ArticlePDF Available

Prenatal Stress and Its Effects on the Fetus and the Child: Possible Underlying Biological Mechanisms

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Many prospective studies have shown that if a mother is depressed, anxious or stressed while pregnant, this increases the risk for her child having a wide range of adverse outcomes including emotional problems, symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) or impaired cognitive development. Although genetics and postnatal care clearly affect these outcomes, evidence for a prenatal causal component also is substantial. Prenatal anxiety/depression may contribute 10–15 % of the attributable load for emotional/behavioural outcomes.
Content may be subject to copyright.
-
-
-
Dear Author/Editor,
Here are the proofs of your chapter as well as the metadata sheets.
Metadata
Please carefully proof read the metadata, above all the names and address.
In case there were no abstracts for this book submitted with the manuscript, the first 10-15
lines of the first paragraph were taken. In case you want to replace these default abstracts,
please submit new abstracts with your proof corrections.
Page proofs
Please check the proofs and mark your corrections either by
entering your corrections online
or
opening the PDF file in Adobe Acrobat and inserting your corrections using the tool
"Comment and Markup"
or
printing the file and marking corrections on hardcopy. Please mark all corrections in
dark pen in the text and in the margin at least ¼” (6 mm) from the edge.
You can upload your annotated PDF file or your corrected printout on our Proofing
Website. In case you are not able to scan the printout , send us the corrected pages via
fax.
Please note that any changes at this stage are limited to typographical errors and serious
errors of fact.
If the figures were converted to black and white, please check that the quality of such
figures is sufficient and that all references to color in any text discussing the figures is
changed accordingly. If the quality of some figures is judged to be insufficient, please send
an improved grayscale figure.
Author's Proof
Metadata of the chapter that will be visualized online
Book Title Perinatal Programming of Neurodevelopment
Chapter Title Prenatal Stress and Its Effects on the Fetus and the Child: Possible
Underlying Biological Mechanisms
Copyright Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014
Corresponding Author Prefix
Family name Glover
Particle
Given name Vivette
Suffix
Division Institute of Reproductive and Developmental Biology
Organization Imperial College London, Hammersmith Campus
Address Du Cane Road, W12 0NN London, UK
Email v.glover@imperial.ac.uk
Abstract Many prospective studies have shown that if a mother is depressed,
anxious or stressed while pregnant, this increases the risk for her
child having a wide range of adverse outcomes including emotional
problems, symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
(ADHD) or impaired cognitive development. Although genetics and
postnatal care clearly affect these outcomes, evidence for a prenatal
causal component also is substantial. Prenatal anxiety/depression
may contribute 10–15 % of the attributable load for
emotional/behavioural outcomes.
The mechanisms underlying these changes are just starting to be
explored. One possible mediating factor is increased exposure of the
fetus to cortisol, as has been shown in animal studies. However, the
human hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis which makes
cortisol functions differently in human pregnancy from in most
animals. The maternal HPA axis becomes gradually less responsive
to stress as pregnancy progresses. And there is only a weak, if any,
association between a mother’s prenatal mood and her cortisol level,
especially later in pregnancy. Cytokines are alternative possible
mediators. An additional explanation is that stress or anxiety causes
increased transfer of maternal cortisol across the placenta to the
fetus. The placenta plays a crucial role in moderating fetal exposure
to maternal factors and presumably in preparing the fetus for the
environment in which it is going to find itself. There is some evidence
in both rat models and in humans that prenatal stress can reduce
placental 11β-HSD2, the enzyme which metabolises cortisol to
inactive cortisone. The level of cortisol in the amniotic fluid,
surrounding the baby in the womb, has been shown to be inversely
correlated with infant cognitive development. However, several other
biological systems are likely to be involved. Serotonin is another
possible mediator of prenatal stress induced programming effects on
offspring neurocognitive and behavioural development. The role of
epigenetic changes in mediating alterations in offspring outcome
following prenatal stress is likely to be important and starting to be
explored.
Keywords Prenatal stress - Fetus - Programming - Neurodevelopment - Cortisol
- HPA - Placenta - 11β-HSD2
Author's Proof
UNCORRECTED PROOF
Book ID: 304585_1_En ChapterID: 13 Dispatch Date: 09/07/2014 ProofNo: 1
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
1
Chapter 13
Prenatal Stress and Its Effects on the Fetus
and the Child: Possible Underlying Biological
Mechanisms
Vivette Glover
M. Antonelli (ed.), Perinatal Programming of Neurodevelopment,
Advances in Neurobiology 10, DOI 10.1007/978-1-4939-1372-5_13,
© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014
V. Glover ()
Institute of Reproductive and Developmental Biology, Imperial College London,
Hammersmith Campus, Du Cane Road, London W12 0NN, UK
e-mail: v.glover@imperial.ac.uk
Abstract Many prospective studies have shown that if a mother is depressed,
anxious or stressed while pregnant, this increases the risk for her child having a
wide range of adverse outcomes including emotional problems, symptoms of atten-
tion deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) or impaired cognitive development.
Although genetics and postnatal care clearly affect these outcomes, evidence for
a prenatal causal component also is substantial. Prenatal anxiety/depression may
contribute 10–15 % of the attributable load for emotional/behavioural outcomes.
The mechanisms underlying these changes are just starting to be explored. One
possible mediating factor is increased exposure of the fetus to cortisol, as has been
shown in animal studies. However, the human hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal
(HPA) axis which makes cortisol functions differently in human pregnancy from in
most animals. The maternal HPA axis becomes gradually less responsive to stress
as pregnancy progresses. And there is only a weak, if any, association between a
mother’s prenatal mood and her cortisol level, especially later in pregnancy. Cy-
tokines are alternative possible mediators. An additional explanation is that stress
or anxiety causes increased transfer of maternal cortisol across the placenta to the
fetus. The placenta plays a crucial role in moderating fetal exposure to maternal fac-
tors and presumably in preparing the fetus for the environment in which it is going
to find itself. There is some evidence in both rat models and in humans that prenatal
stress can reduce placental 11β-HSD2, the enzyme which metabolises cortisol to
inactive cortisone. The level of cortisol in the amniotic fluid, surrounding the baby
in the womb, has been shown to be inversely correlated with infant cognitive devel-
opment. However, several other biological systems are likely to be involved. Sero-
tonin is another possible mediator of prenatal stress induced programming effects
on offspring neurocognitive and behavioural development. The role of epigenetic
changes in mediating alterations in offspring outcome following prenatal stress is
likely to be important and starting to be explored.
Author's Proof
UNCORRECTED PROOF
Book ID: 304585_1_En ChapterID: 13 Dispatch Date: 09/07/2014 ProofNo: 1
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
61
62
63
64
65
66
67
68
69
70
71
72
2 V. Glover
13.1 Prenatal Stress and Child Outcomes
Stress is a generic term, which includes a wide range of different exposures, and
many different types of prenatal stress have been shown to be associated with al-
tered outcome for the child. These include symptoms of maternal anxiety and de-
pression (O’Connor et al. 2003; Van den Bergh et al. 2008), pregnancy-specific
anxiety (Huizink et al. 2002), bereavement (Khashan et al. 2008), life events, in-
cluding a bad relationship with the partner (Bergman et al. 2007) and exposure to
acute disasters such as a Canadian ice storm (Laplante et al. 2008), 9/11 (Yehuda
et al. 2005), Chernobyl (Huizink et al. 2007) or a hurricane in Louisiana (Kinney
et al. 2008a). It is clear that it is not just a diagnosable mental illness or very extreme
or “toxic stress” that can alter the outcome. Exposures which can have an effect
vary from the very severe, such as the death of an older child, to quite mild stresses,
such as daily hassles.
It has been suggested that mild-to-moderate stress may actually improve some
outcomes. Mild prenatal stress has been shown in some studies to accelerate motor
development and cognitive ability (DiPietro et al. 2006). This is an interesting idea
and deserves further investigation. Other studies have found a linear dose response
between prenatal maternal anxiety and emotional/behavioural outcomes for the
child (O’Connor et al. 2002). It is possible that prenatal stress has different patterns
and direction of effect for different outcomes. For example, mild-to-moderate stress
may accelerate physical maturation and cognitive function while also increasing
symptoms of anxiety.
Many independent prospective studies have now shown that if the mother is anx-
ious, depressed or stressed while she is pregnant her child is at increased risk of a
wide range of problems (Van den Bergh et al. 2005; Talge et al. 2007; Glover 2011;
see Table 13.1). These include both neurodevelopmental, such as emotional and be-
havioural disorders (O’Connor et al. 2002), and physical problems, such as asthma
(Khashan et al. 2012). It is important to note that in all these studies, the findings
show only an increased risk. Most children are not affected. But an increased risk,
for example, a doubling of a probable mental disorder, from about 6–12 %, if the
mother in the top 15 % of anxiety or depression as shown in a normal population
(O’Donnell et al. 2013 in press), is of real clinical significance.
Different studies have examined children at times from birth until adulthood.
Many have shown that prenatal stress is associated with somewhat lower birth-
weight and reduced gestational age (Wadhwa et al. 2011; Rice et al. 2010). Studies
have found an increased proportion of children who are mixed handed, rather than
right handed, after prenatal stress (Glover et al. 2004; Rodriguez and Waldenstrom
2008), and also altered fingerprint patterns (King et al. 2009). These physical altera-
tions are of interest because they are features that are known to develop in utero.
Being mixed handed is not a problem in itself, but it is known that people with a
range of neurodevelopmental problems such as autism and schizophrenia are also
more likely to be mixed handed. Recent studies have shown prenatal stress is asso-
ciated with reduced telomere length (Entringer et al. 2011, 2013). This is an intrigu-
ing finding, as well as of concern, as reduced telomere length is associated with a
reduced life span.
