ArticlePDF Available

Gender Differences in Reactions to the Sexualization of Athletes

Authors:

Abstract

Abstract Participants rated a series of photographs that depicted well-known male and female athletes in a sexualized fashion or not. In general, sexualized targets were perceived as more attractive but as less competent than non-sexualized targets, and for ratings of competence, sexualization effects were greater for female targets. Sexualization had no effects on women's perceptions of the competence of male targets, whereas for men, sexualization affected their perceptions of the competence of both male and female targets, although the influence of sexualization was stronger for female targets than it was for male targets. These findings suggest that the gender of the target and of the perceiver needs to be considered when evaluating the influences of sexualization. The implications of these findings are discussed with within the context of objectification theory, which has focused on the objectification of women by women.
This article was downloaded by: [College of William & Mary]
On: 03 March 2015, At: 06:01
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered
office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK
Click for updates
The Journal of Social Psychology
Publication details, including instructions for authors and
subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/vsoc20
Gender Differences in Reactions to the
Sexualization of Athletes
John B. Nezlekab, William Krohna, Dannon Wilsona & Laura Maruskinc
a College of William & Mary
b University of Social Sciences and Humanities
c University of California, Berkeley
Accepted author version posted online: 25 Sep 2014.Published
online: 04 Nov 2014.
To cite this article: John B. Nezlek, William Krohn, Dannon Wilson & Laura Maruskin (2015) Gender
Differences in Reactions to the Sexualization of Athletes, The Journal of Social Psychology, 155:1,
1-11, DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2014.959883
To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2014.959883
PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE
Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the
“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,
our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to
the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions
and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,
and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content
should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources
of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,
proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or
howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising
out of the use of the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any
substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,
systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &
Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-
and-conditions
Downloaded by [College of William & Mary] at 06:01 03 March 2015
The Journal of Social Psychology, 155: 1–11, 2015
Copyright © Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 0022-4545 print / 1940-1183 online
DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2014.959883
ARTICLES
Gender Differences in Reactions to the Sexualization
of Athletes
JOHN B. NEZLEK
College of William & Mary; and
University of Social Sciences and Humanities
WILLIAM KROHN
DANNON WILSON
College of William & Mary
LAURA MARUSKIN
University of California, Berkeley
ABSTRACT. Participants rated a series of photographs that depicted well-known male and female
athletes in a sexualized fashion or not. In general, sexualized targets were perceived as more attrac-
tive but as less competent than non-sexualized targets, and for ratings of competence, sexualization
effects were greater for female targets. Sexualization had no effects on women’s perceptions of the
competence of male targets, whereas for men, sexualization affected their perceptions of the compe-
tence of both male and female targets, although the influence of sexualization was stronger for female
targets than it was for male targets. These findings suggest that the gender of the target and of the
perceiver needs to be considered when evaluating the influences of sexualization. The implications
of these findings are discussed within the context of objectification theory, which has focused on the
objectification of women by women.
Keywords: advertising, objectification, sex differences, sexualization
SOCIAL SCIENTISTS HAVE LONG BEEN CONCERNED about the impact of media images
on people, particularly young people who are presumed to be more likely than adults to be in
some type of identity formation process. There are numerous themes in this research, and the
Address correspondence to John B. Nezlek, College of William & Mary, Department of Psychology, Box 8795,
Williamsburg, VA 23187-8795, USA. E-mail: jbnezl@wm.edu
Downloaded by [College of William & Mary] at 06:01 03 March 2015
2THE JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
present paper focuses on the combination of two of the more common elements in mass adver-
tising: sexuality and using images of athletes in advertising. We examined the influence of the
sexualization of athletes on perceptions of these athletes within the context of what is generally
known as Objectification Theory (e.g., Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997).
Although sexualization has been studied in various contexts, we were interested in the impli-
cations of the sexualization of athletes. Athletes are commonly used in advertising, so often
that a recent study specifically targeted star athletes’ endorsements of junk food products
as an important contributor to increases in childhood obesity (Bragg, Yanamadala, Roberto,
Harris, & Brownell, 2013). Moreover, we were specifically interested in the possible effects that
sexualization of male athletes would have on perceptions of these athletes, and we were interested
in the reactions of both men and women to these images.
Most of the existing research on the impact of the sexualization of mass media images has
concerned the sexualization of women, and very little research has compared the impact of sex-
ualizing men and women (e.g., Moradi & Huang, 2008). Moreover, no research has specifically
examined gender differences in the effects of the sexualization of male and female athletes. For
example, Gurung and Chrouser (2007) found that the provocative dress of female athletes led
them to be sexualized but did not include either male athletes or male participants.
The present study was designed to complement existing research by examining gender differ-
ences in the effects of the sexualization of male and female athletes. Participants in the present
study viewed pictures of prominent male or female athletes, and the pictures were either sexu-
alized or not. To ensure that the type of sexualization we examined was the same as that which
might occur in contemporary media, we used images that had appeared in the popular media.
Based on previous research and theory, we expected that sexualizing both male and female ath-
letes would lead to objectification but that this effect would be stronger for female athletes. Within
the present context, objectification would occur when an individual’s capabilities are presumed
to be diminished when that individual is perceived primarily as a sexual object rather than as a
person.
Objectification Theory
Although our study concerned sexualized images per se, the context within which it was devel-
oped and is grounded is typically referred to as Objectification Theory. Objectification occurs
when individuals focus on a person’s physical characteristics (e.g., attractiveness) and neglect
a person’s psychological characteristics (e.g., intelligence). This leads to (or represents the
fact that) people are perceived as objects, merely physical bodies, rather than as complete
persons (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). Within this context, sexualization, defined as an inten-
tional heightened emphasis on the physical, sexual characteristics of an individual, can lead to
objectification.
Fredrickson and Roberts suggest that objectification can occur within interpersonal encoun-
ters and through visual mass media, and they discuss an important, possible consequence of
objectification, self-objectification. Self-objectification refers to the tendency for people to per-
ceive and value their bodies from a third-person perspective by focusing on physical and
observable attributes (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). Self-objectification can occur when peo-
ple think that they are being viewed in a sexually objectifying manner, and then they internalize
the observer’s perspective. They begin to think of themselves less as people and more as objects.
Downloaded by [College of William & Mary] at 06:01 03 March 2015
NEZLEK ET AL. 3
One of the most important aspects of Objectification Theory is that in contemporary soci-
ety women are objectified more often than men. The plethora of media images of attractive
female bodies in movies, magazines, advertisements, and so forth increases the likelihood of
objectification (Gurung & Chrouser, 2007). For example, print media tend to focus on the head
and face of male images whereas the body is the primary focus for female images (Fredrickson &
Roberts, 1997). As discussed by Fredrickson and Roberts (1997), gazing, or inspecting the body,
is a form of sexualization that can lead to sexual objectification.
