors in
adolescents. In contrast, nonimpulsive,
positive and empathic emotionality was the
best predictor for adolescents’ prosocial
behavior.
It seems, then, that children and
adolescents who are more emotionally
unstable and have fewer skills to restrain
impulsivity would be prone to behave
aggressively, while more empathic and
emotionally controlled subjects would be
more prosocial. In this sense, serenity might
have an important adaptive social function, as
it promotes the substitution of aggressive and
dysfunctional actions for more adequate ones
(Nadeau, 2001). With this idea in mind, some
interventions have been carried out in an
attempt to promote positive socio-emotional
development and diminish violence in
elementary schools, by teaching children to
relax and to employ effective stress control
strategies (Gilbert & Orlick, 2002; Oros,
2008; Taylor & Orlick, 2004). For instance,
Gilbert and Orlick implemented a Positive
Living Skills program in an elementary
school. During the intervention period, the
experimental group was taught different
relaxation techniques and essential concepts
about stress and how to control it. Children
were also taught to identify positive things in
their lives and how to increase the frequency
of these “highlights.” The results showed that
the children in the experimental group learned
to relax at will, and significantly increased the
frequency of their highlights, while children
in the control group did not. Also, these
learned positive coping strategies were
generalized to a variety of situations besides
the intervention sessions (e.g., at school, at
home, with peers, in sports, and when going
to sleep).
In adolescents, Nickel et al. (2005) tested
the effectiveness of a progressive muscle
relaxation program. The results showed that
all stressed aggressive adolescents who
participated in the study experienced a
significantly positive change, as shown by the
measures obtained on the State-Anger, Trait-
Anger, Anger-Out and Anger-Control scales
of the STAXI, as well as in social
functioning, role-emotional, and mental
health scales.
In Argentina, Oros (2008) implemented
an intervention model to prevent
aggressiveness and foster calmness in a group
of six-year old children who attended an
elementary school included in the Federal
Plan for 1,000 Schools in poverty.1 The
intervention strategies were implemented
within the classroom and included, among
others, modeling, positive reinforcement,
mental and physical relaxation techniques,
breathing training, planned games, cost-
benefits analysis, searching for alternatives,
narrations, and behavioral practice. The
relaxation model significantly reduced
disruptive behaviors and promoted a
progressive acquisition of more adapted
responses to interpersonal stressful events. In
the first assessment, previous to the
intervention, not a single child mentioned
specific attempts to relax or calm down
before reacting to an insult or an offense;
however, in the final assessment, 43% of
children’s responses included an attempt to
use relaxation techniques before reacting
impulsively. These results show that the
promotion of positive emotions, such as
serenity, can improve socio-emotional
development and peer relations, even from
early ages.
To summarize, all the studies mentioned
above confirm that the development of
serenity constitutes a valuable and healthy
resource that enables people to cope with life
stressors and to establish positive
relationships with others. Interventions
carried out at schools demonstrate that it is
possible and desirable to educate children in
the development and maximization of their
inner calm. Also, research shows that
teaching children how to manage anxiety and
impulsivity in stressful situations can have
beneficial and long-lasting effects on their
socio-emotional development and
interpersonal relations with others. The
investigations performed with children and
adolescents, although scarce, allow us to
hypothesize that serene youngsters might be
less aggressive and more prosocial than
subjects who are less serene. However, and to
the point of this study, the relation between
serenity and prosocial and aggressive
behaviors in children remains unknown.
1 This plan was included in the National Program for
Educational Equality, designed by the Ministry of Education
of Argentina (2004), in order to support educational
institutions that work with children at greater social
vulnerability, throughout the national territory.
JOURNAL OF LATINO/LATIN AMERICAN STUDIES, 2014, 6(1), 31-39 34
Gender differences in children’s serenity also
remain as an unexplored area of research.
The present study aimed to provide
further information about children’s socio-
emotional development in Latin America. The
main purpose was to examine serenity in
Argentinean children and analyze possible
gender differences, utilizing a self-report
instrument developed by Oros (2011). Our
second goal was to analyze the relationship
between serenity and prosocial behaviors,
using the Prosocial scale developed by
Caprara and Pastorelli (1993). Finally, we
examined the relationship between serenity
and aggressive behaviors, using the Physical
and Verbal Aggression scale developed by
Caprara and Pastorelli (1993). We expected to
find a positive relation between serenity and
prosocial behaviors, and an inverse relation
between serenity and aggression.
Furthermore, based on aforementioned
aggression studies, we hypothesized that girls
would obtain higher scores on serenity.
Showing how serenity relates to both
prosocial and aggressive behaviors may
provide a finer understanding of serenity in
childhood and its possible implications in
social interactions.
Method
Participants
Participants were 615 primary school
students (276 boys, 339 girls) aged 9 to 13
years (M = 11.16 years, SD = 0.97), from
public schools located in the city of Buenos
Aires, Argentina. Students were in grades 5
(34.31%), 6 (33.33%), and 7 (32.36%). All
children were native Spanish speakers, and
came from middle-class backgrounds.
Measures
Serenity. The Serenity Scale was
developed by Oros (2011) to assess children’s
capability to remain calm when facing
stressful situations (e.g., “When someone
upsets me, I try to calm down”) and their
perceptions of their own serenity in everyday
life (e.g., “I like being calm”).
This self-report measure comprises 11
items rated on a 3-point scale (1 = No, 2 =
Sometimes, 3 = Yes). Internal consistency of
the instrument was good, as shown by
Cronbach’s alpha (α = .81), and all items
proved to be discriminative, p < .001.
Prosocial behaviors. Prosocial behaviors
were assessed with the Prosocial Behavior
Scale (Caprara & Pastorelli, 1993; Del Barrio,
Moreno, & López, 2001). The original items
of this self-report measure are 10, plus five
control items that are not included in the final
scores. The items offer a description of a
child’s behavior denoting altruism, trust, and
agreeableness. Participants are asked to
indicate the degree to which they engage in
different types of prosocial behaviors (1 =
Never, 2 = Sometimes, 3 = Always; e.g., I
help my classmates do their homework”).
We included 4 extra items that were
representative of Argentinean children’s
prosocial behaviors: “I lend my school
supplies,” “If somebody accidentally drops
things on the floor, I help him/her to pick
them up,” “I share my sweets with others,”
and “If other person mess the place up, I help
him/her clean.” These items frequently appear
as explicit codes of conduct at schools, and
added more information to our study.
