ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Sexual desire can be operationalized as the motivation to seek out solitary or partnered sexual experiences. A large body of evidence suggests that men experience sexual desire more strongly and more frequently than do women; however, it is not clear whether sexual desire is truly gendered or if gender differences are influenced by how sexual desire is operationalized and assessed. Moreover, little research has examined similarities and differences in trait versus state sexual desire in women and men. Recent changes to the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) reflect the movement away from situating desire as the onset of the traditional linear model to framing desire as a state emerging from sexual excitement. We examine evidence for gender differences and similarities in trait and state sexual desire in both clinical and nonclinical populations. We conclude that sexual desire emerges similarly in women and men and that other factors may influence the observed gender difference in sexual desire. We then discuss the implications of conceptualizing desire as responsive for sexual medicine practitioners.
1 23
Current Sexual Health Reports
ISSN 1548-3584
Curr Sex Health Rep
DOI 10.1007/s11930-014-0027-5
Gender Differences and Similarities in
Sexual Desire
Samantha J.Dawson & Meredith
L.Chivers
1 23
Your article is protected by copyright and
all rights are held exclusively by Springer
Science+Business Media, LLC. This e-offprint
is for personal use only and shall not be self-
archived in electronic repositories. If you wish
to self-archive your article, please use the
accepted manuscript version for posting on
your own website. You may further deposit
the accepted manuscript version in any
repository, provided it is only made publicly
available 12 months after official publication
or later and provided acknowledgement is
given to the original source of publication
and a link is inserted to the published article
on Springer's website. The link must be
accompanied by the following text: "The final
publication is available at link.springer.com”.
CURRENT CONTROVERSIES (P KLEINPLATZ AND C MOSER, SECTION EDITORS)
Gender Differences and Similarities in Sexual Desire
Samantha J. Dawson &Meredith L. Chivers
#Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2014
Abstract Sexual desire can be operationalized as the motiva-
tion to seek out solitary or partnered sexual experiences. A
large body of evidence suggests that men experience sexual
desire more strongly and more frequently than do women;
however, it is not clear whether sexual desire is truly gendered
or if gender differences are influenced by how sexual desire is
operationalized and assessed. Moreover, little research has
examined similarities and differences in trait versus state
sexual desire in women and men. Recent changes to the fifth
edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders (DSM-5) reflect the movement away from situating
desire as the onset of the traditional linear model to framing
desire as a state emerging from sexual excitement. We exam-
ine evidence for gender differences and similarities in trait and
state sexual desire in both clinical and nonclinical populations.
We conclude that sexual desire emerges similarly in women
and men and that other factors may influence the observed
gender difference in sexual desire. We then discuss the impli-
cations of conceptualizing desire as responsive for sexual
medicine practitioners.
Keywords Sexual desire .Gender differences .Gender
similarities .Sexual motivation .Sex drive .Sexual interest .
Incentive motivation model .Trait sexual desire .State sexual
desire
Introduction
Researchers and clinicians use myriad terms to describe the
desire to seek out sexual partners or to engage in sexual
activities (e.g., partnered intercourse or masturbation), such
as libido, sexual interest, sex drive, sexual appetite, and sexual
motivation [15]. In addition to terminology differences, there
is evidence supporting different types of sexual desire; for
example, solitary versus dyadic, state versus trait. In this
review, we first discuss the gender difference observed in
clinical settings for disorders of desire. We then engage in a
discussion of whether sexual desire should be characterized as
a trait or whether it is best conceptualized as an emotional
state, with consideration of how gender interacts with the state
of sexual desire. Despite a large body of literature suggesting
that men experience sexual desire more frequently and more
intensely than women [for a review see 6], using the concep-
tualization of desire as an emergent property of sexual arousal
[7,8] and data from laboratory research [e.g., 9••], we suggest
that sexual motivation arises similarly in women and men and
that other factors (e.g., measurement, gender norms, report
biases) influence whether or not gender differences are
observed.
Sexual Desire in Clinical Populations
Historically, sexual desire and arousal were conceived as
linearly related, whereby desire spontaneously initiated the
sexual response cycle, leading to subsequent arousal and
orgasm [10]. More recently, desire has been framed as emerg-
ing from the experience of sexual arousal, operating in a
circular, reciprocally reinforcing fashion [8,11]. Using this
contemporary framework,sexual desire and sexual arousal are
seen as responsive to sexual cues. This reconceptualization of
sexual desire is now reflected in recent changes to the fifth
This article is part of the Topical Collection on Current Controversies
S. J. Dawson :M. L. Chivers (*)
Department of Psychology, Queens University, 354 Humphrey Hall,
62 Arch Street, Kingston, ON K7L 3N6, Canada
e-mail: Meredith.Chivers@queensu.ca
S. J. Dawson
e-mail: 11sd49@queensu.ca
Curr Sex Health Rep
DOI 10.1007/s11930-014-0027-5
Author's personal copy
revision of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders (DSM-5) [7], whereby hypoactive sexual desire
disorder (HSDD) was expanded to include concomitant diffi-
culties with arousal into a new diagnosis called sexual interest
and arousal disorder (SIAD). The new polythetic criteria for
SIAD make it possible for women to receive a diagnosis based
on symptoms of low arousal, low desire, or a combination of
both. The impetus for this change was, in part, based on the
observed high comorbidity among arousal and desire disor-
ders in women, reported difficulties distinguishing arousal
from desire, and research suggesting that desire can precede
or emerge from arousal [for reviews see 12,13].
There is, however, some evidence to suggest that individ-
uals who have problems with arousal can be distinguished
from those who have problems with desire [1416]. A recent
study showed that women with low desire (HSDD) versus low
arousal (female sexual arousal disorder; FSAD) exhibited
different psychophysiological response patterns to sexual
stimuli. Specifically, women with FSAD, HSDD,
FSAD/HSDD, and controls each exhibited similar patterns
of genital response when exposed to sexual stimuli; however,
women with HSDD and FSAD/HSDD reported significantly
less arousal than the other groups [15]. Of note, women with
FSAD did not show impaired genital response, contrary to the
expectation that weak physiological arousal is characteristic of
the disorder. Kleinplatz [17] argues that women can distin-
guish arousal and desire and that expanding HSDD to include
difficulties with arousal may not be appropriate. However, it is
important to note that despite the somewhat unique patterns of
responding observed across clinical groups of women, women
with HSDD and FSAD were similar with respect to many
psychosocial factors (e.g., Female Sexual Function Index total
score, sexual inhibition and excitation scores, sexual attitudes,
and Beck Depression Inventory scores) [15]suggestingsig-
nificant overlap across these disorders.
In contrast, evidence from one study suggests that it is
possible to distinguish men with low desire from those with
impaired arousal based on psychophysiological patterns and
psychosocial variables [16]. This provides support for the
utility of separate diagnoses of disorders of arousal and desire
for men. The DSM-5 workgroup chose to retain the diagnosis
of HSDD for men, based on a paucity of empirical research
documenting the relationship between sexual arousal and
sexual desire difficulties in men [18].
Gender differences in the prevalence of low sexual desire
suggest that between 746 % of women [reviewed in 12],
compared to 120 % of men, experience difficulties with
desire [reviewed in 18], with fluctuations depending on sam-
ple characteristics (e.g., clinical versus community samples,
age, relationship status). Although not currently recognized in
the DSM-5 [7], the prevalence of hypersexuality (i.e., distress
caused by low control over excessive sexual thoughts, fanta-
sies, or behaviors) also demonstrates a gender difference. A
number of studies have reported that men experience dysreg-
ulated sexuality more frequently than women [1921]. For
example, Långström and Hanson [22]reportedthat12.1%of
men compared with 6.8 % of women in a nationally represen-
tative sample were classified as hypersexual based on their
self-reported sexual interests and behaviors.
