Content uploaded by Anya Skatova
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Anya Skatova on Dec 04, 2014
Content may be subject to copyright.
Data Donation: Sharing Personal Data for Public Good?
Anya Skatova
Horizon Digital Economy Research,
University of Nottingham,
Jubilee Campus, NG8 1BB
anya.skatova@nottingham.ac.uk
Esther Ng
Horizon Centre for Doctoral Training,
University of Nottingham
Jubilee Campus, NG8 1BB
esther.ng@nottingham.ac.uk
James Goulding
Horizon Centre for Doctoral Training,
University of Nottingham
Jubilee Campus, NG8 1BB
james.goulding@nottingham.ac.uk
ABSTRACT
With the advancement of digital technology, large amounts of
our personal data is being recorded and retained by third parties,
constituting an invaluable asset to both governmental and
private organizations. Nonetheless, there is now increasing
interest in whether such data might also generate public good.
Could it be used to illicit environmental consequences upon
behaviour? Could it determine the contribution of nutritional
effects upon illnesses such as asthma and diabetes? Before we
can address such questions, we must first understand what
motivations (if any) would underpin people’s donation of
personal data. To this end, we present the results of two online
studies. We isolate two distinct factors relating to motivations to
donate personal data, the opportunity to achieve self-benefit and
prosocial concern for others, resulting in two distinct
behavioural groups. We further provide evidence for the strong
effect that public reception of the “recipient” research
organization has on such decisions, and discuss the implications
of these results in terms of data sharing practices.
Keywords
Personal Data, Prosocial Behaviour, Donation, Public Good.
1. INTRODUCTION
In the new global digital economy, personal data is increasingly
being recognized as an invaluable business asset. Although there
exists research focusing on what value people place on personal
data (Skatova et al, 2013), we know very little about peoples’
motivations to share such data freely for purposes of public
good. Would individuals would be willing to risk donating their
personal data for prosocial purposes, even when such a decision
might potentially jeopardize their privacy? Are their cases where
such concerns might be outweighed by potential benefits to
society (e.g. supermarket loyalty card data being donated along
with customers’ self-reports of health issues, in order to uncover
hitherto unknown nutritional patterns associated with various
diseases). In this paper we examine these issues, investigating
what factors might motivate, or preclude, the decision to donate
personal data to socially beneficial research causes.
2. BACKGROUND
Prosocial behaviour is an umbrella term used to describe actions
undertaken to benefit other people or society as a whole
(Schwartz & Bilsky, 1990). It includes volunteer work (Foster et
al, 2001), money (Frey & Meier, 2004) or blood donation
(Piliavin & Callero, 1991), and helpful interventions (Batson,
1987). Given the high frequency of prosocial behaviours in our
society, extensive research has explored the individuals’
motivation to benefit others (Weinstein & Ryan, 2010) and it
has been demonstrated that, apart from various objective factors
such as person’s income or attitude to specific cause, people are
highly motivated both by the desire to help others (e.g. “by
donating blood I will contribute to the good of society”) and to
receive direct benefit (e.g. “by donating blood I will get days off
work”) (Evans & Ferguson, 2014). Similar factors may well
influence an individual’s willingness to donate personal data.
Such differences in motivations will have large implications for
any campaigns that encourage the sharing of personal data for
social good. Research has shown that those induced toward a
communal orientation are more likely to help (Clark et al, 1986)
and pay more attention to recipients’ needs, resulting in greater
benefits to both parties (Clark et al, 1987). However, it is not
currently clear whether these conclusions can also be applied to
prosocial behaviour in data donation. Via two initial
investigatory studies, we examined people’s dispositions to the
sharing of personal data for purposes of public good, and
investigate the motivations underpinning those decisions.
3. STUDY 1: Donators’ Motivations
Our first study focused on an examination of potential drivers
for personal data donation, and the issues that might preclude
such behaviour. 125 volunteers were recruited via departmental
mailing lists and word of mouth to complete an online survey.
Participants were incentivized by £20 prize draw, with the
survey being distributed via the Qualtrics platform. The mean
age of respondents was 29.32 (21-57) years old; 72% - female,
28% - Male. Of these 14% said they were not familiar with the
concept of personal data is, with 65% reporting the converse.
34% reported dealing with other people’s personal data at work
in some form, and 4% consider themselves experts in the area.