This extended version of the instrument
proved to have stronger internal consistency
(α = .77) than the instrument with the original
items only (α = .71). All items proved to be
discriminative, p < .001.
Physical and verbal aggression.
Aggression was assessed with the Physical
and Verbal Aggression Scale (Caprara &
Pastorelli, 1993; Del Barrio, Moreno, &
López, 2001). The items offer a description of
the child’s behavior aimed at hurting others
physically (e.g., ‘‘I kick or punch”) and
verbally (e.g., “I say bad things about other
kids”).
This self-report scale comprises 15 items
rated on a 3-point scale (1 = Never, 2 =
Sometimes, 3= Always), plus five control
items that are not included in the final scores.
Cronbach’s alpha for the present study was
satisfactory (α = .89).
Procedure
The study was carried out in 11 public
schools from the city of Buenos Aires,
Argentina, with permission from school
authorities. Recruitment letters were sent to
parents with the cooperation of school
personnel. Active parental consent and
student assents were obtained prior to
participation.
The measures were completed in the
classrooms during 30-minute sessions
(maximum group size of 30 students). Student
35 CUELLO & OROS/SERENITY IN ARGENTINEAN CHILDREN
participation was voluntary. Participants were
assured of anonymity and confidentiality and
were asked to complete the instruments
individually. One investigator was present in
the classroom to answer participants' questions
and to ensure that the instructions were
followed correctly.
Results
An ANOVA was performed to examine
gender differences in serenity. Unexpectedly,
no significant differences were found between
boys (M = 23.28; SD = 0.29) and girls (M =
23.51; SD = 0.26) in the serenity scale, F < 1.
Total scores for the serenity scale range
between 11 and 33 points. Overall,
participants presented a moderate level of
serenity (M = 23.41; SD = 4.75).
A series of linear regression analyses were
conducted to explore the contribution of
children’s serenity to prosocial and aggressive
behaviors in interpersonal situations. As
expected, serenity proved to be positively
related to prosocial behaviors, β = .38, t =
10.20; p < .001, R² = .15. Separate analyses
for girls and boys revealed highly similar
results for both girls, β = .39, t = 7.82; p <
.001, R² = .15, and boys, β = .38, t = 6.77; p <
.001, R² = .14.
On the other hand, it was observed that
serenity was negatively related to physical and
verbal aggression, β = -.48, t = -13.42; p <
.001, R² = .23, and these results were similar
for girls, β = -.52, t = -11.18; p < .001, R² =
.27, and boys, β = -.45, t = -8.43; p < .001, R²
= .21. These outcomes thus indicate that
greater serenity is associated with a weaker
tendency to utilize physical and verbal
aggression in interpersonal relationships.
Additional descriptive information about
the relationship between serenity and social
behaviors is provided in Table 1. More
specifically, we calculated the means and
standard deviations for children’s prosocial and
aggressive behaviors as a function of their
serenity level. Serenity scores were divided into
three levels: high, moderate, and low.
To sum up, the evidence suggests that
children who report greater serenity are also
prone to give help and comfort to others.
Additionally, serene children appear to be less
aggressive towards schoolmates, compared to
less serene participants (see Figure 1).
_____________________________________
Discussion
Serenity is a positive emotion that has
received relatively little attention in empirical
research, despite the enormous psychological
value that has been ascribed to it. The purpose
of the present work was to make a
preliminary step towards the study of
children’s serenity in Latin America,
analyzing possible gender differences and
establishing its association with two opposite
social behaviors: prosocial and aggressive.
Regarding the first objective, we found
that Argentinean boys and girls presented
similarly moderate values of serenity; there
was no evidence of a gender difference. Our
hypothesis that girls would obtain higher
scores emerged from previous findings
concerning certain constructs that are
opposite to a serene state, which are generally
more prevalent in boys. For example,
variables such as emotional instability,
impulsivity, and hyperactivity tend to be
higher in boys than in girls (Rucklidge, 2010;
Samper, Tur, Mestre, & Cortés, 2008;
Sotomonte Ariza, Jiménez Mendoza, &
Cárdenas Niño, 2011; Trianes Torres,
Cardelle-Elawar, Blanca Mena, & Muñoz
Sánchez, 2003; Walker, 2005). One
alternative idea that could explain the lack of
___________________________________________________________________
Table 1
Means and Standard Deviations for Prosocial and
Aggressive Behaviors by Serenity Level
Serenity level
Low Moderate High
Social (n = 206) (n = 244) (n = 164)
behavior M SD M SD M SD
Prosocial 23.15 3.23 24.20 2.88 25.48 2.84
Aggressive 26.57 6.21 22.91 5.02 20.40 5.16
Figure 1. Means for prosocial and aggressive
behaviors as a function of serenity level.
JOURNAL OF LATINO/LATIN AMERICAN STUDIES, 2014, 6(1), 31-39 36
differences in serenity between boys and girls
could be the fact that, although girls present
weaker impulsivity and hyperactivity than
boys, they tend to experience more
internalizing problems (Galambos,
Leadbeater, & Barker, 2004; Keenan & Shaw,
1997). That is, girls tend to show more
concern, anxiety, and depression than boys
(Pérez García & Giménez Barbero, 2011;
Reyes & Mora, 2007; Southam-Gerow &
Kendall, 2002) especially as they come close
to puberty and adolescence (Petersen,
Sarigiani, & Kennedy, 1991). Such qualities
could be incompatible with the ability to
maintain an inner calm despite negative life
events (i.e., serenity), which could partially
explain why there were no gender differences
in serenity in our study. This idea should be
considered in future studies, for example, by
analyzing the relation between children’s
depression, anxiety, and serenity.
Moreover, although girls are generally
identified as more passive than boys, in the
sense that they show less externalizing
behaviors, aggression, anger, and hostility
(Carlo et al., 1999; Lacunza, 2010; Méndez,
Hidalgo, & Inglés, 2002; Samper et al., 2008),
this does not necessarily mean that girls have
a greater internal experience of calmness.
These visible differences in behavior could
rather be attributable to the way that boys and
girls express their emotions, due to gender
characteristics, than to the way they
experience them. This idea is in line with
explicative models that question the supposed
aggressive disposition of boys versus the
conciliatory attitude of girls (Reyes & Mora,
2007). For example, some researchers argue
that gender differences in aggression are
highly dependent on the type of aggressive
manifestation that is measured by the
investigator (Bjorkqvist, 1994; Bjorkqvist,
Lagerspetz, & Kaukiainen, 1992; Lagerspetz,
Bjorkqvist, & Peltonen, 1988; Underwood,
2003).
