ArticlePDF Available

Intelligence and emotional disorders: Is the worrying and ruminating mind a more intelligent mind?

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Previous research has shown that anxiety and depression symptoms are negatively associated with measures of intelligence. However, this research has often not taken state distress and test anxiety into account, and recent findings indicate possible positive relationships between generalized anxiety disorder (GAD), worry, and intelligence. The present study examined the relationships between GAD, depression, and social anxiety symptoms, as well as their underlying cognitive processes of worry, rumination, and post-event processing, with verbal and non-verbal intelligence in an undergraduate sample (N = 126). While the results indicate that verbal intelligence has positive relationships with GAD and depression symptoms when test anxiety and state negative affect were taken into account, these relationships became non-significant when overlapping variance was controlled for. However, verbal intelligence was a unique positive predictor of worry and rumination severity. Non-verbal intelligence was a unique negative predictor of post-event processing. The possible connections between intelligence and the cognitive processes that underlie emotional disorders are discussed.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Short Communication
Intelligence and emotional disorders: Is the worrying and ruminating
mind a more intelligent mind?
Alexander M. Penney
, Victoria C. Miedema, Dwight Mazmanian
Department of Psychology, Lakehead University, 955 Oliver Road, Thunder Bay, ON P7B 5E1, Canada
article info
Article history:
Received 8 June 2014
Received in revised form 30 September
2014
Accepted 1 October 2014
Keywords:
Intelligence
Generalized anxiety disorder
Depression
Social anxiety disorder
Worry
Rumination
Post-event processing
abstract
Previous research has shown that anxiety and depression symptoms are negatively associated with
measures of intellige nce. However, this research has often not taken state distress and test anxiety into
account, and recent findings indicate possible positive relationships between generalized anxiety
disorder (GAD), worry, and intelligence. The present study examined the relationships between GAD,
depression, and social anxiety symptoms, as well as their underlying cognitive processes of worry,
rumination, and post-event processing, with verbal and non-verbal intelligence in an undergraduate
sample (N = 126). While the results indicate that verbal intelligence has positive relationships with
GAD and depression symptoms when test anxiety and state negative affect were taken into account,
these relationships became non-significant when overlapping variance was controlled for. However,
verbal intelligence was a unique positive predictor of worry and rumination severity. Non-verbal
intelligence was a unique negative predictor of post-event processing. The possible connections between
intelligence and the cognitive processes that underlie emotional disorders are discussed.
Ó 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
Intelligence has long been recognized as playing a key role in
human evolution. Adaptive emotional regulation is also considered
to be critically important for survival and reproduction (Darwin,
1872). More recently, some theorists have extrapolated the evolu-
tionary framework to encompass the maladaptive extremes of
emotions the emotional disorders (e.g., Gilbert, 1998, 2001;
Marks & Nesse, 1994). In this view, experiencing the ‘‘right’’
emotion (e.g., anxiety, sadness, or happiness), with the optimal
intensity and duration, in the correct context or situation, would
clearly enhance an organism’s fitness. Emotional disorders, there-
fore, represent the extreme and non-adaptive tails of a normal dis-
tribution of individual variability in emotional reactions. For
example, given the adaptive value of an emotion like anxiety,
which would permit an individual to anticipate and plan for poten-
tial threats, it seems clear that anxiety might have co-evolved with
increased intelligence. Moreover, given the potentially fatal costs
of ‘‘false negatives’’ in decision-making about threats, selection
pressures may have favoured errors in the other direction, or ‘‘false
positives’’. From an evolutionary standpoint, there are fewer costs
associated with worrying about a threatening event that does not
occur than failing to anticipate, plan for, or avoid one that does.
Relevant research exploring these relationships has provided
mixed results, however. Researchers have often found a negative
relationship between intelligence and emotional disorders, across
a diverse range of samples (Feldhusen & Klausmeier, 1962;
Kerrick, 1955; McCandless & Castaneda, 1956). A recent meta-anal-
ysis indicated that gifted children are less likely to have anxiety
than non-gifted children (Martin, Burns, & Schonlau, 2010). Multi-
ple studies have also found that depressed individuals score lower
on measures of processing speed and visual–spatial reasoning than
they do on measures of verbal intelligence (Kluger & Goldberg,
1990; Zillmer, Ball, Fowler, Newman, & Stutts, 1991). However, it
is possible that the symptoms of acute depression might decrease
an individual’s ability to perform optimally on an intelligence test,
and that the individual may not have lower intelligence. Aligning
with this, Ruisel (2000) argued that state anxiety and test anxiety
should be taken into account when interpreting the relationship
between anxiety and intelligence, and Moutafi, Furnham, and
Tsaousis (2006) found that test anxiety mediated the relationship
between neuroticism and intelligence. This research suggests that
the negative relationship between emotional disorders and intelli-
gence may be an artifact of the testing itself.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.10.005
0191-8869/Ó 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, MacEwan University, City
Centre Campus, 10700-104 Avenue, Edmonton, AB T5J 4S2, Canada. Tel.: +1 780 497
4165; fax: +1 780 497 5308.
