ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Purpose: To test the efficacy of an acute intervention derived from acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) for increasing high-intensity constant work rate (CWR) cycle exercise tolerance in a group of low-active women age 18-45 yr. The secondary goals were to examine whether ACT would reduce perceived effort and improve in-task affect during exercise and increase postexercise enjoyment. Methods: In a randomized controlled trial, 39 women were randomized to either the experimental (using ACT-based cognitive techniques and listening to music during the CWR exercise tests) or a control group (listening to music during the CWR exercise tests). Before (CWR-1) and after the intervention (CWR-2), participants completed a CWR cycle exercise test at 80% of maximal incremental work rate (Wmax) until volitional exhaustion. Results: On average, ACT (n = 18) and control (n = 21) groups were matched for age, body mass index, weekly leisure activity scores, and Wmax (all P > 0.05). Exercise tolerance time (ETT) increased by 15% from CWR-1 to CWR-2 for the ACT group (392.05 ± 146.4 vs 459.39 ± 209.3 s; mean ± SD) and decreased by 8% (384.71 ± 120.1 vs 353.86 ± 127.9 s) for the control group (P = 0.008). RPE were lower (e.g., by 1.5 Borg 6-20 scale units at 55% of ETT, P ≤ 0.01) during CWR-2 in the ACT versus that in the control group. By contrast, ACT had no effect on in-task affect. Exercise enjoyment was higher after CWR-2 in the ACT group versus that in the control group (P < 0.001). Conclusions: An acute ACT intervention increased high-intensity ETT and postexercise enjoyment and reduced perceived effort in low-active women. Further investigations of ACT as an effective intervention for enhancing the established health benefits of high-intensity exercise need to be provided.
Acceptance and Commitment Therapy
Improves Exercise Tolerance in
Sedentary Women
ELENA IVANOVA
1
, DENNIS JENSEN
2
, JAMIE CASSOFF
1
, FEI GU
1
, and BA
¨RBEL KNA
¨UPER
1
1
Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, CANADA; and
2
Department of Kinesiology and Physical
Education, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, CANADA
ABSTRACT
IVANOVA, E., D. JENSEN, J. CASSOFF, F. GU, and B. KNA
¨UPER. Acceptance and Commitment Therapy Improves Exercise
Tolerance in Sedentary Women. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc., Vol. 47, No. 6, pp. 1251–1258, 2015. Purpose: To test the efficacy of an acute
intervention derived from acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) for increasing high-intensity constant work rate (CWR) cycle
exercise tolerance in a group of low-active women age 18–45 yr. The secondary goals were to examine whether ACT would reduce
perceived effort and improve in-task affect during exercise and increase postexercise enjoyment. Methods: In a randomized controlled
trial, 39 women were randomized to either the experimental (using ACT-based cognitive techniques and listening to music during the
CWR exercise tests) or a control group (listening to music during the CWR exercise tests). Before (CWR-1) and after the intervention
(CWR-2), participants completed a CWR cycle exercise test at 80% of maximal incremental work rate (W
max
) until volitional exhaustion.
Results: On average, ACT (n= 18) and control (n= 21) groups were matched for age, body mass index, weekly leisure activity scores,
and W
max
(all P90.05). Exercise tolerance time (ETT) increased by 15% from CWR-1 to CWR-2 for the ACT group (392.05 T146.4 vs
459.39 T209.3 s; mean TSD) and decreased by 8% (384.71 T120.1 vs 353.86 T127.9 s) for the control group (P= 0.008). RPE were
lower (e.g., by 1.5 Borg 6–20 scale units at 55% of ETT, Pe0.01) during CWR-2 in the ACT versus that in the control group. By
contrast, ACT had no effect on in-task affect. Exercise enjoyment was higher after CWR-2 in the ACT group versus that in the control
group (PG0.001). Conclusions: An acute ACT intervention increased high-intensity ETT and postexercise enjoyment and reduced
perceived effort in low-active women. Further investigations of ACT as an effective intervention for enhancing the established health
benefits of high-intensity exercise need to be provided. Key Words: ACCEPTANCE AND COMMITMENT THERAPY, EXERCISE
TOLERANCE, RPE, EXERCISE ENJOYMENT
Physical inactivity is an independent risk factor for
increased cardiometabolic morbidity and mortality
(30). Nevertheless, only 3.2% of women in North
America meet the recommended physical activity guidelines
(46). High-intensity exercise offers comparable or superior
health benefits (e.g., improved cardiorespiratory fitness) as
low- or moderate-intensity exercise in both clinical and
healthy populations (16). In fact, patients with cardiac dis-
eases gain equivalent health benefits after a 12-wk low-
volume, high-intensity intervention (10 1-min cycling
intervals at È90% of maximal work rate [W
max
]) compared to
a less time-efficient moderate-intensity intervention (30 min
of continuous cycling at È60% W
max
) (9). In addition to
time efficiency, recent research showed that 10 wk of high-
intensity exercise training was associated with a 2.3-fold
greater improvement (+17.9% vs 7.9%) in V
˙O
2peak
among
patients with coronary artery disease than 10 wk of training
at moderate intensities (40).
Theoretical and empirical research, however, suggests
that participation in high-intensity activities may be under-
mined by perception of effort (as measured by RPE) (5) and
negative affect (as measured by pain ratings or the Feeling
Scale [FS]) (20), particularly among women (33,41). The
psychobiological model of endurance performance (37),
based on the motivational intensity theory (7), suggests that
exercise tolerance is based on the perception of effort (i.e.,
how hard, heavy, and strenuous exercise is being perceived)
(36) and on potential motivation (i.e., the maximum effort an
individual is willing to exert). Consequently, factors that can
target either or both components of the model may have the
potential to increase exercise tolerance (e.g., Blanchfield et al.
[4] and Marcora et al. [37]). In regards to affective responses
to exercise, the Dual-Mode Model (DMM) (10), based on
the hedonic theory of motivation (11), states that pleasant or
unpleasant responses during exercise are determined by the
Address for correspondence: Elena Ivanova, M.A., Department of Psychol-
ogy, McGill University, 1205 Dr. Penfield Avenue, Stewart Biology Building,
Montreal, QC, Canada H3A 1B1; E-mail: elena.ivanova2@mail.mcgill.ca.
Submitted for publication July 2014.
Accepted for publication September 2014.
Supplemental digital content is available for this article. Direct URL citations
appear in the printed text and are provided in the HTML and PDF versions of
this article on the journal’s Web site (www.acsm-msse.org).
0195-9131/15/4706-1251/0
MEDICINE & SCIENCE IN SPORTS & EXERCISE
Ò
Copyright Ó2014 by the American College of Sports Medicine
DOI: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000536
1251
APPLIED SCIENCES
Copyright © 2015 by the American College of Sports Medicine. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
interaction of cognitive factors (e.g., self-efficacy) and in-
teroceptive feedback (e.g., physical discomfort) during ex-
ercise. The exercise intensity influences the relative salience
of each factor in regulating the exercise-related affective
responses (10). Primarily, an increase in exercise intensity
(e.g., above the ventilatory threshold) is accompanied by a
decline in effect for most individuals because the intero-
ceptive cues stimulated by the exercise become more salient
(10). Consequently, to effectively manage the demands of
exercising, optimal techniques that improve affective re-
sponses to exercise and reduce perceived effort need to be
identified, with the aim to sustain exercise tolerance at in-
tensities that confer health benefits (e.g., Rognmo et al.
[40]). Research efforts have so far focused on the use of
diverting attention/distraction (e.g., listening to music) and
association techniques (e.g., self-talk with a focus on the
exercise experience), with some evidence that distraction
techniques can have positive effects on RPE and in-task
effect at low and moderate intensities (e.g., 60% and 75%
of maximal HR) (6), whereas association techniques may
decrease perceived effort at higher intensities (e.g., 80%
of maximal work rate) (4). As the exercise intensity in-
creases (e.g., 90% V
˙O
2max
) (45), distraction techniques such
as music become less effective at reducing perceived effort
and maintaining exercise tolerance. Indeed, a comprehen-
sive review of the literature showed that music has limited
effects on reducing perceived effort during exercise at in-
tensities above the anaerobic threshold (29). According
to the effort-related model (44), sustaining attention on an
external stimulus like music becomes effortful during high-
intensity workloads owing to difficulties with diverting
attention away from interoceptive (sensory) feedback. It
follows that alternative cognitive strategies to distraction are
required to help individuals better cope with the perceived
effort during strenuous exercise. An important study by
Blanchfield et al. (4) recently found that 12 recreationally
active (V
˙O
2peak
È53 mLIkg
j1
Imin
j1
) men and women
randomly assigned to a 2-wk motivational self-talk (i.e., an
associative technique) intervention reported greater mean
improvements in perceived effort and exercise duration
during high-intensity constant work rate (CWR) cycle ex-
ercise testing at 80% of maximal work rate (W
max
) than a
group of 12 age-, sex-, and V
˙O
2peak
-matched adults ran-
domly assigned to usual exercise without self-talk inter-
vention control group. It remains to be determined, however,
whether similar improvements in perceived effort at high-
intensity exercise performance can be achieved through
an acute (single-session) application of cognitive techniques
in physically inactive women, namely, women who are most
in need of such improvements (e.g., Schaeffer et al. [41]).
Acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) is a form of
psychotherapy (21) that is widely used in clinical settings for
the treatment and management of psychological disorders,
and more recently, it has been applied to health-related be-
haviors, including cigarette smoking cessation (17), self-
management of diabetes (19), and weight loss (15). One of
the core assumptions in ACT is that negative and unpleasant
feelings and experiences are neither good nor bad, but rather
a facet of human life (21). Thus, acceptance-based tech-
niques focus on increasing an individuals’ willingness to
experience aversive feelings, thoughts, and sensations, with-
out trying to change or eliminate them (8,34). We proposed
that acceptance-based techniques can help to improve af-
fective responses, reduce perceived effort, and increase ex-
ercise tolerance during a high-intensity exercise test because
ACT teaches individuals how to accept and defuse from the
unpleasant internal experiences (e.g., exercise-related pain),
in turn, for a behavior that they value (i.e., physical activity).
Empirical evidence provides convincing support for the use
of cognitive techniques derived from ACT (22) rather than
suppression or distraction techniques (e.g., Masedo and
Esteve [38]) in helping individuals to tolerate physical dis-
comfort and pain. For example, a study of 219 young adults
by Masedo and Esteve (38) randomized to the ACT inter-
vention group were È60% and 20% more tolerant to the
pain and distress associated with the cold pressor test than
individuals randomized to the suppression and spontaneous
coping intervention groups, respectively. In light of these
findings, the general aim of our theoretically grounded and
randomized study was to examine the potential benefits of
two cognitive techniques derived from ACT on exercise
tolerance time (ETT) during a high-intensity cycle exercise
test in apparently healthy, young, and low-active women.
Our primary aim, therefore, was to test the hypothesis that a
single session of ACT training will improve high-intensity
(80% W
max
) CWR cycle ETT in these women. The sec-
ondary aims were to examine whether these improvements
in ETT are accompanied by improvements in perceived ef-
fort, in-task affect, and postexercise enjoyment.
METHODS
Participants
Thirty-nine nonsmoking, low-active, and nonpregnant or
nursing women age 18–45 yr (23 T5 yr; mean TSD) with no
known or suspected cardiometabolic, pulmonary, and/or
musculoskeletal disorder(s) and not taking psychotropic med-
ications took part in this study.
Experimental Design, Randomization, and Procedures
As illustrated in Figure 1, this was a single-center, pretest–
posttest, parallel-group, randomized controlled trial. The study
protocol received ethical approval from the Institutional Re-
view Board of the Faculty of Medicine at McGill University
(IRB no. A00-B39-12B) in accordance with the Declaration
of Helsinki.
After providing written informed consent, eligible individ-
uals recruited by word of mouth and posted announcements
in Montreal and the surrounding areas and participated in two
testing visits separated by Q48 h and completed within 1 wk.
http://www.acsm-msse.org1252 Official Journal of the American College of Sports Medicine
APPLIED SCIENCES
Copyright © 2015 by the American College of Sports Medicine. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Participants were asked to avoid caffeine and/or energy drinks
before testing and to not exercise 24 h before testing.
Visit 1 included the completion of baseline questionnaires
followed by an exercise test until volitional exhaustion on
an electronically braked cycle ergometer (Ergoline 800s;
SensorMedics, Yorba Linda, CA) for familiarization pur-
poses and for the determination of W
max
. Incremental exer-
cise tests consisted of a steady-rate resting period of 3 min
followed by 25-W increases in work rate (starting at 25 W)
to the point of volitional exhaustion: W
max
was defined as
the highest cycle work rate the participant was able to sus-
tain for Q30 s. The criterion for stopping the incremental
exercise test was volitional exhaustion, which was assessed
by 1) instructing the participants to give a clear indication
(verbal or by waving their hand) that they have reached their
absolute maximum effort and/or 2) by informing the par-
ticipant that the test will be terminated if the pedal cadence
falls below 50 revolutions per minute (rpm). Verbal encour-
agements were provided.
Visit 2 included two CWR cycle exercise tests at 80%
W
max
until volitional exhaustion: one before (CWR-1) and
another after (CWR-2) the ACT or control intervention (see
below). The time between the two CWR exercise tests was
approximately 50 min (È40-min intervention and È10-min
preparatory and waiting period); refer to Figure 1 for a flow-
chart of the study design. High-intensity CWR cycle exercise
tests to exhaustion were selected because they permit opti-
mal control of workload and are as sensitive as cycle time-
trial tests to detect changes in ETT (our primary outcome
variable) in response to an intervention (1). The criterion for
stopping the submaximal incremental exercise tests was
volitional exhaustion, and the same instructions were pro-
vided to participants as during the incremental exercise test
(Visit 1). Verbal encouragements were not provided. CWR
exercise tests consisted of 1) a steady-rate resting period of
3 min, 2) a 1-min warm-up at 25% W
max
while listening to
music followed by a step increase in work rate to 80% W
max
with participants cycling to volitional exhaustion while lis-
tening to music, and 3) a 3-min cool-down period. Partici-
pants provided ratings to the FS, the Felt Arousal Scale (FAS),
and Borg_s 6–20 category ratio scale of perceived effort at
rest, within the last 15 s of every second minute during ex-
ercise and immediately after peak was reached. Similarly, par-
ticipants were asked to identify whether they had an associative
or distractive thought using the Association/Dissociation Scale
within the first 15 s of every second minute during exercise
and immediately after peak was reached. The scales presented
during the exercise tests were shown in a fixed order. Imme-
diately after the 3-min cool-down period, participants com-
pleted the FS, FAS, Physical Activity Enjoyment Scale (PACES),
and responded to the manipulation check items, with all the
scales presented in a fixed order. The manipulation check
items included the following: 1) ‘‘How confident are you that
you learned the acceptance-based techniques that we taught
you?,’’ with responses ranging from 1 (not at all confident) to
5 (very confident); and 2) ‘‘How frequently did you use the
acceptance-based techniques during the last cycle exercise
test?,’’ with responses ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (all the
time). ETT was defined as the duration of loaded pedaling.
Interventions
Online randomization software (www.randomization.com)
was used to assign eligible participants to either of the two
interventions upon arrival to the laboratory for testing. The
research assistants conducting CWR-1 and CWR-2 exercise
tests were blinded to the group allocation, whereas the ACT
interventionist (E.I.) was not present during exercise testing
at Visit 2.
ACT intervention. Participants randomized to the ex-
perimental group received a one-time È40-min intervention
(and additional È10 min in preparatory/waiting period) in
which they were taught cognitive defusion and acceptance
techniques for coping with aversive physical discomfort
(e.g., leg discomfort) and negative affect (e.g., boredom)
during CWR-2. The intent of cognitive defusion was to help
participants disentangle their physical sensations (e.g.,
burning legs) from their thoughts (e.g., ‘‘I must stop exercis-
ing’’) and behaviors (e.g., quit exercising). Acceptance tech-
niques were used to help increase participants_willingness
to experience consequences of a valued behavior (e.g., exer-
cising) that may carry unpleasant physical sensations, without
trying to change, control, or eliminate them (22). To illustrate
both techniques, metaphors (adapted from Hayes et al. [21])
and an experiential exercise involving the use of an ice
cube were utilized. The manual is available as a supplemental
FIGURE 1—Flowchart diagram of the trial.
ACT TECHNIQUES AND EXERCISE DURATION Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise
d
1253
APPLIED SCIENCES
Copyright © 2015 by the American College of Sports Medicine. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
material online. [See table, Supplemental Digital Content 1,
Acceptance-based techniques (defusion + acceptance),
http://links.lww.com/MSS/A449.]
No-ACT intervention. Participants randomized to the
no-ACT intervention control group watched a short video and
created concrete goals for when, where, and how they would
increase their overall physical activity levels upon study
completion. Time spent with the interventionist (È40 min)
and the preparatory and waiting period (È10 min) was
matched to that of the ACT condition. After È50 min, the
participants completed CWR-2 and were asked to refer to
the music as a means of coping with the high intensity.
Music Selection
During both CWR exercise tests, the participants listened
to music (using headphones) that they selected according to
their own preference from four genres: Pop, Hip-Hop and
R&B, Electronic Dance, and Alternative Rock. Twelve songs
from each of the aforementioned genres were selected from
the Billboard Canadian Hot 100 (2011–2013) and scanned
using BeatScanner software: only songs with a tempo of
135–140 beats per minute (considered optimal for high-
intensity exercise) (28) were included. Eligible songs were
then purchased from Yes!Music Fitness, downloaded to
iTunes, and synchronized to an Apple iPod Touch.
