ArticlePDF Available

Illuminating and Applying “The Dark Side”: Insights From Elite Team Leaders

Taylor & Francis
Journal of Applied Sport Psychology
Authors:

Abstract

In contrast to socially desirable behaviors, recent work has implied that effective elite team leadership also relies on socially undesirable behaviors. Accordingly, this study aimed to further explore the authenticity of dark side leadership behaviors, what they look like, and how they may be best used. Via interviews with 15 leaders, behaviors associated with Machiavellianism/mischievousness, skepticism, social dominance, and performance-focused ruthlessness were found. Moreover, these behaviors were enabled by leaders’ sociopolitical awareness and engineering as well as their adaptive expertise. Findings promote practitioner sensitivity to dark side leadership and, for leader effectiveness, sociopolitical and temporal features of its application.
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 1
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
Illuminating and Applying “The Dark Side: Insights from Elite Team Leaders
9
10
Andrew Cruickshank & Dave Collins
11
Institute of Coaching and Performance, University of Central Lancashire
12
13
Accepted author version for:
14
Cruickshank, A., & Collins, D. (2015). Illuminating and Applying “The Dark Side”: Insights
15
from Elite Team Leaders. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 27, 249-267.
16
17
18
19
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 2
Abstract
1
In contrast to socially desirable behaviors, recent work has suggested that effective elite team
2
leadership also relies on socially undesirable behaviors. Accordingly, this study aimed to
3
further explore the authenticity of dark side leadership behaviors, what they look like, and
4
how they may be best used. Via interviews with 15 leaders, behaviors associated with
5
Machiavellianism/mischievousness, skepticism, social dominance, and performance-focused
6
ruthlessness were found. Moreover, these behaviors were enabled by leaders’ sociopolitical
7
awareness and engineering as well as their adaptive expertise. Findings promote practitioner
8
sensitivity to dark side leadership and, for leader effectiveness, sociopolitical and temporal
9
features of its application.
10
Keywords: expertise, management, Olympic sport, performance sport, professional
11
sport
12
13
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 3
Illuminating and Applying “The Dark Side”: Insights from Elite Team Leaders
1
As a core performance construct, leadership has long been an area of interest for sport
2
psychologists. However, as most research has focused on coach leadership, and often within
3
non-elite settings, Fletcher and Arnold (2011) recently stated that “performance leadership at
4
the managerial level of [elite sport] organizations has been somewhat overlooked” (p. 223).
5
While much has still to be explored, one intriguing suggestion from associated work is that
6
leaders at this level use both bright (i.e., socially desirable) and dark (i.e., socially
7
undesirable) traits and behaviors to be effective (Bennie & O’Connor, 2012; Collins &
8
Cruickshank, 2012; Cruickshank, Collins, & Minten, 2014; Elberse & Dye, 2012; Fletcher &
9
Arnold). As the dark side of elite team leadership has yet to be explicitly explored, and wary
10
that some sport psychologists may be consulting against this issue with little supporting
11
evidence, research is required to elucidate the authenticity of dark side leadership, what it
12
looks like, and how it may be best deployed.
13
While Chelladurai’s (1980) multidimensional model has guided much work on sports
14
team leadershippromoting a focus on the interface between a leader, their followers, and the
15
contextresearchers have also explored a number of other perspectives. In the main, this
16
work has focused on leadership styles and the behavioral correlates of effective leadership.
17
Often adapted from organizational disciplines, examples include transactional, servant,
18
passive/avoidant, and paternalistic leadership (Chen, 2013; Rowold, 2006; Lee, Kim, &
19
Kang, 2013; Rieke, Hammermeister, & Chase, 2008; Vidic & Burton, 2011). Based on its
20
standing within business and military fields (e.g., Bass, Avolio, Jung, & Berson, 2003;
21
Walumbwa, Avolio, & Zhu, 2008), however, interest has increasingly centered on
22
transformational leadership. With work on non-elite samples showing repeated links between
23
this style and valued outcomes (e.g., task cohesion; Callow, Smith, Hardy, Arthur, & Hardy,
24
2009), this body of inquiry has reinforced proposals that transformational behaviors are also
25
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 4
constructive for elite team leaders (Arthur, Hardy, & Woodman, 2012; Din & Paskevich,
1
2013). Raising doubt over its ability to fully explain and support success at this level,
2
however, additional recent work has suggested that inherently bright leadership styles, such
3
as the transformational approach, may need to be paired with an entirely different, if perhaps
4
complimentary, repertoire for optimal effectiveness; namely, that related to the dark side of
5
leadership (Bennie & O’Connor, 2012; Collins & Cruickshank, 2012; Cruickshank et al.,
6
2014; Elberse & Dye, 2012; Fletcher & Arnold, 2011).
7
While implicated by others previously (e.g., Conger, 1990), a seminal perspective on
8
the dark side of leadership was provided in the personality based work of Hogan and Hogan
9
(2001). Advancing beyond the most widely researched traits of the time (i.e., the dark triad
10
of Machiavellianism, psychopathy, and narcissism; Paulhus & Williams, 2002) these authors
11
generated and validated a measure of the dysfunctional dispositions known to be associated
12
with failure as a manager [in an organization] (p. 40). Taking the disorders of the
13
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual IV as their basis (American Psychiatric Association, 1994),
14
Hogan and Hogan extrapolated parallel but subclinical traits on which to evaluate the
15
working population. These traits, with DSM IV analog in brackets, are excitable
16
(borderline), skeptical (paranoid), cautious (avoidant), reserved (schizoid), leisurely (passive-
17
aggressive), bold (narcissistic), mischievous (antisocial), colorful (histrionic), imaginative
18
(schizotypal), diligent (obsessive-compulsive), and dutiful (dependent). As the middle
19
ground between “normal” (e.g., Big Five) and pathological traits (Paulhus & Willaims,
20
2002), Harms, Spain, and Hannah (2011) have stated that one might consider them
21
personality quirks that do not greatly inhibit day-to-day functioning, but [that] may cause
22
severely negative outcomes in particular circumstances; such as during leadership social
23
interactions (p. 496). However, while representing “a gallery of many psychological ills”
24
(Marshall, Baden, & Guidi, 2013, p. 560) and therefore often cited, Hogan and Hogan’s traits
25
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 5
are not the universal standard for classifying the dark side of personality in organizational
1
based work. For example, some continue to use more direct and publicly familiar termssuch
2
as the traits in the dark triad (e.g., Marshall et al., 2013)while a recent review of dark side
3
leader traits, which we encourage the interested reader to source, focused on hubris,
4
Narcissism, social dominance, and Machiavellianism (Judge, Piccolo, & Kosalka, 2009).
5
Indeed, this ongoing conceptualization reflects Judge et al.’s (2009) earlier yet enduring point
6
that “dark side traits associated with leadership have been widely ignored [within research]
7
(p. 864).
8
However, despite variance in classification and limited empirical work on dark side
9
leadership, organizational scholars do widely agree that dark side traits are socially
10
undesirable and “viewed negatively by most individuals in society” (Judge et al., 2009, p.
11
864). Accordingly, when leaders possess high levels of these traits, it is also widely agreed
12
that this hinders long term group performance, as well as their own longevity (Carson,
13
Shanock, Heggestad, Andrew, Pugh, & Walter, 2012; Hogan & Kaiser, 2005). For example,
14
Blair, Hoffman, and Helland (2008) discovered that narcissism in a range of organization-
15
based managers was negatively associated with their interpersonal effectiveness and integrity
16
(as rated by their supervisors). Owen and Davidson (2009) have also identified links between
17
leader hubris and impaired risk appraisal, a failure to predict undesirable outcomes, and
18
dangerous decision making. Notwithstanding this consensus on the “dark side of dark traits”
19
(Judge et al., 2009, p. 866), however, socially undesirable aspects of personality are not just
20
apparent in destructive leadership. They also prevail, albeit in lesser concentrations, within
21
inherently constructive approaches. For instance, Davies (2004) found that transformational
22
leadership was linked to the dark side traits of colorful (i.e., wanting to be noticed and the
23
center of attention) and imaginative (i.e., acting and thinking in creative and at times odd or
24
unusual ways). On this basis, an exclusive reliance or overpromotion of transformational
25
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 6
behaviors may actually lead to a megalomania and narcissism that derails group performance
1
(Tourish, 2013).
2
In further contrast to the idea of uniformly negative influence, organizational scholars
3
also widely agree that dark side traits can in fact enhance group functioning and performance.
4
For example, relationships have been found between social dominance and willing followers,
5
hubris and innovation, Machiavellianism and legislative development, and Narcissism and
6
strategic dynamism (Judge et al., 2009). Diverging from the negative impact of consistently
7
demonstrated dark side traits, the suggestion from this work is that intermittent and
8
contextually suitable uses and displays can contribute to effective team leadership (Hogan &
9
Kaiser, 2005). For example, while the recurrent self-promotional acts of a highly narcissistic
10
leader may lead to eventual rejection or resistance from followers, transient acts of a
11
narcissistic nature at contextually suitable moments may optimize group performance.
12
Moreover, this contention parallels findings that bright traits can be ineffective in some
13
situations (Judge et al., 2009). Finally, it also suggests that the priority for practice oriented
14
study is not to explore general frequencies and levels of dark side traits in elite team leaders
15
but rather their dark side behaviors (i.e., socially undesirable actions or interactions engaged
16
in precise contexts). In short, exploring how leaders may usefully act in a dark way.
17
Given the importance of this point for the research presented here, we emphasize that
18
dark side traits are not the same as dark side behaviors. To be explicit, dark side traits reflect
19
stable dispositions and tendencies in personality that lead individuals to behave in a relatively
20
consistent and predictable manner across situations (i.e., so someone high in trait narcissism
21
will frequently act in a narcissistic way). Dark side behaviors, on the other hand, only may
22
be the live enactment of dark side traits (which may or may not be of high levels) but can also
23
be selectively developed and deployed (Pettersson et al., 2006; Yukl, 2006). By taking traits
24
as continuums rather than all or nothing characteristics, someone who does not possess high
25
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 7
levels of a dark side trait (e.g., a leader with low narcissism) is therefore still capable of
1
engaging behaviors which reflect this trait in certain circumstances; in fact, this behavior may
2
well be both functional and positive. In this way, our focus on dark side behaviors over dark
3
side traits in this study reflects a prime interest in what leaders do rather than who they are.
4
Additionally, the emphasis for us as practitioners would be on why or why not such behaviors
5
are used, and how this judgment can be developed for optimum impact (cf. Martindale &
6
Collins, 2010). For clarity, all references to dark side behaviors from here on thereby refer to
7
the overt manifestation of dark side traits; these behaviors are not necessarily the result of
8
high levels of dark side traits but rather a conscious choice to employ such methods, whether
9
for socially desirable or undesirable ends.
