ArticlePDF Available

Should We Let Sleeping Dogs Lie… With Us? Synthesizing the Literature and Setting the Agenda for Research on Human-animal Co-sleeping Practices

Authors:

Abstract

Humans and animals share their lives with one another in many ways. Pets, for example, are often considered valuable members of the family. They are invited into human hearts and homes, where they share routines, food and space. Animals are not only important to human waking lives. Some of them also share human beds – be it by invitation or invasion! Yet, little is known about human-animal co-sleeping practices and their impact on humans, animals and human-animal relations. This is the first paper to critically collate the scant and disparate research on human-animal co-sleeping. Given that the research is risk-centric, the paper turns to established theories on animal attachment to propose explanations for the continued practice of human-animal co-sleeping and its benefits. To redress an anthropocentric bias in the limited existing research, an agenda for further research is outlined on human-animal co-sleeping that considers humans, animals and the human-animal relation.
H U M a N I M A L I A 6:1
Kirrilly Thompson and Bradley Smith
Should We Let Sleeping Dogs Lie… With Us? Synthesizing the
Literature and Setting the Agenda for Research on Human-animal
Co-sleeping Practices
Introduction. Human sleep practices are highly divergent across culture and time
(Blunden, Thompson & Dawson; Worthman and Melby; Munroe, Munroe, and
Whiting). In the same way that sleeping spaces in industrialized societies have
increasingly become divided according to age (adult and child), so have spaces been
segregated according to species (human and non-human animal), although many
cultures have practiced or still practice co-sleeping. This is often related to crowding or
a response to domestic spaces having no physical internal divisions, but also because
co-sleeping is the norm for some cultures devoid of the taboos of incest and
bestiality, or the socio-cultural construction of sleep disorders that can be found at the
core of (or indeed contribute to) solo-sleeping practices in other cultures.
Although co-sleeping in indigenous societies primarily involved humans (particularly
children), it is likely to have also involved companion animals or pets. The practice of
pet keeping dates back to Paleolithic hunters and early agriculturalists, who kept
animals for leisure activities (with inherent social and emotional rewards) and as
functional assets (e.g., to assist in hunting or as educational and play “objects” for
children [Serpell]). Whilst the sleeping arrangements of pets were not the focus of early
anthropological accounts, human-animal co-sleeping has been widely recorded in
ethnographies of indigenous Australians. During cold nights, indigenous Australians
were often reported to sleep alongside their dogs for warmth (Hamilton; Meggitt; Smith
and Litchfield). This practice is implicated in the common Australian expression “three
dog night”: the colder the night, the more dogs are needed to keep warm (Breckwoldt).
Today, humans continue to show strong attachment to their pets, and often consider
them important members of the family (Archer). In return, companion animals are
sources of unconditional support, love, comfort, security, and stability that also provide
health benefits (Smith). These benefits are felt strongly in Australia, which has one of
the highest levels of pet ownership in the developed world, with around sixty per cent
of households owning at least one pet (Australian Bureau of Statistics). Pet ownership
in Australia has remained relatively stable during the past few years, current figures
Kirrilly Thompson and Bradley Smith — “Should We Let Sleeping Dogs Lie… With Us?”
115
indicating that thirty-six per cent of households own dogs, and twenty-three per cent
own cats (Australian Companion Animal Council).
Companion animals not only often live inside the home, but many also share their
guardians’ beds or sleep in their bedrooms (Beck and Katcher). Such practices are
particularly meaningful in contemporary Western cultures, where the parental bed has
been increasingly sexually constructed. Sleeping space has evolved over time,
particularly around the end of the nineteenth century when the individual bed was
regarded as an essential ingredient of civilized society, informed by ideas of privacy.
“The bed remained a highly problematic, indeterminate space, facilitating deviant as
much as civilized behavior, and giving rise to all manner of pathologies, perversities
and phobias” (Crook 15). In many Westernized and industrialized nations such as
Australia, the majority of families encourage infants to sleep alone from any early age
(Blunden, Thompson, and Dawson). Allowing animals in the private space of the bed
and/or bedroom therefore indicates the status and value that is placed on many
companion animals. Some authors have alluded to the untested assumption that letting
a dog sleep on the bed is indicative of a positive pet attachment (e.g., Katcher, as cited
by Archer). Franklin, for example, interprets pets in bedrooms as indicative of their
status as intimate family members. Beck and Katcher go so far as to suggest that the
“pet’s privilege of sharing the master’s bed elevates him [sic] above human children,
who are usually banned from the parental bedroom at night” (20).
Unfortunately, little is known about the prevalence of human-animal co-sleeping
relationships or their impact on sleep. The research reporting human-animal co-
sleeping practices is piecemeal at best, using data from non-dedicated or non-validated
surveys (such as those undertaken by the pet care and pet food industry). An indicative
picture of human-animal co-sleeping practices can be sketched by drawing from
various studies that are based on a variety of tools and which span several decades and
research populations. Estimations of prevalence rates are often limited to online surveys
or those conducted by commercial pet organizations. Various studies from around the
world report that approximately half of pet guardians let their pets sleep in their beds
with them during the night (eg. Katcher et al.; Albert and Bulcroft; Westgarth et al.;
Overgaauw).
Humanimalia: a journal of human/animal interface studies
Volume 6, Number 1 (Fall 2014)
116
The manifestation of this practice differs according to several factors, such as the type of
pet cats and dogs are the most frequent bed visitors, with cats more likely to sleep
with the family than any other type (Albert and Bulcroft); the size of the pet: most dogs
allowed in the bed are smaller breeds (Eckstein); the number of children in the
household, higher for those with no children (Albert and Bulcroft); and guardian
characteristics, such as gender (more common in females) and ethnicity (higher in
Anglo-Saxon than African American [Brown, “Ethnic”; Brown, “Companion
Animals”]). Despite any benefits experienced by the pet owner or guardian, there are
various consequences of having pets in bedrooms. These include health hazards, effect
on sleep quality, and behavioral problems displayed by the pet, as well as its impact on
the interpersonal relations of humans in the bedroom.