Author's Proof
UNCORRECTED PROOF
Book ID: 304585_1_En ChapterID: 13 Dispatch Date: 09/07/2014 ProofNo: 1
73
74
75
76
77
78
79
80
81
82
83
84
85
86
313 Prenatal Stress and Its Effects on the Fetus and the Child
Some investigators have looked at the newborns of mothers who report stress
during pregnancy and found a poorer performance on the Neonatal Behavioral As-
sessment Scale relative to newborns of mothers who do not report stress during
pregnancy (Rieger et al. 2004), showing that adverse behavioural outcomes are also
observable from the very beginning. Studies of infants and toddlers have shown
more difficult temperament (Austin et al. 2005; Werner et al. 2007; Davis et al.
2007; Blair et al. 2011), sleep problems (O’Connor et al. 2007), lower cognitive
performance and increased fearfulness associated with higher maternal stress dur-
ing pregnancy (Bergman et al. 2007). Other studies have examined the association
between prenatal stress and neurodevelopmental outcomes in children aged 3–16
years. Many independent groups have shown that prenatal stress increases child
emotional problems, especially symptoms of anxiety and depression, and symptoms
of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and conduct disorder (O’Connor
et al. 2002, 2003; Van Den Bergh and Marcoen 2004; Rodriguez and Bohlin 2005;
Table 13.1  Studies showing prenatal anxiety, depression or stress is associated with an increased
risk of the following conditions
Psychological/behavioural/cognitive References
Worse function on the Brazelton test in
newborns
(Rieger et al. 2004)
More sleep problems in infants (O’Connor et al. 2007)
More anxiety in infants (Bergman et al. 2007)
More difficult temperament in infants (Austin et al. 2005; Werner et al. 2007; Davis
et al. 2007; Blair et al. 2011)
Worse cognitive ability in infancy
Increased cognitive ability in infancy
(Huizink et al. 2003; Bergman et al. 2007;
Laplante et al. 2004; DiPietro et al. 2006)
ADHD in childhood (O’Connor et al. 2002, 2003; Van Den Bergh
and Marcoen 2004; Rodriguez and Bohlin
2005; Li et al. 2010)
Emotional problems in childhood and
adolescence
(O’Connor et al. 2002, 2003; Van Den Bergh
and Marcoen 2004; Pawlby et al. 2009;
Barker et al. 2011)
Conduct disorder in childhood (O’Connor et al. 2002, 2003; Rice et al. 2010)
Decreased cognitive ability in childhood (Laplante et al. 2008; Barker et al. 2011)
Autism or autism spectrum disorder
No increased risk of autism
(Beversdorf et al. 2005; Kinney et al. 2008b;
Class et al. 2013; Li et al. 2009)
Vulnerability to bullying at school (Lereya and Wolke 2012)
Increased risk of schizophrenia in adulthood (van Os and Selten 1998; Khashan et al. 2008)
Physical
Lower birthweight and/or gestational age (Wadhwa et al. 2011; Rice et al. 2010)
Reduced telomere length (Entringer et al. 2011, 2013)
Oral cleft (Ingstrup et al. 2013)
Altered fingerprint pattern (King et al. 2009)
Mixed handedness (Glover et al. 2004; Rodriguez and
Waldenstrom 2008)
Altered immune function (O’Connor et al. 2013)
Asthma (Khashan et al. 2012)
Obesity (Entringer 2013)
Author's Proof
UNCORRECTED PROOF
Book ID: 304585_1_En ChapterID: 13 Dispatch Date: 09/07/2014 ProofNo: 1
87
88
89
90
91
92
93
94
95
96
97
98
99
100
101
102
103
104
105
106
107
108
109
110
111
112
113
114
115
116
117
118
119
120
121
122
123
124
125
126
127
128
129
130
131
4 V. Glover
Li et al. 2010; Pawlby et al. 2009; Barker et al. 2011; Rice et al. 2010). Studies have
also shown a reduction in cognitive performance associated with prenatal stress
(Laplante et al. 2008; Barker et al. 2011).
Some research (Beversdorf et al. 2005; Kinney et al. 2008b; Class et al. 2013),
although not all (Li et al. 2009), has found an association between prenatal stress
and increased risk of autism or autistic spectrum disorder. Two studies have found
an increased risk of schizophrenia in adults born to mothers who experienced stress
during pregnancy (van Os and Selten 1998; Khashan et al. 2008). Both showed
effects with severe stress, the death of a relative or exposure to the invasion of the
Netherlands in 1940. One group, using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), has
shown associations between prenatal stress and specific regional reductions in grey
matter density in the brain (Buss et al. 2010). Such altered grey matter may be as-
sociated with neurodevelopmental and psychiatric disorders as well as cognitive
and intellectual impairment.
There is little consistency in the literature as to the most sensitive time in gesta-
tion for the influence of prenatal stress. It is likely that there are different times of
sensitivity dependent on the outcome studied and the stage of development of the
relevant brain structures. The two studies of schizophrenia found the most sensitive
period was in the first trimester (van Os and Selten 1998; Khashan et al. 2008).
This is when neuronal cells migrate to their eventual site in brain, a process previ-
ously suggested to be disrupted in schizophrenia. In contrast, two studies of conduct
disorder, or antisocial behaviour, found associations with stress in mid or late preg-
nancy (O’Connor et al. 2003; Rice et al. 2010).
Many human studies, as discussed above, have shown that there is an association
between maternal stress during pregnancy and an altered outcome for the child. The
evidence for this is very strong and has been shown in many independent prospec-
tive studies from around the world. What is harder to establish is that the association
is causal. If a mother is stressed while she is pregnant, she may well be stressed
postnatally and this could affect her parenting. There can be other associated con-
founding factors such as smoking or alcohol consumption, which may affect her
child, for example, in behaviour and birthweight. There also could be genetic con-
tinuity. The mother may have certain genes which make her more likely to become
anxious or depressed and she may pass these genes on to her child, which in turn
makes them more prone to emotional or behavioural problems.
Several studies have tried to address these points but the first evidence to con-
sider is that from animals. With animal studies, it is much easier to establish that
prenatal stress has a direct effect on the outcome for the offspring. Newborn rat
pups of prenatally stressed mothers can be cross-fostered to non-stressed mothers
on the 1st day after birth, with control pups of unstressed mothers cross-fostered
also (Weinstock 2001; Maccari et al. 2003). This can establish that any differences
in outcome are caused by stress in the prenatal period. Many such studies have
shown that there are definite programming effects of prenatal stress on behaviour,
cognitive development and brain structure of the offspring. The nature of the effects
can be affected by the timing of the exposure in gestation, the type of the stress, the
strain of the animal and the age at which the offspring was tested (Weinstock 2007).
Author's Proof
UNCORRECTED PROOF
Book ID: 304585_1_En ChapterID: 13 Dispatch Date: 09/07/2014 ProofNo: 1
132
133
134
135
136
137
138
139
140
141
142
143
144
145
146
147
148
149
150
151
152
153
154
155
156
157
158
159
160
161
162
163
164
165
166
167
168
169
170
171
172
173
174
175
176
513 Prenatal Stress and Its Effects on the Fetus and the Child
Some altered outcomes are not observed in the youngest offspring, but become
apparent as they mature. The effects can also depend on the sex of the offspring.
In general, although not always, prenatal stress increases anxiety and depressive
behaviour to a greater extent in female offspring and impairs learning and cognition
more in the males (Weinstock 2007).
In humans there are several types of evidence which also suggest that prenatal
stress is causing fetal programming, but it is harder to be definitive. There is good
evidence for an association between the mother’s emotional state and the behaviour
or heart rate of her fetus. Experiments in which a pregnant mother is asked to carry
out a stressful computer task, while the fetal heart rate is monitored, showed that the
fetal heart rate went up during the task, but only in mothers who rated themselves
as anxious (Monk et al. 2003). Thus, even before birth, the fetus can be affected by
the maternal emotional state, although we do not know what the mechanism is for
this (it is too quick to be caused by the stress hormone cortisol). There is evidence
for continuity between fetal behaviour and neurological maturation at 2 years of age
(DiPietro et al. 2007). The fact that maternal stress during pregnancy is associated
with altered outcomes at birth including reduced birthweight (Wadhwa et al. 1993),
reduced scores on a neonatal assessment (Rieger et al. 2004) and epigenetic changes
in the glucocorticoid receptor in cord blood (Hompes et al. 2013) is evidence for
some prenatal, independent of postnatal, effects. Findings of altered fingerprint pat-
terns (King et al. 2009) and handedness (Glover et al. 2004) are also strong evi-
dence for prenatal effects as the pattern for both of these is set in utero.
Another approach to establishing that the associations between the maternal
emotional state and long-term outcome for the child are, at least in part, causal
is by controlling for confounding factors such as prenatal smoking and alcohol
consumption and for postnatal maternal mood. Several studies have done this and
found a strong signal remaining for prenatal anxiety or depression, thus controlling
for impaired parenting due to postnatal depression or anxiety, e.g. O’Connor et al.
(2002). If the observed associations are primarily due to an anxious mother passing
on predisposing genes to her child, it would not be expected to be specifically as-
sociated with prenatal as opposed to postnatal mood. In a recent study (O’Donnell
et al. in press) we have shown that the associations with child emotional and behav-
ioural problems last until 13 years of age. In this study, we also show that allowing
for paternal prenatal mood makes little difference to the associations with prenatal
maternal anxiety or depression, thus adding further evidence for maternal prenatal
effects independent of genetics.
