ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Abstract

The acute effects of chewing gum on cognitive performance, stress, and pain have been intensively studied the last decade. The results have been contradicting, and replication studies proved challenging. Here, we review some of the recent findings of this topic, and explore possible explanations for these discrepancies by incorporating knowledge derived from studies into oral habits and bruxism. Both stress and cerebral functional specialization (i.e., the involvement of specific brain structures in distinctive cognitive processes) are hypothesized to play a major role in the underlying physiological mechanisms of the diverse effects of chewing gum on cognition, stress and pain.
Review Article
Chew the Pain Away: Oral Habits to Cope with Pain and
Stress and to Stimulate Cognition
Roxane Anthea Francesca Weijenberg1and Frank Lobbezoo2
1Department of Clinical Neuropsychology, VU University Amsterdam, Van der Boechorststraat 1, 1081 BT Amsterdam, Netherlands
2Department of Oral Kinesiology, Academic Centre for Dentistry Amsterdam (ACTA), University of Amsterdam and
VU University Amsterdam, MOVE Research Institute Amsterdam, Gustav Mahlerlaan 3004, 1081 LA Amsterdam, Netherlands
Correspondence should be addressed to Roxane Anthea Francesca Weijenberg; r.a.f.weijenberg@vu.nl
Received  September ; Accepted  December 
Academic Editor: Jian-Hua Liu
Copyright ©  R. A. F. Weijenberg and F. Lobbezoo. is is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons
Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is
properly cited.
e acute eects of chewing gum on cognitive performance, stress, and pain have been intensively studied in the last decade. e
results have been contradicting, and replication studies proved challenging. Here, we review some of the recent ndings of this
topic and explore possible explanations for these discrepancies by incorporating knowledge derived from studies into oral habits
and bruxism. Both stress and cerebral functional specialization (i.e., the involvement of specic brain structures in distinctive
cognitive processes) are hypothesized to play a major role in the underlying physiological mechanisms of the diverse eects of
chewing gum on cognition, stress, and pain.
1. Introduction
Mastication is essential for grinding our food into smaller
particles []. During chewing, saliva is added to the particles
to lubricate them and create a food bolus that can be
swallowed []. Recently, reports have been published in the
literature, stating that mastication might also serve other
purposes, such as countering negative eects of stress []or
aiding in cognitive function [,].Itcouldbearguedthatthe
positive eects of mastication are similar to those of physical
activity []. Although mastication is of course not the same
as an intensive workout, it does have properties similar to
exercise: it also increases heart rates [,] and cerebral blood
ow []. ere are also direct cardiovascular improve-
ments resulting from exercise []. Physical activity is a key
element of an enriched environment, so while exercising, one
is also experiencing an enriched environment [], which
has been shown to improve cognition []. Exercising can
also attenuate the negative eects of stress []. Several
studies have explored the relationship between mastication,
cognition, stress, and pain. ese studies will be discussed
in more detail below. e aim of this review was to explore
the relationship between mastication, cognition, and pain
and to hypothesize on possible explanations.
2. Oral Habits
2.1. Gum Chewing. In two recent reviews [,], the eects
of chewing a piece of gum on cognition and stress in human
volunteers are described. e outcomes of these reviews
are summarized in Tabl e  . Although the papers have great
overlap in the literature they included, the authors sometimes
come to dierent conclusions. For example, while, in one
paper,theauthorsemphasizethatchewinggumcannotbe
seen as an aid for mental challenges [], in the other paper,
the authors conclude that chewing gum enhances alertness
and that it might very well improve cognitive performance
[]. is discrepancy can partly be explained by the obser-
vation that Allen and Smith [] are more lenient in their
conclusions, as they view the majority vote as convincing,
whereasTuchaandKoerts[] focus on the contradictions
and possible detrimental eects of chewing gum.
Nevertheless, both reviews agree that working memory is
positively aected by chewing a piece of gum. It is interesting
Hindawi Publishing Corporation
BioMed Research International
Volume 2015, Article ID 149431, 7 pages
http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2015/149431
BioMed Research International
T : e outcomes of two reviews on the eects of mastication
on cognition and stress in healthy volunteers.
Vari a b l e
Allen and
Smith, 
[]
Tucha and
Koerts, 
[]
Cognitive outcomes
Academic performance +
Alertness
Subjective +
Attention
Divided 00
Selective ±±
Shiing ±
Sustained/vigilance ±±
Executive functioning 0
Memory
Context dependent
Recall ±±
Recognition
Working + +
Test p e r f o r manc e
Speed + ±
Spatial skill ±
Stress related outcomes
Biomarkers (i.e., pupil
dilation, heart rate) ±
Acute, self-reported 0
Chronic, self-reported +
Salivary cortisol ±
e outcomes of two reviews on mastication, cognition, and stress. + = the
authors report a positive eect; = the authors report a negative eect;  =
the authors report no eect; ±= the authors report contradicting results in
the literature.
that both also agree in their conclusion that divided atten-
tion is unaected. Some earlier papers mentioned that the
distracting (novelty) eect of chewing gum while performing
a task might have inuenced test results, but the fact that
divided attention is not aected argues against this.
As there are still many unexplained and contradictory
ndings, a nal conclusion cannot yet be made on the acute
eects of chewing a piece of gum on cognitive performance
and stress. Underlying physiological mechanisms remain to
be identied, and the time on task might be of inuence,
for example, how long participants chewed and whether they
chewed only prior to examination or also during the test. It is
possible that chewing gum has a transient positive eect, but
only aer cessation of chewing, since chewing while on task
can have a negative eect [,]. It would be interesting to
seewhateectotheroralhabitshaveonstressandcognition.
2.2. Bruxism. In a sample of children (– years), with
attention decit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), it was
observed that levels of oral habits such as nail or pencil
bitingandbruxismwereallelevated[]. Bruxism is “a
repetitive jaw-muscle activity characterized by clenching or
grinding of the teeth and/or by bracing or thrusting of the
mandible” []. e activity is involuntary and can occur
during sleep (sleep bruxism) or during waking moments
(awake bruxism) []. Nail biting is an oral habit that is com-
monlyseeninthegeneralpopulation,andtriggersforitare,
amongst others, anxiety, stress [], boredom, and frustration
[]. Anxiety, stress, boredom, and frustration are also the
most common triggers for other (pathological) body-focused
repetitive behaviors, such as skin picking and hair pulling
[]. e need that is being met by engaging in these self-
damaging behaviors is thought to be “relief from negative
aective states” [].
ose suering from their habits, for example, brux-
ers with complaints of pain or other temporomandibular
disorders (TMD), usually seek the help from their dentist
rather than a psychologist [].isisperhapsreected
in the current popular treatment options for bruxism: it
is typically treated with oral splints, which is reected in
a publication bias with regard to bruxism therapies [].