Provocative Dress and Athletes in the Media
In terms of the focus of the present study, research suggests that women who present themselves
provocatively are perceived by men to be more flirtatious, seductive, and promiscuous than other
women (Koukounas & Letch, 2001). Furthermore, although there may be a positive relationship
between amount of exposed skin and perceptions of sexuality for women, research suggests that
the less clothed a woman is, the less capable and intelligent she is thought to be (Gurung &
Chrouser, 2007). Gurung and Chrouser (2007) suggest that provocatively dressed women are
perceived as less intelligent because of the repeated depiction in the media of unintelligent yet
attractive and provocatively dressed women, such as images of women in advertisements. They
suggest that provocatively dressed women activate stereotypes that downplay personal attributes
at the expense of physical attributes.
We think that such processes are particularly salient when considering media images of ath-
letes. Images of professional athletes pervade the mass media. As a result, professional athletes,
and perhaps female athletes in particular, can become valued by society for their physical appear-
ance rather than their athletic ability. For example, research suggests that media coverage of
female athletes tends to sexualize them (Daniels, 2009). Knight and Giuliano (2001) posit that
references to the attractiveness, femininity, and emotionality of female athletes in media coverage
make their stereotypical gender role more salient than their athletic role.
The media represent female athletes as women first and athletes second, whereas male ath-
letes are portrayed in terms of athleticism, a gender-role consistent schema (Knight & Giuliano,
2001). For example, a television announcer might discuss the number of children a female athlete
has but will not discuss the family status of a male athlete. Additionally, female athletes can con-
tribute to their own sexualization by posing provocatively for national magazines (Daniels, 2009).
This focus on physical appearance may come at the expense of psychological attributes, such as
determination, and may contribute to objectification (Gurung & Chrouser, 2007). In sum, the
mass media strengthens gender-role stereotypes and encourages sexualization and objectification
of athletes (e.g., Daniels, 2009). Moreover, it appears that the mass media emphasize female
athletes’ attractiveness, whereas for male athletes, athletic ability is emphasized (Fredrickson &
Roberts, 1997).
METHOD
Participants
Participants were 59 undergraduates, 30 women and 29 men, who volunteered for a study in
fulfillment of an introductory psychology class requirement. Students were at a public university
Downloaded by [College of William & Mary] at 06:01 03 March 2015
4THE JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
in the United States. We did not record the ages of participants but the mean age of the students
in the courses which these students were taking was 18.23 (SD =3.09, min =18, max =26).
Procedure
Participants volunteered for a study about person perception. Participants completed the study in
same-sex groups of four. After participants provided informed consent, the experimenter read the
instructions, which were to complete background measures and to rate a series of photographs.
The purpose of the study was described as examination of the impressions people form of others
based upon photographs. The photographs were concealed so that participants were not aware
of the photographs they would be rating while they completed the other measures. During the
study, an experimenter remained in the room and answered participants’ questions. Following
this, participants were debriefed, and none offered comments that suggested that their data should
be excluded.
Photographs and Ratings
Each participant was randomly assigned to either the sexualized or non-sexualized condition.
In each condition, participants viewed and rated four images of professional athletes, two men
and two women. In the non-sexualized condition, all images depicted the target in his or her
athletic role. In the sexualized condition each participant viewed the same athletes, but the target
was dressed in a sexually provocative fashion.
The images were taken from popular magazines and were presented in random order. Each
image was printed by a high quality color printer onto 6” ×8” glossy paper. Images of the
following athletes were used: professional football player Tom Brady, professional soccer player
David Beckham, professional racecar driver Danica Patrick, and professional swimmer Amanda
Beard.
In the non-sexualized condition, the images of Tom Brady and David Beckham depicted them
on the field in an action shot. The image of Danica Patrick depicted her on the podium holding a
trophy after a win. The image of Amanda Beard depicted her on the starting block for a race. In the
sexualized condition, Tom Brady was shown seated with a widely opened shirt. David Beckham
was lying across a bed with his shirt open and with only a pair of briefs on. Amanda Beard was
shown in bikini bottoms, seductively lifting her shirt to expose the majority of her midsection.
Danica Patrick was shown in a short tank top and bikini bottoms bent over a sports car.
In each condition the name and profession of the athlete were written next to the picture.
Participants rated the person depicted in each picture on eight attributes, based on those used by
Gurung and Chrouser (2007). These were attractiveness, desirability, sexuality, strength, determi-
nation, capableness, self-respect, and intelligence. The eight attributes were divided into two main
categories: sexualization and competence/ability. The sexualization category was composed of
three attributes (sexual, desirable, and attractive) that assessed the extent to which the target was
seen as a sexual object. The second category was composed of five attributes (strength, deter-
mined, capable, intelligent, and self-respecting) that focused on the individual’s capabilities and
personal characteristics. Participants rated the pictures using 9-point scales with endpoints labeled
“not at all” and “extremely.”
Downloaded by [College of William & Mary] at 06:01 03 March 2015
NEZLEK ET AL. 5
RESULTS
There were 14 male participants in the not sexualized image condition and 15 in the sexualized
image condition. There were 14 female participants in the not sexualized image condition and
16 in the sexualized image condition. To simplify the presentation of these analyses, we dis-
cuss only those effects that involved sexualization.1Finally, although we chose our dependent
measures to represent two categories (sexualization and competence/ability), given the lack of
existing research concerning the conceptual structure underlying such judgments, we analyzed
individual items.
The primary analyses were 2 (participant gender) ×2 (gender of target) ×2 (sexualized or
not) ANOVA, with participant gender and sexualization as between-subjects factors and target
gender as a within-subjects factor. Each participant rated two photographs of male targets and
two photographs of female targets, and we analyzed mean ratings for the two male targets and for
the two female targets. Given that the primary focus of the study was sexualization, in explaining
the results (particularly when explaining interactions) we rely upon what we refer to as the sex-
ualization effect (SE), defined as difference between the ratings for a non-sexualized target and
the corresponding sexualized target. In the terminology of ANOVA, differences in the strength
of sexualization effects can be described in terms of the interaction of sexualization with other
factors in the design, under the assumption that interactions can be understood as tests of the dif-
ference of differences. We also present the results of planned pairwise comparisons of testing the
differences between pairs representing the sexualization effect, separately for male and female
participants and male and female targets.