Gender differences in the prevalence of disorders associat-
ed with sexual desirethat women appear to be overrepre-
sented for disorders associated with lower desire and men are
overrepresented for disorders associated with higher desire
are interesting and warrant explanation. Winters et al. [21]
found that measures of hypersexuality and high sexual desire
loaded onto one uniform factor, suggesting that hypersexual-
ity is one end of the desire spectrum, with hypoactive or low
sexual desire on the opposing end. Sensitivity to sexual cues
may provide clues to the gender difference in prevalence rates
for low versus high sexual desire. Bloemers et al. [23••]
reported that some women with low sexual desire exhibit
relatively low or blunted sensitivity to sexual cues, as assessed
using a behavioral task (variant of the emotional Stroop task
versus self-report [23••]). In a series of studies [23••,24], they
found that responsiveness could be augmented with testoster-
one (T) and phosphodiesterase type-5 inhibitor (PDE5i), with
T increasing sensitivity to sexual cues and PDE5i facilitating
genital blood flow, together resulting in improvements in
sexual desire [24]. Men, on the other hand, generally have
higher levels of circulating T than do women [25], and T is
associated with heightened sensitivity to sexual cues, showing
significant increases after exposure to sexual stimuli or sexual
targets [26,27]. Thus, higher levels of T in men may be
associated with greater sensitivity to sexual cues, leading them
to be more likely to exhibit symptoms consistent with high
sexual desire. These hypotheses (e.g., that hypersexual men
have greater levels of T) are tentative, and additional research
is needed to explore these possibilities.
The relationship between T and sexual desire is complex,
and caution should be exercised when hypothesizing relation-
ships between T and sexual desire. For example, there is a
general misconception that differences in womens and mens
T levels are responsible for the observed gender difference in
sexual desire [reviewed in 28]. Studies examining T and
sexual desire in healthy women yield mixed results [2931],
and studies in healthy men consistently find no evidence
linking T with sexual desire [29,3234]. Complicating mat-
ters further, the different types of sexual desire (i.e., solitary
versus dyadic) yield different associations with T in women
[35], and contextual factors (e.g., sexual activity) strongly
affect the association between T and sexual desire [31]. The
relationship between T and sexual desire is also dependent on
whether the T is protein-bound or unbound, with unbound T
exhibiting a stronger relationship with behavioral outcomes,
such as sexual desire [reviewed in 36]. Thus, differences in
baseline T are unlikely to be sufficient for fully explaining
Curr Sex Health Rep
Author's personal copy
gendered sexual desire in healthy women and men. Among
clinical groups, there is a body of evidence suggesting that the
administration of T among menopausal women [e.g., 37],
women with HSDD [e.g., 38], and hypogonadal men [e.g.,
39] significantly improves sexual functioning, including
levels of sexual desire; however, this does not mean that
differences in desire are caused by differences in T. Instead,
the examination of contextual factors and the different types of
sexual desire in women and men will likely yield more defin-
itive answers regarding these complex relationships.
Is Sexual Desire Best Characterized as a Trait or a State?
Trait theory, an influential guiding principle for understanding
personality, suggests that individuals possess a suite of char-
acteristicstraitsthat are relatively stable across time and
situations [40,41].Atraitcanbeconceptualizedasanindi-
viduals typical patterns of behavior, thought, or emotion [41].
Measures of traits, such as personality (e.g., extraversion),
demonstrate consistency across time points (e.g., from child-
hood through adulthood) and in different situations, indicative
of their temporal stability [40,41]. Furthermore, assessments
of personality traits demonstrate predictive validity for future
behavior [42]. Emotional states are exemplified by an indi-
viduals behaviors, thoughts, or emotions in a given moment;
states are therefore contextually dependent and can fluctuate
across time and situations, whereas traits are less likely to do
so [43]. Endler [44] proposed an interaction model for under-
standing the relationship between traits and states. He sug-
gested that the level of a state (e.g., sexual desire) is dependent
upon the personstrait and requires the presence of a specific
situation or cue (e.g., a sexual partner). The situation or cue
must be consistent with the trait in order to evoke the expected
increase in the state. For example, an erotic image of a woman
is likely to evoke a state of arousal and sexual desire in a
heterosexual man, whereas the same image is unlikely to elicit
arousal and desire in a gay man.
The most widely used assessment tool for assessing sexual
desire as a trait-like constructthe Sexual Desire Inventory
(SDI) [45]asks women and men to report their desired
frequency of sexual activity with a partner and with them-
selves and actual frequency of sexual thoughts with and
without a partner during the last month. The SDI yields two
related but distinct factors for women and men, representing a
desire for sexual activities with a partner (i.e., dyadic sexual
desire) and desire for sexual activity with oneself (i.e., solitary
sexual desire). Studies using this measure have consistently
shown a gender difference (medium effect size) in the male
direction for both types of sexual desire [28,45].
Despite the SDI being a valid measure of sexual desire, and
showing adequate discriminant and convergent validity [see
45], there has been no empirical investigation into whether
this measure assesses sexual desire as a trait or a state.
Reviewing the available evidence suggests that sexual desire
does not exhibit the degree of stability expected of a trait. For
example, womens sexual desire ebbs and flows and is more
fluid than it is stable; womens desire fluctuates across the
menstrual cycle [46,47], during pregnancy [48], and the
postpartum period [4951]aswellasperi-andpost-
menopausally [52,53]. Similarly, the examination of sexual
desire across the lifespan yields data suggesting that both
women and men report steady declines in sexual desire across
time [5456]. Evidence of situational stability of trait sexual
desire is also lacking [28]. Examination of desire across
relationships demonstrates that sexual desire fluctuates
throughout the course of a romantic relationship and can differ
between relationships with different people [56,57]. Addi-
tionally, there is a paucity of research investigating how state
and trait sexual desire interact. One study has examined this
relationship and found that self-reports of trait desire are
influenced by whether or not a person has been exposed to
sexual stimuli (i.e., experience state sexual desire) [58••],
suggesting that trait sexual desire in this study was subject to
state-like variability.
Recently, some researchers have shifted their focus to
examining sexual desire as a state, examining the contextual
factors influencing responsive desire. Three studies provide
empirical support for the conceptualization of desire as re-
sponsive to sexual cues, demonstrating rapid fluctuation in
feelings of sexual desire (within minutes) rather than desire
remaining stable across time [9••,58••,59]. Moreover, these
studies find that desire emerges similarly in women and men
[9••,58••,59]. Both et al. [59] found that women and men
responded with similar intensity in terms of their self-report of
lust (e.g., desire to make love and a desire to masturbate) after
viewing sexual versus nonsexual films. Dawson and Chivers
[9••] observed that heterosexual womens and mens self-
reported levels of dyadic and solitary sexual desire to audio-
visual stimuli depicting a preferred sexual stimulus (e.g.,
malefemale intercourse) were not significantly different from
one another. Goldey and van Anders [58••] reported a similar
finding, such that no gender difference was observed for
reports of solitary or dyadic sexual desire in response to three
different modalities of sexual stimuli (e.g., imagined social
situation exercise, sexual story, and sexual fantasy).
Data from these studies highlight a notable discrepancy in
terms of gender differences in desire; gender differences are
observed for measures of traitdesire, but not for state
desire. One possible explanation for this discrepancy is that
the assessment of state sexual desire, immediately after expo-
sure to a sexual stimulus, is less subject to retrospective
reporting biases that can affect recall of sexual experiences
and cognitions (e.g., over the past month). The current emo-
tional state of the person may also influence self-report.
Goldey and van Anders [58••] observed that reporting of
Curr Sex Health Rep
Author's personal copy
sexual desire was affected by engagement with sexual stimuli
and that this differed by gender. Exposure to sexual stimuli led
to increases in the reporting of trait sexual desire for women,
but not for men, suggesting that contextual factors influence
how desire is reported in women. There is other evidence
suggesting that women are less susceptible to impression
management, that is responding in a socially desirable manner
(in this case, lower desire and arousal, in keeping with gender
stereotypes of men having greater sexual desire than women)
[60], when in a sexually aroused state (i.e., after exposure to
sexual stimuli) [61].
It is possible that reporting biases influence whether or not
a gender difference is detected, such that recall biases are
activated when reporting trait sexual desire retrospectively
(e.g., during the previous month), whereas these are not pres-
ent when reporting sexual desire when in a sexually aroused
state (e.g., after a sexually arousing film). Interestingly, the
three studies reported above found that sexual stimuli elicited
similar ratings of responsive sexual desire in women and men,
suggesting that responsive sexual desiremay not be inherently
gendered. An alternate possibility is that the women recruited
for these sexuality studies are less representative of the female
population, such that they are more sexually open, and expe-
rience and report greater desire than other women thereby
attenuating the gender difference in sexual desire [62].
