ArticlePublisher preview available

"Hot-Headed" Is More Than an Expression: The Embodied Representation of Anger in Terms of Heat

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

Anger is frequently referred to in terms of heat-related metaphors (e.g., hot-headed). The metaphoric representation perspective contends that such metaphors are not simply a poetic means of expressing anger but actually reflect the manner in which the concept of anger is cognitively represented. Drawing upon this perspective, the present studies examined the idea that the cognitive representation of anger is systematically related to the cognitive representation of heat. A total of 7 studies, involving 438 participants, provided support for this view. Visual depictions of heat facilitated the use of anger-related conceptual knowledge, and this occurred in tasks involving lexical stimuli as well as facial expressions. Furthermore, priming anger-related thoughts led participants to judge unfamiliar cities and the actual room temperature as hotter in nature. The results are discussed in terms of their implications for embodied views of emotion concepts and their potential social consequences.
“Hot-Headed” Is More Than an Expression: The Embodied Representation
of Anger in Terms of Heat
Benjamin M. Wilkowski
University of Wyoming Brian P. Meier
Gettysburg College
Michael D. Robinson
North Dakota State University Margaret S. Carter
Gettysburg College
Roger Feltman
University of Rochester
Anger is frequently referred to in terms of heat-related metaphors (e.g., hot-headed). The metaphoric
representation perspective contends that such metaphors are not simply a poetic means of expressing
anger but actually reflect the manner in which the concept of anger is cognitively represented. Drawing
upon this perspective, the present studies examined the idea that the cognitive representation of anger is
systematically related to the cognitive representation of heat. A total of 7 studies, involving 438
participants, provided support for this view. Visual depictions of heat facilitated the use of anger-related
conceptual knowledge, and this occurred in tasks involving lexical stimuli as well as facial expressions.
Furthermore, priming anger-related thoughts led participants to judge unfamiliar cities and the actual
room temperature as hotter in nature. The results are discussed in terms of their implications for
embodied views of emotion concepts and their potential social consequences.
Keywords: anger, heat, metaphor, embodiment, social cognition
When speaking about anger, people from multiple cultures
spontaneously employ heat-related metaphors (Ko¨vecses, 2000).
For example, English speakers refer to anger in terms of one’s
blood “boiling” (Lakoff, 1987). Similarly, speakers of Tunisian
Arabic describe their anger using phrases like “your make my
brain burn” (Maalej, 2004), and Japanese speakers use similar
phrases like “my head got hot” (Ko¨vecses, 2000). It is also notable
that anger-induced aggression is referred to as an “explosion,” and
successfully controlling one’s anger is conceptualized in terms of
“keeping one’s cool” (Lakoff, 1987).
Traditionally, it has been thought that such metaphors are
merely a convenient means of expressing abstract ideas (e.g.,
Fainsilber & Ortony, 1989). However, this position has recently
been challenged in psycholinguistic circles. Lakoff (1987); Gibbs
(1994), and others (e.g., Ko¨vecses, 2000) contend that anger-
related metaphors capture something important and basic to the
manner in which abstract concepts are cognitively represented.
Metaphors are specifically seen as useful in grounding abstract
concepts (e.g., anger) in terms of concrete physical experiences
(e.g., heat). When a child begins to learn the difference between
emotional experiences, communicating such differences can be
inherently difficult. Consider the following dialogue sequence
from the TV show, Star Trek: The Next Generation (Taylor,
Moore, & Singer, 1993), in which the android (i.e., robot) char-
acter, Data, is attempting to discern if he has felt anger for the first
time ever:
GEORDI: Data . . . no offense, but how would you know an actual
flash of anger from some kind of odd power surge?
DATA: You are correct in that I have no frame of reference to confirm
my hypothesis. . . Perhaps you could describe what it feels like when
you get angry . . . I could then use that as a reference.
GEORDI: Well . . . when I get angry, first I. . . begin to feel . . .
hostile. . .
DATA: Could you describe feeling hostile?
GEORDI: It’s like feeling . . . belligerent. . . combative. . .
DATA: Can you describe feeling angry without referring to other
feelings?
GEORDI: No. I guess I can’t. I just . . . feel angry.
Benjamin M. Wilkowski, University of Wyoming; Brian P. Meier,
Gettysburg College; Michael D. Robinson, North Dakota State University;
Margaret S. Carter, Gettysburg College; Roger Feltman, University of
Rochester.