10
Given recent findings on leadership at elite sport’s managerial level (including
11
performance directors, team managers, and head coaches), this focus on dark side behaviors
12
seems timely. For example, it has been briefly stated that best practice as an Olympic sport
13
performance director requires acts that reflect the personality traits of Narcissism, hubris,
14
dominance, and Machiavellianism (Cruickshank et al., 2014; Fletcher & Arnold, 2011). The
15
use of dark side behavior within professional sport has also been noted (Bennie & O’Connor,
16
2012) and reinforced by former Manchester United FC manager Sir Alex Ferguson: “You
17
can’t ever lose control – not when you are dealing with thirty top professionals who are all
18
millionaires . . . . If anyone steps out of my control, that’s them dead (Elberse & Dye, 2012,
19
p. 11). Unfortunately, as none of this prior work aimed to explore the dark side of leadership,
20
with this idea only briefly cited or alluded to in discussion of results, understanding over what
21
dark side behaviors look like across different leadership scenarios is uncertain. Additionally,
22
almost no understanding has been gleaned on how leaders may use dark side behaviors to
23
achieve desired goals (e.g., to increase performer effort) but in a way that limits the chance of
24
derailment (as risked by using such socially undesirable behaviors).
25
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 8
Based on these identified gaps and calls within previous research, the purpose of this
1
study was to extend our knowledge on the use of dark side behaviors by those leading elite
2
sport performance teams (Fletcher & Arnold, 2011). Considered via the lens of team leaders
3
themselves, this work more specifically aimed to (a) unearth further possible evidence on the
4
use of dark side leadership behavior, (b) illuminate what these behaviors look like in a
5
common leadership task, and (c) explore how these behaviors may be used to attain desired
6
outcomes yet minimize the risk of leader derailment. The findings will offer further insight
7
on the potential need for practitioners to address bright and dark side behaviors with elite
8
team leaders, what some of these behaviors look like, and their supporting mechanisms.
9
Method
10
Research Strategy, Philosophy, and Design
11
Reflecting the explorative nature of this research, including its pursuit of detailed and
12
descriptive data on dark side leadership behaviors rather than measurement of dark side traits,
13
a qualitative methodology was adopted (Denzin & Lincoln, 2008). Given the apparent role of
14
context in shaping the use of dark side leadership behaviors, this approach also enabled us to
15
probe the details of particular cases and therefore support our intention to explore how these
16
behaviors, if present, were deployed (Denzin & Lincoln, 2008). Indeed, qualitative research
17
does not develop a correct map of the world but rather a useful one (Strean, 1998).
18
This approach also importantly aligned with our pragmatic research philosophy; an
19
approach which promotes research questions and methods that create practically meaningful
20
knowledge (Giacobbi, Poczwardowski, & Hager, 2005). Specifically, by collecting data on
21
leaders’ use of dark side behaviors during a common and practically meaningful process (see
22
below), our aim was to develop knowledge that reflected tangible applied artifacts rather than
23
generalizable “truths” (as positivists may seek) or representations of individually or socially
24
constructed reality (as constructivists may seek). By rejecting the notion of one observable,
25
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 9
measurable, and correct reality, as well as the belief that no research finding can be more or
1
less accurate than another (as per the respective tenets of extreme positivism and relativism),
2
pragmatism also considers that researcher biases and prejudices can be used to support novel
3
and practically meaningful insights (cf. Bryant, 2009; Giacobbi et al., 2005; Morgan, 2007).
4
In this regard, interpretation and decision making processes within this study were supported
5
by our own experiences of leading, assisting, and performing in elite team sport (cf. Corbin &
6
Strauss, 2008; Giacobbi et al., 2005). Although pragmatism has often underpinned mixed
7
methods research (Culver, Gilbert, & Sparkes, 2012), its combination with a purely
8
qualitative approach for this inquiry was deemed apt given our explorative aims and others’
9
prior use of this pairing (Cruickshank et al., 2014; Gould, Collins, Lauer, & Chung, 2007).
10
Heeding Fletcher and Arnold’s (2011) call for researchers to assess “what [elite team]
11
leaders do . . . in specific contexts and situations (p. 237), as well as the need to explore dark
12
side leadership behavior in specific moments, leader succession was chosen as the practically
13
meaningful process to frame participant interviews (i.e., the period when a newly appointed
14
leader takes over the running and performance of an elite team’s performance department).
15
Beyond providing a contextual backdrop for this study, it was also chosen as the culture
16
change role of incoming elite team leaders has become an area of theoretical and applied
17
interest for sport psychologists (Cruickshank & Collins, 2012; Fletcher & Arnold, 2011).
18
Participants
19
Given the difficulty of acquiring insights from leaders at the managerial level of elite
20
teams, and reflecting the pragmatics of research (Buchanan & Bryman, 2007), a convenience
21
sample was secured by using participants who had agreed to take part in a broader study that
22
was being undertaken by the authors (of which the present study was one part). This sample
23
consisted of 15 leaders: eight were managers or head coaches of UK based professional teams
24
(four from Premiership and Championship soccer, two from Premiership rugby union, and
25
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 10
two from Super League rugby league) and seven were performance directors of UK Olympic
1
sports teams (one from an individual sport, one from a team sport, and five from individual
2
plus team sports). Adhering to the pragmatic approach (cf. Corbin & Strauss, 2008; Giacobbi
3
et al., 2005), leaders for the wider study were selected on the basis that the overall sample
4
should reflect a range of experiences (i.e., a mix of short and long tenures as well as little to
5
major objective success achieved). Such a basis also acknowledged that simply operating in
6
high performance sport does not necessarily make a leader high performing.
7
Although there are differences between professional and Olympic sport, leaders were
8
sampled from both domains given that this was a first exploration into dark side behavior at
9
the managerial level of elite teams and that prior research has suggested that such behavior is
10
used in both settings (Bennie & O’Connor, 2012; Collins & Cruickshank, 2012; Cruickshank
11
et al., 2014; Elberse & Dye, 2012; Fletcher & Arnold, 2011). Additionally, similarities exist
12
in the challenges of leading change in both environments. For example, leaders must handle
13
powerful performers, the politics of a multidiscipline staff, the regularly used power of results
14
oriented boards (or oligarch owners), and a subversive media (Cruickshank & Collins, 2012).
15
All interviewees were male, aged between 37 and 59 years old (M = 50.07 years, SD
16
= 6.93 years), and had held elite sport leadership roles (e.g., manager, head coach, assistant
17
manager or head coach, and performance director) for a total of 147 years (M = 9.80 years,
18
SD = 4.26 years). As a manager, head coach, or performance director, participants had led
19
teams to 13 major professional team titles and 33 Olympic medals. The shortest and longest
20
tenures ranged from 6 months to 21 years. Eight interviewees were employed in roles at the
21
time of interview (four as managers of professional teams and four as Olympic performance
22
directors). The other seven had all been managers or head coaches of professional sides up to
23
one year prior, or performance directors in the two cycles prior to the London Games (2004
24
to 2012). To clarify, while some participants were not currently employed as a manager,
25
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 11
head coach, or performance director at the time of interview, data were only collected on
1
their time working in these specific roles (i.e., no information was collected or analyzed
2
regarding the actions or experiences of these individuals’ within their current or any other
3
previous unrelated roles). Furthermore, at the time of writing, all participants are now
4
currently employed in senior leadership roles either in the UK or abroad. The first author
5
conducted all interviews and had no prior relationship with any participant.
6
Procedure
7
Interviews were conducted using a semistructured guide that was mutually developed
8
for the broader research project described above and piloted with three elite team managers.
1
9
While the main questions of the guide were not changed by this pilot work, follow-up probes
10
were refined to ensure that the main interviews would effectively meet the aims of this study.
11
For example, reflections of the first author (who conducted all interviews) and feedback from
12
the pilot group were used to refine follow-up probes that explored the social conditions under
13
which dark side behaviors were optimally engaged and the rationale that underpinned the
14
deployment of dark side behavior. Interviews with the main participant pool focused on
15
seven open-ended questions covering the interviewees’ initial goals when joining a new team,
16
prechange steps, processes and actions for initiating changes, processes and actions for
17
driving changes through, clarification of any critical personal attributes and skills, processes
18
and actions for evaluating the impact of the changes, and reflections on the ultimate success
19
or failure of their actions. As such, while none of the main questions focused specifically on
20
dark side behavior, they provided a contextually specific frame against which any naturally
21
occurring cases could be elicited and explored. As prior work had only briefly noted dark
22
side behavior in elite team leaders, this approach was deemed a logical first step in exploring
23
the applied relevance of this construct and for enabling an evolution of knowledge (Giacobbi
24
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 12
et al., 2005). All interviews were conducted at locations which were convenient to
1
participants, recorded, and lasted between 110 and 165 minutes. Institutional ethics approval
2
was granted, confidentiality assured, and informed consent provided by all participants.
3
Data Analysis
4
As the collected data were grounded in leaders’ experiences rather than preexisting
5
theory (cf. structure of interview guide), transcribed perceptions were appropriate for both
6
deductive and inductive analyses. Specifically, to reveal what dark side behaviors were used
7
(and deemed useful) by the participants, qualitative analysis software (QSR NVIVO 9) was
8
firstly used to convert relevant quotes into raw data units (i.e., complete sentences that
9
referred to or implicated dark side behaviors). These raw data units were then deductively
10
categorized against the behavioral manifestations of the dark side traits described in Hogan
11
and Hogan (2001) and Judge et al. (2009). These were excitable, skeptical, cautious,
12
reserved, leisurely, bold, mischievous, colorful, imaginative, diligent, and dutiful (as taken
13
from Hogan & Hogan, 2001), and hubris, Narcissism, social dominance, and
14
Machiavellianism (as taken from Judge et al., 2009).
2
These classifications were selected due
15
to their seminal nature and broad integration of prior knowledge respectively. We remind the
16
reader that while these classifications reflect personality traits, this does not necessarily mean
17
that the participants in this study would score highly on these traits if measured. Rather, they
18
simply demonstrate the use of socially undesirable behaviors in specific scenarios.
19
To meet our third aim, an entirely separate inductive content analysis was undertaken
20
to elucidate how dark side behaviors were effectively used according to the participants. This
21
process involved the transformation of raw data units into thematic hierarchies by creating
22
tags, grouping similar tags to create categories, and then organizing categories into a distinct
23
framework (Côté, Salmela, Baria, & Russell, 1993).