E-mail addresses: apenney@lakeheadu.ca (A.M. Penney), vcmiedem@lakeheadu.
ca (V.C. Miedema), dwight.mazmanian@lakeheadu.ca (D. Mazmanian).
Personality and Individual Differences 74 (2015) 90–93
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Personality and Individual Differences
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid
Recent studies by Coplan et al. (2006, 2012) compared healthy
controls to individuals with generalized anxiety disorder (GAD),
and found that individuals with GAD had higher intelligence. Worry
severity also positively correlated with intelligence within the GAD
samples. Unfortunately, both studies had very small samples, and
the authors did not investigate the role of other cognitive processes.
While worry is the proposed cognitive process underlying GAD
(American Psychiatric Association, 2013), rumination and post-
event processing are thought to be the primary cognitive processes
involved in major depressive disorder and social anxiety disorder,
respectively (Clark & Wells, 1995; Nolen-Hoeksema, 2000).
This study sought to further examine the relationships between
emotional disorders and intelligence. Using a large undergraduate
sample, we examined the relationships of GAD, depression, and
social anxiety symptoms, as well as the relationships of worry,
rumination, and post-event processing, with verbal and non-verbal
intelligence while controlling for state negative affect and test
anxiety.
2. Materials and methods
2.1. Participants
A total of 126 undergraduate students participated. The sample
consisted primarily of Caucasian (85.7%), young adult (M
age = 20.46, SD = 4.53) women (77.0%). This study was reviewed
and approved by the university’s research ethics board.
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Generalized Anxiety Disorder Questionnaire-IV (GADQ-IV;
Newman et al., 2002)
The GADQ-IV is a 9-item self-report measure, with higher
scores indicating a higher amount of GAD symptoms. The GADQ-
IV demonstrates strong convergent and divergent validity, as well
as good internal consistency.
2.2.2. Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ; Meyer, Miller, Metzger,
& Borkovec, 1990)
The PSWQ is a 16-item self-report questionnaire. The PSWQ has
been found to have high internal consistency, and high content
validity. Higher scores indicate more frequent worries.
2.2.3. Centre for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D;
Radloff, 1977)
The CES-D is a 20-item self-report measure. Higher scores indi-
cate more frequent depressive symptoms. The CES-D has high
internal consistency, high content validity, and moderate conver-
gent validity.
2.2.4. Ruminative Responses Scale-Brooding and Reflection (RRS-BR;
Treynor, Gonzalez, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2003)
Higher scores on the RRS-BR indicate more frequent rumina-
tion. The RRS-BR is a 10-item self-report questionnaire, which
has been found to have high internal consistency, and strong con-
vergent validity.
2.2.5. Social Phobia Inventory (SPIN; Connor et al., 2000)
The SPIN is a 17-item self-report measure, with higher scores
corresponding to more intense social anxiety symptoms. The SPIN
has excellent internal consistency and good convergent validity.
2.2.6. Post-Event Processing Questionnaire-Revised (PEPQ-R; McEvoy
& Kingsep, 2006)
The PEPQ-R is an 8-item self-report questionnaire. The PEPQ-R
has been found to have high internal consistency and moderate
convergent validity. Higher scores indicate more frequent and
intense post-event processing.
2.2.7. Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI; Wechsler, 2008)
A VCI score was calculated for each participant using the Simi-
larities, Comprehension, and the Vocabulary subscales from the
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV;
Wechsler, 2008). Raw scores on each of the three scales were con-
verted into scaled scores and transformed according to the rules
specified in the WAIS-IV manual. Higher scores on the VCI indicate
higher verbal intelligence. The subscales and the VCI have been
shown to have excellent psychometric properties.
2.2.8. Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices (SPM; Raven, Raven, &
Court, 2000)
The SPM is a series of five matrices sets with a part missing. Par-
ticipants select a pattern that they believe completes the overall
design. The SPM has excellent psychometric properties and higher
scores indicate higher non-verbal intelligence.
2.2.9. Positive and Negative Affect Schedule Negative Affect subscale
(PANAS-NA; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988)
The PANAS-NA is a 10-item self-report measure, with higher
scores indicating more intense state negative affect. The PANAS-
NA has demonstrated high internal consistency, convergent valid-
ity, discriminant validity, and construct validity.
2.2.10. Cognitive Test Anxiety Scale (CTAS; Cassady & Johnson, 2002)
The CTAS is a 27-item self-report measure. The CTAS demon-
strates high levels of internal consistency, stability, and predictive
validity. Higher scores indicate more severe test anxiety.
2.3. Procedure
After expressing interest in the study, potential participants
met individually with one of the primary researchers, or one of
seven research assistants. The primary researchers provided exten-
sive training to the research assistants on how to complete the
WAIS-IV subscales and the SPM. Participants were fully informed
of the nature of the study, and then completed a demographic
characteristics questionnaire, followed by the measures in the fol-
lowing order: the PEPQ-R, the CES-D, the WAIS-IV: Similarities, the
SPIN, the PSWQ, the WAIS-IV: Comprehension, the PANAS-NA,
the CTAS, the WAIS-IV: Vocabulary, the RRS-BR, the GADQ-IV,
and the SPM.