Perceptions of Attractiveness/Sexualization
The three measures of how attractive a target was seen to be—attractiveness, desirability, and
sexuality—were not the focus of any hypotheses per se; they functioned primarily as manip-
ulation checks. Consistent with this conceptualization, there were significant main effects for
sexualization in the analyses of these three variables. Sexualized targets were seen as more attrac-
tive, desirable, and sexual. The results of significance tests and effect sizes (partial eta-squared)
and the means for these main effects are presented in Table 1. This table also contains the MSerror
at both the between and within subjects levels from each analysis.
Although not the focus of our hypotheses, we examined the fact that some of these main
effects were qualified by higher order interactions. The important pattern from these analyses
was a significant three-way interaction of participant gender, target gender, and sexualization in
the analyses of attractiveness and desirability (Fs(1,55) =8.43, 8.50; ps<.01; ηp2=.133, .134).
The means for these analyses are presented in Table 2.
These three-way interactions were due to the fact that the effects of sexualizing images varied
as a joint function of participant and target gender. Broadly speaking, the effect of sexualization
for female targets was greater for men than for women. Follow-up analyses found that for men,
the sexualization effect was greater for female targets than for male targets for both attractiveness
and desirability (Fs(1,27) =9.25, 16.73; ps <.01, ηp2=.255, .383). This interpretation is based
upon the significant sexualization effects for female targets and the lack of sexualization effects
for male targets. In contrast, for women, the sexualization effect did not vary between male and
female targets for either of these measures (Fs <1).
Downloaded by [College of William & Mary] at 06:01 03 March 2015
6THE JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
TABLE 1
Main Effects of Sexualization
Trait Not Sexualized Sexualized F-ratio Effect size Betw-MSerror With-MSerror
Attractive 5.94 7.31 14.1 ηp2=.203 3.95 1.39
Desirable 5.49 7.28 16.5 ηp2=.231 5.66 1.25
Sexual 5.14 7.95 65.1 ηp2=.542 3.56 1.07
Intelligent 5.71 4.31 12.7 ηp2=.187 4.57 .98
Self-respect 6.50 4.50 31.5 ηp2=.364 3.76 1.25
Strength 7.37 5.84 31.9 ηp2=.367 2.17 1.27
Capable 7.51 5.69 33.4 ηp2=.378 2.94 1.90
Determined 7.78 5.49 47.7 ηp2=.465 3.21 1.10
Note: All ps<.001.
TABLE 2
Perceptions of Attractiveness
Male participants
Male targets Female targets
Rating Not sex Sex SE Not sex Sex SE
Attractive 6.21 6.77 0.56 5.32 8.00 2.68∗∗
Desirable 5.96 6.47 0.51 5.00 8.23 3.23∗∗
Sexual 5.61 7.13 1.524.93 8.50 3.57∗∗
Female participants
Male targets Female targets
Rating Not sex Sex SE Not sex Sex SE
Attractive 6.64 7.97 1.335.57 6.50 0.93
Desirable 5.93 7.47 1.545.07 6.94 1.87∗∗
Sexual 5.71 7.78 2.07∗∗ 4.29 8.38 4.09∗∗
Note: For test of the sexualization effect: p<.05, ∗∗p<01, ap<.10.
Perceptions of Competence and Ability
We performed a similar set of analyses on our five measures of target competence/ability: intelli-
gence, self-respect, strength, capability, and determination. Consistent with objectification theory,
sexualized targets were also seen as less intelligent, strong, capable, determined, and as having
less self-respect. The means for these main effects and the results of significant tests are presented
in Table 1.
All of these main effects were qualified by a significant interaction of target gender and sexu-
alization, and the results of these significance tests are presented in Table 3. As can be seen from
the means and sexualization effects presented in Table 4, consistent with our expectations, this
Downloaded by [College of William & Mary] at 06:01 03 March 2015
NEZLEK ET AL. 7
TABLE 3
Interaction of Target Gender and Sexualization: Analyses of Competence/Ability
Rating F-ratio Effect size
Intelligent 32.7∗∗ ηp2=.373
Self-respect 22.9∗∗ ηp2=.295
Strength 24.6∗∗ ηp2=.309
Capable 5.33ηp2=.088
Determined 20.14∗∗ ηp2=.268
Note: p<.05, ∗∗p<.01.
TABLE 4
Perceptions of Competence and Ability
Male participants
Male targets Female targets
Rating Not sex Sex SE Not sex Sex SE
Intelligent 5.57 4.77 0.80 5.82 4.07 1.75∗∗
Self-respect 6.75 5.20 1.556.29 4.37 1.92∗∗
Strength 7.71 6.77 0.94a6.82 4.33 2.49∗∗
Capable 7.86 5.83 2.03∗∗ 7.57 4.97 2.60∗∗
Determined 7.82 6.00 1.82∗∗ 7.79 4.90 2.89∗∗
Female participants
Male targets Female targets
Rating Not sex Sex SE Not sex Sex SE
Intelligent 5.04 5.13 0.09 6.39 3.25 3.14∗∗
Self-respect 6.14 5.66 0.48 6.82 2.75 4.07∗∗
Strength 7.68 7.63 0.05 7.25 4.61 2.64∗∗
Capable 7.11 6.66 0.45 7.50 5.28 2.22∗∗
Determined 7.64 6.63 1.01a7.86 4.44 3.42∗∗
Note: For test of the sexualization effect: p<.05, ∗∗p<.01, ap<.10.
interaction was due to the fact that the sexualization effect was larger for female targets than it
was for male targets. Collapsed across participant gender, the sexualization effects for female and
male and male targets were, respectively: intelligent, –2.45 vs. .45; self-respect, –4.07 vs. –1.02,
strength, –2.57 vs. –.50; capable, –2.41 vs. –1.24; and determined, –3.16 vs. –1.42.
There were also significant three-way interactions of participant gender, target gender, and sex-
ualization in the analyses of intelligence and self-respect (Fs(1,55) =9.73, and 15.21; ps<.01;
ηp2=.150, .217). For both measures, the difference between the sexualization effects for male
and female targets was smaller for men than it was for women (self-respect, .37 vs. 3.59; intelli-
gence, .90 vs. 3.05). It should also be noted that for self-respect, the sexualization effect was not
significant when women rated male targets, whereas it was when men rated male targets.
Downloaded by [College of William & Mary] at 06:01 03 March 2015
8THE JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
DISCUSSION
Consistent with our expectations, our results suggest that both male and female athletes can be
objectified through the type of clothing they wear. When both male and female athletes were
depicted in sexualized fashion through provocative dress, they were seen as more attractive, but
also as weaker, less capable, less determined, less intelligent, and having less self-respect than
when they were not depicted in a sexualized fashion. Moreover, female athletes were objecti-
fied more than male athletes were objectified. In particular, women objectified female athletes,
whereas they did not objectify male athletes, whereas men objectified both male and female
targets, albeit more so for female targets.