24
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 13
Addressing Trustworthiness
1
As trust and rapport shape the process and outcomes of interviews (Sparkes & Smith,
2
2009), these components were enhanced by: (a) gaining an understanding of all participants’
3
careers to demonstrate an understanding of their history, achievements, and challenges; and
4
(b) our awareness of the challenges of elite sport through our previous and continued roles in
5
this setting. The resulting high level of trust and rapport was evidenced by a mean interview
6
length of 142 minutes. Addressing the trustworthiness of the deductive analysis process, the
7
second author examined samples of meaning units from each leader against the traits to
8
which they had been coded by the first author. In cases where alternate coding was proposed,
9
both authors engaged in critical discussion until an agreement was reached. For the inductive
10
analysis process, constant comparison was used to repeatedly evaluate, modify, and reinforce
11
developing tags and categories (Corbin & Strauss, 2008). Additionally, transparency of this
12
process was enhanced through the use of qualitative analysis software (QSR NVIVO 9). In
13
particular, conceptual memos enabled a trail of the rationale underpinning (and challenging)
14
the first author’s interpretation (Davis & Meyer, 2009). The first author also kept a reflexive
15
journal which provided opportunities for reflection on the research process and how personal
16
experiences and biases were interacting with the interpretation and categorization of the data
17
(Patton, 2002). Additionally, the second author acted as a critical friend by questioning and
18
challenging the first author’s developing discoveries and explanations (Faulkner & Sparkes,
19
1999). Finally, member checks were conducted over telephone and email to assess the extent
20
to which participants perceived that their quotes used in this paper were “accurate, balanced,
21
fair, and respectful” (Sparkes & Smith, 2009, p. 495). No quotes, or the positions of quotes,
22
were changed by this process.
23
Results
24
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 14
The aims of this work were to (a) unearth possible further evidence on the use of dark
1
side leadership behaviors by those at the managerial level of elite teams, (b) illuminate what
2
these behaviors look like in a common leadership task, and (c) explore how these behaviors
3
were used to achieve desired outcomes yet minimize the risk of derailment. To address the
4
first two of these aims, the dark side behaviors described by elite team leaders are presented
5
first. The way in which these behaviors were used is presented second. Supporting quotes
6
from leaders of professional teams are indicated by PM and those of Olympic teams by
7
PD. Table 1 details the type of team led by each participant.
8
Exemplar Dark Side Leadership Behaviors Used During the Delivery of Change
9
Machiavellian/Mischievous behaviors. Given the contested and political nature of
10
elite team settings, Machiavellian (as per Judge et al., 2009) or mischievous behaviors (as per
11
Hogan & Hogan, 2001; including “manipulative, deceitful, cunning, and exploitative” acts)
12
were evident across all participants’ accounts. Including tactics to shape interpersonal and
13
political relationships in a way that furthered the team’s interests and performance, one PD
14
provided a telling insight on how they used Machiavellian behaviors to shape the focus of
15
their system:
16
[To instigate a] focus on [new event] I put up [in a presentation] what it would take to
17
. . . medal [in historically targeted event] and . . . 95% of [the performers] said, “Well,
18
I can’t do that!” So [new event] made sense because . . . it was achievable . . . . It was
19
clever, I didn’t say “you’re going to do [new event] . . . [but I knew] very well at that
20
point . . . no one was going to . . . medal in [historically targeted event] . . . . What I
21
was trying to do was also say: “Coaches; you need to step up. You have to show that
22
you’re raising the game, because . . . an athlete will leave you if they’ve got a bigger
23
goal than you [i.e., medal success in new event].” (PD5)
24
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 15
As conveyed by this quote, the ability to strategically manipulate as well as directly address,
1
challenge, or confront the choices made by powerful performers and staff was perceived to be
2
a vital behavior for elite team leaders to call upon.
3
From a psychosocial perspective, participants also discussed the importance of being
4
able to manipulate performer and support staff decision making through the construction and
5
reconstruction of “normal” behaviors. For example, take the actions of one incoming leader
6
who wanted his players to engage with new early morning conditioning sessions:
7
[The change] came . . . at the same time as Ryan Giggs [yoga] DVD . . . . [So I just]
8
made a point of, “have you seen this? . . . Three or four players [then] came to see
9
my analyst . . . and he got the DVD . . . [Then when] even the most ardent of [cynics]
10
. . . sees four then six or seven people doing it . . . and getting results, ultimately
11
something is going to click . . . . So from having three or four in the gym before
12
training all of a sudden eight or nine were in [without any overt demand]. (PM6)
13
While this quote seems to resemble the transformational behavior of inspirational motivation,
14
it is important to note that the leader in this instance was not actively developing, articulating,
15
or inspiring their vision for the future through positive and reassuring dialogue (cf. Bass,
16
1985; Callow et al., 2009) but rather encouraging behavior change via an inherently darker
17
manipulation of group norms. Clarifying this as a socially undesirable act, consider the
18
perceptions of the performers had known that they were being manipulated. Focused again
19
on performer manipulation, another leader described a more overt approach to facilitating
20
acceptance and uptake of new behaviors:
21
It’s a matter of the subtle sell to tell you the truth . . . In a team sport players are quite
22
selfish, they look after themselves . . . I felt that the way for them to get ownership
23
[and prioritize the group] was for them to feel that they are being consulted. Now,
24
your degree of consultation might vary from group to group but they have got to feel
25
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 16
that they have had some input. In all honesty they don’t because you can sell them a
1
certain bias, but if they feel they’ve been consulted they are more receptive than
2
otherwise. (PM4)
3
Interestingly, another interviewee also noted how peer-review processes were introduced and
4
discreetly manipulated to expose and consequently suppress particularly troublesome
5
performers:
6
[Each] player would . . . fill a sheet in . . . [with] three words [on] how you think the
7
group would describe you, three words how you want to be described, and then what
8
. . . you need to stop doing, start doing, and keep doing . . . . The group would [then]
9
directly deliver feedback [to the player on their perceptions] . . . . This became pretty
10
uncomfortable for some . . . . It was like the bullies being bullied. (PM2)
11
Skeptical behaviors. Evident in those who are “cynical, distrustful, and doubting
12
others’ true intentions” (Hogan & Hogan, 2001, p. 42), the need for behaviors that reflected
13
skepticism when leading elite teams was also revealed. Given the high potential for support
14
staff conflict and the need to interact with a powerful performer group, a level of suspicion
15
over others’ actions was described by many interviewees. This was particularly the case for
16
those closest to the leader:
17
[For] developing a good team . . . you can start through the [senior players] . . . . With
18
some senior players [however] . . . it’s your old enemy isn’t it; keep your friends close
19
but . . . keep your bloody enemies even closer! . . . [Failing to do so] may well have
20
been a mistake that I made at [previous team] and perhaps is something that I’ve
21
learned from. (PM4)
22
Of note here, it is telling that this leader had learned to behave in a more skeptical manner;
23
therefore reinforcing our earlier suggestion that individuals do not need to have high levels of
24
dark side traits to act in a dark manner. Additionally, it is also interesting to note that it was
25
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 17
the absence of a dark side behavior (i.e., doubting and protecting oneself from the potentially
1
ulterior motives of influential performers) that was deemed detrimental to this leader’s
2
effectiveness and survival. Indeed, one highly successful PD described how skeptical
3
behaviors pervaded their approach to managing their support staff:
4
We have a formal feedback process after every championship . . . [where] each
5
performance manager will . . . [conduct] one-on-ones with all staff, all [performers],
6
and [external partners], and then I also speak to a few people and just check that what
7
I’m getting from the performance managers is au fait [emphasis added]. (PD1)
8
Again, the “darkness” of this behavior lies in the view that the performance managers would
9
likely hold if they knew the PD was speaking to others to verify their feedback without their
10
awareness. Reflecting their ability to constrain and even control a leader’s actions, behaviors
11
of a skeptical nature were further evident in scenarios relating to powerful board members:
12
[A key board figure] left [n] months ago and . . . one of the things that we did . . . was
13
to say [to our funding agency] that there is actually now a real risk of somebody new
14
coming in halfway through the Olympic cycle and throwing everything upside down .
15
. . [We were] very upfront . . . . Actually almost making a preemptive strike. (PD2)
16
While highlighting this PD’s cynicism and doubts over the possible intentions of an incoming
17
powerful board member, this quote also effectively shows how dark side behaviors could be
18
combined to achieve multiple goals simultaneously; in this case, pairing actions of a skeptical
19
nature with the manipulation of a powerful external partner so that the PD would be more
20
likely to be favored by this body in any potential struggle. Again, a lack of skepticism-
21
related behavior in one professional team manager - when they were being provided with
22
little insight on board-level plans and reviews - was felt to be a key factor in the eventual
23
derailment of their culture change program:
24
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 18
I think at times [not having sufficient doubts over a lack of communication with the
1
CEO] was detrimental to understanding where we were trying to be because there
2
were cases where we were . . . getting on with . . . a really important part of our . . .
3
plan only to learn that there was some real hardship behind the scenes (PM1)
4
Social dominance behaviors. Evident in preferences for hierarchy, achievement, and
5
control, as well as projection as a highly and consistently competent figure, interviewees also
6
described the importance of socially dominant behavior when attempting to establish and
7
sustain a high performing team (Judge et al., 2009). For instance, many participants noted
8
the importance of ensuring that support staff did not deviate from the leader’s agenda once
9
this had been set:
10
We had a few issues with [performance lifestyle staff] . . . because all the time they’re
11
trying to get our athletes on college courses . . . [during] core time. [I said], “these
12
guys need to train, the Olympics is here, stop ******* off with them!” . . . . It’s as
13
hard as that because all the time I knew [nation] were training here and doing this
14
[type of work] and we were ******** around giving them this and this to do. (PD3).
15
In a further notable example, another participant pointed to the expression of ultimate control
16
by even exerting influence over players away from the team setting:
17
I find out what players are doing off the pitch. I went to some places in [city] where
18
[X] was drinking and said to the [owner], “if you let [X] back in here . . . I’ll ban all
19
the players from coming in here.” And he tells me when he’s been in. (PM5)
20
Corroborating these quotes, another PD reflected that they may have achieved greater success
21
had they been more dominant, in this case over negotiations with an external support partner:
22
All of a sudden I said [to the English Institute of Sport], “I don’t want to be . . . in
23
[region] anymore.” “Oh, what will we do with our Centre?” “I don’t ******* care . .
24
. . I don’t want the service done like this” . . . . What I should have done I think was to
25
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 19
have been even more bloody . . . instead of being, [as I eventually was], slightly more
1
corporate and going, “well ok, we can adjust this and do that.” (PD4)
2
Interestingly, some professional team managers who had intentionally refrained from acts of
3
social dominance in their first jobs reflected that such behavior, at contextually apt moments,
4
would also have significantly increased their chances of role survival and team success:
5
Sometimes you don’t want [players] to question everything, you just want them to do
6
what we want them to do because we are the ones that have spent the hours looking at
7
film and deciding the best way [to approach a game]. . . . There might have been
8
times where we really encouraged their participation . . . but we probably just needed
9
to tell them [in other instances], this is the way we are doing it; end of!” (PM2)
10
Performance-focused ruthlessness behaviors. Extending an idea briefly noted in
11
Collins and Cruickshank (2012), perceptions were provided on socially undesirable acts
12
which did not fit neatly with the traits of Hogan and Hogan (2001) and Judge et al. (2009).
