ArticlePDF Available

Examination of Neural Systems Sub-Serving Facebook “Addiction”

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

-Because addictive behaviors typically result from violated homeostasis of the impulsive (amygdala-striatal) and inhibitory (prefrontal cortex) brain systems, this study examined whether these systems sub-serve a specific case of technology-related addiction, namely Facebook "addiction." Using a go/no-go paradigm in functional MRI settings, the study examined how these brain systems in 20 Facebook users (M age = 20.3 yr., SD = 1.3, range = 18-23) who completed a Facebook addiction questionnaire, responded to Facebook and less potent (traffic sign) stimuli. The findings indicated that at least at the examined levels of addiction-like symptoms, technology-related "addictions" share some neural features with substance and gambling addictions, but more importantly they also differ from such addictions in their brain etiology and possibly pathogenesis, as related to abnormal functioning of the inhibitory-control brain system.
Content may be subject to copyright.
ISSN 0033-2941DOI 10.2466/18.PR0.115c31z8
© Psychological Reports 2014
Psychological Reports: Disability & Trauma
EXAMINATION OF NEURAL SYSTEMS SUB-SERVING
FACEBOOK “ADDICTION”1, 2
OFIR TUREL
California State University, Fullerton
Brain and Creativity Institute, University of Southern California
QINGHUA HE
Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University
Beibei, Chongqing, China
GUI XUE
National Key Laboratory of Cognitive
Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal
University, China
Brain and Creativity Institute, University of Southern California
LIN XIAO
Brain and Creativity Institute, University of
Southern California
ANTOINE BECHARA
Department of Psychology and Brain and
Creativity Institute, University of Southern
California
Summary.—Because addictive behaviors typically result from violated homeo-
stasis of the impulsive (amygdala-striatal) and inhibitory (prefrontal cortex) brain
systems, this study examined whether these systems sub-serve a specic case of
technology-related addiction, namely Facebook “addiction.” Using a go/no-go par-
adigm in functional MRI settings, the study examined how these brain systems in
20 Facebook users (M age = 20.3 yr., SD = 1.3, range = 18–23) who completed a Face-
book addiction questionnaire, responded to Facebook and less potent (trac sign)
stimuli. The ndings indicated that at least at the examined levels of addiction-like
symptoms, technology-related “addictions” share some neural features with sub-
stance and gambling addictions, but more importantly they also dier from such
addictions in their brain etiology and possibly pathogenesis, as related to abnormal
functioning of the inhibitory-control brain system.
While the Internet is largely benecial to society, it can also bring
about negative consequences (D'Arcy, Gupta, Tarafdar, & Turel, 2014),
including behavioral and psychological signs that have been labeled by
some researchers as “addiction”3 to the use of specic applications on the
Internet (Griths, 1998; Young, 1998a; Griths, 1999; Young, 2004; Turel,
2014, 115, 3, 675-695.
1Address correspondence to Or Turel, 800 N. State College Blvd., Fullerton, CA 92834 or
e-mail (ot_739@usc.edu or oturel@fullerton.edu).
2This research was supported by research grants from the National Institute on Drug Abuse
(NIDA) R01DA023051, National Cancer Institute (NCI) R01CA152062, and the National
Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute and the National Institute of Child Health and Human
Development (U01HL097839). The authors would also like to thank Alexandra Hollihan and
Stephanie Castillo, who helped with the data collection.
3The authors use quotation marks to indicate that the appropriateness of the term “addiction”
to the described cases is still being examined.
04_PR_Turel_140140.indd 675 16/12/14 10:58 AM
O. TUREL, ET AL.
676
Serenko, & Giles, 2011). Such “addictions” can result in academic failure,
sleep deprivation, social isolation, health issues, and many other impair-
ments for adolescents and young adults; they also result in reduced work
performance and marital discord and separation for adults (cf. Griths,
1995; Young, 1998b; Pratarelli, Browne, & Johnson, 1999; Chou, Condron,
& Belland, 2005; Block, 2008; Byun, Runi, Mills, Douglas, Niang, Step-
chenkova, et al., 2009; Young, 2010; Kuss, Griths, & Binder, 2013). It is
therefore worthwhile to examine the possible neural basis of such “addic-
tions.”
Research across multiple countries, including the United States, es-
timates the prevalence of such “addictions” to be between 0.7% and 11%
(Greeneld, 1999; Johansson & Götestam, 2004; Kim, Ryu, Chon, Yeun,
Choi, Seo, et al., 2006; Cao & Su, 2007; Rendi, Szabo, & Szabó, 2007; Ghas-
semzadeh, Shahraray, & Moradi, 2008; Park, Kim, & Cho, 2008; Shaw &
Black, 2008; Siomos, Dafouli, Braimiotis, Mouzas, & Angelopoulos, 2008;
Bakken, Wenzel, Götestam, Johansson, & Oeren, 2009),4 and that it is more
prevalent among youth and young adults (Kuss, et al., 2013), presumably
because the inhibitory system of such individuals develops more slowly
than their impulsive system (Casey, Giedd, & Thomas, 2000; Casey, Tot-
tenham, Liston, & Durston, 2005; Steinberg, 2005; Casey, Getz, & Galvan,
2008; Steinberg, 2008; Steinberg, Graham, O'Brien, Woolard, Cauman, &
Banich, 2009). Given the symptoms and prevalence of this phenomenon,
calls have been issued to study its possible neurological roots (Block, 2008)
and to focus on “addiction” to specic, intrinsically rewarding applica-
tions on the Internet (e.g., Facebook, videogames) (Yellowlees & Marks,
2007). Consequently, the concept of “Internet Gaming Disorder” was in-
cluded in the Appendix (section 3, potential disorders requiring further
research) of the DSM–V, and it is possible that more application-specic
“addictions” will be considered for inclusion in future versions of the
DSM. Moreover, several scales for measuring such “addictions” have been
developed (van Rooij, Schoenmakers, Vermulst, van den Eijnden, & van
de Mheen, 2011; Andreassen, Torsheim, Brunborg, & Pallesen, 2012). Nev-
ertheless, the DSM is not conclusive on the existence of this possible dis-
order, and many researchers also still question whether the observed phe-
nomenon reects a pathological “addictive” state or merely a “bad habit,”
especially when applied to the vast general population of users who show
addiction-like symptoms in relation to Internet application use (Griths,
1998, 1999; LaRose, 2010; Bergmark, Bergmark, & Findahl, 2011).
4This is a wide range and it can be assumed that it is a consequence of multiple factors,
including the type of Internet application examined (e.g., Facebook, videogames), demo-
graphics and socio-economic dierences between the samples, and national dierences in
accessibility to technologies and the availability of alternative activities.
04_PR_Turel_140140.indd 676 16/12/14 10:58 AM
FACEBOOK AND NEURAL SYSTEMS 677
This study attempts to address one aspect of this issue by focusing
on a possibly “addictive” Internet technology, namely Facebook. Several
studies have demonstrated that Facebook “addiction” is a plausible phe-
nomenon and that addiction-like symptoms in relation to Facebook use
may be prevalent in the general population (Echeburua & de Corral, 2010;
Karaiskos, Tzavellas, Balta, & Paparrigopoulos, 2010; Kuss & Griths,
2011; Griths, 2012). These behaviors are usually labeled as “addictive”
based on DSM criteria for dependence on substances, including tolerance,
withdrawal, and loss of control to the point that the behavior causes a sig-
nicant impairment to the individual (World Health Organization, 1992;
American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Perhaps many Facebook users
may be labeled as “addicts” simply because they easily meet several of
these criteria, especially when the denition of “signicant impairment” is
subjective and variable. The following research question is therefore posed:
Research question 1. Does Facebook “addiction” constitute a path-
ological problem similar to those observed in the case of other
substance and behavioral addictions, in the general user pop-
ulation?
One objective way to identify similarities or fundamental dierences
between Facebook (Internet) and other addictions is to look at the neu-
ral systems sub-serving these possible disorders. Thus, one goal of this
study was to examine neural activities in two key brain systems impli-
cated in substance addiction, the impulsive, amygdala-striatal system
and the reective-inhibitory prefrontal brain system (e.g., Jentsch & Tay-
lor, 1999; Volkow & Fowler, 2000; Arnsten & Li, 2005; Bickel, Miller, Yi,
Kowal, Lindquist, & Pitcock, 2007) when Facebook users are exposed to
Facebook cues. The amygdala-striatal (mesolimbic dopamine-dependent)
neural system is critical for the incentive motivational eects of a variety
of rewards (Stewart, Dewit, & Eikelboom, 1984; Robbins, Cador, Taylor, &
Everitt, 1989; Wise & Rompre, 1989; Robinson & Berridge, 1993; Di Chiara,
Tanda, Bassareo, Pontieri, Acquas, Fenu, et al., 1999; Everitt, Parkinson,
Olmstead, Arroyo, Robledo, & Robbins, 1999; Balleine & Dickinson, 2000;
Koob & Le Moal, 2001). It becomes hyperactive and begins to intensify the
incentive value of rewards in individuals with substance abuse problems
(Bechara, 2005). As cue-behavior-reward associations are strengthened,
they begin to drive behavior without the necessary involvement of con-
scious processes (Everitt, et al., 1999; Robinson & Berridge, 2003; Everitt &
Robbins, 2005). Because Facebook use can provide strong rewards (Turel
& Serenko, 2012; Meshi, Morawetz, & Heekeren, 2013),5 it is expected that
5The terms “rewards” and “incentives” are used interchangeably. However, please note that
rewards are one form of incentives, and other incentives can include avoiding negative con-
sequences.