An interesting study compared the association between prenatal stress and child
outcome in children born after in vitro fertilisation, in those who were genetically
related to the mother with those who were not (Rice et al. 2010). They showed that
there was an association between maternal stress in pregnancy and child symptoms
of ADHD and conduct disorder, and that the association with conduct disorder was
apparent in the unrelated mothers. This gives strong support to the idea that the
association between prenatal stress and child conduct disorder can be independent
of genetic factors. However, the fact that the increase in symptoms of ADHD was
apparent only in those with related mothers does not conclusively rule out a prenatal
AQ1
Author's Proof
UNCORRECTED PROOF
Book ID: 304585_1_En ChapterID: 13 Dispatch Date: 09/07/2014 ProofNo: 1
177
178
179
180
181
182
183
184
185
186
187
188
189
190
191
192
193
194
195
196
197
198
199
200
201
202
203
204
205
206
207
208
209
210
211
212
213
214
215
216
217
6 V. Glover
environmental component. There may be a gene–environment interaction. Prenatal
stress may only have the effect of increasing symptoms of ADHD in the genetically
vulnerable mother and child pairs. More research is needed to disentangle the role
of genetic factors for all outcomes.
One further indication that the effects of prenatal stress are not just due to genetic
continuity is the group of studies that have shown children of mothers exposed to
acute disasters, such as the Canadian ice storm (Laplante et al. 2008). With these
“natural experiments”, the level of stress was objectively assessed, and the exposure
was of a specific duration. This reduces the confounding effects of pre-existing
emotional problems and genetic continuity, and also postnatal emotional and par-
enting effects.
There clearly are additional effects of both postnatal maternal mood and par-
enting. For example, the association between prenatal anxiety and child fearful-
ness was found to be greater in those children with an insecure attachment to their
mother (Bergman et al. 2008). In our recent study (O’Donnell et al. in press), we
found that the magnitude of the effect of prenatal maternal mood was similar to that
of postnatal and that the two were additive.
Although there is some room for scepticism, the evidence is mounting that in
humans, as in animals, prenatal stress has a direct causal effect on fetal development
including neurodevelopment. However, the early postnatal environment is equally
important, and can either exacerbate or ameliorate the prenatal effects.
It is important to consider the clinical magnitude of these effects. When we com-
pared the outcome for children of the 15 % most prenatally anxious or depressed
women with the rest, in a large normal population (O’Donnell et al. in press), we
found that the rate of probable mental disorder doubled from about 6–12 % at age 13
years, after allowing for a very wide range of possible confounders. This is of clear
clinical and public health significance but also shows that most children are not
affected. Those children that are affected are often affected in different ways (Berg-
man et al. 2007). One reason for this may be differential genetic vulnerabilities.
13.2 Mechanisms
In animal studies, there has been much research on underlying mechanisms with an
especial focus on the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis (Weinstock 2005;
Harris and Seckl 2011; Khulan and Drake 2012). The effects of prenatal stress on
the offspring can be partially mimicked by giving the pregnant animal a synthetic
glucocorticoid such as dexamethasone (Matthews and Phillips 2011; Crudo et al.
2013) or adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) to stimulate the production of cor-
tisol (or corticosterone in rodents), and at least partially blocked by adrenalectomy
(Weinstock 2008).
A model of some potential underlying mechanisms, based on the animal data, is
shown in Fig. 13.1, with references in Table 13.2. The hypothesis is that prenatal
stress causes an increase in maternal cortisol; this then crosses the placenta in a
Author's Proof
UNCORRECTED PROOF
Book ID: 304585_1_En ChapterID: 13 Dispatch Date: 09/07/2014 ProofNo: 1
218
219
220
221
222
223
224
225
226
227
228
229
230
231
232
233
234
235
236
237
238
239
240
241
242
243
713 Prenatal Stress and Its Effects on the Fetus and the Child
quantity sufficient to affect the development of the fetal brain. However, each stage
of this needs to be tested in humans, and this is only just starting.
There is some evidence that in humans, as in animal models, prenatal administra-
tion of dexamethasone is associated with more behavioural and emotional problems
in the child (Khalife et al. submitted for publication), lasting at least until adoles-
cence, where a thinning of the cortex has been shown (Davis et al. 2013). There is
also evidence for the potentially widespread role for exposure to increased cortisol
in human fetal brain development by a study showing, by microarray analysis, that
increased cortisol exposure affects the expression of over a thousand genes in fetal
brain cells (Salaria et al. 2006).
However, there is either a weak or no correlation found in many studies be-
tween a motherl brain development by a study showing, by microarray analysis,
that increase (Sarkar et al. 2006; O’Donnell et al. 2009; Davis and Sandman 2010;
Baibazarova et al. 2013). In human pregnancy, the placenta produces increasing
concentrations of corticotrophin-releasing hormone (CRH) which stimulates ma-
ternal production of cortisol; towards the end of pregnancy, plasma levels reach
those found in melancholic depression. This in turn is associated with a dampened
cortisol response to stress (Kammerer et al. 2002). Maternal plasma cortisol levels
correlate strongly with cord blood (Gitau et al. 1998) and amniotic fluid (Sarkar
et al. 2007; Baibazarova et al. 2013), and prenatal maternal cortisol levels can be
a predictor of child outcome independent of maternal mood (Davis and Sandman
2010). However, the maternal mediator between prenatal stress, anxiety and depres-
sion and altered child outcome is currently not known.
One possible biological group of maternal mediators could be those associated
with the immune system and inflammation, such as the pro-inflammatory cyto-
kines. There is a growing literature associating them with depression (Hepgul et al.
Fig. 13.1  A model of some potential underlying mechanisms, based on the animal data
Author's Proof
UNCORRECTED PROOF
Book ID: 304585_1_En ChapterID: 13 Dispatch Date: 09/07/2014 ProofNo: 1
244
245
246
247
248
249
250
251
252
253
254
255
256
257
258
259
260
261
262
263
264
265
266
267
268
269
270
271
272
273
274
275
8 V. Glover
2013). Increased cytokines have been associated with psychosocial stress during
pregnancy (Coussons-Read et al. 2007). Elevated stress was related to higher se-
rum interleukin-6 (IL-6) both in early and late pregnancy. No relationships between
stress and cytokines were apparent during the second trimester. However, elevated
stress levels across pregnancy were predictive of elevated production of the pro-
inflammatory cytokines IL-1B and IL-6 by stimulated lymphocytes in the third
trimester, suggesting that stress during pregnancy affects the function of immune
system cells. A recent study has confirmed that depressed pregnant women have
higher levels of IL-6 in the first trimester (Haeri et al. 2013). However, another
study has failed to find any association between maternal symptoms of anxiety and
depression during pregnancy and levels of IL-6 (Blackmore et al. 2011), at 18 or 32
weeks. This is clearly an area that needs further exploration.
Increase in activity of the sympathetic system may also be important although it
has been much less studied than the HPA axis. However, noradrenaline, unlike cor-
tisol which is only partially metabolised (Gitau et al. 1998), is totally metabolised
by the placenta (Giannakoulopoulos et al. 1999).
The placenta plays a crucial part in fetal programming. Dependent on the chemi-
cal signals it receives from the mother, it can alter its filtering capacity and thus alter
the exposure of the fetus to specific chemicals (Jansson and Powell 2007). Animal
studies have shown that prenatal stress can have an effect on placental function,
including on the regulation of 11β-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase 2 (11β-HSD2),
the enzyme that breaks down cortisol (corticosterone in rodents) to inactive product.
Two studies have shown that prenatal stress in the last week of gestation caused a
downregulation of expression of this enzyme (Mairesse et al. 2007; Jensen Pena
et al. 2012). However, acute stress on day 20 of gestation caused an upregulation
(Welberg et al. 2005).
We have shown that with increasing maternal anxiety the correlation between
maternal plasma and amniotic fluid cortisol increased significantly (Glover et al.
2009). We have more recently shown directly, in women having an elective cae-
sarean section, that maternal symptoms of anxiety on the previous day were as-
sociated with a downregulation of 11β-HSD2 (O’Donnell et al. 2012). This would
be compatible with some chemical signal from the mother causing an alteration in
Table 13.2  Possible mechanisms
Mother References
Cortisol (Sarkar et al. 2006)
Cytokines (Coussons-Read et al. 2007)
Placenta
11 βHSD2 (O’Donnell et al. 2012)
Amniotic fluid
Cortisol (Bergman et al. 2010)
Child
HPA axis function (O’Donnell et al. 2013; Entringer et al. 2009)
Brain structure (Buss et al. 2010)
Epigenetic changes (Hompes et al. 2013)
AQ2
Author's Proof
UNCORRECTED PROOF
Book ID: 304585_1_En ChapterID: 13 Dispatch Date: 09/07/2014 ProofNo: 1
276
277
278
279
280
281
282
283
284
285
286
287
288
289
290
291
292
293
294
295
296
297
298
299
300
301
302
303
304
305
306
307
308
309
310
311
312
313
314
315
316
317
318
319
320
913 Prenatal Stress and Its Effects on the Fetus and the Child
the filtering capacity of the placenta, allowing more cortisol to pass through to the
fetus. Another study has failed to find this downregulation of placental 11β-HSD2
in association with prenatal maternal symptoms of anxiety or depression, (Ponder
et al. 2011), although they did find alterations in the noradrenaline transporter. This
may be due to differences in the design of the study, and especially in the inclusion
of women who have experienced labour. There is some evidence that labour alters
the expression of 11β-HSD2 (personal communication).