13
Specifically, and despite links with Hogan and Hogan’s reserved (i.e., aloof, detached,
14
uncommunicative) and Judge et al.’s social dominance (as above), leaders described their use
15
of behaviors that reflected a performance-focused ruthlessness. Part of a “no compromise”
16
take on certain challenges, this behavior did not relate to exerting command or influence (as
17
per social dominance: Judge et al.) or wholly dismissing the feelings of others (as per Hogan
18
& Hogan’s reserved), but rather to a robust promotion of the team’s vision, values, and
19
standards:
20
[I had to] tell a [n] times world champion their career is over . . . unless they change. .
21
. . [But] he’s troublesome . . . strong willed . . . [and] I can’t change him, so I am
22
going to have to face him down and . . . be prepared when he . . . disrupts my next
23
team meeting . . . to say, “seriously . . . if you don’t want to do this that’s not a
24
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 20
problem. There’s the door. Leave”. And maybe [other] people will walk behind him
1
and maybe they won’t . . . . It sounds a bit crude but it’s the only way. (PD6)
2
As shown here, while performers’ reputation and prior achievements may have influenced the
3
precise manner in which leaders enacted such cold bloodedness, these factors did not provide
4
immunity from failing to meet evolving standards; a point echoed by another participant who
5
stated that “you keep chipping away at [athletes who won’t engage] . . . but ultimately one of
6
two things will happen: they will perform and they’ll get selected; they won’t perform and we
7
kick them off the program (PD7).
8
Indeed, such a ruthless approach to protecting desired values and acceptable behaviors in the
9
performance environment was clear across participants; including the following manager who
10
experienced difficulties with challenging young performers:
11
[Young modern players are] moaning away behind the scenes, and that’s what you’re
12
going to get these days . . . . Society’s made it, for every young man or young person,
13
to have an excuse for failing; there is an excuse for everything now. There’s a
14
syndrome for people being lazy, rude. You come from a broken family? I don’t give
15
a **** . . . . I come from X; people getting stabbed and smacking each other so don’t
16
give me that ****. I’ve never been in trouble in my life, and all my mates have never
17
been in trouble with the police. (PM5)
18
Beyond application with performers, one highly successful PD also attested to the importance
19
of, when required, adopting similar behavior in interactions with support staff:
20
We had a physiotherapist who . . . couldn’t come [to Olympic training camp] for some
21
poxy reason . . . . [After further problems, I had to say], “sorry, you’re not committed:
22
you’re gone.” And it’s as simple as that. He had some personal problems, which is
23
fine, but we’re not prepared to bring that into the program (PD3)
24
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 21
Recognized by another particularly successful PD, such apparently unforgiving actions were
1
at times deemed mandatory given the number and scale of external distractions which leaders
2
faced:
3
[At the start of my tenure] the . . . athletes and the coaches and the staff were all at
4
one and heading in the same direction, and if folk weren’t then I was fairly ruthless
5
that we weren’t going to be working together because there were too many challenges
6
outside to have people inside that didn’t see . . . where [program] was going. (PD7)
7
Deploying Dark Side Behavior: Mechanisms for Optimizing Impact and Minimizing
8
Risk
9
To address the third aim of this research, focus now turns to the way leaders used dark
10
side behaviors to achieve desired goals yet concurrently reduce the threat or scale of follower
11
suspicion, challenge, resistance, or rejection (as socially undesirable behaviors run the risk of
12
invoking). Specifically, findings from the inductive analysis revealed that interviewees used
13
two metaskills to facilitate their use of dark side leadership behavior; namely, sociopolitical
14
awareness and engineering and an adaptive expertise.
15
Sociopolitical awareness and engineering. As leading an elite team is a socially
16
complex and contested task, interviewees described how they relied on their awareness of and
17
ability to engineer beneficial social and political conditions when using dark side behaviors.
18
With links to Hogan and Hogan’s (2002) notion of sociopolitical intelligence, or the ability to
19
read reactions in others, this metaskill was frequently dark in its own right. For example, one
20
leader worked to establish early political support from influential players so that, when later
21
required, his dark side behavior would be largely supported by this key group (or deemed as
22
desirable or acceptable as it possibly could be):
23
When you take over . . . focus on three of the better, older players and . . . get them on
24
your side . . . . And then [when I had to take socially undesirable actions], “I’ve got a
25
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 22
problem with [X]; what am I going to do lads? . . . “Get rid of him” . . . . I was going
1
to do it anyway but it keeps them [onside]. They think they’re doing it. Then they . .
2
. spread it round the dressing room: The gaffer’s got it under control.” (PM5)
3
Beyond engineering internal contexts, this leader also noted how the impact of their dark side
4
leadership behavior was further enhanced by jointly engineering contexts outside of the team
5
environment. For instance, when internally exposing a group of older and influential players
6
who did not fit with his values, this individual worked to dampen any negative repercussions
7
(both internally and externally) by proactively and positively speaking about these players in
8
the media: “Oh, he’s doing great . . . he’s fantastic.
9
Further demonstrating the foundations needed to make the use of socially undesirable
10
behaviors less undesirable, and ideally acceptable, one PD also described how the power of a
11
key political ally was used to soften their (relatively) ruthless decision to introduce a new and
12
culturally controversial selection system:
13
We moved to a very subjective selection process because I knew who the best players
14
were and I wanted them at the Olympics . . . but that unfortunately [only agreed with]
15
a tiny minority . . . . [We had to] explain that [funding agency] was extremely anxious
16
about the [current one-off selection event so] I got a guy from [the funding agency to
17
say], “this is the [new] system . . . . Ask the questions now . . . [as] ultimately there is
18
a big degree of subjectivity” . . . . And you’d get two, three guys stand up and go, “I
19
think that’s ****. . . . [However], in the time I was there . . . plenty of them were
20
unhappy [when not selected] . . . but not one athlete made an appeal. (PD3)
21
Similarly, and albeit in lighter tone, another PD revealed how he engineered organizational
22
structures in a way that would ensure his authority but without overt displays of dominance:
23
It doesn’t take brains . . . to work out areas . . . we can tweak within [the program] . . .
24
. [So] instead of twelve of us getting round a table . . . I made up some working
25
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 23
groups with three or four people on each . . . [and asked them to] report back in three
1
to four months . . . . Because you never really decide anything do you [with twelve
2
people]. A good committee is three with one in hospital, one absentee and me! (PD1)
3
As a final example, another leader stated how their desired control over performance-related
4
issues without appropriate political sensitivity likely contributed to their eventual sacking:
5
[To upgrade] all three [training] pitches would cost something like £65,000 . . . [and]
6
it was decided [by the Board] to do one pitch . . . . Yet they built one of the corners up
7
of the [stadium] as a media center and . . . restaurant for £1M . . . . I don’t see the
8
logic in it . . . and I made my feelings known . . . I’m not saying that cost me, but
9
maybe . . . banging on about [it] was something that didn’t help. (PM6)
10
Adaptive expertise. Beyond appreciating what general behaviors could elicit general
11
outcomes in general scenarios (e.g., in times of conflict, acts of social dominance may stop
12
issues from escalating), optimal use of dark side behavior was also found to rely on knowing
13
precisely when, where, why, and how specific behaviors could be shaped and combined to
14
deliver specific outcomes in specific scenarios (e.g., how to use acts of social dominance and
15
manipulation during “live” conflict with a performer who has recently been dropped from the
16
team but cannot be isolated due their status within the squad) Termed adaptive expertise, this
17
skill reflects an ability to perform effectively, flexibly, and innovatively in unstructured and
18
unpredictable situations (Fazey, Fazey, & Fazey, 2005; Tozer, Fazey, & Fazey, 2007); in
19
short, supporting the ability to act effectively in very different ways in very different
20
contexts. For instance, one leader noted that “there are times when you’ve got to lose it . . .
21
call it staged call or not, for the right reaction [at that moment] (PM3). Another leader
22
provided a particularly eloquent example of opportune Machiavellian behavior with two
23
players who were not regularly playing in the team but who remained key proponents of his
24
desired culture:
25
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 24
“Listen, you two [are] good . . . players but as lads you’ve made this training area far,
1
far better.” I know . . . that’s stuck with [X] . . . I did believe that though, I wouldn’t
2
tell a lie. I’d just tell them the truth at the right time [emphasis added]. (PM5)
3
Suggesting that the impact of dark side behaviors was greatest when these were strategically
4
molded and deployed at particular moments, such expertise appeared to be underpinned by a
5
strong declarative understanding of interpersonal dynamics and context. Indeed, knowing
6
when and when not to use dark side behavior was also supported by PDs:
7
You have to be . . . clear on what you . . . expect of [British Olympic Association] and
8
be pretty damn quick to say . . . “I think that’s very ****” . . . . [But] I suppose you
9
have to taper . . . so when you are three years out from the Games you can be dealing
10
in strategic stuff, telling [the BOA] what you think. Three months from the Games
11
that’s all irrelevant. You’ve got to get on and deal with whatever’s there. (PD1)
12
Similarly, another leader noted how perceptions of those outside the performance department
13
were targeted and manipulated via the media at precise moments during a team’s evolution:
14
[When you are successful] people are wondering, Well, how are you doing it? . . .
15
And slowly those questions are asked [by the media] and sometimes we didn’t answer
16
them directly [to maintain advantage] but there were other times when we were very
17
clear [as] we wanted people to know . . . this is what we are doing. (PM1)
18
As suggested by the preceding quotes, the coherence of a leader’s action with their short and
19
longer term plans underpinned the effective use of dark side leadership behaviors according
20
to participants; a process aided by creating and working against nested agendas (cf. Abraham
21
& Collins, 2011). Indeed, greatest impact was felt to arrive when dark side behavior (in the
22
following case, behavior related to performance-focused ruthlessness) was tied to the leader’s
23
core values and their program’s ultimate direction:
24
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 25
I never quite did it but someone said [remove a problematic or incompatible player] a
1
year early rather than a year late. [Sir Alex Ferguson went] . . . a year early rather
2
than a year late. No matter how big a personality [or] how talented you are . . . . [You
3
need to be] close enough to care but detached enough to make tough decisions. (PM7)
4
Discussion
5
Based on recent suggestions that best practice as an elite team leader involves the use
6
of dark side leadership behaviors (Bennie & O’Connor, 2012; Collins & Cruickshank, 2012;
7
Cruickshank et al., 2014; Elberse & Dye, 2012; Fletcher & Arnold, 2011), this study aimed to
8
(a) unearth possible further evidence on the use of dark side leadership behaviors by those
9
operating at the managerial level of elite teams, (b) illuminate what these behaviors look like
10
in a common leadership task, and (c) explore how these behaviors may be applied to achieve
11
desired outcomes yet minimize the risk of leader derailment. Findings showed that dark side
12
behaviors were used (and deemed useful) by our interviewees and were supported by the key
13
metaskills of sociopolitical awareness and engineering and adaptive expertise.
