ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Do nature-deficit routines undermine affinity with the biosphere? We assessed social-ecological features in Stockholm that afford nature experiences and analyzed the accessibility of these natural areas to preschools. We then selected preschools with contrasting accessibilities. The nature routines resulting from differing outdoor possibilities in preschool life were investigated in relation to children's affinity with the biosphere. Preschools with routines closer to nature have children who are more empathetic and concerned for non-human life forms, and more cognitively aware of human-nature interdependence. We conclude that, nature-rich routines in cities significantly correlate with higher children's ability to develop affinity with the biosphere.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool Children in
Stockholm
Author(s): Giusti Matteo, Barthel Stephan and Marcus Lars
Source:
Children, Youth and Environments,
Vol. 24, No. 3, Special Section on Greening Early
Childhood Education (2014), pp. 16-42
Published by: The Board of Regents of the University of Colorado, a body corporate, for the benefit
of the Children, Youth and Environments Center at the University of Colorado Boulder
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.7721/chilyoutenvi.24.3.0016 .
Accessed: 12/01/2015 09:25
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.
.
The Board of Regents of the University of Colorado, a body corporate, for the benefit of the Children, Youth
and Environments Center at the University of Colorado Boulder is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize,
preserve and extend access to Children, Youth and Environments.
http://www.jstor.org
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
2014 Children, Youth and Environments
Children, Youth and Environments 24(3), 2014
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere:
A Case Study of Preschool Children in Stockholm
Giusti Matteo
Stockholm Resilience Centre
Stockholm University
Barthel Stephan
Institute of Urban History and Stockholm Resilience Centre
Stockholm University
Marcus Lars
School of Architecture and the Built Environment
KTH Royal Institute of Technology
Citation: Matteo, Giusti, Barthel Stephan and Marcus Lars (2014). “Nature
Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool Children in
Stockholm.Children, Youth and Environments 24(3): 16-42. Retrieved [date]
from: http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublication?journalCode=chilyoutenvi.
Abstract
Do nature-deficit routines undermine affinity with the biosphere? We assessed
social-ecological features in Stockholm that afford nature experiences and analyzed
the accessibility of these natural areas to preschools. We then selected preschools
with contrasting accessibilities. The nature routines resulting from differing outdoor
possibilities in preschool life were investigated in relation to children’s affinity with
the biosphere. Preschools with routines closer to nature have children who are more
empathetic and concerned for non-human life forms, and more cognitively aware of
human-nature interdependence. We conclude that, nature-rich routines in cities
significantly correlate with higher children’s ability to develop affinity with the
biosphere.
Keywords: nature routine, affinity with the biosphere, extinction of experience,
preschool children, urban design
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 17
Connection with the Biosphere and Urban Routines
The spatial design of a city is a leading factor in how urban dwellers relate to the
physical landscape. With the onset of the Industrial Revolution, anthropocentric
forces have progressively separated urban systems from their surrounding natural
environments (Steffen, Crutzen and McNeill 2007; Steffen et al. 2011). The
commodities that a modern city requires are now not only drawn from the adjacent
regional landscape, but increasingly from the furthest reaches of the planet (Folke
et al. 1997; Barthel et al. 2005; Grimm et al. 2008; Deutsch 2004). While
historically cities were designed to fruitfully co-exist with local and regional
ecosystems, for instance to enable urban agriculture or to benefit from local
sources of waterpower (Mumford 1961; Barthel and Isendahl 2013), today’s urban
dynamics conceal from urban dwellers both presence and importance of the
ecological life-support systems. The socio-technical experience of the urban
landscape has left the vast majority of urban citizens systematically deprived of in
situ nature experiences, especially on a daily basis. Such urban routines can be said
to be affected by an extinction of nature experiences,” which is often negatively
associated with people’s engagement in nature conservation (Miller 2005; Samways
2007; Stokes 2006). The lack of personal nature experiences seems to initiate a
self-reinforcing cycle of disaffection and carelessness for the environment with
direct implications for the emergence of sustainable lifestyles (Pyle 1993). On a
larger scale, such estrangement from the biosphere is considered a systematic
barrier for a decisive shift of civil society towards sustainable development (Folke et
al. 2011; Folke, Holling and Perrings 1996).
On the other hand, the processes that lead someone to engage in environmental
stewardship are found in the early-life establishment of positive affective and
cognitive assumptions towards nature (Stern 2000; Schultz 2000; Dunlap et al.
2000; Stern, Dietz and Kalof 1993; Kals, Schumacher and Montada 1999).
However, the research that investigates the emergence of positive environmental
attitudes relies mostly on retrospective interviews (Chawla 1999) and surveys
(Ewert, Place and Sibthorp 2005; Wells and Evans 2003). Such methodology
highlights how conscious attributes of the human mind shape one’s attitude
towards the environment, but implicitly neglects the unconscious mind. Hence, only
memories of nature experiences that are available to consciousness are taken into
consideration. However, nature experiences that are not available for memory
retrieval, but are the background of one’s routine, may have great significance on
forming the human appreciation of nature. Indeed, the impact of unconscious
contextual factors in shaping one’s mindset is evident in many other aspects of
human life, from parents’ role in children’s development to the function that
advertising plays in society for adults.
In neural processes, persistent experiences play a leading role in how
environmental perceptions come to constitute one’s internal representation of
reality. In academia, the significance of consistent stimuli to shape one’s identity is
recognized, for instance, in the learning process of psychological conditioning, and
increasingly in biological and cognitive neurosciences. Indeed, the persistent
exposure to the same neurological stimulus physically nurtures the same neural
networks (Maletic-Savatic, Malinow and Svoboda 1999). It follows that, persistent
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 18
access to nature might be closely related to the development of a mindset more
neurologically familiar and accustomed with the biosphere. For instance, the
impossibility to interact with ecological dynamics over a long period of time might
nurture the blind assumption that humankind is a detached self-regulating body
separate from the biosphere. The degree to which the human-nature separation is
embedded in one’s mindset is likely to influence one’s cognitive decision-making
process over issues of environmental concern. A persistent and unconscious
deficiency in first-hand nature experiences might therefore be a key barrier for the
initiation of future sustainable practices.
In this study, we intend to complement previous research and shed light on how
long-lasting nature experiences, i.e. nature routines, embedded in the
configurations of urban landscapes relate to the development of an affiliation with
the biosphere. To reveal the relevance of contextual factors in this process the
methodology is carefully designed to address persistency rather than intensity of
nature interactions. First, we utilize spatial accessibility to natural environments as
a medium for the assessment of nature routines. We perform a geographical
evaluation of how nature experiences are distributed in the landscape of our study
area, i.e. the municipality of Stockholm, and evaluate how frequently they are used
for outdoor activities in preschools. Second, we evaluate if differing nature
experiences over four years of preschool life relate to different affinity with the
biosphere in 5-year-old children. In summary, we analyze children’s emotional and
cognitive affinity with the biosphere in relation to nature-rich and nature-deficit
routines.
Research Question and Hypothesis
As humans generally “learn to love what has become familiar” (Orr 2002), or
cannot learn to love what has not become familiar, we assume that the nature-
deficit routines enforced by modern urban landscapes undermine humans’ capacity
to be familiar and eventually affiliate with the biosphere. This article explores
whether individuals’ unconscious appreciation of nature, or one’s degree of
identification with nature, is influenced by a consistent loss of contact with nature
determined by the spatial features of the urban environment. We examine how
consistent access to nature experiences in Stockholm, Sweden affects the
development of emotional, cognitive, and attitudinal affinity with the biosphere. The
research focuses on childhood, when nature experiences are considered to be most
formative (Kahn, Jr. and Kellert 2002; Chawla 1998; 1999; Louv 2005), and on the
urban landscape, where nature experiences are likely to be the most homogeneous
in type and their accessibility likely to be unevenly distributed in the landscape.
The research analyzes the extinction of nature experiences in the surroundings of
urban preschools in Stockholm and relates it to the degree of affinity with the
biosphere that 5-year-old children have developed during four years. Children
during this period spend large amounts of their waking time in preschools (on
average from 8am to 3pm) some of which (14 hours/week) occur outside the gates
of the preschool. The design of preschools itself has already been acknowledged to
be of importance for the children’s indoor learning experience (Gandini 1993), but
does the design of urban outdoors influence children’s understanding of the
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 19
biosphere? More specifically, does the extinction of nature experiences in urban
areas correlate with reduced affinity with the biosphere in preschool children? We
hypothesize that children who have persistent access to nature experiences over a
period of four years, because their preschool is located in a greener area, develop
stronger affinity with the biosphere than those who have limited nature experiences
available in the surroundings of their preschool. We expect the results to indicate a
relationship between the spatial configuration of urban nature in Stockholm and the
degree to which children learn to affiliate with the biosphere.
Theoretical and Methodological Design
The methodological approach is constituted of three interrelated steps; 1)
classification and mapping of nature experiences using pre-existing geographical
data, 2) analysis of preschool accessibility and frequency of use of such nature
experiences, and 3) analysis of affinity with the biosphere in preschool children with
differing accessibility to nature experiences.
Analysis of Spatial Accessibility to Nature Experiences
In order to remain consistent with previous studies in this field, a nature
experience is defined here as a human interaction with non-human species and
natural environments (Finch 2008; Miller 2005; Pyle 1993; Samways 2007).