Sexual Desire as Measured by Sexual Behaviors
In the sections that follow, we further discuss sexual desire as
a motivational state rather than a trait and the impact of how
and when desire is assessed on the gender difference. First, we
discuss how the presence of sexual cues leads to sexual
arousal and motivated sexual behaviors. Second, we consider
how arousal and desire shift depending on the sexual cues that
are present. Third, we review how sexual deprivation influ-
ences sexual arousal and desire.
It has been proposed that sexual arousal and desire emerge
only in the presence of sexual incentives and that the quality of
these incentives determines the strength of sexual motivation
[8]. For example, seeing an attractive sexual partner might,
depending on the context, evoke strong feelings of sexual
motivation [8,11]. Sexual actions have often been quantified
using an outletapproach, whereby the frequencies of sexual
activity with a partner or with oneself are assessed. Studies
using this approach find that men report that they engage in
significantly more sexual activity with partners, and with
themselves, than do women [6,63,64]. This particular gender
difference may be due, in part, to the influence of gender
norms, social desirability biases, and practical restrictions on
womens expression of their sexuality as opposed to actual
differences in sexual behavior.
The sexual double standard hypothesis posits that men are
socialized and encouraged to hold more permissive attitudes
towards sexuality, whereas women have been socialized to
hold more conservative sexual attitudes and suffer greater
social repercussions when they are sexually expressive and
assertive [65,66]. Alexander and Fisher [67] examined the
influence of these gender stereotypes on the reporting of
sexual behaviors using a bogus pipeline paradigm. In this
experiment, participants were told that the veracity of their
responses could be detected while they responded to questions
about their sexuality. Gender difference in the reporting of
sexual behaviors was attenuated for women and men assessed
under bogus pipeline conditions. When in the bogus pipeline
condition, men reported fewer sexual partners and women
more sexual partners compared to the men and women in
the control condition, consistent with the prediction from the
sexual double standard hypothesis. It is possible that gender
stereotypes have a similar impact on the reporting of sexual
desire using an outlet approach, whereby men may overreport
and women may underreport their sexual encounters, thereby
obscuring similarities between genders and inflating a gender
difference in frequency of sexual behaviors. Fisher [68]re-
cently emphasized that this reporting bias is specific to ques-
tions about sexuality and is not present for questions about
nonsexual, but still traditionally gendered behaviors such as
exaggerating strength (men) and misrepresenting body
size/weight (women).
Women s sexual expressionboth partnered and solitary
and their reported frequency of behaviors may be influ-
enced by physiological factors that do not affect men, thereby
falsely creating a gender difference; this would be particularly
true when an outlet approach is used to estimate sexual desire.
For example, cycling women may experience practical restric-
tions on sexual expression, owing to factors such as premen-
strual dysphoria, discomfort of menstruation, and breast ten-
derness, among others [e.g., 6972]. In the average month,
this could reduce the time available for comfortable sexual
activity by approximately 20 %, depending on the number of
days a woman menstruates [70,72,73], whereas men do not
experience similarly restricting physiological events. To our
knowledge, no research has adjusted the estimate of womens
sexual desire to accommodate for this window of reduced
opportunity to engage in sexual behavior.
While sexual desire may lead to motivated sexual behav-
iors, this does not necessarily mean that behavior is a good
proxy for sexual desire; this assumption may lead to underes-
timates or overestimates of desire and may affect women and
men differently. For instance, the presence of sexual desire
does not always lead to sexual action (e.g., if no partner is
present or if it is an inopportune time), thus making it possible
to experience sexual desire without resulting sexual behavior.
This finding in particulardesire in the absence of sexual
action or behaviorappears to be more applicable to women
Curr Sex Health Rep
Author's personal copy
than to men [6,7476]. Thus, gender differences in sexual
desire could be an underestimate for women (and possibly for
men), if other factors prevent sexual action or behavior from
taking place. Alternatively, using sexual behavior as a proxy
can also inflate desire estimates, failing to recognize that
sexual behavior can occur for a multiplicity of reasons, some
of which do not involve sexual desire (e.g., stress reduction, to
attain resources or status, to boost self-esteem) [7681].
Along similar lines, there is evidence suggesting that wom-
en and men differ in the objects or goals of sexual desire.
Mark, Fortenberry, Herbenick, Sanders, and Reece [82]re-
ported gender differences in what people want when they
experience sexual desire, finding that men desired pleasing
their partner, pleasure, and orgasm, whereas women desired
intimacy, feeling sexually desired, and emotional closeness.
Similarly, Brotto and colleagues [83] found that women with
and without sexual difficulties reported desiring enhanced
intimacy rather than sexual activity. Meana [84] has asserted
that the goal of desire, particularly among women, may be the
reward of being desired. Across all of these goals or outcomes
of desire, the presence of a sexual cue (e.g., a partner, an
emotional connection) elicits feelings of sexual desire, even
if the goal of desire is not always sexual behavior or even
sexual in nature [11].
Research suggesting that women and men may differ in
terms of the outcomes of their sexual desire is worth consid-
ering in terms of understanding the most valid and reliable
indicators for assessing sexual desire. For example, sexual
desire in men appears to be closely related to sexual behavior
(e.g., pleasure, orgasm), thus an outlet approach to sexual
desire may accurately capture mens sexual desire. For wom-
en, sexual behavior does not appear to be the sole goal of
desire. The inclusion of relational variables (e.g., intimacy and
emotional closeness) that more accurately capture womens
experiences of sexual desire may be more appropriate (e.g.,
asking about how frequently they desire intimacy with their
partner) when trying to operationalize and assess womens
degree of sexual motivation or desire.
The alternative to a behavioral outlet approach, which may
produce a bias towards finding higher desire in men, is to
focus on cognitions (e.g., sexual thoughts) associated with
sexual desire. Studies using nationally representative samples
find that men report thinking about and desiring sex more
often than women and that this pattern is reflected across the
lifespan [54,55]. Similarly, studies using convenience sam-
ples (e.g., university students) report medium to large effect
sizes for gender differences in favor of men for cognition-
based items such as lifetime experience of sexual desire,
frequency of sexual desire, and overall level of sexual desire
[4]. While these single-shot, cognition-based approaches
avoid some of the limitations of the behavioral outlet ap-
proach, they are limited in that they fail to capture the flux
in sensitivity to sexual cues and sexual desire over time [5,85]
and may still be subject to the report biases mentioned above
[67]. In addition, recent research has found that relying on
cognitions and frequency of sexual thoughts as a proxy for
sexual desire may be problematic for other reasons. Men
report more frequent thoughts about a variety of internal
states, not just thoughts about sex, suggesting that perhaps
the gender difference may have more to dowith the report bias
of internal states than actual differences in sexual desire [86].
Objective assessment of sexual motivation is one method
of addressing the limitations of self-report, although this in-
troduces other challenges. One method is to examine action
tendencies or automatic physiological responses that are as-
sociated with motivation; these can be avoidant, for instance
in the presence of threat, or appetitive, in the instance of a
rewarding incentive. To objectively assess sexual motivation,
Both et al. [59,87,88]examinedAchillestendonreflex(T-
reflex) modulationa reliable indicator of motor preparation,
showing sensitivity to cues of stimulus intensity [88,89]in
women and men in response to sexual stimuli. Both et al. [87]
found that both womens and mens T-reflexes were similarly
activated in the presence of sexual stimuli. Sexual stimuli also
evoked reports of approach motivation (e.g., focused on the
pursuit of positive experiences with a partner), and these were
positively correlated with T-reflexes. In a second study, Both
et al. [59] found that, in addition to activated T-reflexes in the
presence of sexual stimuli, subsequent sexual behaviors in the
24-hrs post-testing were positively related to T-reflex activa-
tion for women and men, demonstrating positive relationships
between T-reflexes elicited by sexual stimuli, self-reported
approach goals (i.e., pursuit of a partner), and subsequent
sexual behavior. These three studies demonstrate that not only
do sexual incentives elicit sexual arousal and desire but they
also elicit action tendencies associated with future sexual
behaviors and that this process occurs similarly for women
and men [59,87,88].
Sexual Desire Across Time
Circular and incentive models of arousal and desire propose
that desire is influenced by the presence of sexual cues and
that the hedonic quality of these cues may change across time
resulting in similar shifts in arousal and desire [8,11]. Studies
of habituation to sexual stimuli in women and men demon-
strate that genital [9096] and subjective sexual arousal [90,
91,9396] decline with repeated exposure to the same sexual
stimulus, presumably because the hedonic quality associated
with the stimulus changes as a function of repeated exposure.