Health hazards: There are various health hazards associated with pet ownership that are
accentuated by allowing pets into the bedroom and bed. These include immunologic
responses resulting in allergic disease, asthma, and/or hypersensitivity pneumonitis;
bites and scratches from pets causing tissue damage and inducing infections and
infectious diseases associated with pets (see Plaut, Zimmerman, and Goldstein for a
comprehensive review). A dog sleeping in a family member’s bedroom may also be a
risk factor for biting (Messam et al.). The most susceptible include small children,
pregnant women, and immuno-deficient patients (Smith). However, overall health risks
are relatively low particularly if the animals are kept clean and routine veterinarian
care is maintained.
Effect on sleep quality: While common causes of sleep disturbance involve partners (e.g.,
kicking, snoring, visits to the toilet) and children, having pets can also represent a
significant cause of disturbance for those who allow pets in the bed or bedroom. For
instance, the Mayo Sleep Clinic in the US surveyed 300 patients with an existing sleep
disorder, and found that fifty-three per cent of pet guardians who allowed their pet(s)
to sleep in their beds were disturbed every night by the animal in some way. It is
difficult to determine the significance of these disruptions, with only one per cent of
patients feeling that their sleep was disrupted for more than twenty minutes on average
per night (Fayerman). Smith et al. compared the self-reported sleep of pet versus non-
pet co-sleepers, and found that sleeping with pets in the bed had a small impact on
sleep quality e.g., time taken to fall asleep, and feelings of tiredness upon waking a
sign of disrupted sleep). The authors inferred that the continued practice of co-sleeping
Kirrilly Thompson and Bradley Smith — “Should We Let Sleeping Dogs Lie… With Us?”
117
with pets indicates attendant benefits such as social support and social interaction, and
increased feelings of personal security, despite any disadvantages.
Disruptions may be related to mismatches between human, dog, and cat core
temperatures, as well as differences in sleep-wake cycles, as pets do not spend eight
hours in a sleep state (see Campbell and Tobler). For example, Adams and Johnson
examined the sleep-wake cycles of dogs in various urban backyards, and found that
dogs had an average of 23 sleep/wake episodes (or 3 sleep/wake cycles per hour), with
active sleep followed immediately by spontaneous arousal. They found that dogs are
also responsive to auditory stimuli regardless of whether the sleep state was quiet or
active (Adams and Johnson). Their responses to such stimuli often led to dogs being a
nuisance to people in the neighborhood (e.g. due to barking) and potentially disrupting
the sleep of guardians and non-guardians alike.
Pet behavioral problems: Jagoe and Serpell found that dogs who are allowed to sleep in
their guardians’ beds or bedrooms at night may experience such behavioral problems as
an increase in competitive aggression (i.e., aggression when attention is paid to others;
aggression to other dogs in the household) and separation-related problems (i.e.,
separation-related urination and defecation). Beck and Katcher describe a woman who
complained that she was unable to make the bed in the morning because when her
husband went to work, the dog would jump on the bed and growl, snap, or bite if she
tried to remove him. Although Jagoe and Serpell could not determine the direction of
causation, they note it is likely that dogs sleeping with or close to the guardian will
develop an “unbalanced” attachment for that person and thus react adversely to
separation. Another explanation may relate to the aforementioned impact of dog bed-
sharing practices on the ranking of household individuals human and animal. The
guardian may be forced to accept on overly attached canine sleeping partner to avoid
nocturnal separation problems in the dog (Jagoe and Serpell).
Effect on interpersonal relationships: Pets sleeping in their guardians’ beds can be a source
of conflict for couples, sometimes leading to problems with intimacy, particularly if the
pet is unwelcome to one partner. Jagoe and Serpell, for instance, describe a couple
whose dog was interfering in their sex life. “The husband felt sexually inhibited and
turned off when the animal was in the bed, yet his wife would not otherwise have
intercourse” (50).
Humanimalia: a journal of human/animal interface studies
Volume 6, Number 1 (Fall 2014)
118
Broad as it may be, the literature on human-animal co-sleeping is risk-centric. That is, it
is equated with risks to health or sleep hygiene, or considered a threat to the intimacy of
couples. Moreover, the practice is associated with deviant sexuality in the form of
bestiality, or, at the least, unhealthy romantic attachments (Beetz and Podberscek;
Dekkers). This risk-centric bias, which emphasizes the disadvantages of human-animal
co-sleeping, has contributed to the psychological, social, and cultural benefits of human-
animal co-sleeping practices being overlooked and under-researched.
Why do people human-animal co-sleep? Despite a lack of rigorous data on the
prevalence of human-animal co-sleeping, there is no doubt that people continue to
share their beds with pets. In some instances, the practice occurs despite negative
impacts, such as an increase in the time taken to fall asleep (extended sleep latency), or
sensitivity to sleep disturbance from animal noises (Smith et al.). This raises the
question of why people (continue to) co-sleep with pets. First, there are some very
practical considerations that provide ready explanations. For example, some pets may
be more distracting if they are not in their guardians’ beds. That is, they might scratch at
doors or vocalize their attempts to sleep in beds with humans. Voith describes a woman
who allowed her dog to sleep in her bed (even though it would bite her during the
night) because it refused to sleep on the floor and would bark incessantly if shut out of
the room. Moreover, some human sleepers who would prefer to sleep without pets
might not have any choice, such as when the final decision is made by another human
with whom they share their bed.
Second, there are also several theoretical and practical explanations that provide
insight. Attachment theory, originally developed by John Bowlby, explains
interpersonal relations between children and adults, whereby children feel secure in the
presence of adults to whom they are attached (Bowlby, Attachment), and feel separation
anxiety at the perceived short- or long-term loss of those “attachment figures” (Bowlby,
Separation). The theory has been extended to discuss human relations with animals
(Beck and Madresh; Julius, Beetz and Kotrschal). Whilst many people who feel attached
to their pets have no need or desire to share their beds with them, attachment theory
suggests that some humans are motivated to sleep with their pets in the bed to feel
secure and avoid separation anxiety.