Furthermore, in the present work we used
a brief one-dimensional scale to explore
serenity in boys and girls. The serenity scale
developed by Oros (2011) proved to be
reliable but, in future investigations, it could
be interesting to evaluate different
manifestations of this emotion, as other
researchers have explored in adult population
(Kreitzer et al., 2009; Roberts & Aspy, 1993;
but see Kruse, Heinermann, Moody,
Beckstead, & Conley, 2005, for further
psychometric examinations of the scale). In
this sense it could be appropriate to analyze,
for example, different aspects of serenity
associated with the regulation of positive
emotions, coping with negative events,
feelings of confidence, inner haven, and the
natural tendency to experiment calmness in
order to deeply explore gender profiles.
Regarding the second goal, the study
demonstrated that serenity plays an important
role in the expression of prosocial and
aggressive behaviors. The results indicate that
this emotion could facilitate the development
of prosocial behaviors in childhood. Children
with higher levels of serenity expressed their
disposition to give comfort to someone who
was sad, help a classmate do homework,
share sweets with others, and accompany
friends, among other positive behaviors.
These results are consistent with the idea that
serenity constitutes a high social impact
psychological resource (Oros, 2008). The
tendency of serene children to be caring,
sensitive, and altruistic could be, in part, due
to the fact that serenity, like other positive
emotions, broadens the scope of attention
(Fredrickson & Branigan, 2005; Fredrickson
et al., 2000), thus allowing them to detect
more easily when someone needs assistance.
Also, it has been found that serenity increases
thought-action repertoires (Fredrickson &
Branigan, 2005; Fredrickson et al., 2000),
which could amplify children’s social
responses when facing a stressful situation
(i.e., using functional behaviors instead of
indifference or aggression).
Results also revealed that serenity was
inversely related to physical and verbal
aggression. Serene children reported that they
were less inclined to kick, bite, punch, insult,
and make fun of others. These findings are in
line with the studies performed by Peterson
and Park (2004) and Kreitzer et al. (2009),
who reported that participants scoring high on
serenity also scored high on spirituality and
forgiveness, and presented lower scores on
negative affect, anxiety, and perceived
stressors, when compared to less serene
subjects. Although these two studies were
carried out with adults, it is plausible that
similar results might be found in children.
However this hypothesis needs to be tested in
future investigations.
Other investigations carried out with
Argentinean children support the findings of
the present study. Richaud de Minzi and Oros
(2009) and Schmidt (2008) found that
children’s serenity diminished the probability
37 CUELLO & OROS/SERENITY IN ARGENTINEAN CHILDREN
of behaving aggressively in social
interactions, facilitated assertive responses,
and moderated inadequate emotional
discharge in stressful situations. Furthermore,
if we consider that serenity is closely related
to emotional regulation (as it implies keeping
calm despite negative events), its inverse
relation with aggression is reasonable.
Numerous authors express that emotional
regulation reduces negative affect and inhibits
aggressive reactions (Capella & Mendoza,
2011; Carthy, Horesh, Apter, Edge, & Gross,
2010; DeWall, Baumeister, Stillman, &
Gailliot, 2007; Zeman, Cassano, Perry-
Parrish, & Stegall, 2006).
In general, the present study provides
evidence that serenity is a positive emotion
with adaptive social value, which could allow
children to develop healthy relationships with
significant others, as it may facilitate the
development of prosocial behaviors and
inhibit aggressive reactions. This information
could be useful to plan interventions aiming
to promote serenity during this developmental
stage. However, the study of serenity still
faces many challenges, and present results
open new questions that should be considered
in future investigations. Specifically, it is
advisable to explore the psychological
variables that underlie the processes analyzed
in the present study, for example, emotional
regulation, willingness to forgive, cognitive
ability to decode and interpret others needs,
perception of social stressors, and coping with
negative events, among others. Furthermore,
social interactions and behaviors in school
settings depend also on the characteristics of
the classroom in which each child is
immersed. To promote an ecological
perspective on children’s social behavior, it
would be desirable to analyze to what extent
prosocial and aggressive behaviors are
explained by person variables and how much
they depend on environmental characteristics.
The effect of the interaction between person
and environmental variables could be also
analyzed as well. Given the evidence that
family characteristics and school context
constitute significant predictors of children’s
social behavior (Sandoval, 2006), it could be
useful to use stratified samples to obtain
representative subsamples of children from
different family backgrounds, school courses
and schools, in order to analyze how much
variance depends on individual differences
and how much it relies on family and school.
References
Ayala Velázquez, H., Pedroza Cabrera, F., Morales Chainé,
S., Chaparro Caso-López, A., & Barragán Torres, N.
(2002). Factores de riesgo, factores protectores y
generalización del comportamiento agresivo en una
muestra de niños en edad escolar. Salud Mental,
25(3), 27-40.
Bjorkqvist, K. (1994). Sex differences in physical, verbal,
and indirect aggression: A review of recent research.
Sex Roles, 30, 177-188. doi:10.1007/BF01420988
Bjorkqvist, K., Lagerspetz, K. M. J., & Kaukiainen, A.
(1992). Do girls manipulate and boys fight?
Developmental trends in regard to direct and indirect
aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 18, 117-127. doi:
10.1002/1098-2337
Capella, C., & Mendoza, M. (2011). Regulación emocional
en niños y adolescentes: artículo de revisión.
Nociones evolutivas y clínica psicopatológica. Revista
Chilena de Psiquiatría y Neurología de la infancia y
adolescencia, 22, 155-168.
Caprara, G. V., & Pastorelli, C. (1993). Early emotional
instability, prosocial behaviour, and aggression: Some
methodological aspects. European Journal of
Personality, 7, 19-36. doi:10.1002/per.2410070103
Card, N. A., Stucky, B. D., Sawalani, G. M., & Little, T. D.