These studies also find that when novel stimuli are introduced,
sexual arousal is restored [9096]. Results from observational
studies find a similar pattern; sexual desire can wane for both
women and men in long-term relationships [56,57]. There is
some evidence to suggest that for women, sexual desire
Curr Sex Health Rep
Author's personal copy
declines while in a relationship and is reenergized with the
addition of a new sexual partner [56,57], whereas for men,
desire declines with age rather than with a specific sexual
partner (i.e., less responsivity or sensitivity to sexual cues over
time) [56]; however, more research is needed to examine
changes in desire from a longitudinal perspective.
Sensitivity to sexual cues may also be affected by the
timing of assessment. The single-shot versus longitudinal
approaches likely have implications for whether or not gender
differences or similarities are observed. This is especially true
given that women experience greater variability in hormone
levels than do men, and womens sexuality appears to be
sensitive to these fluctuations across the menstrual cycle [for
a review, see 97] and to oral contraceptive use [for a review,
see [98]. Prospective studies of naturally-cycling womens
sexual desire, as measured by sexual fantasies and sexual
thoughts recorded daily across entire menstrual cycles, show
marked increases during ovulation [e.g., 46,47,99,100],
suggesting a greater sensitivity to sexual cues at midcycle.
Unfortunately, there are very few equivalent studies examin-
ing fluctuation in mens sensitivity to sexual incentives over
time. Irrespective of menstrual cycle status, however, Mark
[101] found that partnered women and men reported similar
degrees of sexual desire when desire was assessed daily over a
30-day period. This particular finding points to the importance
of how and when sexual desire is assessed and the subsequent
impact on the gender difference in sexual desire. Thus, it is
possible that, at a given point in time, women may experience
desire equal to or greater than men; when hormonal status is
not taken into account or if the assessment of sexual desire is
limited to one point in time, then these effects may be ob-
scured [85].
Sexual Desire in the Context of Deprivation
Some researchers have conceptualized sexual desire as a
drive, stemming from Allports[102] view that depriva-
tion, or lack of sex, acts as an intrinsic motivator [8].
This has led to research focused on understanding the
consequences of sexual deprivation, that is, whether
deprivation motivates sexual activity or results in in-
creased arousal and desire. Incentive motivation theory
would suggest that deprivation alone does not facilitate
or drive sexual motivation, but rather deprivation acts
on the reward value associated with the sexual cue [8,
102]. Thus, the incentive value of a sexual stimulus will
be greater for someone in a sexually deprived state,
thereby rendering them more sensitive to its reward
value, and thus more motivated to seek out sexual
activity.
Men report experiencing greater aversive effects of absti-
nence and more frequent desire for partnered and solitary
sexual activity than do women [5,6], suggesting that men
are more affected by, or more aware of, the effects of depri-
vation than women. Recent research suggests that this effect
may not reflect a gender difference in sexual drive per se, but a
gender difference in overall perception and reporting of other
drives. Fisher and colleagues [85] found that men were more
likely to report thoughts about hunger, sleep, and sex, sug-
gesting that men may be more sensitive to range of biological
drives than are women, not just sex drive. Additional research
is needed to examine the effects of abstinence in women and
men to establish whether or not a gender difference in sexual
desire exists in the context of sexual deprivation.
Implications for Sexual Medicine Practitioners
Throughout this review, we have argued that the methodology
used to assess sexual desire, whether it be a single question
about sexual behavior or a validated questionnaire, assessed at
one point in time or longitudinally, influences whether or not a
gender difference is observed. In a clinical context, this gender
difference can manifest as a desire discrepancy in heterosexual
couples, that is, the perceived or real difference between
partners in their desire for sex [5,101], with men typically
reporting more desire than women. We believe that this dis-
crepancy may be exacerbated or attenuated depending on how
sexual desire is operationalized and assessed in the clinical
context and suggest that careful consideration should be given
to how clinicians choose to assess sexual desire in their clients.
Educating clients about the sexual double standard and gender
stereotypes about sexual desire may aid in identifying unhelp-
ful beliefs about sexual response, such as unreasonable ex-
pectations about performance or levels of desire.
Common conjecture and meta-analytic findings sug-
gest that men are the more lustful gender [6,63,64].
Moving away from this trait-based view of sexual desire
toward a state-based conceptualization may offer some
utilityinasextherapycontext.Doingsomay
remoralize couples, helping them to see that sexual
desire is contingent on the presence of sexual cues
and that the desire discrepancy is not necessarily hard-
wired or gender-linked, but rather is perhaps evidence
of the absence of suitable incentives and triggers. Thus,
challenging the assumptions that men experience more
desire than women, by situating desire as responsive,
may be a useful therapeutic tool when educating clients
about the nature of their sexual responses. Clinicians
can then use this framework to encourage clients to
explore the cues relevant in eliciting their arousal and
desire, noting that the cues for women may be different
than the cues for men and that finding gender-specific
cues may be integral to reducing the desire discrepancy
that many couples face [reviewed in 5].
Curr Sex Health Rep
Author's personal copy
Conclusions
Sexual medicine has moved away from linear conceptualiza-
tions of sexual desire towards models that situate sexual desire
as responsive [7,8,11]. Based on the available research, it
appears that state or responsive desire manifests similarly in
women and men, emerging from exposure to relevant sexual
incentives and accompanying sexual arousal. Observed gen-
der differences in the magnitude of sexual desire are likely
influenced by the ways in which desire is assessed, that is, by
the timing and methods of assessment. Studies examining
state sexual desire or sexual desire longitudinally find no
gender differences in the frequency of sexual desire. Future
research could focus less on differences between women and
men, and more on factors influencing within-gender variations
in the contextual, relational, and physiological elements
influencing sexual desire using longitudinal and multidimen-
sional methods of assessment. Sexual medicine practitioners
should carefully consider how they choose to operationalize
and assess sexual desire with their clients, with special focus
on the impact of these various methodologies on the desire
discrepancy (i.e., gender difference in desire). Situating desire
as responsive may be therapeutically useful for identifying the
pertinent sexual cues and incentives necessary to elicit arousal
and desire in clients with sexual difficulties.
Acknowledgments The authors wish to thank Michael C. Seto for his
helpful comments on an earlier draft of this review.
Compliance with Ethics Guidelines
Conflict of Interest Meredith L. Chivers has received grant support
from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research.
Samantha J. Dawson declares that she has no conflict of interest.
Human and Animal Rights and Informed Consent This article does
not contain any studies with human or animal subjects performed by any
of the authors.
References
Papers of particular interest, published recently, have been
highlighted as:
Of importance
•• Of major importance
1. Bancroft J. Sexual desire and the brain. J Sex Marital Ther.
1988;3:1127.
2. Levine SB. An essay on the nature of sexual desire. J Sex Marital
Ther. 1984;10:8396.
3. Levine SB. The nature of sexual desire: a clinician's perspective.
Arch Sex Behav. 2003;32:27985.
4. Regan PC, Atkins L. Sex differences and similarities in frequency
and intensity of sexual desire. Soc Behav Personal. 2006;34:95
102.
5. Regan PC. Sexual desire in women. In: Castañeda D, editor. The
essential handbook of women's sexuality. California: ABC-CLIO;
2013. p. 324.
6. Baumeister RF, Catanese KR, Vohs KD. Is there a gender differ-
ence in strength of sex drive? Theoretical views, conceptual
distinctions, and a review of relevant evidence. Pers Soc Psychol
Rev. 2001;5:24273.
7. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical man-
ual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author 2013.
8. Singer B, Toates FM. Sexual motivation. J Sex Res. 1987;23:481
501.
9.•• Dawson SJ, Chivers ML. Gender-specificity of solitary and dyadic
sexual desire among gynephilic and androphilic women and men.
J Sex Med. 2014;11:98094. This paper examined gender and
sexual orientation differences in patterns of responsive sexual
desire to sexual stimuli that differ in their incentive value, finding
no evidence for gender differences in responsive dyadic or solitary
sexual desire.
10. Masters WH, Johnson VE. Human sexual response. Boston: Little
Brown; 1966.
11. Basson R. Human sex-response cycles. J Sex Marital Ther.
2001;27:3343.
12. Brotto LA. The DSM diagnostic criteria for hypoactive sexual
desire disorder in women. Arch Sex Behav. 2010;39:22139.