Further theoretical explanation for human-animal co-sleeping can be found in Russell
Belk’s consideration of animals and pets as “extended selves” (“Possessions”). Belk
considers the ways in which relationships with pets can be so important to the identity
Kirrilly Thompson and Bradley Smith — “Should We Let Sleeping Dogs Lie… With Us?”
119
of some humans that pets might best be understood as special cases of extended human
selves. He describes how animals (like objects, money, other people, prostheses, and so
on) can become extensions or parts of human selves that “define who we are”
(“Possessions” 139). This idea is established empirically, not only in relation to typical
pets such as dogs, but also to non-traditional companion animals such as horses (Belk
“Metaphoric”). Sue-Ellen Brown (“Ethnic” and “Companion”) similarly considers
animals as “self-objects“ that are integral to human identity, sometimes surpassing the
ability of other humans to provide a sense of self.
The idea of animals as human extensions challenges the very question of why humans
would want to sleep with pets in their beds at all. That is, if a pet is considered an
extension of a particular human, it would be expected to accompany that human self in
sleep. The concept of pet as extended self enables an immediate recognition that asking
some people to sleep without their pets is akin to asking them to sleep without a part of
their selves, and as absurd as asking them to leave their pets behind during evacuation
from disasters (Thompson). Nonetheless, humans who consider or act as if their pets
and animals are extended selves are unlikely to be accompanied by that pet throughout
all their daily activities (such as work, travel, showering). Therefore, the decisions
humans make about where lines of pet accompaniment are drawn are complex and
dependent upon other philosophical, psychological, cultural, emotional, and practical
factors.
Future research on human-animal co-sleeping: setting the research agenda. The
literature reviewed in this paper suggests that pet owners frequently share their beds
with their pets, yet human-animal co-sleeping has been taken for granted, failing to
attract dedicated interest from (or funding for) animal studies scholarship or sleep
research. Rather, the implications have been considered narrowly through a focus on
threats to human health and social relations. As such, existing research can be
considered both risk-centric and anthropocentric, providing only a partial
understanding of human-animal co-sleeping and its multiple implications. To ensure a
comprehensive and symmetrical consideration of the practice of human-animal co-
sleeping, there is a need to identify benefits or trade-offs and understand the
implications from a bi-directional and relational perspective. As humans and animals
exist in relation with (Haraway Companion and When Species Meet) and in “being
alongside” (Latimer) one another, the impact of pet-bed sharing from a relational
Humanimalia: a journal of human/animal interface studies
Volume 6, Number 1 (Fall 2014)
120
perspective seems warranted. To establish a more comprehensive body of knowledge
on human-animal co-sleeping which is free of biases towards human beings and risk,
we propose the following three broad areas of focus: on humans, on animals and on the
human-animal relation.
Humans: In the preceding discussion, we proposed some practical and theoretical
explanations for (continued) human-animal co-sleeping. To fully evaluate our
propositions, there is a need to collect empirical data on motivations and rewards for
human-animal co-sleeping, using inductive research techniques, such as open-ended
surveys and interviews that can capture the unanticipated dimensions of human-animal
co-sleeping. From a more positivistic perspective, there is a need for a dedicated and
comprehensive survey of human-animal co-sleeping practices that combines and uses
knowledge from the disciplines of sleep research and animal studies. As a minimum
data set, and based on our own survey research (Smith et al.), we recommend that the
following four demographic questions be included in basic sleep research participant
demographic collection, as well as in research on human-animal co-sleeping: 1) do you
own a pet, 2) do you sleep with your pet, 3) where in the room is your pet (on floor, on
bed, in bed), and 4) can the pet toilet independently (i.e., can it let itself in and out of the
room for toileting without waking the respondent).
The third question of the minimum data set on human-animal co-sleeping is designed
to capture the location of the pet in relation to the human. This would enable an
evaluation of any differences in sleep hygiene, quality, and quantity between pets being
in the bedroom and pets being in the bed. The resulting knowledge could be used to
assist those with sleep problems to review their sleeping practices and justify and
support any behavior change interventions. For example, having pets in the same room
rather than the same bed might provide an incremental pathway for necessary behavior
change (as noted above), without causing undue emotional stress to the pet or
guardian, or reducing the benefits that guardians enjoy when human-animal co-
sleeping.
A particularly novel application for an understanding of the impact of the pet’s location
in the bedroom supports the need to undertake more research on the impacts of human-
animal co-sleeping on the sleep of children. Triebenbacher found that “children
perceive their pets as special friends, important family members, and providers of social
interactions, affection, and emotional support” (191). The role of pets in children’s lives
can change according to developmental stages, but Robin and Bensal identify their
Kirrilly Thompson and Bradley Smith — “Should We Let Sleeping Dogs Lie… With Us?”
121
general roles as “parents” as well as transitional objects. Psychotherapist D.W.
Winnicott discusses transitional objects in relation to children’s development,
considering objects such as a baby’s favorite toy as mediating and crossing the
boundary between inner mind and outer reality. The object is both independent of the
baby and, consistent with Belk’s “extended selves,” a part of the baby who “created”
and modified it. If we replace the object with a pet (real or toy), the pet can be seen to be
part of the child. As a result, a child required to sleep without a pet may experience
difficulty falling asleep due to anxiety over the absence of something that can be
understood as a part of the child.
As transitional objects, there may be a role for pets in developing solo sleeping practices
amongst children (where “solo” is understood to exclude other humans). Parent-child
co-sleeping is frequently constructed as problematic in Western society (Blunden,
Thompson, and Dawson). However, pets could play a role as transitional objects
(subjects) in encouraging solo sleeping practices amongst children who would
otherwise seek comfort and security by sleeping in their parents’ or siblings’ beds
(Triebenbacher). Research evaluating this proposition should compare real versus toy
pets as transitional objects, and consider the moral and ethical implication of using pets
for this purpose. Beetz and Podberscek, for example, found that amongst “male
children with insecure/disorganized attachment, the interaction with a real dog rather
than with a toy dog ... lowered salivary cortisol levels during a social challenge
situation” (361). The greater ability for a real dog to serve as an attachment figure and
reduce stress does not necessarily preclude the ability for a toy dog to provide some
benefit. A toy dog may be a preferable alternative in many situations, especially where
families may lack the space or resources to care for a real pet or where a householder
may be allergic to other animals.