2.4. Statistical analyses
Partial correlations were first examined between the VCI and
SPM and the symptom measures, as well as between the VCI and
SPM and the cognitive process measures, controlling for scores
on the PANAS-NA and the CTAS. To examine if the associations
between the VCI and SPM and the measures of interest were
unique (i.e., not due to overlapping variance among measures),
hierarchical regression analyses were conducted.
3. Results and discussion
When controlling for test anxiety and state negative affect, the
VCI positively partially correlated (pr) with the GADQ-IV,
pr(122) = .18, p = .045, and with the CES-D, pr(122) = .20, p = .023.
The VCI also positively correlated with the PSWQ, pr(122) = .21,
A.M. Penney et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 74 (2015) 90–93
91
p = .018, and the RRS-BR, pr(122) = .24, p = .007. The VCI did not
correlate with the SPIN or the PEPQ-R, ps > .590. The SPM nega-
tively correlated with the PEPQ-R, pr(122) = .20, p = .027, but
did not correlate with any other measure, ps > .085. Table 1 reports
the results of the hierarchical regression analyses. The VCI was a
unique positive predictor of the PSWQ and RRS-BR, while the
SPM was a unique negative predictor of the PEPQ-R. The results
of this study indicate that verbal intelligence is positively associ-
ated with the tendency to worry and ruminate. Non-verbal intelli-
gence, on the other hand, is negatively associated with the
tendency to process past social events.
Overall, the results of this study support and extend the find-
ings of Coplan et al. (2006, 2012). While Coplan et al. (2006,
2012) found a positive association between worry and intelligence
only in a clinical sample, the current study extended this finding to
a non-clinical sample. The present findings have also revealed that
rumination is positively related to verbal intelligence. However,
post-event processing was negatively related to non-verbal intelli-
gence. It is possible that more verbally intelligent individuals are
able to consider past and future events in greater detail, leading
to more intense rumination and worry. Individuals with higher
non-verbal intelligence may be stronger at processing the non-ver-
bal signals from individuals they interact with in the moment,
leading to a decreased need to re-process past social encounters.
Previous studies in this area found a negative relationship
between intelligence and anxiety and depression (e.g., Feldhusen
& Klausmeier, 1962; Kluger & Goldberg, 1990). By controlling for
state distress and test anxiety, this study found positive correla-
tions between GAD and depression symptoms and verbal intelli-
gence, although the relationship was lost when controlling for
overlapping variance. No relationship was found between social
anxiety symptoms and verbal or non-verbal intelligence. These
findings indicate that while intelligence may be related to the
symptoms of emotional disorders, it is more strongly linked to
the cognitive processes that underlie these disorders.
One additional implication that arises from this study is that
future researchers who wish to examine the relationship between
intelligence and various psychological variables, such as psychopa-
thology, cognitive processes, or personality, should consider the
role of state negative affect (i.e., current emotional state) and test
anxiety. This study illustrates that these variables may play a
significant role in the observed strength and direction of the
relationships higher-level constructs have with intelligence.
The present study was not without limitations. The sample for
this study was undergraduate students, with few individuals over
the age of 30 participating. It is difficult to be certain how the
results obtained from this sample would generalize to older popu-
lations. However, this did not restrict the range of VCI scores,
which ranged from 74 to 130. Similarly the range of the emotional
disorder symptoms varied considerably, with the mean scores for
the sample at or above the empirical cut-offs for the GADQ-IV,
SPIN, and CES-D. Yet, without clinical interviews it is unclear
how many participants would have met diagnostic criteria for
the emotional disorders examined in this study.
The present study examined the relationships between verbal
and non-verbal intelligence and the symptoms and cognitions of
emotional disorders. Although only small positive correlations
between verbal intelligence and the symptoms of GAD and depres-
sion were found, positive associations between verbal intelligence
and worry and rumination and a negative association between
non-verbal intelligence and post-event processing emerged. Future
studies are needed to provide a thorough explanation and interpre-
tation of the relationships between these cognitive processes and
intelligence. However, these preliminary results indicate that a
worrying and ruminating mind is a more verbally intelligent mind;
a socially ruminative mind, however, might be less able to process
non-verbal information.
Authors’ note
We would like to acknowledge Stephanie Cottrell for her
assistance with preparation of study materials, data collection,
and data entry. We would also like to thank Alyssa Mervin for
preparation of study materials and data collection, and Kimberly
Ongaro, Amy Killen, Sarah Kaukinen, Dylan Antoniazzi, and
Matthew Nordlund for assisting with data collection.
Declaration of conflicting interests
The authors declare no potential conflicts of interest with
respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this
article.
Funding
This study was not funded by any external funding source.
Table 1
Hierarchical regression analyses.