Implications for Objectification Theory
The results of the present study are consistent with various aspects of Objectification Theory
(e.g., Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). According to the theory, sexually provocative dress calls
attention to the physical body or physical features of a person, leading to a de-emphasis on other
personal characteristics, such as performance, and we found such effects. In turn, this emphasis on
physical features provides a basis for seeing an individual more as an object than as a person—i.e.,
objectification occurs.
Research and scholarship on Objectification Theory has focused much more on women, both
as targets and in terms of the impact of objectification on self-processes (e.g., self-evaluation)
than on men (e.g., Moradi & Huang, 2008). The present study was not designed to exam-
ine changes in self-objectification, and it is not likely that the brief exposure to a series of
photographs of sexualized athletes that occurred in the present study influenced participants’ self-
objectification. Nevertheless, an important potential precursor or cause of self-objectification is
exposure to objectified images, and within this context, the present results suggest that although
a greater emphasis on objectification among women versus among men is probably justified,
objectification among men should also be considered.
Consistent with the emphasis on women in research and scholarship on Objectification Theory,
we found that although sexualizing an image led to decreases in the perceived competence and
ability of both male and female targets, this effect was somewhat stronger for female targets.
Moreover, considering only the reactions of female participants (as is the case in many studies of
objectification that have only women as participants), would lead to the conclusion that men are
not objectified. There were no significant sexualization effects when women rated male targets,
although one rating, determined, was significant at p<.10.
Nevertheless, the results of our study involving male participants are somewhat inconsistent
with the emphasis on women of existing research and scholarship on Objectification Theory.
Assuming that same-sex targets are the most relevant for the types of processes with which
Objectification Theory is concerned, we found that sexualizing a male target led men to objectify
that target. The sexualization effect was significant for three of the five measures (self-respect,
capable and determined), and it was marginally significant (p<.10) for strength.
Admittedly, objectifying media images may not necessarily lead to self-objectification, which
is the negative outcome with which many are concerned. Other factors, such as broad social
norms, may come into play. Regardless, the present results suggest that sexualized images
Downloaded by [College of William & Mary] at 06:01 03 March 2015
NEZLEK ET AL. 9
of men may have similar implications for self-processes among men that sexualized images
of women have for the self-processes of women, a possibility that is consistent with some
existing research (e.g., Vandenbosch & Eggermont, 2013). Moreover, there is increasing aware-
ness that self-objectification occurs in men and may be a precursor to other problems (e.g.,
Strelan & Hargreaves, 2005). Clearly, such possibilities require research explicitly designed to
understand them.
Implications for Advertising
In addition to possible implications for understanding objectification, although the present study
is limited in scope, the present results may also have implications for advertising. Athletes are role
models, particularly for young people (e.g., Bush, Martin, & Bush, 2004), and they are commonly
used in advertising. The present results suggest that how they are portrayed in advertising may
have implications for their effectiveness.
Star athletes, such as those we portrayed in the present study, are used in advertisements
because they are successful—strong, capable, determined, and so forth. Yet, when athletes are
sexualized, we found that these positive attributes are diminished, particularly for female ath-
letes. Such a negative effect has been found in other studies. For example, Antil, Burton, and
Robinson (2012) found that sexually focused advertisements featuring women athletes were
viewed less positively by women than non-sexually focused advertisements. See Bushman (2007)
for a discussion of the potential negative effects of sexuality (and violence) on the effectiveness
of advertisements.
As noted by Gurung and Chrouser (2007), much of the research on objectification has
concerned the impact of self-objectification, and assuming that objectifying others leads to self-
objectification, more needs to be known about the circumstances that lead people to objectify
others. Moreover, it appears that how men and women are portrayed in the media (broadly
defined) may be a particularly important influence on self-objectification. As media exposure
increases, and the availability of images increases through the use of various types of devices
(e.g., handheld devices that can display movies), it would seem to be particularly important to
understand how such images are interpreted and evaluated.
Assuming sexually focused media images engender self-objectification (regardless if it is more
among women than men), raises interesting questions for society at large. “Sex sells” is a tru-
ism among advertisers, and many athletes enjoy international reputations and appeal particularly
to younger segments of the market. From a marketer’s perspective, sexy athletes combine two
very powerful characteristics and make a good “package.” For example, since 2009, ESPN–The
Magazine has featured a “body issue” that contains pictures of nude athletes.
The exposure to the images in our study was (by design) too brief to create changes in trait
levels of self-objectification, but it is quite possible that repeated exposure to such images does
change such characteristics. Although sexy athletes may be a boon for advertising campaigns,
they may have negative consequences for the psychological development of young people, partic-
ularly it seems, for women. As a society, we should consider the possible negative consequences
(however unintended) of exposing people to images that attract their attention while undermining
their perception of people (themselves included) as individuals rather than as objects. See Grabe
and Hyde (2009) for a study supporting this possibility.
Downloaded by [College of William & Mary] at 06:01 03 March 2015
10 THE JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
Limitations and Future Research
As is the case with any individual study, ours has its limitations. Similar to much of the research
on this topic, our participants were collegians, and they were collegians living in a western, indus-
trialized, society. It would be interesting to determine if the types of objectification effects we
found characterized individuals who were in a different stage of their lives or living in a different
culture. For example, more mature adults might not be influenced as much by media portrayals
as younger adults, who might be more involved with contemporary media.
In addition, we examined the objectification of athletes only. Although athletes are prominent
in contemporary media, individuals from all walks of life appear in contemporary media. We can-
not be certain if the objectification effects we found would occur if another occupation or social
role were examined, for example, news commentators. Also, we examined the effects of sexu-
alizing four specific individuals. Although we have no reason to suspect that there was anything
idiosyncratic about the athletes we used as target stimuli that qualified our results, other athletes
may have led to different results. Finally, objectification can occur through various means, and
we examined objectification specifically as engendered by provocative dress depicted visually.
Would verbal descriptions have similar effects?
Regardless, our study meaningfully extends existing research on the effects of sexualizing
media images. Our results suggest that men and women objectify both same- and opposite-sex
others, albeit to different degrees. At the least, our results suggest that future researchers may
want to consider expanding the scope of their studies to include men and women in all aspects of
their studies.