However, we consider the general distinction between human-made and natural
areas arbitrary because, especially in urban and peri-urban landscapes, there often
is a long history of human land use that has shaped most natural environments
(Barthel, Crumley and Svedin 2013; Berkes, Colding and Folke 2000; Crumley
1994). We acknowledge that the biophysical attributes of the landscape that are
considered natural are to some extent embedded in the local cultural system that
defines the meaning of what is natural (Geertz 1973). Biophysical and cultural
properties are therefore at the core of this study in both the geographical
classification of natural experiences and in the analysis of spatial accessibility. We
map where experiences of nature are present in the city of Stockholm by
synthesizing both social and ecological properties of the urban landscape. The
overall analysis adopts the well-established theoretical and methodological
approach of space syntax that pioneered the convergence of spatial analysis and
spatial cognition (Dalton, Hölscher and Turner 2012; Peponis, Zimring and Choi
1990; Hillier and Hanson 1984; Hillier 1996). Today, this approach has an
extensive body of documented research detailing how cognitive understanding of
the environment can establish behavioral patterns (Koch, Marcus and Steen 2009;
Greene, Reyes and Castro 2012). Our spatial analysis utilizes two methodological
elements of space syntax to assess the distribution of nature experiences in the
municipality of Stockholm: the sociotope map, and the attraction accessibility
algorithm.
The sociotope map categorizes geographical areas of the Stockholm municipality
into sociotopes (Sociotophandboken 2003). Sociotopes are defined and classified
by triangulation of expert evaluation, expert observation and public dialogue, and
are not mutually exclusive (Ståhle 2006). Hence, sociotopes of nature are
delimited topographical areas in Stockholm that synthesize how natural properties
in the landscape are perceived and used by the local civil society (Ståhle 2006). We
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 20
identify the existence of many sociotopes of “nature” and we categorize them in
four different nature experiences: natural recreation, natural beauty, wilderness,
and rurality (Table 1).
Table 1. Sociotopes categorized in four nature experiences
Nature
experience
Recreation
Natural beauty
Wilderness
Rurality
Sociotopes
of “nature”
Playing in water
Playing in nature
Picnicking
Riding
Flower display
Contact with
water bodies
Green oases
Land formations
Panoramas
Forest feeling
Domesticated
animals present
Small-scale
cultivation
Farmer’s market
The accessibility of the preschools to the categorized nature experiences is analyzed
using the attraction accessibility algorithm from the locations of the preschools. This
analytical tool takes into account the walking distances measured on the street
networks, as well as the required amount of changes in directioni.e., perceived
distances (Ståhle, Marcus and Karlström 2005). The accessibility analysis combines
the physical effort, measured as number of meters, and the mental distance,
measured as number of changes in direction, inherent in distances (Hillier 2003).
This approach has proven more successful in predicting both pedestrian and
vehicular movement than regular metric measures of distance (Hillier et al. 1993;
Hillier and Iida 2005).
Our analysis thus begins with an assessment of nature activities, such as playing
with water, having a picnic in the park, being in a peaceful environment, having the
feeling of being in a forest, or being in contact with domesticated animals, that are
accessible in preschool surroundings. This information is then combined with how
frequently the available nature experiences are included in the outdoor curricula of
preschools. By joining the classification of urban green areas in the sociotope map
with our accessibility analysis using GIS, we consider our analysis to be an accurate
assessment of how much nature experiences are available to preschool for outdoor
activities. It is important to notice that, in line with the intention of the study, the
geographical assessment of nature experiences performed here does not rely on
self-reported questionnaires, and therefore does not rely on the participants’ ability
to recall memories and recognize which ones have been the most formative. This
study identifies the degree of nature experiences occurring over a long period of
time using availability, accessibility and frequency of use of geographical areas that
have social-ecological attributes of nature.
Analysis of Affinity with the Biosphere
Affinity with the biosphere is analyzed with measures similar to several other
empirical studies (Clayton 2003; Mayer and Frantz 2004; Nisbet, Zelenski and
Murphy 2008; Schultz 2002; Stern 2000). The emotional components of this
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 21
relationship are included in the assessment to create a holistic depiction of the
participant’s affiliation with nature (Kals, Schumacher and Montada 1999).
However, the methods we apply have been adapted from the aforementioned
empirical studies to better suit the age of the participants. Biospheric, altruistic, and
egoistic values associated with the development of environmental attitudes and
concern, as in the Value-Belief-Norm model (Stern, Dietz and Kalof 1993; Stern
and Dietz 1994), could not be measured because such values are not yet developed
for this age group (Kellert 2002; Bronfenbrenner 1986). We adapt structural-
developmental methods (Kahn, Jr. 1999) that were originally designed to address
children’s conception of the world (Piaget 1960) and that have been successfully
employed in a multitude of studies examining children’s environmental morality
(Kahn, Jr. et al. 2008; Kahn, Jr. and Friedman 1995; Howe, Kahn, Jr. and Friedman
1996). We intentionally avoid the use of self-reporting questionnaires because the
sense of connectedness with nature is regarded as a zero-order belief, i.e. a
primitive belief of which we are consciously unaware, and therefore it cannot be
verbalized, especially by 5-year-old children (Schultz et al. 2004). Children are
indeed incapable of deep self-exploration and have very limited capacity to express
the complexity of their emotions and beliefs. Hence, we used previously employed
image-based techniques (Bryant 1982; Feshbach 1975; Pell and Jarvis 2001; Borke
1973) to design an assessment that non-verbally evaluates affinity with the
biosphere, similar to the Connectedness to Nature Scale (CNS) (Mayer and Frantz
2004), the Nature Relatedness Scale (NR) (Nisbet, Zelenski and Murphy 2008), and
the connection to nature index (Cheng and Monroe 2010; Evans et al. 2007).
All tasks that children are asked to perform in the assessment had been tested in
two previous pilot studies to be suitable for the cognitive abilities of children of five
years of age and are separated in two complementary sections: emotional and
cognitive affinity with the biosphere. The third section, attitudinal affinity with the
biosphere, is a qualitative exploration of children’s desire to engage in playful
nature experiences. Using short interviews, we intend to clarify children’s spoken
attitudes towards nature, and to explore some of the conscious causes that drive a
child to actively engage in nature-oriented entertainment.
Emotional Affinity with the Biosphere
Children’s emotional affinity with the biosphere is here quantified by the capacity
for emotional perspective-taking and empathic concern for nature. The tasks
related to emotional perspective-taking (EA1-EA8) assess the child’s empathetic
capacity to experience pain for living beings (e.g., marine life, birds, plants,
animals) in comparison to empathy for damages to manufactured objects (e.g.,
vehicles). A similar method is used for adults in the Environmental Motives Scale
(Schultz 2000; 2001; Schultz et al. 2004). The tasks related to empathic concern
for nature (EA9-EA14) assess the positive or negative emotional response after
participants are shown pictures of positive or negative environmental behaviors
(e.g., polluting water/littering, watering plants). These emotional attributes indicate
a degree of self-other overlap (Cialdini et al. 1997) and are also employed
elsewhere to measure unconscious affection for nature (Mayer and Frantz 2004;
Nisbet, Zelenski and Murphy 2008; Cheng and Monroe 2010).
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 22
Cognitive Affinity with the Biosphere
Children’s cognitive affinity with the biosphere is measured by their basic
awareness of the ecological resources required for human sustenance (CA1-CA9).
That is, children’s capacity to link everyday items to the natural resources required
for their production, and their ability to recognize the negative impact that pollution
has on ecosystems and the biosphere (CA10-CA21).
Attitudinal Affinity with the Biosphere
The analysis of children’s attitudinal affinity with the biosphere is based on
qualitative methods. Children are interviewed about their appreciation of nature
experiences and their conscious willingness to play in natural environments. In
order to provide a holistic description of the children’s attitudinal affinity with
nature they are also asked to rationalize their previous answers. This approach
enables us to explore children’s reasoning and to recognize what constitutes and
drives attitudes towards nature at an age when both spatial familiarity and
environmental awareness begin to emerge (Piaget 1960).
Collection of Data
The data collection process is divided into three phases. In Phase 1, we assess
children’s frequency of nature experiences during preschool activities. In Phase 2,
we assess children’s emotional, cognitive, and attitudinal affinity with the
biosphere. Finally, in Phase 3, we assess the children’s social context. Each phase is
described below.
Phase 1: Assessing Children’s Frequency of Nature Experiences in
Preschools
All 134 municipal preschools in Stockholm that adhere to the Reggio Emilia
pedagogical approach were ranked according to the availability of nature
experiences of recreation, natural beauty, wilderness, and rurality in their
surroundings. We chose preschools that use the Reggio Emilia approach because
they are widely available in the Stockholm municipality, and because they
emphasize the importance of the physical learning environment. We carried out the
ranking of preschools using the following procedure:
1. A GIS analysis is performed using the attraction accessibility function in the
Place Syntax Tool (Ståhle, Marcus and Karlström 2005), a plug-in application
for MapInfo. Preschool accessibility to nature experiences is calculated by
measuring the distances between each preschool’s location and every
sociotope included in the categorized nature experiences (see Table 1).
2. The distances resulting from the GIS analysis are coupled with how often
preschool teachers and pupils cover such distances for outdoor activities.
Distances to each sociotope are then translated into values of frequency of
use to create a more direct evaluation of children’s nature routines. How
frequently a distance range is used for outdoor activities determines its
relevance in children’s nature routines. We therefore classify the distance
from each preschool to each sociotope into five different ranges previously
identified by the preschool teachers as corresponding to the frequency of
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 23
their outdoor routines. We then weight the presence of sociotopes in those
ranges accordingly (frequency values from 0 to 5). If a sociotope of nature is
present in near environments (e.g., 0-25 meters) it has a stronger impact on
children’s routines (frequency value 5) because it is systematically
experienced, while sociotopes in a distant range (e.g., 500-1000 meters)
have less impact (frequency value 1) because they are experienced only
sporadically. The frequency values in relation to the distance ranges are:
0 to 25 meters (Daily exposure) frequency value 5
25 to 100 meters (11-15 times per month) frequency value 4
100 to 500 meters (5-10 times per month) frequency value 3
500 to 1000 meters (2-4 times per month) - frequency value 2
1000 to 2000 meters (3-4 time per year) - frequency value 1
>2000 meters (Not walkable daily) - frequency value 0
3. The resulting frequency values for each sociotope are then summed up in a
single value. At this stage, each preschool has a unique value that represents
a reliable estimation of how often the nature experiences available in the
preschool’s surroundings are experienced during outdoor activities. All 134
preschools are ranked according to this value.