Variable R Adjusted R
2
R
2
change tpr
Dependent variable: GADQ-IV
Step 1 .60 .35 .36
**
PANAS-NA 3.63
**
.31
**
CTAS 6.36
**
.50
**
Step 2 .67 .43 .09
**
SPIN 1.00 .09
CES-D 4.21
**
.36
**
Step 3 .68 .43 .01
VCI 1.26 .11
Dependent variable: CES-D
Step 1 .53 .26 .28
*
PANAS-NA 4.96
**
.41
**
CTAS 3.39
**
.29
**
Step 2 .61 .35 .10
**
SPIN 0.61 .05
GADQ-IV 4.21
**
.36
**
Step 3 .62 .36 .01
VCI 1.67 .15
Dependent variable: PSWQ
Step 1 .61 .36 .36
**
PANAS-NA 4.28
**
.36
**
CTAS 6.07
**
.48
**
Step 2 .68 .44 .09
**
PEPQ-R 3.65
**
.31
**
RRS-BR 1.28 .11
Step 3 .69 .46 .02
*
VCI 2.17
*
.19
*
Dependent variable: RRS-BR
Step 1 .42 .16 .18
**
PANAS-NA 4.16
**
.35
**
CTAS 1.94 .17
Step 2 .51 .24 .09
**
PEPQ-R 2.85
*
.25
*
PSWQ 1.28 .11
Step 3 .55 .27 .04
*
VCI 2.52
*
.22
*
Dependent variable: PEPQ-R
Step 1 .43 .17 .18
PANAS-NA 2.80
*
.24
*
CTAS 3.63
**
.31
**
Step 2 .58 .31 .15
PSWQ 3.65
**
.31
**
RRS-BR 2.85
*
.25
*
Step 3 .61 .35 .04
SPM 2.79
*
.25
*
*
p < .05.
**
p < .001.
92 A.M. Penney et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 74 (2015) 90–93
References
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Cassady, J. C., & Johnson, R. E. (2002). Cognitive test anxiety and academic
performance. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 27, 270–295. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1006/ceps.2001.1094.
Clark, D. M., & Wells, A. (1995). A cognitive model of social phobia. In R. Heimberg,
M. Liebowitz, D. A. Hope, & F. R. Schneier (Eds.), Social phobia: Diagnosis,
assessment, and treatment (pp. 69–93). New York: Guilford Press.
Connor, K. M., Davidson, J. R., Churchill, L. E., Sherwood, A., Foa, E., & Weisler, R. H.
(2000). Psychometric properties of the social phobia inventory (SPIN): New self-
rating scale. British Journal of Psychiatry, 176, 379–386. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1192/bjp.176.4.379.
Coplan, J. D., Hodulik, S., Mathew, S. J., Mao, X., Hof, P. R., Gorman, J., et al. (2012).
The relationship between intelligence and anxiety: An association with
subcortical white matter metabolism. Frontiers in Evolutionary Neuroscience, 3.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnevo.2011.00008. Article 8 (7pp.).
Coplan, J. D., Mathew, S. J., Mao, X., Smith, E. L., Hof, P. R., Coplan, P. M., et al. (2006).
Decreased choline and creatine concentrations in centrum semiovale in
patients with generalized anxiety disorder: Relationship to IQ and early
trauma. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 147, 27–39. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.pscychresns.2005.12.011.
Darwin, C. A. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. London: John
Murray
.
Feldhusen, J. F., & Klausmeier, H. J. (1962). Anxiety, intelligence, and achievement in
children of low, average, and high intelligence. Child Development, 33, 403–409.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1126453.
Gilbert, P. (1998). The evolved basis and adaptive functions of cognitive distortions.
British Journal of Medical Psychology, 71, 447–463. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
j.2044-8341.1998.tb01002.x.
Gilbert, P. (2001). Evolution and social anxiety: The role of attraction, social
competition, and social hierarchies. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 24,
723–751. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0193-953X(05)70260-4.
Kerrick, J. S. (1955). Some correlates of the Taylor Manifest Anxiety scale. The
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 50, 75–77. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
h0040104.
Kluger, A., & Goldberg, E. (1990). IQ patterns in affective disorder, lateralized and
diffuse brain damage. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12,
182–194. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01688639008400966.
Marks, I. M., & Nesse, R. M. (1994). Fear and fitness: An evolutionary analysis of
anxiety disorders. Ethology and Sociobiology, 15, 247–261. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/0162-3095(94)90002-7.
Martin, L. T., Burns, R. M., & Schonlau, M. (2010). Mental disorders among gifted and
nongifted youth: A selected review of the epidemiologic literature. Gifted Child
Quarterly, 54, 31–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0016986209352684.
McCandless, B. R., & Castaneda, A. (1956). Anxiety in children, school achievement,
and intelligence. Child Development, 27, 379–382. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/
1126207.
McEvoy, P. M., & Kingsep, P. (2006). The post-event processing questionnaire in a
clinical sample with social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44,
1689–1697. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2005.12.005.
Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., & Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and
validation of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 28, 487–495. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(90)90135-6.