Measured Parameters
HR. This was monitored at rest and during exercise using
a Polar HR monitor with chest band (Lachine, QC, Canada).
RPE. These were measured using Borg_s 6–20 category
ratio scale (5), where ‘‘6’’ represents ‘‘no exertion at all’’ and
‘20’’ represents ‘‘maximal exertion.’
In-task affective responses. The circumplex model
guided the selection of measures used to repeatedly assess
affect during the CWR exercise tests. Specifically, affect can
be defined by two dimensions: 1) affective valence (pleasure–
displeasure) assessed by the FS (20), with responses ranging
from j5 (very bad) to +5 (very good); and 2) perceived
activation (low-high) assessed using the FAS (42), with re-
sponses ranging from 1 (low arousal) to 6 (high arousal). Both
the FS and FAS are single-item measures that can be repeat-
edly administrated over a short period. The FS demonstrates
strong concurrent and discriminant validity (12,42).
Attentional focus. The Association/Dissociation Scale
(43) is a 10-point bipolar scale, with 1 representing associa-
tion (e.g., thoughts related to the exercise) and 10 representing
dissociation (e.g., distraction-related thoughts). This scale has
established concurrent validity (3).
Physical activity enjoyment scale. The PACES (31),
an 18-item measure incorporating a 7-point Likert scale
(e.g., 1 [It is very pleasant or I dislike it] to 7 [It is no fun at
all or I like it]), was administered immediately after the cool-
down period of CWR-1 and CWR-2. The PACES has good
predictive and discriminant validity (31).
Statistical Analysis
All statistical analyses were implemented by SAS 9.3, and
power analysis was conducted using G*Power 3 (13). On
the basis of past findings on the effectiveness of ACT tech-
niques on promoting physical activity adherence (8), we
determined a priori that Q36 individuals (Q18 per group)
were required to detect a moderate effect size of G
2
= 0.15
on our primary outcome variable (ETT) with >and power
set at 0.05 and 0.80, respectively. The ETT variables were
positively skewed, and natural log transformations were
applied to the original variables before the main analysis
(14). Owing to the variability in ETT for each participant, in-
task affect (FS), activation (FAS), RPE, and association/
dissociation were generated through linear interpolations
between obtained adjacent values and anchored at baseline,
20% of ETT, 40% of ETT, 55% of ETT, and at peak exercise.
Baseline characteristics between the groups were com-
pared using independent-sample t-tests and analyzed only
for the participants who completed all testing sessions. Mixed-
effect model was estimated using PROC MIXED, and con-
trasts were specified in the model to compare ETT from
CWR-1 and CWR-2 both within and between groups and
to test the associations between the groups (ACT vs control)
and the repeated measurements during CWR-2 (RPE, FS,
FAS, association/dissociation) and immediately after the
CWR-2 (FS, FAS, and PACES). Owing to the technical dif-
ficulties in data collection, some outcome variables contained
missing values, which were accommodated for by using the
full information maximum likelihood estimator in PROC
MIXED. An advantage of using the full information maxi-
mum likelihood estimator is that the observation in which
the outcome variable is missing can be used in parameter
estimation. Otherwise, in conventional repeated-measures
ANOVA, the observation would be deleted. Also, the mixed-
effect model gives the flexibility of specifying a patterned
covariance structure for model residuals. In this study, we
specified a first-order autoregressive AR(1) structure in PROC
MIXED for residuals. The AR(1) structure is appropriate for
data collected from repeated measures designs (27).
RESULTS
Forty-seven participants were recruited. Of these, seven
participants failed to complete all testing sessions (e.g., be-
cause of dizziness) and were excluded from data analysis.
One participant was excluded because she reported engaging
in regular physical activity for 96 months before study par-
ticipation. Thirty-nine participants were thus included in the
analyses (Fig. 1): ACT (n= 18) versus control (n= 21).
These participants had an average weekly leisure activity
score (Godin-Leisure Time Exercise Questionnaire) of 13 T
12 units, indicating that they were insufficiently active,with
24 units or more reflecting sufficiently active (18). The ma-
jority of the participants identified as East Asian (44.7%) and
white (39.5%), with the remaining participants identified as
http://www.acsm-msse.org1254 Official Journal of the American College of Sports Medicine
APPLIED SCIENCES
Copyright © 2015 by the American College of Sports Medicine. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
South Asian (13.2%), African (2.6%), Aboriginal (2.6%),
Biracial (5.3%), or Middle Eastern (5.3%). Body mass index
(BMI), W
max
, peak incremental cycle HR (HR
peak
), and peak
incremental cycle RPE for all 39 participants were 21.7 T
4.1 kgIm
j2
(range = 14.8–39.7 kgIm
j2
), 130 T30 W (82% T
16% predicted; range = 49%–119%; [2]), 179 T15 beats per
minute (96% T7% predicted; range = 68%–109%), and 18.0 T
1.5 (range = 14–20) Borg 6–20 scale units, respectively. These
findings suggest that, on average, our participants were normal
weight, low-active, and that they gave a maximal/near-maximal
effort during exercise at Visit 1. The ACT and control groups were
well matched for age (24 T6vs22T5 yr), BMI (22.6 T5.4
vs 20.9 T2.4 kgIm
j2
), average weekly leisure activity scores
(11 T12 vs 15 T12 units), W
max
(83% T18% vs 81% T15%
predicted), HR
peak
(94% T9% vs 97% T5% predicted), cycle
work rate at 80% W
max
(100 T23 vs 108 T25 W), and HR
responses to CWR-1 and CWR-2 (all P90.05).
Manipulation check for the ACT group. Participants
understood the ACT-based techniques and used them fre-
quently during CWR-2: 83% of the ACT group reported feel-
ing ‘‘confident’ or ‘‘very confident’’ that they learned the ACT
techniques. When asked if they frequently applied the tech-
niques during the CWR-2 exercise test, 88.9% endorsed op-
tions 4 (no verbal anchor) and 5 (representing ‘‘all the time’’)
on the 5-point Likert scale.
ETT. As hypothesized, a significant group–time inter-
action was observed for ETT: F
1,39
=7.81,P=0.008,pseudo
R
2
= 0.05 (see Fig. 2). The ETT at CWR-1 was 392.05 T
146.4 and 384.71 T120.1 s for the ACT and control groups,
respectively (P= 0.98). After the intervention, ETT was
higher for the ACT group (459.39 T209.3 s) than for the
control group (353.86 T127.9 s): post hoc planned contrasts
indicated that ETT increased by 15% from CWR-1 to CWR-2
for the ACT group (F
1,39
=4.94,P= 0.03) and decreased by
8% for the control group (P=0.09).
RPE. As expected, a significant group–time interaction
was observed for RPE: F
4,148
=5.92,P= 0.0002, pseudo R
2
=
0.81 (see Fig. 3). Planned contrasts revealed a significant
difference in Borg 6–20 scale RPE values between the ACT
and control group at 40% (12.4 T2.8 vs 14.2 T2.2 units;
F
1,148
= 9.81, P= 0.002) and 55% (14.0 T2.8 and 15.4 T2.3
Borg 6–20 scale units; F
1,148
= 6.70, P= 0.01) of ETT.
Attentional focus, in-task affect, and exercise
enjoyment. Contrary to our expectations, a group–time
interaction for in-task affect (F
5,185
= 1.01, P= 0.41) or
activation levels during CWR-2 and after immediately post
cool-down (F
5,185
= 1.42, P= 0.22) were not observed. By
contrast, a significant group–time interaction was observed
for perceived exercise-related enjoyment (F
1,31
= 21.61, PG
0.0001, pseudo R
2
= 0.172): pairwise comparisons showed
no significant change in exercise enjoyment post CWR-1 (P=
0.68), whereas exercise enjoyment after CWR-2 was signif-
icantly higher in the ACT group versus that in the control
group (95.6 T13.7 vs 78.6 T18.9 arbitrary units; F
1,31
=8.06,
P= 0.008). The exercise enjoyment scale further demon-
strated high internal consistency: Cronbach >was 0.90 and
0.96 for CWR-1 and CWR-2 exercise tests, respectively.
Lastly, a group–time interaction was not observed for at-
tentional allocation during CWR-2 (F
4,148
=0.77,P=0.55).
DISCUSSION
To our knowledge, this is the first randomized controlled
study to show that an acute ACT intervention improved
high-intensity cycle ETT by È15% in apparently healthy,
young, normal-weight, and low-active women. In keeping
with the psychobiological model of endurance (37), the
present study further showed that ACT-induced improve-
ments in ETT were associated with improvements inperceived
effort and ratings of postexercise enjoyment. Furthermore, our
findings complement the results of a randomized trial by
Blanchfield et al. (4) who recently showed that a 2-wk mo-
tivational self-talk intervention improved both ETT and
perceived effort during CWR cycle exercise testing at 80%
W
max
in healthy, young recreationally active men and women.