In other words, each preschool is ranked according to the highest frequency of use
of the greatest variety of nature experiences in its surroundings (mean = 40.3, SD
= 7.7). The top ten preschools with the most frequent use of all nature experiences
and the ten preschools with the least frequent use were selected for comparison.
Preschools in the second-ranked deciles could not be selected to complement this
data because they were pilot-cases used to consolidate the methods for the
assessment of children’s affiliation with the biosphere (Giusti 2012). Of the selected
20 preschools, 13 were eliminated because they did not meet the requirements for
Phase 2 (having a minimum of three Swedish children of five years of age who have
been in the same preschool since their first year). The remaining seven preschools
selectedtwo from the top ranking and five from the bottom rankingreported an
average time of outdoor activities with children of 14 hours/week (SD = 1.7) in line
with the Swedish national curriculum (Skolverket 2010).
Phase 2: Assessing Children’s Affinity with the Biosphere
Each component of this phase is designed to prevent known demographic and
cultural factors from influencing the results (Dunlap et al. 2000). Children’s age,
their number of years of education, their nationality, the nationality of both their
parents, the pedagogical approach of the preschool, and the overall urban
environment (Stockholm) are personal, social, and spatial characteristics that are
uniform in this study. Restricting variation in such a wide variety of factors limits
the size of the dataset, but makes the research more consistent with its intent:
assessing how four years of experiencing urban landscapes with different natural
features relates to children’s affinity with the biosphere.
The final participants were 16 children from preschools with low access to nature
experiences and 11 from preschools with high access to nature experiences.
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 24
Teachers individually interviewed all children inside their own preschool to create a
safe, relaxing environment for the children to respond to the tasks given to them
(Chawla 2006b). In so doing, we also minimize eventual unconscious transfer of
desired results from researchers to the children.
Materials for Phase 2
The assessment of children’s affinity with the biosphere is designed to address
affective and cognitive aspects of connectedness with the biosphere using two
complementary sets of image-based tasks, or “games. The games were designed
to be general and not locally specific, as well as straightforward to interpret for the
researchers. Children’s motivations and intentions of playing in natural
environments, i.e. attitudinal affinity, is then evaluated with a short interview
(Table 2). Before performing the interview, the teachers were briefed about the
project and its assumptions, and were provided with written instructions on how to
perform all the requested tasks.
Table 2. Games testing for Emotional Affinity (EA1-EA14), Cognitive
Affinity (CA1-CA21), and Attitudinal Affinity with the Biosphere
assigned task
image sample
Emotional Affinity with the
Biosphere
Answer yes-no to
“Does [image] feel
pain?”
Tasks EA1 to EA8
Choosing a smile after
showing an image of
positive/negative
environmental
behavior.
Tasks EA9 to EA14
Cognitive Affinity with the
Biosphere
Answer “What do you
need to have [image
first column]?” by
selecting image from
the second column.
Tasks CA1 to CA9
Answer: “Is [image]
harmful to [image]?”
First column images
are shown one after
the other for each of
the four image groups
in the second column:
animals, vehicles, you,
[animals] [vehicles]
[you] [people]
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 25
people.
Each image group in
the second column is
composed of three
distinct images.
Tasks CA10 to CA21
Attitudinal Affinity with the
Biosphere
Select image and
motivate:
“Where do/don’t you
like to play?” “Why?”
“Where do/don’t you
feel free to play?”
“Why?”
“Where do/don’t you
feel safe to play?”
“Why?”
Recreation
area
Videogames
Playground
Farm
Toys
Green area
Outdoor
street
Forest
Emotional Affinity (EA) with the Biosphere
The games EA1-EA8 use images to assess empathic perspective taking through
eight yes/no questions. Children are shown images (e.g., animals, tree, plane) and
asked: “Does this image feel pain?” Positive answers to images of vehicles are
considered to represent non-affinity with nature. In the games EA9-EA14, six
images of positive or negative environmental behaviors (e.g., planting a
tree/positive, littering/negative) are shown to the children who are asked to
respond by using the image of a smiling or sad face. Smiling faces for images of
negative environmental behaviors are considered to represent non-affinity with the
biosphere.
Cognitive Affinity (CA) with the Biosphere
In the games CA1-CA9, participants are asked to couple nine images of finite
products (“What do you need to have [image],” e.g., milk, wooden table, paper) to
the corresponding image of the natural resource required (e.g., cow, forest, tree).
Coupling the products with man-made resources (e.g., factory, money) are
considered as representing non-affinity with nature. In the games CA10-CA21,
participants are asked 12 yes/no questions about the harmful effects of
environmental pollution on living beings (e.g., “Is [image of water/air/ground
pollution] harmful to [image of people/child/animals/vehicles]?”). Positive answers
about vehicles are considered to represent non-affinity with the biosphere.
Attitudinal Affinity with the Biosphere
Children are asked six questions during a short interview: “Where do/don’t you like
to play?”, “Where do/don’t you feel free to play?”, and “Where do/don’t you feel
safe to play?” Participants answer by selecting one of eight images of different
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 26
environments (park, indoor videogames, playground, farm, indoor toys, green
areas, outdoor street, and forest). The interview focuses on playing in natural
environments because it resulted to be the easiest hypothetical activity for children
to imagine. In order to enhance comprehension and facilitate the envisioning of
such hypothetical behavior the interview is facilitated by picture selection. Children
are then asked to rationalize their choices using their own words.
Phase 3: Assessing Children’s Social Context
The respondents during Phase 3 are the parents (N = 27) of the 27 children that
were interviewed in Phase 2. This phase attempts to assess the socio-economic
factors that might have influenced children’s degree of connectedness with nature
that could not be controlled a priori.
Materials for Phase 3
Three surveys are used during this phase. Survey one evaluates children’s exposure
to natural experiences during family activities. Parents are asked to provide the
amount of hours per week their child usually plays, excluding the time spent
playing at the preschool, in eight different indoor and outdoor environments (park,
video games, playground, farm, indoor toys, green areas, streets, and forest).
Survey two assesses social factors that might have facilitated, or constrained,
children’s engagement in nature experiences (Table 3). The children’s parents rate
16 statements using a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5
(strongly agree) inspired by the NEP (Bamberg and Moser 2007).
Table 3. Survey of children’s parents
Exploration tolerance
“I tolerate when my child has scratches on knees and arms”
“I tolerate when my child has very muddy clothes or shoes”
“I tolerate when my child walks outdoor barefoot”
“I tolerate when my child collects objects from the ground
“I tolerate when my child puts in his/her mouth objects from the ground”
Environmental values
“I am involved in protecting the environment I live in”
“I am fascinated by the beauty of natural elements”
“I feel pleasure spending time in natural environments”
Perceived safety
“Playing in parks is safe for my child”
“Playing in agricultural environments with animals is safe for my child”
“Playing in green areas with animals, water, flowers, etc. is safe for my child”
“Playing in the forest is safe for my child”
Perceived importance
“Playing in parks is important for my child”
“Playing in agricultural environments with animals is important for my child”
“Playing in green areas with animals, water, flowers, etc. is important for my child”
“Playing in the forest is important for my child”
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 27
The third survey assesses parents’ socio-economic characteristics. Parents are
asked standard demographic information (e.g., age, number of children) and their
motivation for selecting the preschool in which their child is enrolled. Additionally,
parents’ income range is assessed through district statistical data provided online
by the city of Stockholm (Stockhom Stad).
Results
Phase 1: Children’s Frequency of Nature Experiences in Preschools
Figure 1 shows the geographical location of all the selected 134 preschools and
their different frequency of use of nature experiences, together with the locations of
the top and the bottom ten preschools selected for comparison. The standard
deviation values obtained from the statistical analysis confirm that nature
experiences of recreation (mean = 11.3, SD = 2.2) and natural beauty (mean =
11.6, SD = 3.3) are more evenly distributed than experiences of wilderness
(mean = 11.3, SD = 4.3), while experiences of rurality are considerably less
available across the whole municipality (mean = 6.1, SD = 2.9). In fact, the spatial
configuration of Stockholm provides very few experiences of rurality (Figure 1d)
and mostly isolates experiences of wilderness in peripheral locations of the
municipality (Figure 1c). The two preschools with highest frequency of nature
experiences, compared to the five lowest ones, have considerably more access to
all four nature experiences (mean = 38.7, SD = 18): recreation (mean = 11, SD =
6.4), natural beauty (mean = 11.1, SD = 6.4), wilderness (mean = 11.4, SD = 5.5)
and rurality (mean = 6.2, SD = 3.8).
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 28
Figure 1. Distribution of nature experiences of a) recreation, b) natural
beauty, c) wilderness, and d) rurality from preschools in Stockholm.
a)
b)
c)
d)
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 29
Phase 2: Children’s Affinity with the Biosphere
Phase 2 analyzes emotional and cognitive affinity with nature in 11 Children with
Nature-Rich routines (CNR) and in 16 Children with Nature-Deficit routines (CND).
Because of the small dataset, the computation of reliability using Cronbach’s Alpha
is presented in combination with Spearman-Brown coefficients (Yurdugul 2008).