Moutafi, J., Furnham, A., & Tsaousis, I. (2006). Is the relationship between
intelligence and trait neuroticism mediated by test anxiety? Personality and
Individual Differences, 40, 587–597. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.paid.2005.08.004.
Newman, M. G., Zuellig, A. R., Kachin, K. E., Constantino, M. J., Przeworski, A.,
Erickson, T., et al. (2002). Preliminary reliability and validity of the Generalized
Anxiety Disorder Questionnaire-IV: A revised self-report diagnostic measure of
generalized anxiety disorder. Behavior Therapy, 33, 215–233. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/S0005-7894(02)80026-0.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). The role of rumination in depressive disorders and
mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109,
504–511. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.109.3.504.
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in
the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1177/014662167700100306.
Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (2000). Standard progressive matrices. San
Antonio, TX: Pearson
.
Ruisel, I. (2000). Is the anxiety–intelligence relationship relevant? Studia
Psychologica, 42, 3–13.
Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered:
A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 247–259. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1023910315561.
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief
measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
0022-3514.54.6.1063.
Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Fourth Edition. San Antonio,
TX: Psychological Corporation
.
Zillmer, E. A., Ball, J. D., Fowler, P. C., Newman, A. C., & Stutts, M. L. (1991). Wechsler
Verbal-Performance IQ discrepancies among psychiatric inpatients:
Implications for subtle neuropsychological dysfunctioning. Archives of Clinical
Neuropsychiatry, 6, 61–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0887-6177(91)90022-2.
A.M. Penney et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 74 (2015) 90–93
93
... Pollet and Schnell (2017) find that people with superior intelligence rate themselves as having a substantially lower sense of meaningful life and happiness than ordinary people. Penney et al. (2015) also suggest that generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) was positively correlated with higher levels of intelligence. ...
... Second, outstanding cognitive ability may lead people to focus on their inner world so that they are often alone, lack interest in real life, and pay less attention to family (Lecklider, 2013). Finally, some suggest that intelligent people are more likely to be aware of the world's imperfections and feel more deeply about life's shortcomings (Pollet & Schnell, 2017), making them more prone to depression and anxiety (Penney et al., 2015). However, based on the rich information on individual subjective perception provided by the CFPS, each of these hypotheses is refuted by empirical evidence in our study. ...
... bliss and that intelligent people are more likely to be aware of the world's imperfections. It, in turn, may lead to a more profound sense of life's shortcomings and unhappiness (Penney et al., 2015). If this assumption is correct, one would expect a positive association between cognitive ability and psychological depression status, which is not supported by the results in Table 11. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study examines the association between cognitive ability and happiness and presents three main findings using a nationally representative sample from China. First, there is only a tiny and insignificant correlation between cognitive ability and happiness. Second, cognitive ability is not associated with greater expectations of worldly success or disregarding real life. Third, people with higher cognitive ability have advantages regarding objective physical and mental health, income level, and social status. Still, they tend to underestimate their relative socioeconomic position and have less confidence about their future, which inhibits their subjective well-being. This study contributes to understanding the new human capital theory and explores the underlying causes of perceptual bias in socioeconomic status.
... This strategy has been previously accomplished in a large national sample , and facilitates the inclusion of marginalized subgroups which would be underrepresented in a typical research sample. Additionally, given evidence of linkages between cognitive abilities (e.g., intelligence quotient [IQ]) and social, emotional, and behavioral profiles (Brislin et al., 2022;Guénolé et al., 2013;Karpinski et al., 2018;Penney et al., 2015), as well as of overlap between gender diversity and polygenic propensity for greater cognitive performance (Thomas et al., 2022), we investigate how cognitive abilities relate to the experience and perception of SGM stigmatization in transgender youth. Finally, we probe whether older age relates to greater cumulative experience and report of stigmatization. ...
... A broad body of research has reported linkages between general cognitive abilities and a range of social, behavioral, and emotional problems (Brislin et al., 2022;Guénolé et al., 2013;Karpinski et al., 2018;Penney et al., 2015). As highlighted in a systematic review by Tasca et al. (2022), these relationships are complex. ...
... As highlighted in a systematic review by Tasca et al. (2022), these relationships are complex. A range of studies have found that higher-level cognitive abilities, measured as IQ, verbal comprehension index (VCI), or a general neurocognition factor score, are associated with elevated internalizing problems (e.g., Brislin et al., 2022;Karpinski et al., 2018;Penney et al., 2015); these studies have examined higher-level cognitive abilities both categorically (e.g., IQs in the upper 2% of the population; Karpinski et al., 2018) and continuously (Brislin et al., 2022;Penney et al., 2015). A metaanalysis by Edirisooriya et al. (2021) found that higher IQ (measured continuously or categorically) was positively associated with depression diagnosis and symptomatology in autistic adolescents, specifically. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Sexual and gender minority (SGM) stigmatization is a key factor related to transgender adolescent mental health. While previous research has focused on direct associations between stigmatization and mental health, the present study of transgender youth, equitably recruited across the autism spectrum, examines cognitive and developmental factors in relation to the self- report of experienced and perceived SGM stigmatization. Method: 65 binary transgender adolescents (43% transfeminine; ages 13–21 years) were intentionally recruited across the spectrum of autism traits from no traits to full criteria autism. Participants completed measures of autism-related social differences, cognitive abilities, and self- reported directly experienced and perceived SGM stigma. Autism-related social differences, cognitive abilities, and age were studied in relation to both SGM stigma factors. Results: Autism-related social differences were negatively associated with level of directly experienced SGM stigma but unassociated with perceived stigma. Greater cognitive ability was positively associated with level of perceived SGM stigma, but unassociated with report of directly experienced stigma. Older age was positively associated with level of perceived SGM stigma. There was a statistical trend toward older age positively associated with level of directly experienced stigma. Conclusions: The present study identifies candidate cognitive and developmental influences on self-reported SGM stigmatization among transgender adolescents, evenly recruited across the autism spectrum. The factors which may impact the perception and experience of stigmatization have been notably under-explored in the mental health field. The examination of these individual characteristics may allow for more precise predictive models for research with transgender youth, and ultimately, in clinical care.