The first author acknowledges support from NSF’s Experimental Pro-
gram to Stimulate Competitive Research (NSF/EPSCoR). The second
author acknowledges the support of students (Maura Downey, Kate Har-
grove, Dave Hauser, Alicia Kelly, Claire Kelly, Anna Markowtiz, Andrew
Mitchell, Brita Robson, Matt Salter, Sarah Sherman, Megan Tanguay, and
Lauren Von Heill) from his Spring, 2007 Experimental Social Psychology
course at Gettysburg College.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Benjamin
M. Wilkowski, University of Wyoming, Department of Psychology, Depart-
ment 3415, 1000 East University Avenue, Laramie, WY 82071. E-mail:
BWilkows@uwyo.edu
Emotion © 2009 American Psychological Association
2009, Vol. 9, No. 4, 464– 477 1528-3542/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0015764
464
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
... People who frequently behave aggressively can be called "hot-headed," and those who temporarily act in a hostile manner are described by themselves and others as "being heated." Research has even demonstrated that impulsive and risky behaviors are associated with warm words [8]. However, this is not merely an implicit association; something casual about this relationship between heat and aggression makes us more reactive to potential adverse events and how it changes how humans perceive the environment around them. ...
... This starts even before actual heat is applied, as some experiments have demonstrated that even simply priming participants with the idea of heat can increase aggressive behaviors, thoughts, and feelings. In one such study, participants were shown images and words commonly associated with heat [8]. The research team found that priming some participants with heat-related words and imagery led participants to perceive neutral facial expressions as aggressive and to have more aggressive thoughts. ...
Article
Full-text available
As the climate crisis has progressed, scientists have begun to ask, "How does a rapidly warming ecosystem impact human behavior?" As aggression scholars and non-professional environmentalists, it has become increasingly clear that the impact of the climate crisis, without intervention, will dramatically increase humans' exposure to risk factors known to cause aggressive and violent behavior. This article describes a model that explains the indirect and direct effects of the climate crisis on aggression and violence, both on an individual level and broadly on group dynamics. We propose that systemic and global intervention strategies must be adopted to mitigate the severity of the climate crisis. This manuscript outlines the research demonstrating how climate change will increase human aggression and violence and then discusses potential interventions, such as holistic policies addressing immigration and income inequality. In discussing this topic, we provide insight into the intersectional nature of the consequences of rapid global warming.
... In this study, we used the concept of bodily weight as a proxy for valence. Besides bodily weight, other metaphors have also been related to emotions, such as body temperature (warm vs. cold), pressure, comfort/discomfort, tension, or enhancement (e.g., Barbosa Escobar et al., 2021;Bergman et al., 2015;Borhani et al., 2017;Kövecses, 1990;Rolls et al., 2008;Waggoner, 2010;Wilkowski et al., 2009). Thus, weight is only one form in which valence can be grounded in the body, and our choice of using bodily weight limits the generalizability of our findings. ...
... Future research should consider alternative ways to capture valence by BSMs to better understand which emotion is best captured by which concept, and whether similar BSMs can be found for different valencerelated concepts. Regarding body temperature, it should be noted, however, that both positive and negative emotions (e.g., love, anger) have been associated with warmth (e.g., Kövecses, 2003;Lakoff & Kövecses, 1987;Wilkowski et al., 2009), suggesting that body temperature might not be a straightforward proxy for valence across different emotions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Bodily sensation mapping (BSM) is a recently developed self-report tool for the assessment of emotions in which people draw their sensations of activation in a body silhouette. Following the circumplex model of affect, activity and valence are the underling dimensions of every emotional experience. The aim of this study was to introduce the neglected valence dimension in BSM. We found that participants systematically report valence-related sensations of bodily lightness for positive emotions (happiness, love, pride), and sensations of bodily heaviness in response to negative emotions (e.g., anger, fear, sadness, depression) with specific body topography (Experiment 1). Further experiments showed that both computers (using a machine learning approach) and humans recognize emotions better when classification is based on the combined activity-and valence-related BSMs compared to either type of BSM alone (Experiments 2 and 3), suggesting that both types of bodily sensations reflect distinct parts of emotion knowledge. Importantly, participants found it clearer to indicate their bodily sensations induced by sadness and depression in terms of bodily weight than bodily activity (Experiment 2 and 4), suggesting that the added value of valence-related BSMs is particularly relevant for the assessment of emotions at the negative end of the valence spectrum.