The reliability of the 35 tasks assessing children’s affinity with the biosphere is α =
.701 and odd-even splits report a split-half reliability of r = .758. These results
ensure data reliability and strong internal consistency of both emotional and
cognitive tasks as well as the overall quantitative assessment of the children’s
affinity with the biosphere. The affinity rates for CNR and CND are compared with
independent two-sample t-tests (Table 4).
Table 4. Mean results, Standard Deviation (SD), t, p, df, and effect size (d)
of independent two-sample t-test for Emotional and Cognitive
Affinity with the Biosphere in Children with Nature-Rich routines
(CNR) and Children with Nature-Deficit routines (CND)
affinity CNR
(N = 11)
affinity CND
(N = 16)
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
t
p
df
d
Emotional
Affinity with the
Biosphere
.792
.121
.665
.154
2.289
.031
25
.916
Cognitive
Affinity with the
Biosphere
.771
.134
.660
.133
2.114
.045
25
.845
Children with nature-rich routines score significantly and notably higher in
Emotional Affinity (EA) with nature (mean= .792, two-sample t(25) = 2.289, p =
.031, d = .916) than children with nature-deficit routines (mean = .665). CNR show
higher capacities to emotionally affiliate with the biosphere in almost all questions,
regardless of the difficulty characterizing the single tasks. Specifically, CNR perform
better in the tasks EA1 (37 percent higher empathy towards trees; mean-CNR =
.55, mean-CND = .38), EA7 (28 percent higher empathy towards fishes; mean-CNR
= .91, mean-CND = .69), EA9 (46 percent higher sadness related to water
pollution; mean-CNR = 1, mean-CND = .63) and EA10 (37 percent higher sadness
related to deforestation; mean-CNR = .73, mean-CND = .50). CNR have therefore
developed stronger capacity for perspective-taking towards non-human life forms
and higher empathic concern towards environmental degradation. Overall, children
with nature-rich routines consistently show on average 17 percent stronger
emotional affinity with the biosphere than children with nature-deficit routines
(Figure 2).
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 30
Figure 2. Average results of games testing for Emotional and Cognitive
Affinity with the Biosphere
Children with nature-rich routines score (mean = .771) significantly and notably
higher in Cognitive Affinity (CA) with nature (two-sample t(25) = 2.114, p = .045,
d = .845) than the children with nature-deficit routines (mean = .660). CNR also
show higher cognitive capacity to link finite products with the ecological resources
required for their manufacture and better perception of the harmful effects of
environmental degradation (Figure 2). Specifically, children with nature-rich
routines perform better in the tasks CA5 (37 percent higher understanding of the
link between wool and sheep, mean-CNR = .73, mean-CND = .5), CA8 (37 percent
higher understanding of the link between blueberries and forest, mean-CNR = .73,
mean-CND = .5), and CA19 (50 percent higher understanding the harmful effects
of water pollution for animals, mean-CNR = .73, mean-CND = .44). Once again,
those children with nature-rich routines consistently show on average 15 percent
higher cognitive capacities to affiliate with the biosphere in almost all questions,
regardless of the levels of difficulty characterizing the single tasks.
The qualitative analysis of children’s attitudinal affinity with the biosphere shows
that children with high and low nature experiences share very similar attitudes
towards playing in natural environments. Indeed, children of both groups consider
indoor environments the safest to play in (n = 13) and wild environments the least
safe (n = 12). The fear of getting lost and losing their parents (n = 4) and the fear
of dangerous animals such as wolves, bears, and moose (n = 6) motivate a large
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 31
number of such attitudes. Supplementary results are provided by the short
interviews in which children are asked to rationalize their affiliation with nature.
Children might want to play indoors just because they can be part of choosing the
dinner, or might want to play outdoors because they do not want to be disturbed by
their little sibling. The reasoning for their choices is mostly based on contextual
memories. Similarly, their emotional reactions to the biosphere relate to their
contextual understanding of reality. “Deer and dinosaurs do not feel pain because
they can hit back (Karolina) or “because you are afraid of them” (Fabian), and
airplanes and cars are alive because they drink gas” (Erwin) are some examples of
how the conscious motivations that justify their positive or negative emotional
reactions are constructed upon their own vision of the world.
Phase 3: Assessing Children’s Social Context
The influence of the social context on children’s affinity with nature is estimated,
but the size of the dataset (N = 27) limits the conclusions that can be drawn.
Parents are surveyed on children’s exposure to natural environments during family
activities. On average parents estimate that children play 49.7 percent indoors,
18.2 percent in playgrounds and streets, and 32.1 percent in natural environments
(mean = 25.4, SD = 12.1). However, parents’ answers are very inconsistent
because they reported an unrealistically wide range of playing hours per week (min
= 8, max = 72). Such data and results are therefore unsuitable for robust
conclusions.
The social factors that might facilitate children’s engagement with nature are
assessed using a survey of 16 Likert-based items given to children’s parents. The
results have been normalized in a single score (mean = .83) with a Cronbach’s
Alpha α = .841, and the computation of odd-even splits produces split-half
reliability of r = .712. However, such results show no correlation with children’s
emotional (r = .03, p > .05) or cognitive affinity with the biosphere (r = -.19, p >
.05). The homogeneity of the social sample taken into consideration (i.e., all
parents are Swedish persons living in Stockholm in similar age groups) seems
indeed to have produced highly analogous answers (SD = .08). Parents’ main
motivation for choosing the preschool for their child is “because it’s close to home”
(97 percent) which makes the analysis of available nature experiences in the
surroundings of preschools somewhat also indicative of home circumstances.
Parents’ economic status shows no correlation with the degree of emotional (r = -
.16, p > .05) and cognitive affinity children have (r = -.20, p >.05), thus a
hypothetical association between low affinity with the biosphere and low income
ranges is not supported by this study.
Discussion and Implications
The main results of this study indicate that the spatial configuration of nature
experiences in Stockholm is associated with children’s development of an affinity
with the biosphere. More specifically, we show that of the 27 participants, those
who have had nature-deficit routines for four years in preschool have developed
considerably weaker emotional and cognitive affinity with the biosphere. The strict
selection of participants (same age, years of education, nationality, parents’
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 32
nationality, preschools’ pedagogical approach), the consistency in the analysis of
social factors that facilitate children in engaging with nature experiences (mean =
.83, SD= .08), and the proximity of children’s homes to preschools (in 97 percent
of the respondents) control for possible confounding variables. The significant
variance found in the results for both children’s emotional (17 percent) and
cognitive affinity (15 percent) is in close relation to the nature routines that children
have had during preschool outdoor activities. The results show that deficient
contact with urban nature over time is negatively associated with children’s
development of an attachment to the biosphere. Additionally, the results on
attitudinal affiliation with nature show that children’s attitudes towards playing in
nature are limited by highly contextual factors. For instance, the choice to play
outdoors may have to do with the presence of friends rather than an outspoken
preference for any particular environment. Thus, although firsthand experiences of
nature have a significant role in enabling positive attitudes towards the
environment, other complex contextual circumstances might hinder the emergence
of such attitudes.
Supplementary results are provided by the short interviews in which children are
asked to rationalize their affiliation with the biosphere. The emotional attraction or
repulsion for the biosphere that children show with their answers seem to emerge
from their own understanding of reality. Children do not show a positive attitude for
nature for what nature is in itself, but for what nature is for them. As suggested by
the biological theory of cognition, the internal representation of reality is built upon
neural interactions between an individual and the external environment (Maturana
and Varela 1987). In this neural dynamic, persistency plays a leading role. Indeed,
persistent exposure to the same neurological experience materially nurtures the
same neural networks (Maletic-Savatic, Malinow and Svoboda 1999). Accordingly,
for one group of children in this study, the persistent high frequency of nature
experiences have nurtured an affinity with the biosphere that might be supported
by a somatic neural familiarity with experiences of biological rather than of
artificial character. Such neural familiarity is an unconscious attribute that is
physically wired in one’s neural configuration, and so it is an embedded
characteristic of one’s own biological identity (Zull 2002; Maturana and Varela
1987). As already hypothesized (Naess 1973; Bragg 1996; Schultz 2002), nature-
deficit routines not only impact one’s worldview and set of assumptions, but might
be literally part of one’s biological identity. A disappearance of nature experiences
from everyday life may leave human beings with a void of memories that would
impede nature and natural dynamics from entering the neural representation of self
upon which humans build emotions and reasoning.
From the spatial analysis it should be noted that the experiences of wilderness are
the most dissimilar in access across the two groups of children, and the games that
measure children’s capacity to emotionally affiliate with elements in wild
environments are the ones with the highest variation. Although the research design
does not allow us to conclusively discuss the influence of each nature experience
(i.e., recreation, natural beauty, wilderness, rurality) separately, this result further
supports our hypothesis that persistent deficiency of nature experiences relates to
reduced affiliation with natural features.
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 33
Due to the limited number of respondents, we acknowledge that additional studies
of this kind are needed to ensure the statistical power of the results and to consider
them for nomothetic conclusions. However, our findings add to the literature cited
herein and suggest that the patterns we have observed offer important insights
that are not only characteristic of Stockholm and Swedish children, but are likely to
have more general applicability. This paper hence calls for much needed
interdisciplinary research linking urban design, environmental psychology,
neuroscience and anthropology, in order to further evaluate how nature routines
that are designed in cities nurture or constrain one’s identification with nature. The
implications of such research could dramatically impact the state-of-the-art of
urban design, and offer a generous opportunity to provide urban dwellers with the
kind of emotional attachment to nature that acts as stepping stone for the
emergence of environmental stewardship.