... Thus, cognition may serve as a valuable resource for children to rely on when faced with stressors that increase the risk of psychopathology. In the context of anxiety, findings between IQ and anxiety are mixed, with a body of research finding positive associations between IQ and anxiety symptoms in adults (Penney et al., 2015). The role cognitive ability plays in the development of psychopathology in young children with and without anxiety, however, remains untested. ...
... For children with anxiety, this greater understanding of their environment might result in more rumination and worry. Among adults with generalized anxiety disorder, for example, worry and rumination over past events have been positively correlated with cognitive ability (Coplan et al., 2012;Penney et al., 2015). Since worry and rumination are cognitive processes, individuals with higher cognitive ability may also have more cognitive resources to engage in maladaptive thinking, increasing the risk for further psychopathology. ...
Article
Preschool anxiety is highly prevalent and well known to predict risk for future psychopathology. The present study explores whether a diagnosis of an anxiety disorder in preschool interacts with (a) social skills and (b) cognitive ability to longitudinally predict psychopathology, two well-known protective factors, among a sample of 207 children measured at preschool (Mage = 4.34 years) and early childhood (Mage = 6.61 years). To assess social skills and cognitive ability, we utilized the Social Skills Rating Scale and the Differential Abilities Scale, respectively. To assess psychopathology, we utilized the parent report of the Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment. Hierarchical linear regression models revealed significant interactions between both social skills and cognitive ability with preschool anxiety. We observed that social skills protected against emergent psychopathology for both children with and without anxiety, although this association was stronger for children with preschool anxiety. Contrastingly, cognitive ability served as a protective factor against future psychopathology primarily among children without preschool anxiety. Results from this study identify targets for future intervention and inform our understanding of how preschool anxiety, a common disorder among young children, shapes future psychopathology risk in childhood.
... Studies have demonstrated that verbal intelligence is a predictor of rumination severity, so a ruminating mind is a more verbally intelligent mind (57). ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Severe depression is a prevalent psychiatric illness in children and adolescents associated with high levels of morbidity, disability, and a high risk of suicidal behavior. Cognitive factors associated with depression severity in juveniles have been poorly reported. Methods We investigated the relationship between depression severity and intelligence quotient (IQ)with its subscales in 65 juveniles (aged 10–17 years) with a current major depressive episode evaluated at the Mood Disorder Program of Bambino Gesù Children’s Hospital in Rome. Pearson’s correlation analyses were followed by a Benjamini–Hochberg correction and linear multivariable regression model. Results Depression severity measured with the total score of the Children’s Depression Rating Scale-Revised (CDRS-R) was positively associated with the Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI; Pearson’s r = 0.309 [0.042−0.534]; p = 0.024). The CDRS-R subscales positively associated with VCI by Pearson’s correlation were depressed feelings, suicidal ideation, excessive weeping, and reduced facial expressions. Suicidal ideation was the only factor independently and significantly associated with higher VCI in the multivariable linear regression model. Discussion Suicidal ideation was significantly and independently associated with higher verbal comprehension, indicating that depressed juveniles with better verbal ability may be at a greater risk of showing suicidal ideation.
... Another study found that participants with high levels of neuroticism who also had high levels of emotional intelligence were able to successfully mediate their levels of task anxiety better than those with low emotional intelligence (Smith, Saklofske, & Nordstokke, 2014). Verbal intelligence specifically has been found to correlate with higher levels of rumination in a non-depressed sample (Penney, Miedema, & Mazmanian, 2015). This link between general intelligence and ruminative cognition has been proposed before in the Analytical Rumination Hypothesis (Andrews & Thomson, 2009), which suggested that rumination may actually be an adaptive strategy of the mind that is meant to increase the number of cognitive resources allocated to problem solving, which can subsequently result in the cardinal symptoms of depression. ...