14
Most fundamentally, these findings corroborate recent suggestions that the enactment
15
of dark side traits (or use of dark side behavior as defined in this paper) is a tangible feature
16
of elite team leadership. Specifically, our sample reported that skeptical, social dominance,
17
Machiavellian/mischievous, and performance-focused ruthlessness behaviors were all
18
employed during leaders’ efforts to deliver change in their teams. Given the stigmatized
19
nature of socially undesirable actions, as well as their links to destructive forms of leadership
20
when over-represented (Hogan & Hogan, 2001), it was notable that these leaders also felt that
21
these behaviors, when appropriately engaged, were important and effective parts of their
22
repertoire. Indeed, while a causal relationship cannot be inferred and the negative impact of
23
inappropriately used darks side leadership is well documented (cf. Judge et al., 2009), these
24
behaviors were applied by highly successful interviewees within our sample (as based on
25
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 26
objective success) and acknowledged as something which could have been used to prevent
1
derailment in others. As prior work has provided little description of the dark side behaviors
2
used by elite team leaders, this research has also provided a timely depiction of what dark
3
side behavior can look like during a commonly undertaken leadership activity. Taken against
4
the inherently bright approaches to leadership often applied in sport psychology, most
5
notably the transformational approach given its continued exposure and influence in sport
6
psychology, it would therefore seem that theories need to be developed which need to better
7
account for the bright and dark side of leadership at the elite performance team level.
8
As usefully pointed out by one reviewer of this paper, some of the dark side behaviors
9
used by participants in this study could be interpreted as working towards some of the same
10
goals as those espoused within the transformational approach. For example, some quotes on
11
Machiavellian/mischievous behavior presented earlier could possibly be interpreted as a form
12
of inspirational motivation (although also note that many of reported actions were primarily
13
aimed at furthering the leader’s agenda). However, contrary to the entirely bright items used
14
to measure inspirational motivation in many transformational leadership questionnaires (e.g.,
15
Callow et al., 2009), it would appear that these dark pathways have not yet been sufficiently
16
considered or accounted for (at least for elite team environments). Additionally, while some
17
of our findings shared similarities in terms of the ultimate goal of leadership behavior (e.g., to
18
inspire or stimulate), they were often completely non-transformational at the same time. For
19
example, take the PD who removed the physiotherapist for not committing due to personal
20
issues: This figure might be deemed to have high performance expectations and be fostering
21
acceptance of group goals and teamwork in others (as per transformational leadership theory;
22
Bass, 1985; Callow et al. 2009); but he also completely overlooked aspects of individual
23
consideration and intellectual stimulation (as per transformational leadership theory; Bass,
24
1985; Callow et al., 2009) to be effective (in his view at least) in the given scenario.
25
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 27
Moreover, rather than create a new vision, engage in inspirational exchanges, and develop
1
followers to their full potential (as per the key tenets of transformational leadership theory;
2
Bass, 1985), some reported behaviors were used to protect the status quo and undermine or
3
expose followers at specific times (e.g., the pre-emptive strike to gain funding agency support
4
before the arrival of a new board member; the peer-review process that bullied the bullies).
5
In this manner, our findings support prior research and suggest that elite team leaders
6
cannot be entirely transformational (or entirely bright) if they are to be optimally effective.
7
Given the power of modern performer groups, the self-serving politics in multidiscipline
8
staff, the regularly wielded power of results-consumed boards (or oligarch owners), and
9
subversive acts of the media (cf. Cruickshank & Collins, 2012), it is perhaps unsurprising that
10
elite team leaders cannot adopt an entirely positive, inspirational, and attentive approach (at
11
least not all the time). Rather, our findings suggest that, for delivering change at least,
12
successful leaders empower and inspire yet retain ultimate control over those who deliver or
13
assist performance. As such, we hope that our results trigger a more balanced consideration
14
of dark side behaviors within leadership approaches, including those which are inherently but
15
not exclusively bright. Additionally, it would seem that future models of elite team
16
leadership also need to better account for behaviors related to “managing upwards” (e.g., to
17
boards) and “managing outwards” (e.g., to the media) as well as the historical top-down
18
perspective (i.e., leadership targeted at performers and, to a lesser extent, support staff).
19
Beyond these conceptual insights and implications, this study has also extended prior
20
work by developing understanding on how dark side leadership behaviors may be effectively
21
applied by elite team leaders. Taking sociopolitical awareness and engineering first, the first
22
element of this metaskill (i.e., sociopolitical awareness) resonates strongly with contingency
23
approaches to leadership (e.g., contingency theory, situational leadership theory, path-goal
24
theory; Seyranian, 2010). In this way, dark side behavior was reported to be best engaged in
25
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 28
socially and politically conducive conditions. The second element of this metaskill (i.e.,
1
sociopolitical engineering), however, suggests that such situations were not just responded to
2
but also proactively shaped. As précised by one PD, surviving and succeeding in one’s role
3
involved “creating situations before they create you” (PD4); a finding which resonates with
4
Hogan and Hogan’s (2001) view that the dark side of personality is protected and masked by
5
strong social skills. As such, dark side behaviors were considered most effective when
6
contexts had been earlier molded in a way that meant support staff and performers would be
7
more likely to perceive them as appropriate, acceptable, or normal for the given situation.
8
With similar links to contingency based approaches to leadership, reliance on adaptive
9
expertise also suggests that effective use of dark side behaviors is ultimately a test of decision
10
making. However, while contingency based approaches often judge leader behavior against
11
immediate situations, the present research has extended prior work by revealing that optimal
12
impact was felt to occur when dark side behaviors were locked into a leader’s short, medium,
13
and long term plans. Enabled by a clear understanding of their program’s ultimate direction,
14
coherence across the leader’s short, intermediate, and long term behavior may therefore help
15
followers to grasp why such socially undesirable behavior was (and will continue to be) used
16
to promote and protect performance.
17
Strengths, Limitations, and Future Research
18
To support judgments on research quality, it is useful to consider some characterizing
19
traits of this study (Sparkes & Smith, 2009). Specifically, and beyond strategies to enhance
20
trustworthiness (cf. method section), methodological coherence was demonstrated in that our
21
pragmatic approach guided our practice based research questions, use of theories (i.e., Hogan
22
& Hogan, 2001; Judge et al., 2009) as tools to develop practically relevant insights rather any
23
truths about reality, participant selection, data collection (i.e., framing interviews against
24
a practically relevant task), and data analysis (i.e., a focus on the process and mechanisms of
25
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 29
dark side behaviors: Corbin & Strauss, 2008; Denzin & Lincoln, 2008; Giacobbi et al., 2005;
1
Gould et al., 2007). As the major goal for pragmatic research is to develop novel and useful
2
ways of addressing applied challenges (Giacobbi et al., 2005), we also ask the reader to apply
3
the “so what?” principle (Bryant, 2009). In short, what difference to practice-oriented theory
4
and consultancy have our findings made if they do relate to tangible applied artifacts?
5
Despite such strengths, this research was of course not without limits that future work
6
should address. First, scholars should seek to assess the extent to which dark side behavior is
7
implicated in other specific and practically meaningful leadership challenges; for example, in
8
the lead up to major events (e.g., the Olympics). Certainly, it is not impossible that dark side
9
behaviors are particularly used when delivering programs of change or that our interviewees
10
were particularly disposed to dark side behavior. This work should further focus on specific
11
settings to create bespoke theory and practice (e.g., professional versus Olympic sport; soccer
12
versus rugby) and would also sensibly explore the extent to which dark side behaviors reflect
13
the manifestation of underlying traits or learned behaviors (Hogan & Hogan, 2001). Future
14
work should also seek to incorporate support staff and performer perceptions. It is likely that
15
some behaviors linked to traits not reported here but elsewhere (e.g., Narcissism; Fletcher &
16
Arnold, 2011) will emerge from such investigation and offer a more complete picture of
17
leaders’ dark side repertoire. Including these followers” (as well as other stakeholders; e.g.,
18
board members, media, fans) and pairing their perceptions with performance and outcome
19
data would also allow for an assessment of the actual impact of dark behaviors on those who
20
such action is targeted; particularly given the well-reported negative impact of
21
inappropriately deployed dark side behaviors (cf. Judge et al., 2009). Indeed, Hogan and
22
Hogan (2001) have already argued that the observer’s view is needed to fully elucidate dark
23
side leadership. Such inquiry would also fit with growing awareness that leader effectiveness
24
is shaped by follower characteristics (Arthur et al., 2011). Lastly, researchers should also
25
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 30
seek to develop expertise-based models of dark side (and wider) leadership. Extending
1
beyond competency-based approaches, which detail what general behaviors leaders should be
2
able to exhibit, expertise-based models would include aspects of why, when, where, and how
3
specific behaviors are used (and in what combination) to deliver a specific impact for a
4
specific challenge (cf. Collins, Burke, Martindale, & Cruickshank, 2014). For this purpose,
5
case studies that assess leader behaviors against their short, medium, and long term goals
6
would be a useful pursuit.
7
Concluding Comments
8
Responding to recent suggestions in sport psychology and more established trends in
9
organizational literature, this study has presented further evidence of the use of dark side
10
behaviors by elite team leaders, illuminated what some of these behaviors look like during a
11
common leadership task, and revealed mechanisms through which they were considered to be
12
effectively applied. Theoretically, these findings provide a platform upon which researchers
13
may continue to explore both the bright and dark side of elite team leadership. Moreover, our
14
findings also encourage the development of elite team leadership models that are underpinned
15
by expertise (i.e., knowing why, when, where, and how to use specific behaviors) rather than
16
competence (i.e., the possession of, or ability to call on general behaviors). On a practical
17
level, and while great care is required given the potential for negative implications if misused,
18
the appropriate use of dark side behaviors by elite team leaders is supported by their reported
19
use and usefulness by participants in this study. More specifically, elite team leaders and
20
supporting consultants are advised to: (a) critically assess and engineer the sociopolitical
21
contexts in which dark side behavior is to be used; and (b) develop and work from clear and
22
coherent long term plans so that, when needed, dark side behavior is viewed by followers as
23
sufficiently appropriate, acceptable, or normal for optimizing performance.
24
25
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 31
Table 1
1
Type of Sports Team Managed by Participants
2
3
4
5
6
Note. a Since interview, one professional soccer team manager has requested
7
that their data not be quoted in this paper due to changes in their employment
8
circumstances; this participant’s data were, however, still used in the analysis.