Conclusion
The spatial configurations of modern cities consistently marginalize natural
environments in both presence and diversity (Mumford 1961; Miller 2005). In this
study, the consequences of such planned and designed exclusion of nature
experiences are measured on children’s affiliation with the biosphere. Our results
indicate that persistent interaction with natural environments correlates with a
higher capacity to emotionally and cognitively affiliate with the biosphere. Exposure
to nature experiences during the first four years of life does not seem to weaken
children’s fear of predators or of getting lost in the forestwhich are known
biophobic traits (Ulrich 1993)but significantly correlates with children’s ability to
distinguish living from lifeless entities, to be empathetic for non-human life forms
(EA1-EA8), to be concerned for environmental degradation (EA9-EA14), to be
aware of the harmful effects of environmental pollution (CA1-CA9), and finally to
recognize the role of ecological resources for the production of everyday objects
(CA10-CA21).
This study supports the hypothesis that long-lasting exposure to natural
environments, even in an urban context, is closely related to the development of a
conscious and unconscious affinity with the biosphere and its dynamics. The nature
routines embedded in urban forms seem to act as contextual factors that nurture
the development of mindsets more or less affiliated with nature. Such affinity has
previously been suggested to have significant effects on the foundation of a lifelong
commitment to environmental concern (Schultz 2001; 2000) and environmental
stewardship (Chawla 2006a; 1998). As the urban population of the world is
projected to surpass six billion in 2050 (United Nations 2007), urban spaces that
nurture nature routines, especially during childhood, are likely to support the re-
connection with the biosphere which many scholars in the sustainability sciences
are calling for (Folke et al. 2011; Bragg 1996; Samways 2007; Miller 2005). Is it
possible to design cities that mitigate the on-going trend of extinction of nature
experiences (Miller 2005), environmental generational amnesia (Kahn, Jr. 2002),
or nature-deficit disorder (Louv 2005)? Humankind is aspiring to a sustainable
stewardship of the biosphere. Such stewardship might be dependent not only on
the human capacity to rationally comprehend the limits of natural resources, but
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 34
also on the human capacity to emotionally bond with local ecosystems and existing
non-human life. Considering that cities are the most common human habitat, the
intentional design of their very forms have great potential to remind, cherish or
neglect the interdependence of human and biotic well-being.
Acknowledgements
We wish to thank Terry Hartig for the insightful discussion on an earlier version of the
paper, and three anonymous reviewers for their feedback.
Matteo Giusti’s research aims at defining principles of urban design that nurture
identification with nature. The extinction of nature experience is commonplace in
the urban lifestyle and its consequences are widely assumed to trigger a self-
reinforcing cycle of disaffection that hinders people’s engagement in environmental
stewardship. Matteo’s research revolves on how to design spatial configurations of
urban nature to support the psychological foundation of environmental
stewardship—identification with nature. Matteo’s research embraces aspects of
resilience theory, spatial cognition, biophilic design, neuro-biological theories of
cognition, and more generally environmental psychology and urban design.
Stephan Barthel's research revolves around the management of urban ecosystem
services and resilience. Stephan’s research shows that urban ecosystem services
are a product of human-driven co-evolution, consequently sustaining ecosystems in
urban landscapes is not about conservation without people, but shaped by and
dependent on management practice by people. Practice that links to generation of
ecosystem services are facilitated by social-ecological memory of local
communities-of-practice that holds long-term management rights. Consequently,
local communities of practice that contribute to the production of ecosystem
services should explicitly be taken into account in urban green governance of urban
landscapes.
Lars Marcus is an architect and professor in Urban Design at KTH School of
Architecture. He is director of the research group Spatial Analysis and Design (SAD)
in the field of Spatial Morphology, the study of how spatial form generated by
architecture and urban design structures people’s use of space in their everyday life
and, in extension, conditions critical social, economic and ecological processes. He
is co-founder of the international MSc-program in Sustainable Urban Planning and
Design (SUPD) and also founder of the consultancy firm Spacescape, which
performs spatial analysis and policy development for urban projects.
References
Andersson, E, S. Barthel, and K. Ahrné (2007). “Measuring Social-Ecological
Dynamics behind the Generation of Ecosystem Services.” Ecological Applications
17(5): 12671278.
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 35
Andersson, Erik (2006). “Urban Landscapes and Sustainable Cities.” Ecology and
Society 11(1).
Bamberg, S., and G. Möser (2007). Twenty Years after Hines, Hungerford, and
Tomera: A New Meta-Analysis of Psycho-Social Determinants of Pro-Environmental
Behaviour. Journal of Environmental Psychology 27(1): 14-25.
Barthel, S., J. Colding, T. Elmqvist, and C. Folke (2005). “History and Local
Management of a Biodiversity-Rich, Urban Cultural Landscape.” Ecology and Society
10(2).
Barthel, S., C. Crumley, and U. Svedin (2013). “Bio-Cultural Refugia
Safeguarding Diversity of Practices for Food Security and Biodiversity.” Global
Environmental Change 23(5): 1142-1152.
Barthel, S., and C. Isendahl (2013) “Urban Gardens, Agriculture, and Water
Management: Sources of Resilience for Long-Term Food Security in Cities.”
Ecological Economics 86: 224-234.
Berkes, F., J. Colding, and C. Folke (2000). Rediscovery of Traditional
Ecological Knowledge as Adaptive Management. Ecological Applications 10(5):
1251-1262.
Berkes, F., and C. Folke (1998). Linking Social and Ecological Systems:
Management Practices and Social Mechanisms for Building Resilience. Cambridge,
UK: Cambridge University Press.
Borke, H. (1973). “The Development of Empathy in Chinese and American Children
between Three and Six Years of Age: A Cross-Cultural Study.” Developmental
Psychology 9(1): 102108.
Bragg, E.A. (1996). “Towards Ecological Self: Deep Ecology Meets Constructionist
Self-Theory.” Journal of Environmental Psychology 16(2): 93108.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). “Ecology of the Family as a Context for Human
Development: Research Perspectives.” Developmental Psychology 22(6): 723742.
Bryant, B. (1982). “An Index of Empathy for Children and Adolescents.” Child
Development 53(2): 413425.
Chawla, L. (2006a). "Learning to Love the Natural World Enough to Protect It."
Barn 2(2): 5778.
----- (2006b). "Research Methods to Investigate Significant Life Experiences:
Review and Recommendations." Environmental Education Research 12(3-4): 359
374.
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 36
----- (1999). “Life Paths into Effective Environmental Action.” The Journal of
Environmental Education 31(1): 1526.
----- (1998). “Significant Life Experiences Revisited: A Review of Research on
Sources of Environmental Sensitivity.” Environmental Education Research 4(4):
369382.
Cheng, J.C.H., and M.C. Monroe (2010). “Connection to Nature: Children’s
Affective Attitude toward Nature.” Environment and Behavior 44(1): 3149.
Cialdini, R.B., S.L. Brown, B.P. Lewis, C. Luce, and S.L. Neuberg (1997).
“Reinterpreting the Empathy-Altruism Relationship: When One into One Equals
Oneness.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 73(3): 48194.
Clayton, S.D. (2003). “Environmental Identity: A Conceptual and Operational
Definition.” In S.D. Clayton and S. Opotow, eds. Identity and the Natural
Environment: The Psychological Significance of Nature. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press,
4566.
Colding, J., J. Lundberg, and C. Folke (2006). “Incorporating Green-Area User
Groups in Urban Ecosystem Management.” Ambio 35(5): 237244.
Crumley, C. (1994). Historical Ecology: Cultural Knowledge and Changing
Landscapes. Santa Fe, NM: School of American Research Press.
Dalton, R.C., C. Hölscher, and A. Turner (2012). “Understanding Space: the
Nascent Synthesis of Cognition and the Syntax of Spatial Morphologies.”
Environment and Planning B: Planning and Design 39(1): 711.
Deutsch, L. (2004). Global Trade, Food Production and Ecosystem Support:
Making the Interactions Visible. Stockholm University.
Dunlap, R.E., and K. Van Liere (1978). “The New Environmental Paradigm.”
Journal of Environmental Education 9: 1019.
Dunlap, R.E., K. Van Liere, A. Mertig, and R. Jones (2000). “Measuring
Endorsement of the New Ecological Paradigm: A Revised NEP Scale.” Journal of
Social Issues 56(3): 425442.
Evans, G.W., G. Brauchle, A. Haq, R. Stecker, K. Wong, and E. Shapiro
(2007). “Young Children’s Environmental Attitudes and Behaviors.” Environment
and Behavior 39(5): 635658.
Ewert, A., G. Place, and J. Sibthorp (2005). “Early-Life Outdoor Experiences and
an Individual’s Environmental Attitudes.” Leisure Sciences 27(3): 225239.
Feshbach, N.D. (1975). “Empathy in Children: Some Theoretical and Empirical
Considerations.” The Counseling Psychologist 5(2): 2530.
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 37
Finch, K. (2008). “Extinction of Experience: A Challenge for Environmental
Education.” New England Journal of Environmental Education. Available from:
http://kidsandnature.org/blog/wp-content/uploads/2008/10/kenfincharticle.pdf
Folke, C., C.S. Holling, and C. Perrings (1996). “Biological Diversity,
Ecosystems, and the Human Scale.” Ecological Applications 6(4): 1018-1024.
Folke, C., Å. Jansson, J. Larsson, and R. Costanza (1997). “Ecosystem by
Cities Appropriation.” Ambio 26(3): 167172.
Folke, C., Å. Jansson, J. Rockström, P. Olsson, S.R. Carpenter, F.S. Chapin,
A.S. Crépin, G. Daily, D. Kjell, J. Ebbesson, T. Elmqvist, V. Galaz, F. Moberg,
M. Nilsson, H. Österblom, E. Ostrom, Å. Persson, G. Peterson, S. Polasky,
W. Steffen, B. Walker, and F. Westley (2011). “Reconnecting to the Biosphere.
Ambio 40(7): 719738.
Gallese, V. (2003). “The Roots of Empathy: The Shared Manifold Hypothesis and
the Neural Basis of Intersubjectivity.” Psychopathology 36(4): 17180.