04_PR_Turel_140140.indd 677 16/12/14 10:58 AM
O. TUREL, ET AL.
678
similar learning mechanisms take place with Facebook use, which can
lead to “addiction”-like symptoms.
Hypothesis 1. If Facebook “addiction” is sub-served by similar
pathological issues underlying other addictions, the impul-
sive amygdala-striatal system activity in response to Face-
book stimuli will be positively correlated with “addiction”-
like symptoms.
While the amygdala-striatal system provides the drive for impul-
sive behaviors, diagnosis of addictions typically also requires poor con-
trol abilities that fail to inhibit impulsive behaviors and the consideration
of long-term goals (Noel, Brevers, & Bechara, 2013). This inhibitory sys-
tem depends primarily on the functions of the prefrontal cortex (Fellows,
2004; Wheeler & Fellows, 2008). A critical neural region in this system is
the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (which is considered inclusive of the
medial orbitofrontal cortex; Bechara, Tranel, & Damasio, 2000). Other im-
portant regions in the inhibitory system include the lateral orbitofrontal
and inferior frontal gyrus regions, as well as the anterior cingulate cortex,
which are involved in a variety of simple inhibitory processes (Glascher,
Adolphs, Damasio, Bechara, Rudrauf, Calamia, et al., 2012). Good inhibi-
tory functioning reects the ability to actively stop a pre-potent behav-
ioral response after it has been triggered (Logan, Schachar, & Tannock,
1997; Braver & Ruge, 2006). Inhibitory processes are activated primarily
by antecedent cues (e.g., Wood & Neal, 2007), and inhibition is therefore
especially relevant in the face of these cues. Individuals with hypoactivity
of these systems have a tendency to act more impulsively (Bechara, 2005).
Hypothesis 2. If Facebook “addiction” is sub-served by similar
pathological issues underlying other addictions, the reective-
inhibitory prefrontal system activity in response to Facebook
stimuli will be negatively correlated with “addiction”-like
symptoms.
Method
Participants
The sample was recruited in two phases. The rst involved an on-
line questionnaire which captured demographic, exclusion criteria (self-
reported neurological or psychiatric history as well as uncorrected vision),
and “addiction” variables. It was administered to 45 Facebook users who
were recruited using an announcement on a bulletin board at a North
American university, and who were given a small gift card in exchange
for their time. None of the users who completed this questionnaire met
any of the exclusion criteria. In the second phase, twenty participants who
04_PR_Turel_140140.indd 678 16/12/14 10:58 AM
FACEBOOK AND NEURAL SYSTEMS 679
completed the screening survey were invited and agreed to participate in
the fMRI scan. The selection was made such that the sexes are balanced,
and that there is sucient variability in addiction scores. The sample was
equally distributed between men and women, and the average age of the
participants was 20.3 yr. (SD = 1.30, range = 18–23). The participants had
normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and were free of neurological or
psychiatric history (self-reported). All participants gave informed consent
to the experimental procedure, which was approved by the University of
Southern California Institutional Review Board.
Measures
Participants were asked to complete an online version of the Facebook
“addiction” scale (adapted from van Rooij, et al., 2011). This scale asked
them to report the frequency (1: Never, 5: Very Often) of typical Facebook
“addiction” symptoms such as withdrawal, salience, relapse, loss of con-
trol, and conict. It therefore presumably captures the “level of ‘addic-
tion’” and was valid and reliable (α = .92, Spearman-Brown Coecient for
split-half reliability = 0.91, Guttman split-half coecient = 0.91, Average
Variance Extracted (AVE) = 0.53, and composite reliability = 0.91). Given
its validity and reliability, the mean of all items was calculated, which
represents the average severity of the addiction symptoms per individ-
ual. The mean score was 2.20 (SD = 0.72, range = 1.07–3.64), and the scores
seemed to be reasonably normally distributed (skewness = 0.32, SE = 0.51;
kurtosis = –0.59, SE = 0.99); the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test (statistic = 0.097,
df = 20, p = .20) and the Shapiro-Wilks test (statistic = 0.97, df = 20, p = .81)
were non-signicant. Hence, no transformations to normality were ap-
plied. The behavioral questionnaire also captured age, sex, mental history,
and exclusion criteria (e.g., non-corrected bad vision and any peripheral
neuropathies). No mental issues, including drug and alcohol abuse were
reported by the sample; no participant met any of the exclusion criteria.
fMRI Procedures and Tasks
fMRI scans were performed one week after the completion of the be-
havioral questionnaire. In these scans, participants rested in the supine
position on the fMRI scanner bed to view the task back-projected onto a
screen through a mirror attached to the head coil. Foam pads were used
to minimize head motion. Stimulus presentation and timing of all stimuli
and response events were achieved using Matlab (Mathworks) and Psych-
toolbox (www.psychtoolbox.org) on an IBM-compatible PC. The partici-
pants' responses were collected online using an MRI-compatible button
box.
The participants performed two Facebook-specic go/no-go tasks while
in the scanner: (1) a sign go/Facebook no-go task (SGo task) in which they
04_PR_Turel_140140.indd 679 16/12/14 10:58 AM
O. TUREL, ET AL.
680
were asked to press a button when they saw a trac sign image, and refrain
from pressing the button when they saw a Facebook-related image; and (2) a
Facebook go/sign no-go task (FGo task) in which they were asked to press a
button when they saw a Facebook-related image, and refrain from pressing
the button when they saw a trac sign image. This go/no-go paradigm al-
lows examination of both the brain responses to Facebook stimuli and the in-
hibition of pre-potent responses to Facebook stimuli. Examples of stimuli are
shown in Fig. 1. Trac signs included common (excluding red) signs.
Each task consisted of 120 go trials (75%) and 40 no-go trials (25%).
No-go trials were presented in pseudo-randomized order, designed so
that no-go trials appeared with equal probability after 1–5 consecutive go
trials, and no two no-go trials appeared consecutively. Each stimulus was
presented for 500 msec., followed by a xation cross for 1.5–4 sec. with a
mean of 2.5 sec. The sequence was optimized for design eciency using
an in-house program. Each task ran for 8 min. The order of the two ver-
sions of go/no-go tasks was counterbalanced across participants.
Following signal detection theory, the hit rate, false alarm rate, sensi-
tivity index
dZ Z'
'
=−
Hitsrate fals ealaramrate,
and decision bias
CZZ=− ×+
()
05.Hits rate falsealaramrate
Fig. 1. The illustration of the event-related Facebook-specic go/no-go tasks: (1) sign
go/Facebook no-go task (SGo task), and (2) Facebook go/sign no-go task (FGo task). Partici-
pants were asked to press a button as soon as possible in the go trials (trac sign pictures in
SGo task and Facebook-related pictures in FGo task) and withhold the response in the no-go
trials (Facebook-related pictures in SGo task and trac sign pictures in FGo task). The order
of tasks was counterbalanced across subjects and across sessions.
04_PR_Turel_140140.indd 680 16/12/14 10:58 AM
FACEBOOK AND NEURAL SYSTEMS 681
were calculated for each task (Macmillan & Creelman, 1996). The mean
reaction time for go trials and no-go trials (false alarm trials only) for
each task were also calculated. The reaction time for go trials served as an
index for habitual-impulsive responding to the stimuli, with longer reac-
tion times indicating less habitual response, while decision bias C served
as an index of response inhibition, with higher values indicating better in-
hibitory control.
fMRI Protocol
Functional MRI (fMRI) imaging was conducted in a 3T Siemens
MAGNETOM Tim/Trio scanner. Functional scanning used a z-shim gra-
dient echo EPI sequence with PACE. This sequence was aimed at reduc-
ing signal loss in the prefrontal and orbitofrontal areas. The parameters
were: TR/TE = 2000/25 msec.; ip angle = 90°; 64 × 64 matrix size with res-
olution 3 × 3 mm2. Thirty-one 3.5-mm axial slices were used to cover the
whole cerebral cortex and most of the cerebellum with no gap. The slices
were tilted about 30° clockwise along the AC-PC plane to obtain better
signals in the orbitofrontal cortex. An anatomical T1-weighted structural
scan was also done (TR/TE = 1950/2.26 msec.; ip angle = 7°; 176 sagittal
slices; spatial resolution = 1 × 1 × 1.95 mm) for registration purposes.
fMRI Analysis
Image preprocessing and statistical analyses were carried out using
FSL (www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). Images were realigned to compensate for
small residual head movements (Jenkinson & Smith, 2001). Translational
movement parameters never exceeded one voxel in any direction for any
participant. Data were spatially smoothed using a 5-mm full-width-half-
maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel and were ltered using a nonlinear
high pass lter with a 100-second cuto.
A two-step registration procedure was used whereby EPI images
were rst registered to the MPRAGE structural image, and then into stan-
dard MNI space, using ane transformations (Jenkinson & Smith, 2001).
Registration from MPRAGE structural image to standard space was fur-
ther rened using FNIRT nonlinear registration (Andersson, Jenkinson,
& Smith, 2007a, 2007b). Statistical analyses were performed in the native
image space, with the statistical maps normalized to the standard space
prior to higher-level analyses. The data were modeled at the rst-level
using a general linear model within FSL's FILM module. Brain activation
in every trial was modeled for go and no-go trials, respectively, at the sin-
gle-participant level. Error-related trials (misses and false alarms) were
modeled together as a nuisance variable. The event onsets were convolved
with canonical hemodynamic response function (HRF, double-gamma) to
generate regressors. Temporal derivatives were included as covariates of
04_PR_Turel_140140.indd 681 16/12/14 10:58 AM
O. TUREL, ET AL.
682
no interest to improve statistical sensitivity. Null events were not explic-
itly modeled, and therefore constituted an implicit baseline. The six move-
ment parameters were also included as covariates in the model.