Behavioral therapies are the least popular therapies to be sci-
entically explored, while pharmaceutical approaches have
been gaining popularity []. Current therapies seem to
be focused on preventing damage, rather than nding and
treating the cause []. is is most unfortunate,of course, as
options for treatments are disregarded this way. Risk factors
for bruxism were dened as peripheral, such as malocclusion,
or central (pathophysiological or psychosocial) [,]. It
was concluded that peripheral causes are not likely to play
a signicant role in the etiology of bruxism [,]. It
can be argued that bruxism is a disorder from the central
neurotransmitter system since the basal ganglia (part of
the extrapyramidal system) and the thalamic pathways are
implicated in the origin of bruxism, with a crucial role to play
by the neurotransmitter dopamine []. Experienced stress
plays a mediating role, at least in awake bruxism [], while
others argue that stress might even be the main cause for
bruxism [].
is latter theory is tting with the observations that
experienced daily life stress is related to daytime clenching
[] and that self-reported bruxers have higher anxiety levels
and more oen experience severe stress compared to healthy
controls [].
3. Mastication, Stress, and Pain
3.1. Mastication and Stress Relief. Chewing and clenching
have been implied as a way to relieve stress and provide
relaxation []. e chewing force needed to chew a piece of
gum correlated to the amount of salivary cortisol reduction
aer performing a stressful task []. In restrained rats, the
length of stress-induced bruxism activity correlated inversely
to physiological parameters of stress, such as blood cortisol
and adrenaline levels []. Whether this stress relieving eect
is robust over prolonged periods of time is not yet clear. A lon-
gitudinal study examined the eect of regular gum chewing
during  days (leisurely chewing a piece of gum for at least 
minutes, twice per day) in young adults []. Aer these two
BioMed Research International
weeks, chewing gum was associated with decreased scores
for anxiety, depression, fatigue, and confusion compared to a
control group. is benet of chewing was transient; however,
as aer  weeks (i.e., two weeks aer stopping with the
intervention), there was no longer a dierence between the
groups []. Another experiment showed a transient cerebral
response to changes in the masticatory domain: using fMRI, it
was shown that aer being tted with a new dental prosthesis,
adaptive brain activation in the right and the le precentral
and postcentral gyrus occurred during oromotor tasks like
jaw clenching, but only in the rst three months, even though
the prosthesis was worn continuously [].
3.1.1. Salivary Cortisol. Some comments with regard to cor-
tisol assessments need to be made. ere are two types
of human cortisol that can be assessed: total cortisol and
free cortisol. e latter can be sampled in blood, urine,
or saliva []. Levine and colleagues []emphasizethat
despite current popularity of these “biomarkers of stress,
the question remains unanswered whether these assessments
reect actual metabolic and cerebral functioning, namely,
activity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis,
as the hypotheses on which these assessments rely are, in
fact, still hypotheses []. Salivary cortisol assessments can
provide some useful information, but the sampling technique
is prone to false results, for example, due to pH changes aer
eating or drinking or due to contamination by blood from
oral lesions []. Furthermore, only small correlations have
been found between salivary cortisol and plasma free cortisol
measurements (the latter being the gold standard) []. In
another comprehensive review discussing salivary cortisol, it
is shown that there is in fact little scientic support for the
popularly assumed correlation between psychological stress
and the endocrine response []. Hellhammer and colleagues
[] emphasize that although salivary cortisol can be used as
a biomarker for perceived stress, this can only be done with
great caution, and one must be aware that there will only
be a moderate association with perceived, or task-induced,
stress []. e salivary cortisol response is inuenced by
many factors, such as estrogens (gender, menstrual phase,
and oral contraceptives), certain drugs, presence of chronic
stress, long-term exercise [], and overall physical tness
[]. Sleep and the circadian rhythm also inuence the HPA
axis activity [,]andnallyonemustkeepinmindthat
cortisol levels also are not stable during the waking hours but
exhibit ultradian oscillations []. Clearly, reports relying on
salivarycortisolassessmentshavetobeviewedwithsome
caution.
3.2. Mastication and Pain. It is known that, in newborn
babies, rhythmic oral motions, such as during breastfeed-
ing or sucking on a pacier, but also sweet taste such as
that from breast milk, or glucose or sucrose solutions, are
nonpharmacological approaches for pain relief []. Building
on this knowledge, it was investigated whether sweet taste,
chewing gum, or a combination of both could relieve pain
in –-year-old children, undergoing venipuncture or vac-
cination []. e authors did not nd an overall eect of
theinterventiononpainresponses.Inboys,continuously
chewing unsweetened gum reduced pain scores and ratings of
unpleasantness. In girls, however, the opposite was observed:
chewing sweetened gum reduced pain scores, but chewing
unsweetened gum increased them []. It should be added
that since the control group was also given chewing gum,
prior to the procedure, it is possible that some carry-over
eect of this chewing took place. On the other hand, the chil-
dren chewed gum for only a brief period: the control group
chewed for  minute, and the intervention group chewed for 
minutes and during the short medical procedure. It is possible
that the overall time was too brief to evoke a strong response
to chewing.
In another experiment to investigate the eect of chewing
on pain and possible underlying neural mechanisms, partici-
pants were submitted to nociceptive exion reex (NFR) pro-
tocol []. e NFR protocol encompasses a painful electro-
cutaneous stimulation of the lower leg, aer which the muscle
activity in the upper leg on the same side is measured [].
Blood samples were taken and brain perfusion was assessed
with near-infrared spectroscopy measuring (de)oxygenated
hemoglobin []. e subjects chewed a piece of mint-
avored gum leisurely for  minutes. Assessments (applying
the NFR and taking blood samples) were made at baseline,
immediately aer chewing and  minutes aer chewing.
Gum chewing decreased the NFR both immediately aer and
 minutes aer chewing []. Serotonergic blood levels were
increased aer chewing, and signicant cerebral perfusion
was increased in the ventral part of prefrontal cortex (PFC)
[]. e authors conclude that chewing a piece of gum
apparently has analgesic eects, with the PFC mediating
this eect through serotonergic neurons of the dorsal raphe
nucleus []. e same dorsal raphe nucleus is implicated
in the origin of disordered eating [], in a hypothesis that
by changing the eating behavior (e.g., adhering to a food-
restricted diet) one also changes the serotonergic pathways
betweenthePFCanddorsalraphenucleusandthusalters
mood. is theory seems to t with the other observations
that stimulation through chewing might have benecial
eects on aect.