Research attention also needs to be paid to the potential disadvantages of human-
animal co-sleeping in general, or of using pets specifically for the purpose of developing
solo sleeping practices amongst children. For example, there may be instances where
humans become dependent on the presence of pets to fall asleep. This may cause
problems in relation to short-term changes to sleeping routines (traveling or going to
hospital), as well as longer-term changes such as the unavoidable death of a pet.
Researching such cases would provide interesting perspectives on the social dimensions
Humanimalia: a journal of human/animal interface studies
Volume 6, Number 1 (Fall 2014)
122
of sleep, where “social” is defined in a way that includes “humans and other animals,”
as is accepted practice in the field of animal studies.
Animals: The limited and disparate research on human-animal co-sleeping has been
concerned with the impact of human-animal co-sleeping on human sleep quality and
quantity. However, there is an equivalent need to consider the impact of human-animal
co-sleeping on animals, especially in relation to their health and welfare. In the same
way that there are moral and ethical considerations for working animals, such as those
used in animal-assisted therapy (Serpell), so too should the health and well-being of
animals involved in human-animal co-sleeping be of central importance. Concerns
include the unlikely risk of anthroponotic diseases as well as negative impacts on
animal social relations and behavior.
For example, if attachment theory can be applied to an animal’s attachment to humans,
could human-animal co-sleeping practices exacerbate its separation anxiety during
waking hours when attachment figures are not physically present? Where human-
animal co-sleeping practices differ in a household with more than one pet (that is, one
dog sleeps inside and another outside), there is a need to understand the impact on
inter-pet relations. This is especially the case where negative inter-pet relations can lead
to anti-social behaviors which can impact inter-personal relations (between husband
and wife; for example, Jagoe and Serpell), jeopardize human health (through increased
biting behavior; for example, Smith) or cause community conflict (through barking
complaints; for example, Kobelt et al.). Furthermore, as noted above animals have
different circadian rhythms, temperatures, and sleep-wake cycles (Campbell and
Tobler; Adams and Johnson). Accelerometers may provide a non-invasive and cost
effective method for determining circadian rhythms in pets, particularly dogs and cats
(Hansen et al.).
Human-animal relations: Further research should be conducted that determines any
significant relationship between objective scores on animal attachment scales such as
the owner-pet relationship scale (Winefield et al.), and the practice of human-animal co-
sleeping. This research could assist with an understanding of the role that attachment
plays in decisions for humans and animals to co-sleep or in the impact of human-animal
co-sleeping on attachment. It could also be used to critically review a fundamental
assumption inherent in previous research that human-animal co-sleeping is indicative
of positive attachment (e.g., Katcher, as cited by Archer). In addition to the use of
surveys, researchers should employ open-ended techniques to consider the impact of
Kirrilly Thompson and Bradley Smith — “Should We Let Sleeping Dogs Lie… With Us?”
123
human-animal co-sleeping on the human-animal relationship. Where sharing a bed
with a pet is shown to influence a particular kind of human-animal relation, research
should consider the implications for human-animal relations where bed-sharing is
improbable (with larger animals such as horses, for example). Whilst human-animal co-
sleeping may be found to be indicative of positive human-animal attachment, the
inability to share a bed (for health reasons or because the animal is large) or its non-
practice does not necessarily preclude particular experiences of positive or profound
attachment between humans and animals.
Conclusion. The benefits of pet ownership and companionship have been extolled by
researchers for several decades and by owners and guardians for centuries more. There
is little doubt that pets contribute to the everyday experiences of humans, and some
pets contribute to their “everynight” experiences too. In fact, more than half of the sixty
per cent of pet owners and guardians in developed countries will spend approximately
one third of their lives sleeping alongside their pets. In this paper, we have synthesized
the scant research on the topic to establish the high incidence and significant
implications of this taken-for-granted behavior for humans and animals and their
relations with one another. It seems that humans and animals are not strange
bedfellows after all. What kind of bedfellows they are remains to be determined.
Works Cited
Adams, Graham, and Kevin Johnson. “Behavioural responses to barking and other
auditory stimuli during night-time sleeping and waking in the domestic dog (Canis
familiaris).” Applied Animal Behaviour Science 39.2 (1994): 151-162.
Albert, Alexa, and Kris Bulcroft. “Pets and Urban Life.” Anthrozoös 1.1 (1987): 9-25.
Archer, John. “Why do people love their pets?” Evolution and Human Behavior 18.4
(1997): 237-259.
Australian Bureau of Statistics. Australian Social Trends. Canberra, Australia: Australian
Bureau of Statistics, 1995.
Australian Companion Animal Council. “Contribution of the pet care industry to the
Australian Economy- 7th edition.” Australian Companion Animal Council, 2010.
Humanimalia: a journal of human/animal interface studies
Volume 6, Number 1 (Fall 2014)
124
Beck, Aaron, and Alan Katcher. Between Pets and People: The Importance of Animal
Companionship. West Lafayette, IN: Purdue UP, 1996.
Beck, Lisa, and Elizabeth Madresh. “Romantic partners and four-legged friends: an
extension of attachment theory to relationships with pets.” Anthrozoös 21.1 (2008): 43-56.
Beetz, Andrea, Kurt Kotrschal, Dennis Turner, Karin Hediger, Kerstin Unvas-Moberg,
and Henri Julius. “The effect of a real dog, toy dog and friendly person on insecurely
attached children during a stressful task: An exploratory study.” Anthrozoös 24.4 (2011):
349-368.
Beetz, Andrea, and Anthony Podberscek. Bestiality and Zoophilia: Sexual Relationships
with Animals. West Lafayette, IN: Purdue UP, 2005
Belk, Russell. “Metaphoric relationships with pets.” Society and Animals 4.2 (1996): 121-
145.
_____. “Possessions and the extended self.Journal of Consumer Research 15.2 (1988): 139-
168.
Blunden, Sarah, Kirrilly Thompson, and Drew Dawson. “Behavioral sleep treatments
and night time crying in infants: Challenging the status quo.” Sleep Medicine Review
15.5 (2011): 327-334.