(2008). Direct and indirect aggression during
childhood and adolescence: A meta-analytic review of
gender differences, intercorrelations, and relations to
maladjustment. Child Development, 79, 1185-1229.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01184.x
Carlo, G., Mestre, M. V., Samper, P., Tur, A., & Armenta,
B. E. (2010). Feelings or cognitions? Moral
cognitions and emotions as longitudinal predictors of
prosocial and aggressive behaviors. Personality and
Individual Differences, 48, 872-877. doi:
10.1016/j.paid.2010.02.010
Carlo, G., Raffaelli, M., Laible, D. J., & Meyer, K. A.
(1999). Why are girls less physically aggressive than
boys? Personality and parenting mediators of physical
aggression. Sex Roles, 40, 711-729. doi:
10.1023/A:1018856601513
Carthy, T., Horesh, N., Apter, A., Edge, M. D., & Gross, J.
J. (2010). Emotional reactivity and cognitive
regulation in anxious children. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 48, 384-393. doi:
10.1016/j.brat.2009.12.013
Connors, G. J., Toscova, R. T., & Tonigan, J. S. (1999).
Serenity. In W. R. Miller (Ed.), Integrating
spirituality into treatment. Resources for practitioners
(pp. 235-250). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Crick, N. R., & Grotpeter, J. K. (1995). Relational
aggression, gender, and social-psychological
adjustment. Child Development, 66, 710-722.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00900.x
Del Barrio, V., & Carrasco, M. A. (2009). Detección y
prevención de problemas psicológicos emocionales en
el ámbito escolar. Retrieved from
http://www.apega.org/attachments/article/472/Detecci
on-
Prevencion%20problemas%20emocionales%20escuel
a-FOCAD.pdf
Del Barrio, V., Moreno, C., & López, R. (2001). Evaluación
de la agresión e inestabilidad emocional en niños
españoles y su relación con la depresión. Clínica y
Salud, 12(1), 33-50.
Del Barrio, V., & Roa, N. L. (2006). Factores de riesgo y
protección en agresión infantil. Acción Psicológica,
4(2), 39-65.
DeWall, C. N., Baumeister, R. F., Stillman, T. F., &
Gailliot, M. T. (2007). Violence restrained: Effects of
self-regulation and its depletion on aggression.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 62-
76. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.12.005
JOURNAL OF LATINO/LATIN AMERICAN STUDIES, 2014, 6(1), 31-39 38
Dorado Mesa, M., & Jané Ballabriga, M. C. (2001). La
conducta agresiva en preescolares: revisión de factores
implicados y evolución. Psiquiatría.com, 5(1).
Retrieved from
http://www.psiquiatria.com/trastornos_infantiles/la-
conducta-agresiva-en-preescolares-revision-de-
factores-implicados-y-evolucion-2/
Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1998). Prosocial
development. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.),
Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 3: Social,
emotional, and personality development (pp. 701-
778). New York, NY: Wiley.
Farrington, D. P. (2005). Childhood origins of antisocial
behavior. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 12,
177-190. doi:10.1002/cpp.448
Fredrickson, B. L. (2003). The value of positive emotions.
American Scientist, 91, 330-335.
Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive
emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-
actions repertoires. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 313-
332. doi:10.1080/02699930441000238
Fredrickson, B. L., & Levenson, R. W. (1998). Positive
emotions speed recovery from the cardiovascular
sequelae of negative emotions. Cognition and
Emotion, 12, 191-220. doi:
10.1080/026999398379718
Fredrickson, B. L., Mancuso, R. A., Branigan, C., &
Tugade, M. M. (2000). The undoing effect of positive
emotions. Motivation and Emotion, 24, 237-258.
doi:10.1023/A:1010796329158
Galambos, N. L., Leadbeater, B. J., & Barker, E. T. (2004).
Gender differences in and risk factors for depression
in adolescence: A 4-year longitudinal study.
International Journal of Behavioral Development, 28,
16-25. doi:10.1080/01650250344000235
Galen, B. R., & Underwood, M. K. (1997). A
developmental investigation of social aggression
among children. Developmental Psychology, 33, 589-
600. doi:10.1037//0012-1649.33.4.589
Gilbert, J., & Orlick, T. (2002). Teaching skills for stress
control and positive thinking to elementary school
children. Journal of Excellence, 7, 54-66.
Justicia, F., Benítez, J. L., Pichardo, M. C., Fernández, E.,
García, T., & Fernández, M. (2006). Aproximación a
un nuevo modelo explicativo del comportamiento
antisocial. Revista Electrónica de Investigación
Psicoeducativa, 4, 131-150.
Keenan, K., & Shaw, D. (1997). Developmental and social
influences on young girls' early problem behavior.
Psychological Bulletin, 121, 95-113. doi:
10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.95
Kreitzer, M. J., Gross, C., Waleekhachonloet, O., Reilly-
Spong, M., & Byrd, M. (2009). The brief serenity
scale: A psychometric analysis of a measure of
spirituality and well-being. Journal of Holistic
Nursing, 27, 7-16. doi:10.1177/0898010108327212
Kruse, B., Heinermann, D., Moody, L., Beckstead, J., &
Conley, C. (2005). Psychometric properties of the
serenity scale. Journal of Hospice and Palliative
Nursing, 7, 337-344.
Lacunza, A. B. (2010). Las habilidades sociales como
recursos para el desarrollo de fortalezas en la infancia.
Psicodebate, 10, 231-248.
Lagerspetz, K. M. J., Bjorkqvist, K., & Peltonen, T. (1988).
Is indirect aggression typical of females? Gender
differences in aggressiveness in 11-to 12-year-old
children. Aggressive Behavior, 14, 403-414. doi:
10.1002/1098-2337(1988)14:6<403::AID-
AB2480140602>3.0.CO;2-D
Lemos, V. (2009). Evaluación de la eficacia de un programa
para promover la conducta prosocial en niños en
riesgo social por pobreza. In M. C. Richaud & J. E.
Moreno (Eds.), Investigación en ciencias del
comportamiento. Avances iberoamericanos (pp. 137-
152). Buenos Aires: CIIPME-CONICET.
Levinson, M. H. (2006). Anger management and violence
prevention: A holistic solution. ETC: A Review of
General Semantics, 63, 187-199.
Marín Sanchez, M. (2002). Aspectos psicosociales de la
violencia en el contexto educativo. Revista Aula
Abierta, 79, 71-84.
Méndez, F. X., Hidalgo, M. D., & Inglés, C. (2002). The
Matson evaluation of social skills with youngsters:
Psychometric properties of the Spanish translation in
the adolescent population. European Journal of
Psychological Assessment, 18, 30-42. doi:
10.1027//1015-5759.18.1.30
Mestre, V., Frías, M. D., & Samper, P. (2004). La medida
de la empatía: análisis del Interpersonal Reactivity
Index. Psicothema, 16, 255-260.