Participants were first supplied with a paragraph describing how
retail transactional data might be recorded, with the possibility
then being raised that such data could be donated to charitable or
research organizations (e.g. Cancer Research UK, to facilitate
research of illness/disease and thus contribute to the public
good). Participants were subsequently asked to rate how likely
they would be to consent to such a request using a seven point
scale ranging from unlikely to likely, with additional option to
comment. Participants were next presented with 37 questions,
each containing a statement reflecting a reason to donate/not
donate personal data (e.g. I would donate data to charity/health
organizations: due to genuine concern about social issues; if
research would benefit a family member; based on how it would
be used.). Questions were based on previous literature on
prosocial behavior (e.g., Evans & Ferguson, 2014), and
participants were instructed to rate how strongly they agree or
disagree using a five point Likert scale.
3.1 Study 1 Results
60% of participants stated that they would be willing to donate
transactional personal data to research if it could lead to public
good, with only 25% stating that they would not (see figure 1 for
the distribution of results). While these results strongly
evidenced prosocial data sharing behaviour, 90 participants also
chose to comment on this decision. The majority of these were
justifications of positive responses (e.g. “I see no reason to
object to this data being used, as long as it is anonymized” / “all
manner of things are already being done with my shopping
data... it can’t hurt to contribute to public good”). Several
participants, however, provided clear qualification that decisions
were dependent on how data would be treated (e.g., “This
depends on who was leading data collection and their policies”
and “this would depend on the research involved”).
We then applied Exploratory Factor Analysis (Fabrigar et al,
1999) to the 37 motivational items, uncovering that individuals
appeared willing to donate personal data for 2 distinct reasons:
(i) concern about others or society and (ii) concern about
personal benefits (e.g. tokens and reputation). Results showed
that the higher the concern an individual declared for others, the
more likely they were to donate (see Figure 2). On the contrary,
the self-benefit motivation was stronger in those who stated low
likelihood to donate (see Figure 3). Projecting participant
responses onto the two exploratory factors also revealed two
distinct behavioural groups (see Figure 4): a group of
individuals with high prosocial motivation acting independently
of self-benefit, and a group with lower prosocial tendency
exhibiting a strong linear relationship with self-benefit
motivations. Experience of working with personal data did not
appear to affect such motivations or likelihood to donate.
4. STUDY 2: Donators’ Perceptions
An interesting output of participant comments from Study 1 was
that the perception of receiver organization was stated as an
important factor in the decision to donate personal data. Study 2
therefore examined how different “receiving” organizations
were perceived, concentrating on the attributes by which
individuals differentiated them most significantly. 65 volunteers
were recruited via departmental mailing lists and word of mouth
to fill in an online survey for no monetary incentive. The survey
was again distributed via Qualtrics software. The mean age was
32.25 years old, with a range of 21 to 67; 48% of participants
were female; 40% were born in UK and 50% have resided in
UK for more than 5 years. 12 items were presented to
participants to assess their perception of various organizations.
Items represented dimensions of trustworthiness (e.g. “I have
confidence and trust in this organization”) and familiarity (e.g.
“This organization is well-known”). Participants were requested
to rate 12 items using five-point Likert scale. Each participant
had to separately rate 3 randomly chosen organizations out of
list of 14 (e.g., Cancer Research, Experian, UK Government,
etc. - see Figure 5 for the full list), using the set of twelve items.
4.1 Study 2 Results
Applying factor analysis on the twelve items demonstrated that
we only needed two dimensions (which we were able to
characterize as “familiarity” and “trustworthiness”) to succinctly
explain how people differentiated between organizations. We
found that some organizations were rated as both “trustworthy
and well-known” (e.g. Cancer Research UK), while others were
“well-known but untrustworthy” (e.g., UK Government) or
“trustworthy but not well-known” (e.g., Institute of Food
Research UK). Finally, there were organizations, which were
rated as not well-known and not trustworthy (e.g., Mintel
Marketing Research Company; see Figure 3).
5. CONCLUSIONS
The main contributions of this paper were to demonstrate that
(a) a majority of respondents were willing to donate personal
data to research intended to lead to public good, and (b)
participants associated their motivations to donate their personal
data with self-benefit and concern for others. These motivations
were associated with the likelihood to donate: those who were
likely to donate made these decisions mostly in order to help
others; participants who were less likely to donate were
motivated by guaranteed direct personal benefits. Our results
also showed that the way people perceived the “recipient”
research organization that would analyze the donated data was
key to their decision to donate. In Study 2 we explored public
perceptions of different organizations and demonstrated that two
aspects were the most relevant: the trustworthiness and
familiarity of the organization.