In an animal experimental study, rats were fed a so diet
for  days []. Subsequently, they were injected in one of the
paws with complete Freund’s adjuvant (CFA) to temporarily
increase their sensitivity to pain (hyperalgesia). In the fol-
lowing – days, they were fed hard food (intervention) or
continued on so food (control). en, a heat stimulus was
separately applied to both paws, and the reaction time (with-
drawal latency) was measured. e dierence in reaction
speed between the injected and the control paw was taken
as a measure for the induced hyperalgesia. Rats on the hard
diet showed less CFA-induced hyperalgesia. is protective
eect of hard food was gone aer injection with the opioid-
antagonist naloxone []. Hard food was also protective
against inammation, as was shown by a decrease in activity
in immunoreactive cells. Inhibiting sensory pathways, by
cutting the inferior alveolar nerve or removing the primary
somatosensory cortex, reduced but not completely reversed
this eect []. e authors conclude that a protective eect
from hard food might involve the opioid system, which is
aected by sensory pathways, but perhaps also by other
BioMed Research International
neural pathways, such as the brainstem reticular formation
[].
A coupling of the endogenous opioid system and the
stress response has been widely studied and is well established
[,]. For example, it was found that endogenous opioid
systems attenuate the stress response in pregnancy [].
Underlying mechanisms are being studied: the attenuating
eect of suppression of kappa-opioid receptors on the stress
response has been shown in a wide range of animal models
[], and endogenous opioids were found to negate the
detrimental eects of stress hormones by protecting the
endothelial function, a condition which is thought to underlie
cardiovascular disease []. e locus coeruleus is thought to
play a key role in the interaction of the opioid system and the
stress response [].
4. Discussion
e literature discussed above has shown that there currently
is an interest in the relationship between mastication, cogni-
tion, stress, and pain. e acute eects of chewing gum on
cognitive measures and biomarkers of stress, such as salivary
cortisol, have not yet generated univocal results. However,
a closer examination of cerebral functional specialization of
cognitive functions might provide insight into the ambiguous
results that are currently being reported.
4.1. Cerebral Functional Specialization. e subcortical basal
gangliaarehypothesizedtoplayaroleinbruxism[].
Interestingly,theyarealsoinvolvedincognitivefunctions,
such as the nondeclarative memory (also known as the pro-
cedural memory) []. Basal ganglia are also known to play a
role in habit learning, most notably through feedback based
learning (rewards and/or punishment) regardless of whether
this learning is implicit or explicit []. is concurs with the
nding that chewing a piece of gum can enhance working
memory [], which was assessed by having participants
performing a routine that had to become habituated, and the
nding that spatial memory (learning the route to an escape
platform) was impaired in mice by removing the upper molar
crowns [].
e working memory enhancement of chewing gum
would indicate a positive stimulus of chewing for the PFC
and basal ganglia [] and also for the medial temporal lobe
and hippocampus for longer tasks []. Interestingly, short-
term working memory function was not negatively impacted
by physiological stress, but it was observed that task diculty
(which could be considered psychological stress) negatively
inuenced performance []. Participants showed elevated
levels of activation in the PFC while maintaining their
performance levels, a cerebral response which is thought to
compensate for the distraction of the stressor []. If chewing
reduces stress, this would facilitate the compensatory process
and thus positively aect working memory performance.
Both the hippocampus and the PFC are known for their
sensitivity to stress [], and a reduction of stress due to
chewing would explain the positive eect of gum on the
behavioral outcomes.
Declarative memory function (which can be subdi-
vided into episodic/autobiographic and semantic memory)
is involved in recall and recognition tasks []. is type
of memory is localized throughout the brain, including the
medial and temporal lobes [], but key areas for recall
memory are the frontal and parietal lobes and the cerebellum,
while learning and storing new memories (i.e., reproduction
and recognition) involve the hippocampus and the parahip-
pocampal gyrus []. e fact that these kinds of memory
are not enhanced by chewing a piece of gum is therefore
perhapsnotsurprising,astheydonotrelyprimarilyon
the basal ganglia. Most of the studies included in the two
reviews discussed here [,] did not use giving feedback
(punishment or rewards) in their paradigm, and thus, they
did not activate the basal ganglia []. Recently, Bos et al.
showed that recall and recognition of newly learned words
were improved in a stress condition (i.e., putting the hand in
painfully cold water) [].isisalsottingwiththecurrent
observations of the eects of chewing gum: if chewing indeed
reduces stress, this would also negate any stress-induced
enhancement of the long-term memory.
e lack or even detrimental eect of chewing on vigi-
lance is most likely a negative eect on the vigilance network
and the ascending reticular activating system (ARAS) [].
e ARAS has also been suggested to play a role in sleep
bruxism: minutes prior to the onset of sleep bruxism, arousal
responses such as increased heart rate and muscle tone are
observed in bruxers, which are indicators of ARAS activation
[]. It might be possible that active mastication reverses the
same pathway and thus downregulates the ARAS, causing
the negative eect on vigilance. Others reported that sleep
bruxers do not perform better or worse than controls in
neuropsychological tests for vigilance or motor response [],
which is tting with the current observations.
5. Conclusion
Mastication, as other physical activities, can most likely
relieve (acute) stress and even pain. Bruxers and nail biters
might unknowingly draw upon this eect, in order to alleviate
their commonly reported anxiety. Active mastication might
improvesomemeasuresofcognitiveperformance,suchas
working memory [,] or subjective alertness []. Bruxers
have not been shown to display cognitive advantages over
nonbruxers, with regard to vigilance []. is is perhaps
not surprising, as chewing has not been shown to enhance
vigilance either and, in fact, negatively eects it in children
[]. It would be interesting to see if bruxers outperform
nonbruxers in other cognitive domains, such as spatial or
working memory. Treatment for bruxism typically shows a
dental focus, oering splints or other occlusal appliances.
Counselingbyapsychologistandaphysicaltherapistinorder
to learn relaxation can be complementary to this [].
Otherpopulationsthatmightbenetfromtheseinsights
are persons at risk for cognitive decline or mental insta-
bility, such as older persons suering from dementia and
psychiatric patients. ey might experience positive eects
from oral and dental care and eating a diet that consists of
hard, chewing-enhancing foods. Maintenance of masticatory
BioMed Research International
function should be endeavored for all clinical groups. How-
ever, the long-term use of chewing gum and engaging in other
habits should not be encouraged, as this increases the risk
for complaints of fatigue, tenderness, and even pain in the
musculoskeletal structures of the masticatory system [
].
Highlights
(i) e heterogeneous eects of chewing gum on cogni-
tive performance can partially be explained by cere-
bral functional specialization and the involvement of
the basal ganglia.
(ii) Stress and relief of stress can play an important role
in the physiological mechanisms underlying these
eects.
(iii) Oral habits such as bruxism might draw upon the
same eects for stress relief.
(iv) Active chewing might relieve stress or pain, but long-
term engagement in oral habits increases the risk of
fatigue, pain, or temporomandibular disorders.
Conflict of Interests
e authors declare that there is no conict of interests
regarding the publication of this paper.
References
[] A. van der Bilt, “Assessment of mastication with implications for
oral rehabilitation: a review,Journal of Oral Rehabilitation,vol.