Preprint
The anxious-misery spectrum of disorders encompasses some of the most debilitating mental disorders that effect the population at-large. In addition, neuroticism is a trait that has been shown to correlate with higher levels of reported symptomatology for individuals diagnosed with anxious-misery disorders. However, it is less understood which of these might be more predictive of the cognitive impairments typically associated with mental illness. Therefore, we selected a group of participants with high trait neuroticism and comorbid anxious-misery symptoms. Using confirmatory factor analysis, we loaded the results of seventeen neuropsychological measures from each participant onto five distinct factors that exists on the anxious-misery spectrum: Depression, Anxiety, Impulsivity, Insomnia, and Somatizing. These factors were then compared to the results that participants had on to neurocognitive batteries: the University of Pennsylvania Computerized Neurocognitive Test Battery as well and the National Institute of Health Toolbox. Contrary to our expectations, we found that those in the top quartile of depressive symptom severity performed better than controls on working memory measures. These results are thought to be the result of correlations that have been shown previously between high levels of neuroticism and higher levels of intelligence
Article
Full-text available
Cognitive models of social anxiety disorder (SAD) emphasize post-event processing as a prominent maintaining factor that occurs after social-evaluative events. Post-event processing involves repetitive negative thinking revolved around perceived social failure. The present review concentrates on the relevant and available empirical literature on post-event processing in social anxiety which centres on Clarke and Wells (1995) theoretical framework. Correlational and experimental studies have investigated the relationship between post-event processing and the behavioural, physiological, cognitive and affective outcomes for socially anxious individuals. The majority of study designs include those investigating post-event processing in response to social-evaluative threat, and in response to treatment. Limitations of the existing literature are discussed and suggestions for future research examining the underlying cognitive functions of post-event processing are proposed.
Article
Introduction: Since the COVID-19 pandemic, many individuals have turned to peer-led online depression support groups (DSGs) to cope with depressive symptoms. However, relatively little research has articulated how use of DSGs may relate to specific behaviors associated with depression. This study assessed healthy behaviors (healthy support-seeking, healthy normalization, problem-solving) and unhealthy behaviors (unhealthy support-seeking, adverse normalization, co-rumination) associated with the use of peer-led DSGs. Method: Participants (N = 525) were recruited through the Qualtrics platform, and measures of depressive symptoms, self-reported social media use, DSG use, and healthy/unhealthy DSG behaviors were administered online. Path analysis estimated the relations of DSG use to each of the six behaviors. Results: Results showed that all six DSG behaviors were positively associated with DSG use (ps < .001). Standardized coefficients ranged from 0.18 to 0.49 for healthy outcomes and from 0.28 to 0.36 for the unhealthy outcomes. Greater use of DSGs was associated with higher levels of all healthy and unhealthy behaviors. Education level moderated the relation of DSG use to the three unhealthy behaviors, with higher education leading to stronger relations between DSG use and all three unhealthy behaviors. Discussion: These findings suggest that DSGs may be associated with both healthy and unhealthy effects on their users.
Article
Full-text available
تهدف الدراسة الحالية إلى فحص العلاقة بين الذكاءات المتعددة وممارسة الأنشطة السارة وشدة السعادة المدركة لدى طلاب الجامعة. وقد أجريت هذه الدراسة على عينة قوامها 574 طالبًا وطالبة من أربع كليات بجامعة القاهرة هي كليات الآداب والحقوق والصيدلة والهندسة. وأجاب المشاركون في الدراسة عن مقياسين، يختص المقياس الأول بقياس سبعة أنواع من الذكاءات وفقاً لنموذج جاردنر هى الذكاء اللغوي والذكاء الموسيقي والذكاء الاجتماعي والذكاء الشخصي والذكاء الحركي والذكاء الحسابي المنطقي والذكاء البصري المكاني، بينما يختص المقياس الثاني بقياس تكرار ممارسة الأنشطة السارة وشدة السعادة المدركة في سبعة مجالات هي المجال الديني، والثقافي، والاجتماعي، والرياضي، والفني، والأكاديمي، والترويحي. وقد بينت النتائج تباين الارتباطات بين الأنواع المختلفة للذكاء ومدى تكرار الأنشطة السارة وشدة السعادة المدركة في مجالاتها المختلفة، كما أوضحت تباين الأوزان النسبية لأنواع الذكاءات في التنبؤ بتكرار الأنشطة السارة وشدة السعادة المدركة. الكلمات المفتاحية: الذكاءات المتعددة؛ الذكاء؛ السعادة المدركة؛ الأنشطة السارة.
Article
Full-text available
تهدف الدراسة الحالية إلى فحص العلاقة بين الذكاءات المتعددة وممارسة الأنشطة السارة وشدة السعادة المدركة لدى طلاب الجامعة. وقد أجريت هذه الدراسة على عينة قوامها 574 طالبًا وطالبة من أربع كليات بجامعة القاهرة هي كليات الآداب والحقوق والصيدلة والهندسة. وأجاب المشاركين في الدراسة عن مقياسين، يختص المقياس الأول بقياس سبعة أنواع من الذكاءات وفقاً لنموذج جاردنر هى الذكاء اللغوي والذكاء الموسيقي والذكاء الاجتماعي والذكاء الشخصي والذكاء الحركي والذكاء الحسابي المنطقي والذكاء البصري المكاني، بينما يختص المقياس الثاني بقياس تكرار ممارسة الأنشطة السارة وشدة السعادة المدركة في سبعة مجالات هي المجال الديني، والثقافي، والاجتماعي، والرياضي، والفني، والأكاديمي، والترويحي. وقد بينت النتائج تباين الارتباطات بين الأنواع المختلفة للذكاء ومدى تكرار الأنشطة السارة وشدة السعادة المدركة في مجالاتها المختلفة، كما أوضحت تباين الأوزان النسبية لأنواع الذكاءات في التنبؤ بتكرار الأنشطة السارة وشدة السعادة المدركة.