A higher-level analysis created cross-run contrasts for each partici-
pant, using a xed-eect model. A 2 task (go vs no-go) × 2 stimuli (trac
sign vs Facebook pictures) within-subjects factor design was used. The
main eects and interaction were modeled as well as 4 single-condition
eects (Facebook go; Facebook no-go; trac sign go; trac sign no-go).
Higher-level random-eects models were tested for group analyses using
FMRIB's Local Analysis of Mixed Eect stage 1 only (Beckmann, Jenkin-
son, & Smith, 2003; Woolrich, Behrens, Beckmann, Jenkinson, & Smith,
2004) with automatic outlier detection (Woolrich, 2008). The brain activa-
tion associated with each contrast was rst tested in all participants using
one-sample t tests. Then, the brain activation was correlated with the ad-
diction score. Group images were thresholded with a height threshold of
Z > 2.3 and a cluster probability of p < .05, corrected for whole-brain multi-
ple comparisons based on a Gaussian random eld theory. The sex of par-
ticipants was included as a covariate for all fMRI analyses.
Regions of interest (ROI) were used to show the direction of the ac-
tivation and in correlation analyses and scatterplots. ROIs were created
from clusters of voxels with signicant activation in the voxel-wise anal-
ysis. Analyses were performed by extracting parameter estimates (betas)
for each event type from the tted model, and averaging them across
all voxels in the cluster for each participant and session. Percent signal
changes were calculated using a method suggested by Mumford.6
Results
Behavioral Results
There was no signicant correlation between “addiction” score and
age (r = −.20, p = .40), but the “addiction” score was signicantly correlated
with sex (r = .45, p = .05). This sex dierence was also supported by a t test
(t18 = −2.12, p < .05, Cohen's d = 0.96), and variance was reasonably equal (Lev-
ene's test F = 1.03, p < .32). This implies that at least in the sample, women
(coded as 1) presented stronger addiction-like symptoms with regards to
Facebook use (female M = 2.52) than did men (male M = 1.89). There was no
signicant correlation between “addiction” score and other behavioral mea-
sures of the go/no-go tasks (all with p > .05).
Table 1 summarizes the major behavioral measures for both fMRI
tasks, including hit rates, false alarm rates, sensitivity index d', decision
bias C, and reaction times for go trials and no-go trials (false alarm rates
6http://mumford.fmripower.org/perchange_guide.pdf
04_PR_Turel_140140.indd 682 16/12/14 10:58 AM
FACEBOOK AND NEURAL SYSTEMS 683
for inhibitory failures only). For every behavioral measure, a paired t test
was performed to test the dierence between tasks (SGo vs FGo task). In
all cases Levene's test statistics were non-signicant (all p > .10), indicating
that equal variance can be assumed. Analysis revealed that the average
reaction time for go trials was signicantly longer in the SGo task than in
the FGo task (p < .05 corrected). Analysis also revealed dierences in the
false alarm rate and reaction time for no-go trials between the two tasks,
but they were no longer signicant after multiple comparison correction
(Table 1).
fMRI Results
The fMRI analyses were used in a conrmatory manner. First, it was ex-
amined whether the amygdala-striatal system was engaged in the Facebook-
go trials. As showed in Table 2 and Fig. 2, this pattern was supported because
the Facebook-go trials activated a large sector of the amygdala-striatum sys-
tem along with other brain regions, including the occipital cortex, parietal
cortex, and precentral gyrus/insula. Next, in order to test for possible hy-
peractivity of the amygdala-striatal system as a function of one's “addiction”
score (Hypothesis 1), the “addiction” scores were correlated with the brain ac-
tivity in Facebook-go trials. The results revealed that bilateral ventral striatum
activity in Facebook-go trials was correlated positively with the “addiction”
score (Fig. 3). Hypothesis 1 was further supported by the results in Table 1,
which demonstrate that on average there are signicantly more false alarms
in the case of sign-go (i.e., Facebook-no-go) tasks, and that response times
were shorter in Facebook-go trials.
To test Hypothesis 2, it was rst examined whether the inhibitory con-
trol system (especially the ACC and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) was
engaged in Facebook-no-go trials. Table 2 and Fig. 2 lend support to such
an eect, with activation of the ACC, right DLPFC/insula, and bilateral
TABLE 1
BehavioRal MeasuRes FRoM the FaceBook-speciFic go/No-go task
Variable SGo Task FGo Task t p Cohen's
d
M SD M SD
Hits rate 0.90 0.11 0.90 0.12 −0.02 .98 −.009
False alarm rate 0.13 0.13 0.06 0.05 2.42 .03 1.08
Go trial response time (msec.) 522.3 86.7 487.3 78.2 44.2 .002 1.66
No-go trial response time (msec.) 433.2 50.7 389.0 44.2 2.53 .03 1.13
d' 2.85 0.54 3.04 0.61 −1.10 .29 −0.49
C−0.31 0.46 −0.07 0.27 −1.70 .12 −0.76
Note.—SGo = a sign go/Facebook no-go task; FGo = a Facebook go/sign no-go task. *p < .05
corrected for multiple comparison with a Bonferroni correction.
04_PR_Turel_140140.indd 683 16/12/14 10:58 AM
O. TUREL, ET AL.
684
occipital/parietal cortex. Furthermore, the prefrontal activation in Face-
book-no-go trials was compared with this of trac sign-no-go trials. No
signicant dierences were found. This suggests that in both conditions
participants engaged in similar levels of inhibition.
Fig. 2. Activation of Facebook-go (in red) and Facebook-no-go (in green) trials. Yellow
areas indicate common activation for go and no-go trials.
TABLE 2
suMMaRy oF FMRi Results
Brain Region Voxels MNI Coordinates Z
x y z
Brain activity of Facebook-go trials
B Occipital cortex/Amygdala/Striatum 31,860 30 −62 −18 6.41
L Insula 1,596 −50 4 2 3.60
L Postcentral cortex 968 −62 −16 46 4.28
Brain activity of Facebook-no-go trials
B Occipital/ Parietal/ Temporal cortices 31,386 −40 −80 −14 5.94
R DLPFC 3,239 50 10 42 4.03
Cingulate cortex 781 4 12 48 3.16
Whole brain correlation between brain activity in Facebook-go trials and addiction score
(Positive)
Bilateral ventral striatum 1,021 −20 14 −8 3.67
Whole brain correlation between brain activity in Facebook-no-go trials and addiction
score
No signicant correlations were observed
04_PR_Turel_140140.indd 684 16/12/14 10:58 AM
FACEBOOK AND NEURAL SYSTEMS 685
Next, the authors tested whether the activity of the inhibitory control
neural systems, as a manifestation of inhibition attempts in response to
Facebook cues, was associated negatively with “addiction” scores. That is,
the “addiction” scores of the participants were correlated with the brain
activity in Facebook-no-go trials. The results indicated no signicant as-
sociation between any component of the inhibition system (ventromedial
prefrontal cortex, lateral orbitofrontal, and inferior frontal gyrus regions,
and anterior cingulate cortex) and “addiction” scores (all with p > .05).
discussioN
While the activation of the amygdala-striatal (impulsive) brain system
was positively associated with one's Facebook “addiction” score (i.e., the level
of addiction-like symptoms presented), there was no association between this
score and activation of the prefrontal cortex (inhibition) brain system. The
ndings, therefore, suggested that at least individuals with low to medium
levels of addiction-like symptoms have a hyperactive amygdala-striatal sys-
tem, which makes this “addiction” similar to many other addictions, but they
do not have a hypoactive prefrontal lobe inhibition system, which makes it
dierent from many other addictions, such as to illicit substances. Hence,
technology “addictions” may not present the exact same brain etiology and
possibly pathogenesis that drives substance and gambling addictions. The
detected hyperactivity of the impulsive brain system supplements and con-
rms ndings of other studies which discovered similarities between brain
systems sub-serving technology-related addictions and other addictions (Ko,
Liu, Hsiao, Yen, Yang, Lin, et al., 2009; Han, Hwang, & Renshaw, 2010; Han,
Kim, Lee, Min, & Renshaw, 2010; Han, Bolo, Daniels, Arenella, Lyoo, & Ren-
shaw, 2011; Han, Kim, Lee, & Renshaw, 2012; Han, Lyoo, & Renshaw, 2012;
Fig. 3. The ventral striatum signal showed positive correlation with the addiction score
in Facebook go trials. (A) Coronal image shows the ventral striatum signal. (B) Scatter plot
shows the correlation pattern.
04_PR_Turel_140140.indd 685 16/12/14 10:58 AM
O. TUREL, ET AL.
686
Ko, Liu, Yen, Yen, Chen, & Lin, 2013; Ko, Liu, Yen, Chen, Yen, & Chen, 2013).
However, the ndings regarding the frontal lobe inhibition system also point
to possible dissimilarities between substance and gambling addictions, and
Facebook “addiction,” which exist at least at the examined levels of addic-
tion symptoms.
Several implications of these ndings should be noted. First, studies
on technology-related “addictions” indicate that many individuals present
at least some (and in some cases many) addiction-like symptoms with low-
medium frequency and intensity (and in some cases high) in relation to the
use of presumably addictive technologies (La Barbera, La Paglia, & Valsavoia,
2009; Turel & Serenko, 2012). This has raised public and scientic awareness
of this potential problem (Block, 2008; Byun, et al., 2009), and has resulted in
the inclusion of Internet Gaming Disorder as a “condition for further study”
in DSM–V (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).