In addition to cortisol, another factor which may be important in altering fetal
brain development is 5-hydroxytryptamine (5-HT), which acts as a trophic factor
to regulate fetal neuronal cell division, differentiation and synaptogenesis (Gaspar
et al. 2003). 5-HT has a different role during early development from that in adult-
hood (Oberlander 2012). Whilst selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors can alleviate
anxiety and depression later in life, treatment of newborn mice with these drugs
causes an increase in these symptoms (Ansorge et al. 2004). Recent work has iden-
tified an endogenous serotonin biosynthetic pathway within the human placenta,
which plays a role in offspring neurodevelopment (Bonnin et al. 2011).
A major mechanism for removing 5-HT is its metabolism to inactive 5-hydroxy-
indoleacetic acid by the enzyme monoamine oxidase A (MAO A). This is the en-
zyme that metabolises a range of monoamine neurotransmitters including noradren-
aline and dopamine. The placenta is a very rich source of MAO A suggesting its
importance in the regulation of fetal monoamine exposure. We have recently shown
that prenatal maternal depression is associated with a downregulation of expression
of placental MAO A (Blakeley et al. in press), suggesting that another mechanism
underlying the effects of prenatal mood on fetal brain development may be via in-
creased exposure to 5-HT. This is a promising area for future research.
There is little direct human evidence yet that fetal overexposure to specific
chemicals is mediating the effects of prenatal stress. However, amniotic fluid corti-
sol levels have been shown to be inversely correlated with cognitive development
in the infant (Bergman et al. 2010), but only in those children who were insecurely
attached. It is clear that at least some prenatal effects can be buffered or modified
by the postnatal environment.
Animal studies have shown that many of the long-term effects of the early envi-
ronment, including the psychosocial, are due to epigenetic changes, which can be
maintained through the life span and even the grandchild generation (Meaney and
Szyf 2005; Bale et al. 2011; Monk et al. 2012; Gudsnuk and Champagne 2012).
These are changes “on top of the DNA” which alter whether a specific gene is
turned on or off, and if turned on, how much of it is expressed. Prenatal stress has
been shown to cause, through microRNA regulation, certain epigenetic signatures
of psychiatric and neurological diseases in the offspring (Zucchi et al. 2013).
In human studies too, epigenetic changes in the child are starting to be found
after prenatal stress, with an initial focus on the glucocorticoid receptor, the recep-
tor that responds to cortisol (Harris and Seckl 2011). Methylation in the promoter
of the glucocorticoid receptor NR3C1 in the newborn has been found to be altered
after prenatal stress, in a cohort from the Congo, and the changes were associated
with reduced birthweight (Mulligan et al. 2012). High pregnancy-specific anxiety
Author's Proof
UNCORRECTED PROOF
Book ID: 304585_1_En ChapterID: 13 Dispatch Date: 09/07/2014 ProofNo: 1
321
322
323
324
325
326
327
328
329
330
331
332
333
334
335
336
337
338
339
340
341
342
343
344
345
346
347
348
349
350
351
352
10 V. Glover
has also been shown to be associated with epigenetic changes in the promoter for
this receptor in the newborn (Hompes et al. 2013). And maternal exposure to inti-
mate partner violence has been shown to be associated with epigenetic changes to
the promoter for the same receptor in the blood of their adolescent children (Radtke
et al. 2011).
In animal models, it has been found that prenatal stress can have a long-term ef-
fect on the function of the HPA axis in the offspring, although the patterns are quite
complex (Weinstock 2005). There has been little work so far in humans but our
group has shown that prenatal anxiety was associated with raised morning cortisol
in 10-year-old children (O’Connor et al. 2005), but that the pattern had changed by
adolescence (O’Donnell et al. 2013). In 15-year-old children, there were modest but
significant effects, with the morning rise being reduced and a flatter diurnal slope.
It is unlikely that these changes in the diurnal cortisol pattern underlie any of the
emotional, behavioural or cognitive changes seen in older children as they are much
too small.
A notable finding of all the prenatal stress and child outcome studies is that most
of the children are not affected. This is probably due at least in part to different
genetic vulnerabilities and gene–environment interactions (Caspi et al. 2003). Al-
though no interactions have been found between prenatal anxiety, genetic variation
in the 5-HT transporter and child outcome (Braithwaite et al. 2013) we are finding
small interactions between prenatal anxiety and variants of the COMT and BDNF
genes (unpublished observations). This is certainly an area where further research
is warranted.
13.3 Conclusion
There is good evidence that various forms of prenatal stress contribute to long-
term neurodevelopmental changes in the child. The underlying mechanisms are just
starting to be understood, and probably include the HPA axis, changes in the filter-
ing capacity of the placenta and epigenetic changes in the child. However, much
work is needed before we understand these underlying mechanisms and are able to
evaluate and target different interventions properly.
13.4 Conflict of Interest
The author declares no conflict of interest.
Author's Proof
UNCORRECTED PROOF
Book ID: 304585_1_En ChapterID: 13 Dispatch Date: 09/07/2014 ProofNo: 1
353
354
355
356
357
358
359
360
361
362
363
364
365
366
367
368
369
370
371
372
373
374
375
376
377
378
379
380
381
382
383
384
385
386
387
388
389
390
391
392
393
394
395
396
397
398
399
400
401
402
403
404
1113 Prenatal Stress and Its Effects on the Fetus and the Child
References
Ansorge MS, Zhou M, Lira A, Hen R, Gingrich JA (2004) Early-life blockade of the 5-HT trans-
porter alters emotional behavior in adult mice. Science 306:879–881
Austin MP, Hadzi-Pavlovic D, Leader L, Saint K, Parker G (2005) Maternal trait anxiety, depres-
sion and life event stress in pregnancy: relationships with infant temperament. Early Hum Dev
81:183–190
Baibazarova E, van de Beek C, Cohen-Kettenis PT, Buitelaar J, Shelton KH, van Goozen SH
(2013) Influence of prenatal maternal stress, maternal plasma cortisol and cortisol in the am-
niotic fluid on birth outcomes and child temperament at 3 months. Psychoneuroendocrinology
38:907–915
Bale TL, Baram TZ, Brown AS, Goldstein JM, Insel TR, Mccarthy MM, Nemeroff CB, Reyes TM,
Simerly RB, Susser ES, Nestler EJ (2011) Early life programming and neurodevelopmental
disorders. Biol Psychiatry 68:314–319
Barker ED, Jaffee SR, Uher R, Maughan B (2011) The contribution of prenatal and postnatal ma-
ternal anxiety and depression to child maladjustment. Depress Anxiety 28:696–702
Bergman K, Sarkar P, O’Connor TG, Modi N, Glover V (2007) Maternal stress during pregnan-
cy predicts cognitive ability and fearfulness in infancy. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
46:1454–1463
Bergman K, Sarkar P, Glover V, O’Connor TG (2008) Quality of child-parent attachment moder-
ates the impact of antenatal stress on child fearfulness. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 49:1089–
1098
Bergman K, Sarkar P, Glover V, O’Connor TG (2010) Maternal prenatal cortisol and infant cog-
nitive development: moderation by infant-mother attachment. Biol Psychiatry 67:1026–1032
Beversdorf DQ, Manning SE, Hillier A, Anderson SL, Nordgren RE, Walters SE, Nagaraja HN,
Cooley WC, Gaelic SE, Bauman ML (2005) Timing of prenatal stressors and autism. J Autism
Dev Disord 35:471–478
Blackmore ER, Moynihan JA, Rubinow DR, Pressman EK, Gilchrist M, O’Connor TG (2011) Psy-
chiatric symptoms and proinflammatory cytokines in pregnancy. Psychosom Med 73:656–663
Blair MM, Glynn LM, Sandman CA, Davis EP (2011) Prenatal maternal anxiety and early child-
hood temperament. Stress 14:644–651
Bonnin A, Goeden N, Chen K, Wilson ML, King J, Shih JC, Blakely RD, Deneris ES, Levitt P
(2011) A transient placental source of serotonin for the fetal forebrain. Nature 472:347–350
Braithwaite EC, Ramchandani PG, O’Connor TG, van Ijzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg
MJ, Glover V, Netsi E, Evans J, Meaney MJ, Murphy SE (2013) No moderating effect of 5-HT-
TLPR on associations between antenatal anxiety and infant behavior. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
Psychiatry 52:519–526
Buss C, Davis EP, Muftuler LT, Head K, Sandman CA (2010) High pregnancy anxiety during
mid-gestation is associated with decreased gray matter density in 6–9-year-old children. Psy-