9
Participant identifier
Managed sports team type
PM1, PM7
Professional rugby union
PM2, PM4
Professional rugby league
PM3, PM5, PM6
Professional soccera
PD1, PD2, PD4, PD5, PD7
Olympic individual plus team sport
PD3
Olympic team sport
PD6
Olympic individual sport
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 32
References
1
American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
2
disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
3
Abraham, A., & Collins, D. (2011). Taking the next step: New directions for coaching
4
science. Quest, 6, 366-384.
5
Arthur, C., Hardy, L., & Woodman, T. (2012). Realising the Olympic dream: Vision, support
6
and challenge. Reflective Practice, 13, 399-406.
7
Arthur, C., Woodman, T., Ong, C. W., Hardy, L., & Ntoumanis, N. (2011). The role of
8
athlete narcissism in moderating the relationship between coaches’ transformational
9
leader behaviors and athlete motivation. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 33,
10
3-19.
11
Bass, B. M. (1985). Leadership and performance beyond expectations. New York: Free
12
Press.
13
Bass, B. M., Avolio, B. J., Jung, D. I., & Berson, Y. (2003). Predicting unit performance by
14
assessing transformational and transactional leadership. Journal of Applied
15
Psychology, 88, 207218.
16
Bennie, A., & O’Connor, D. (2012). Perceptions and strategies of effective coaching
17
leadership: A qualitative investigation of professional coaches and players.
18
International Journal of Sport and Health Science, 10, 82-89.
19
Blair, C. A., Hoffman, B. J., & Helland, K. R. (2008). Narcissism in organizations: A
20
multisource appraisal reflects different perspectives. Human Performance, 21,
21
254276.
22
Bryant, A. (2009). Grounded theory and pragmatism: The curious case of Anselm Strauss.
23
Forum: Qualitative Social Research, 10(3): Art. 2.
24
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 33
Buchanan, D. A., & Bryman, A. (2007). Contextualising methods choice in organizational
1
research. Organizational Research Methods, 10, 483-501.
2
Callow, N., Smith, M. J., Hardy, L., Arthur, C. A., & Hardy, J. (2009). Measurement of
3
transformational leadership and its relationship with team cohesion and performance
4
level. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 21, 395-412.
5
Carson, M. A., Shanock, L. R., Heggestad, E. D., Andrew, A. M, Pugh, S. D., & Walter, M.
6
(2012). The relationship between dysfunctional interpersonal tendencies, derailment
7
potential behavior, and turnover. Journal of Business and Psychology, 27, 291-304.
8
Chelladurai, P. (1980). Leadership in sports organizations. Canadian Journal of Applied
9
Sport Sciences, 5, 226-231.
10
Chen, C-C. (2013). How does paternalistic style leadership relate to team cohesiveness in
11
soccer coaching? Social Behavior and Personality, 41, 83-94.
12
Collins, D., Burke, V., Martindale, A., & Cruickshank, A. (2014). The illusion of competency
13
versus the desirability of expertise: Seeking a common standard for support
14
professions in sport. Sports Medicine. doi:10.1007/s40279-014-0251-1
15
Collins, D., & Cruickshank, A. (2012). ‘Multidirectional management’: Exploring the
16
challenges of performance in the World Class Programme environment. Reflective
17
Practice, 13, 455-469.
18
Conger, J. A. (1990). The dark side of leadership. Organizational Dynamics, 19(2), 44-55.
19
Corbin, J., & Strauss, A. (2008). Basics of qualitative research: Techniques and procedures
20
for developing ground theory (3rd ed.). London: Sage.
21
Côté, J., Salmela, J. H., Baria, A., & Russell, S. J. (1993). Organizing and interpreting
22
unstructured qualitative data. The Sport Psychologist, 7, 127-137.
23
Corbin, J., & Strauss, A. (2008). Basics of qualitative research: Techniques and procedures
24
for developing ground theory (3rd ed.). London: Sage.
25
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 34
Cruickshank, A., & Collins, D. (2012). Culture change in elite sport performance teams:
1
Examining and advancing effectiveness in the new era. Journal of Applied Sport
2
Psychology, 24, 338-355.
3
Cruickshank, A., Collins, D., & Minten, S. (2013). Culture change in a professional sports
4
team: Shaping environmental contexts and regulating power. International Journal of
5
Sports Science and Coaching, 8, 271-290.
6
Cruickshank, A., Collins, D., & Minten, S. (2014). Driving and sustaining culture change in
7
Olympic sport performance teams: A first exploration and grounded theory. Journal
8
of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 36, 107-120.
9
Culver, D. M., Gilbert, W., & Sparkes, A. (2012). Qualitative research in sport psychology
10
journals. The Sport Psychologist, 26, 261-281.
11
Davies, M. (2004). Prediction of transformational leadership by personality constructs for
12
senior Australian organizational executive leaders. Unpublished dissertation, Faculty
13
of Health Science, Griffith University, Queensland, Australia.
14
Davis, N. W., & Meyer, B. B. (2009). Qualitative data analysis: A procedural comparison.
15
Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 21, 116-124.
16
Denzin, N. K., & Lincoln, Y. S. (2008). Strategies of qualitative inquiry (3rd ed.). Los
17
Angeles: Sage.
18
Elberse, A., & Dye, T. (2012). Sir Alex Ferguson: Managing Manchester United. Harvard
19
Business School, Case 513-051.
20
Faulkner, G., & Sparkes, A. (1999). Exercise as therapy for schizophrenia: An ethnographic
21
study. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 21, 52-69.
22
Fazey, I., Fazey, J. A., & Fazey, D. M. A. (2005). Learning more effectively from experience.
23
Ecology & Society, 10(2): 4.
24
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 35
Fletcher, D., & Arnold, R. (2011). A qualitative study of performance leadership and
1
management in elite sport. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 23, 223-242.
2
Giacobbi, P. R., Jr., Poczwardowski, A., & Hager, P. (2005). A pragmatic research
3
philosophy for applied sport psychology. The Sport Psychologist, 19, 18-31.
4
Gould, D., Collins, K., Lauer, L., & Chung, Y. (2007). Coaching life skills through football:
5
A study of award winning high school coaches. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology,
6
19, 16-37.
7
Harms, P. D., Spain, S. M., & Hannah, S. T. (2012). Leader development and the dark side of
8
personality. The Leadership Quarterly, 22, 495-509.
9
Hogan, R., & Hogan, J. (2001). Assessing leadership: A view from the dark side.
10
International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 9, 40-51.
11
Hogan, R., & Hogan, J. (2002). Leadership and sociopolitical intelligence. In R. E. Riggio, S.
12
E. Murphy, & F. J. Pirozzolo (Eds.), Multiple intelligences and leadership (pp. 75
13
88). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
14
Hogan, R., & Kaiser, R. (2005). What we know about leadership. Review of General
15
Psychology, 9, 169180.
16
Judge, T. A., Piccolo, R. F., & Kosalka, T. (2009). The bright and dark side of leader traits: A
17
review and theoretical extension of the leader trait paradigm. The Leadership
18
Quarterly, 20, 855-875.
19
Lee, Y., Kim, S-H., & Kang, J-H. (2013). Coach leadership effect on elite handball players’
20
psychological empowerment and organizational citizenship behavior. International
21
Journal of Sports Sciences and Coaching, 8, 327-342.
22
Marshall, A., Baden, D., & Guidi, M. (2013). Can an ethical revival of prudence within
23
prudential regulation tackle corporate psychopathy. Journal of Business Ethics, 117,
24
559-568.
25
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 36
Martindale, A., & Collins, D. (2010). But why does what works work? A response to Fifer,
1
Henschen, Gould, and Ravizza. The Sport Psychologist. 24, 113-116.
2
Morgan, D. L. (2007). Paradigms lost and pragmatism regained: Methodological implications
3
of combining qualitative and quantitative methods. Journal of Mixed Methods
4
Research, 1, 48-76.
5
Owen, D., & Davidson, J. (2009). Hubris syndrome: An acquired personality disorder? A
6
study of US presidents and UK prime ministers over the last 100 years. Brain, 132,
7
1396-1406.
8
Patton, M. Q. (2002). Qualitative research & evaluation measures. London: Sage.
9
Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. (2002). The dark triad of personality: Narcissism,
10
Machiavellianism, and Psychopathy. Journal of Research in Personality, 36, 556-568.
11
Pettersson, E., Turkheimer, E., Horn, E. E., Simms, L. J., Mendle, J., Ford, D. C., & Clark, L
12
A. (2014). Do maladaptive behaviors exist at one or both ends of personality traits?
13
Psychological Assessment, 26, 433-446.
14
Rieke, M., Hammermeister, J., & Chase, M. (2008). Servant leadership in sport: A new
15
paradigm for effective coach behavior. International Journal of Sports Science and
16
Coaching, 3, 227-239.
17
Rowold, J. (2006). Transformational and transactional leadership in martial arts. Journal of
18
Applied Sport Psychology, 18, 312325.
19
Seyranian, V. (2010). Contingency theories of leadership. In J. M. Levine, & M. A. Hogg
20
(Eds.), Encyclopedia of group processes & intergroup relations (pp. 152-156).
21
Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
22
Sparkes, A., & Smith, B. (2009). Judging the quality of qualitative inquiry: Criteriology and
23
relativism in action. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 10, 491-497.
24
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 37
Strean, W. B. (1998). Possibilities for qualitative research in sport psychology. The Sport
1
Psychologist, 12, 333-345.
2
Tourish, D. (2013). The dark side of transformational leadership. Hove, UK: Routledge.
3
Tozer, M., Fazey, I., & Fazey, J. (2007). Recognising and developing adaptive expertise
4
within outdoor and expedition leaders. Journal of Adventure Education and Outdoor
5
Learning, 7, 55-75.
6
Vidic, Z., & Burton, D. (2011). Developing effective leaders: Motivational correlates of
7
leadership styles. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 23, 277-291.
8
Walumbwa, F. O., Avolio, B. J., & Zhu, W. (2008). How transformational leadership weaves
9
its influence on individual performance: The role of identification and efficacy
10
beliefs. Personnel Psychology, 61, 793-825.
11
Yukl, G. (2006). Leadership in organizations (6th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson
12
Prentice Hall.
13
Running Head: ILLUMINATING AND APPLYING THE DARK SIDE 38
Footnotes
1
1 The broader research project used Corbin and Strauss’ (2008) grounded theory
2
methodology to explore perceptions of elite performance team leaders. As such, the
3
interview guide was not structured around any formal theory and instead allowed for an
4
essentially open-ended discussion on a practically meaningful process.
5
2 Space precludes the provision of details on the behavioral manifestations of all of
6
these dark side traits as described by Hogan and Hogan (2001) and Judge et al. (2009).