Without discounting the existence and importance of this problem
and its possible adverse consequences, one may question (1) whether the
term “addiction” is the most appropriate one for this problem (LaRose,
Lin, & Eastin, 2003; Yellowlees & Marks, 2007; Turel, et al., 2011; Kuss &
Griths, 2011); and (2) whether, at least when applied to the general pop-
ulation of users, commonly used addiction scales, which include relatively
easy-to-meet criteria and benign symptoms (LaRose, et al., 2003; LaRose,
2010), actually capture “addiction” or merely capture symptoms emerg-
ing from a strong bad habit of implicit and automatic high engagement
with a technology (Charlton & Danforth, 2007; Yellowlees & Marks, 2007;
Turel & Serenko, 2012). The ndings of this study partially addressed
these questions and implied that at least at low-medium levels of addic-
tion-like symptoms, the observed symptoms are associated with some
brain changes (sensitization of the amygdala-striatal system), but not with
changes in all key brain systems associated with substance addictions (es-
pecially the prefrontal cortex). In this sense, it adheres to calls by research-
ers (Block, 2008) and the DSM–V (American Psychiatric Association, 2013)
to further examine the pathology of technology-related addictions.
Second, the ndings lend support to past research pointing to the im-
portance of the amygdala-striatal system in addiction pathology (Everitt,
et al., 1999; Jentsch & Taylor, 1999; Volkow & Fowler, 2000; Everitt & Rob-
bins, 2005; Koob & Volkow, 2010). In this study, this system responded to
Facebook cues, produced more false alarms in sign-go (Facebook inhibi-
tion) tasks, and resulted in shorter response times in Facebook-go trials
(compared to the response times with regards to neutral signs). Further-
more, the activation of this system was positively and signicantly corre-
lated with the “addiction” scores. Thus, these symptoms, at least in part,
manifest from automatically and easily retrieved implicit associations and
04_PR_Turel_140140.indd 686 16/12/14 10:58 AM
FACEBOOK AND NEURAL SYSTEMS 687
the consequent hyperactivity in the bilateral ventral striatum (Everitt, et
al., 1999). In this respect, Facebook “addiction” is similar to substance and
gambling addictions.
It was also hypothesized that the level of addiction-like symptoms in re-
lation to Facebook use would be negatively associated with activation of pre-
frontal inhibition brain structures, including the ventromedial prefrontal cor-
tex, lateral orbitofrontal, and inferior frontal gyrus regions, and the anterior
cingulate cortex (Di Chiara, 2000; Goldstein & Volkow, 2002; Volkow, Fowler,
Wang, & Swanson, 2004). These results were consistent with previous reports
that no-go trials with various stimuli activate the inhibitory control system
(Menon, Adleman, White, Glover, & Reiss, 2001; Garavan, Ross, Murphy,
Roche, & Stein, 2002), but also suggested that participants, regardless of their
level of addiction-like symptoms, presented normal functioning of the inhi-
bition system (i.e., no signicant hypo-activity was detected). That is, no im-
pairment of the inhibition system was observed.
These ndings imply that technology-related “addictions,” at least at
lower-to-medium levels of addiction-like symptoms, dier from other ad-
dictions, e.g., to illicit substances, on at least one dimension. While gam-
bling and substance addictions often involve the impairment of both the
impulse and inhibition brain systems (Noel, et al., 2013), technology-re-
lated “addictions,” at least at the examined levels of addiction-like symp-
toms, involve only changes to the amygdala-striatal system. Perhaps this
is a result of dierences between the adverse consequences in the case
of technology-related “addictions” (e.g., missing school) and those in the
case of other addictions (e.g., major health risks and troubles with the
law), the latter of which are likely to be more severe. While substance ad-
dicts often respond to substance-related cues without reection, and have
weak abilities to inhibit or exert cognitive control over their behaviors,
it seems that technology-related ”addicts” respond to Facebook cues in
a similar way, but have the capacity to inhibit such behaviors. Given the
abovementioned possible dierences between the adverse consequences
of substance and technology use, users of applications such as Facebook
perhaps lack the motivation to engage the prefrontal brain system (Shus-
ter & Toplak, 2009). This proposition, however, warrants further research.
Lastly, the ndings point to potential practical implications. They imply
that individuals who present low-medium levels of addiction-like symptoms
in relation to Facebook have an imbalance between their amygdala-striatal
and prefrontal cortex systems. Thus, their problematic use of Facebook (i.e.,
use that results in at least some addiction-like symptoms) can be overcome by
restoring the homeostasis between these two systems. This could be achieved
by cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT). Indeed, several attempts to apply
CBT in technology-related addiction cases have been reported to be success-
04_PR_Turel_140140.indd 687 16/12/14 10:58 AM
O. TUREL, ET AL.
688
ful (Young, 2007; van Rooij, Zinn, Schoenmakers, & van de Mheen, 2012), and
can perhaps also help with Facebook “addiction.”
Limitations and Future Research
Several limitations of this study that point to future research should
be acknowledged. First, the sample included educated young adults re-
siding in one country. Given possible age-, socio-economic-, and nation-
based dierences in technology “addictions,” future research may extend
the generalizability of the ndings to other populations, and in line with
the ecological model include a range of ecological risk factors, beyond
the individual, in the model (Catala-Minana, Lila, & Oliver, 2013; Raynor,
2013; Sterk, Elifson, & DePadilla, 2014). It can perhaps also focus on ad-
ditional possible consequences of such “addictions,” e.g., obesity. Second,
the sample was limited in the range of addiction-like symptoms it pre-
sented. Because the sample included participants with low-medium lev-
els of addiction-like symptoms, future research could examine those few
extreme users with very high addiction scores to nd if there is a point
of inection after which prefrontal cortex impairments might be observ-
able. This study was correlational in nature, and hence caution regarding
causality arguments should be exercised. Addiction co-morbidity did not
exist in the current sample. Nevertheless, longitudinal studies can be ex-
ecuted to see if prefrontal cortex weaknesses in these few extreme users
progress into not only Internet “addiction,” but also into other addictive
behaviors. Since Internet use starts at a very early age, typically before any
exposure to addictive substances, this would help address an important
theoretical question in addiction research on whether brain abnormalities
precede substance abuse, or whether these abnormalities are actually the
consequences of substance abuse (Ersche, Jones, Williams, Turton, Rob-
bins, & Bullmore, 2012).
While the authors have acknowledged the yet-unknown appropriate-
ness of the term “addiction” when applied to Facebook by using quotation
marks, more research on similarities and dierences between Facebook
“addiction” and substance and gambling addictions should be conducted.
Noteworthy is the fact that this study focused on “addiction” to an activ-
ity that is legal and has socially acceptable symptoms, or at least symptoms
not judged harshly by society. In contrast, addictions to substances or gam-
bling can have much more severe consequences. Perhaps these dierences
in societal view of such addictions and possible dierences in the sever-
ity of the consequences act as inhibition (de)motivators, and consequently
people may lack the motivation to inhibit their Facebook use, rather than
having impaired inhibition systems. This proposition, however, merits fur-
ther research. Similarly, Facebook “addiction” may not be a cause but a me-
diator or moderator of some other facet of experience. It could also serve
04_PR_Turel_140140.indd 688 16/12/14 10:58 AM
FACEBOOK AND NEURAL SYSTEMS 689
as a gateway for developing other addictions (i.e., when prefrontal cortex
changes are observed), at which point early interventions could help avert
more serious addictions. This idea, too, merits further research. Lastly,
this study points to the possibility that some interventions, such as CBT
or trans-cranial magnetic stimulation (Hallett, 2000), may be ecacious in
treating Facebook “addiction.” However, more research on the ecacy of
these therapeutic strategies to deal with such “addictions” is needed.
REFERENCES
aMeRicaN psychiatRic associatioN. (2000) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disor-
ders–text revision. (4th ed.) Washington, DC: Author.
aMeRicaN psychiatRic associatioN. (2013) Internet gaming disorder diagnostic and statistical
manual of mental disorders. (5th ed.) Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publish-
ing. Pp. 795-798.
aNdeRssoN, J. L. R., JeNkiNsoN, M., & sMith, S. (2007a) Non-linear optimisation. (FMRIB
Technical Report TR07JA1) Retrieved from www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/analysis/techrep.
aNdeRssoN, J. L. R., JeNkiNsoN, M., & sMith, S. (2007b) Non-linear registration, aka spatial
normalisation. (FMRIB Technical Report TR07JA2) Retrieved from www.fmrib.
ox.ac.uk/analysis/techrep.
aNdReasseN, C. S., toRsheiM, T., BRuNBoRg, G. S., & palleseN, S. (2012) Development of a
Facebook addiction scale. Psychological Reports, 110(2), 501-517. DOI: 10.2466/02.09.
18.PR0.110.2.501-517
aRNsteN, A. F. T., & li, B. M. (2005) Neurobiology of executive functions: catecholamine
inuences on prefrontal cortical functions. Biological Psychiatry, 57(11), 1377-1384.
DOI: 10.1016/j.bps.2004.08.019
BakkeN, I. J., WeNzel, H. G. R. O., götestaM, K. G., JohaNssoN, A., & oeReN, A. (2009)
Internet addiction among Norwegian adults: a stratied probability sample study.
Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 50(2), 121-127.
BalleiNe, B. W., & dickiNsoN, A. (2000) The eect of lesions of the insular cortex on
instrumental conditioning: evidence for a role in incentive memory. Journal of Neu-
roscience, 20, 8954-8964.
BechaRa, A. (2005) Decision-making, impulse control, and loss of willpower to resist
drugs: a neurocognitive perspective. Nature Neuroscience, 8(11), 1458-1463.
BechaRa, A., tRaNel, D., & daMasio, H. (2000) Characterization of the decision-making
decit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain, 123(11), 2189-
2202.