Gandini, L. (1993). “Fundamentals of the Reggio Emilia Approach to Early
Childhood Education.” Young Children 49(1): 4-8
Geertz, C. (1973). The Interpretation of Cultures (3rd ed.). London: Fontana Press.
Giusti, M. (2012). Reconnecting to the Biosphere: Childrens Socio-Ecological
Emotions for Nature. Stockholm University, Unpublished MSc Thesis. Available
from: http://www.diva-portal.org/smash/record.jsf?searchId=1&pid=diva2:512443
Greene, M., J. Reyes, and A. Castro (2012). No Title. In Proceedings of the 7th
International Space Syntax Symposium. Santiago de Chile, Chile: Pontefica
Universidad Catholica de Chile.
Grimm, N.B., S.H. Faeth, N.E. Golubiewski, C.L. Redman, J. Wu, X. Bai, and
J.M. Briggs (2008). “Global Change and the Ecology of Cities.” Science 319(5864):
75660.
Hillier, B. (1996). Space Is the Machine. London: Cambridge University Press.
----- (2003). “The Architecture of Seeing and Going: Or, Are Cities Shaped by
Bodies or Minds?” In Hanson, J., ed. Proceedings of 4th International Space Syntax
Symposium. London: University College London.
Hillier, B., and J. Hanson (1984). The Social Logic of Space. New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Hillier, B., and S. Iida (2005). “Network Effects and Psychological Effects: A
Theory of Urban Movement.” In A. Cohn and D. Mark, eds. Spatial Information
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 38
Theory: Lecture Notes in Computer Science 3603. Berlin, Germany: Springer
Verlag, 473490.
Hillier, B., A. Penn, J. Hanson, T. Grajewski, and J. Xu (1993). “Natural
Movement: Or, Configuration and Attraction in Urban Pedestrian Movement.”
Environment and Planning B: Planning and Design 20: 2966.
Howe, D., P.H. Kahn, Jr., and B. Friedman (1996). “Along the Rio Negro:
Brazilian Children’s Environmental Views and Values.” Developmental Psychology
32(6): 979987.
Kahn, Jr., P.H. (1999). “Structural-Developmental Methods.” In The Human
Relationship with Nature: Development and Culture. Cambridge, MA: The MIT
Press. Available from: http://psycnet.apa.org/psycinfo/1999-02840-000.
----- (2002). “Children’s Affiliations with Nature: Structure, Development, and the
Problem of Environmental Generational Amnesia.” In P.H. Kahn, Jr. and S.R.
Kellert, eds. Children and Nature. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 93116.
Kahn, Jr., P.H., and B. Friedman (1995). “Environmental Views and Values of
Children in an Inner-City Black Community.” Child Development 66(5): 14031417.
Kahn, Jr., P.H., and S.R. Kellert (2002). Children and Nature. Cambridge, MA:
The MIT Press.
Kahn, Jr., P.H., C.D. Saunders, R.L. Severson, O.E. Myers, and B.T. Gill
(2008). “Moral and Fearful Affiliations with the Animal World: Children’s
Conceptions of Bats.” Anthrozoos: A Multidisciplinary Journal of The Interactions of
People & Animals 21(4): 375386.
Kals, E., D. Schumacher, and L. Montada (1999). “Emotional Affinity toward
Nature as a Motivational Basis to Protect Nature.” Environment and Behavior 31(2):
178202.
Kellert, S.R. (2002). “Experiencing Nature: Affective, Cognitive, and Evaluative
Development in Children.” In S.R. Kellert and P.H. Kahn, Jr. eds. Children and
Nature. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press, 117151.
Koch, D., L. Marcus, and J. Steen (2009). No Title. In Proceedings of the 7th
International Space Syntax Symposium. Stockholm, Sweden: Trita-Ark-Research
Publication, Royal Institute of Technology.
Korhonen, K., and A. Lappalainen (2004). “Examining the Environmental
Awareness of Children and Adolescents in the Ranomafana Region, Madagascar.”
Environmental Education Research 10(2): 195216.
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 39
Krasny, M., C. Lundholm, and R. Plummer (2010). “Environmental Education,
Resilience, and Learning: Reflection and Moving Forward.” Environmental Education
Research 16(5): 665672.
Louv, R. (2005). Last Child in the Woods. London: Atlantic Books.
Maletic-Savatic, M., R. Malinow, and K. Svoboda (1999). “Rapid Dendritic
Morphogenesis in CA1 Hippocampal Dendrites Induced by Synaptic Activity.”
Science 283(5409): 19237.
Marcinkowski, T. (2001). “Predictors of Responsible Environmental Behavior: A
Review of Three Dissertation Studies.” Essential Readings in Environmental
Education (1973): 247276.
Marcus, L., and J. Colding (2011). “Towards a Spatial Morphology of Urban
Social-Ecological Systems.” Conference Proceedings for the 18th International
Conference on Urban Form, ISUF2011. Concordia University, Montreal, August 26-
29.
Maturana, H.R., and F.J. Varela (1987). The Tree of Knowledge: The Biological
Roots of Human Understanding. Boston and London: Shambhala.
Mayer, F.S., and C.M. Frantz (2004). “The Connectedness to Nature Scale: A
Measure of Individuals’ Feeling in Community with Nature. Journal of Environmental
Psychology 24(4): 503-515.
Miller, J.R. (2005). “Biodiversity Conservation and the Extinction of Experience.”
Trends in Ecology and Evolution 20(8): 430-434.
Mumford, L. (1961). The City in History. New York: Harcourt, Inc.
Nabhan, G.P., and S. St. Antoine (1993). “The Loss of Flora and Faunal Story:
The Exctinction of Experience.” In S.R. Kellert and E.O. Wilson, eds. The Biophilia
Hypothesis. Washington, D.C.: Island Press, 229250.
Naess, A. (1973). “The Shallow and the Deep, Long Range Ecology Movements: A
Summary. Inquiry 16: 95-100.
Nisbet, E.K., J.M. Zelenski, and S.A. Murphy (2008). “The Nature Relatedness
Scale: Linking Individuals’ Connection with Nature to Environmental Concern and
Behavior.” Environment and Behavior 41(5): 715740.
Orr, D.W. (2002). “Love It or Lose It: The Coming Biophilia Revolution.” In S.R.
Kellert and E.O. Wilson, eds. The Biophilia Hypothesis. Washington, D.C.: Island
Press.
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 40
Pell, T., and T. Jarvis (2001). “Developing Attitude to Science Scales for Use with
Children of Ages from Five to Eleven Years.” International Journal of Science
Education 23(8): 847-862.
Peponis, J., C. Zimring, and Y.K. Choi (1990). “Finding the Building in
Wayfinding.” Environment and Behavior 22(5): 555590.
Peterson, N., and H.R. Hungerford (1982). Developmental Variables Affecting
Environmental Sensitivity in Professional Environmental Educators.PhD diss.,
Southern Illinois University at Carbondale.
Piaget, J. (1960). The Child’s Conception of the World. Totowa, New Jersey:
Adams and Co.
Pyle, R.M. (1993). The Thunder Tree: Lessons from an Urban Wildland. Boston:
Houghton Mifflin.
Samways, M.J. (2007). “Rescuing the Extinction of Experience.” Biodiversity and
Conservation 16(7): 19951997.
Schultz, P.W. (2000). “Empathizing With Nature: The Effects of Perspective Taking
on Concern for Environmental Issues.” Journal of Social Issues 56(3): 391-406.
----- (2001). “The Structure of Environmental Concern: Concern for Self, Other
People, and the Biosphere.” Journal of Environmental Psychology 21(4): 327-339.
----- (2002). “Inclusion with Nature: Understanding Human-Nature Interactions.
In P. Schmuck and P.W. Schultz, eds. Psychology of Sustainable Development. New
York: Kluwer Academic Publishers, 61-78.
Schultz, P.W., C. Shriver, J.J. Tabanico, and A.M. Khazian (2004). “Implicit
Connections with Nature.” Journal of Environmental Psychology 24(1): 31-42.
Sia, A.P., H.R. Hungerford, and A.N. Tomera (1986). “Selected Predictors of
Responsible Environmental Behavior: An Analysis.” The Journal of Environmental
Education 17(2): 31-40.
Singer, T., B. Seymour, J. O’Doherty, H. Kaube, R.J. Dolan, and C.D. Frith
(2004). “Empathy for Pain Involves the Affective but Not Sensory Components of
Pain.” Science 303(5661): 1157-62.
Skolverket (2010). Lpfö 98. Curriculum for the Preschool. Stockholm.
Sociotophandboken (2003). “Planering av det offentliga uterummet med
Stockholmarna och sociotopkartan.” Rapport SBK 2.
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 41
Ståhle, A. (2006). “Sociotope Mapping - Exploring Public Open Space and Its
Scape Planning Practice.” Nordic Journal of Architectural Research 19(4): 59-71.
Ståhle, A., L. Marcus, and A. Karlström (2005). “A Space Syntax Approach to
Accessibility.” In Proceedings of the 5th International Space Syntax Symposium.
Delft, Netherlands: Technische Universität Delft, 131-139.
Stedman, R.C. (2002). “Toward a Social Psychology of Place: Predicting Behavior
from Place-Based Cognitions, Attitude, and Identity.” Environment and Behavior
34(5): 561-581.
Stedman, R.C. (2003). “Is It Really Just a Social Construction?: The Contribution
of the Physical Environment to Sense of Place.” Society & Natural Resources 16(8):
671-685.
Steffen, W., J. Crutzen, and J.R. McNeill (2007). “The Anthropocene: Are
Humans Now Overwhelming the Great Forces of Nature? Ambio 36(8- 61421.