7
However, details are described for those presented within the results section.
8
... Destructive or toxic leadership behaviors or processes within teams or organizations that negatively impact individuals' wellbeing and performance, and foster unethical behavior, are increasingly documented in high-performance settings such as business, politics, the military, and sports (Carleton et al., 2016;Cruickshank & Collins, 2016;Krasikova et al., 2013;Padilla et al., 2007;Schyns & Schilling, 2013;Serpell et al., 2021;Tepper et al., 2017). High-performance sports are traditionally characterized by a 'win-atall-costs' culture (Lundqvist et al., 2024;Roberts et al., 2020), implying a normative view that effective leadership can be evaluated through medals and sporting success (Cruickshank & Collins, 2015). The detrimental effects of leadership can be mitigated by sport performance gains aligned with the leader(s) having a high-status within the sports context. ...
... destructive, toxic). Cruickshank and Collins (2015) investigated the use of 'dark side behaviors' which was defined as the manifestation of the traits constituting the dark triad: Psychopathy, Machiavellianism, and Narcissism. Lopez et al. (2020) defined abusive leadership as instances 'when formal leaders engage in aggressive or punitive behaviors toward subordinates' (p. ...
... Coaches in Stirling (2013) were presented with a definition of emotional abuse, and five out of nine coaches interviewed reported previous use of emotionally abusive coaching practices. Cruickshank and Collins (2015) interviewed leaders at top-level sporting organizations and found that all 15 of the included leaders reported using leadership behaviors associated with dark-side traits. Relatedly, Serpell et al. (2021) found that leaders in elite sports had higher levels of three (out of eleven) dark-side traits than leaders in other settings. ...
Article
Full-text available
This scoping review aimed to identify the current state of knowledge on toxic leadership in high-performance sports and its consequences for mental health and performance at individual, interpersonal, and organizational levels. Abstract screening (n=768) and full-text review (n=83) resulted in the inclusion of 26 studies, predominantly conducted in North America and Europe. Studies used inconsistent defini-tions, theoretical frameworks, and methodological approaches to study darker or toxic leadership. Several antecedents and maintaining factors to toxic leadership were identified, such as individual leader characteristics, a performance-oriented culture, status/power structures, and a lack of reporting processes or consequences within organizations. The empirical literature provided initial support for the negative consequences of toxic leadership on athletes’ mental health, injuries, and motivation to remain in high-performance sports. Few studies have investigated the consequences of toxic leadership on sports organizations, staff or coaches. We conclude that research on toxic leadership in high-performance sports is in its early stages, with an underdeveloped understanding of how problematic leadership can be detected, prevented, and addressed to minimize their impact in sports. Researchers, governing bodies, and sports organizations must intensify their efforts to comprehend the emergence and persistence of toxic leadership within high-performance sports environments.
... Two papers (4%) on ML followed a qualitative methodological design. Following the qualitative methodology, Cruickshank and Collins (2015) presented open-ended interviews of leaders and Zheng et al. (2017) proposed a qualitative analysis and compilations of a single-case study of a corporate family business in China. This systematic literature identified a single research paper following a historimetric methodology (i.e. ...
... The ML style focuses on asserting control over subordinates (Belschak et al., 2018a, b;Cruickshank and Collins, 2015), thus leading to an absence of trust between leader and follower. In leadership positions, those with higher Machiavellian tendencies cause high levels of organisational cynicism among subordinates, which can lead to emotional exhaustion (Mirkovic and Bianchi, 2019;Stradovnik and Stare, 2018;Gkorezis et al., 2015). ...
... There is a common understanding that Machiavellian leaders are "dark" and negatively impact employees and organisations (Mohamed and Samman, 2022;Annebel et al., 2021;Wisse and Sleebos, 2016;Jaiswal and Bhal, 2014). Yet, research by Genau et al. (2022) and Cruickshank and Collins (2015) argues that perhaps Machiavellians are situationally effective. For instance, Genau et al. (2022) suggest that leaders with high Machiavellianism can be successful transformational leaders with leader effectiveness when they are also politically skilled. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose During the past decade, there has been a steep increase in the scholarly literature on Machiavellian leadership. However, no systematic literature review has been conducted to synthesise, analyse and identify the trends, tensions and gaps in Machiavellian leadership. Therefore, the authors examine empirical studies published in peer-reviewed journals since 2000, with an impact factor of Q2 or above in the SCImago ranking list. This paper aims to discuss the aforementioned objective. Design/methodology/approach Using the systematic literature review approach, a review protocol was developed to define the focus of the study, the search strategy and the criteria for inclusion and exclusion of studies in the review process. The authors developed a keyword search query using terms “Machiavellian” OR “Dark Triads” and “leadership”. The authors’ search was limited to publications in the English language and with the search terms in either the abstract or keywords of the publication. The review consisted of 576 peer-reviewed scholarly journal articles, out of which 53 articles were selected for the final analysis. Findings The analysis reveals how the unidimensional nature of most studies and limited contextual and ontological orientations curtail the development of the relational perspective of Machiavellian leadership. The authors propose a way forward, new insights and opportunities for future research in the field. Originality/value This is the first systematic literature review on Machiavellian leadership. Although the research in Machiavellian leadership is reaching a stage of maturity, this review exposes limitations and gaps in research.
... Machiavellian techniques have shown to be most effective in dyads where one-toone interactions are used to create positive relationships (Hacker & Gaitz, 1970), comparable to an elite sport environment with coaches only working with a few athletes. Indeed, head coaches of elite teams have frequently demonstrated Machiavellian approaches to shape interpersonal relationships and performance (Cruickshank & Collins, 2015). However, at present these studies do not consider the interdependence of dyadic relationships. ...
Article
Full-text available
The Dark Triad is highly relevant in high-performance environments as it offers explanation for feelings of superiority , opportunities to be ruthless in the pursuit of victory, and a heightened belief of influence over others for individual success. High-performance dyads focus on achieving a collective goal to achieve additional individual glory. The aim of this study was to investigate how levels of these traits and (dis)similarity in them was associated with relationship quality of 316 high-performance dyads. Greater dissimilarity in narcissism resulted in higher relationship quality for both dyadic members. Actor effects indicated that higher levels of narcissism, psychopathy, and Machiavellianism were related to a reduction in coaches' own perceptions of relationship quality, whilst only higher levels of Machiavellianism were associated with a reduction in athletes' own perceived levels of relationship quality. Partner effects showed higher levels of athlete Machiavellianism reduced the relationship quality of coaches.
... This is particularly relevant for applied research disciplines where 'making a difference' to the individuals or groups that it examines is a key goal (e.g., Corbin & Strauss, 2008). This contrasts with, for example, positivists who may seek generalisable 'truths', or constructionists who may seek representations of socially constructed reality (e.g., Cruickshank & Collins, 2015). Using this approach, methods are chosen with the aim of providing solutions to context-specific problems without being driven by a definitive epistemological approach. ...
Article
Full-text available
Parents are known to be important influencers in the talent development process but are often overlooked. Understanding the parent perspective may help National Governing Bodies and coaches improve their support for young talented athletes. This study aims to understand the parent perspective of the talent development experience across a national multi-sport landscape using mixed methods. Four hundred and eighty-five parents of pathway athletes completed the talent development environment questionnaire for parents (TDEQ-P), which included the opportunity for parents to respond in an open fashion. The results revealed several key relative strengths and weaknesses of the talent development environment. Areas identified for improvement included 1) Communication related to pathway understanding, planning, guidance, and feedback, 2) Individualised training, goal setting, and feedback, 3) Facilities, 4) Access to a specialist support network, in particular psychology support, 5) Engagement between coaches and, 6) Dual career management and engagement with educational institutions. The results also revealed evidence of context specific needs between performance level, sport type, and age group. The TDEQ-P may be an efficient and effective way coaches can access important feedback, improve communication, and build relationships with parents in sport.
... In accordance with the other two constructs and the overall dark triad tendency, Machiavellianism displayed similar patterns between groups in various literature [4,[13][14][15]. People high in Machiavellianism not only were considered to be elite team leaders as their acute decision-making ability often masked their manipulation [21], but also were committed to their own goals and achievements [19]. Vaughan and Madigan [4] manifested that Machiavellian's traits seemed to influence individuals' performance in the short term but might be detrimental in the long term since Machiavellianism leads to undermining coworkers or teammates. ...
Article
Successful businesspeople and politicians seem to have a high tendency in the Dark Triad of personality. Since achieved individuals often share similar personality traits while striving in a similar environment (prospering at others' expense), high-performing athletes are most likely to have similar characteristics. Through the examination on kinds of literature that are relative to the topic of the relationship between high-performing athletes and the Dark Triad of personality, homogeneous results were found across studies: the better the performance an athlete has, the more he/she would score in both overall and subsets of the Dark Triad scale. Potential drawbacks of such phenomena, such as doping and cheating, were also presented by literature, which should be paid attention to in future research.
Book
Full-text available
Sporda Liderliğin Çerçevesi Pervin BİLİR Liderlik Nedir? Pervin BİLİR Tolga ÇELİK Liderliğin Özellik Teorileri Pervin BİLİR Seyhan BİLİR GÜLER Liderlik Stilleri Selahattin AKPINAR Öznur AKPINAR Liderlik, Takımlar ve Etkileşim: Ya Grubun İçindesiniz ya da Dışında Melih N. SALMAN Yıkıcı ve Toksik Liderlik Ergun YURDADÖN Spor Yönetiminde Kolaylaştırıcı Liderlik Pervin BİLİR Spor Yönetiminde İletişim Canan SAYIN TEMUR Saadet GÖNEN Stratejik Planlama ile Stratejik Liderlik Pervin BİLİR Nurşen ŞAHİN Kurumsal Değişime Liderlik Etmek Enver DÖŞYILMAZ Spor Liderliğinde Çeşitlilik Özge AYDIN Antrenör Tayfun ŞİRİN Ozan Ç. SARIKAYA Antrenörlükte Liderlik Modelleri Yeliz ERATLI ŞİRİN Antrenörlükte Örgütsel Liderlik Yaklaşımları Nurşen ŞAHİN Antrenörlükte Liderliğin Ölçülmesi Eren ULUÖZ Gamze ÜNALDI Empati ile Sürdürülebilir Performans Zeynep Filiz DİNÇ Cem Yoksuler YILMAZ Gelecek: Spor Liderliğindeki Yönelimler ve Zorluklar Erdem EROĞLU Sultan EROĞLU
Article
Full-text available
Social factors and psychological characteristics can influence participation and development in talent pathways. However, the interaction between these two factors is relatively unknown. The aim of this study was to investigate the implications of socioeconomic status and psychological characteristics in English academy soccer players (n=58; aged 11 to 16 years). To assess socioeconomic status, participants' home postcodes were coded according to each individual's social classification and credit rating, applying the UK General Registrar Classification system and CameoTM geodemographic database, respectively. Participants also completed the six factor Psychological Characteristics for Developing Excellence Questionnaire (PCDEQ). A classification of 'higher-potentials' (n=19) and 'lower-potentials' (n=19) were applied through coach potential rankings. Data were standardised using z-scores to eliminate age bias and data were analysed using independent sample t-tests. Results showed that higher-potentials derived from families with significantly lower social classifications (p=0.014) and reported higher levels for PCDEQ Factor 3 (coping with performance and developmental pressures) (p=0.007) compared to lower-potentials. This study can be used to support the impetus for researchers and practitioners to consider the role of social factors and psychological characteristics when developing sporting talent. For example, facilitating player-centred development within an academy and, where necessary, providing individuals with additional support.