In addition, although DMM based on hedonic theory of ex-
ercise motivation (10) provides an important understanding
of the mechanisms associated with affective responses dur-
ing exercise, these findings demonstrate that DMM cannot
FIGURE 2—Exercise tolerance time means TSEM for the control and
ACT condition at CWR-1 (preintervention 80% W
max
exercise test) and
CWR-2 (postintervention 80% W
max
exercise test).
FIGURE 3—Mean RPE at baseline, 20%, 40%, and 55% of exercise
tolerance and peak (100%) for the control and ACT condition during
CWR-2 (postintervention 80% W
max
exercsie test).
ACT TECHNIQUES AND EXERCISE DURATION Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise
d
1255
APPLIED SCIENCES
Copyright © 2015 by the American College of Sports Medicine. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
fully predict exercise tolerance among low-active individuals.
The differential effect of affect and perceived effort on exer-
cise behavior is elucidated by the fact that different theoretical
and neurobiological mechanisms influence perceived effort
and affective responses. Specifically, based on DMM, affec-
tive responses are influenced by afferent feedback from sen-
sory receptors (e.g., chemoreceptors) that reach the affective
centers of the brain (e.g., amygdala) (10), whereas perception
of effort is generated by corollary discharges from motor to
sensory areas of the cerebral cortex (35). The role of ACT in
influencing the neurobiological signals associated with per-
ceived effort remains to be determined by using neurophysi-
ological methods and study designs. Moreover, future research
needs to establish the long-term effect of these findings in
relation to improving exercise adoption, maintenance, and,
ultimately, health outcomes. The psychobiological model of
exercise (37) based on the motivational intensity theory (7)
can be used to guide this research. On the basis of the model,
hypotheses can be generated with regard to how perception of
effort may affect exercise-related decisions, for instance
whether to engage or not with exercise, or when to disengage
during exercise (i.e., volitional exhaustion).
An increase in high-intensity exercise tolerance among
low-active women carries a twofold significance. First, a
pervasive barrier to physical activity is lack of time (26),
thereby prolonging exercise tolerance at high intensities may
be a time-efficient alternative to low- and moderate-intensity
activities with potentially important implications for adher-
ence (32). Second, increasing exercise duration at high in-
tensities (e.g., 80% W
max
) may confer important health
benefits including improvements in insulin sensitivity in
sedentary, overweight, or obese adults (24) and in aerobic
working capacity/cardiorespiratory fitness (e.g., V
˙O
2peak
)
and cardiometabolic risk factors (e.g., blood pressure) in
healthy and clinical populations (9,16,23). Considering that
ACT increased high-intensity exercise tolerance by È15%,
application of this simple psychological intervention with
sedentary populations may be associated with vital improve-
ments in health outcomes. Randomized, controlled, and lon-
gitudinal studies are necessary in this regard.
Another finding of the present study that may carry im-
portant implications for health and adherence was that the
use of ACT techniques reduced perceived effort at an in-
tensity of exercise at which distractive techniques (e.g.,
music) have previously been shown to become less effective
in reducing perceived effort (29). It is noteworthy that,
contrary to our expectations, the ACT techniques did not
improve in-task affect, which is in support of the DMM,
stating that affect is predominantly unpleasant during exer-
cise above the ventilatory threshold (10). Whether or not
ACT techniques such as those used in our study are capable
of improving in-task affect during exercise at lower inten-
sities (e.g., 50% W
max
) requires further investigation.
The mechanism explaining the observed changes in per-
ceived effort using ACT techniques remains less clear. The
finding that perception of effort was reduced after the ACT
training may be explained by motivational intensity theory
(7,49), which postulates that actual effort is withheld for
difficult tasks because ‘Ieffort requirements exceed what
the performer is capable of doing’’ (49, p. 686). Hence, the
individual discontinues effort because it is perceived as in-
effective, as opposed to because the individual is not willing
to engage in the behavior (49). The ACT techniques thus
may have reduced perception of effort indirectly through
increasing the perceived ability to perform the high-intensity
exercise task (4). This hypothesized cognitive mechanism,
however, needs to be examined in future research investi-
gations. Differences in attention allocation cannot account
for the perception of effort because both groups were asso-
ciating (focusing on the physical sensations) throughout the
exercise session, as opposed to distracting. This supports the
Effort-Related Model, which states that association pre-
dominates at high intensities (44). This model further sug-
gests that both association and diverting attention may
reduce perceived effort at low and moderate intensities but
not at high intensities. In contrast, research on individual
differences in attention allocation shows that individuals
whose dominant attentional style is association report lower
perceived effort at high intensities than individuals whose
dominant attentional style is dissociation (e.g., 85% of HR
reserve) (25). To address the inconsistent findings on the
efficacy of association during high-intensity exercise, future
research needs to identify the cognitive mechanisms (e.g.,
active association with cognitive interpretations imposed on
the physiological sensations vs passive association of mon-
itoring body sensations) that lead to the effectiveness of as-
sociation techniques. ACT techniques may be one possible
cognitive mechanism with implications for enhancing asso-
ciation during high-intensity exercise. Lastly, the present
study results also suggest that use of ACT techniques may
improve postexercise enjoyment, even in the absence of var-
iations in music and exercise setting (i.e., external factors that
may affect enjoyment), which may have important implica-
tions for physical activity adoption and adherence (48). Fu-
ture research efforts are required to further understand the
relation between perceived effort and postexercise enjoy-
ment and their relationship to exercise adherence.
METHODOLOGICAL CONSIDERATIONS
The CWR exercise tests until volitional exhaustion were
completed before and after intervention on the same day,
which had the potential to undermine the effect of the
ACT intervention on measured parameters by exacerbating
perceived effort and decreasing enjoyment and exercise
tolerance during CWR-2 versus CWR-1 in this sample of
low-active women. Therefore, the same-day testing may
have undermined the È15% increase in ETT and the RPE.
Nevertheless, women randomly assigned to the ACT inter-
vention demonstrated significantly greater mean improve-
ments in ETT, perceived effort, and exercise enjoyment
from CWR-1 to CWR-2 than those women randomized to
http://www.acsm-msse.org1256 Official Journal of the American College of Sports Medicine
APPLIED SCIENCES
Copyright © 2015 by the American College of Sports Medicine. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
the no-ACT intervention control group. Furthermore, al-
though the exercise test used in this study is not relevant to
the current exercise guidelines (47), an improvement in ex-
ercise tolerance at high intensities can help individuals with
high-intensity interval training, which can be a time-efficient
alternative to exercising at lower intensity levels for longer, and
it is associated with equivalent health improvements (e.g., Gibala
et al. [16]).
Women were characterized as ‘‘low active’’ based on their
responses to the Godin-Leisure Time Questionnaire (18) with-
out the use of objective assessment (e.g., accelerometers).
Nevertheless, the Godin-Leisure Time Questionnaire has pre-
viously been shown to be significantly related to objective
measures (e.g., CalTrac accelerometer) of physical activity (r=
0.45, PG0.01) assessed during a 1-wk period (39). Further-
more, mean values of W
max
were 82% of the age-, sex-, and
height-predicted normal values (n= 39), thus supporting our
participants_self-report of being low active.
Music is widely used by exercisers for its real and/or
perceived psychological benefits (e.g., affect, motivation). It
follows that our use of music during preintervention and
postintervention CWR exercise tests increased the ecologi-
cal validity of our study and its findings. The generaliz-
ability of our findings may be limited to apparently healthy,
young, and low-active women. Furthermore, our interven-
tion was acute (single-session) and we cannot comment on
the potential benefits of repeated ACT interventions on
measures of high-intensity exercise performance. Thus, fu-
ture research should examine the potential performance-
enhancing effects of short- and long-term ACT interventions
in various other healthy (e.g., elderly, endurance-trained
athletes) and clinical samples of men and women. Moreover,
future research needs to establish whether it was the accep-
tance, cognitive defusion, or a combination of both ACT
techniques that was the active ingredient in the reduction in
perception of effort found in the present study.
CONCLUSIONS
In summary, the novel results from this randomized con-
trolled study suggest that psychological techniques derived
from ACT can be used in combination with music to help
low-active women to perform high-intensity exercise for lon-
ger while also feeling less perceived effort and experiencing
more enjoyment. Ultimately, the observed reduction in per-
ception of effort and behavioral benefits derived from the
ACT intervention, if sustained, may be associated with im-
proved physical activity adoption and adherence with atten-
dant health benefits. In this regard, the theoretically grounded
findings of our study provide a rationale for further investi-
gation of short- and long-term ACT interventions to enhance
the established health benefits of high-intensity exercise train-
ing in both health and disease.