choneuroendocrinology 35:141–153
Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Craig IW, Harrington H, McClay J, Mill J, Martin J,
Braithwaite A, Poulton R (2003) Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a poly-
morphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science 301:386–389
Class QA, Abel KM, Khashan AS, Rickert ME, Dalman C, Larsson H, Hultman CM, Langstrom
N, Lichtenstein P, D’Onofrio BM (2013) Offspring psychopathology following preconception,
prenatal and postnatal maternal bereavement stress. Psychol Med 44:1–14
Coussons-Read ME, Okun ML, Nettles CD (2007) Psychosocial stress increases inflammatory
markers and alters cytokine production across pregnancy. Brain Behav Immun 21:343–350
Crudo A, Suderman M, Moisiadis VG, Petropoulos S, Kostaki A, Hallett M, Szyf M, Matthews SG
(2013) Glucocorticoid programming of the fetal male hippocampal epigenome. Endocrinology
154:1168–1180
Davis EP, Sandman CA (2010) The timing of prenatal exposure to maternal cortisol and psycho-
social stress is associated with human infant cognitive development. Child Dev 81:131–148
Author's Proof
UNCORRECTED PROOF
Book ID: 304585_1_En ChapterID: 13 Dispatch Date: 09/07/2014 ProofNo: 1
405
406
407
408
409
410
411
412
413
414
415
416
417
418
419
420
421
422
423
424
425
426
427
428
429
430
431
432
433
434
435
436
437
438
439
440
441
442
443
444
445
446
447
448
449
450
451
452
453
454
455
456
457
12 V. Glover
Davis EP, Glynn LM, Schetter CD, Hobel C, Chicz-Demet A, Sandman CA (2007) Prenatal ex-
posure to maternal depression and cortisol influences infant temperament. J Am Acad Child
Adolesc Psychiatry 46:737–746
Davis EP, Sandman CA, Buss C, Wing DA, Head K (2013) Fetal glucocorticoid exposure is as-
sociated with preadolescent brain development. Biol Psychiatry 74:647–655
DiPietro JA, Novak MF, Costigan KA, Atella LD, Reusing SP (2006) Maternal psychological
distress during pregnancy in relation to child development at age two. Child Dev 77:573–587
DiPietro JA, Bornstein MH, Hahn CS, Costigan K, Achy-Brou A (2007) Fetal heart rate and vari-
ability: stability and prediction to developmental outcomes in early childhood. Child Dev
78:1788–1798
Entringer S (2013) Impact of stress and stress physiology during pregnancy on child metabolic
function and obesity risk. Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care 16:320–327
Entringer S, Kumsta R, Hellhammer DH, Wadhwa PD, Wust S (2009) Prenatal exposure to ma-
ternal psychosocial stress and HPA axis regulation in young adults. Horm Behav 55:292–298
Entringer S, Epel ES, Kumsta R, Lin J, Hellhammer DH, Blackburn EH, Wust S, Wadhwa PD
(2011) Stress exposure in intrauterine life is associated with shorter telomere length in young
adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108:E513–E518
Entringer S, Epel ES, Lin J, Buss C, Shahbaba B, Blackburn EH, Simhan HN, Wadhwa PD (2013)
Maternal psychosocial stress during pregnancy is associated with newborn leukocyte telomere
length. Am J Obstet Gynecol 208:134.e1–e7
Gaspar P, Cases O, Maroteaux L (2003) The developmental role of serotonin: news from mouse
molecular genetics. Nat Rev Neurosci 4:1002–1012
Giannakoulopoulos X, Teixeira J, Fisk N, Glover V (1999) Human fetal and maternal noradrena-
line responses to invasive procedures. Pediatr Res 45:494–499
Gitau R, Cameron A, Fisk NM, Glover V (1998) Fetal exposure to maternal cortisol. Lancet
352:707–708
Glover V (2011) Annual research review: prenatal stress and the origins of psychopathology: an
evolutionary perspective. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 52:356–367
Glover V, O’Connor TG, Heron J, Golding J (2004) Antenatal maternal anxiety is linked with
atypical handedness in the child. Early Hum Dev 79:107–118
Glover V, Bergman K, Sarkar P, O’Connor TG (2009) Association between maternal and amniotic
fluid cortisol is moderated by maternal anxiety. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34:430–435
Gudsnuk K, Champagne FA (2012) Epigenetic influence of stress and the social environment.
ILAR J 53:279–288
Haeri S, Baker AM, Ruano R (2013) Do pregnant women with depression have a pro-inflammato-
ry profile? J Obstet Gynaecol Res 39:948–952
Harris A, Seckl J (2011) Glucocorticoids, prenatal stress and the programming of disease. Horm
Behav 59:279–289
Hepgul N, Cattaneo A, Zunszain PA, Pariante CM (2013) Depression pathogenesis and treatment:
what can we learn from blood mRNA expression? BMC Med 11:28
Hompes T, Izzi B, Gellens E, Morreels M, Fieuws S, Pexsters A, Schops G, Dom M, Van Bree
R, Freson K, Verhaeghe J, Spitz B, Demyttenaere K, Glover V, Van den Bergh B, Allegaert K,
Claes S (2013) Investigating the influence of maternal cortisol and emotional state during preg-
nancy on the DNA methylation status of the glucocorticoid receptor gene (NR3C1) promoter
region in cord blood. J Psychiatr Res 47:880–891
Huizink AC, de Medina PG, Mulder EJ, Visser GH, Buitelaar JK (2002) Psychological measures
of prenatal stress as predictors of infant temperament. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
41:1078–1085
Huizink AC, Robles deMPG, Mulder EJ, Visser GH, Buitelaar JK (2003) Stress during pregnancy
is associated with developmental outcome in infancy. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 44:810–818
Huizink AC, Dick DM, Sihvola E, Pulkkinen L, Rose RJ, Kaprio J (2007) Chernobyl exposure
as stressor during pregnancy and behaviour in adolescent offspring. Acta Psychiatr Scand
116:438–446
Author's Proof
UNCORRECTED PROOF
Book ID: 304585_1_En ChapterID: 13 Dispatch Date: 09/07/2014 ProofNo: 1
458
459
460
461
462
463
464
465
466
467
468
469
470
471
472
473
474
475
476
477
478
479
480
481
482
483
484
485
486
487
488
489
490
491
492
493
494
495
496
497
498
499
500
501
502
503
504
505
506
507
508
509
510
1313 Prenatal Stress and Its Effects on the Fetus and the Child
Ingstrup KG, Liang H, Olsen J, Nohr EA, Bech BH, Wu CS, Christensen K, Li J (2013) Maternal
bereavement in the antenatal period and oral cleft in the offspring. Hum Reprod 28:1092–1099
Jansson T, Powell TL (2007) Role of the placenta in fetal programming: underlying mechanisms
and potential interventional approaches. Clin Sci (Lond) 113:1113
Jensen Pena C, Monk C, Champagne FA (2012) Epigenetic effects of prenatal stress on 11beta-
hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase-2 in the placenta and fetal brain. PLoS ONE 7:e39791
Kammerer M, Adams D, Castelberg BV, Glover V (2002) Pregnant women become insensitive to
cold stress. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 2:8
Khashan AS, Abel KM, McNamee R, Pedersen MG, Webb RT, Baker PN, Kenny LC, Mortensen
PB (2008) Higher risk of offspring schizophrenia following antenatal maternal exposure to
severe adverse life events. Arch Gen Psychiatry 65:146–152
Khashan AS, Wicks S, Dalman C, Henriksen TB, Li J, Mortensen PB, Kenny LC (2012) Prenatal
stress and risk of asthma hospitalization in the offspring: a Swedish population-based study.
Psychosom Med 74:635–641
Khulan B, Drake AJ (2012) Glucocorticoids as mediators of developmental programming effects.
Best Pract Res Clin Endocrinol Metab 26:689–700
King S, Mancini-Marie A, Brunet A, Walker E, Meaney MJ, Laplante DP (2009) Prenatal maternal
stress from a natural disaster predicts dermatoglyphic asymmetry in humans. Dev Psycho-
pathol 21:343–353
Kinney DK, Miller AM, Crowley DJ, Huang E, Gerber E (2008a) Autism prevalence follow-
ing prenatal exposure to hurricanes and tropical storms in Louisiana. J Autism Dev Disord
38:481–488
Kinney DK, Munir KM, Crowley DJ, Miller AM (2008b) Prenatal stress and risk for autism. Neu-
rosci Biobehav Rev 32:1519–1532
Laplante DP, Barr RG, Brunet A, Galbaud duFG, Meaney ML, Saucier JF, Zelazo PR, King S
(2004) Stress during pregnancy affects general intellectual and language functioning in human
toddlers. Pediatr Res 56:400–410
Laplante DP, Brunet A, Schmitz N, Ciampi A, King S (2008) Project ice storm: prenatal maternal
stress affects cognitive and linguistic functioning in 5 1/2-year-old children. J Am Acad Child
Adolesc Psychiatry 47:1063–1072
Lereya ST, Wolke D (2012) Prenatal family adversity and maternal mental health and vulnerability
to peer victimisation at school. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 54:644–652
Li J, Vestergaard M, Obel C, Christensen J, Precht DH, Lu M, Olsen J (2009) A nationwide
study on the risk of autism after prenatal stress exposure to maternal bereavement. Pediatrics
123:1102–1107
Li J, Olsen J, Vestergaard M, Obel C (2010) Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in the off-
spring following prenatal maternal bereavement: a nationwide follow-up study in Denmark.
Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 19:747–753
Maccari S, Darnaudery M, Morley-Fletcher S, Zuena AR, Cinque C, Van Reeth O (2003) Prenatal
stress and long-term consequences: implications of glucocorticoid hormones. Neurosci Biobe-
hav Rev 27:119–127
Mairesse J, Lesage J, Breton C, Breant B, Hahn T, Darnaudery M, Dickson SL, Seckl J, Blondeau
B, Vieau D, Maccari S, Viltart O (2007) Maternal stress alters endocrine function of the feto-
placental unit in rats. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 292:E1526–E1533
Matthews SG, Phillips DI (2011) Minireview: transgenerational inheritance of the stress response:
a new frontier in stress research. Endocrinology 151:7–13
Meaney MJ, Szyf M (2005) Environmental programming of stress responses through DNA meth-
ylation: life at the interface between a dynamic environment and a fixed genome. Dialogues
Clin Neurosci 7:103–123
Monk C, Myers MM, Sloan RP, Ellman LM, Fifer WP (2003) Effects of women’s stress-elicited
physiological activity and chronic anxiety on fetal heart rate. J Dev Behav Pediatr 24:32–38
Monk C, Spicer J, Champagne FA (2012) Linking prenatal maternal adversity to developmental
outcomes in infants: the role of epigenetic pathways. Dev Psychopathol 24:1361–1376
Author's Proof
UNCORRECTED PROOF
Book ID: 304585_1_En ChapterID: 13 Dispatch Date: 09/07/2014 ProofNo: 1
511
512
513
514
515
516
517
518
519
520
521
522
523
524
525
526
527
528
529
530
531
532
533
534
535
536
537
538
539
540
541
542
543
544
545
546
547
548
549
550
551
552
553
554
555
556
557
558
559
560
561
562
563
14 V. Glover
Mulligan CJ, D’Errico NC, Stees J, Hughes DA (2012) Methylation changes at NR3C1 in new-
borns associate with maternal prenatal stress exposure and newborn birth weight. Epigenetics
7:853–857
O’Connor TG, Heron J, Golding J, Beveridge M, Glover V (2002) Maternal antenatal anxiety and
children’s behavioural/emotional problems at 4 years. Report from the Avon longitudinal study
of parents and children. Br J Psychiatry 180:502–508
O’Connor TG, Heron J, Golding J, Glover V (2003) Maternal antenatal anxiety and behavioural/
emotional problems in children: a test of a programming hypothesis. J Child Psychol Psychia-
try 44:1025–1036
O’Connor TG, Ben-Shlomo Y, Heron J, Golding J, Adams D, Glover V (2005) Prenatal anxiety
predicts individual differences in cortisol in pre-adolescent children. Biol Psychiatry 58:211–
217
O’Connor TG, Caprariello P, Blackmore ER, Gregory AM, Glover V, Fleming P, ALSPAC Team
(2007) Prenatal mood disturbance predicts sleep problems in infancy and toddlerhood. Early
Hum Dev 83:451–458
O’Connor TG, Winter MA, Hunn J, Carnahan J, Pressman EK, Glover V, Robertson-Blackmore E,
Moynihan JA, Lee FE, Caserta MT (2013) Prenatal maternal anxiety predicts reduced adaptive
immunity in infants. Brain Behav Immun 32:21–28
O’Donnell K, O’Connor TG, Glover V (2009) Prenatal stress and neurodevelopment of the child:
focus on the HPA axis and role of the placenta. Dev Neurosci 31:285–292
O’Donnell KJ, Bugge Jensen A, Freeman L, Khalife N, O’Connor TG, Glover V (2012) Maternal
prenatal anxiety and downregulation of placental 11beta-HSD2. Psychoneuroendocrinology
37:818–826
O’Donnell KJ, Glover V, Jenkins J, Browne D, Ben-Shlomo Y, Golding J, O’Connor TG (2013)
Prenatal maternal mood is associated with altered diurnal cortisol in adolescence. Psychoneu-
roendocrinology 38:1630–1638
Oberlander TF (2012) Fetal serotonin signaling: setting pathways for early childhood development
and behavior. J Adolesc Health 51:S9–S16
Pawlby S, Hay DF, Sharp D, Waters CS, O’Keane V (2009) Antenatal depression predicts depres-
sion in adolescent offspring: prospective longitudinal community-based study. J Affect Disord
113:236–243
Ponder KL, Salisbury A, McGonnigal B, Laliberte A, Lester B, Padbury JF (2011) Maternal de-
pression and anxiety are associated with altered gene expression in the human placenta with-
out modification by antidepressant use: implications for fetal programming. Dev Psychobiol
53:711–723
Radtke KM, Ruf M, Gunter HM, Dohrmann K, Schauer M, Meyer A, Elbert T (2011) Transgenera-
tional impact of intimate partner violence on methylation in the promoter of the glucocorticoid
receptor. Transl Psychiatry 1:e21
Rice F, Harold GT, Boivin J, van den Bree M, Hay DF, Thapar A (2010) The links between prena-
tal stress and offspring development and psychopathology: disentangling environmental and
inherited influences. Psychol Med 40:335–345
Rieger M, Pirke KM, Buske-Kirschbaum A, Wurmser H, Papousek M, Hellhammer DH (2004)
Influence of stress during pregnancy on HPA activity and neonatal behavior. Ann N Y Acad
Sci 1032:228–230
Rodriguez A, Bohlin G (2005) Are maternal smoking and stress during pregnancy related to
ADHD symptoms in children? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 46:246–254
Rodriguez A, Waldenstrom U (2008) Fetal origins of child non-right-handedness and mental
health. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 49:967–976
Salaria S, Chana G, Caldara F, Feltrin E, Altieri M, Faggioni F, Domenici E, Merlo-Pich E, Everall
IP (2006) Microarray analysis of cultured human brain aggregates following cortisol exposure:
implications for cellular functions relevant to mood disorders. Neurobiol Dis 23:630–636
Sarkar P, Bergman K, Fisk NM, Glover V (2006) Maternal anxiety at amniocentesis and plasma
cortisol. Prenat Diagn 26:505–509
Author's Proof
UNCORRECTED PROOF
Book ID: 304585_1_En ChapterID: 13 Dispatch Date: 09/07/2014 ProofNo: 1
564
565
566
567
568
569
570
571
572
573
574
575
576
577
578
579
580
581
582
583
584
585
586
587
588
589
590
591
592
593
594
595
596
597
598
599
600
601
602
603
1513 Prenatal Stress and Its Effects on the Fetus and the Child
Sarkar P, Bergman K, Fisk NM, O’Connor TG, Glover V (2007) Ontogeny of foetal exposure
to maternal cortisol using midtrimester amniotic fluid as a biomarker. Clin Endocrinol (Oxf)
66:636–640
Talge NM, Neal C, Glover V (2007) Antenatal maternal stress and long-term effects on child neu-
rodevelopment: how and why? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 48:245–261
Van den Bergh BR, Marcoen A (2004) High antenatal maternal anxiety is related to ADHD symp-
toms, externalizing problems, and anxiety in 8- and 9-year-olds. Child Dev 75:1085–1097
Van den Bergh BR, Mulder EJ, Mennes M, Glover V (2005) Antenatal maternal anxiety and stress
and the neurobehavioural development of the fetus and child: links and possible mechanisms.
A review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29:237–258
Van den Bergh BR, Van Calster B, Smits T, Van Huffel S, Lagae L (2008) Antenatal maternal
anxiety is related to HPA-axis dysregulation and self-reported depressive symptoms in adoles-
cence: a prospective study on the fetal origins of depressed mood. Neuropsychopharmacology
33:536–545
van Os J, Selten JP (1998) Prenatal exposure to maternal stress and subsequent schizophrenia. The
May 1940 invasion of The Netherlands. Br J Psychiatry 172:324–326
Wadhwa PD, Sandman CA, Porto M, Dunkel-Schetter C, Garite TJ (1993) The association be-
tween prenatal stress and infant birth weight and gestational age at birth: a prospective investi-
gation. Am J Obstet Gynecol 169:858–865
Wadhwa PD, Entringer S, Buss C, Lu MC (2011) The contribution of maternal stress to preterm
birth: issues and considerations. Clin Perinatol 38:351–384
Weinstock M (2001) Alterations induced by gestational stress in brain morphology and behaviour
of the offspring. Prog Neurobiol 65:427–451
Weinstock M (2005) The potential influence of maternal stress hormones on development and
mental health of the offspring. Brain Behav Immun 19:296–308
Weinstock M (2007) Gender differences in the effects of prenatal stress on brain development and
behaviour. Neurochem Res 32:1730–1740
Weinstock M (2008) The long-term behavioural consequences of prenatal stress. Neurosci Biobe-
hav Rev 32:1073–1086
Welberg LA, Thrivikraman KV, Plotsky PM (2005) Chronic maternal stress inhibits the capacity
to up-regulate placental 11beta-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase type 2 activity. J Endocrinol
186:R7–R12
Werner EA, Myers MM, Fifer WP, Cheng B, Fang Y, Allen R, Monk C (2007) Prenatal predictors
of infant temperament. Dev Psychobiol 49:474–484
Yehuda R, Engel SM, Brand SR, Seckl J, Marcus SM, Berkowitz GS (2005) Transgenerational
effects of posttraumatic stress disorder in babies of mothers exposed to the World Trade Center
attacks during pregnancy. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 90:4115–4118
Zucchi FC, Yao Y, Ward ID, Ilnytskyy Y, Olson DM, Benzies K, Kovalchuk I, Kovalchuk O, Metz
GA (2013) Maternal stress induces epigenetic signatures of psychiatric and neurological dis-
eases in the offspring. PLoS ONE 8:e56967
Author's Proof
Chapter 13: Author Query
AQ1. The following authors are not present in the reference list. Please provide the complete reference for the citations “O’Donnell et al.
in press”, “Khalife et al. submitted for publication”, “Blakeley et al. in press”.
AQ2. We have changed “11 bHSD2” to “11 βHSD2” in table 13.2 to maintain consistency. Please check.
Author's Proof
... stress hormones (cortisol, catecholamine, and norepinephrine) during pregnancy are likely related to the development of preeclampsia and birth complications (Glover 2015;Paarlberg et al. 1995). Pregnant women with insufficiently treated depression are also more prone to pregnancy complications (Bonari et al. 2004;Dennis and Dowswell 2013) and cesarean deliveries (Chung et al. 2001;Oberlander et al. 2006). ...
Chapter
The time of pregnancy, as the earliest stage of human development, can influence the individual’s biological and psychological development into adulthood. From this perspective, good conditions for optimal development are desirable. Human touch and massage have the potential to trigger complex biochemical reactions that can positively affect the physical and psychological state of an individual. Human touch and massage of the maternal body during pregnancy influence both the mother and the fetus and can reduce pregnancy complications. The chapter also contains information on perineal massage to prevent perineal injuries during birth and manual techniques to reduce pain during labor.