Article
Full-text available
A pragmatic research philosophy is introduced that embraces mixed-method approaches to applied research questions. With its origins in the work of Peirce (1984), James (1907), Dewey (1931), and contemporary support from Rorty (1982, 1990, 1991), pragmatism emphasizes the practical problems experienced by people, the research questions posited, and the consequences of inquiry. As a way to highlight applications of pragmatism in sport psychology, pragmatism is compared to constructivism and positivism in terms of philosophical underpinnings and methodological applications. The pragmatic researcher is sensitive to the social, historical, and political context from which inquiry begins and considers morality, ethics, and issues of social justice to be important throughout the research process. Pragmatists often use pluralistic methods during multiphase research projects. Exemplar design types are discussed that logically cohere to a pragmatic research philosophy.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this article is to critically synthesize theoretical frameworks from leadership research and present a new integrated model for the study of contemporary Olympic sport leadership. The empirical work and leadership theories that this model grows out of are reviewed. This discussion builds support for the heuristic value of the integrated research model of Olympic podium performance. Although research models have been problematised by some coaching scholars, it is argued that the proposed research framework has the potential to inspire questions, challenge assumptions, and begin a leadership dialogue which foregrounds Olympic medalists' and their coaches' voices and experiences. The integrated research model is an antecedent, behavior, and consequence model. The antecedent phase is inspired by Côté et al.'s Coaching Model and includes the coach's cognitive map for sport performance. The behavior phase of this model is largely aligned with transformational leadership behaviors outlined in Chelladurai's sport in the pursuit of excellence framework and the coach-athlete relationship is included in this phase of the model. The consequence or outcome is narrowly defined as an Olympic podium performance. Future research in this unique sport leadership context and inspired by this research model is proposed.
Article
Full-text available
This article examines the significance of the correlation between coach leadership behavior and players' organizational citizenship behavior, and the mediating role of psychological empowerment in the process. A total of 73 professional handball players from five different teams assessed leadership style, psychological empowerment, and organizational citizenship behavior based on the scale adapted from Multifactor Leadership Questionnaire form 5X. Regression analysis and bootstrap methods were utilized to test the proposed hypotheses. Findings indicated that both transactional and transformational leadership styles significantly predicted players' organizational citizenship behavior. In particular, psychological empowerment played a mediating role in the association between coach leadership and organizational citizenship behavior.
Article
Full-text available
In this paper we examine and challenge the competency-based models which currently dominate accreditation and development systems in sport support disciplines, largely the sciences and coaching. Through consideration of exemplar shortcomings, the limitations of competency-based systems are presented as failing to cater for the complexity of decision making and the need for proactive experimentation essential to effective practice. To provide a better fit with the challenges of the various disciplines in their work with performers, an alternative approach is presented which focuses on the promotion, evaluation and elaboration of expertise. Such an approach resonates with important characteristics of professions, whilst also providing for the essential 'shades of grey' inherent in work with human participants. Key differences between the approaches are considered through exemplars of evaluation processes. The expertise-focused method, although inherently more complex, is seen as offering a less ambiguous and more positive route, both through more accurate representation of essential professional competence and through facilitation of future growth in proficiency and evolution of expertise in practice. Examples from the literature are also presented, offering further support for the practicalities of this approach.
Article
Full-text available
Part of the on-going dialogue on qualitative research in sport and exercise psychology, this review portrays the qualitative articles published in three sport psychology journals and examines how qualitative research can deepen our knowledge in applied sport psychology. Eighty-four of the 485 research ar-ticles published in these journals used a qualitative data collection technique. The interview was used in 67 studies. Peer review and reliability tests were often used for establishing trustworthiness. Member checking was mostly lim-ited to participant verification of interview transcripts. Results were usually presented using both words and numbers. Selected studies are discussed in relation to applied sport psychology knowledge. Published qualitative articles suggest a conservative effort by sport psychology researchers to include the qualitative approach as a legitimate way to do research. In sport psychology, numerous authors have advocated the value of qualita-tive research as an alternative to the traditional approach. This phenomenon reflects an interest in a "new paradigm" that allows researchers to study what people have to say and how they act (Tesch, 1990). In fact, it has been 15 years since Martens (1987) issued a call for qualita-tive research in sport psychology. At the time, Martens hypothesized that with an The Sport Psychologist, 2003, 17, 1-15 © 2003 Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc.
Article
Full-text available
A follow-up of the 1990s review of qualitative research articles published in three North American sport psychology journals (Culver, Gilbert, & Trudel, 2003) was conducted for the years 2000–2009. Of the 1,324 articles published, 631 were data-based and 183 of these used qualitative data collection techniques; an increase from 17.3% for the 1990s to 29.0% for this last decade. Of these, 31.1% employed mixed methods compared with 38.1% in the 1990s. Interviews were used in 143 of the 183 qualitative studies and reliability test reporting increased from 45.2% to 82.2%. Authors using exclusively quotations to present their results doubled from 17.9% to 39.9%. Only 13.7% of the authors took an epistemological stance, while 26.2% stated their methodological approach. We conclude that positivist/ postpositivist approaches appear to maintain a predominant position in sport psy-chology research. Awareness of the importance of being clear about epistemology and methodology should be a goal for all researchers. A review of qualitative research articles published in the 1990s provided a timely overview of how qualitative methods are employed, and how results are reported, in the field of sport and exercise psychology (Culver, Gilbert, & Trudel, 2003). That original review appears to have become a useful resource for sport and exercise psychology researchers, having been cited in at least 65 academic publications in the seven years since it was published (Google Scholar, n.d.). Although the original review has contributed to the continuing dialogue about research methods in sport and exercise psychology, no similar review has been completed for the decade following the 1990s. In the first decade of the 21st century qualitative research in
Article
The article "What works when working with athletes" by Fifer, Henschen, Gould, and Ravizza (2008) offers an interesting array of information and insights used by three highly experienced applied sport psychology consultants. This response article, however, contends that it may be possible to glean a further, and crucial, level of understanding by exploring the metacognition behind the selection of such courses of action. This may be provided through applied cognitive task analysis (ACTA) techniques to access the cognitive mechanisms underpinning professional practice. A suggested research direction is to use ACTA techniques such as in-depth interviews and cognitive mapping with highly experienced applied sport psychology consultants. Specifically, these techniques would enable readers to access judgments and decisions, attentional demands, critical cues and patterns, and problem solving strategies (Gore & McAndrew, 2009). This level of understanding may help to establish how these cognitive processes impact on the support provided to clients, and in turn, assist in developing more conceptually rigorous training methods.
Article
Most research into leadership has presented leaders as heroic, charismatic and transformational ‘visionaries’. The leader, whether in business, politics or any other field, is the most important factor in determining whether organizations succeed or fail. Indeed, despite the fundamental mistakes which have, arguably, directly led to global economic recession, it is often still taken for granted that transformational leadership is a good thing, and that leaders should have much more power than followers to decide what needs to be done.
Article
As part of the emergence of alternative research paradigms in exercise and sport psychology, we draw upon data from an ethnographic study of 3 individuals with schizophrenia to explore the use of exercise as an adjunct therapy for schizophrenia. A 10-week exercise program of twice-weekly sessions was implemented. Participant observation and interviews with participants and their assigned key-workers were the primary sources of data collection used. The influence of exercise on the lives of participants and their mental health and the underlying mechanisms of change were explored. Our findings indicate that exercise has the potential to help reduce participants' perceptions of auditory hallucinations, raise self-esteem, and improve sleep patterns and general behavior. The process of exercising, via the provision of distraction and social interaction rather than the exercise itself, was very influential in providing these benefits. In conclusion, we strongly recommend the inclusion of exercise as an adjunct treatment in psychiatric rehabilitation.
Article
Research on leadership has typically focused on the positive side of the phenomenon and its beneficial implications for organizations and followers. However, history and recent events, such as the collapses of Enron and Lehman Brothers, have illustrated that there are undeniably plenty of ‘dark’ leaders and that their behaviors can be disastrous for organizations. Research is only beginning to explore the characteristics and outcomes of destructive forms of leadership. This symposium will present an array of research that showcases the darker side of leadership and its effects on group outcomes. The papers in this symposium address several types of negative leadership styles, thereby emphasizing the many forms that negative leadership can take in organizations. Our symposium includes research that has explored leadership and its impacts at the team level as well as outcomes at an organizational level. More specifically, the first presentation will discuss the effects of entitlement on the relationship between emergent leadership and team performance outcomes, which sheds light on why entitlement is not always bad. The second presentation delves into the impact of transformational and pseudo-transformational of CEOs on team human capital and performance in baseball teams. The third paper examines abusive supervision as a team-level phenomenon (abusive climate) and how it impacts team level outcomes. The final presentation examines how in-group members’ acquisition of power may be gained through norm violation. Collectively, this symposium provides a unique contribution to the research on leadership in general and will improve our understanding of the dark side of leadership. • Abusive Supervision Climate: A Multiple Mediation Model of Its Impact on Group & Individual Outcomes • Presenter: Manuela Priesemuth; Wilfrid Laurier U. • Presenter: Marshall Schminke; U. of Central Florida • Presenter: Maureen L. Ambrose; U. of Central Florida • Presenter: Robert Folger; U. of Central Florida • Authentic vs. Pseudo-Transformational Leadership and Team Human Capital • Presenter: Christian J. Resick; Drexel U. • Presenter: Daniel Whitman; Louisiana State U. • Presenter: Marco S. DiRenzo; Naval Postgraduate School • Presenter: Steven M. Weingarden; Thinking Ahead, LLC • Entitlement in Leaders: Is It Really That Bad All the Time? • Presenter: Emma Y. Zhao; Melbourne U. • Presenter: Karen A. Jehn; Melbourne U. • Presenter: Carol Gill; Melbourne Business School • Obtaining Power by Breaking the Rules Is Not for Everyone: The Importance of Group Membership • Presenter: Astrid C. Homan; U. of Amsterdam • Presenter: Gerben A. Van Kleef; U. of Amsterdam