13. Brotto LA, Bitzer J, Laan E, et al. Women's sexual desire and
arousal disorders. J Sex Med. 2010;7:586614.
14. Sarin S, Amsel RM, Binik YM. Disentangling desire and arousal:
a classificatory conundrum. Arch Sex Behav. 2013;42:1079100.
15. Sarin S: A streetcar named derousal? An empirical examination
of the arousal/desire distinction. Paper presented at the 37th
Annual Meeting of the Society for Sex Therapy and Research,
Chicago, IL, USA 2012.
16. Sarin S, Amsel RM, Binik YM. How hot is he? A psy-
chophysiological and psychosocial examination of the
arousal patterns of sexually functional and dysfunctional
men. J Sex Med in press.
17. Kleinplatz PJ. Arousal and desire problems: conceptual, research
and clinical considerations or the more things change the more
they stay the same. Sex Relat Ther. 2011;26:315.
18. Brotto LA. The DSM diagnostic criteria for hypoactive sexual
desire disorder in men. J Sex Med. 2010;7:201530.
19. Black DW, Kehrberg LL, Flumerfelt DL, Schlosser SS.
Characteristics of 36 subjects reporting compulsive sexual behav-
ior. Am J Psych. 1997;154:2439.
20. Carries PJ, Delmonico DL. Childhood abuse and multiple addic-
tions: research findings in a sample of self-identified sexual ad-
dicts. Sexual Addict Compulsiv: J Treatment Prevention. 1996;3:
25868.
21. Winters J, Christoff K, Gorzalka BB. Dysregulated sexuality and
high sexual desire: distinct constructs? Arch Sex Behav. 2010;39:
102943.
22. Långström N, Hanson RK. High rates of sexual behavior in the
general population: correlates and predictors. Arch Sex Behav.
2006;35:3752.
23.•• Bloemers J, van Rooij K, Poels S, et al. Toward personalized
sexual medicine (part 1): integrating the dual control modelinto
differential drug treatments for hypoactive sexual desire disorder
and female sexual arousal disorder. J Sex Med. 2013;10:791809.
This paper proposed the hypothesis that low sexual desire in
women is the result of different causal mechanisms: insensitivity
to sexual cues or enhanced activity of inhibitory responses.
24. Poels S, Bloemers J, van Rooij K, et al. Toward personalized
sexual medicine (part 2): testosterone combined with a PDE5
inhibitor increases sexual satisfaction in women with HSDD and
FSAD, and a low sensitive system for sexual cues. J Sex Med.
2013;10:81023.
Curr Sex Health Rep
Author's personal copy
25. Dabbs JM, Dabbs MG. Heroes, rogues, and lovers: testosterone
and behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill; 2000.
26. Hellhammer DH, Hubert W, Schürmeyer T. Changes in saliva
testosterone after psychological stimulation in men.
Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1985;10:7781.
27. Roney JR, Mahler SV, Maestripieri D. Behavioral and hormonal
responses of men to brief interactions with women. Evol Hum
Behav. 2003;24:36575.
28.van Anders SM. Testosterone and sexual desire in healthy women
and men. Arch Sex Behav. 2012;41:147184. This paper reviewed
the mixed evidence for gender differences in associations between
testosterone and sexual desire. The study found positive associa-
tions between testosterone and solitary sexual desire and negative
associations between testosterone and dyadic sexual desire in
women, and noassociation between testosterone and sexual desire
in men.
29. van Anders SM, Dunn EJ. Are gonadal steroids linked with
orgasm perceptions and sexual assertiveness in women and
men? Horm Behav. 2009;56:20613.
30. van Anders SM, Hampson E. Testing the prenatal androgen hy-
pothesis: measuring digit ratios, sexual orientation, and spatial
abilities in adults. Horm Behav. 2005;47:928.
31. van Anders SM, Hamilton LD, Schmidt N, Watson NV.
Associations between testosterone secretion and sexual activity
in women. Horm Behav. 2007;51:47782.
32. Sadowsky M, Antonovsky H, Sobel R, Maoz B. Sexual activity
and sex hormone levels in aging men. Intern Psychoger. 1993;5:
1816.
33. Mazur A, Mueller U, Krause W, Booth A. Causes of sexual
decline in aging married men: Germany and America. Int J
Impot Res. 2002;14:1016.
34. van Anders SM, Hamilton LD, Watson NV. Multiple partners are
associated with higher testosterone in North American men and
women. Horm Behav. 2007;51:4549.
35. van Anders SM, Brotto L, Farrell J, Yule M. Associations among
physiological and subjective sexual response, sexual desire, and
salivary steroid hormones in healthy premenopausal women. J Sex
Med. 2009;6:73951.
36. Chivers ML, Suschinsky KD, Timmers A, Bossio J.
Experimental, neuroimaging, and psychophysiological methods.
In: Bauermeister J, Diamond L, Tolman D, editors. Handbook of
sexuality and psychology. Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association; 2013.
37. Al-Azzawi F, Bitzer J, Brandenburg U, et al. Therapeutic options
for postmenopausal female sexual dysfunction. Climacteric.
2010;13:10320.
38. Davis SR, Braunstein GD. Efficacy and safety of testosterone in
the management of hypoactive sexual desire disorder in postmen-
opausal women. J Sex Med. 2012;9:113448.
39. Khera M, Bhattacharya RK, Blick G, Kushner H, Nguyen D,
Miner MM. Improved sexual function with testosterone replace-
ment therapy in hypogonadal men: Realworld data from the
Testim Registry in the United States (TRiUS). J Sex Med.
2011;8:320413.
40. Eysenck HJ, Eysenck MW. Personality and individual differences:
a natural science approach. New York: Plenum; 1985.
41. McCrae RR, Costa PT. Personality in adulthood: a five-factor
theory perspective. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press; 2003.
42. Conley JJ. Longitudinal stability of personality traits: a multitrait
multimethodmultioccasion analysis. J Pers Soc Psychol.
1985;49:126682.
43. Fleeson W, Noftle E. Where does personality have its
influence? A supermatrix of consistency concepts. J Pers.
2008;76:135586.
44. Endler NS. Stress, anxiety and coping: the multidimensional in-
teraction model. Can Psychol. 1997;38:13653.
45. Spector IP, Carey MP, Steinberg L. The Sexual Desire Inventory:
development, factor structure, and evidence of reliability. J Sex
Marital Ther. 1996;22:17590.
46. Bullivant SB, Sellergren SA, Stern K, et al. Women's sexual
experience during the menstrual cycle: identification of the sexual
phase by noninvasive measurement of luteinizing hormone. J Sex
Res. 2004;41:8293.
47. Stanislaw H, Rice FJ. Correlation between sexual desire and
menstrual cycle characteristics. Arch Sex Behav. 1988;17:499
508.
48. Pauleta JR, Pereira NM, Graça LM. Sexuality during pregnancy. J
Sex Med. 2010;7:13642.
49. Fischman SH, Rankin EA, Soeken EL, Lenz ER. Changes in
sexual relationships in postpartum couples. J Obstet Gynecol
Neonatal Nurs. 1986;15:5863.
50. Rowland M, Foxcroft L, Hopman WM, Patel R. Breastfeeding
and sexuality immediately post partum. CFP. 2005;51:13667.
51. Rupp HA, James TW, Ketterson ED, et al. Lower sexual interest in
postpartum women: relationship to amygdala activation and intra-
nasal oxytocin. Horm Behav. 2013;63:11421.
52. Avis NE, Brockwell S, Randolph Jr JF, et al. Longitudinal changes
in sexual functioning as women transition through menopause:
results from the Study of Womens Health Across the Nation
(SWAN). Menopause. 2009;16:44252.
53. Nappi RE, Lachowsky M. Menopause and sexuality: prevalence
of symptoms and impact on quality of life. Maturitas. 2009;63:
13841.
54. Eplov L, Giraldi A, Davidsen M, et al. Sexual desire in a
nationally representative Danish population. J Sex Med.
2007;4:4756.
55. Hamilton L. Kulseng Berg A, Traeen B, Lundin Kvalem I.
Self-reported frequency of feeling sexual desire among a
representative sample of 1849 year old men and women in
Oslo, elucidated by epidemiological data. Scand J Sexol.
2001;4:2542.
56. Klusmann D. Sexual motivation and the duration of partnership.
Arch Sex Behav. 2002;31:27587.