E.I. was supported by the Coca-Cola Company Doctoral Student
Grant on Behavior Research Fund from the American College of
Sports Medicine Foundation, by the Fonds de la Recherche en sante
du Que´ bec (FRSQ) doctoral research fellowship, and by the Canadian
Psychological Association Foundation Student Research Grant Awards.
D.J. was supported by a Chercheurs-Boursier Junior 1 salary award
from the FRSQ and by a William Dawson Research Scholars Award
(McGill University). The authors would like to thank Daniel Kopala-
Sibley for his thoughtful comments and statistical advice; Sabrina
Cesare, Monique du Boulay, Diane Moliva, Liseanne Nelson, and Regina
Yung for their assistance with data collection; and all the women who
volunteered their time and efforts to participate in this study. The au-
thors thank the anonymous reviewers for their comments that helped
to strengthen the article.
The authors declare that there are no conflicts of interest.
The findings from the present study do not constitute endorse-
ment by the American College of Sports Medicine.
REFERENCES
1. Amann M, Hopkins WG, Marcora SM. Similar sensitivity of time
to exhaustion and time-trial time to changes in endurance. Med Sci
Sports Exerc. 2008;40(3):574–8.
2. ATS/ACCP. Statement on cardiopulmonary exercise testing. Am J
Respir Crit Care Med. 2003;167(2):211–77.
3. Baden DA, Warwicj-Evans L, Lakomy J. Am I nearly there? the
effect of anticipated running distance on perceived exertion and
attentional focus. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2004;26(2):215–31.
4. Blanchfield AW, Hardy J, de Moree HM, Staiano W, Marcora SM.
Talking to yourself out of exhaustion: the effects of self-talk on en-
durance performance. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2014;46(5):998–1007.
5. Borg G. Perceived exertion: a note on Fhistory_and methods. Med
Sci Sports. 1973;5(2):90–3.
6. Boutcher SH, Trenske M. The effects of sensory deprivation and
music on perceived exertion and affect during exercise. J Sport
Exerc Psychol. 1990;12(2):167–76.
7. Brehm JW, Self EA. The intensity of motivation. Annu Rev Psychol.
1989;40:109–31.
8. Butryn ML, Forman E, Hoffman K, Shaw J, Juarascio A. A pilot
study of acceptance and commitment therapy for promotion of
physical activity. J Phys Act Health. 2011;8(4):516–22.
9. Currie KD, Dubberley JB, McKelvie RS, MacDonald MJ. Low-
volume, high-intensity interval training in patients with CAD. Med
Sci Sports Exerc. 2013;45(8):1436–42.
10. Ekkekakis P. Pleasure and displeasure from the body: perspective
from exercise. Cogn Emot. 2003;17(2):213–39.
11. Ekkekakis P, Dafermos M. Exercise is a many-splendored thing,
but for some it does not feel so splendid: staging a resurgence of
hedonistic ideas in the quest to understand exercise behavior. In:
Acevedo EO, ed. The Oxford Handbook of Exercise Psychology.
New York (NY): Oxford University Press; 2014. pp. 1–45.
12. Ekkekakis P, Hall EE, Petruzzello SJ. Practical markers of the
transition from aerobic to anaerobic metabolism during exercise:
rationale and a case for affect-based exercise prescription. Prev Med.
2004;38(2):149–59.
13. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: a flexible
statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and
biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39(2):175–91.
14. Field A. Discovering Statistics Using SPPS. 3rd ed. Thousand Oaks
(CA): SAGE Publications Ltd; 2009. p. 155.
15. Forman EM, Butryn ML, Hoffman KL, Herbert JD. An open trial
of acceptance-based behavioral intervention for weight loss. Cogn
Behav Pract. 2009;16(2):223–35.
ACT TECHNIQUES AND EXERCISE DURATION Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise
d
1257
APPLIED SCIENCES
Copyright © 2015 by the American College of Sports Medicine. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
16. Gibala MJ, Little JP, MacDonald MJ, Hawley JA. Physiological
adaptations to low-volume, high-intensity interval training in health
and disease. JPhysiol. 2012;590(5):1077–84.
17. Gifford EV, Kohlenberg BS, Hayes SC, et al. Acceptance theory–
based treatment for smoking cessation: an initial trial of acceptance
and commitment therapy. Behav Ther. 2004;35:689–706.
18. Godin G. The Godin-Shephard Leisure-Time Physical Activity
Questionnaire. Health Fitness J Can. 2011;4(1):18–22.
19. Gregg JA, Callaghan GM, Hayes SC, Glenn-Lawson JL. Improv-
ing diabetes self-management through acceptance, mindfullness,
and values: a randomized controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol.
2007;75(2):336–43.
20. Hardy CJ, Rejeski WJ. Not what, but how one feels: the measurement
of affect during exercise. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 1989;11:304–17.
21. Hayes SC, Bissett RT, Korn Z, et al. The impact of acceptance versus
control rationales on pain tolerance. Psychol Record. 1999;49:33–47.
22. Hayes SC, Wilson KG. Acceptance and commitment therapy: al-
tering the verbal support for experiential avoidance. Behav Anal.
1994;17:289–303.
23. Haykowsky MJ, Timmons MP, Kruger C, McNeely M, Taylor
DA, Clark AM. Meta-analysis of aerobic interval training on exer-
cise capacity and systolic function in patients with heart failure and
reduced ejection fractions. Am J Cardiol. 2013;111(10):1466–9.
24. Houmard JA, Tanner CJ, Slentz CA, Duscha BD, McCartney JS,
Kraus WE. Effect of the volume and intensity of exercise training
on insulin sensitivity. J Appl Physiol. 2004;96(1):101–6.
25. Hutchinson JC, Karageorghis CI. Moderating influence of domi-
nant attentional style and exercise intensity on responses to asyn-
chronous music. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2013;35(6):625–43.
26. Johnson CA, Corrigan SA, Dubbert PM, Gramling SE. Perceived
barriers to excercise and weight control practices in community
women. Women Health. 1990;16(3–4):177–91.
27. Jones RJ. Longitudinal Data with Serial Correlation: A State–Space
Approach. London (UK): Chapman & Hall; 1993. pp. 1–224.
28. Karageorghis CI, Jones L, Priest DL, et al. Revisiting the rela-
tionship between exercise heart rate and music tempo preference.
Res Q Exeric Sport. 2011;82(2):274–84.
29. Karageorghis CI, Priest D-L. Music in the exercise domain: a review
and synthesis (Part II). Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol. 2012;5(1):67–84.
30. Katzmarzyk PT, Janssen I, Ardern CI. Physical inactivity, excess
adiposity and premature mortality. Obes Rev. 2003;4(4):257–90.
31. Kendzierski D, DeCarlo KJ. Physical activity enjoyment scale: two
validation studies. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 1991;13(1):50–64.
32. King AC, Haskell WL, Young DR, Oka RK, Stefanick ML. Long-
term effects of varying intensities and formats of physical activity
on participation rates, fitness, and lipoproteins in men and women
aged 50 to 65 years. Circulation. 1995;91(10):2596–604.
33. Killian KJ, Summers E, Jones NL, Campbell EJ. Dyspnea and leg
effort during incremental cycle ergometry. Am Rev Respir Dis.1992;
145(6):1339–45.
34. Marcks BA, Woods DW. A comparsison of thought suppression
to an acceptance-based technique in the management of personal
intrusive thoughts: a controlled evaluation. Behav Res Ther. 2005;
43(4):433–45.
35. Marcora S. Perception of effort during exercise is independent of
afferent feedback from skeletal muslces, heart, and lungs. J Appl
Physiol. 2009;106(6):2060–2.
36. Marcora S. Effort: perception of effort. In: Goldstein EB, editor.
Encyclopedia of Perception Vol. 1. Thousands Oaks (CA): Sage
Publications Inc; 2010. p. 380.
37. Marcora SM, Bosio A, de Morree HM. Locomotor muscle fatigue
increases cardiorespiratory responses and reduces performance dur-
ing intense cycling exercise independently from metabolic stress. Am
J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2008;294(3):R874–83.
38. Masedo AI, Esteve MR. Effects of suppression, acceptance and
spontaneous coping on pain tolerance, pain intensity and distress.
Behav Res Ther. 2007;45(2):199–209.
39. Miller DJ, Freedson PS, Kline GM. Comparison of activity levels
using the CalTrac acceleremeter and five questionnaires. Med Sci
Sports Exerc. 1994;26(3):376–82.
40. Rognmo K, Hetland E, Helgerud J, Hoff J, SlLrdahl SA. High
intensity aerobic interval exercise is superior to moderate intensity
exercise for increasing aerobic capacity in patients with coronary
artery disease. Eur J Cardiovasc Prev Rehabil. 2004;11(3):216–22.
41. Schaeffer MR, Mendonca CT, Levangie MC, Andersen RE,
Taivassalo T, Jensen D. Physiological mechanisms of sex differ-
ences in exertional dyspnoea: role of neural respiratory motor drive.