Bowlby, John. Attachment: Attachment and Loss. 2nd ed. Vol. 1. New York: Basic Books,
1999.
_____. Separation: Anxiety and Anger. Vol. 2. London: Hogarth, 1973.
Breckwoldt, Roland. A very elegant animal: The dingo. North Ryde, Australia: Angus and
Robertson, 1988.
Brown, Sue-Ellen. “Companion Animals as Selfobjects.” Anthrozoös 20.4 (2007): 329-343.
_____. “Ethnic variations in pet attachment among students at an American School of
Veterinary medicine.” Society and Animals 10.3 (2002): 249-266.
Campbell, Scott, and Irene Tobler. “Animal sleep: a review of sleep duration across the
phylogeny.” Neuroscience and Behavioral Reviews 8.3 (1984): 269-300.
Crook, Tom. “Norms, Forms and Beds: Spatializing Sleep in Victorian Britain.” Body
Society 14.4 (2008): 15-35.
Kirrilly Thompson and Bradley Smith — “Should We Let Sleeping Dogs Lie… With Us?”
125
Dekkers, Midas. Dearest Pet: On Bestiality. Trans. Paul Vincent. London: Verso, 1994.
Dement, William, and Christopher Vaughan. The Promise of Sleep: A Pioneer in Sleep
Medicine Explores the Vital Connection Between Health, Happiness, and a Good Night's Sleep.
New York: Dell 1999.
Eckstein, Sandy. “Pets in Your Bed.“ WebMD. 2010. Online. 14 Jul. 14, 2014
Fayerman, Pamela. “Pet’s love outweighs other factors for owners.” North Bay Nugget. 2
Mar. 2002. n..p.
Franklin, Adrian. Animal nation: The True Story of Animals in Australia. Sydney: New
South Wales University Press, 2006.
Hamilton, Annette. “Aboriginal man’s best friend?” Mankind 8.4 (1972): 287-295.
Hansen, Bernard, et al. “Evaluation of an accelerometer for at-home monitoring of
spontaneous activity in dogs.” American Journal of Veterinary Research 68.5 (2007): 468-75.
Haraway, Donna. The Companion Species Manifesto: Dogs, People, and Significant
Otherness. Chicago: Prickly Paradigm Press, 2003.
_____. When Species Meet. Minneapolis: U of Minnesota P, 2007.
Jagoe, Andrew, and James Serpell. “Owner characteristics and interactions and the
prevalence of canine behaviour problems.” Applied Animal Behaviour Science 47.1-2
(1996): 31-42.
Julius, Henri, Andrea Beetz, and Kurt Kotrschal. Attachment to Pets: An Integrative View
of Human-Animal Relationships with Implications for Therapeutic Practice. Toronto, CA:
Hogrefe, 2012.
Katcher, Aaron, E. Friedmann, M. Goodman, and L. Goodman. “Men, women, and
dogs.” Californian Veterinarian 2 (1983): 14–16.
Kobelt, Amanda, P. Hemsworth, J. Barnett, and G. Coleman. “A survey of dog
ownership in suburban Australia—conditions and behaviour problems.” Applied Animal
Behaviour Science 82.2 (2003): 137–148.
Latimer, Joanna. “Being Alongside: Rethinking Relations Amongst Different Kinds.”
Theory, Culture & Society 30.7-8 (2013): 77-104.
Humanimalia: a journal of human/animal interface studies
Volume 6, Number 1 (Fall 2014)
126
Meggitt, Mervyn. “Australian Aborigines and Dingoes.” Man, Culture and Animals. Ed
Anthony Leeds and Andrew Peter Vayda. American Association for the Advancement
of Science Symposium Publication, 1965. 7-26.
Messam, Locksley, P. Kass, B. Chomel, and L. Hart. “The human-canine environment: a
risk factor for non-play bites.” The Veterinary Journal 177.2 (2008): 205-215.
Munroe, Robert, Ruth Munroe, and Beatrice Whiting. Handbook of Cross-Cultural Human
Development. New York: Garland STPM Press, 1980.
Overgaauw, Paul, et al. “Zoonotic parasites in fecal samples and fur from dogs and cats
in the Netherlands.” Vet Parasitology 163.1-2 (2009): 115-122.
Plaut, Marshall, Eugene Zimmerman, and Robert Goldstein. “Health hazards to
humans associated with domestic pets.” Annual Review of Public Health 17 (1996): 221-45.
Robin, Michael, and Robert Bensal. “Pets and the socialization of children.” Marriage and
Family Review 8.3-4 (1985): 63-78.
Serpell, James. “Pet-keeping and animal domestication: A reappraisal.” The Walking
Larder: Patterns of Domestication, Pastorialism and Predation. Ed. Juliet Clutton-Brock.
London: Unwin Hyman, 1989, 11-21.
Serpell, James, R. Coppinger, A. Fine, and J. Peralta. “Welfare considerations in therapy
and assistance animals.” Handbook on Animal-Assisted Therapy (Third Edition): Theoretical
Foundations and Guidelines for Practice. Ed. Aubrey Fine. London: Academic Press, 2010.
481-503.
Smith, Bradley. “‘The pet effect’: health related aspects of companion animal
ownership.” Australian Family Physician 41.6 (2012): 439-442.
Smith, Bradley, and Carla Litchfield. “A review of the relationship between Indigenous
Australians, dingoes (Canis dingo) and domestic dogs (Canis familiaris).” Anthrozoös
22.2 (2009): 111-128.
Smith, Bradley, Kirrilly Thompson, Larissa Clarkson and Drew Dawson. “The
prevalence and implications of human-animal co-sleeping in an Australian sample.”
Anthrozoös. In press.
Kirrilly Thompson and Bradley Smith — “Should We Let Sleeping Dogs Lie… With Us?”
127
Thompson, Kirrilly. “Save me, save my dog: Increasing natural disaster preparedness
and survival by addressing human-animal relationships.” Australian Journal of
Communication 40.1 (2013): 123-136.
Triebenbacher, Sandra. “Pets as transitional objects: Their role in children's emotional
development.” Psychological Reports 82.1 (1998): 191-200.