BeckMaNN, C. F., JeNkiNsoN, M., & sMith, S. M. (2003) General multilevel linear modeling
for group analysis in fMRI. Neuroimage, 20(2), 1052-1063.
BeRgMaRk, K. H., BeRgMaRk, A., & FiNdahl, O. (2011) Extensive Internet involvement–
addiction or emerging lifestyle? International Journal of Environmental Research and
Public Health, 8(12), 4488-4501. DOI: 10.3390/ijerph8124488
Bickel, W. K., MilleR, M. L., yi, R., koWal, B. P., liNdquist, D. M., & pitcock, J. A. (2007)
Behavioral and neuroeconomics of drug addiction: competing neural systems and
temporal discounting processes. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 90, S85-S91. DOI:
10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.09.016
04_PR_Turel_140140.indd 689 16/12/14 10:58 AM
O. TUREL, ET AL.
690
Block, J. J. (2008) Issues for DSM–V: Internet addiction. American Journal of Psychiatry,
165(3), 306-307. DOI: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07101556
BRaveR, T. S., & Ruge, H. (2006) Functional neuroimaging of executive functions. In R.
Cabeza & A. Kingstone (Eds.), Handbook of functional neuroimaging of cognition. (2nd
ed.) Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press. Pp. 307-348.
ByuN, S., RuFFiNi, C., Mills, J. E., douglas, A. C., NiaNg, M., stepcheNkova, S., lee, S. K.,
loutFi, J., lee, J. K., atallah, M., & BlaNtoN, M. (2009) Internet addiction: meta-
synthesis of 1996–2006 quantitative research. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 12(2),
203-207. DOI: 10.1089/cpb.2008.0102
cao, F., & su, L. (2007) Internet addiction among Chinese adolescents: prevalence and
psychological features. Child: Care, Health and Development, 33(3), 275-281.
casey, B. J., getz, S., & galvaN, A. (2008) The adolescent brain. Developmental Review,
28(1), 62-77. DOI: 10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.003
casey, B. J., giedd, J. N., & thoMas, K. M. (2000) Structural and functional brain devel-
opment and its relation to cognitive development. Biological Psychology, 54(1-3),
241-257. DOI: 10.1016/s0301-0511(00)00058-2
casey, B. J., totteNhaM, N., listoN, C., & duRstoN, S. (2005) Imaging the developing
brain: what have we learned about cognitive development? Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 9(3), 104-110. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.01.011
catala-MiNaNa, A., lila, M., & oliveR, A. (2013) Alcohol consumption in men punished
for intimate partner violence: individual and contextual factors. Adicciones, 25(1),
19-28.
chaRltoN, J. P., & daNFoRth, I. D. W. (2007) Distinguishing addiction and high engage-
ment in the context of online game playing. Computers in Human Behavior, 23(3),
1531-1548.
chou, C., coNdRoN, L., & BellaNd, J. C. (2005) A review of the research on Internet
addiction. Educational Psychology Review, 17(4), 363-388. DOI: 10.1007/s10648-005-
8138-1
d'aRcy, J., gupta, A., taRaFdaR, M., & tuRel, O. (2014) Reecting on the “Dark Side”
of information technology use. Communications of the Association for Information
Systems, 35(1), 109-118.
di chiaRa, G. (2000) Role of dopamine in the behavioural actions of nicotine related
to addiction. European Journal of Pharmacology, 393(1-3), 295-314. DOI: 10.1016/
s0014-2999(00)00122-9
di chiaRa, G., taNda, G., BassaReo, V., poNtieRi, F., acquas, E., FeNu, S., cadoNi, C., &
caRBoNi, E. (1999) Drug addiction as a disorder of associative learning: role of
nucleus accumbens shell/extended amygdala dopamine. Annals of the New York
Academy of Sciences (Advancing from the Ventral Striatum to the Extended Amygdala),
877, 461-485.
echeBuRua, E., & de coRRal, P. (2010) Addiction to new technologies and to online social
networking in young people: a new challenge. Adicciones, 22(21), 91-95.
eRsche, K. D., JoNes, P. S., WilliaMs, G. B., tuRtoN, A. J., RoBBiNs, T. W., & BullMoRe, E. T.
(2012) Abnormal brain structure implicated in stimulant drug addiction. Science,
335(6068), 601-604. DOI: 10.1126/science.1214463
eveRitt, B. J., paRkiNsoN, J. A., olMstead, M. C., aRRoyo, M., RoBledo, P., & RoBBiNs, T.
W. (1999) Associative processes in addiction and reward: the role of amygdala
and ventral striatal subsystems. In J. F. McGinty (Ed.), Advancing from the ventral
striatum to the extended amygdala. Vol. 877. New York: Annals of the New York
Academy of Sciences. Pp. 412-438.
04_PR_Turel_140140.indd 690 16/12/14 10:58 AM
FACEBOOK AND NEURAL SYSTEMS 691
eveRitt, B. J., & RoBBiNs, T. W. (2005) Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addic-
tion: from actions to habits to compulsion. Nature Neuroscience, 8(11), 1481-1489.
DOI: 10.1038/nn1579
FelloWs, L. K. (2004) The cognitive neuroscience of human decision making: a review
and conceptual framework. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 3(3),
159-172.
gaRavaN, H., Ross, T. J., MuRphy, K., Roche, R. A. P., & steiN, E. A. (2002) Dissociable
executive functions in the dynamic control of behavior: inhibition, error detection,
and correction. NeuroImage, 17(4), 1820-1829. DOI: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1326
ghasseMzadeh, L., shahRaRay, M., & MoRadi, A. (2008) Prevalence of Internet addic-
tion and comparison of Internet addicts and non-addicts in Iranian high schools.
Cyberpsychology & Behavior: The Impact of the Internet, Multimedia and Virtual Reality
on Behavior and Society, 11(6), 731.
glascheR, J., adolphs, R., daMasio, H., BechaRa, A., RudRauF, D., calaMia, M., paul, L.
K., & tRaNel, D. (2012) Lesion mapping of cognitive control and value-based deci-
sion making in the prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
of the United States of America, 109(36), 14681-14686. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1206608109
goldsteiN, R. Z., & volkoW, N. D. (2002) Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiolog-
ical basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. Ameri-
can Journal of Psychiatry, 159(10), 1642-1652. DOI: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.10.1642
gReeNField, D. (1999) Virtual addiction: help for netheads, cyberfreaks, and those who love
them. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.
gRiFFiths, M. D. (1995) Technological addictions. Clinical Psychology Forum, 76(1), 14-19.
gRiFFiths, M. D. (1998) Internet addiction: does it really exist? In J. Gackenbach (Ed.),
Psychology and the Internet: intrapersonal, interpersonal and transpersonal implications.
New York: Academic Press. Pp. 61-75.
gRiFFiths, M. D. (1999) Internet addiction: fact or ction? The Psychologist, 12(5), 246-251.
gRiFFiths, M. D. (2012) Facebook addiction: concerns, critics, and recommendations:
a response to Andreassen and colleagues. Psychological Reports, 110(2), 518-520.
DOI: 10.2466/01.07.18.pr0.110.2.518-520
hallett, M. (2000) Transcranial magnetic stimulation and the human brain. Nature,
406(6792), 147-150. DOI: 10.1038/35018000
haN, D. H., Bolo, N., daNiels, M. A., aReNella, L., lyoo, I. K., & ReNshaW, P. F. (2011)
Brain activity and desire for Internet video game play. Comprehensive Psychiatry,
52(1), 88-95. DOI: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2010.04.004
haN, D. H., hWaNg, J. W., & ReNshaW, P. F. (2010) Bupropion sustained release treatment
decreases craving for video games and cue-induced brain activity in patients with
Internet video game addiction. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 18(4),
297-304. DOI: 10.1037/a0020023
haN, D. H., kiM, Y. S., lee, Y. S., MiN, K. J., & ReNshaW, P. F. (2010) Changes in cue-
induced, prefrontal cortex activity with video-game play. Cyberpsychology, Behav-
ior, and Social Networking, 13(6). DOI: 10.1089/cyber.2009.0327
haN, D. H., kiM, S. M., lee, Y. S., & ReNshaW, P. F. (2012) The eect of family therapy on
the changes in the severity of on-line game play and brain activity in adolescents
with on-line game addiction. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 202(2), 126-131.
DOI: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.02.011
04_PR_Turel_140140.indd 691 16/12/14 10:58 AM
O. TUREL, ET AL.
692
haN, D. H., lyoo, I. K., & ReNshaW, P. F. (2012) Dierential regional gray matter vol-
umes in patients with on-line game addiction and professional gamers. Journal of
Psychiatric Research, 46(4), 507-515. DOI: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.01.004
JeNkiNsoN, M., & sMith, S. (2001) A global optimisation method for robust ane registra-
tion of brain images. Medical Image Analysis, 5(2), 143-156.
JeNtsch, J. D., & tayloR, J. R. (1999) Impulsivity resulting from frontostriatal dysfunc-
tion in drug abuse: implications for the control of behavior by reward-related
stimuli. Psychopharmacology, 146(4), 373-390. DOI: 10.1007/pl00005483
JohaNssoN, A., & götestaM, K. G. (2004) Internet addiction: characteristics of a question-
naire and prevalence in Norwegian youth (12–18 years). Scandinavian Journal of
Psychology, 45(3), 223-229.
kaRaiskos, D., tzavellas, E., Balta, G., & papaRRigopoulos, T. (2010) P02-232 - Social
network addiction: a new clinical disorder? European Psychiatry, 25(Suppl. 1), 855.
kiM, K., Ryu, E., choN, M-Y., yeuN, E-J., choi, S-Y., seo, J-S., & NaM, B-W. (2006) Inter-
net addiction in Korean adolescents and its relation to depression and suicidal
ideation: a questionnaire survey. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 43(2), 185-
192.
ko, C-H., liu, G-C., hsiao, S. M., yeN, J-Y., yaNg, M-J., liN, W-C., yeN, C-F., & cheN, C-S.