,no.,pp.,.
[]Y.Ono,T.Kataoka,S.Miyakeetal.,“Chewingameliorates
stress-induced suppression of hippocampal long-term potenti-
ation,Neuroscience, vol. , no. , pp. –, .
[] Y.Ono,T.Yamamoto,K.-Y.Kubo,andM.Onozuka,“Occlusion
and brain function: mastication as a prevention of cognitive
dysfunction,JournalofOralRehabilitation,vol.,no.,pp.
–, .
[] K.-Y. Kubo, Y. Ichihashi, C. Kurata et al., “Masticatory function
and cognitive function,Okajimas Folia Anatomica Japonica,
vol.,no.,pp.,.
[] R. A. F. Weijenberg, E. J. A. Scherder, and F. Lobbezoo,
“Mastication for the mind-e relationship between mastica-
tion and cognition in ageing and dementia,Neuroscience and
Biobehavioral Reviews,vol.,no.,pp.,.
[] E.Nitta,Y.Iwasa,M.Sugita,C.Hirono,andY.Shiba,“Roleof
mastication and swallowing in the control of autonomic ner-
vous activity for heart rate in dierent postures,Journal of Oral
Rehabilitation,vol.,no.,pp.,.
[] Y.Shiba,E.Nitta,C.Hirono,M.Sugita,andY.Iwasa,“Evalua-
tion of mastication-induced change in sympatho-vagal balance
through spectral analysis of heart rate variability,Journal of
Oral Rehabilitation,vol.,no.,pp.,.
[] B. Kordass, C. Lucas, D. Huetzen et al., “Functional magnetic
resonance imaging of brain activity during chewing and occlu-
sion by natural teeth and occlusal splints,Annals of Anatomy,
vol. , no. , pp. –, .
[]T.Ono,Y.Hasegawa,K.Hori,T.Nokubi,andT.Hamasaki,
“Task-induced activation and hemispheric dominance in cere-
bral circulation during gum chewing,Journal of Neurology,vol.
,no.,pp.,.
[] Y. Hasegawa, T. Ono, J. Sakagami et al., “Inuence of voluntary
control of masticatory side and rhythm on cerebral hemody-
namics,Clinical Oral Investigations,vol.,no.,pp.,
.
[] M. Hamer, “Psychosocial stress and cardiovascular disease risk:
theroleofphysicalactivity,Psychosomatic Medicine,vol.,no.
,pp.,.
[] K. M. Volkers and E. J. A. Scherder, “Impoverished environ-
ment, cognition, aging and dementia,Reviews in the Neuro-
sciences,vol.,no.,pp.,.
[] E. Kra, “Cognitive function, physical activity, and aging:
possible biological links and implications for multimodal inter-
ventions,Aging,Neuropsychology,and Cognition,vol.,no.-,
pp. –, .
[] J. Tortosa-Mart´
ınez and A. Clow, “Does physical activity reduce
risk for Alzheimer’s disease through interaction with the stress
neuroendocrine system?” Stress,vol.,no.,pp.,.
[] A. P. Allen and A. P. Smith, “A review of the evidence that
chewing gum aects stress, alertness and cognition,Journal of
Behavioral and Neuroscience Research,vol.,no.,pp.,
.
[] L. K. Tucha and J. Koerts, “Gum chewing and cognition: an
overview,Neuroscience & Medicine,vol.,no.,pp.,
.
[] O. Sabuncuoglu, C. Orengul, A. Bikmazer, and S. Y. Kaynar,
“Breastfeeding and parafunctional oral habits in children with
and without attention-decit/hyperactivity disorder,Breast-
feeding Medicine,vol.,no.,pp.,.
[] F. Lobbezoo, J. Ahlberg, A. G. Glaros et al., “Bruxism dened
and graded: an international consensus,JournalofOralReha-
bilitation,vol.,no.,pp.,.
[] F. Lobbezoo, J. Ahlberg, D. Manfredini, and E. Winocur,
Arebruxismandthebitecausallyrelated?Journal of Oral
Rehabilitation,vol.,no.,pp.,.
[] O.M.Tanaka,R.W.F.Vitral,G.Y.Tanaka,A.P.Guerrero,and
E. S. Camargo, “Nailbiting, or onychophagia: a special habit,
American Journal of Orthodontics and Dentofacial Orthopedics,
vol. , no. , pp. –, .
[] T. I. Williams, R. Rose, and S. Chisholm, “What is the function
of nail biting: an analog assessment study,Behaviour Research
and erapy, vol. , no. , pp. –, .
[] S. Roberts, K. O’Connor, and C. B´
elanger, “Emotion regulation
and other psychological models for body-focused repetitive
behaviors,Clinical Psychology Review,vol.,no.,pp.
, .
[]F.Lobbezoo,J.vanderZaag,M.K.A.vanSelms,H.L.
Hamburger, and M. Naeije, “Principles for the management of
bruxism,Journal of Oral Rehabilitation,vol.,no.,pp.
, .
[] J. L. de-la-Hoz, “Sleep bruxism: review and update for the
restorative dentist,Alpha Omegan,vol.,no.-,pp.,
.
[] F. Lobbezoo and M. Naeije, “Bruxism is mainly regulated
centrally, not peripherally,Journal of Oral Rehabilitation,vol.
, no. , pp. –, .
[] F. Lobbezoo, J. van der Zaag, and M. Naeije, “Bruxism: its
multiple causes and its eects on dental implants—an updated
BioMed Research International
review,JournalofOralRehabilitation,vol.,no.,pp.
, .
[] M. Wieckiewicz, A. Paradowska-Stolarz, and W. Wieckiewicz,
“Psychosocial aspects of bruxism: the most paramount factor
inuencing teeth grinding,BioMed Research International,vol.
,ArticleID,pages,.
[] D. Manfredini and F. Lobbezoo, “Role of psychosocial factors in
the etiology of bruxism,JournalofOrofacialPain,vol.,no.
, pp. –, .
[] M.K.A.VanSelms,F.Lobbezoo,D.J.Wicks,H.L.Hamburger,
and M. Naeije, “Craniomandibular pain, oral parafunctions,
and psychological stress in a longitudinal case study,Journal
of Oral Rehabilitation,vol.,no.,pp.,.
[]J.Ahlberg,F.Lobbezoo,K.Ahlbergetal.,“Self-reported
bruxism mirrors anxiety and stress in adults,Medicina Oral,
Patologia Oral y Cirugia Bucal,vol.,no.,pp.ee,.
[] Y.Tahara,K.Sakurai,andT.Ando,“Inuenceofchewingand
clenching on salivary cortisol levels as an indicator of stress,
Journal of Prosthodontics, vol. , no. , pp. –, .