Voith, Victoria. “Attachment between people and their pets: behavior problems of pets
that arise from the relationship between pets and people.” Interrelations between People
and Pets. Ed. Bruce Fogle. Springfield, IL: Charles Thomas, 1981, 271-294.
Westgarth, Carrie, G. Pinchbeck, J. Bradshaw, S. Dawson, R. Gaskell, and R. Christley.
“Dog-human and dog-dog interactions of 260 dog-owning households in a community
in Cheshire.” Veterinary Record 162.14 (2008): 436-442.
Winefield, Helen, Anne Black, and Anne Chur-Hansen. “Heath effects of ownership of
and attachment to companion animals in an older population.” International Journal of
Behavioral Medicine 15 (2008): 303-310.
Winnicott, D.W. Playing and Reality. London: Tavistock, 1971.
Worthman, Carol, and Melissa Melby. “Towards a comparative developmental ecology
of human sleep.” Adolescent Sleep Patterns: Biological, Social and Psychological Influences.
Ed. Mary Carskadon. Cambridge: Cambridge UP, 2002. 69-117.
... The human is also deterritorialized such that human essence is deconstructed and effaced, and then the binary between humans and property is also effaced once human essence is gone. Put differently, in the emergent revolutions, one witnesses the deconstruction of the human in terms of effacing the essence of humans such that humans become indistinct from animals (Porcher 2014; Thompson and Smith 2014 There are even efforts to deterritorialize human sexuality through eco-sexuality and erobotic sexuality where humans are forced into a revolution that would see them preferring to have sex with erobots or humanoid sex robots than to have sex with biological humans (Ndung'u and Signe 2020; Zhang et al. 2009;Finley 2018, July 17;Kronfeldner, Roughley, and Toepfer 2014;Valera 2014;Schussler 2022). For Munro and Thanem (2017), capitalism is itself a deterritorializing force, extending itself over the surface of the earth, uprooting existing settlements and reshaping them in its own image. ...
... As companion animals, it can be argued that the human-canine bond could not become much stronger or closer; over half of American dogowning households reported co-sleeping with their dog(s) , despite more disruptions to human sleep (Patel et al. 2017;Smith et al. 2018;Hoffman et al. 2020). Similar prevalence of cosleeping with dogs is reported throughout Australia (Thompson and Smith 2014) and elsewhere. Furthermore, of all domesticated species, only dogs (and cats) require no physical barriers (e.g., cages, fences, and tethers) to enforce their close association with humans (Serpell 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
Synopsis Dogs (Canis familiaris) were the first domesticated species and, at an estimated population of 1 billion individuals, are globally ubiquitous today. Describing the tremendous morphometric diversity and evolutionary origins of dogs is a scientific endeavor that predates Darwin, yet our interdisciplinary understanding of the species is just beginning. Here, I present global trends in dog abundance, activity, and health. While the human–dog relationship has for millennia been close, it is also complicated. As pets, companion dogs are often treated as family members and constitute the largest sector of the ever-growing >$200 billion USD global pet care industry. As pests, free-roaming dogs are an emerging threat to native species via both predation and nonconsumptive effects (e.g., disturbance, competition for resources, and hybridization). Furthermore, I briefly discuss mounting evidence of dogs as not only infectious disease reservoirs but also as bridges for the transmission of pathogens between wild animals and humans in zoonotic spillover events, triggering intensive dog population management strategies such as culling. Dog mobility across the urban-wildland interface is an important driver for this and other adverse effects of canines on wildlife populations and is an active topic of disease ecologists and conservation biologists. Other canine scientists, including veterinary clinicians and physiologists, study more mechanistic aspects of dog mobility: the comparative kinetics, kinematics, and energetics of dog locomotor health. I outline the prevalent methodological approaches and breed-specific findings within dog activity and health research, then conclude by recognizing promising technologies that are bridging disciplinary gaps in canine science.
... Nearly half of dog and cat owners share their bed with their pet. 78,79 Although growingly commonplace, few studies have explored the connections between pet bed sharing and sleep quality 80 (Table 4). ...
Article
Background Bed sharing is common practice across the global population. However, the vast majority of research on bed sharing has focused solely on mother-infant bed sharing. Methods Here, we provide a holistic review of research on bed sharing. Articles investigating the relationship between bed sharing and sleep were identified in 4 dyad categories: (1) parent and child, (2) couples, (3) siblings, and (4) pet owners and pets. Of interest was whether sleep-promoting factors such as psychological comfort were generalizable across bed-sharing dyads; alternatively, sleep-demoting factors such as movement or heat may be commonalities. Results We found that, across dyad types, in general, subjective reports of sleep quality were better when bed sharing despite generally worse objective measures of sleep. Conclusions Understanding bed sharing is important to treating sleep disturbances, given the prevalence of shared beds. This scoping review points to critical gaps in our understanding of bed sharing that motivate future research.
... Sleeping is a vulnerable process and a sense of safety can assist in reducing the psychological arousal that can interfere with sleep onset and quality [2]. Research shows that co-sleeping with a pet encourages these feelings, which may have a positive influence on sleep quality levels [3,4]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Pet–owner co-sleeping is increasingly common in some parts of the world. Adult owners often subjectively report benefits of co-sleeping with pets, although objective actigraphy reports conversely indicate sleep disruptions due to the pet. Because limited research is available regarding pet–owner co-sleeping in non-adult samples, the aim of this two-part study was to explore whether co-sleeping improves sleep quality in adolescents, an age group in which poor sleep patterns are well documented. In Study One, an online survey with 265 pet-owning 13-to-17-year-old participants found that over 78% co-slept with their pet. Average sleep quality scores for co-sleepers and non-co-sleepers indicated generally poor sleep, with no differences in sleep quality depending on age, gender, or co-sleeping status. Study Two consisted of two preliminary case studies, using actigraphy on dog–adolescent co-sleepers. In both cases, high sleep concordance was observed, but owners again experienced generally poor sleep quality. Future actigraphy research is needed, including larger sample sizes and a control group of non-co-sleepers, to validate the preliminary findings from this study, but our limited evidence suggests that co-sleeping with a pet may not impact sleep quality in adolescents.