Exp Physiol. 2014;99(2):427–41.
42. Svebak S, Murgatroyd S. Metamotivational dominance: a multi-
method validation of reversal theory constructs. J Pers Soc Psychol.
1985;48(1):107–16.
43. Tammen VV. Elite middle and long distance runners associative/
dissociative coping. J Appl Sport Psychol. 1996;8(1):1–8.
44. Tenenbaum G. A social–cognitive perspective of perceived exer-
tion and exertion tolerance. In: Singer RN, Hausenblas HA, Janelle
C, eds. Handbook of Sport Psychology, New York (NY): Wiley;
2001. pp. 810–22.
45. Tenenbaum G, Lidor R, Lavyan N, et al. The effect of music type
on running perserverance and coping with effort sensations. Psychol
Sport Exerc. 2004;5(2):89–109.
46. Troiano RP, Berrigan D, Dodd KW, Ma
ˆsse LC, Tilert T, McDowell M.
Physical activity in the United States measured by accelereometer.
Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2008;40(1):181–8.
47. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. 2008 Physical
Activity Guidelines for Americans. Hyattsville (MD): US Depart-
ment of Health and Human Services. 2008 [cited 2014 Sept 1].
Available from: http://www.health.gov/paguidelines.
48. Williams DM, Lewis BA, Dunsiger S, et al. Comparing psycho-
social predictors of physical activity adoption and maintenance.
Ann Behav Med. 2008;36(2):186–94.
49. Wright RA. Refining the predicion of effort: Brehm_s distinction
between potential motivation and motivation intensity. Soc Personal
Psychol Compass. 2008;2(2):682–701.
http://www.acsm-msse.org1258 Official Journal of the American College of Sports Medicine
APPLIED SCIENCES
Copyright © 2015 by the American College of Sports Medicine. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
... For example, Ussher et al. (2014) showed that after a single 10-minute mindfulness-based intervention including a body scan, individuals with CLBP significantly reduced their level of pain-related distress in comparison with a control group. In individuals without CLBP, an acute mindfulness-based intervention improved perceived effort, perceived exercise-related enjoyment, and exercise tolerance/duration during a high-intensity cycling exercise in low activity-level women (Ivanova, Jensen, Cassoff, Gu, & Knäuper, 2015). In this experiment, a one-time 40-min intervention was designed to teach cognitive defusion (i.e., a skill or technique that is primarily used to detach, separate, or get some distance from thoughts and emotions) and acceptance techniques for coping with aversive physical discomfort, such as leg discomfort when walking on a treadmill, for example, and negative affect (e.g., boredom). ...
... Based on previous studies showing no significant effect of time in individuals performing two Sorensen tests 15 minutes apart under similar experimental conditions, it was expected that exercise tolerance in participants from the control group would not significantly change over time (Alahmari et al., 2020;Latimer, Maher, Refshauge, & Colaco, 1999). Consistent with Ivanova et al. (2015), it was hypothesized that only participants undergoing the mindfulness-based intervention would increase their tolerance to exercise over time, but that both groups would show a similar MVC evolution over time. Following Ivanova et al. (2015) and Cox et al. (2018), it was also hypothesized that only participants from the mindfulness-based intervention would decrease their levels of pain unpleasantness and perceived efforts, and increase their level of acceptance over time, while both groups of participants would maintain their level of pain intensity over time. ...
... Nevertheless, the results partly replicate those of Ivanova et al. (2015), who found a trend towards decreased exercise tolerance for the control group, and showed significant improvements in exercise tolerance and perceived effort after a one-time mindfulness-based intervention associated with a cycling task. Both interventions were performed until volitional exhaustion, whether it was a high-intensity cycle exercise (Ivanova et al., 2015) or an isometric endurance task in the present study. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: This study investigated the effects of an acute mindfulness-based intervention on exercise tolerance, maximal voluntary contraction (MVC), pain and effort-related experiences in individuals with primary chronic low back pain. Method: Participants were randomly assigned to an experimental group and a control group. Before and after the intervention, participants completed a MVC test, a modified Sorensen test and numerical rating scales about pain intensity, pain unpleasantness, perceived effort and use of coping strategies. Dispositional measures such as pain catastrophizing, trait anxiety, dispositional mindfulness and impulsivity were also assessed. T -tests and linear mixed models were performed. Results: Participants from the control group significantly decreased their MVC across time and showed a trend towards a decrease in exercise tolerance over time, which was not the case for the experimental group. For both groups, pain unpleasantness and effort were perceived higher during the second modified Sorensen test. Analyses revealed neither significant effect of time nor group on pain intensity and the use of coping strategies. Discussion: The results highlight the value of involving individuals with primary chronic pain in mindfulness-based interventions to improve adaptations to effort while targeting referred pain. Larger samples and controlling for individuals’ functional status appear necessary for further research.
... This model comprises acceptance, mindfulness, and value processes to produce psychological flexibility, which helps the individual adopt value-based action in the presence of unwanted thoughts, feelings, and bodily sensations. ACT interventions have already shown improvements in health among inactive populations (Forman and Butryn, 2015;Ivanova et al., 2015). The relationship between the core constructs of SDT and ACT received minimal research attention, although these frameworks share analogous assemblies in the forms of human motivation and basic psychological needs (Gazla, 2015). ...
... conjunction with those of previous interventions points to the possibility that receiving autonomy-supportive PA counseling over the course of 3 months increased the participants' motivation for exercise (Fortier et al., 2007). Moreover, an ACTbased intervention among sedentary women has been found to lead to improvements in perceived effort and ratings of postexercise enjoyment, compared to a control group that engaged in similar tasks, such as the traditional intervention group in our study (Ivanova et al., 2015). Taken together with these findings, the current study offers a solid research direction to explore integrating SDT and ACT to increase motivation for exercise. ...
... Nevertheless, the SDT and ACTbased intervention group exhibited a significant increase in the parameter of activity intensity from time 1 to time 2, while the two control groups showed only insignificant differences in this index. This pattern is also evident in previous studies utilizing ACT techniques (Ivanova et al., 2015) and SDT inclinations (Burn and Niven, 2019) under constant work rate conditions of high intensity. With respect to the non-significant differences in overall training frequency per week, we propose that participants did not assimilate the SDT and ACT assets under low-intensity PA conditions since they perceived these as ordinary activities. ...
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of the present study was to evaluate the effectiveness of a physical activity (PA) intervention program designed to enhance levels of engagement in PA. Despite robust evidence supporting the beneficial effects of PA on overall health, only about 22% of individuals engage in the recommended minimum amount of PA. Recent surveys suggested that most individuals express intentions to be physically active, though the psychological state of amotivation dismissed these struggles. In the current study, we pilot-tested a new intervention program, aimed at enhancing engagement in PA among sedentary individuals. The intervention was based on two behavioral change and motivational psychological frameworks: Self-Determination Theory (SDT) and Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT). During a 14-week intervention program, 94 sedentary Israeli college students (Mage = 24.4, SD = 1.42, Females = 89) were randomly assigned into one of three groups: SDT and ACT-based intervention, traditional intervention, and a non-treatment group. Prior to and following the intervention, participants completed the Behavioral Regulation in Exercise Questionnaire-3 (BREQ-3) to examine motivation to exercise and the International Physical Activity Measurement IPAQ to evaluate their training frequency. Results showed that the SDT and ACT-based intervention group exhibited a significant increase in motivation to exercise between time 1 and time 2, while the other two groups (i.e., the traditional intervention program and the non-treatment group) showed insignificant differences in motivation to exercise. Furthermore, neither of the groups showed significant differences in their training frequency per week. However, those in the SDT and ACT-based groups reported an increase in activity intensity from time 1 to time 2 compared to the two other groups. Further, exercise psychology consultants and scholars can use the intervention protocol and utilize these findings to improve PA behaviors and promote health in the general population. Limitations, future directions, and implications are discussed in detail.
... Additionally, college women randomized to an 8-session ACTbased group intervention reported significant immediate and sustained increases in physical activity-related experiential acceptance across the year-long follow-up relative to the control group (Katterman, Goldstein, Butryn, Forman, & Lowe, 2014). Meanwhile, researchers have further shown improvements in physical activity engagement after exposure to ACT-informed interventions among minimally-active Canadian women (e.g., Ivanova, Jensen, Cassoff, Gu, & Knauper, 2015), Australian adults (Moffitt & Mohr, 2015) and U.S. adults (Martin, Galloway-Williams, Cox, & Winett, 2015). Gains in exercise-related experiential acceptance and in objective measures of cardiorespiratory fitness were also observed (Martin et al., 2015). ...
... Interventions ranged from ACT applied to a single bout of high-intensity cardiovascular exercise (Ivanova et al., 2015), ACT combined with a 10-week fitness walking program (Martin et al., 2015), and ACT delivered via DVD at home in combination with a 12-week walking program (Moffitt & Mohr, 2015). ...