Article
Professional immigrants from South Asian countries make a rapidly growing percentage of the US workforce. High-functioning anxiety is a latent mental state coupled with challenges faced due to their intersectional identities. This study examines the effectiveness of an online meditation lessons (OML) intervention in mitigating anxieties, enabling socio-cultural adaptation and wellbeing of professional US-based Indian immigrants as compared to online support group sharing sessions (OSGSS). OML attendees reported lower anxieties, better socio-cultural adaptation, self-construal, wellbeing, and psychological flourishing. OSGSS participants also reported statistically significant gains in socio-cultural adaptation scores. Latent class analysis revealed eight subgroups of participants likely to gain the most from OML: men, with doctoral and postdoctoral qualifications, university academics, medical professionals, currently married, cohabiting, above benchmark OML attendees and regular homework doers. With some refinements addressing specific issues of women, banking, and IT sector professionals, and singles, OML would be impactful for professional Indian immigrants to the US.
Article
Full-text available
Several studies have shown that people who engage in ruminative responses to depressive symptoms have higher levels of depressive symptoms over time, after accounting for baseline levels of depressive symptoms. The analyses reported here showed that rumination also predicted depressive disorders, including new onsets of depressive episodes. Rumination predicted chronicity of depressive disorders before accounting for the effects of baseline depressive symptoms but not after accounting for the effects of baseline depressive symptoms. Rumination also predicted anxiety symptoms and may be particularly characteristic of people with mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms.
Article
The conceptual bases of current research of intelligence and anxiety are presented. In researching intelligence, it is necessary to pay greater attention not only to mechanics and pragmatics but to reflexics of intelligence as well. Of all the research concepts of anxiety, attention is paid mainly to models of state and trait anxiety, to the interaction model and to the the test anxiety model. So far, mainly the negative effect of state anxiety on cognitive performance has been proven. Test anxiety usually lowers academic performance, mainly by increased self-observance which results in lack of self-confidence, fear of failure and evaluation, decreased concentration of attention and decreased resistance to stress. It seems necessary to pay more attention to alternating manipulation with dependent and independent variables, but mainly to the shift from predominantly forced psychometric intelligence to a more detailed study of intelligent behavior in real life conditions.
Article
Given the ongoing debate over whether giftedness is associated with mental health disorders, there is a great need to highlight and compare results from the most methodologically rigorous studies. Surprisingly, the vast majority of literature reviews and background sections of research articles include studies that do not directly compare gifted and nongifted youth. Furthermore, almost no attention has been paid to how differences in the definitions of giftedness or mental health outcomes of interest affect the interpretation and comparison of studies. The authors apply an epidemiologic perspective for thinking about these issues, highlight many of the challenges of studying this population, and present results from meta-analyses that include the most methodologically rigorous studies comparing rates of depression, bipolar disorder, anxiety, suicide ideation, and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) between gifted and nongifted youth. They conclude with recommendations to strengthen research in this area. Putting the research to use Through this review we sought to provide concrete recommendations to strengthen future research on the mental health of gifted children. Most notably, future studies should assess large, population-based cohorts that include both gifted and nongifted individuals. Researchers should also move toward describing their study population according to their specific aptitudes, talents, skills or abilities, rather than using the general term ‘gifted’. Thinking more specifically about these relationships will not only help us to understand the association between giftedness and mental health, but will also increase the potential to effectively shape programs and policies for gifted youth, focusing resources on the most vulnerable.
Article
This article outlines some basic ideas of an evolutionary approach to psychopathology. It focuses on human competition to be seen as attractive in order to elicit the investment of resources from others (e.g., approval, support, and care). It is argued that social anxiety may be a form of competitive anxiety, triggered in contexts where individuals see themselves as relatively low in the status hierarchy of desirable attributes and/or at risk of losing status (and control over social resources such as approval, help, and support) by being seen as having undesirable attributes. To improve (or defend) their position and garner the investments of others (e.g., win approval, support, friendships or status, or defend their status) requires a competitive venture; however, in attempting to compete, social phobics automatically recruit various evolved modules and mentalities for behaving in competitive arenas when one is low in the hierarchy (e.g., social comparison, placating dominant others and various submissive defenses such as concealment, high self-monitoring, and eye-gaze avoidance). These previously adaptive subordinate defenses interfere with status acquisition based on demonstrating attractive attributes to others.