Article
Full-text available
This article explores the motifs of pet and play in the Alien franchise.It is grounded in biocultural theory and draws on play research fromanthropology, ethology, and linguistics (Huizinga, Burghardt, Bateson)and research in pets from animal studies and philosophy (Melson,Tuan, Fudge). The article develops three levels of play to discuss theaudience’s engagement: immersed play (play1), shifting in and outof a play frame (play2), and distanced looking-at-play (play3). Thepet function in the Alien series is performed by various beings, suchas the cat Jonesy and dog Spike, the Alien, the clone Ripley 8, andthe androids David and Walther. Pet and play have until now beenoverlooked in analyses of the franchise, but this article develops a newperspective that concludes with a reflection on the Alien as an animal"’good to think’" (Lévi-Strauss) with, in that the audience can use theAlien as play pivot in a game of pet domestication, domination, andmastery.
Article
Full-text available
This cross‑sectional study tested the direct and stress‑buffering effects of co‑sleeping with pets on human sleep characteristics in a nationally‑representative sample of United States adults. Participants completed questionnaires assessing their sleep characteristics, including perceived sleep quality, perceived sleep efficiency, insomnia severity, and multidimensional sleep health. We evaluated whether co‑sleeping with pets was associated with sleep characteristics and whether co‑sleeping with pets moderated the association of stress and sleep characteristics. Exploratory analyses examined whether sleep characteristics were impacted by number of pets, pet type, and bondedness to pets. Our final sample of 1591 participants (Mage = 46.4 years, SD = 17.5; 56% female; 76% White) included 758 participants who reported co‑sleeping with pets (47.6%). Co‑sleeping with pets was associated with poorer sleep characteristics—specifically, poorer perceived sleep quality and greater insomnia severity. Although higher levels of stress were associated with poorer sleep, we did not observe evidence for a stress‑buffering effect of co‑sleeping with pets. Exploratory analyses indicated that the negative impact of co‑sleeping with pets on human sleep was associated with dog ownership but not cat ownership, more pronounced when individuals own a greater number of pets, and not impacted by bondedness to pets. Our findings contribute to emerging evidence for the impact of co‑sleeping with pets on human sleep. Study was pre‑registered at: https://aspredicted.org/3VN_WF6.
Article
Full-text available
This narrative review describes the current state of the literature that has examined associations between non‐sexual social touch (i.e., affectionate touch, touch therapies, touch with animals and inanimate objects that mimic social touch) and sleep quality. It also highlights areas for future research to clarify the links and to identify underlying mechanisms. Most existing studies have focussed on and shown positive effects of touch therapies (e.g., massage, therapeutic touch) on sleep quality in clinical populations. Although there are fewer studies examining how other forms of social touch are linked with sleep quality, the existing research provides preliminary evidence supporting affectionate touch (e.g., hugging, skin‐to‐skin contact) and tactile contact with animals (e.g., dogs) and objects that mimic social touch (e.g., robots, weighted blankets) as predictors of better sleep quality, while touch deprivation and touch aversion are associated with worse sleep quality. Informed by the existing literature, we additionally reviewed potential relational‐cognitive (e.g., felt‐security) and neurobiological (e.g., oxytocin) mechanisms likely to underlie associations between social touch and sleep quality. Overall, current research supports associations between non‐sexual social touch and sleep quality. However, future research is needed to establish these links for specific forms of social touch (and in various populations), to test explanatory mechanisms, and to identify boundary conditions. Understanding associations between non‐sexual social touch and sleep quality can inform the development of touch‐based interventions to improve sleep quality and health.
Article
Full-text available
Human-animal co-sleeping is relatively common among dog owners; however, the nature of this practice is not well understood. Recent investigations have focused on the impact of human-dog co-sleeping on human sleep but have largely ignored the contextual nature of the practice, including with whom, why, and how people share their beds and bedrooms with their dogs. We explored the nature of human-dog co-sleeping among a large population of Australian dog owners ( n = 1136). Nearly half (49%) of participants reported sleeping with their dog in their bed, 20% indicated their dog slept in their bedroom but not in their bed, and 31% reported their dog slept outside their bedroom. The likelihood of bedsharing with one’s dog increased with participant age and bed size and was higher for individuals with small dogs than those with larger dogs. In addition, bedsharing with one’s dog was more common among individuals who did not have a human bed partner. For each unit increase in the MDORS Dog-Owner Interaction scale, the odds of sleeping with one’s dog increased by 1.39, and for each unit increase in the MDORS Emotional Closeness sub-scale, the odds increased by 1.08. For each unit increase in the MCPQ-R Motivation sub-scale, the odds of sleeping with one’s dog increased by 1.21.We found no association between whether the dog slept on the bed and self-reported sleep quality. However, participants whose dog slept somewhere other than their owner’s bed were 1.45 times more likely to report frequently waking up tired. Bedsharing appears unlikely to impact sleep quality negatively in any meaningful way. In fact, in many cases, dog(s) in the bed may facilitate a more restful night’s sleep than when they sleep elsewhere.
Article
Background: Pets are often thought to be detrimental to sleep. Up to 75% of households with children have a pet, and 30-50% of adults and children regularly share their bed with their pets. Despite these high rates, few studies have examined the effect of pet-human co-sleeping on pediatric sleep. This study compared subjective and objective sleep in youth who never, sometimes, or frequently co-slept with pets. Methods: Children (N = 188; aged 11-17 years; M = 13.25 years) and their parents answered standardized sleep questionnaires assessing timing, duration, onset latency, awakenings, and sleep quality. Children completed a home polysomnography (PSG) sleep study for one night and wore an actigraph for two weeks accompanied with daily sleep diary. Based on reported frequency of bedsharing with pets, children were stratified into three co-sleeping groups: never (65.4%), sometimes (16.5%), frequently (18.1%). Results: Overall, 34.6% of children reported co-sleeping with their pet sometimes or frequently. Results revealed largely identical sleep profiles across co-sleeping groups; findings were congruent across sleep measurement (subjective: child, parent report; objective: PSG, actigraphy). Effect sizes indicated that frequent co-sleepers had the highest overall subjective sleep quality, but longest PSG onset-latency compared to the sometimes group. Conclusions: Co-sleeping with pets was prevalent in one third of children. Sleep dimensions were similar regardless of how frequently children reported sharing their bed with their pet. Future research should examine dyadic measurement of co-sleepers to derive causal evidence to better inform sleep recommendations.