(2009) Brain activities associated with gaming urge of online gaming addiction.
Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43(7), 739-747. DOI: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.09.012
ko, C-H., liu, G-C., yeN, J-Y., cheN, C-Y., yeN, C-F., & cheN, C-S. (2013) Brain corre-
lates of craving for online gaming under cue exposure in subjects with Internet
gaming addiction and in remitted subjects. Addiction Biology, 18(3), 559-569. DOI:
10.1111/j.1369-1600.2011.00405.x
ko, C-H., liu, G-C., yeN, J-Y., yeN, C-F., cheN, C-S., & liN, W-C. (2013) The brain acti-
vations for both cue-induced gaming urge and smoking craving among subjects
comorbid with Internet gaming addiction and nicotine dependence. Journal of Psy-
chiatric Research, 47(4), 486-493. DOI: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.11.008
kooB, G. F., & le Moal, M. (2001) Drug addiction, dysregulation of reward, and allosta-
sis. Neuropsychopharmacology, 24(278), 97-129.
kooB, G. F., & volkoW, N. D. (2010) Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacol-
ogy, 35(1), 217-238. DOI: 10.1038/npp.2009.110
kuss, D. J., & gRiFFiths, M. D. (2011) Online social networking and addiction—a review
of the psychological literature. International Journal of Environmental Research and
Public Health, 8(9), 3528-3552. DOI: 10.3390/ijerph8093528
kuss, D. J., gRiFFiths, M. D., & BiNdeR, J. F. (2013) Internet addiction in students: preva-
lence and risk factors. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(3), 959-966. DOI: 10.1016/j.
chb.2012.12.024
la BaRBeRa, D., la paglia, F., & valsavoia, R. (2009) Social network and addiction.
Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 12(5), 628-629.
laRose, R. (2010) The problem of media habits. Communication Theory, 20(2), 194-222.
DOI: 10.1111/j.1468-2885.2010.01360.x
laRose, R., liN, C. A., & eastiN, M. S. (2003) Unregulated Internet usage: addiction,
habit, or decient self-regulation? Media Psychology, 5(3), 225-253.
logaN, G. D., schachaR, R. J., & taNNock, R. (1997) Impulsivity and inhibitory control.
Psychological Science, 8(1), 60-64.
04_PR_Turel_140140.indd 692 16/12/14 10:58 AM
FACEBOOK AND NEURAL SYSTEMS 693
MacMillaN, N. A., & cReelMaN, C. D. (1996) Triangles in ROC space: history and theory
of “nonparametric” measures of sensitivity and response bias. Psychonomic Bul-
letin & Review, 3(2), 164-170. DOI: 10.3758/bf03212415
MeNoN, V., adleMaN, N. E., White, C. D., gloveR, G. H., & Reiss, A. L. (2001) Error-
related brain activation during a Go/NoGo response inhibition task. Human Brain
Mapping, 12(3), 131-143. DOI: 10.1002/1097-0193(200103)12:3<131::aid-hbm1010>
3.0.co;2-c
Meshi, D., MoRaWetz, C., & heekeReN, H. R. (2013) Nucleus accumbens response to
gains in reputation for the self relative to gains for others predicts social media
use. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7. DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00439
Noel, X., BReveRs, D., & BechaRa, A. (2013) A neurocognitive approach to understand-
ing the neurobiology of addiction. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23(4), 632-638.
paRk, S. K., kiM, J. Y., & cho, C. B. (2008) Prevalence of Internet addiction and cor-
relations with family factors among South Korean adolescents. Adolescence (San
Diego): An International Quarterly Devoted to the Physiological, Psychological, Psychi-
atric, Sociological, and Educational Aspects of the Second Decade of Human Life, 43(172),
15.
pRataRelli, M. E., BRoWNe, B. L., & JohNsoN, K. (1999) The bits and bytes of computer/
Internet addiction: a factor analytic approach. Behavior Research Methods, Instru-
ments, & Computers, 31(2), 305-314.
RayNoR, P. A. (2013) An exploration of the factors inuencing parental self-ecacy
for parents recovering from substance use disorders using the social ecological
framework. Journal of Addictions Nursing, 24(2), 91-99. DOI: 10.1097/JAN.0b013
e3182922069
ReNdi, M., szaBo, A., & szaBó, T. (2007) Exercise and Internet addiction: communalities
and dierences between two problematic behaviours. International Journal of Men-
tal Health and Addiction, 5(3), 219-232.
RoBBiNs, T. W., cadoR, M., tayloR, J. R., & eveRitt, B. J. (1989) Limbic-striatal interac-
tions in reward-related processes. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 13(2-3),
155-162.
RoBiNsoN, T. E., & BeRRidge, K. C. (1993) The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-
sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Research Reviews, 18, 247-291.
RoBiNsoN, T. E., & BeRRidge, K. C. (2003) Addiction. Annual Review of Psychology, 54(1),
25-53.
shaW, M., & Black, D. W. (2008) Internet addiction: denition, assessment, epidemiol-
ogy and clinical management. CNS Drugs, 22(5), 353-365.
shusteR, J., & toplak, M. E. (2009) Executive and motivational inhibition: associations
with self-report measures related to inhibition. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(2),
471-480. DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2009.01.004
sioMos, K. E., daFouli, E. D., BRaiMiotis, D. A., Mouzas, O. D., & aNgelopoulos, N. V.
(2008) Internet addiction among Greek adolescent students. CyberPsychology &
Behavior, 11(6), 653-657.
steiNBeRg, L. (2005) Cognitive and aective development in adolescence. Trends in Cog-
nitive Sciences, 9(2), 69-74. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.005
steiNBeRg, L. (2008) A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Devel-
opmental Review, 28(1), 78-106. DOI: 10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002
04_PR_Turel_140140.indd 693 16/12/14 10:58 AM
O. TUREL, ET AL.
694
steiNBeRg, L., gRahaM, S., o'BRieN, L., WoolaRd, J., cauFFMaN, E., & BaNich, M. (2009)
Age dierences in future orientation and delay discounting. Child Development,
80(1), 28-44. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01244.x
steRk, C. E., eliFsoN, K. W., & depadilla, L. (2014) Neighbourhood structural charac-
teristics and crack cocaine use: exploring the impact of perceived neighbourhood
disorder on use among African Americans. International Journal of Drug Policy,
25(3), 616-623. DOI: 10.1016/j.drugpo.2013.12.007
steWaRt, J., deWit, H., & eikelBooM, R. (1984) Role of unconditioned and conditioned
drug eects in the self-administration of opiates and stimulants. Psychological
Review, 91(2), 251-268.
tuRel, O., & seReNko, A. (2012) The benets and dangers of enjoyment with social net-
working websites. European Journal of Information Systems, 21(5), 512-528. DOI:
10.1057/ejis.2012.1
tuRel, O., seReNko, A., & giles, P. (2011) Integrating technology addiction and use: an
empirical investigation of online auction users. MIS Quarterly, 35(4), 1043-1061.
vaN RooiJ, A. J., schoeNMakeRs, T. M., veRMulst, A. A., vaN deN eiJNdeN, R., & vaN de
MheeN, D. (2011) Online video game addiction: identication of addicted ado-
lescent gamers. Addiction, 106(1), 205-212. DOI: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03104.x
vaN RooiJ, A. J., ziNN, M. F., schoeNMakeRs, T. M., & vaN de MheeN, D. (2012) Treating
Internet addiction with cognitive-behavioral therapy: a thematic analysis of the
experiences of therapists. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 10(1),
69-82. DOI: 10.1007/s11469-010-9295-0
volkoW, N. D., & FoWleR, J. S. (2000) Addiction, a disease of compulsion and drive:
involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 10(3), 318-325. DOI:
10.1093/cercor/10.3.318
volkoW, N. D., FoWleR, J. S., WaNg, G. J., & sWaNsoN, J. M. (2004) Dopamine in drug
abuse and addiction: results from imaging studies and treatment implications.
Molecular Psychiatry, 9(6), 557-569. DOI: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001507
WheeleR, E. Z., & FelloWs, L. K. (2008) The human ventromedial frontal lobe is critical
for learning from negative feedback. Brain, 131(5), 1323-1331.
Wise, R. A., & RoMpRe, P. P. (1989) Brain dopamine and reward. Annual Reviews of Psy-
chology, 40, 191-225.
Wood, W., & Neal, D. T. (2007) A new look at habits and the habit-goal interface. Psy-
chological Review, 114(4), 843-863.
WoolRich, M. (2008) Robust group analysis using outlier inference. NeuroImage, 41(2),
286-301.
WoolRich, M. W., BehReNs, T. E. J., BeckMaNN, C. F., JeNkiNsoN, M., & sMith, S. M. (2004)
Multilevel linear modelling for fMRI group analysis using Bayesian inference.
NeuroImage, 21(4), 1732-1747.
WoRld health oRgaNizatioN. (1992) The ICD-10 classication of mental and behavioral dis-
orders: clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva, Switzerland: Author.
yelloWlees, P. M., & MaRks, S. (2007) Problematic Internet use or Internet addiction?
Computers in Human Behavior, 23(3), 1447-1453. DOI: 10.1016/j.chb.2005.05.004
youNg, K. S. (1998a) Caught in the net: how to recognize the signs of Internet addiction—and
a winning strategy for recovery. New York: Wiley.
youNg, K. S. (1998b) Internet addiction: the emergence of a new clinical disorder. Cyber-
psychology & Behavior, 3(2), 237-244.