57. Dennerstein L, Lehert P, Burger H. The relative effects of
hormones and relationship factors on sexual function of
women through the natural menopausal transition. Fertil
Steril. 2005;84:17480.
58.•• Goldey KL, van Anders SM. Sexual arousal and desire: interrela-
tions and responses to three modalities of sexual stimuli. J Sex
Med. 2012;9:231529. This study found no gender differences in
responsive dyadic and solitary sexual desire and was the first
study to show that exposure to sexual stimuli influences reporting
of trait sexual desire in women.
59. Both S, Spiering M, Everaerd W, Laan E. Sexual behavior and
responsiveness to sexual stimuli following laboratoryinduced
sexual arousal. J Sex Res. 2004;41:24258.
60. Kiefer AK, Sanchez DT. Scripting sexual passivity: a gender role
perspective. Pers Relat. 2007;14:26990.
61. Huberman JS, Suschinsky KD, Lalumière ML, Chivers ML.
Relationship between impression management and three measures
of womens self-reported sexual arousal. Can J Behav Sci.
2013;45:25973.
62. Strassberg DS, Lowe K. Volunteer bias in sexuality research. Arch
Sex Behav. 1995;24:36982.
63. Oliver MB, Hyde JS. Gender differences in sexuality: a meta-
analysis. Psych Bull. 1993;114:2951.
64. Petersen JL, Hyde JS. A meta-analytic review of research on
gender differences in sexuality, 19932007. Psych Bull.
2010;136:2138.
65. Brown NR, Sinclair RC. Estimating number of lifetime sexual
partners: men and women do it differently. J Sex Res. 1999;36:
2927.
Curr Sex Health Rep
Author's personal copy
66. Jonason PK, Fisher TD. The power of prestige: why young men
report having more sex partners than young women. Sex Roles.
2009;60:1519.
67. Alexander MG, Fisher TD. Truth and consequences: using the
bogus pipeline to examine sex differences in selfreported sexual-
ity. J Sex Res. 2003;40:2735.
68. Fisher TD: Gender roles and pressure to be truthful: the bogus
pipeline modifies gender differences in sexual but not non-sexual
behavior. Sex Roles 2013;68:40114
69. Barnhart K, Fuman I, Devoto L. Attitudes and practice of couples
regarding sexual relations during the menses and spotting.
Contraception. 1995;51:938.
70. Dennerstein L, Gotts G, Brown JB, et al. The relationship between
the menstrual cycle and female sexual interest in women with
PMS complaints and volunteers. Psychoneuroendocrinology.
1994;19:293304.
71. Harvey SM. Female sexual behavior: fluctuations during the
menstrual cycle. J Psychosom Res. 1987;31:10110.
72. Hedricks CA. Sexual behavior across the menstrual cycle: a
biopsychosocial approach. Annu Rev Sex Res. 1994;5:12272.
73. Regan PC. Rhythms of desire: the association between menstrual
cycle phases and female sexual desire. Can J Hum Sex. 1996;5:
14556.
74. Regan PC, Berscheid E. Gender differences in beliefs about the
causes of male and female sexual desire. Pers Relat. 1995;2:345
58.
75. Graham CA, Sanders SA, Milhausen RR, McBride KR. Turning
on and turning off: a focus group study of the factors that affect
women's sexual arousal. Arch Sex Behav. 2004;33:52738.
76. Hill CA. The distinctiveness of sexual motives in relationto sexual
desire and desirable partner attributes. J Sex Res. 1997;34:13953.
77. Beck JG, Bozman AW, Qualtrough T. The experience of sexual
desire: psychological correlates in a college sample. J Sex Res.
1991;28:44356.
78. Hill CA, Preston LK. Individual differences in the experience of
sexual motivation: theory and measurement of dispositional sex-
ual motives. J Sex Res. 1996;33:2745.
79. Impett EA, Peplau LA. Why some women consent to unwanted
sex with a dating partner: insights from attachment theory. Psychol
Women Quart. 2002;26:36070.
80. Impett EA, Peplau LA. Sexual compliance: gender, motivational,
and relationship perspectives. J Sex Res. 2003;40:87100.
81. Meston CM, Buss DM. Why humans have sex. Arch Sex Behav.
2007;36:477507.
82. Mark KP, Fortenberry JD, Herbenick D, et al.: The object of
sexual desire: examining the whatin what do you desire?.
Oral presentation at the Annual Meeting of the Canadian Sex
Research Forum, Ottawa, ON, Canada, 2012.
83. Brotto LA, Heiman JR, Tolman DL. Narratives of desire in mid-
age women with and without arousal difficulties. J Sex Res.
2009;46:38798.
84. Meana M. Elucidating women's (hetero) sexual desire: definition-
al challenges and content expansion. J Sex Res. 2010;47:10422.
85. Wallen K. Sex and context: hormones and primate sexual motiva-
tion. Horm Behav. 2001;40:33957.
86. Fisher TD, Moore ZT, Pittenger MJ. Sex on the Brain?: an
examination of frequency of sexual cognitions as a function of
gender, erotophilia, and social desirability. J Sex Res. 2012;49:69
77.
87. Both S, Everaerd W, Laan E. Modulation of spinal reflexes by
aversive and sexually appetitive stimuli. Psychophysiology.
2003;40:17483.
88. Both S, Boxtel G, Stekelenburg J, et al. Modulation of spinal
reflexes by sexual films of increasing intensity. Psychophysiology.
2005;42:72631.
89. Bonnet M, Bradley MM, Lang PJ, Requin J. Modulation of spinal
reflexes: arousal, pleasure, action. Psychophysiology. 1995;32:
36772.
90. Both S, Laan E, Everaerd W. Focusing hotor focusing cool:
attentional mechanisms in sexual arousal in men and women. J
Sex Med. 2011;8:16779.
91. Dawson SJ, Lalumière ML, Allen SW, et al. Can habitua-
tion of sexual responses be elicited in men and women
when attention is maintained? Can J Behav Sci. 2013;45:
27485.
92. Dawson SJ, Suschinsky KD, Lalumière ML. Habituation of sex-
ual responses in men and women: a test of the preparation hy-
pothesis of women's genital responses. J Sex Med. 2013;10:990
1000.
93. Koukounas E, Over R. Habituation and dishabituation of male
sexual arousal. Behav Res Ther. 1993;31:57585.
94. Koukounas E, Over R. Allocation of attentional resources during
habituation and dishabituation of male sexual arousal. Arch Sex
Behav. 1999;28:53952.
95. Koukounas E, Over R. Changes in the magnitude of the eyeblink
startle response during habituation of sexual arousal. Behav Res
Ther. 2000;38:57384.
96. Koukounas E, Over R. Habituation of male sexual arousal: effects
of attentional focus. Biol Psychol. 2001;58:4964.
97. Thornhill R, Gangestad SW. The evolutionary biology of
human female sexuality. New York: Oxford University
Press; 2008.
98. Burrows LJ, Basha M, Goldstein AT. The effects of hormonal
contraceptives on female sexuality: a review. J Sex Med. 2012;9:
221323.
99. Dawson SJ, Suschinsky KD, Lalumière ML. Sexual fantasies and
viewing times across the menstrual cycle: a diary study. Arch Sex
Behav. 2012;41:17383.
100. Diamond LM, Wallen K. Sexual minority womens sexual moti-
vation around the time of ovulation. Arch Sex Behav. 2012;40:
23746.
101. Mark KM. The impact of daily sexual desire and daily sexual
desire discrepancy on the quality of sexual experience in couples.
Can J Hum Sex. 2014;23:2733.
102. Allport FJ. Social psychology. Cambridge: Houghton-Mifflin;
1927.
Curr Sex Health Rep
Author's personal copy
... According to Dewitte et al. (2020), SDD was first defined by Zilbergeld and Ellison (1980). This discrepancy is one of the most frequently reported sexual problems by couples in romantic relationships (Carvalho & Nobre, 2010;Dawson & Chivers, 2014;Dewitt et al., 2020;Herbenick et al., 2014, Mark, 2015Vowels et al., 2018). It is associated with sexual and relationship dissatisfaction (Ellison, 2001;Mark, 2015;Mark & Murray, 2012;Mark et al., 2018;Santtila et al., 2007;Sutherland et al., 2015;Vowels et al., 2018;Willoughby et al., 2014). ...