Chapter
Full-text available
There is a growing concern in relation to the problem of insufficient sleep, particularly in the United States. In the early 1990s a Congressionally mandated commission noted that insufficient sleep is a major contributor to catastrophic events, such as Chernobyl and the Exxon Valdez, as well as personal tragedies, such as automobile accidents. Adolescents appear to be among the most sleep-deprived populations in our society, though they are rarely included in sleep assessments. This book explores the genesis and development of sleep patterns in adolenscents. It examines biological and cultural factors that influence sleep patterns, presents risks associated with lack of sleep, and reveals the effects of environmental factors such as work and school schedules on sleep. Originally published in 2002, Adolescent Sleep Patterns will appeal to psychologists and sociologists of adolescence who have not yet considered the important role of sleep in the lives of our youth.
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter demonstrates how the use of therapy and assistance animals significantly enhances human health and well-being. It addresses whether this end morally justifies the means of achieving it. The goal is to re-examine the animal/human partnership from the animal's viewpoint to see what the benefits might be for the animal, or to see if the raising, training, and use of therapy and assistance animals is causing significant degradation in their welfare. Further welfare challenges arise when therapy and assistance animals begin to age. There are many potential sources of chronic stress in the lives of therapy and assistance animals. Trainers, practitioners, and end-users of these animals should be educated to recognize the warning signs and act accordingly. Recent advances in avian medicine, nutrition, and behavior reveal that most of these birds have highly specialized needs relating to air quality, nutrition, lighting, housing, sleep, and both environmental and social enrichment. The lifecycle of the typical assistance animal generally involves a series of relatively abrupt changes in its social and physical environment. Assistance animals are expected to obey complex commands and perform relatively challenging physical activities that also create a potential for welfare problems.
Article
Full-text available
Sleep research is characterized by an interest in humans, with the realm of animal sleep left largely to ethologists and animal scientists. However, the lives of sleep-study participants and those with sleep problems frequently involve animals. For the majority of the population in developed countries who own pets, their waking lives are impacted by the duties of animal care and ownership. For many, their sleeping lives are also impacted through sharing their bedrooms or their beds with pets. Yet, little is known about the prevalence of human–animal co-sleeping relationships or their impact on sleep. The aim of this study was to determine the prevalence and implications of human–animal co-sleeping in an Australian sample. The study uses data collected from the 2012 Sealy Sleep Census, a national online survey of sleep wellness that included a sample of 10,128 after data cleaning. The population of respondents (aged 18–74) who co-slept with pets (n = 1,018 or 10% of the sample) was then matched to a sample of respondents who did not co-sleep with pets, according to gender and age. Those who co-slept with pets took longer to fall asleep (p = 0.029), were more likely to wake up tired (p = 0.025), and although they were not more likely to wake up due to a disturbance, those who did had a greater chance of being disturbed by dog barking/animals making noises (p < 0.001). However, there were no significant differences found in total self-reported sleep length or feelings of tiredness during the day. The continued practice of co-sleeping with pets suggests that there may be some benefits such as social support and social interaction, and increased feelings of personal security. The survey provides a preliminary understanding of the prevalence and implications of human–animal co-sleeping, and highlights areas for further examination of its implications on sleep research and clinical practice.
Article
Full-text available
ABSTRACT: The willingness of people to risk their own lives during disasters to save those of animals has been well documented. Existing research characterises animals and pet ownership as risk factors for early evacuation from—and survival of—natural disasters. However, given high pet ownership levels in industrialised countries, this paper considers how animals might alternatively be reconfigured as protective factors. It offers some preliminary thoughts on how this might be achieved with innovative communication initiatives informed by post-structural approaches to human-animal relations. Specifically, the paper encourages communicators to take advantage of human-animal relations by addressing the human-animal relationship
Article
This introductory chapter provides readers not only with a basic foundation to appreciate and understand this unique kinship with all living creatures but also to discover the roots to the overwhelming growing interest in animal-assisted intervention (AAI). The chapter should also help solidify and clarify how the benefits witnessed within this unique bond have prompted numerous professionals to become more curious about the advantages of animal-assisted interventions. It is apparent that dogs have been bred to coexist with their human counterparts and have filled many roles including herding, guarding, hunting, fishing and being our best friend (Clutton-Brock, 1995). Dogs have also been widely used as service animals, supporting the quality of life of people in need. There have been increasing insights into science’s current understanding of dog behavior and cognition. Perhaps one of the strongest insights that she discusses pertains to dogs’ ability to understand our behaviors (Hare, 2007; Hare et al., 2002). Horowitz (2009) explains that dogs’
Article
This paper broadens out existing challenges to the divisions between the human and the animal that keep humans distinct, and apart, from other animals. Much attention to date has focused on how the Euro-American individuation of the human subject intensifies the asymmetries inculcated by these divisions. This paper rehearses some of this literature but goes on to attend to how these divisions undercut understandings of sociality and limit social organization to interaction between persons. Drawing together debates around the human/animal relation, the paper juxtaposes different perspectives of nature-cultures to bring 'worlds' of relations into view. Specifically, I distinguish here between the state of 'being alongside' and the process of 'being-with'. Ranging from approaches that try to settle ideas of difference through appeals to 'ethical health', through to work on identity that 'unconceals' a wealth of connection, this distinction will help to keep apart those situated moments of relations, where the constituent parts are left more provisional and contingent, from more sought-out relationships, where a sense of togetherness purposefully dominates the conjoining of activities. Contrasting hybridity as a totalizing form of 'being-with', with alongsideness, as a form of intermittent and partial connection, the analysis eschews the obfuscation of difference entrenched in contemporary emphasis on connectivity. Proposing instead the importance of creatures as 'division preserving', the paper theorizes ways to sustain regard for division as well as connection as key to understanding the arts of dwelling amidst different kinds.