Steffen, W., J. Grinevald, P. Crutzen, and J. McNeill (2011). “The
Anthropocene: Conceptual and Historical Perspectives.” Philosophical Transactions.
Series A, Mathematical, Physical, and Engineering Sciences 369(1938): 842-67.
Steg, L., and C. Vlek (2009). “Encouraging Pro-Environmental Behaviour: An
Integrative Review and Research Agenda.Journal of Environmental Psychology
29(3): 309-317.
Stern, P.C. (2000). “Toward a Coherent Theory of Environmentally Significant
Behavior.” Journal of Social Issues 56(3): 407-424.
Stern, P.C., and T. Dietz (1994). “The Value Basis of Environmental Concern.”
Journal of Social Issues 50(3): 65-84.
Stern, P.C., T. Dietz, and L. Kalof (1993). “Value Orientations, Gender, and
Environmental Concern.” Environment and Behavior 25(5): 322-348.
Stockhom Stad (n.d.) "Statistik om Stockholm." Available from:
http://www.statistikomstockholm.se/
Stokes, D.L. (2006). “Conservators of Experience.” BioScience 56(1): 7.
Ståhle, A. (2006). “Sociotope Mapping: Exploring Public Open Space and Its
Multiple Use in Urban and Landscape Planning Practice.” Nordic Journal of
Architectural Research 19(4): 59-71.
Szagun, G., and V.I. Pavlov (1995). “Environmental Awareness: A Comparative
Study of German and Russian Adolescents.” Youth & Society 27(1): 93-112.
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nature Routines and Affinity with the Biosphere: A Case Study of Preschool… 42
Tidball, K.G. (2012). “Urgent Biophilia: Human-Nature Interactions and Biological
Attractions in Disaster Resilience.Ecology and Society 17(2): 5.
Tidball, K.G., and M.E. Krasny (2010). Urban Environmental Education from a
Social-Ecological Perspective: Conceptual Framework for Civic Ecology Education.
Cities and the Environment (CATE) 3(1): 11.
Ulrich, R.S. (1993). “Biophilia, Biophobia and Natural Landscape.” In S.R. Kellert
and E.O. Wilson, eds. The Biophilia Hypothesis. Washington, D.C.: Island Press, 73-
137.
United Nations (2007). World Urbanization Prospects The 2007 Revision
Highlights. New York.
Wells, N., and G. Evans (2003). “Nearby Nature: A Buffer of Life Stress among
Rural Children.” Environment and Behavior 35(3): 311-330.
Yurdugul, H. (2008). “Minimum Sample Size for Cronbach’s Coefficient Alpha: A
Monte-Carlo Study.” H.U. Journal of Education 35: 397405.
Zull, J. (2002). The Art of Changing the Brain: Enriching the Practice of Teaching
by Exploring the Biology of Learning. Sterling, Virginia: Stylus Publishing, LLC.
This content downloaded from 132.236.139.20 on Mon, 12 Jan 2015 09:25:25 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
... There are several terms that refer to a connection with nature, such as nature relatedness, biophilia, or nature connectedness (NC), which we will adopt in this study. The NC of young children is considered to involve factors such as their enjoyment of nature (Rice & Torquati, 2013;Sobko, Jia, & Brown, 2018), sense of responsibility (Elliott, Ten Eycke, & Chan, Mü ller, 2014; Giusti, Barthel & Marcus, 2014;Sobko et al., 2018), empathy and awareness of nature (Giusti et al., 2014;Sobko et al., 2018), as well as their preference for playing outside (Elliott et al., 2014;Giusti et al., 2014;Rice & Torquati, 2013). ...
... There are several terms that refer to a connection with nature, such as nature relatedness, biophilia, or nature connectedness (NC), which we will adopt in this study. The NC of young children is considered to involve factors such as their enjoyment of nature (Rice & Torquati, 2013;Sobko, Jia, & Brown, 2018), sense of responsibility (Elliott, Ten Eycke, & Chan, Mü ller, 2014; Giusti, Barthel & Marcus, 2014;Sobko et al., 2018), empathy and awareness of nature (Giusti et al., 2014;Sobko et al., 2018), as well as their preference for playing outside (Elliott et al., 2014;Giusti et al., 2014;Rice & Torquati, 2013). ...
... There are several terms that refer to a connection with nature, such as nature relatedness, biophilia, or nature connectedness (NC), which we will adopt in this study. The NC of young children is considered to involve factors such as their enjoyment of nature (Rice & Torquati, 2013;Sobko, Jia, & Brown, 2018), sense of responsibility (Elliott, Ten Eycke, & Chan, Mü ller, 2014; Giusti, Barthel & Marcus, 2014;Sobko et al., 2018), empathy and awareness of nature (Giusti et al., 2014;Sobko et al., 2018), as well as their preference for playing outside (Elliott et al., 2014;Giusti et al., 2014;Rice & Torquati, 2013). ...
... Several scholars warn of a negative cycle, where this lack of exposure can result in growing disaffection and poor understanding of natureexemplified by nature phobias (Bixler & Floyd, 1997), and modernist preference for neat, uniform landscapes, which people might erroneously associate with ecosystem health (Kühne, 2012;Nassauer, 1992;Niemelä et al., 2011). Policy responses have included biophilic planning and design (Beatley, 2011;Beery et al., 2015;Scott et al., 2016), targeting school children and youth for significant experiences in nature (Giusti et al., 2014;Zanini et al., 2020), and using the pedagogic function of the ecosystem services concept to educate decision-makers and the public (Beery et al., 2016). ...
... Another perspective is that of children and areas for experimental play, considering that such nature experiences in childhood are especially formative Barthel et al., 2018;Giusti et al., 2014Giusti et al., , 2018. Policy examples include a child-centred perspective in urban planning, playgrounds that integrate natural elements for free play, app-based exploration of nature, and outdoor preschools. ...
... If a person does not travel anywhere outside their locality, they have no idea what the environmental situation is outside it, what problems people face in other regions. Physical access to natural experiences provides a repetitive interaction with nature, which has been shown to correlate with environmental awareness and sensitivity [48]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Improving the quality of life through increased well-being is the goal of many modern people. An increase in income through the formation of a habit to raise the standard of living and an appropriate attitude leads people to increase consumption, which in turn can negatively affect the environmental situation. A deterrent to the negative impact of an increase in the standard of living is adherence to stable habits and attitudes that people may have formed during the period when their financial condition was at a lower level. The article presents the results of a study of the stable habits and attitudes of Russian-speaking members of the international investor club aimed at increasing their own financial flow, standard of living, and quality of life. The results showed that more than 70% of respondents correlate their habits and behavior with the impact they have on the world around them, and more than 90% note the presence of habits and attitudes of sustainable behavior. The results of the study show that the formation of the psychological attitude "I am worthy" has a positive impact on the environmental behavior of people aimed at improving their own well-being.
... These environments offer children opportunities to play independently and to engage in physical activities (El-Kholy et al., 2022), and to foster physical and mental growth (Sandseter, 2012). These settings are also important for developing nature connectedness and fostering future pro-environmental behavior (Giusti et al., 2014). However, parental concerns regarding their children's safety often limit children's access to and opportunities for physically active leisure (Allin et al., 2014;Rader et al., 2015;Thompson et al., 2005;Truong et al., 2022). ...
Article
Exposing children to green environments has multiple benefits. However, safety concerns may discourage parents from allowing their children to participate in physical leisure activities. This study employed a public participation geographic information system (PPGIS) questionnaire to explore whether and how park characteristics influence safety perceptions of 241 parents regarding their chil-dren's active, unstructured play and independent mobility. The survey was distributed to parents who were accompanying their children in Cairo's Al-Azhar and Al-Dawliya parks. Respondents identified places they considered safe and unsafe and reported their overall perceived level of safety based on photographs taken in the two parks. The results showed that parents were more afraid of the threat of crime compared to other potential risks. Moreover, secluded and densely vegetated places with inadequate lighting were perceived as unsafe. The study contributes to the ongoing discussion on creating safe environments for children and proposes ways to reduce parents' safety concerns.
... Previous studies have examined the urban contextual conditions in different distance ranges around schools as the key predictors. For example, the greenness within and surrounding school boundaries positively correlated with children's academic performance [12], stress declined caused by having nothing to do [13], mental health improvement [14], and increased empathy and concern for nature [15] which have lasting effects throughout their lives. Having a closer proximity between the school site and the urban green spaces provides more positive benefits that children acquire through daily direct experiences both for educational and physical activities. ...
Article
The aim of this study is to measure the extent of children’s direct experiences of nature within primary school environments, in relation to contextual differences according to urban, social, and pedagogical aspects. In order to achieve this, we have identified primary schools in Glasgow and Ho Chi Minh City as case studies, allowing the collection of information provided by parents of children studying in six investigated schools, and demonstrating the application of the ‘Child-Nature-Distance’ methodology. The results indicate: - a global trend in declined natural experiences of children across different social contexts; - educational approaches and urban environments importantly dominate the opportunities for children to be in contact with nature in both living and studying environments. In primary school spaces (indoor and outdoor), findings also indicate that children’s multi-sensorial natural experiences are significantly influenced by factors relating to a school’s neighbourhood areas, its built environment master planning, architectural features, and its interior design. These findings have decisive implications for the decision-making process involved in primary school architecture, and an enhanced ability to (re)connect children and nature.