04_PR_Turel_140140.indd 694 16/12/14 10:58 AM
FACEBOOK AND NEURAL SYSTEMS 695
youNg, K. S. (2004) Internet addiction: a new clinical phenomenon and its consequences.
American Behavioral Scientist, 48(4), 402-415. DOI: 10.1177/0002764204270278
youNg, K. S. (2007) Cognitive behavior therapy with Internet addicts: treatment out-
comes and implications. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 10, 671-679. DOI: 10.1089/
cpb.2007.9971
youNg, K. S. (2010) Internet addiction over the decade: a personal look back. World
Psychiatry, 9(2), 91.
Accepted November 13, 2014.
04_PR_Turel_140140.indd 695 16/12/14 10:58 AM
... Empirical evidence suggests a positive correlation between authentic self-presentation and improved mental health and well-being (Kernis and Goldman, 2006;Smallenbroek, et al., 2017;Wood, et al., 2008). False self-presentations, conversely, may bring about negative outcomes to subjective and psychological well-being (Wood, et al., 2008), such as getting addicted to social media and limiting face-to-face interactions (Gil-Or, et al., 2015;Turel, et al., 2014;Walker, 2000). A nuanced understanding of social media users' motives and how they express themselves on those platforms is needed to propose treatments for therapists and strategies for social media corporations. ...
... The relationship is applicable in the context of social media. People with low self-esteem spend more time on social media and are less likely to present their true selves (Gil-Or, et al., 2015;Tazghini and Siedlecki, 2013;Turel, et al., 2014;Vogel, et al., 2014). Social compensation theory suggests that people struggling with relationships (e.g., low self-esteem people) are more motivated to use social media, or virtual environments in general, as a means of compensating for their less satisfactory real-life interactions (McKenna, et al., 2002). ...
Article
Full-text available
Empirical evidence has suggested a positive correlation between authentic self-presentation on social media and improved mental health and well-being. False self-presentation, conversely, may bring about negative outcomes to subjective and psychological well-being. Our study examines the different selves that users present, as well as factors influencing self-presentation, on social media platforms. Results from 392 survey responses suggest that, first, users present themselves differently on Facebook, LinkedIn, and TikTok; generally, they tend to present their real and ideal selves more than the false selves. Second, users seek significantly different uses and gratifications from different social media platforms (e.g., maintaining existing relationships on Facebook, meeting new people and socializing on LinkedIn, or entertainment purposes on TikTok); nevertheless, education and information is a common motivation for social media use. Third, social media uses and gratifications, social media usage, and self-esteem are influential factors of users’ self-presentation on social media. The dynamics of these influential factors, however, differ across platforms. Theoretical implications, as well as empirical suggestions to enhance the quality of social media interactions, are further discussed.
... Both Reed (2023) and Meshi et al. (2019) correlated PSMU with risky decisions on the IGT, while Meshi et al. (2019) found no such relationship during the BART. While investigating other types of impulsiveness, some studies have found no correlation between PSMU and motor impulsivity as tested by the Go-Nogo task (Gao et al., 2019;Turel et al., 2014), although one recent study found increased errors in PSMU groups (Reed, 2023). The link between PSMU and inhibitory control is therefore inconclusive and often contradictory, requiring further research. ...
... Although the automatic urge to engage with social media is thought to be a potential expression of motor impulsivity (Lewin et al., 2023), the present study did not find a significant relationship between PSMU and Go-Nogo performance. The absence of this relationship is consistent across behavioral addictions, whereby it is thought that the Go-Nogo Task may not detect subtle impairments in inhibitory control (Chung et al., 2019;Turel et al., 2014;Wegmann et al., 2020). Both neuroimaging and electrophysiology techniques have uncovered slight cognitive differences that were not apparent behaviorally (Ding et al., 2014;Gao et al., 2019). ...
... Social media's design promotes overstimulation of dopamine pathways. Notifications, likes, and shares trigger dopamine release, reinforcing compulsive usage patterns [20]. This neural response mirrors addiction mechanisms, where prolonged stimulation leads to desensitization and an increasing need for engagement to achieve the same level of satisfaction [6]. ...
Article
The pervasive influence of social media platforms on cognitive function and mental health has become a significant concern in the digital age. These platforms, designed to capture and sustain user attention through features like notifications and algorithm-driven content, contribute to fragmented attention, reduced focus, and overstimulation. Neuroimaging studies highlight the activation of reward pathways, prioritizing instant gratification at the expense of sustained cognitive engagement. This digital overstimulation, coupled with multitasking, impairs cognitive efficiency and memory retention, leading to the phenomenon of "digital amnesia." Social media use also influences decision-making and impulse control, particularly in younger users, by fostering impulsive behaviors and undermining critical thinking. Additionally, exposure to idealized portrayals of others' lives exacerbates feelings of inadequacy, which can negatively impact mental health, particularly in adolescents and young adults. Strategies for mitigating these effects include digital detoxes, mindful social media usage, and education on emotional intelligence, promoting offline activities, platform responsibility, community support, and research into long-term consequences. Ultimately, while social media facilitates connection and information sharing, its excessive use poses significant risks to cognitive function and emotional well-being.
... Se considera que, siquiera una minoría de los jóvenes expuestos a las redes sociales, podrían convertirse en adictos a las mismas, en los mismos términos que experimentarían en una adicción a sustancias o al juego (Kuss & Griffiths, 2017;Turel et al., 2014a), variando la prevalencia de la adicción general a internet entre 12.6% al 67.5%, por la relevancia de las diferencias intercultura-les (Kuss et al., 2021)this review summarizes and investigates studies published outside of Europe between April 2013 and September 2019.\nMethods\nLiterature on Internet addiction was included and reviewed critically following the PRISMA guidelines, with the objectives to (i. Griffiths (2018) señala que, aunque el número de adolescentes que pudieran ser considerados "adictos", en sentido estricto, pueda no ser considerado elevado, no puede olvidarse que muchos otros considerados "heavy users" o usuarios habituales de estas redes sociales pueden también experimentar consecuencias problemáticas derivadas relacionadas con las herramientas de "enganche" psicológico se usan en estas redes sociales y plataformas. ...
Article
Full-text available
Resumen El objeto de este trabajo es cuestionarse si el Derecho Penal habría de intervenir o no en el control de los riesgos derivados del diseño adictivo de las redes sociales y plataformas, a la vista de las conclusiones aportadas por la literatura científica especializada. Se concluye afirmativamente sobre esta cuestión, sin perjuicio de la tutela administrativa que pueda también darse, formulando los rudimentos de una propuesta político-criminal incriminadora, articulada en dos pilares fundamentales. El primero de ellos es la preponderancia de dos principios fundamentales: el cri-terio de precaución, frente a riesgos muy importantes o sistémicos, y el principio de responsabilidad pro víctima, que prioriza el control del riesgo ante la posibilidad de los daños y riesgos que la literatura científica conecta al diseño adictivo de las redes sociales y plataformas. Como segundo pilar, considerar estos diseños adictivos fuera del riesgo permitido con relación a los menores, sirviéndonos del paragüas que proporciona ya el art. 5 del Reglamento IA de la UE y centrándonos muy par-ticularmente, en la responsabilidad de las plataformas que usan tales herramientas de IA para conseguir la atención y la constante y compulsiva conexión del usuario. Palabras clave Adicción a redes sociales, IA persuasiva, algoritmos adictivos, responsabilidad penal por diseño algorítmico adictivo Abstract The purpose of this work is to examine whether Criminal Law should intervene in the regulation of risks associated with the addictive design of social networks and platforms, in light of the findings provided by specialized scientific literature. This analysis reaches an affirmative conclusion on the matter, without prejudice to the administrative oversight that may also apply, and outlines the foundational elements of a criminal policy framework based on two fundamental pillars. The
... It indicates that young adults with higher levels of TikTok addiction might also act and make decisions more impulsively, especially in a digital context. Specifically, those who are more addicted to TikTok tend to act more impulsively, likely because the platform encourages quick responses and instant gratification, a pattern supported by Turel et al., [46] who found that social media addiction disrupts selfcontrol. Moreover, digital maturity and morality are both negatively significant predictors of impulsivity in young adults. ...
Article
Full-text available
TikTok, a popular social media network, has gained an immense worldwide following because of its entertaining short video format. The main aim of this study was to understand the dynamic association between the variables TikTok addiction, impulsivity, digital maturity, and morality in young adults. For this purpose, a quantitative study with a correlational research design was adopted. Purposive sampling was used to select 423 TikTok users from private universities, with 209 males and 214 females included. The data was collected through the TikTok Addiction Scale, which has been adapted from the Social Media Addiction Scale (Sahin, 2018), the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale Version 11 (Barratt et al., 1995), the multidimensional Digital Maturity Inventory (Laaber et al., 2023), and the Moral Character Questionnaire (Furr et al., 2022). The results showed a strong significant positive relationship between TikTok addiction and impulsivity, but significant negative correlation with digital maturity and morality. Impulsivity had a strong significant negative relationship with digital maturity and morality, whereas digital maturity had a strong positive relationship with morality. The results of this study can be helpful in raising awareness about the adverse effects of TikTok addiction and in promoting digital literacy through workshops or seminars.
... As a result, the mental health issue among college students is becoming an increasing concern (Ripon et al., 2022). Previous evidence suggests that, compared to male students, female students are at higher risk of FBA (Andreassen et al., 2012;Marino et al., 2018;Ripon et al., 2022;Shakir et al., 2017;Turel et al., 2014) and PD (Vuelvas-Olmos et al., 2023); however, the level of LS among female students is also higher (Becchetti & Conzo, 2022;Joshanloo & Jovanović, 2020). The above conflicting evidence prompted us to explore the association between FBA and mental health in female college students, as well as the mechanism underlying this relationship. ...