... Uncomfortably warm participants were not only more likely to be hostile themselves than comfortable participants (thereby replicating similar effects found in but also perceived the video clip interactions as containing more hostility and aggression. This effect works not only when participants are hot themselves, but also when they are only primed with the idea of heat (e.g., Wilkowski et al., 2009). They found that participants who were exposed to images related to heat were more likely to judge neutral facial expressions as aggressive and were more likely to have aggressive thoughts than control condition participants. ...
... Now, consider some basic psychological findings that also underpin the heat/aggression relation. The first comes from embodied cognition research, which suggests that humans are incredibly responsive and susceptible to the stimuli in their environment, which ultimately influences how we think and feel (Wilkowski et al., 2009). Research shows that hot temperatures produce discomfort and irritability, and then we can clearly see how being in an uncomfortably hot environment can yield biases in aggression-related precursors such as hostile perceptions, hostile feelings, and aggressive inclinations. ...
Book
Much of the current rhetoric surrounding climate change focuses on the physical changes to the environment and the resulting material damage to infrastructure and resources. Although there has been some dialogue about secondary effects (namely mass migration), little effort has been given to understanding how rapid climate change is affecting people on group and individual levels. In this Element, we examine the psychological impacts of climate change, especially focused on how it will lead to increases in aggressive behaviors and violent conflict, and how it will influence other aspects of human behavior. We also look at previously established psychological effects and use them to help explain changes in human behavior resulting from rapid climate change, as well as to propose actions that can be taken to reduce climate change itself and mitigate harmful effects on humans.
... For example, metaphors linking anger with heat ("HOT-HEADED," "BURNING, SIMMERING, OR BOILING WITH RAGE") occur in very different languages and presumably reflect an embodied experience and metonymic connection: anger is commonly associated with flushing and feelings of heat in the face or head. This metaphoric association is found across disparate cultures, even where emotion lexicons vary substantially (Matsuki, 1995;Yu, 1995) and reflects the physiology of anger (Wilkowski et al., 2009). In contrast, loneliness, loss of social connection, and isolation may be described as cold (IJzerman et al., 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
This issue of Transcultural Psychiatry presents selected papers from the McGill Advanced Study Institute on “Cultural Poetics of Illness and Healing.” The meeting addressed the cognitive science of language, metaphor, and poiesis from embodied and enactivist perspectives; how cultural affordances, background knowledge, discourse, and practices enable and constrain poiesis; the cognitive and social poetics of symptom and illness experience; and the politics and practice of poetics in healing ritual, psychotherapy, and recovery. This introductory essay outlines an approach to illness experience and its transformation in healing practices that emphasizes embodied processes of metaphor as well as the social processes of self-construal and positioning through material and discursive engagements with the cultural affordances that constitute our local worlds. The approach has implications for theory building, training, and clinical practice in psychiatry.
... Several experimental psychological studies have found a direct positive relationship between temperature and aggression-high temperatures enhance aggression and criminal tendencies (Anderson, 1989;DeWall and Bushman, 2009). Being randomly assigned to a hot room rather than a suitably warm room makes subjects more likely to be hostile and to behave more aggressively toward others (DeWall et al., 2011); presenting words or pictures associated with high temperatures also makes subjects more irritable (Wilkowski et al., 2009). In addition to studies based on experimental paradigms, many scholars have also studied the relationship between temperature and crime based on correlational data. ...
Article
Full-text available
Climate change is having profound effects on natural and socio-economic systems, especially via extreme climate events. Using panel data from 129 prefectural-level cities in China from 2013 to 2019, this paper explores the effects of extreme climate on crime rates based on a climate index and manual collection of crime data. The results showed that extreme climate has a significant positive effect on crime rates, increasing by 0.035% for every 1% increase in the extreme climate index. This occurs through two mechanistic pathways: reduced agricultural output and lower employment income. The heterogeneity analysis shows that extreme climate has a greater impact on crime rates in eastern areas which are economically developed and have high levels of immigration. This study provides new perspectives on the impact of extreme climate on the economy and society, in which governments can actively participate in climate governance through environmental protection, energy conservation and emission reduction, and technological innovation to reduce crime rates by reducing the occurrence of extreme climate.
... It has been suggested that emotion concepts are represented in language (Lindquist & Gendron, 2013) and that metaphors reflect their cognitive representation (Wilkowski et al., 2009). Nevertheless, it remains unclear how the metaphorical meaning that links red with anger developed. ...