... Some studies have been conducted on the impact of sociocultural factors on women's sexuality and women's sexual desire in particular, including those by Nasserzadeh (2015), Rosenkrantz and Mark (2018), Rubin et al. (2019), and van Anders et al. (2022). The present study aims to complement these by focusing solely on the issue of sexual desire in women committed in a relationship, because sexual desire discrepancy is one of the most frequently reported sexual problems in couples (Carvalho & Nobre, 2010;Dawson & Chivers, 2014;Dewitte et al., 2020;Herbenick et al., 2014, Mark, 2015. The present study also aims to see which sociocultural conceptions of sexuality have a greater influence on women's sexual desire in relationships. ...
... 6 months). It focuses solely on women in relationships because sexual desire discrepancy is one of the sexual problems most frequently reported by couples (Carvalho & Nobre, 2010;Dawson & Chivers, 2014;Dewitt et al., 2020;Herbenick et al., 2014, Mark, 2015. The present study also aims to see which sociocultural conceptions of sexuality have a greater influence on women's sexual desire in relationships. ...
Article
Full-text available
Numerous studies have described that women’s sexual desire is lower than men’s desire. Within a heterosexual couple, a sexual desire discrepancy can be associated with sexual and relationship dissatisfaction. The factors that negatively influence women’s sexual desire are biological, psychological, relational, cognitive and sociocultural. The present study aimed to determine the extent to which adherence to sexual norms predicted sexual desire in women engaged in a romantic relationship (min. 6 months). These norms were addressed by Simon and Gagnon’s (1984) notion of sexual scripts, which designate the sociocultural norms governing the sexual behaviors of men and women. A sample of women (N = 829, M age = 32 years, SD = 12.20, range = 18–72) completed a questionnaire about sexual desire and heterosexual scripts. The results showed that the more women in relationships say they felt desire, the less they subscribed to heterosexual scripts and particularly the conception that a man’s sex drive is higher than a woman’s. This conception seems to be the most determinant of women’s sexual desire in relationships. The results attest also to the persistence and strength of heterosexual scripts which, despite greater equality between men and women, continue to negatively influence women’s sexual desire. By becoming aware of these conditionings, women suffering from low sexual desire and sexual desire discrepancy within their couple can break free from them and experience a more fulfilling sexuality. Lastly, some interesting contradictory results are addressed in discussion.
... Men are socialized to hold more open attitudes towards sexuality, whereas sexually expressive women often suffer social aversion. Such stereotypes may affect sexual values and support gender differences independent from biological differences [12]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Although recognized for their high impact on sexual and relationship satisfaction, scientifically sound information on differences in the perception of partnership sexuality between both partners is sparse.
... In turn, such techniques might then be used to examine individual differences in the processing of sexual stimuli [4][5][6] including the possible use in the detection of sexual deviancy [7][8][9]. As there is widespread agreement that men are more sexually motivated than women [10] (though see Dawson and Chivers [11] for a more nuanced approach) and more interested in sexual images [12], it seems obvious that such effects of sexual images should be greater in men than in women. ...
Article
Full-text available
Sexual stimuli are thought to be highly salient and have been shown to automatically attract attention at the cost of processing other stimuli. We examined whether this effect was greater for men and whether men would show a category-specific effect with greater effects due to female images than male images. In two studies, participants performed a simple perceptual task while trying to ignore a distractor stimulus that could have sexual or neutral content. As expected, sexual stimuli produced a slowing of decision times under all conditions. The effect of erotic stimuli was greater for men (Experiment 1) and was category-specific (Experiment 2) while the response of women was not category specific (Experiment 2). However, all indices of distraction showed poor levels of reliability. The results show that early automatic distraction from sexual images show both quantitative and qualitative gender differences.
... To match the college age range of the Paulhus et al. (2021) (Furnham & Cuppello, 2024;Neumann et al., 2022) as well as on sex drive (Baumeister et al., 2001;Dawson & Chivers, 2014;Lippa, 2009) and pornography use (Schmitt et al., 2012). Effect sizes (Cohen's d) were as follows: Machiavellianism (.41), narcissism (.22), psychopathy (.56), sadism (1.12), sex drive (.89), and porn use (1.02). ...
... Men are more likely to consume pornographic content than women, and do so more frequently (6,(23)(24)(25). While studies have demonstrated no meaningful difference in sexual desire between men and women (26), a substantial portion of this gender disparity can be attributed to internalized expectations and societal norms regarding "acceptable" sexual desires and behaviors for both genders (27,28). In other words, Patterns of behavior related to sexual desire are likely to be significantly influenced by the specific social contexts we experience, which are partially characterized by gender distinctions (29). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Pornography, encompassing explicit images, videos, audio, and written content, has adverse impacts on individuals' well-being. We aimed to explore the relationship between mental health and communication quality with parents and the use of pornography, considering the moderating effect of gender. Methods: Quantitative data collection and descriptive-correlation analysis were employed. The study included university students in Tehran, Iran during the 2021-2022 academic year. Through purposive sampling, 210 participants completed an online questionnaire on Porsline. The survey assessed pornography use, quality of communication with parents, and general health. SPSS 23 software facilitated correlation and hierarchical regression analysis. Results: Gender significantly influenced pornography use (β=0.557). Anxiety symptoms (β=0.45) and social dysfunction (β=0.351) had significant positive effects on pornography use. Moreover, quality of relationships with parents, both mother (β=-0.47) and father (β=-0.27), exhibited negative associations with pornography use (P=0.01). Conclusion: High quality relationships with parents, lower anxiety and social dysfunction could potentially decrease the likelihood of pornography use on the internet. It is recommended that educational efforts and cultural interventions focus on protecting young adults from the negative effects of pornography use. Such interventions may include psychoeducational training on effective communication skills, mental health literacy, and stress management skills.
... It is plausible that as the level of intimacy increases, emerging adult men may strongly believe that their partner desires sexual intercourse, and therefore processes any incoming information through this lens as corroborating evidence. This is problematic given the evidence that men, on average, pursue sexual activity for pleasure whereas women more typically seek it to achieve greater emotional intimacy (Dawson & Chivers, 2014). As a result of these differing motivations, men may converge on a myopic interpretation of the situation centered upon the achievement of intercourse that may become incorrigible after passing some level of sexual intimacy. ...
Article
Full-text available
A woman’s passive response to a sexual advance can be misconstrued by men as signaling consent when it does not. Characterological factors and situational variances may further shape how men perceive a woman’s passive response and impact their sexual decisions during hookups, leading to unwanted sexual experiences for the partner. A sample of men (n = 357) completed first-person factorial vignettes depicting a sexual hookup in which a woman reacts to their partner’s sexual advance passively, either with or without signs of tension. Men were asked to rate their perceptions of consent and their hypothetical likelihood of engaging in different sexual behaviors, and completed assessments that were used to extract hostile masculinity and impersonal sexual orientation factors. Consent perceptions had strong effects on men’s sexual decision-making and mediated situational influences (e.g., passive response type), impersonal sexual orientation, and, to some extent, hostile masculinity; and hostile masculinity had strong direct effects on sexual decision-making irrespective of consent perceptions. Men can discriminate between passive responses and appear to calibrate their decision-making according to their perceptions of consent. Some men, however, are prone to perceive consent in passive responding irrespective of the situation, with others inclined to continue or advance intimacy without considering the woman’s level of consent.
Article
Background The association between stress and sexuality, both of which are linked to health, is unclear. We examined the feasibility of an ecological momentary assessment study including time-based and event-based measurements in this context (aim 1) and investigated concurrent and time-lagged bidirectional associations between subjective stress and sexual desire/arousal/activity in the daily lives of healthy individuals over 14 days (aim 2). Purpose The aim of this study was to gain insight into the interplay between stress and sexual experience and behavior while considering potential gender differences. Methods Between May 2015 and January 2016, 59 heterosexual, healthy men and women in relationships (M = 23.66 years old, SD = 2.86, range: 18-30 years) rated subjective stress, sexual desire, and sexual arousal at 6 fixed timepoints daily as well as after sexual activity. Feasibility was investigated considering dropout rates, missing data, and representativeness of data. Data were analyzed using mixed-effects models. Results Higher subjective stress was associated with a lower likelihood of concurrent sexual desire and arousal, and occurrences of sexual desire and arousal were associated with lower concurrent subjective stress. Sexual desire and activity were associated with lower subsequent subjective stress, and the latter association was stronger in women than in men. Rates of dropout, missing data, and nonrepresentative data were low. Conclusions There appear to be bidirectional associations between higher subjective stress and a lower likelihood of concurrent sexual desire and arousal. Sexual desire and sexual activity seem to be associated with lower subsequent subjective stress. The study design appears to be feasible, although the generalizability of the findings is limited. Future studies might explore stress reduction interventions to promote sexual health.