Chapter
Contemporary scholarship seeks to give increased attention to identifying and evaluating treatment modalities that may be useful in enhancing more positive forms of embodiment. In this context, acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) offers a promising approach to shift disrupted embodiment into a more embodied connection and engaged living via strengthening a range of interrelated embodied flexibility processes. This chapter is designed to address the following aims: (a) to propose a heuristic model integrating the conceptual foundations of ACT and embodiment; (b) to provide a clinical case illustrating the potential benefits of applying ACT strategies in the context of embodiment; (c) to offer a brief overview of select empirical studies applying ACT in the domains of body image, weight-related acceptance and stigmatization, eating, and physical activity; and (d) to briefly consider the strengths and limitations of this emerging evidence base to serve as a springboard for future research to pursue.
... A large number of peer-reviewed studies have demonstrated the effectiveness of ACT across a variety of populations and behaviors, including increased exercise in adults (e.g., Ivanova et al., 2015;Wang et al., 2020), decreased rehospitalization (e.g., Gaudiano et al., 2017) and overall believability of hallucinations for individuals with psychotic episodes (e.g., Bach & Hayes, 2002;Guadiano & Hebert, 2006), decreased psychological side-effects of chronic pain (e.g., Vowles & McCracken, 2008;Wetherell et al., 2016), increased psychological flexibility and parent involvement with children diagnosed with autism (e.g., Blackledge & Hayes, 2006;Gould et al., 2018), effective treatment of patients diagnosed with clinical depression (e.g., Zettle, 2015), and a great deal more. Many studies have included all six processes to produce socially significant behavior change (e.g., Bach & Hayes, 2002;Blackledge & Hayes, 2006;Gaudiano et al., 2017;Gould et al., 2018;Wang et al., 2020;Wetherell et al., 2016), whereas other studies have yielded positive outcomes using fewer components (e.g., Dixon et al., 2019;Hooper & McHugh, 2013;Ivanova et al., 2015;Masuda et al., 2009;Vowles & McCracken, 2008). ...
... A large number of peer-reviewed studies have demonstrated the effectiveness of ACT across a variety of populations and behaviors, including increased exercise in adults (e.g., Ivanova et al., 2015;Wang et al., 2020), decreased rehospitalization (e.g., Gaudiano et al., 2017) and overall believability of hallucinations for individuals with psychotic episodes (e.g., Bach & Hayes, 2002;Guadiano & Hebert, 2006), decreased psychological side-effects of chronic pain (e.g., Vowles & McCracken, 2008;Wetherell et al., 2016), increased psychological flexibility and parent involvement with children diagnosed with autism (e.g., Blackledge & Hayes, 2006;Gould et al., 2018), effective treatment of patients diagnosed with clinical depression (e.g., Zettle, 2015), and a great deal more. Many studies have included all six processes to produce socially significant behavior change (e.g., Bach & Hayes, 2002;Blackledge & Hayes, 2006;Gaudiano et al., 2017;Gould et al., 2018;Wang et al., 2020;Wetherell et al., 2016), whereas other studies have yielded positive outcomes using fewer components (e.g., Dixon et al., 2019;Hooper & McHugh, 2013;Ivanova et al., 2015;Masuda et al., 2009;Vowles & McCracken, 2008). Overall, data on ACT exhibit one common outcome-ACT works to facilitate socially significant behavior change in complex verbal organisms. ...
Article
The goal of acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) is to help people engage in experiential avoidance less and committed action toward values more. One possible behavior analytic interpretation is that it helps people choose short-term negative reinforcement less and choose longer-term positive reinforcement more. To our knowledge, no laboratory research to date has evaluated the effect of ACT interventions on the behavior of choosing between immediate negative reinforcement or delayed positive reinforcement. The purpose of the current study was to evaluate the effects of defusion exercises on participants’ choice making on a delay discounting computer task that presented choices between immediate avoidance of an aversive sound or presentation of the aversive sound and delayed positive reinforcement in the form of money. A multiple baseline design across participants was used to measure changes in discounting before and after a brief defusion session. All three participants discounted less steeply (i.e., selected to listen to the sound in order to earn money more often) following defusion training when compared to baseline.
... Overall, according to the findings of this study, a cognitive behavioural intervention that addresses the fear-avoidance response to breathlessness is deemed helpful for engaging individuals living with HF in physical activity. The common approaches include Cognitive Behavioural Therapy (CBT) and Acceptance and Commitment Therapy, ACT [56,57]. While CBT can address the maladaptive beliefs about breathlessness and negative emotional response (i.e., anxiety, fear), ACT can promote acceptance, reduce avoidant behaviour triggered by breathlessness and change maladaptive coping. ...
Article
Full-text available
In heart failure (HF), increased physical activity is associated with improved quality of life, reduced hospitalisation, and increased longevity and is an important aim of treatment. However, physical activity levels in individuals living with HF are typically extremely low. This qualitative study with one-to-one interviews systematically explores perceived clinical, environmental, and psychosocial barriers and enablers in older adults (≥70 years old) living with HF. Semi-structured interviews (N = 16) based on the Theoretical Domains Framework elicited 39 belief statements describing the barriers and enablers to physical activity. Theoretical domains containing these beliefs and corresponding constructs that were both pervasive and common were deemed most relevant. These were: concerns about physical activity (Beliefs about Consequences), self-efficacy (Beliefs about Capabilities), social support (Social Influences), major health event (Environmental Context and Resources), goal behavioural (Goal), action planning (Behavioural Regulation). This work extends the limited research on the modifiable barriers and enablers for physical activity participation by individuals living with HF. The research findings provide insights for cardiologists, HF-specialist nurses, and physiotherapists to help co-design and deliver a physical activity intervention more likely to be effective for individuals living with HF.
Article
Within this paper, we provide an expert opinion on five evidence-based psychological strategies that could help endurance participants overcome slowing down and stopping during performance: goal setting, motivational self-talk, relaxation, distraction, and pacing. We argue that these strategies are well-suited for delivery as brief-contact, educational interventions that could be accessible to large numbers of participants who do not have access to a sport and exercise psychologist. These interventions could be delivered using websites, online videos, workshops, or magazine articles. We propose a novel use for implementation intentions (i.e. if-then planning) to develop endurance participants’ conditional knowledge of when to use specific strategies. In addition, although research evidence suggests that these psychological strategies may be efficacious for overcoming thoughts of slowing down or stopping, there are important limitations in the research evidence. In particular, there is a dearth of ecologically valid, field-based effectiveness studies. Finally, we consider situations where attempts to resist slowing down or stopping during endurance activity may not be advisable. Scenarios include when there is an increased likelihood of injury, or when environmental conditions increase the risk of life-threatening events.
Article
Full-text available
Background Survivors of breast cancer can face internal barriers to physical activity, such as uncertainty and frustration stemming from physical limitations, decreased physical functioning, fatigue, and pain. Interventions that draw from the principles of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) may help survivors of breast cancer overcome some of the internal barriers associated with physical activity. Objective The primary aim of this study was to investigate the acceptability of an electronically delivered physical activity intervention for survivors of breast cancer, centered on ACT processes. Methods This study used a 1-group pretest-posttest design. We recruited 80 insufficiently active female survivors of breast cancer using a web-based recruitment strategy. The 8-week intervention consisted of weekly modules that featured didactic lessons and experiential exercises targeting key ACT processes in the context of physical activity promotion (namely, values, committed action, acceptance, defusion, and contacting the present moment). We determined intervention acceptability according to study retention (≥70%), adherence rates (≥75% of the participants completing ≥50% of the modules), and posttest survey scores reflecting the perceived ease of use, perceived usefulness, and interest and enjoyment of the intervention (≥5 on a 7-point Likert-type scale). We also evaluated changes in self-reported aerobic and muscle strengthening–physical activity, physical activity acceptance, physical activity regulation, and health-related outcomes. Results The retention rate (61/80, 76%), adherence rate (60/80, 75%), average perceived ease of use (6.17, SD 1.17), perceived usefulness (5.59, SD 1.40), and interest and enjoyment scores (5.43, SD 1.40) met the acceptability criteria. Participants increased their self-reported aerobic physical activity (Cohen d=1.04), muscle strengthening–physical activity (Cohen d=1.02), physical activity acceptance (cognitive acceptance: Cohen d=0.35; behavioral commitment: Cohen d=0.51), physical activity regulation (identified regulation: Cohen d=0.37; integrated regulation: Cohen d=0.66), increased their ability to participate in social roles and activities (Cohen d=0.18), and reported less fatigue (Cohen d=0.33) and sleep disturbance (Cohen d=0.53). Conclusions Electronically delivered acceptance- and mindfulness-based interventions may be useful for promoting physical activity in survivors of breast cancer. Further research is needed to refine these approaches and evaluate their effectiveness.