[] R. Soeda, A. Tasaka, and K. Sakurai, “Inuence of chewing force
on salivary stress markers as indicator of mental stress,” Journal
of Oral Rehabilitation,vol.,no.,pp.,.
[] C. Sato, S. Sato, H. Takashina, H. Ishii, M. Onozuka, and K.
Sasaguri, “Bruxism aects stress responses in stressed rats,
Clinical Oral Investigations,vol.,no.,pp.,.
[] A. Sasaki-Otomaru, Y. Sakuma, Y. Mochizuki, S. Ishida, Y.
Kanoya, and C. Sato, “Eect of regular gum chewing on levels
of anxiety, mood, and fatigue in healthy young adults,” Clinical
PracticeandEpidemiologyinMentalHealth,vol.,pp.,
.
[] J. Luraschi, M. S. Korgaonkar, T. Whittle, M. Schimmel, F.
M¨
uller, and I. Klineberg, “Neuroplasticity in the adaptation to
prosthodontic treatment,Journal of Orofacial Pain,vol.,no.
, pp. –, .
[] A. Levine, O. Zagoory-Sharon, R. Feldman, J. G. Lewis, and A.
Weller, “Measuring cortisol in human psychobiological studies,
Physiology and Behavior,vol.,no.,pp.,.
[] D. H. Hellhammer, S. W ¨
ust, and B. M. Kudielka, “Salivary
cortisol as a biomarker in stress research,Psychoneuroen-
docrinology,vol.,no.,pp.,.
[] E. Van Cauter, M. Balbo, and R. Leproult, “Impact of sleep and
its disturbances on hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis activ-
ity,International Journal of Endocrinology,vol.,ArticleID
,  pages, .
[] S.Postnova,R.Fulcher,H.A.Braun,andP.A.Robinson,“A
minimal physiologically based model of the HPA axis under
inuence of the sleep-wake cycles,Pharmacopsychiatry,vol.,
supplement , pp. S–S, .
[] R. Carbajal, S. Gr´
eteau, C. Arnaud, and R. Guedj, “Pain
in neonatology. Non-pharmacological treatment,Archives de
P´
ediatrie,.
[] M. D. Lewkowski, R. G. Barr, A. Sherrard, J. Lessard, A. R.
Harris,andS.N.Young,“Eectsofchewinggumonresponses
to routine painful procedures in children,Physiology and
Behavior,vol.,no.,pp.,.
[] K. Kamiya, M. Fumoto, H. Kikuchi et al., “Prolonged gum
chewing evokes activation of the ventral part of prefrontal
cortex and suppression of nociceptive responses: involvement
of the serotonergic system,JournalofMedicalandDental
Sciences,vol.,no.,pp.,.
[] J. L. Rhudy and C. R. France, “Dening the nociceptive exion
reex (NFR) threshold in human participants: a comparison of
dierent scoring criteria,Pain,vol.,no.,pp.,.
[] I. Ioakimidis, M. Zandian, F. Ulbl, C. Bergh, M. Leon, and P.
S¨
odersten, “How eating aects mood,Physiology and Behavior,
vol. , no. -, pp. –, .
[] A. Ogawa, T. Morimoto, J. W. Hu et al., “Hard-food mastication
suppresses complete Freund’s adjuvant-induced nociception,
Neuroscience,vol.,no.,pp.,.
[] A. VanT Veer and W. A. Carlezon Jr., “Role of kappa-opioid
receptors in stress and anxiety-related behavior,Psychophar-
macology, vol. , no. , pp. –, .
[] E. J. Van Bockstaele, B. A. S. Reyes, and R. J. Valentino, “e
locus coeruleus: a key nucleus where stress and opioids intersect
to mediate vulnerability to opiate abuse,Brain Research,vol.
, pp. –, .
[] P. J. Brunton and J. A. Russell, “Neuroendocrine control of
maternal stress responses and fetal programming by stress in
pregnancy,Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological
Psychiatry, vol. , no. , pp. –, .
[] N. Toda and M. Nakanishi-Toda, “How mental stress aects
endothelial function,Pugers Archiv European Journal of Phys-
iology,vol.,no.,pp.,.
[] M. H. M. Meeter, “Geheugen,” in Klinische Neuropsychologie,
R. Kessels, Ed., pp. –, Uitgeverij Boom, Amsterdam, e
Netherlands, .
[] K. Foerde and D. Shohamy, “e role of the basal ganglia
in learning and memory: insight from Parkinsons disease,
Neurobiology of Learning and Memory,vol.,no.,pp.
, .
[] Y.Hirano,T.Obata,K.Kashikuraetal.,“Eectsofchewingin
working memory processing,Neuroscience Letters,vol.,no.
, pp. –, .
[] M. Onozuka, K. Watanabe, S. M. Mirbod et al., “Reduced
mastication stimulates impairment of spatial memory and
degeneration of hippocampal neurons in aged SAMP mice,
Brain Research,vol.,no.,pp.,.
[] F. McNab and T. Klingberg , “Prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia
control access to working memory,Nature Neuroscience, vol. ,
no. , pp. –, .
[] N. K. R. Gudrun, “Ruimtelijke cognitie,” in Klinische Neu-
ropsychologie,R.Kessels,Ed.,pp.,UitgeverijBoom,
Amsterdam, e Netherlands, .
[] A. J. Porcelli, D. Cruz, K. Wenberg, M. D. Patterson,B. B. Biswal,
and B. Rypma, “e eects of acute stress on human prefrontal
working memory systems,Physiology & Behavior,vol.,no.
, pp. –, .
[] B. S. McEwen, “Central eects of stress hormones in health and
disease: understanding the protective and damaging eects of
stress and stress mediators,European Journal of Pharmacology,
vol.,no.-,pp.,.
[] M. G. N. Bos, J. Schuijer, F. Lodestijn, T. Beckers, and M.
Kindt, “Stress enhances reconsolidation of declarative memory,
Psychoneuroendocrinology,vol.,pp.,.
[] D. H. E. Boelen, L. Fasotti, and J. M. Spikman, “Aandacht en
executieve functies,” in Klinische Neuropsychologie,R.Kessels,
Ed.,vol.,pp.,UitgeverijBoom,Amsterdam,e
Netherlands, .
[] G. J. Lavigne, N. Huynh, T. Kato et al., “Genesis of sleep bruxism:
motor and autonomic-cardiac interactions,Archives of Oral
Biology,vol.,no.,pp.,.
BioMed Research International
[] M. Major, P. H. Rompr´
e,F.Guitardetal.,“Acontrolleddaytime
challenge of motor performance and vigilance in sleep bruxers,
JournalofDentalResearch,vol.,no.,pp.,.
[] L. Tucha, W. Simpson, L. Evans et al., “Detrimental eects of
gumchewingonvigilanceinchildrenwithattentiondecit
hyperactivity disorder,Appetite, vol. , no. , pp. –,
.