... Prenatal stress can adversely affect offspring development, with several mechanisms hypothesized for this effect. One such pathway could be increased prenatal glucocorticoid exposure, which in humans is reflected in elevated amniotic fluid cortisol (CORT) (Glover, 2015). This hypothesis is additionally supported by research showing a mediating role for corticosterone in the adverse effects of prenatal stress on offspring in rodent models (Weinstock, 2008). ...
... Similar results were found in a recent study, evidencing that female offspring have also shown higher reactivity to stress [41]. Hypothetic pathways to explain the influence are in maternal HPA activation, which could alter fetuses' HPA triggering the activation or deactivation of different mechanisms implicated [42]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Prenatal stress could have serious consequences on maternal and fetal health. In this sense, some studies have stated that maternal HCC during pregnancy could contribute to sex-specific effects on infant neurodevelopment, following the Developmental Origins of Health and Disease Hypothesis. Aim: This study aimed to determine whether maternal hair cortisol concentration (HCC) during each trimester of pregnancy and postpartum could predict the neurodevelopmental outcomes of their 12-month-old offspring, with sex-specific differences considered. Study design: longitudinal. Subjects: The study involved 93 pregnant women and their babies. Outcome measure: Hair samples collected during each trimester and postpartum and The Bayley Scales for Infant Development III was used to assess the infants' abilities. Results: The results showed that maternal HCC during the first and second trimesters could predict language and motor abilities. However, when discriminated by sex, only females' cognitive, expressive language, and fine and gross motor skills were predicted by cortisol, not males. Conclusions: These findings support the idea that non-toxic levels of cortisol can positively influence infants' neurodevelopment.
... It highlights the multifaceted nature of prenatal development, where many factors converge to shape an unborn child's life trajectory. Understanding these intricate dynamics enriches our comprehension of prenatal stress and underscores the necessity of holistic, multidisciplinary approaches to prenatal care and support [18]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Prenatal stress is increasingly recognized as a significant factor impacting an individual's life from the beginning. This comprehensive review explores the intricate relationship between prenatal stress and its effects on behaviour, cognition, and psychopathology. Key findings reveal that prenatal stress can lead to a wide range of adverse outcomes in offspring, including neurodevelopmental disorders, emotional dysregulation, cognitive deficits, mood disorders, and an increased risk of psychopathological conditions. These effects' mechanisms involve epigenetic modifications, hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis dysregulation, neurodevelopmental alterations, inflammatory processes, and changes in brain structure and function. Moreover, moderating factors such as maternal stress levels, maternal mental health, socioeconomic status, social support, and early-life adversity can significantly influence the impact of prenatal stress. The review also discusses intervention and prevention strategies, emphasizing the importance of prenatal stress reduction programs, maternal mental health support, nutritional interventions, and targeted early interventions for at-risk populations. These findings have substantial implications for public health and clinical practice, highlighting the need for a holistic approach to prenatal care that prioritizes maternal well-being and mitigates the lasting effects of prenatal stress. Addressing this critical issue promises healthier generations and stronger communities in the future.
... However, there remains a paucity of work that has examined whether pandemic-related stress during pregnancy is prospectively related to later child development disruptions. More broadly, studies conducted prior to the pandemic have established links between perinatal distress and poorer child outcomes including worse cognitive performance [27], psychiatric disorders [28,29], and more socioemotional [30] and behavioral [31] problems. Pregnancy-related anxiety (i.e., specific anxiety about the emotions, thoughts, and physical symptoms experienced by pregnant individuals; [32]), has also been uniquely linked to negative developmental outcomes in offspring, including developmental delays [33] and risk for emotional and behavioral problems [34]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The COVID-19 pandemic has been linked to increased risk for perinatal anxiety and depression among parents, as well as negative consequences for child development. Less is known about how worries arising from the pandemic during pregnancy are related to later child development, nor if resilience factors buffer negative consequences. The current study addresses this question in a prospective longitudinal design. Data was collected from a sub-study (n = 184) of a longitudinal study of pregnant individuals (total n = 1173). During pregnancy (April 17–July 8, 2020) and the early postpartum period (August 11, 2020–March 2, 2021), participants completed online surveys. At 12 months postpartum (June 17, 2021–March 23, 2022), participants completed online surveys and a virtual laboratory visit, which included parent–child interaction tasks. We found more pregnancy-specific pandemic worries were prospectively related to lower levels of child socioemotional development based on parent report (B = − 1.13, SE = .43, p = .007) and observer ratings (B = − 0.13, SE = .07, p = .045), but not to parent-reported general developmental milestones. Parental emotion regulation in the early postpartum period moderated the association between pregnancy-specific pandemic worries and child socioemotional development such that pregnancy-specific pandemic worries did not relate to worse child socioemotional development among parents with high (B = − .02, SE = .10, t = − .14, p = .89) levels of emotion regulation. Findings suggest the negative consequences of parental worry and distress during pregnancy on the early socioemotional development of children in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. Results highlight that parental emotion regulation may represent a target for intervention to promote parental resilience and support optimized child development.
... The importance of the placenta, an endocrine tissue that facilitates fetal-maternal exchange, as a mediator of the effects of stress during pregnancy has become increasingly recognized [9,70,71]. Placental gene expression patterns have been shown to be altered by maternal stress exposure, and these changes may lead to long-term effects on the offspring's neurodevelopment [9,72,73]. For instance, placental gene expression profiles have been linked to changes in brain morphology, altered stress reactivity, and behavioral outcomes in rodents and humans [74][75][76][77]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) has been associated with a complex interplay between genetic and environmental factors. Prenatal stress exposure has been identified as a possible risk factor, although most stress-exposed pregnancies do not result in ASD. The serotonin transporter (SERT) gene has been linked to stress reactivity, and the presence of the SERT short (S)-allele has been shown to mediate the association between maternal stress exposure and ASD. In a mouse model, we investigated the effects of prenatal stress exposure and maternal SERT genotype on offspring behavior and explored its association with maternal microRNA (miRNA) expression during pregnancy. Pregnant female mice were divided into four groups based on genotype (wildtype or SERT heterozygous knockout (Sert-het)) and the presence or absence of chronic variable stress (CVS) during pregnancy. Offspring behavior was assessed at 60 days old (PD60) using the three-chamber test, open field test, elevated plus-maze test, and marble-burying test. We found that the social preference index (SPI) of SERT-het/stress offspring was significantly lower than that of wildtype control offspring, indicating a reduced preference for social interaction on social approach, specifically for males. SERT-het/stress offspring also showed significantly more frequent grooming behavior compared to wildtype controls, specifically for males, suggesting elevated repetitive behavior. We profiled miRNA expression in maternal blood samples collected at embryonic day 21 (E21) and identified three miRNAs (mmu-miR-7684-3p, mmu-miR-5622-3p, mmu-miR-6900-3p) that were differentially expressed in the SERT-het/stress group compared to all other groups. These findings suggest that maternal SERT genotype and prenatal stress exposure interact to influence offspring behavior, and that maternal miRNA expression late in pregnancy may serve as a potential marker of a particular subtype of ASD pathogenesis.
Article
Background Adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) are pernicious events (e.g., physical abuse) that occur before 18 years of age within the household. Mothers’ ACEs are associated intergenerationally with their preschool children’s (two-to-five-year-old) behavioural problems, impacting lifelong mental health trajectories. Mediators (e.g., mental health) may explain how mothers’ ACEs exert their influence on children's behavioural development whereas moderators (e.g., child sex) may affect the strength and direction of the association, revealing potential points of intervention. Therefore, this study aimed to: (1) identify mediators and moderators of this association; and (2) describe and compare moderation and mediation outcomes. Methods This review (#CRD42022307214) extracted data from peer-reviewed literature formally analyzing at least one moderating or mediating variable of the association between maternal ACEs and preschool children’s behavioural problems using MEDLINE, Embase, APA PsycINFO, Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials, CINAHL, SCOPUS, and Web of Science databases (n = 7). Validity was assessed using the Joanna Briggs Institute checklists. Results Thirteen full-text studies with moderate-to-high validity were identified. Most studies examined mediating variables, with unanimous support for mediation through maternal depression. The mediating roles of adult attachment styles and anxiety were conflicting. No studies examined environmental factors (e.g., neurotoxins). Conclusions Mothers’ depression is a significant contributor to children’s behavioural problems in the context of ACEs and should be a target of early intervention to prevent lifelong challenges. Future research should focus on examining more moderating variables and consider the roles of environmental factors. More research is needed on the moderating and mediating roles of genes.
Article
China’s growing prominence as a trade superpower has placed competitive pressure on manufacturing industries in Brazil, while simultaneously bolstering demand for its commodities. I investigate the effects of this so-called manufactures-for-commodities boom on Brazilian birth outcomes from 2000 to 2010. Exploiting exogenous variation in patterns of trade growth with China across different regions within Brazil, I find that both import and export growth led to higher birth weights for babies, and lower infant mortality rates. I also find that negative import shocks reduced fertility rates across all age groups for women, suggesting that selectivity in births induced by negative income shocks, combined with concentration of household resources on the children that are born led to better infant health outcomes. Additional evidence is consistent with income effects playing a role in explaining the results, while ruling out better provision of healthcare and changes to household composition as mechanisms. I also explore changes in trade-induced pollution levels and social assistance programs as a potential mechanism. The findings indicate that increased import and export growth can improve infant health, highlighting another potential benefit from trade liberalization.