... El análisis de los conocimientos y las percepciones sobre el medio ambiente permite identificar lo que las personas observan sobre los problemas del deterioro ambiental y las posibilidades de solución para manejarlos, así como su influencia en diversos aspectos que preocupan y afectan a la comunidad (Pineda et al., 2018). Sin embargo, aunque varios investigadores han encontrado que los patrones de sustentabilidad, el consumo de recursos naturales, así como las percepciones sobre el medio ambiente se gestan en las experiencias tempranas de la vida (Chawla y Cushing, 2007;Giusti, Barthel, y Marcus, 2014), aún no hay suficiente conocimiento sobre las percepciones de la naturaleza en la infancia (del Socorro Aguilar-Cucurachi, Merçon, y Silva, 2016). Por ello, el objetivo de esta investigación es analizar la relación entre tres características demográficas (género, edad y grado escolar) y la actitud al cambio climático, el nuevo paradigma ecológico y el comportamiento pro-ambiental en niños y niñas estudiando la primaria superior, en la cabecera del municipio de Santa María Coronango, una población suburbana ubicada a 25 kilómetros de la capital del estado de Puebla, en México. ...
Article
Full-text available
El objetivo de esta investigación consistió en analizar la relación entre género, edad y grado escolar respecto la actitud al cambio climático, el paradigma ecológico y el comportamiento pro-ambiental en niños y niñas que estudian la primaria superior, en Santa María Coronango, en el estado de Puebla, México, una población suburbana que se está transformando rápidamente en fraccionamientos habitacionales y comerciales, debido a su cercanía con la urbe capital. Se realizó una investigación de tipo concluyente, correlacional y transversal. Se utilizó la técnica de encuesta personal a 302 estudiantes de cuarto, quinto y sexto de primaria. Los resultados evidenciaron que no hay diferencias significativas entre niños y niñas respecto la actitud, el comportamiento y las dimensiones del paradigma ecológico. Sin embargo, se encontraron asociaciones significativas entre edad, grado escolar y la dimensión de eco-crisis del paradigma ecológico; así como entre el grado escolar y la actitud al cambio climático. El hallazgo más relevante evidenció que conforme crecen los infantes y están por entrar a la pubertad, se muestran indiferentes a la eco-crisis. Los resultados se discuten y se proponen investigaciones futuras.
... Nature exposurein particular, the amount of physical contact that an individual has with nature (30) in childhood-is thought to develop habits, preferences, and mental health (31,32). More nature exposure during childhood is thought to foster better habits, preferences, and mental health (32), for example, providing more nature exposure in daily life leads to a greater understanding of the biosphere and greater empathy for other organisms (33). In addition, the level of nature exposure during childhood is positively correlated with children's perceptions of the natural world as adults, as well as influencing their environmental behavior and awareness of environmental issues (34). ...
Article
Full-text available
Urban green spaces are critical to the healthy development of children's social interactions and activities. However, the relationship between urban green spaces for children's activities and the alleviation of children's social anxiety remains unclear. In this study, we aimed to explore the link between spatial characteristics and social anxiety in children. To explore the coupling relationships among green space, play space, and social anxiety, an assessment of children's play spaces and levels of social anxiety was performed using self-reported data, OpenStreetMap captured the characteristic indicators of urban parks, and the ArcGIS and SPSS softwares were used for the mathematical analysis. The results showed that (1) both the NDVI and 10-min accessibility were significantly negatively related to children's social anxiety; (2) the diversity of service facilities, surfacing materials, and the attractiveness and challenge of the environment were negatively related to children's social anxiety; (3) there were significant differences between activity types and social anxiety. These results provide design references and a theoretical basis for improving the benefits of urban green space on children's health and wellbeing.
... Küçük yaşlardan itibaren, bir bireyin ekonomik, ekolojik ve sosyal alanlarda sürdürülebilirliğe yönelik tutumunu arttırmak ve bilgilendirmek için ekolojik okuryazarlığın geliştirilmesine önem verilmelidir (de Brito Miranda, Jófili & dos Carneiro-Leão, 2017). Okul öncesi çocuklarının çevreye yönelik bilgi, farkındalık ve tutumlarını inceleyen çalışmalar (Akbayrak ve Kuru Turaşlı, 2017;Ärlemalm-Hagsér, 2013;Borg, Winberg & Vinterek, 2019;Buldur & Ömeroğlu, 2018, Cohen & Horm-Wingerd, 1993Evans vd, 2007;Giusti, Barthel & Marcus, 2014;Gülay Ogelman, 2012;Hsiao & Shih, 2015, Kahriman-Öztürk, Olgan & Tuncer 2012Samur, 2018;Simsar,2021;Soydan & Samur, 2014;Özen Uyar ve Yılmaz Genç, 2016) incelendiğinde, çevresel farkındalık ve tutumun gelişiminde erken çocukluk yıllarının duyarlı bir dönem olduğu, uygulanan çevre eğitimlerinin çoğunluğunun çocukların çevresel bilgi, farkındalık ve tutumlarında etkili olduğu görülmektedir. Bunun yanı sıra ekolojik okuryazarlık eğitimine yönelik çalışmaların sınırlı sayıda yer aldığı, özellikle insan-doğa ilişkisinin vurgulandığı, ekolojik sorunları tanımlama, farkında olma ve çözümün bir parçası olmayı destekleyen eğitimlere ihtiyaç olduğu belirlenmiştir. ...
Article
Full-text available
Bu araştırmada, Sürdürülebilir Yaşam İçin Ekolojik Okuryazarım Eğitimi’nin okul öncesi çocuklarının çevreye yönelik farkındalık ve tutumlarına etkisinin incelenmesi amaçlanmıştır. Bu amaçla çocuklara Eymir Gölü, TBMM Botanik Bahçesi ve Okul bahçelerinde-TEMA Etkinlikleri olmak üzere üç aşamada ekolojik okuryazarlık eğitimi verilmiştir. Tek gruplu ön test-son test deneysel deseninde yürütülen çalışmaya 24 okuldan 360 okul öncesi dönem çocuğu dâhil edilmiştir. Çocuklardan veriler, Çevreye Yönelik Tutum Ölçeği ile Çevreye Yönelik Farkındalık Ölçeği ile toplanmıştır. Araştırmanın verilerinin çözümlenmesi amacıyla betimsel istatistikler ve ilişkili ölçümlerde t testi hesaplanmıştır. Araştırmanın sonucunda ekolojik okuryazarlık eğitimi kapsamındaki etkinliklerin çocukların çevreye yönelik farkındalık ve tutumlarını anlamlı bir şekilde artırdığı tespit edilmiştir.
Chapter
That green areas in rapidly urbanising landscapes are important for human well-being is well established. Parks, woodlands, nature reserves, street trees and backyards provide multiple benefits, e.g. through providing physical, mental, social, educational and cultural benefits. These effects should reach all ‘the people’. However, a precondition is accessibility, which has received growing research attention. The aim of this chapter is to contribute to this body of knowledge by presenting and discussing accessibility to urban nature from the viewpoint of people whose mobility is dependent on a wheelchair. Very few studies have focused on this group, and then primarily on physical access. We include a broader perspective on underlying factors affecting accessibility—juxtaposed with an environmental justice approach. Our exploration rests primarily on interviews with people dependent on a wheelchair and representatives of supporting organisations. To this is added data from an online survey. Our results highlight that access to urban green areas must be understood in a broad sense, e.g. where physical access includes the whole route from home and back again, and where mental and social access is equally crucial. Further, green areas include the whole spectrum from a window-view to an urban national park. The study sends valuable signals to planners, e.g. concerning and including people in wheelchairs in all stages of planning and maintenance.KeywordsAccessibility in a broad senseVulnerabilityEnvironmental justiceUrban landscapesGreen commons
Chapter
The often impassioned nature of environmental conflicts can be attributed to the fact that they are bound up with our sense of personal and social identity. Environmental identity—how we orient ourselves to the natural world—leads us to personalize abstract global issues and take action (or not) according to our sense of who we are. We may know about the greenhouse effect—but can we give up our SUV for a more fuel-efficient car? Understanding this psychological connection can lead to more effective pro-environmental policymaking. Identity and the Natural Environment examines the ways in which our sense of who we are affects our relationship with nature, and vice versa. This book brings together cutting-edge work on the topic of identity and the environment, sampling the variety and energy of this emerging field but also placing it within a descriptive framework. These theory-based, empirical studies locate environmental identity on a continuum of social influence, and the book is divided into three sections reflecting minimal, moderate, or strong social influence. Throughout, the contributors focus on the interplay between social and environmental forces; as one local activist says, "We don't know if we're organizing communities to plant trees, or planting trees to organize communities."
Conference Paper
In my first paper to this Symposium, it was argued that the human cognitive subjectplayed a key part the shaping and working of the city. The key mechanism was thesynchronisation of diachronically experienced (and usually diachronically created)information into higher order pictures of spatial relations, the guiding form for whichwas an abstracted notion of a grid formed by linearised spaces. This notion wasargued to be both perceptual and conceptual, serving at once as an abstractedrepresentation of the space of the city and as a means of solving problems, such asnavigational problems. In this paper, the question addressed is where the notion ofthe ideal grid comes from, why it has the properties it does, and what it has to dowith the real grids of cities, which are commonly of the 'deformed' or 'interrupted'rather than 'ideal' kinds (Hillier, 1996). The answer, it is proposed, lies in the verynature of complex spaces, defining these as spaces in which objects are placed so asto partially block seeing and going, and, in particular, in certain divergences in thelogics of metric and visual accessibility in such spaces. The real grid, deformed orinterrupted, is, it is argued the practical resolution of these divergent logics, and theideal grid its abstract resolution. In both resolutions, however, the resolution is moreon the terms of the visual than the metric, suggesting that cognitive factors are morepowerful than metric factors in shaping the space of the city. The question is thanraised: do people have or acquire the concept of the grid, perhaps as some kind ofperceptual-conceptual invariance of spatial experience in complex spaces, and dothey use it as a model to interact with complex spatial patterns of the urban kind?This possibility is examined against the background of current opinion in the cognitivesciences.