Our findings would appear to find that the concept of Data
Donation holds promise as a useful tool in digital economy,
providing value to third sector and well-being researchers as
well as marketing and the private sector. We further
demonstrated that motivation to donate personal data for public
good match those identified in previous research on prosocial
behavior in domains such as blood donation (Evans and
Ferguson, 2014), as well as with more generic motivational
constructs (Skatova, 2011). We suggest that self-benefit and
other-regarding dimensions of motivation should be accounted
for when prompting people to provide their personal data for
public good. Future research could look into whether the way
organizations are perceived (e.g., high on trust and familiarity
versus low on trust and familiarity) will influence the likelihood
to donate personal data and the reasons behind it.
6. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This work was funded by the RCUK Horizon Digital Economy
Research Hub grant, EP/G065802/1.
7. REFERENCES
Batson, C. D. (1990). How social an animal? The human
capacity for caring.American Psychologist, 45(3), 336.
Clark, M. S., Mills, J., & Powell, M. C. (1986). Keeping track of
needs in communal and exchange relationships. Journal of
personality and social psychology, 51(2), 333.
Clark, M. S., Oullette, R., Powell, M. C., & Milberg, S. (1987).
Recipient's mood, relationship type, and helping. Journal of
personality and social psychology, 53(1), 94.
Evans, R., & Ferguson, E. (2014). Defining and measuring
blood donor altruism: a theoretical approach from biology,
economics and psychology. Vox sanguinis, 106(2), 118-126.
Fabrigar, L. R., Wegener, D. T., MacCallum, R. C., & Strahan,
E. J. (1999). Evaluating the use of exploratory factor analysis in
psychological research. Psychological methods, 4(3), 272.
Foster, V., Mourato, S., Pearce, D. W., & Ozdemiroglu, E.
(2001). The Price of Virtue: The Economic Value of Charities.
Frey, B. S., & Meier, S. (2004). Social comparisons and pro-
social behavior: Testing" conditional cooperation" in a field
experiment. American Economic Review, 1717-1722.Piliavin, J.
A., & Callero, P. L. (1991). Giving blood: The development of
an altruistic identity. Johns Hopkins University Press.
Schwartz, S. H., & Bilsky, W. (1990). Toward a theory of the
universal content and structure of values: Extensions and cross-
cultural replications. Journal of personality and social
psychology, 58(5), 878.
Skatova, A., Johal, J., Houghton, R., Mortier, R., Bhandari, N.,
Lodge, T., Wagenr, C., Goulding, J., Crowcroft, J., and
Madhavapeddy, A. 2013. Perceived risks for personal data
sharing. Perceived risks of personal data sharing. Proceedings of
Digital Engagement 2013, 11.
Skatova, A.A. (2011). Underpinnings of higher level
motivational orientations (PhD Thesis, University of
Nottingham).
Weinstein, N., & Ryan, R. M. (2010). When helping helps:
autonomous motivation for prosocial behavior and its influence
on well-being for the helper and recipient. Journal of personality
and social psychology, 98(2), 222.
8. FIGURES
Figure 1. Percentage of participants who are likely, unlikely and neither
likely nor unlikely to donate their personal data from loyalty cards to
research that may lead to public good
Figure 2. Likelihood to donate personal data versus concern for others.
Figure 3. Likelihood to donate versus desire for direct benefits.
Figure 4. Cluster Analysis of the two explanatory factors underpinning
motivation to donate, illustrating distinct motivational groups.. Blue dots
represent individuals with a low willingness to donate (3 or less); Red
dots individuals with a high willingness to donate (5or more).
Figure 5. Perception of different organizations on trustworthiness and
familiarity: (1) Cancer Research UK; (2) British Heart Foundation; (3)
Alzheimer’s Society; (4) Diabetes Foundation UK; (5) Defense Science
and Technology Laboratory UK; (6) National Institute for Medical
Research, UK; (7) Department for Business Innovation and Skills, UK;
(8) Institute of Food Research UK; (9) School of Medicine, University
of Nottingham; (10) Mintel Marketing Research Company; (11)
International Institute for Environment and Development; (12) Experian
Plc; (13) UK Government; (14) National Health Service (NHS).