Mestre, V., Samper, P., & Frías, M. D. (2002). Procesos
cognitivos y emocionales predictores de la conducta
prosocial y agresiva: la empatía como factor
modulador. Psicothema, 14, 227-232.
Mestre, V., Samper, P., & Frías, M. D. (2004). Personalidad
y contexto familiar como factores predictores de la
disposición prosocial y antisocial de los adolescentes.
Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 36, 445-457.
Nadeau, M. (2001). Juegos de relajación para niños de 5 a
12 años. España: Sirio.
Nansel, T. R., Craig, W., Overpeck, M. D., Saluja, G., &
Ruan, W. J. (2004). Cross-national consistency in the
relationship between bullying behaviors and
psychosocial adjustment. Archives of Pediatrics &
Adolescent Medicine, 158, 730-736. doi:
10.1001/archpedi.158.8.730.
Nickel, C., Lahmann, C., Tritt, K., Loew, T. H., Rother, W.
K., & Nickel, M. K. (2005). Stressed aggressive
adolescents benefit from progressive muscle
relaxation: A random, prospective, controlled trial.
Stress and Health, 21, 169-175. doi:10.1002/smi.1050
Oros, L. B. (2008). Promoviendo la serenidad infantil en el
contexto escolar. Experiencias preliminares en una
zona de riesgo ambiental. Interdisciplinaria, 25, 181-
195.
Oros, L. B. (2011). Evaluación de la serenidad en niños.
Poster session presented at the XXXIII Congress of
the Inter American Society of Psychology, Medellín,
Colombia.
Pérez García, M., & Jiménez Barbero, J. A. (2011).
Influencia del género en el desarrollo de trastornos de
conducta. Article presented at the 12th Virtual
Conference on Psychiatry. Retrieved from
http://www.psiquiatria.com/bibliopsiquis/handle/1040
1/2189.
Petersen, A. C., Sarigiani, P. A., & Kennedy, R. E. (1991).
Adolescent depression: Why more girls? Journal of
Youth and Adolescence, 20, 247-271. doi:
10.1007/BF01537611
Peterson, C., & Park, N. (2004). Classification and
measurement of character strengths: Implications for
practice. In P. A. Linley & S. Joseph (Eds.), Positive
psychology in practice (pp. 433-446). Hoboken, NJ:
Wiley.
Regner, E. (2009a). Diseño y validez de un instrumento
para evaluar emociones positivas en personas
adultas. Presented at the IV Iberoamerican
Conference on Positive Psychology, Buenos Aires,
Argentina.
Reyes, M. E., & Mora, C. M. (2007). Regulación emocional
de la ira y diferencias de género en la edad preescolar.
Psicodebate Psicología Cultura y Sociedad, 8, 87-
110.
Richaud de Minzi, M. C., & Oros, L. B. (2009). Emociones
positivas, flexibilidad cognitiva y afrontamiento del
estrés en niños. Article presented at the XXXII
Congress of the Inter American Society of
Psychology, Guatemala.
Roberts, K. T., & Aspy, C. B. (1993). Development of the
Serenity Scale. Journal of Nursing Measurement, 1,
145-164.
39 CUELLO & OROS/SERENITY IN ARGENTINEAN CHILDREN
Roberts, K. T., & Cunningham, G. (1990). Serenity concept
analysis and measurement. Educational Gerontology:
An International Journal, 16, 577-589. doi:
10.1080/0380127900160607
Roberts, K. T., & Whall, A. (1996). Serenity as a goal for
nursing practice. Journal of Nursing Scholarship, 28,
359-364. doi:10.1111/j.1547-5069.1996.tb00388.x
Roberts, W., & Strayer, J. (1996). Empathy, emotional
expressiveness, and prosocial behavior. Child
Development, 67, 449-470. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
8624.1996.tb01745.x
Rucklidge, J. J. (2010). Gender differences in attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder. The Psychiatric Clinics
of North America, 33, 357-373. doi:
10.1016/j.psc.2010.01.006
Ryan-Wenger, N. M. (1992). A taxonomy of children's
coping strategies: A step toward theory development.
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 62, 256-263.
doi:10.1037/h0079328
Samper, P., Aparici, G., & Mestre, V. (2006). La
agresividad auto y heteroevaluada: variables
implicadas. Acción Psicológica, 4, 155-168.
Samper, P., Tur, A. M., Mestre, V., & Cortés, M. T. (2008).
Agresividad y afrontamiento en la adolescencia. Una
perspectiva intercultural. International Journal of
Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 8, 431-440.
Sandoval, J. (2006). Ambiente escolar, familiar y
comunitario en relación con los comportamientos
agresivos y prosociales en niños de 3 a 12 años,
Medellín, Colombia, 2001. Revista Facultad Nacional
de Salud Pública, 24, 30-39. Retrieved from
http://www.scielo.org.co/scielo.php?pid=S0120-
386X2006000100004&script=sci_arttext
Schmidt, C. M. (2008). Construcción de un cuestionario de
emociones positivas en población entrerriana. RIDEP,
26, 117-139.
Sotomonte Ariza, S. M., Jiménez Mendoza, G. L., &
Cárdenas Niño, L. (2011). Construcción y validación
de un instrumento exploratorio que haga parte del
proceso de evaluación del trastorno por déficit de
atención con hiperactividad en niños de las edades de
7 a 10 años de la población de Tunja. Psychologia:
Avances de la Disciplina, 5, 95-105.
Southam-Gerow, M. A., & Kendall, P. C. (2002). Emotion
regulation and understanding: Implications for child
psychopathology and therapy. Clinical Psychology
Review, 22, 189-222. doi:10.1016/S0272-
7358(01)00087-3
Taylor, S., & Orlick, T. (2004). An analysis of a children’s
relaxation/stress control skills program in an
alternative elementary school. Journal of Excellence,
9, 95-113.
Trianes Torres, M. V. (2002). Estrés en la infancia: su
prevención y tratamiento. España: Narcea ediciones.