NOTE
1. Although it was possible to compare evaluations within the sexualized and non-sexualized conditions (e.g., sexualized
males vs. sexualized females), we did not. Given that we had only two male and two female images (arbitrarily
selected), if we had found differences between the ratings of male and female targets, we could not be certain if such
differences reflected gender differences per se or if they were differences due to the specific targets we chose (i.e.,
the individual athletes). The analyses on which we focused (i.e., differences between sexualized and non-sexualized
images) controlled for whatever differences existed among our targets per se.
AUTHOR NOTES
John B. Nezlek is affiliated with the Department of Psychology, College of William & Mary, and with the University
of Social Sciences and Humanities, Faculty in Poznan. William Krohn is affiliated with the Department of Psychology,
College of William & Mary. Dannon Wilson is affiliated with the Department of Psychology, College of William &
Mary. Laura Maruskin is affiliated with the Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley.
REFERENCES
Antil, J. H., Burton, R., & Robinson, M. (2012). Exploring the challenges facing female athletes as endorsers. Journal of
Brand Strategy,1, 292–307.
Bragg, M. A., Yanamadala, S., Roberto, C. A., Harris, J. L. & Brownell, K. D. (2013). Athlete endorsements in food
marketing. Pediatrics,132, 1–6. doi:10.1542/peds.2013-0093
Downloaded by [College of William & Mary] at 06:01 03 March 2015
NEZLEK ET AL. 11
Bush, A. J., Martin, C. A., & Bush, V. D. (2004). Sports celebrity influence on the behavioral intentions of Generation Y.
Journal of Advertising Research,44(1), 108–118. doi:10.1017/S0021849904040206
Bushman, B. J. (2007). That was a great commercial, but what were they selling? Effects of violence and sex on memory
for products in television commercials. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,37, 1784–1796.
Daniels, E. A. (2009). Sex objects, athletes, and sexy athletes: How media representations of women athletes can impact
adolescent girls and college women. Journal of Adolescent Research,24, 399–422. doi:10.1177/0743558409336748
Fredrickson, B. L., & Roberts, T. A. (1997). Objectification theory: Toward understanding women’s lived experiences
and mental health risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly,21, 173–206. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1997.tb00108.x
Grabe, S., & Hyde, J. S. (2009). Body objectification, MTV, and psychological outcomes among female adolescents.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology,39, 2840–2858. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2009.00552.x
Gurung, R. A. R., & Chrouser, C. J. (2007). Predicting objectification: Do provocative clothing and observer
characteristics matter? Sex Roles,57, 91–99. doi:10.1007/s11199-007-9219-z
Knight, J. L., & Giuliano, T. A. (2001). He’s a Laker; She’s a “Looker”: The consequences of gender-stereotypical
portrayals of male and female athletes by the print media. Sex Roles,45, 217–229. doi:10.1023/A:1013553811620
Koukounas, E., & Letch, N. M. (2001). Psychological correlates of perception of sexual intent in women. The Journal of
Social Psychology,141, 443–456. doi:10.1080/00224540109600564
Moradi, B., & Huang, Y. P. (2008). Objectification theory and psychology of women: A decade of advances and future
directions. Psychology of Women Quarterly,32, 377–398. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.2008.00452.x
Strelan, P., & Hargreaves, D. (2005). Reasons for exercise and body esteem: Men’s responses to self-objectification. Sex
Roles,53, 495–503. doi:10.1007/s11199-005-7137-5
Vandenbosch, L., & Eggermont, S. (2013). Sexualization of adolescent boys: Media exposure and boys’ internal-
ization of appearance ideals, self-objectification, and body surveillance. Men and Masculinities,16, 283–306.
doi:10.1177/1097184X13477866
Received June 24, 2013
Accepted August 12, 2014
Downloaded by [College of William & Mary] at 06:01 03 March 2015
... Objectification is defined as the emphasis on a person's physical aspects and sexual characteristics while neglecting their skills and physiological characteristics (Nezlek et al. 2015). While the term objectification suggests a lack of agency of the objectified person, it has been argued that athletes consent to some degree of objectification and sexualization by the media when they pose in provocative ways in front of cameras (Daniels 2009). ...
... However, they also noted that men's appearance concern has grown over time and that males with negative body image who have a negative body image also tend to socially compare themselves to females (Fox and Vendemia 2016). Nezlek et al. (2015) noted that the media's sexualization decreased the perceived athletic competence of both male and female athletes. Overall, evidence suggests that the gap between both genders' appearance concerns is narrowing. ...
... Our findings show that homophobia plays a role in the objectification of male and female athletes. While there is a body of literature on how erotic images impact the effectiveness of advertisement, including findings that sexualized images of women in advertising trigger the attention of male consumers (Nezlek et al. 2015;Greenwell et al. 2017), we do not know how homophobia impacts this relationship. Our study suggests that the mediating effect of homophobia may play out differently depending on when nude men are depicted in the advertisement or on endorsed online profiles. ...
... Furthermore, the male targets in the limited extant literature are generally White or the race/ethnicity of the target is not reported (for an exception see Gray et al., 2011;Study 3). The existing work has found that sexualized or sexually objectified men or boys are regarded less positively, for example, as less competent, less intelligent, less capable, having fewer mental states, less determined, less agentic, being less fully human, having less self-respect, and being less worthy of moral concern, compared to non-sexualized or non-sexually objectified men or boys (Daniels, 2020;Daniels & Linder, 2021;Fasoli et al., 2018;Gray et al., 2011;Loughnan et al., 2010Loughnan et al., , 2015Nezlek et al., 2015;Pacilli et al., 2019). Some work has also found that sexualized men are perceived to be more sexual, attractive, and desirable than non-sexualized men (Daniels & Linder, 2021;Nezlek et al., 2015). ...
... The existing work has found that sexualized or sexually objectified men or boys are regarded less positively, for example, as less competent, less intelligent, less capable, having fewer mental states, less determined, less agentic, being less fully human, having less self-respect, and being less worthy of moral concern, compared to non-sexualized or non-sexually objectified men or boys (Daniels, 2020;Daniels & Linder, 2021;Fasoli et al., 2018;Gray et al., 2011;Loughnan et al., 2010Loughnan et al., , 2015Nezlek et al., 2015;Pacilli et al., 2019). Some work has also found that sexualized men are perceived to be more sexual, attractive, and desirable than non-sexualized men (Daniels & Linder, 2021;Nezlek et al., 2015). These patterns are similar to those found with sexualized female targets (for a review, see Ward, 2016). ...
... The present findings indicate that a man's sexualized self-presentation on a dating profile, even on an app known as a site for hook-ups, elicits diminished perceptions of his competence and greater perceptions that he engages in risky sexual behavior. The competence findings are consistent with prior research showing that sexualized or sexually objectified men are perceived to be less competent than nonsexualized or non-objectified men (Daniels, 2020;Daniels & Linder, 2021;Fasoli et al., 2018;Loughnan et al., 2010;Nezlek et al., 2015). These patterns demonstrate that participants considered the sexualized male target to be a sexual object with diminished competence, consistent with the tenets of objectification theory (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997) and sexualization (APA, 2007). ...