Article
Objective Supporting families during the first 1001 days from conception to the age of two is vital for setting the emotional, cognitive, and physical building blocks for children's futures. Families with twins, triplets, or higher order multiples (multiple birth families) have unique challenges due to caring for more than one baby at the same time. Therefore, identifying the needs of multiple birth families is necessary to provide optimum support during the first 1001 critical days. Design A rapid review was undertaken to synthesize knowledge of the needs of multiple birth families in the United Kingdom (UK) during the first 1001 critical days. Findings from five databases (MEDLINE, APA PsycArticles, APA PsycInfo, CINAHL, and Web of Science) for peer‐reviewed studies and grey literature published between 2012 and 2022 were synthesized. Fifteen studies were reviewed using narrative synthesis. Results Multiple birth families have unique and complex emotional and practical needs across the first 1001 critical days, and in particular, the first‐year post birth, impacted further by complicated pregnancies and prematurity. Needs were identified within the four key themes: high risk pregnancy and birth; transformed reality of raising multiples; inadequate support; and positively affecting experiences. Health professional support was inconsistent and particularly lacking in intrapartum, postnatal, and community care including transition. Conclusion Multiple birth families’ needs should be considered in the design and delivery of care within the first 1001 critical days, especially within the first year after birth. Multiples specific advice across the first 1001 critical days is needed and training for health professionals to adapt universal advice for this population is one way to achieve this. Further research is needed to ensure this advice is evidence based and effective.
Article
Prenatal maternal stress (PNMS)—characterized by exposure to stress, anxiety, depression, or intimate partner violence—has been linked to biological alterations in infants, including disruptions to their intestinal microbiota, which have long‐term implications for children's developmental outcomes. Significant research has been done examining the effects of PNMS on the microbiome in animals, but less is known about these effects in human research. The current systematic review aimed to synthesize current findings on the association between PNMS and mother and infant microbiomes. Medline, Embase, PsycInfo, Web of Science, and Eric databases were searched through to February 2022. A total of eight studies ( n = 2219 infants, 2202 mothers) were included in the qualitative synthesis. Findings provided promising evidence of the role that PNMS plays in altering the microbial composition, diversity, and gut immunity in mothers and infants. Notably, majority of included studies found that higher PNMS was linked to increases in genera from the phylum Proteobacteria . The factors influencing these effects are explored including nutrition, birth mode, and parenting behaviors. Potential interventions to mitigate the adverse effects of PNMS are discussed, along with recommendations for future studies with longitudinal designs to better understand the appropriate type and timing of interventions needed to promote “healthy” maternal and infant microbial functioning.
Article
Full-text available
New genetic models that target the serotonin system show that transient alterations in serotonin homeostasis cause permanent changes to adult behaviour and modify the fine wiring of brain connections. These findings have revived a long-standing interest in the developmental role of serotonin. Molecular genetic approaches are now showing us that different serotonin receptors, acting at different developmental stages, modulate different developmental processes such as neurogenesis, apoptosis, axon branching and dendritogenesis. Our understanding of the specification of the serotonergic phenotype is improving. In addition, studies have revealed that serotonergic traits are dissociable, as there are populations of neurons that contain serotonin but do not synthesize it.
Article
Full-text available
Maternal antenatal anxiety is associated with an increased risk of behavioral disturbances in offspring. Recent work has suggested that the effect of maternal antenatal anxiety on infant temperament at 6 months is moderated by the serotonin transporter polymorphism 5-HTTLPR, with carriers of the short allele more susceptible to the adverse behavioral outcomes of maternal antenatal anxiety. These findings, however, are yet to be replicated and extended beyond infancy. The aim of the current study was to assess this same potential moderator (5-HTTLPR) in a large population-based cohort study, and to determine whether or not the effects persist into childhood and early adolescence. Data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Children and Parents (ALSPAC) cohort (N = 3,946) were used to assess whether the 5-HTTLPR genotype moderated the association between self-reported maternal antenatal anxiety (Crown Crisp Index) in pregnancy, and child temperament at 6 months (Infant Temperament Questionnaire), and also later behavioral and emotional problems on the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire from age 4 to 13 years. We found no evidence to suggest that the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism moderated the effects of maternal antenatal anxiety on infant temperament at 6 months or infant behavioral and emotional problems from childhood through to adolescence. Our results, based on a large prospective community sample that assessed children from infancy to early adolescence, provide a thorough test of, but no evidence for, a genetic moderation of the effects of maternal antenatal anxiety by 5-HTTLPR.
Article
Animal models of early-life stress and variation in social experience across the lifespan have contributed significantly to our understanding of the environmental regulation of the developing brain. Plasticity in neurobiological pathways regulating stress responsivity, cognition, and reproductive behavior is apparent during the prenatal period and continues into adulthood, suggesting a lifelong sensitivity to environmental cues. Recent evidence suggests that dynamic epigenetic changes—molecular modifications that alter gene expression without altering the underlying DNA sequence—account for this plasticity. In this review, we highlight studies of laboratory rodents that illustrate the association between the experience of prenatal stress, maternal separation, maternal care, abusive caregiving in infancy, juvenile social housing, and adult social stress and variation in DNA methylation and histone modification. Moreover, we discuss emerging evidence for the transgenerational impact of these experiences. These experimental paradigms have yielded insights into the potential role of epigenetic mechanisms in mediating the effects of the environment on human development and also indicate that consideration of the sensitivity of laboratory animals to environmental cues may be an important factor in predicting long-term health and welfare.
Article
Background: Glucocorticoids play a critical role in normative regulation of fetal brain development. Exposure to excessive levels may have detrimental consequences and disrupt maturational processes. This may especially be true when synthetic glucocorticoids are administered during the fetal period, as they are to women in preterm labor. This study investigated the consequences for brain development and affective problems of fetal exposure to synthetic glucocorticoids. Methods: Brain development and affective problems were evaluated in 54 children (56% female), aged 6 to 10, who were full term at birth. Children were recruited into two groups: those with and without fetal exposure to synthetic glucocorticoids. Structural magnetic resonance imaging scans were acquired and cortical thickness was determined. Child affective problems were assessed using the Child Behavior Checklist. Results: Children in the fetal glucocorticoid exposure group showed significant and bilateral cortical thinning. The largest group differences were in the rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC). More than 30% of the rACC was thinner among children with fetal glucocorticoid exposure. Furthermore, children with more affective problems had a thinner left rACC. Conclusions: Fetal exposure to synthetic glucocorticoids has neurologic consequences that persist for at least 6 to 10 years. Children with fetal glucocorticoid exposure had a thinner cortex primarily in the rACC. Our data indicating that the rACC is associated with affective problems in conjunction with evidence that this region is involved in affective disorders raise the possibility that glucocorticoid-associated neurologic changes increase vulnerability to mental health problems.
Article
Background: Preconception, prenatal and postnatal maternal stress is associated with increased offspring psychopathology, but findings are inconsistent and need replication. We estimated associations between maternal bereavement stress and offspring autism spectrum disorder (ASD), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, suicide attempt and completed suicide. Method: Using Swedish registers, we conducted the largest population-based study to date examining associations between stress exposure in 738,144 offspring born 1992-2000 for childhood outcomes and 2,155,221 offspring born 1973-1997 for adult outcomes with follow-up to 2009. Maternal stress was defined as death of a first-degree relative during (a) the 6 months before conception, (b) pregnancy or (c) the first two postnatal years. Cox proportional survival analyses were used to obtain hazard ratios (HRs) in unadjusted and adjusted analyses. Results: Marginal increased risk of bipolar disorder and schizophrenia following preconception bereavement stress was not significant. Third-trimester prenatal stress increased the risk of ASD [adjusted HR (aHR) 1.58, 95% confidence interval (CI) 1.15-2.17] and ADHD (aHR 1.31, 95% CI 1.04-1.66). First postnatal year stress increased the risk of offspring suicide attempt (aHR 1.13, 95% CI 1.02-1.25) and completed suicide (aHR 1.51, 95% CI 1.08-2.11). Bereavement stress during the second postnatal year increased the risk of ASD (aHR 1.30, 95% CI 1.09-1.55). Conclusions: Further research is needed regarding associations between preconception stress and psychopathological outcomes. Prenatal bereavement stress increases the risk of offspring ASD and ADHD. Postnatal bereavement stress moderately increases the risk of offspring suicide attempt, completed suicide and ASD. Smaller previous studies may have overestimated associations between early stress and psychopathological outcomes.
Article
Aim: We tested the hypothesis that maternal depression is associated with a pro-inflammatory state in pregnancy. Material and methods: In this nested case-control study, pro-inflammatory cytokine levels were compared between women with depression in pregnancy (n = 100) and a computer-generated referent group of healthy women known not to be depressed (n = 100). We only included cases with a documented Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders depression diagnosis in the current pregnancy. Serum samples drawn at 11-14 weeks of gestation were analyzed for levels of tumor necrosis factor-alpha and interleukin-6 using high-sensitivity immunoassays. Results: Maternal demographics were similar between the groups except for older age (34.1 vs 32.7 years, P = .05), and lower body mass index (27.3 vs 28.9 kg/m², P = 0.03) among the depressed subjects. Compared to control women, tumor necrosis factor-alpha (5.8 ± 3.4 vs 3.2 ± 2.8 pg/ml, P < 0.0001) and interleukin-6 (2.4 ± 3.8 vs 1.5 ± 1.4 pg/ml, P = 0.03) levels were higher among women with depression. The higher rate of inflammatory cytokines remained significant after controlling for potential confounders, including maternal age and body mass index. Conclusion: Women with depression may have higher levels of inflammatory markers in early pregnancy. Our findings support the hypothesis that inflammation may be a mediator in the association between maternal depression and adverse perinatal outcomes.