Preprint
Full-text available
In heart failure (HF), increased physical activity is associated with improved quality of life, reduced hospitalisation, and increased longevity and is an important aim of treatment. However, physical activity levels in individuals living with HF are typically extremely low. This qualitative study with one-to-one interviews systematically explores perceived clinical, environmental, and psychosocial barriers and enablers in older adults (>70 years old) living with HF. Semi-structured interviews (N = 16) based on the Theoretical Domains Framework elicited 78 belief statements describing the barriers and enablers to physical activity. Theoretical domains containing these beliefs and corresponding constructs that were both pervasive and common were deemed most relevant. These were: concerns about physical activity (Beliefs about Consequences), self-efficacy (Beliefs about Capabilities), social support (Social Influences), major health event (Environmental Context and Resources), goal behavioural (Goal), action planning (Behavioural Regulation). This work extends the limited research on the modifiable barriers and enablers for physical activity participation by individuals living with HF. The research findings provide insights for cardiologists, HF-specialist nurses, and physiotherapists to help co-design and deliver a physical activity intervention more likely to be effective for individuals living with HF.
Chapter
While it is natural to avoid mental and physical pain, this effort typically contributes to mental illness. Accepting negativity without avoidance and committing to actions leading to valued goals, shifts a person from negative to positive outcomes. The concepts of experiential avoidance and psychological (behavioral) flexibility are important, with the latter achieved through six steps. Acceptance and commitment therapy is shown to advance the states and processes for mental health, and most robustly for activity, psychological defense mechanisms (adaptive dissociation), emotion regulation, self-acceptance, and adaptability. Social connectedness is also enhanced when social relationships are subject to experiential avoidance.
Article
Full-text available
This study provides evidence of construct validity for the Brazilian version of the Godin- Shephard Leisure-Time Physical Activity Questionnaire (GSLTPAQ), a 1-item instrument used among 236 participants referred for cardiopulmonary exercise testing. The Baecke Habitual Physical Activity Questionnaire (Baecke-HPA) was used to evaluate convergent and divergent validity. The self-reported measure of walking (QCAF) evaluated the convergent validity. Cardiorespiratory fitness assessed convergent validity by the Veterans Specific Activity Questionnaire (VSAQ), peak measured (VO2peak) and maximum predicted (VO2pred) oxygen uptake. Partial adjusted correlation coefficients between the GSLTPAQ, Baecke-HPA, QCAF, VO2pred and VSAQ provided evidence for convergent validity; while divergent validity was supported by the absence of correlations between the GSLTPAQ and the Occupational Physical Activity domain (Baecke-HPA). The GSLTPAQ presents level 3 of evidence of construct validity and may be useful to assess leisure-time physical activity among patients with cardiovascular disease and healthy individuals.
Article
Full-text available
Contemporary theories of exercise behavior have been the products of the so-called cognitive revolution, which has shaped the dominant paradigm in psychology over the past several decades. Cognitive theories rely on the assumption that, in making behavioral decisions, humans collect relevant information and make their selections on the basis of a more-or-less rational analysis of this information. Although the dominance of cognitive theories in the field of exercise psychology is unquestionable, evidence suggests that they leave most of the variance in exercise behavior unaccounted and interventions based on them are of limited effectiveness in changing exercise behavior. This chapter reviews the history and evaluates the potential of an alternative approach, namely the hedonic theory of motivation. This idea, long neglected due the fascination of psychologists with informationprocessing models of the mind, attributes a substantial portion of the variance in decision-making to affective processes. Modern iterations of the idea emerging from the fields of neurology and behavioral economics reaffirm the ancient thesis that, in the long run, humans tend to repeat what makes them feel better and tend to avoid what makes them feel worse. Evidence from studies in the context of exercise suggests that affective responses to exercise vary greatly between individuals. Furthermore, despite a still-evolving methodological platform, preliminary studies show that affective responses to exercise predict subsequent exercise behavior. This line of research and theorizing offers a novel and intriguing perspective on the mechanisms underlying behavioral decision-making in the context of exercise. The literature reviewed in this chapter highlights the need for further research on the motivational implications of affective processes and lays the foundation for the development of a hedonic theory of exercise behavior.
Article
Full-text available
It is self-evident that resource allocation is better understood when a distinction is made between what people are willing to do to achieve a purpose and what they will do to achieve the purpose. Yet, motivation theorists have largely ignored this distinction in attempting to understand effort aspects of motivated behavior. An exception is Brehm, who has distinguished potential motivation from motivation intensity. Potential motivation refers to the upper limit of what people would be willing to do to satisfy a motive. It is proposed to vary with factors traditionally thought to determine motive strength (importance). Motivation intensity refers to effort and is proposed to vary proximally with the difficulty of instrumental behavior, first rising and then falling precipitously. In this article, I discuss Brehm's distinction and elaborate on its significance.
Article
Full-text available
Acceptance approaches, which have been receiving increased attention within behavior therapy, seek to undermine the linkage between private events and overt behavior, rather than attempting to control the form or frequency of private events per se. Research comparing control versus acceptance strategies is limited. The present study examined the behavioral and subjective impact of a control-based versus acceptance rationale, using a cold pressor task. Subjects in the acceptance group demonstrated greater tolerance of pain compared to the control-based and placebo groups. Only the control-based rationale targeted the subjective experience of pain but it did not differ across rationales. Results confirmed that acceptance was effective in manipulating the believability of reason giving, a key process measure. By encouraging individuals to distance themselves from their private events, acceptance methods may help reduce the use of emotional reasons to explain behavior and hence shift concern from moderating thoughts and feelings to experiencing the consequences of one's action. Acceptance is a promising new technique. Its effect is all the more surprising given that it teaches principles (e.g., "thoughts do not cause behavior") that run counter both to the popular culture and to the dominant approaches within empirical clinical intervention.
Chapter
Full-text available
Article
Full-text available
Acceptance approaches , which have been receiving increased attention within behavior therapy, seek to undermine the linkage between private events and overt behavior, rather than attempting to control the form or frequency of private events per se. Research comparing control versus acceptance strategies is limited. The present study examined the behavioral and subjective impact of a control-based versus acceptance rationale, using a cold pressor task. Subjects in the acceptance group demonstrated greater tolerance of pain compared to the control-based and placebo groups. Only the control-based rationale targeted the subjective experience of pain but it did not differ across rationales. Results confirmed that acceptance was effective in manipulating the believability of reason giving, a key process measure. By encouraging individuals to distance themselves from their private events, acceptance methods may help reduce the use of emotional reasons to explain behavior and hence shift concern from moderating thoughts and fee lings to experiencing the consequences of one's action. Acceptance is a promising new technique. Its effect is all the more surprising given that it teaches principles (e.g., "thoughts do not cause behavior") that run counter both to the popular culture and to the dominant approaches within empirical clinical intervention.
Article
In the present study, we investigated a hypothesized quartic relationship (meaning three inflection points) between exercise heart rate (HR) and preferred music tempo. Initial theoretical predictions suggested a positive linear relationship (Iwanaga, 1995a, 1995b); however, recent experimental work has shown that as exercise HR increases, step changes and plateaus that punctuate the profile of music tempo preference may occur (Karageorghis, Jones, & Stuart, 2008). Tempi bands consisted of slow (95-100 bpm), medium (115-120 bpm), fast (135-140 bpm), and very fast (155-160 bpm) music. Twenty-eight active undergraduate students cycled at exercise intensities representing 40, 50, 60, 70, 80, and 90% of their maximal HR reserve while their music preference was assessed using a 10-point scale. The Exercise Intensity x Music Tempo interaction was significant, F(6.16, 160.05) = 7.08, p < .001, ηp² = .21, as was the test for both cubic and quartic trajectories in the exercise HR-preferred-music-tempo relationship (p < .001). Whereas slow tempo music was not preferred at any exercise intensity, preference for fast tempo increased, relative to medium and very fast tempo music, as exercise intensity increased. The implications for the prescription of music in exercise and physical activity contexts are discussed.
Article
The purpose of this study was to compare the physical activity levels of physical therapists using five activity questionnaires and one electronic motion sensor (Caltrac(R)). Thirty-three physical therapists (26 female, seven male) were monitored for seven consecutive days. The Caltrac(R) data were compared with five questionnaires, including the Baecke (Baecke); Godin and Shephard (Godin); Ross and Jackson (NASA); 3-d record (3-d); and 7-d recall (7-d). A significant (P < 0.01) Spearman rank order correlation coefficient was observed between the Caltrac(R) reading and the 7-d recall (r = 0.79). A significant correlation was also observed between the Caltrac(R) and the Godin (r = 0.45, P < 0.01). Three of the questionnaires were also significantly (P < 0.01) correlated with each other, including the NASA with the Baecke (r = 0.61). The 3-d record was not significantly correlated with any of the other measures. The strong, significant correlation observed between the Caltrac(R) data and the 7-d recall. The 7-d recall questionnaire would be quicker, easier, and less expensive to use in a large population study than the Caltrac(R)