Trianes Torres, M. V., Cardelle-Elawar, M., Blanca Mena,
M. J., & Muñoz Sánchez, A. M. (2003). Contexto
social, género y competencia social autoevaluada en
alumnos andaluces de 11-12 años. Revista Electrónica
de Investigación Psicoeducativa y Psicopedagógica,
1(2), 37-56. Retrieved from http://www.investigacion-
psicopedagogica.org/revista/articulos/2/espannol/Art_
2_25.pdf
Underwood, M. K. (2003). Social aggression among girls.
New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Walker, S. (2005). Gender differences in the relationship
between young children’s peer-related social
competence and individual differences in theory of
mind. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 166, 297-312.
doi:10.3200/GNTP.166.3.297-312
Zeman, J., Cassano, M., Perry-Parrish, C., & Stegall, S.
(2006). Emotion regulation in children and
adolescents. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral
Pediatrics, 27, 155-168. doi:10.1097/00004703-
200604000-00014
... Se aplicó la Escala de Agresión de Little (16) , la cual ha sido traducida al castellano y adaptada a varios contextos, tal es el caso de Buelga (22) en España y Cuello y Oros. (23) En la investigación se trabaja con la versión propuesta por estas últimas autoras, quienes validaron la prueba para la población de niños argentinos en edad escolar. Las propiedades psicométricas de la escala demostraron una elevada consistencia interna alpha de Cronbach=0,92. ...
... También se modeló la propuesta factorial hallada por Cuello y Oros (23) , donde los ítems se agruparon en dos dimensiones solamente, de acuerdo a la forma en que se presentó la agresividad: directa o relacional. Los resultados obtenidos en la muestra de niños cubanos estudiada tampoco avalaron esta estructura factorial del instrumento (χ2= 818,621 (g.l = 209); p = 0,000; χ2/g.l.= 3,917; GFI = 0,760; CFI = 0,899; TLI = 0,789; RMSEA = 0,105). ...
... Estos resultados indicaron, que en la población de niños cubanos, a diferencia de la validación argentina de Cuello y Oros (23) , resultó importante la diferenciación entre las formas de presentación (directa o indirecta) y las funciones de la agresividad (con fines de autodefensa o instrumentales) como elemento explicativo para el establecimiento de las dimensiones en las que se agrupó la escala. Ello coincidió con la propuesta original de Little, et al. (16) , aunque como elemento distintivo se eliminó la dimensión agresividad pura (y sus ítems correspondientes), donde se hace referencia a una tendencia estable a ser agresivo, como característica personal. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introducción: La agresión en la etapa infanto-juvenil es un problema que incide notablemente en la mayor parte de los países. Resulta frecuente en los niños de etapa escolar, por lo que se hace necesario contar con instrumentos válidos y confiables que permitan su evaluación. Objetivo: Validar la escala de agresividad de Little en escolares cubanos del municipio de Santa Clara, Villa Clara, Cuba. Método: Se asumió un paradigma cuantitativo con un diseño no experimental y un tipo de estudio instrumental. Se empleó un muestreo probabilístico estratificado por medio de una selección aleatoria. La muestra quedó conformada por 266 escolares del municipio Santa Clara, de un universo de 21 106 escolares. Se utilizaron esencialmente técnicas de análisis descriptivo de datos, en particular frecuencias, a su vez estadísticos descriptivos clásicos y técnicas inferenciales (alpha de Cronbach, coeficiente de correlación de Pearson, análisis factorial exploratorio y confirmatorio). Resultados: Se obtuvo una estructura factorial coherente de 4 factores con adecuados índices de bondad de ajuste. No correspondió con la propuesta de los autores de la versión original. Conclusiones: Se corrobora la confiabilidad y validez de la escala de agresividad de Little, así como la factibilidad de su aplicación para evaluar el constructo en niños cubanos de etapa escolar.
... Se aplicó la Escala de Agresión de Little (16) , la cual ha sido traducida al castellano y adaptada a varios contextos, tal es el caso de Buelga (22) en España y Cuello y Oros. (23) En la investigación se trabaja con la versión propuesta por estas últimas autoras, quienes validaron la prueba para la población de niños argentinos en edad escolar. Las propiedades psicométricas de la escala demostraron una elevada consistencia interna alpha de Cronbach=0,92. ...
... También se modeló la propuesta factorial hallada por Cuello y Oros (23) , donde los ítems se agruparon en dos dimensiones solamente, de acuerdo a la forma en que se presentó la agresividad: directa o relacional. Los resultados obtenidos en la muestra de niños cubanos estudiada tampoco avalaron esta estructura factorial del instrumento (χ2= 818,621 (g.l = 209); p = 0,000; χ2/g.l.= 3,917; GFI = 0,760; CFI = 0,899; TLI = 0,789; RMSEA = 0,105). ...
... Cada una de las dimensiones, al igual que la escala global, mostraron una elevada consistencia interna con resultados similares a los mostrados en procesos de validación precedentes, lo que avaló la equivalencia transcultural de la prueba y la confiabilidad de la propuesta teórica-metodológica para la evaluación de la agresividad infantil.Estos resultados indicaron, que en la población de niños cubanos, a diferencia de la validación argentina de Cuello y Oros(23) , resultó importante la diferenciación entre las formas de presentación (directa o indirecta) y las funciones de la agresividad (con fines de autodefensa o instrumentales) como elemento explicativo para el establecimiento de las dimensiones en las que se agrupó la escala. Ello coincidió con la propuesta original de Little, et al.(16) , aunque como elemento distintivo se eliminó la dimensión agresividad pura (y sus ítems correspondientes), donde se hace referencia a una tendencia estable a ser agresivo, como característica personal. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introducción: la agresión en la etapa infanto-juvenil es un problema que incide notablemente en la mayor parte de los países. Resulta frecuentes en los niños de etapa escolar, por lo que se hace necesario contar con instrumentos válidos y confiables que permitan su evaluación. Objetivo: validar la escala de agresividad de Little en escolares cubanos del municipio de Santa Clara, Villa Clara, Cuba. Método: se asumió un paradigma cuantitativo con un diseño no experimental y un tipo de estudio instrumental. Se empleó un muestreo probabilístico estratificado por medio de una selección aleatoria. La muestra quedó conformada por 266 escolares del municipio Santa Clara, de un universo de 21 106 escolares. Se utilizaron esencialmente técnicas de análisis descriptivo de datos, en particular frecuencias, a su vez estadísticos descriptivos clásicos y técnicas inferenciales (alpha de Cronbach, coeficiente de correlación de Pearson, análisis factorial exploratorio y confirmatorio). En la investigación se trabajó con la versión de la escala propuesta por Cuello y Oros. Resultados: se obtuvo una estructura factorial coherente de 4 factores con adecuados índices de bondad de ajuste. No correspondió con la propuesta de los autores de la versión original. Conclusiones: se corrobora la confiabilidad y validez de la escala de agresividad de Little, así como la factibilidad de su aplicación para evaluar el constructo en niños cubanos de etapa escolar.