[] P. Maillou, S. W. Cadden, and F. Lobbezoo, “e inhibitory
eect of a chewing task on a human jaw reex,Muscle & Nerve,
vol. , no. , pp. –, .
[] M. Koutris, F. Lobbezoo, M. Naeije et al., “Eects of intense
chewing exercises on the masticatory sensory-motor system,
JournalofDentalResearch, vol. , no. , pp. –, .
[] N. C. W. van der Kaaij, P. Maillou, J. J. van der Weijden, M.
Naeije, and F. Lobbezoo, “Reproducible eects of subjectively
assessed muscle fatigue on an inhibitory jaw reex in humans,
Archives of Oral Biology,vol.,no.,pp.,.
[] O. F. Molina, J. Dos Santos, M. Mazzetto, S. Nelson, T. Nowlin,
and ´
E. T. Mainieri, “Oral jaw behaviors in TMD and bruxism: a
comparison study by severity of bruxism,Cranio,vol.,no.,
pp. –, .
[] E. Winocur, A. Gavish, T. Finkelshtein, M. Halachmi, and E.
Gazit, “Oral habits among adolescent girls and their association
with symptoms of temporomandibular disorders,” Journal of
Oral Rehabilitation,vol.,no.,pp.,.
... The psychosomatic benefits of chewing gum as a distractor in general pain studies have been demonstrated to some degree (Kamiya et al., 2010;Weijenberg & Lobbezoo, 2015). ...
... Positive 'placebo effects' have been discussed in general pain studies (Weijenberg & Lobbezoo, 2015). However, the literature shows scanty, convoluted information about this 'effect' in orthodontics. ...
... The introduction of conventional sugar-free chewing gum at the onset of orthodontic treatment has been suggested for reducing the pain experience due to the ability of gum to suppress nociceptive responses via serotonergic (5-HT) descending inhibitory pathways (Kamiya et al., 2010). In addition, it is possible that conventional gum may activate other endogenous mechanisms as part of a 'placebo effect' (Weijenberg & Lobbezoo, 2015). No other study to date has compared the effects of conventional sugar-free chewing gum with a placebo medicament. ...
... In the literature, a relation between gum chewing and increased cerebral blood flow is reported, and it is stated that gum chewing increased mental endurance for anxiety (Sasaki-Otomaru et al., 2011). Studies have analysed the influence of gum chewing on easing pain and anxiety given its positive effect on human psychology (Kamiya et al., 2010;Lewkowski et al., 2003;Sasaki-Otomaru et al., 2011;Weijenberg & Lobbezoo, 2015). In these studies, varying results regarding the efficacy of gum chewing in pain control have been reported. ...
... In these studies, varying results regarding the efficacy of gum chewing in pain control have been reported. Some studies have stated that gum chewing has a positive effect on pain and anxiety (Kamiya et al., 2010;Lewkowski et al., 2003;Sasaki-Otomaru et al., 2011;Weijenberg & Lobbezoo, 2015). Lewkowski et al. (2003) reported that the rhythmic oral movements produced by chewing gum increase the release of serotonin. ...
... Although there are many studies about the efficacy of different nonpharmacological methods on paediatric procedural pain and managing children's pain and anxiety (Koller & Goldman, 2012;Sadeghi et al., 2013), the number of studies investigating the effect of gum chewing on pain is very limited in the literature. In a limited number of studies, Kamiya et al. (2010), Sasaki-Otomaru et al. (2011) and Weijenberg and Lobbezoo (2015) suggest that gum chewing may help to manage pain and anxiety in children as a simple and easily applicable method. Our study reported similar results to these studies; according to the reporting of the children and the nurse, children who chewed gum during the procedure experienced less pain than children in the control group. ...
Article
Purpose To evaluate the effect of an interactive robot on Turkish children's post-operative anxiety, mobilization, and parents' satisfaction related to post-operative care. Method A randomized controlled study was conducted with 84 children who will undergo day surgery aged 5–10 years and their parents at a university hospital between June 2020–April 2022. The interactive robot was provided to accompany the children during the postoperative mobilization. Children in the control group received standard care during mobilization. Data were collected using the Personal Information Form, Children's State Anxiety (CSA), Parental Satisfaction Scale-Visual Analog Scale, and Mobilization Chart. Results It was determined that the CSA score of the children in the control group were higher than the intervention group before their first mobilization after surgery (p = 0.005). During the first (p = 0.042) and second (p = 0.012) mobilization, it was determined that the mobilization duration of children in the intervention group was longer than the children in the control group. It was found that the parents of the children in the intervention group had a high level of satisfaction. Conclusion It has been determined that interactive robots positively affect postoperative mobilization in children undergoing day surgery, reduce the anxiety level of children before mobilization, and increase the duration of mobilization. In addition, the use of interactive robots increased parents' satisfaction with post-operative mobilization care. Practice implications Using interactive robots to reduce the stress and anxiety of children during the perioperative process can be a promising approach to improve their recovery by providing early mobilization.
... (Kamiya et al., 2010). The added benefits of gum emanate from its ability to improve oral hygiene through saliva production and mechanical cleaning (Imfeld, 1999) and to decrease anxiety through various psychosomatic pathways (Kamiya et al., 2010;Weijenberg and Lobbezoo, 2015). According to some studies, gum could possibly reduce or replace NSAIDs as a treatment option for patients undergoing fixed orthodontic appliance therapy (Ireland et al., 2017). ...
... 92 In the case of AB, a certain amount of activity could be even considered a physiological strategy to cope with stress. [93][94][95][96] Thus, the diagnostic grading (i.e., possible, probable, definite) that was originally proposed in the first consensus publication 2 has been progressively reconceptualized. 21 Based on that, the STAB is a multidimensional instrument aiming to collect as much information as possible without any preconceived assumption of standard of reference. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: In line with a similar recent proposal for sleep bruxism (SB), defining clinically oriented research routes to implement knowledge on awake bruxism (AB) metrics is important for an enhanced comprehension of the full bruxism spectrum, i.e. better assessment and more efficient management. Methods: We summarised current strategies for AB assessment and proposed a research route for improving its metrics. Results: Most of the literature focuses on bruxism in general or SB in particular, whilst knowledge on AB is generally fragmental. Assessment can be based on non-instrumental or instrumental approaches. The former include self-report (questionnaires, oral history) and clinical examination, whilst the latter include electromyography (EMG) of jaw muscles during wakefulness as well as the technology-enhanced ecological momentary assesment (EMA). Phenotyping of different AB activities should be the target of a research task force. In the absence of available data on the frequency and intensity of wake-time bruxism-type masticatory muscle activity, any speculation about the identification of thresholds and criteria to identify bruxers is premature. Research routes in the field must focus on the improvement of data reliability and validity. Conclusions: Probing deeper into the study of AB metrics is a fundamental step to assist clinicians in preventing and managing the putative consequences at the individual level. The present manuscript proposes some possible research routes to advance current knowledge. At different levels, instrumentally-based and subject-based information must be gathered in a universally accepted standardized approach.