Preprint
Past studies have shown that higher levels of anger are inferred from faces that show increased facial redness (red-anger effect). However, all previous research in support of this effect has used within-subjects designs with varying levels of facial redness. We hypothesized that very frequent random changes in facial redness paired with limited options to respond to such changes can induce demand characteristics. As the color red has also been found to be strongly associated with anger, it may be obvious for participants that they are expected to respond to facial redness by indicating higher levels of perceived anger. In a preregistered close replication (Study 1, N = 40) conducted online, we observed the red-anger effect but were able to show that approximately one fifth of the participants were suspicious about the purpose of the study. If the effect is independent of demand characteristics, it should also emerge when facial redness is manipulated between-subjects. In a preregistered conceptual replication (Study 2, N = 329), we found the red-anger effect in a within-subjects design condition but not in a between-subjects design condition. We conclude that past findings of the red-anger effect were (at least partially) due to demand characteristics. Future research should (a) attend to demand characteristics, (b) use a broader diversity of methodological approaches, and (c) develop and test explanations about processes that are related to color in emotion perception.
... For example, a metaphorical expression of "cold and lonely" can be constructed by repeated experiencing body warmth and physical proximity of caregivers from infancy. The CMT has been an influential theory in embodied research and a variety of embodied metaphors have been explored, such as importance is heaviness (Jostmann et al., 2009), affection is physical warmth (Zhang & Risen, 2014), anger is heat (Wilkowski et al., 2009), and power is up (Schubert, 2005). As afore introduced, sourness is a common linguistic metaphor for envy and jealousy. ...
Article
Full-text available
In the present study, five experiments (N = 233) were designed to explore whether sourness as a sensory experience could implicitly impact social emotions of envy and jealousy in Chinese culture. Experiment 1 (n = 63) explored the implicit conceptual association between sourness words (vs. bitterness words) and envy/jealousy words. Experiment 2 (n = 70) and 3 (n = 20) examined the priming effects of imagined and tasted sourness (vs. bitterness and sweetness) on self-rated emotional intensity in envy- and jealousy-arousing situations, respectively. Experiment 4 (n = 40) and 5 (n = 40) further testified the priming effects of imagined and tasted sourness (vs. bitterness and sweetness) on self-rated emotional intensity in four types of social situations (i.e., envy, jealousy, sad and happy events), respectively. In the results, sourness was found as the only taste that not only conceptually associated with envy/jealousy, but also significantly primed envy/jealousy feelings. The possible mechanism underlying the association of sourness-envy/jealousy was discussed.
... This is consistent with the embodied perspective that states a person's understandings of abstract concepts are connected with sensorimotor experiences (Barsalou, 2008). This supports research WEIGHT CUES 13 in other areas that show additional concepts, such as anger, can influence a person's physical experience with temperature (Wilkowski, Meier, Robinson, Carter, & Feltman, 2009). ...
Thesis
http://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/77631/1/davidre.pdf
... A possibility is that valence is related to embodied process of comfort as warmer temperatures are comfortable but extreme temperatures at both ends can generate discomfort [47]. These results are consistent with Wilkowski et al. [93] as the authors suggested people from different cultures use metaphorical expressions of hot and negative emotions (e.g., anger). These results also are also in line with Baylis et al. [62], who found that expressions of positive emotions in social media were the highest at 20˚C and decreased beyond 30˚C, at which point negative emotions also increased. ...
Article
Full-text available
Emotions and temperature are closely related through embodied processes, and people seem to associate temperature concepts with emotions. While this relationship is often evidenced by everyday language (e.g., cold and warm feelings), what remains missing to date is a systematic study that holistically analyzes how and why people associate specific temperatures with emotions. The present research aimed to investigate the associations between temperature concepts and emotion adjectives on both explicit and implicit levels. In Experiment 1, we evaluated explicit associations between twelve pairs of emotion adjectives derived from the circumplex model of affect, and five different temperature concepts ranging from 0˚C to 40˚C, based on responses from 403 native speakers of four different languages (English, Spanish, Japanese, Chinese). The results of Experiment 1 revealed that, across languages, the temperatures were associated with different regions of the circumplex model. The 0˚C and 10˚C were associated with negative-valanced, low-arousal emotions, while 20˚C was associated with positive-valanced, low-to-medium-arousal emotions. Moreover, 30˚C was associated with positive-valanced, high-arousal emotions; and 40˚C was associated with high-arousal and either positive- or negative-valanced emotions. In Experiment 2 (N = 102), we explored whether these temperature-emotion associations were also present at the implicit level, by conducting Implicit Association Tests (IATs) with temperature words (cold and hot) and opposing pairs of emotional adjectives for each dimension of valence (Unhappy/Dissatisfied vs. Happy/Satisfied) and arousal (Passive/Quiet vs. Active/Alert) on native English speakers. The results of Experiment 2 revealed that participants held implicit associations between the word hot and positive-valanced and high-arousal emotions. Additionally, the word cold was associated with negative-valanced and low arousal emotions. These findings provide evidence for the existence of temperature-emotion associations at both explicit and implicit levels across languages.