... Peace of heart is an inner spiritual experience that generates an emotional feeling of harmony (Dewi & Hamzah, 2019;Cuello & Oros, 2014). Peace of heart is an important condition to free the mind's suffering and to go through many obstacles. ...
... Various measurements of peacefulness were developed to measure peacefulness: including peacefulness on environmental perspective (Marafa et al., 2018); nonviolence approach (Mayton, 2014); emphasis on chronic illness (Gittzus et al., 2019); emphasis on anxiety (Wolfradt et al., 2014); emphasis on educational dimension (Arnout, 2019); character strengths and well-being (Soysa et al., 2021;Demirci & Ekşi, 2018); emphasis on children stress (Cuello & Oros, 2014); and construction for Asian-Chinese culture (Lee et al., 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
Peace of heart is an inner spiritual experience that generates an emotional feeling of harmony. Peace of heart (taṭmainn al-qulūb) is an important aspect applicable for the life of the Muslim community as a treatment for the psychological problems of the modern era, mainly due to the absence of peace. Thus far, no research has been done on the development of peace of heart to capture the uniqueness, specific aspect, and distinctive aspect of peace in Muslim community. A total of 744 respondents responded to this research. Six experts were involved to examine item wording in this research (CVR= .67-1.00). This study has found that the TQS consists of two components (al-sukūn and al-yaqīn) with a good loading factor (.687-.884), Cronbach's Alpha (.844), and item consistency (.545-.857). All items had a good difficulty level and appropriate rating. Convergence validity on this scale was indicated by its correlation with The Brief Serenity Scale (r= .612). The criterion-related validity on this scale was indicated by its correlation with dhikr quality (r= .629), Islamic positive religious coping (r= .518), perceived stress (r= -.453), and sensibility (r= .103). Further studies need to be done based on the followings: 1) Examining the measurement on this topic with larger respondents having equal variance; 2) Considering gender, education level, and age in norming study of this scale; 3) Validating with other measurements to add validity information, especially discriminant validity
... Zhang & Zhao, 2021). Evidence shows that serenity is positively associated with prosocial behaviors among children aged 9 to 13 years (Cuello & Oros, 2014). Similarly, a recent meta-analysis indicated that meditation (i.e., practices that involve focusing the mind on a specific object, thought, or activity to achieve mental clarity, calmness, focus, and serenity) has a substantial association with self-reported assessments of prosociality and prosocial behavior toward known people and has a medium association with other-reported assessments of prosociality and prosocial behavior toward strangers (Donald et al., 2019). ...
... Learning English for future opportunities, such as studying abroad, is also seen as a motivating factor (Tarigan, 1990). Calming down and thinking positively are mentioned as personal strategies to overcome fear and anxiety (Cuello & Oros, 2014). Seeking solace through prayer is another coping mechanism mentioned by some students (Keene, 2006). ...
Article
Full-text available
and their solutions in speaking English. The research design of this study was a mixed method with an explanatory sequential design approach. The population of this study was second-grade students, and its sample was two classes consisting of 60 students and 3 English teachers who taught them. The instruments used in this study were questionnaires and interviews. The quantitative data were analyzed using the formula from Sugiyono (2005) f/n x 100%, while the qualitative data was analyzed using the technique by Miles and Huberman (1992:16) data reduction, data display, and conclusion drawing/verifying. The results of this study indicate that students still have problems speaking English, especially psychological problems such as fear of making mistakes, lack of self-confidence, lack of motivation, and anxiety. Therefore, based on the problems above, there are several solutions provided by teachers and students to overcome these problems.
... En la misma línea, la emoción positiva de la serenidad implica una emoción de paz y confianza a pesar de que se experimenten eventos externos adversos (Connors et al., 1999). En lo relativo a la serenidad, una investigación halló que los niños y adolescentes que se percibían con mayor serenidad tenían mayor tendencia a ayudar a los demás (Cuello & Oros, 2014). Otro constructo que mostró estar asociado a las conductas prosociales son las actitudes de perdón hacia una persona que podría haber dañado o causado algún mal a uno mismo. ...
Article
Full-text available
Implementation of the Hero Program to promote empathy, positive emotions, forgiving attitudes and prosocial behavior in Mexican adolescents Implementação do Programa Herói para promover a empatia, emoções positivas, atitudes de perdão e comportamento pró-social em adolescentes mexicanos. R E S U M E N El programa Héroe es una intervención virtual con evidencia empírica para favorecer el desarrollo psicosocial positivo de los adolescentes. El presente estudio tenía como objetivo evaluar su eficacia en un contexto nunca antes explorado, como los adolescentes mexicanos. Los objetivos de este trabajo eran determinar si el programa Héroe era efectivo para incrementar la conducta prosocial, la empatía, las emociones positivas y el perdón en adolescentes mexicanos. Para este fin, se constituyó una muestra de 241 participantes de ambos géneros (50.2% eran mujeres y el 49.8%, varones). Todos los participantes eran estudiantes mexicanos de secundaria de entre 12 y 15 años de edad, pertenecientes a dos escuelas secundarias. Los resultados arrojaron que el programa fue eficaz para promover conducta prosocial hacia los extraños, la empatía (en su dimensión cognitiva y emocional), las emociones positivas y el perdón. Estos hallazgos señalan que la combinación de la psicología y la tecnología, a través del desarrollo de sistemas y aplicaciones, pueden contribuir a que los adolescentes desarrollen su potencial.