... According to another hypothesis, chewing increases the level of brain activity [4]. It is also possible that active chewing helps to reduce stress and/or pain [14]. It is known that stress, whether caused by pain or not, negatively affects cognitive abilities [15]. ...
... Chewing gum has been identifi ed as an eff ective mode to relieve stress and it has been reported that due to various psychosocial problems one might resort to this parafunctional habit to alleviate their stress and anxiety 23,24 . According to our study the highest mean value for the personality traits of agreeableness (11.00±2.236) ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives: To fi nd the correlation between personality traits and oral para-functional habits. Methods and materials: A Cross-sectional descriptive study was conducted in College of Dentistry, Sharif Medical and Dental College, Lahore over a period of 5 months from July to November 2021 on 200 participants. Data was collected using medical questionnaire and ten item personality inventory scale (TIPI). SPSS 23 was used for statistical analysis. P values less than equal to 0.05 was considered signifi cant Kendall tau b test was used to fi nd the correlation of the personality trait scores and the parafunctional habits. Results: A statistically signifi cant weak negative correlation was seen between emotional stability and teeth grinding (τb=-0.117, p=0.038), teeth clenching (τb=-0.124, p=0.026) and the parafunctional habit of biting on hard objects (τb=-0.156, p=0.005). Conclusion: Emotional stability was negatively correlated to tooth grinding, tooth clenching and biting on hard objects. Individuals with a high score of emotional stability did not have these parafunctional habits while those with a high score of extraversion had all the parafunctional habits except chewing gum.
... 89 Other research in human subjects has also shown that gum chewing has an acute and positive effect on working memory. 90 Mood might be positively influenced by the esthetic of the smile. 91 Esthetic judgments are constantly involved in many activities of our daily life, influencing or affecting numerous decisions. ...
Article
Objectives: To review the dynamic analytical elements used in the functional assessment of the stomatognathic system, summarize the available scientific evidence, and consider interrelations with body posture and cognition. Method and materials: A thorough literature search was conducted using PubMed, the Cochrane Library database, and Google Scholar. Peer-reviewed articles and literature reviews provided up-to-date information addressing three topics: (a) the available knowledge and recent evidence on the relationship between the morphologic aspects of dental/craniofacial anatomy and oral function/dysfunction, (b) mandibular dynamics, considering mobility, functional activity, and existing methodologies of analysis, and (c) a possible correlation between the stomatognathic system, body posture, and cognition. Results: Modern dentistry may be regarded as a human adaptation strategy, helping to conserve healthy teeth for much longer without risking overall health. It is futile to treat patients using a mechanistic, sectorial approach that misrepresents patient behavior and requests, just as it is to affirm the absence of any structure-function relationships. However, it is also evident that there is a lack of general consensus on the precise functional assessment of the stomatognathic system, mostly due to the methodologic heterogeneity employed and the high risk of bias. Despite the abundant evidence produced with the aim of providing solid arguments to define dynamic models of functional assessment of the stomatognathic system, it is yet to become highly empirical, based as it is on operator experience in daily clinical practice. Conclusions: Further efforts from the scientific and clinical community, with the help of progress in technology, remain should this gap be filled and should substantial data on differences between pathologic and physiologic dynamic models of function be provided. Dentistry needs to employ - on a larger scale - objective, dynamic methods of analysis for the functional evaluation of the stomatognathic system, embracing concepts of "personalized medicine" and "interprofessional collaborations."
Article
Full-text available
Amaç: Nedeni açıklanamayan infertil kadınlarda sakız çiğnemenin stres düzeyi üzerine etkisinin incelenmesidir. Yöntem: Üçüncü basamak bir merkezde In-vitro Fertilizasyon tedavisi alan 61 kadın çalışmaya dahil edildi. Araştırmada randomize kontrollü deneysel bir tasarım oluşturuldu. Günde en az 2 kez 5 dakika sakız çiğneyen grup “Deney Grubu-1”, en az 2 kez 20 dakika sakız çiğneyen grup “Deney Grubu-2”, sakız çiğnemeyen grup ise “Kontrol Grubu” olarak adlandırıldı. İnfertil kadınların üreme bilgileri ve stres düzeyleri verileri Tanıtıcı Bilgi Formu ve Fertilite Sorun Envanteri ile toplanmıştır. Bulgular: Gruplara göre sosyodemografik ve infertiliteye özgü özellikler arasında istatistiksel olarak anlamlı bir fark bulunmamıştır. Gruplar arası Fertilite Sorunu Envanteri ölçek puanlarının karşılaştırılmasında istatistiksel olarak anlamlı bir fark bulunamamıştır. Ancak Vaka 2 grubu zamanlara göre Fertilite Sorunu Envanteri alt ölçeklerinden çocuksuz yaşamın reddi ölçek puanı ortalama değerleri arasında istatistiksel olarak anlamlı bir fark bulunmuştur (p
Article
Full-text available
In recent years, there was a debate about the effects of gum chewing on various aspects of cognitive functioning. In this review, the results of previous studies are presented and summarized. There is a clear indication that gum chewing can improve various aspects of cognitive functioning including memory, attention and both executive and intellectual functioning. However, there is also clear evidence that chewing gum during cognitive tasks can adversely affect task performance. Therefore, it can be concluded that the replication of (beneficial or detrimental) effects of gum chewing on cognition has been proven to be very difficult. Consequently, the robustness of reported effects of gum chewing on cognition has to be questioned. Suggestions for future research are given.
Article
Full-text available
In clinical practice, patients suffering from an occlusal parafunctional activity have increased. It can be observed that a negative influence of environment aggravates patient's health. The aim of this paper is to present the impact of environment and development of human civilization on the prevalence of bruxism and the correlation between them. The authors grasp the most relevant aspects of psychological and anthropological factors changing over time as well as their interactions and describe a relationship between chronic stress and bruxism. Current literature shows how contemporary lifestyle, working environment, diet, and habits influence the patient's psychoemotional situation and the way these factors affect the occluso-muscle condition.