Article
Full-text available
Very little is known about viewers’ attitudes toward sexualized portrayals of men on social media platforms. Using an experimental methodology, the present study investigated the effect of a sexualized versus non-sexualized Tinder profile of a young White man on college students’ perceptions of the profile owner. The impact of multiple aspects of sexualization including a self-sexualizing self-description on one’s profile (i.e., interested in casual sex) as well as a sexualizing appearance (i.e., muscular and shirtless) were investigated. U.S. college students (N = 567) viewed a mock Tinder profile and then rated the target’s competence, social appeal, physical attractiveness, and likelihood of engaging in risky sexual behavior. Findings indicate a young White man with a sexualized self-presentation on a dating app may be perceived as less competent and as a sexual risk-taker. These results contribute to the small body of research documenting the impact of self-sexualization on a dating app profile.
... To our knowledge, there has been only one small-scale study (n = 29 men; n = 30 women) of attitudes toward sexualized male athletes (Nezlek et al., 2015). In this study, U.S. football player, Tom Brady, and UK soccer player, David Beckham, were depicted in a sexualized (seated with shirt open and shirt open lying across a bed in underwear briefs, respectively) or non-sexualized (in action shots on the field) manner. ...
... The present study expands the research literature on attitudes towards sexualized targets to include male athlete targets. More specifically, we improved on limitations in prior research on attitudes toward male athletes (Nezlek et al., 2015) by using a larger sample, including adolescent boys as well as college men, and using less famous and noncontroversial male athletes for the experimental stimuli, thereby improving the external and internal validity of our study. Participants in the present study viewed either sexualized (i.e., skin exposed, sexual pose) or performance (i.e., in uniform and in action poses on the field/court) images of male athletes. ...
... Similar to research on explicit attitudes towards sexualized/objectified men (Daniels, 2020;Fasoli et al., 2018;Gray et al., 2011;Loughnan et al., 2010Loughnan et al., , 2015 and sexualized athletes (Nezlek et al., 2015), male viewers in the present study evaluated the competence of the sexualized male athletes less positively and held them in lower esteem as compared to the performance athletes. Consistent with prior research (Nezlek et al., 2015), sexualized athletes were also rated as higher in sexual appeal than performance athletes. ...
Article
Full-text available
Using a between-participants experimental paradigm, the present study investigated U.S. adolescent boys’ (n = 96) and college men’s (n = 194) attitudes toward media images of male athletes as well as their thoughts about the athletes. Participants viewed either sexualized (i.e., skin exposed, sexual pose) or performance (i.e., in uniform and in action poses on the field/court) images of male athletes. They then rated the athletes’ competence, esteem, and sexual appeal and completed a writing task about their reactions to the images. Male viewers rated the sexualized athletes lower in competence and esteem and higher in sexual appeal compared to the performance athletes. These results have implications for advocacy efforts calling for less sexualization in today’s media environment.
... Finally, sport media exploit sexualization built in to some women's sports, such as beach volleyball in which players are required to wear skimpy uniforms by sport governing bodies, by zeroing in on female athletes' buttock in their photographs and adding sexualized captions, such as "buns of gold, silver, and bronze" (Bissell & Duke, 2007;Sailors, Teetzel, & Weaving, 2012, p. 469). Sexualized depictions of female athletes distract viewers from focusing on their athleticism (Daniels, 2012;Daniels & Wartena, 2011) and diminish viewers' beliefs about their competence (Gurung & Chrouser, 2007;Nezlek, Krohn, Wilson, & Maruskin, 2015). The negative effects of sexualized portrayals on viewers, including girls, are described next. ...
... Finally, some researchers have examined media consumers' perceptions of athletes' personal attributes (e.g., intelligence, attractiveness, sport competence) based on whether they are depicted in a sexualized versus non-sexualized manner. These studies consistently find that sexualized female athletes are perceived to be more attractive, desirable, and sexual, but less capable, less intelligent, less self-respecting, and less athletically skilled than non-sexualized athletes (Gurung & Chrouser, 2007;Harrison & Secarea, 2010;Nezlek et al., 2015). In sum, female athletes who are depicted in a sexualized manner in media are objectified by media consumers and considered less capable. ...
... It is clear from the research that media coverage of women's sports is minimal and disproportionate to women's participation in sport (Cooky et al., 2015), yet media images of female athletes that focus on their athletic competence generate positive self-perceptions (Daniels, 2009a) and favorable attitudes toward female athletes (Daniels, 2012;Gurung & Chrouser, 2007;Harrison & Secarea, 2010;Nezlek et al., 2015) and women's sports (Kane & Maxwell, 2011). Therefore, increasing the visibility of girls' and women's athletics in media as well as improving the quality of media coverage are worthy goals. ...
Technical Report
Full-text available
Wiese-Bjornstal, D. M.APA, & Wood, K. N.* (2018). Sport injuries among female children and youth. In the 2018 Report of the Tucker Center for Research on Girls & Women in Sport, Developing physically active girls: An evidence-based multidisciplinary approach (pp. 73-98). University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN.
... This form of portrayal can negatively impact viewers' attitudes toward sexualized targets in a variety of ways including perceptions of competence and moral standing (for a review, see Ward 2016). In contrast, when the physical competence of female athletes is highlighted in media (termed sport performance or performance images), media consumers regard the athletes more positively (Daniels 2012;Daniels and Wartena 2011;Gurung and Chrouser 2007;Harrison and Secarea 2010;Knight and Giuliano 2001;Nezlek et al. 2015). However, existing research has dichotomized media images of female athletes as either sexualized or sport performance. ...
... Findings demonstrate that objectified women are perceived to possess less self-respect, be more sexually experienced, and be less competent, determined, intelligent, agentic, fully human, warm, and moral (Daniels 2016;Fasoli et al. 2018;Glick et al. 2005;Graff et al. 2012;Heflick and Goldenberg 2009;Heflick et al. 2011;Loughnan et al. 2010;Schooler 2015;Ward 2016;Wookey et al. 2009). A number of studies have found similar patterns in response to sexually objectified images of female athletes such that objectified athletes are perceived more negatively by viewers than sport performance athletes (Daniels 2012;Daniels and Wartena 2011;Everbach and Mumah 2014;Gurung and Chrouser 2007;Harrison and Secarea 2010;Kane and Maxwell 2011;Kane et al. 2013;Knight and Giuliano 2001;Nezlek et al. 2015). For example, Nezlek et al. (2015) showed college students either sexualized or non-sexualized media images of male and female athletes (the same athletes were used in both photo conditions). ...