Article
Full-text available
There is still a lack of research investigating the association between Facebook addiction and mental health (psychological distress and life satisfaction) among female college students, as well as the factors that may explain this association. This study aimed to examine whether Facebook addiction affects the mental health of female college students and whether resilience mediates this relationship. A cross-sectional study was conducted with Vietnamese female college students who use Facebook, with a sample of 987 participants (mean age = 19.99, SD = 1.405) from two universities in Vietnam. The results indicated that 81% of the participants are at risk of Facebook addiction, and resilience was found to mediate the association between Facebook addiction and mental health (β = 0.02, CI = [0.005; 0.042] and β = -0.046, CI = [-0.078; -0.018]). These findings provide further understanding of the pathways through which Facebook addiction impacts mental health among female college students. More importantly, the results suggest that measures to improve mental health in this population should focus on reducing Facebook addiction and enhancing resilience.
... Conversely, proponents of the addiction framework emphasize the neurological and psychological similarities between behavioral and substance addictions. They note that the same brain reward circuits activated by drugs can also be triggered by social media interactions, especially those involving likes, shares, and comments (Turel et al., 2014). This debate highlights the need for nuanced research and precise diagnostic criteria to better understand problematic social media use and its impact on youth. ...
Article
Full-text available
Despite existing research on the factors associated with problematic social media use (PSMU), cyberbullying (CB), and cybervictimization (CV) among youth, few studies have explored the relationships between these variables. Rooted in the Maladaptive Coping Theory, this study investigates the complex dynamics of digital behaviors among Italian preadolescents, exploring the interplay between PSMU, moral disengagement, retaliation, and their connections with cyberbullying and cybervictimization. A total of 489 participants (Mage = 11.90, SD = 0.783; 50.3% female) from schools in southern Italy completed an online self-report questionnaire. Descriptive analyses were conducted and correlations between variables were computed. Mediation and moderation analyses were performed using PROCESS in SPSS 27.0. The results of the study indicated that PSMU positively predicted both CB and CV. Moral disengagement (MD) was identified as a mediator in this association, elucidating the role of justifications and cognitive restructuring in facilitating negative online behaviors. Most notably, retaliation emerged as a moderator that amplified the effects of PSMU on cyberbullying and cybervictimization. These findings underscore the pivotal role of PSMU in shaping digital landscapes for preadolescents and highlight the need for tailored interventions. In addition, they highlight MD as a maladaptive coping strategy in the online realm, revealing its instrumental role in perpetuating negative online behaviors. This study’s insights suggest the need for interventions to address the complexity of young people’s online interactions and promote a safer digital environment.
... The same case can be made for the excessive use of smartphones and social media, which is often attributed to poor self-control (Berger et al., 2018;Kim et al., 2016). However, these behaviors are significantly influenced by the design of digital platforms that heavily invest in user engagement (Alter, 2017;Turel et al., 2014;Zuboff, 2019) or by societal shifts toward digital connectivity and the normalization of constant online presence (Twenge et al., 2019). ...
... Agogo and Hess, 2018;James et al., 2017;Ning et al., 2022;Polites et al., 2018;Vaghefi et al., 2023;Xu et al., 2017;Xu et al., 2022) and have a common thread: they explain the psycho-physiological processes that underlie addiction development (as sometimes exacerbated by technological affordances), and the manifestation of addiction in various negative outcomes. Despite this reliance on substance addiction theories and models, it has been shown that there are also notable differences among the types of addiction in pathogenesis and treatments (Marci, 2022;Turel et al., 2014;Xu et al., 2021). As such, we conclude that IS scholars could attempt to reduce technology addiction by leveraging substance addiction interventions, but not doing so blindly. ...
Article
Purpose In this current review, we aimed to understand technology addiction interventions and provide guidelines for IS scholars to use IT to prevent or attenuate technology addiction. Design/methodology/approach We systematically reviewed articles associated with technology and substance addiction interventions. These articles included review articles, peer-reviewed articles, conference proceedings, and online articles. Findings We propose a roadmap for technology addiction intervention development and testing based on the review. Next, we summarize the similarities and differences between substance addiction and technology addiction in terms of antecedents, negative consequences, and neurobiological mechanisms. Based on this, two types of potential interventions for substance addiction were reviewed to explore how they can be used for technology addiction. To conclude, IT-mediated interventions were summarized, and promising avenues for future research were highlighted. Originality/value Technology addiction has a broad range of adverse impacts on mental health and well-being. With the knowledge and insight from this review, the Information Systems community can become part of the solution to technology addiction.
Article
Full-text available
Resumen Abstract Alcohol consumption is often associated with violence against women. The aim of this paper is to analyze the relationship between alcohol and other relevant variables in the intervention with men convicted of intimate partner violence, both at the individual and contextual spheres. Clinical symptomatology, Drug abuse, Impulsivity, Self-esteem, Assumption of responsibility, Intimate support perception, Social rejection perception, Accumulation of stressful life events, Income perception and Social support in community are assessed in a sample of 291 participants in an intervention program for men condemned for intimate partner violence. Data were analyzed using bivariate correlations and ANOVAs. Statistically significant differences were obtained among Risk consumers and Not risk consumers in Clinical symptomatology, Drug abuse, Impulsivity, Self-esteem and Attribution of blame to personal context as individual variables and Intimate support perception, Social rejection and Accumulation of stressful life events as contextual variables. Results of previous work are confirmed and the importance of considering social factors in the participants' environment when considering decreasing alcohol consumption and intimate partner violence is demonstrated. New tools for enhancing interventions in rehabilitation programs with men convicted for violence against women is provided.
Article
Full-text available
The authors of this article participated in a panel session at the Americas Conference on Information Systems (AMCIS) 2012 with the objective to advance knowledge in areas related to the "dark side" of information technology (IT) use in organizations. We introduced new areas of exploration and disseminated new points of view on the potentially negative impacts of IT use. We drew upon our collective research and practice-related insights in five areas that characterize the dark side of IT use, namely-IT-usage-related stress, work overload, interruptions, addiction, and misuse. These are clearly important areas to examine, given that the ubiquitous and functionally pervasive nature of IT use in organizations is expected to expose users to ever greater levels of conditions that are potent for experiencing this dark side. We discussed the relevance and implications of the topic to the IS research and practice communities.
Article
Full-text available
Article
Technology addiction is a relatively new mental condition that has not yet been well integrated into mainstream MIS models. This study bridges this gap and incorporates technology addiction into technology use processes in the context of online auctions. It examines how user cognition and ultimately usage intentions toward an information technology are distorted by addiction to the technology. The findings from two empirical studies of 132 and 223 eBay users, using three different operationalizations of addiction, indicate that the level of online auction addiction distorts the way the IT artifact is perceived. Informing a range of cognitionmodification processes, addiction to online auctions augments user perceptions of enjoyment, usefulness, and ease of use attributed to the technology, which in turn influence usage intentions. Overall, consistent with behavioral addiction models, the findings indicate that users ' levels of online auction addiction influence their reasoned IT usage decisions by altering users ' belief systems. The formation of maladaptive perceptions is driven by a combination of memory-, learning-, and bias-based cognition modification processes. Implications of the findings are discussed.
Article
This paper presents a biopsychological theory of drug addiction, the 'Incentive-Sensitization Theory'. The theory addresses three fundamental questions. The first is: why do addicts crave drugs? That is, what is the psychological and neurobiological basis of drug craving? The second is: why does drug craving persist even after long periods of abstinence? The third is whether 'wanting' drugs (drug craving) is attributable to 'liking' drugs (to the subjective pleasurable effects of drugs)? The theory posits the following. (1) Addictive drugs share the ability to enhance mesotelencephalic dopamine neurotransmission. (2) One psychological function of this neural system is to attribute 'incentive salience' to the perception and mental representation of events associated with activation of the system. Incentive salience is a psychological process that transforms the perception of stimuli, imbuing them with salience, making them attractive, 'wanted', incentive stimuli. (3) In some individuals the repeated use of addictive drugs produces incremental neuroadaptations in this neural system, rendering it increasingly and perhaps permanently, hypersensitive ('sensitized') to drugs and drug-associated stimuli. The sensitization of dopamine systems is gated by associative learning, which causes excessive incentive salience to be attributed to the act of drug taking and to stimuli associated with drug taking. It is specifically the sensitization of incentive salience, therefore, that transforms ordinary 'wanting' into excessive drug craving. (4) It is further proposed that sensitization of the neural systems responsible for incentive salience ('for wanting') can occur independently of changes in neural systems that mediate the subjective pleasurable effects of drugs (drug 'liking') and of neural systems that mediate withdrawal. Thus, sensitization of incentive salience can produce addictive behavior (compulsive drug seeking and drug taking) even if the expectation of drug pleasure or the aversive properties of withdrawal are diminished and even in the face of strong disincentives, including the loss of reputation, job, home and family. We review evidence for this view of addiction and discuss its implications for understanding the psychology and neurobiology of addiction.
Article
This study investigated the prevalence of Internet addiction among South Korean adolescents and explored family factors associated with such addiction. The study participants were middle and high school students residing in Seoul. One-tenth (10.7%) of the 903 adolescents surveyed scored at least 70 on the Internet Addiction Scale. These youths were considered at high risk for Internet addiction and in need of further assessment and intervention. Results show that parenting attitudes, family communication, family cohesion, and family violence exposure (e.g., conjugal violence and parent-to-child violence) were associated with Internet addiction. These findings indicate that families play an important role in preventing Internet addiction and must be considered when programs are developed to minimize excessive Internet usage by high-risk adolescents.