Article
Full-text available
Anxiety in communicating in public is a condition in which individuals feel unsafe or unpleasant, followed by physical and psychological reactions due to fears of not being able to adapt or deal with situations when speaking in public that arise before or during public speaking. in public. The purpose of this study is to determine the description of anxiety in communicating in public in adolescents before individual counseling is applied, to determine the implementation of individual counseling in the anxiety of communicating in public among adolescents, and to determine the description of anxiety in communicating in public in adolescents after counseling. individual. This research approach is a qualitative approach with descriptive research methods. The subjects of the study were 3 teenagers. Data collection techniques were observation, interviews, documentation. Data analysis techniques, the Miller and Huberman models, collected data, presented data, and reduced data. The results of this study indicate that the physiological aspects of anxiety in communicating in public in adolescents are shaking, palms are sweaty, heart rate is beating faster, regarding psychological aspects it is difficult to express words when speaking, difficulties in conveying messages smoothly, and from a behavioral aspect namely walking around the room, unable to sit for a long time. The implementation of counseling is carried out in three stages, namely the initial stage, the middle stage, the final stage, counseling is given 6x meetings, showing changes in reducing anxiety when speaking in public, the client is able to overcome situations that make him anxious and can be relieved by suggesting himself everything will be fine. fine.
Article
Full-text available
El propósito de esta investigación de tipo tecnológica psicométrica, fue construir y validar un instrumento que hiciera parte del procesoexploratorio del trastorno por déficit de atención con hiperactividad (TDAH) llamada Escala del Trastorno por Déficit de Atención conHiperactividad (ETDAH). La muestra estuvo conformada por padres y maestros de 315 niños de 7 a 10 años de seis colegios de Tunja. Elinstrumento comprendió una escala para padres con una confiabilidad de 0,93, una validez de constructo de 0,96 y en la escala para maestrosde .97 y .97 respectivamente. El análisis factorial sugirió tres factores en la escala de padres y dos factores en la escala de docentes. Las comparacionespor grupos demostraron la pertinencia de realizar baremos de interpretación según edad y género.
Article
Full-text available
El comportamiento antisocial ha sido objeto de numerosos estudios que han intentado establecer sus causas etiológicas así como los factores de riesgo que ayudan a su mantenimiento a lo largo del desarrollo vital del individuo. En este trabajo, pretendemos ordenar y clarificar los factores de riesgo asociados al nacimiento y desarrollo de las conductas antisociales des-de la infancia hasta la adolescencia. El objetivo final es establecer un modelo explicativo de este tipo de comportamiento que ayude a detectar los factores de riesgo, así como para establecer medidas de intervención específicas para prevenir y/o paliar los efectos derivados de los mismos. En este sentido, observaremos la existencia e influencia de ciertos factores de riesgo tanto del ámbito intrapersonal (temperamento, inteligencia verbal, etc.) como del interpersonal (entorno familiar, estilos de crianza, grupos de iguales, contexto sociocultural, etc.) que tienen diferente peso específico en la génesis y evolución del comportamiento anti-social y que resultan vitales a la hora de establecer programas de intervención.
Article
Full-text available
Social aggression consists of actions directed at damaging another's self-esteem, social status, or both, and includes behaviors such as facial expressions of disdain, cruel gossipping, and the manipulation of friendship patterns. In Study 1, 4th, 7th, and 10th graders completed the Social Behavior Questionnaire; only boys viewed physical aggression as more hurtful than social aggression, and girls rated social aggression as more hurtful than did boys. In the 1st phase of Study 2, girls participated in a laboratory task in which elements of social-aggression were elicited and reliably coded. In the 2nd phase of Study 2, another sample of participants (elementary, middle, and high school boys and girls) viewed samples of socially aggressive behaviors from these sessions. Girls rated the aggressor as more angry than boys, and middle school and high school participants viewed the socially aggressive behaviors as indicating more dislike than elementary school children.
Article
Full-text available
A strong relationship between social competence during childhood and the subsequent psychological functioning has been found. Therefore, learning and practising social skills contribute to the development of psychological strengths in children. The aims of this paper were a) to describe a series of social skills in pre-school children from San Miguel de Tucuman (Argentina) who live under poverty conditions, and b) to identify if the presence of social skills reduces the frequency of occurrence of disruptive behaviour. We worked with a sample of 120 5- year olds from outskirts of San Miguel de Tucuman who attend public schools kindergartens. In the research their parents were administered the Scale of Social Skills (Lacunza, 2005), the Guide for Behaviour Observation (Ison & Fachinelli, 1993) and a sociodemographic survey. The results showed significant statistical differences in the social skills according to the gender and in the dimensions of physical and/or verbal aggression and transgression in the behavioural scale. Children with disruptive behaviour showed fewer social skills, according to their parents’ perception. These results show that social skills in children prevent the occurrence of dysfunctional behaviour, particularly aggressiveness and negativism. These data show that the practice of social behaviour contributes to adaptation, acceptance of others, positive reinforcement, well-being, among other salugenic resources.
Article
Full-text available
This study explored the emotion regulation processes and gender differences between girls and boys of preschooler’s age from Bogota. 48 children between 54 and 60 months old participated in this study, which generated self regulation and social regulation responses associated to the increase of anger of a character. The multivariate analysis showed four types of regulatory behavior that allows a description of regulatory characteristics in preschooler age. There was not find evidence of correlation with gender, gender didn’t establish a behavior’s type. The results are discuss and contrast with the surveys of Koop (1989), Eisenberg, Fabes, Bernzweig, Karbon, Poulin, and Hanish (1993), Brody and Hall (1993) and McCoy and Masters (1985).
Article
Full-text available
Shaunna Taylor is a graduate of the Master's Program in Sport Psychology at the University of Ottawa. She is a former athlete who has been working as a performance and life enhance-ment consultant in a variety of contexts. Shaunna is the mother of three young girls and con-tinues to do consulting work with children, teachers, coaches and athletes, while living the most active and balanced life she can. Terry Orlick had the pleasure of serving as Shaunna's thesis supervisor while she was pur-suing her Master's Degree at the University of Ottawa. Terry is the father of three wonder-fully spirited girls, and in his spare time works as a Professor, applied researcher, writer and consultant. He has devoted much of his life to enhancing the lives of children and high level performers. Abstract The findings of this study clearly showed that the elementary school children who took part in this Relaxation/Stress Control Skills program learned to relax and successfully imple-mented the stress control strategies taught in the program in a variety of real world situations. The classroom teachers who taught the program made adaptations along the way to meet the specific needs of their students. The children and the teachers enjoyed the program activities and felt that it affected them in positive ways. Suggestions are provided for anyone who wants to introduce a positive living skills program to elementary school aged children.
Article
The Values in Action Classification of StrengthsAssessment of the Via StrengthsThe Via Inventory of StrengthsImplications for PracticeConclusion