... A number of studies have found similar patterns in response to sexually objectified images of female athletes such that objectified athletes are perceived more negatively by viewers than sport performance athletes (Daniels 2012;Daniels and Wartena 2011;Everbach and Mumah 2014;Gurung and Chrouser 2007;Harrison and Secarea 2010;Kane and Maxwell 2011;Kane et al. 2013;Knight and Giuliano 2001;Nezlek et al. 2015). For example, Nezlek et al. (2015) showed college students either sexualized or non-sexualized media images of male and female athletes (the same athletes were used in both photo conditions). The sexualized athletes were perceived to be less capable, determined, strong, intelligent, and possessing less self-respect than the non-sexualized athletes. ...
Article
Full-text available
Using an experimental methodology, the present study investigated college students’ attitudes toward media images of female athletes. We are particularly focused on how viewers perceive media images of female athletes that have both an appearance and athleticism focus, such as those found in ESPN’s The Body Issue. An aim of our study was to assess viewers’ attitudes toward these images that are not purely objectified, thereby contributing to the objectification literature and providing empirical data relevant to theorizing on the social impact of these images. U.S. college students (n = 563) viewed one of four types of images of the same athletes including: (a) sexualized athletes, (b) sexualized performance athletes (in which both athleticism and sexualization are present), (c) sport performance athletes (in which athletes are depicted playing their sport), or (d) non-sexualized athletes. They then rated the athletes’ competence, esteem, and sexual appeal. Overall, sexualized performance athletes were rated more positively than sexualized athletes, but less positively than sport performance athletes. These results have implications for advocacy efforts calling for more media coverage in which women are depicted as athletes rather than as sexual objects.
... Notably, female athletes are more likely to be objectified than male athletes, especially male participants (Nezlek et al., 2015). Similarly, emphasizing the gender attributes of women tended to overlook their performance and abilities (Gurung and Chrouser, 2007;Knight and Giuliano, 2001). ...
Article
Full-text available
Sports have traditionally had gendered connotations in society and culture, resulting in solidified gender stereotypes that influence impression evaluations. China has a special gender social culture; however, how sport–gender stereotypes (SGS) influence the gender evaluation of people in China in the Global South is still unknown. This study obtained gender-typed sports and attribute adjectives and proved the existence of SGS through a pilot study (392 college students, n1 = 207, n2 = 185) and then used two studies to explore the influence of both explicit and implicit SGS on evaluations and compared the differences between these stereotypes and general gender stereotypes. Study 1 (395 college students, n1a = 192, n1b = 203) examined the explicit level using a questionnaire experiment. The results of two experiments showed that (1) stereotype-consistent targets were more masculine or feminine in correspondence with their gender, while stereotype-inconsistent targets had higher anti-gender traits; and (2) the inclusion of stereotype-consistent sports activities led targets to be evaluated as more masculine, while stereotype-inconsistent sport activities showed gender evaluation reversal, especially for women. Study 2 (103 college students, n2a = 61, n2b = 42) measured the implicit attitudes using the Implicit Association Test. The results of two experiments showed that (1) implicit evaluations of stereotype-consistent targets were associated faster than stereotype-inconsistent targets and (2) the inclusion of gender-typed sports weakened implicit gender evaluations. In conclusion, this is the first quantitative study to explore the unique effect of SGS on individual evaluations and how they differ from general gender stereotypes in the Chinese context. These findings could provide valuable insights for research and the application of sports social science and physical education.
... It is widely acknowledged that sexualisation of women and girl generally and as athletes athlete exists (Daniels & Wartena, 2011;Nezlek et al., 2015;Petty & Pope, 2018), as per this study revealed many comments that were highly sexualised similar to: ...
Article
Research question This paper explores the recollections of twelve women’s physical education experiences in England, and the impact of this on their future footballing opportunities; six of whom went on to become semi-professional footballers and six whom did not continue in football post school life. Research methods The age range of participants in the qualitative study was from 18 to 37 years. These themes were addressed using Bourdieu’s sensitising notions of symbolic violence and collective expectations. Furthermore, the study embeds itself within the broad critical feminist writings of Judith Butler and the theory of gender performativity. Results and findings Following the data collection, reflexive thematic analysis identified limiting narratives of success, sexism, and the significant role that gender played within footballing environments. The study identified key themes around gendered experiences of PE, school sport and community pathways in the context of girls’ and women’s football. Implications The recommendations suggest the promotion of a more inclusive landscape for women and girls’ participation in football, and physical education more broadly, is required if we wish to improve women and girls experiences within the sport.
... 23 78 Findings consistently indicate that although sexualised women athletes are often rated as more attractive, desirable or sexual, they are also seen as less capable, as having less athletic ability, having lower intelligence and as having less self-respect than nonsexualised women athletes. 85 The sexualised portrayal of women athletes can have substantial social and economic outcomes in terms of the general acceptance of women athletes in society and their marketability as elite athletes. 86 On a society level, after viewing sexualised images of women athletes, girls, women and men make objectified appraisals of the athletes and themselves, as well a critique of women's status in society. ...
Article
Full-text available
Elite and semielite athletes commonly experience mental health concerns and disorders. Compared with men athletes, women athletes are at greater risk of a range of psychological stressors that contribute to health concerns and mental health disorders, which can impact their career satisfaction and longevity. In order to address and improve the mental health of women athletes, it is necessary to simultaneously tackle the gender specific psychosocial stressors that contribute to mental health outcomes. This narrative review examines the gender-specific stressors that affect mental health and well-being in women athletes, some of which are modifiable. Psychosocial stressors identified include exposure to violence, be it psychological, physical or sexual in nature, which can result in a myriad of acute and long-lasting symptoms; and inequities as reflected in pay disparities, under-representation in the media, fewer opportunities in leadership positions and implications associated with family planning and motherhood. Strategies to promote mental health in women athletes should be considered, and where possible, should proactively address gender-specific stressors likely to influence mental health in order to maximise positive outcomes in women athletes.