Article
Full-text available
The present research examines visual perception of emotion in both typical and atypical development. To examine the processes by which perceptual mechanisms become attuned to the contingencies of affective signals in the environment, the authors measured the sequential, content- based properties of feature detection in emotion recognition processes. To evaluate the role of experience, they compared typically developing children with physically abused children, who were presumed to have experienced high levels of threat and hostility. As predicted, physically abused children accurately identified facial displays of anger on the basis of less sensory input than did controls, which suggests that physically abused children have facilitated access to representations of anger. The findings are discussed in terms of experiential processes in perceptual learning.
Article
Full-text available
An evolved module for fear elicitation and fear learning with 4 characteristics is proposed. (a) The fear module is preferentially activated in aversive contexts by stimuli that are fear relevant in an evolutionary perspective. (b) Its activation to such stimuli is automatic. (c) It is relatively impenetrable to cognitive control. (d) It originates in a dedicated neural circuitry, centered on the amygdala. Evidence supporting these propositions is reviewed from conditioning studies, both in humans and in monkeys; illusory correlation studies; studies using unreportable stimuli; and studies from animal neuroscience. The fear module is assumed to mediate an emotional level of fear learning that is relatively independent and dissociable from cognitive learning of stimulus relationships.
Article
Full-text available
Do people aggress to make themselves feel better? We adapted a procedure used by G. K. Manucia, D. J. Baumann, and R. B. Cialdini (1984), in which some participants are given a bogus mood-freezing pill that makes affect regulation efforts ineffective. In Study 1, people who had been induced to believe in the value of catharsis and venting anger responded more aggressively than did control participants to insulting criticism, but this aggression was eliminated by the mood-freezing pill. Study 2 showed similar results among people with high anger-out (i.e., expressing and venting anger) tendencies. Studies 3 and 4 provided questionnaire data consistent with these interpretations, and Study 5 replicated the findings of Studies I and 2 using measures more directly concerned with affect regulation. Taken together, these results suggest that many people may engage in aggression to regulate (improve) their own affective states.
Article
Two experiments used a priming paradigm to investigate the influence of racial cues on the perceptual identification of weapons. In Experiment 1, participants identified guns faster when primed with Black faces compared with White faces. In Experiment 2, participants were required to respond quickly, causing the racial bias to shift from reaction time to accuracy. Participants misidentified tools as guns more often when primed with a Black face than with a White face. L. L. Jacoby's (1991) process dissociation procedure was applied to demonstrate that racial primes influenced automatic (A) processing, but not controlled (C) processing. The response deadline reduced the C estimate but not the A estimate. The motivation to control prejudice moderated the relationship between explicit prejudice and automatic bias. Implications are discussed on applied and theoretical levels.
Book
The now-classic Metaphors We Live By changed our understanding of metaphor and its role in language and the mind. Metaphor, the authors explain, is a fundamental mechanism of mind, one that allows us to use what we know about our physical and social experience to provide understanding of countless other subjects. Because such metaphors structure our most basic understandings of our experience, they are "metaphors we live by"--metaphors that can shape our perceptions and actions without our ever noticing them. In this updated edition of Lakoff and Johnson's influential book, the authors supply an afterword surveying how their theory of metaphor has developed within the cognitive sciences to become central to the contemporary understanding of how we think and how we express our thoughts in language.
Book
To what extent and in what ways is metaphorical thought relevant to an understanding of culture and society? More specifically: can the cognitive linguistic view of metaphor simultaneously explain both universality and diversity in metaphorical thought? Cognitive linguists have done important work on universal aspects of metaphor, but they have paid much less attention to why metaphors vary both interculturally and intraculturally as extensively as they do. In this book, Zoltán Kövecses proposes a new theory of metaphor variation. First, he identifies the major dimension of metaphor variation, that is, those social and cultural boundaries that signal discontinuities in human experience. Second, he describes which components, or aspects of conceptual metaphor are involved in metaphor variation, and how they are involved. Third, he isolates the main causes of metaphor variation. Fourth Professor Kövecses addresses the issue to the degree of cultural coherence in the interplay among conceptual metaphors, embodiment, and causes of metaphor variation. © Cambridge University Press 2005 and Cambridge University Press, 2010.