Article
Full-text available
Sex work is highly gendered, with 80 percent of sex workers being female, and the vast majority of buyers of sex being male. It is often taken for granted that this is how it is, and implicit in much of the debate around sex work is the assumption that it is inherently gendered. In this paper, I question this assumption, drawing on sociological research to challenge arguments which purport that it is inconceivable that women would ever want to pay for sex, or that sex work would exist under conditions of gender equality. I argue that gendered sexual norms likely are a significant reason for why sex work is so gendered, but sex work would probably continue to exist under conditions of gender equality, due to the diversity in motivations people have for buying and selling sex. Acknowledging that sex work is not inherently gendered is important for (at least) two reasons. First, it is probable that the gendered nature of sex work contributes to the stigma and bad treatment that sex workers, particularly female ones, face. Secondly, if sex work is not inherently gendered, this will have implications for how we should think about it, morally, practically, and legally.
Article
Full-text available
Background Negative sexual cognition and sexual anxiety create a substantial amount of distress and guilt in an individual, which in turn can lead to reduced well-being. However, the impact of sexual cognition and sexual anxiety on mental health are less researched topics, especially in the Indian context. Aim To investigate the effect of sexual cognition and sexual anxiety on mental health. Method 124 participants of age range 18–35 years were recruited using purposive sampling method. Sexual Cognition Checklist, Sexual Anxiety Inventory, and Mental Health Inventory were used to assess sexual cognition, sexual anxiety, and mental health respectively. Results Mental health was significantly negatively associated with sexual anxiety and negative sexual cognition. Also, a significant positive correlation was found between positive sexual cognition and mental health. Negative sexual cognition and sexual anxiety emerged as significant predictors, accounting for approximately 20% and 6% of the variance in mental health, respectively. Finally, an independent sample t-test suggests that males and females significantly differ in their level of sexual cognition, mental health, and sexual anxiety. Conclusions Negative sexual cognition and sexual anxiety are associated with poor mental health while positive sexual cognition is associated with better mental health. Unmarried Indian women tend to experience more negative sexual cognition and sexual anxiety than married women.
Article
Background Transactional sex relationships (TSRs) create financial and emotional support for men and women, as well as an increased sexual risk. Studies have reported high HIV and STI transmission rates among young women in transactional sex relationships. However, little is known about TSR prevalence in Jamaica and risky sexual practices among participants. This study investigates the sexual behaviour of Jamaicans in TSR. Methods Secondary data analysis of a national survey revealed that 586 participants (38%) self-reported being in at least one TSR in the last 12 months. We also identified a third category called “Benefluids”, who play both roles of benefactor and beneficiary in transactional sex relationships. Results 59 percent of male Benefluids had two to five transactional sex relationship partners in the last 12 months, compared to 40% of female Benefluids. Twenty-eight percent of female Benefluids reported sexually transmitted infection symptoms in the last 12 months compared to 13.5% of male Benefluids. While females reported more sexually transmitted infection symptoms, young men had the highest sexual risk precursors. Conclusions People in transactional sex relationships often play the role of beneficiaries and benefactors to meet material and sexual needs but this increases the risk of STI symptoms.
Article
Full-text available
This meta-analysis surveyed 177 usable sources that reported data on gender differences on 21 different measures of sexual attitudes and behaviors. The largest gender difference was in incidence of masturbation: Men had the greater incidence (d = .96). There was also a large gender difference in attitudes toward casual sex: Males had considerably more permissive attitudes (d = .81). There were no gender differences in attitudes toward homosexuality or in sexual satisfaction. Most other gender differences were in the small-to-moderate range. Gender differences narrowed from the 1960s to the 1980s for many variables. Chodorow's neoanalytic theory, sociobiology, social learning theory, social role theory, and script theory are discussed in relation to these findings.
Article
Full-text available
The longitudinal stability of personality was investigated in a group of several hundred adults who were rated by themselves, their marriage partners, and their acquaintances in 1935–1938 and by themselves and their marriage partners in 1954–1955. For both men and women, there were very similar factorial structures in all five sources of ratings. Individual differences in neuroticism, social extraversion, and impulse control had reasonably high levels of longitudinal stability over a 19-year period. Both the synchronic and diachronic correlations converged across methods and discriminated among traits. Self-report personality inventory data available in 1935–1938 and 1954–1955 provided corroborating evidence of the longitudinal and methodological robustness of personality traits. In data gathered on the same panel in 1980–1981, the questionnaire and the life history correlates of neuroticism and social extraversion displayed patterns indicative of temporal stability, methodological convergence, and discrimination among constructs. The data of this longitudinal study carried out over five decades strongly indicate that there is a set of personality traits that are generalizable across methods of assessment and are stable throughout adulthood.
Article
Full-text available
Married men in Germany (n=48) and America (n=50) between 50 and 80 years old, none in poor health, provided comparable information on sexual behavior and attitudes, and gave saliva samples from which testosterone was assayed. Sexuality declines with age, as expected. Neither testosterone nor psychological depression explain levels of sexuality. In both nations, wife's desire for intercourse, subject's ability to maintain an erection, and subject's imagination about other women, explain certain aspects of sexuality. Subject's health and marital satisfaction are related to sexuality among Americans but not among Germans. Behavioral models for the two nations are compared.
Article
Full-text available
We discuss the long‐standing “sex‐as‐drive‐or‐appetite” controversy—whether sexual desires may arise on their own, from internal states, or whether they only arise when attractive stimuli are presented. The issue is approached through integration of sexual motivation within an umbrella theory of motivational systems that closely follows currently dominant incentive motivation theories. In this formulation sexual motivation, like hunger or thirst, emerges from an interaction of external incentives and internal states. Deprivation acts to enhance the palatability of incentives but does not create an internal goad. That is, there is no aversive internal sensation associated with sexual deprivation. Through this perspective we integrate sex with findings from other motivational systems such as hunger or thirst and clarify otherwise puzzling phenomena: why orgasm and sexual motivation can be decoupled; how female sexual motivation arises and is similar to males'; how novel stimulation affects sexual motivation; and why measured sexual motivation seems to vary with experimental technique. Sexual self‐stimulation is accounted for under this model. Predictions are generated for the outcomes of both human and animal experiments. Finally, practical implications are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
The recognition that sexual desire is associated with several significant individual and interpersonal life events has led to a corresponding interest in delineating and exploring the correlates and potentially causal antecedents of this sexual phenomenon. Researchers interested in biological causes have focused on the sex hormones and on hormonally-mediated (female)life events. In this review, I first define sexual desire, distinguish sexual desire from other sexual experiences (i.e., arousal, activity), and discuss commonly used operationalizations. I then summarize empirical research exploring the relationship between one hormonally-mediated female life event - the menstrual cycle - and sexual desire. I conclude that sexual desire does appear to increase during certain menstrual cycle phases for some women (in particular, at ovulation and during the mid follicular and late luteal phases). However, no single rhythmic pattern emerges that can be said to definitively characterize the sexual experience of the human female.
Article
Full-text available
Recent research has found associations between sexual desire, desire discrepancy, and satisfaction outcomes in individuals and couples on a broad level. The present study aimed to extend these findings to the event level through examining daily experiences of sexual desire, sexual desire discrepancy, and quality of the sexual experience in a sample of 87 mixed-sex couples (174 individuals) over a 30-day period through daily electronic report. Participants were in their relationships for an average of 9.3 years. Data were analyzed using over-time Actor Partner Interdependence Models (APIM). For women and men, higher actor daily sexual desire predicted higher actor quality of the sexual experience. In addition, higher partner daily sexual desire predicted higher actor quality of the sexual experience. Event-level desire discrepancy between the couple was also a significant predictor of actor quality of the sexual experience for women, though not for men. These results confirm that day-to-day sexual desire and desire discrepancy are important indicators of quality of the sexual relationship and emphasize the importance of considering event-level characteristics when examining sexual behaviour and couple dynamics. Implications and suggestions for future research are also discussed.
Article