Article
Full-text available
The hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis (also called the HPA or stress axis) exhibits distinct circadian and ultradian rhythms in cortisol release that cannot be explained solely by the feedback loops from cortisol to the control systems in the paraventricular nucleus (PVN) and pituitary gland. The HPA axis is intimately connected with other brain functions. In particular, it is strongly aff ected by the sleep-wake cycles via direct and indirect eff ects of the circadian and homeostatic mechanisms. For example, the HPA axis has direct inputs from the master circadian clock in the suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN), and from the various sleep-wake related neuronal populations, which themselves are under the eff ects of the circadian and homeostatic processes. In this paper a first step towards a physiologically based mathematical model of the HPA-axis under eff ects of the sleep-wake cycles is presented. This model accounts for 3 major characteristics of daily cortisol profi le in the blood: i) abrupt increase of cortisol concentration in response to awakening, the so-called cortisol-awakening response (CAR); ii) reduced cortisol levels during daytime with underlying ultradian oscillations; and iii) suppression of cortisol release during sleep.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: Although children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) were reported to have insufficient breastfeeding, consequences and oropharyngeal implications of this finding have not been studied. In this case-control study, we aimed to investigate early feeding practices and parafunctional oral habits in children with ADHD. Subjects and methods: The study group consisted of 200 children and adolescents, 7-17 years old, diagnosed as having ADHD at Marmara University Child Psychiatry Clinics in Istanbul, Turkey. The Conners Parent and Teacher Rating Scales were used to assess behavioral disturbances. A questionnaire was developed consisting of items pertaining to breastfeeding period, early feeding history, and parafunctional oral habits. The study data were compared with those for 175 healthy schoolchildren after exclusion of possible ADHD cases. Results: The children with ADHD were found to have insufficient exclusive breastfeeding (less than 6 months) (p=0.0001). The children with insufficient exclusive breastfeeding were more likely to have a history of bottle feeding, longer duration of bottle feeding, and early introduction of bottle feeding (p=0.01). Overall, significant differences were detected on the domains of duration of bottle feeding, introduction of bottle feeding, introduction of pacifier use, variables of nail and toenail biting, as well as pencil biting, bruxism, and snoring between the ADHD group and the control group. Conclusions: The present results indicate that early in life, children with ADHD are subject to insufficient exclusive breastfeeding, different feeding practices, and elevated parafunctional oral habits more often than typically developing children. For all professionals who provide healthcare to children, increased awareness and attention to these factors are suggested.
Article
Diagnostic and therapeutic skin-breaking procedures have become ubiquitous in current medical practice and neonatology does not constitute an exception. One of the main sources of neonatal pain is procedure-induced pain. It has recently become clear that pain prevention must be a health care priority. Non-pharmacological approaches constitute a first option for the analgesia of common procedures performed in neonatology. This article reviews the non-pharmacological treatments most frequently used in this context: swaddling, tucking, containment, sweet solutions, non-nutritive sucking (NNS), breastfeeding analgesia, breast milk and music. In practice, the dose of 1 to 2 mL of 24% or 30% sucrose solution or 30% glucose solution immediately followed by NNS can be given for minor painful procedures in term neonates or those weighing more than 2500 g. In the preterm, 0.3 mL of a sweet solution (sucrose or glucose) can be given for infants weighing less than 1500 g and 0.5 mL for those weighing between 1500 and 2500 g. The synergistic effect of sweet solutions and NNS has been clearly shown and thus their association is largely justified in practice. For breast-fed term neonates, breastfeeding can be given to sooth procedure-induced pain. All these non-pharmacological options can be effective to relieve pain from minor or moderate procedures. However, when more painful procedures are performed, stronger analgesics must be used.
Article
Sleep bruxism (SB) is a parafunctional oromotor activity that can sometimes pose a threat to the integrity of the structures of the masticatory system if the magnitude and direction of the forces exerted exceed the system's adaptive capacity. Over the years science has tried to provide a consistent explanation of the etiopathogenesis and physiopathology of SB, although the pathophysiological mechanisms are, even now, not fully understood yet. There is at present no specific, effective treatment to permanently eliminate the habit of SB. There are only palliative therapeutic alternatives steered at preventing the pathological effects of SB on the stomatognathic system and alleviating the negative clinical consequences of the habit. The aim of this paper is to review and update the fundamental scientific concepts of SB based on the scientific literature and to furnish an approach to the main types of therapy available, in an attempt to assist the general and restorative dentist to manage those clinical situations in which SB is a significant risk factor for the oral health and/or dental treatment of the patient.
Article
Aims: To investigate cerebral cortical changes by using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) after denture renewal and to test how these relate to prosthodontic treatment adaptability as measured by chewing efficiency and maximum bite force. Methods: Ten complete denture wearers (five women and five men, mean age ± standard deviation: 70.3 ± 9.1 years) participated in the study. Each had their complete dentures renewed and underwent an fMRI examination with three functional tasks (lip pursing; jaw tapping; jaw clenching) as well as a color-mixing test for chewing efficiency and unilateral maximum bite force measurements. Recordings were performed with the old dentures (T0) and with the new dentures on insertion (T1) and at 1 week (T2) and 3 months postinsertion (T3). At T1, denture stability and retention (S/T) were assessed by two independent operators. Wilcoxon signed rank tests and Spearman's rho correlation were carried out for data analysis. Results: The right and the left precentral gyrus (PRCG) and postcentral gyrus (POCG) were identified with significant activation across all three functional tasks. A statistically significant increase in the level of activity between T0 and T2 (POCG: P = .022; PRCG: P = .017) was found during jaw clenching tasks. Both regions of interest (PRCG, POCG) appeared to correlate with S/T of the new dentures while the subject performed a lip-pursing task (PRCG: r = 0.689, P = .027; POCG: r = 0.665, P = .036). The chewing efficiency and maximum bite force increased significantly during the adaptation to replacement dentures (chewing efficiency: T1-T2 P = .032, T2-T3 P = .012; maximum bite force right side: T2-T3 P = .047). Conclusion: Changes in brain activity occurred in the adaptation to replacement dentures and appeared to regain preinsertion activity levels during motor tasks involving the dental occlusion after 3 months postinsertion.
Article
Accumulating evidence indicates that brain kappa-opioid receptors (KORs) and dynorphin, the endogenous ligand that binds at these receptors, are involved in regulating states of motivation and emotion. These findings have stimulated interest in the development of KOR-targeted ligands as therapeutic agents. As one example, it has been suggested that KOR antagonists might have a wide range of indications, including the treatment of depressive, anxiety, and addictive disorders, as well as conditions characterized by co-morbidity of these disorders (e.g., post-traumatic stress disorder) A general effect of reducing the impact of stress may explain how KOR antagonists can have efficacy in such a variety of animal models that would appear to represent different disease states. Here, we review evidence that disruption of KOR function attenuates prominent effects of stress. We will describe behavioral and molecular endpoints including those from studies that characterize the effects of KOR antagonists and KOR ablation on the effects of stress itself, as well as on the effects of exogenously delivered corticotropin-releasing factor, a brain peptide that mediates key effects of stress. Collectively, available data suggest that KOR disruption produces anti-stress effects and under some conditions can prevent the development of stress-induced adaptations. As such, KOR antagonists may have unique potential as therapeutic agents for the treatment and even prevention of stress-related psychiatric illness, a therapeutic niche that is currently unfilled.