Article
Media images depicting idealized female physiques have been shown to heighten body dissatisfaction and body objectification. A potentially buffering factor in media exposure are depictions of female athletes performing their sports, which are associated with reduced objectification. These findings have not been extended to social physique anxiety (SPA), a heightened concern that one’s body does not meet comparative standards of physicality and beauty. Sixty-nine college-aged women reported levels of SPA following exposure to images of the same female professional athletes performing their sport, or in a sexualized pose. Visual attention to body parts on the images was measured via an eye tracker to explore whether fixations corresponded with the experience of SPA. Performance images lowered feelings of SPA relative to sexual images, and induced a lesser percentage of time visually fixating on the head/face, and more time fixating on arms and legs, relative to sexual images of the athletes. No differences emerged for fixations on the torso across conditions. Exploratory mediation models were also conducted to explore the influence of visual attention on the relationship between image type and SPA. These findings are considered in light of the nature of objectifying images of women and the importance of promoting empowering images to audiences.
Article
Full-text available
This article offers objectification theory as a framework for understanding the experiential consequences of being female in a culture that sexually objectifies the female body. Objectification theory posits that girls and women are typically acculturated to internalize an observer's perspective as a primary view of their physical selves. This perspective on self can lead to habitual body monitoring, which, in turn, can increase women's opportunities for shame and anxiety, reduce opportunities for peak motivational states, and diminish awareness of internal bodily states. Accumulations of such experiences may help account for an array of mental health risks that disproportionately affect women: unipolar depression, sexual dysfunction, and eating disorders. Objectification theory also illuminates why changes in these mental health risks appear to occur in step with life-course changes in the female body.
Article
Full-text available
This study (N = 911) investigated how exposure to sexualizing prime-time television programs, music television, men’s magazines, and pornographic websites was related to the internalization of appearance ideals, self-objectification, and body surveillance among adolescent boys. A structural equation model showed direct relationships between exposure to sexualizing television and pornographic websites and the internalization of appearance ideals. Indirect relationships between these types of sexualizing media, and self-objectification and body surveillance through the internalization of appearance ideals were also reported. A direct relationship with self-objectification was only found for sexualizing pornographic websites. Discussion warns attention for adolescent boys at risk of sexualization.
Article
Full-text available
Although an extensive qualitative literature shows that coverage of women's sport often focuses on female athletes' attractiveness (to the exclusion of their athleticism), there is a dearth of quantitative research examining exactly what effect this coverage has on people's perceptions of athletes. As part of a 2 (Gender of the Athlete: Female or Male) 2 (Gender of the Participant: Female or Male) 2 (Focus of the Article: Physical Attractiveness or Athleticism) between-subjects design, 92 predominantly White undergraduates (40 men, 52 women) read a fictitious newspaper profile about an Olympic athlete in which the article focused on the athlete's attractiveness (as coverage of female athletes often does) or on the athlete's athleticism (as coverage of male athletes often does). Interestingly, participants neither had favorable impressions of nor liked articles about female and male athletes when attractiveness was the main focus of an article. These findings suggest that the media should be cognizant of the harmful and erroneous impressions that can result from portraying athletes in terms of their personal attributes rather than their athletic accomplishments.
Article
Full-text available
This study provides empirical evidence for the objectification of women and unearths factors that increase objectification. Objectification theory (Fredrickson and Roberts 1997) suggests that women from Western cultures are the targets of male gaze. Although this seems self-evident from a look at the media, little empirical evidence exists to document the phenomenon or unravel underlying processes. Undergraduate female participants (N = 82) from the Midwestern part of the United States rated three photographs of well-known female Olympic athletes shown either provocatively dressed or in sport-appropriate outfits. Results showed that when shown provocatively attired the women were objectified. Furthermore, participants’ own levels of social physique anxiety were significant predictors of objectification. Sexism and trait objectification were not significantly related to ratings.
Article
In contrast to the large body of research examining the negative effects of idealized media images on girls' and women's body image, little research has investigated whether media images can positively impact body concept among females. Using a between-participants experimental design, this study examined how images of performance athletes, sexualized athletes, sexualized models, and nonsexualized models impacted adolescent girls' and college women's tendency to self-objectify. Participants were 350 adolescent girls and 225 college women who completed a measure of body objectification after viewing photographs. As expected, performance athlete images prompted less self-objectification, suggesting the need for more of this imagery in mainstream media.
Article
Objective: This study quantified professional athletes' endorsement of food and beverages, evaluated the nutritional quality of endorsed products, and determined the number of television commercial exposures of athlete-endorsement commercials for children, adolescents, and adults. Methods: One hundred professional athletes were selected on the basis of Bloomberg Businessweek's 2010 Power 100 rankings, which ranks athletes according to their endorsement value and prominence in their sport. Endorsement information was gathered from the Power 100 list and the advertisement database AdScope. Endorsements were sorted into 11 endorsement categories (eg, food/beverages, sports apparel). The nutritional quality of the foods featured in athlete-endorsement advertisements was assessed by using a Nutrient Profiling Index, whereas beverages were evaluated on the basis of the percentage of calories from added sugar. Marketing data were collected from AdScope and Nielsen. Results: Of 512 brands endorsed by 100 different athletes, sporting goods/apparel represented the largest category (28.3%), followed by food/beverages (23.8%) and consumer goods (10.9%). Professional athletes in this sample were associated with 44 different food or beverage brands during 2010. Seventy-nine percent of the 62 food products in athlete-endorsed advertisements were energy-dense and nutrient-poor, and 93.4% of the 46 advertised beverages had 100% of calories from added sugar. Peyton Manning (professional American football player) and LeBron James (professional basketball player) had the most endorsements for energy-dense, nutrient-poor products. Adolescents saw the most television commercials that featured athlete endorsements of food. Conclusions: Youth are exposed to professional athlete endorsements of food products that are energy-dense and nutrient-poor.
Article
Fredrickson and Roberts (1997) proposed objectification theory as an integrative framework for understanding how women's socialization and experiences of sexual objectification are translated into mental health problems. This article reviews the past decade of research grounded in objectification theory and highlights needed directions for future scholarship in this area. Specifically, this article reviews research organized according to the following themes: (a) self-objectification and its proposed consequences, (b) sexual objectification experiences as a proposed precursor, and (c) disconnections from bodily functions. An overview of emerging objectification theory research with men is also provided. The review concludes with needed directions for future theoretical and research efforts aimed to advance the psychology of women.
Article
College students (N = 324) watched a television program containing violence, sex, or no violence or sex. Each program contained 3 violent ads, 3 sexual ads, and 3 neutral ads. Participants were less likely to remember the advertised brands when the ads were embedded in a violent or sexual program than when the ads were embedded in a neutral program. Violent ads were the least memorable. This memory impairment occurred for both males and females, regardless of the content of the ads. If advertisers want viewers to remember advertised brands, they should think twice about sponsoring programs containing violence and sex.
Article
In response to the American Psychological Association's Task Force on the Sexualization of Girls, the present study explored the role of sexually objectifying media—in this case, music television—in a host of psychological consequences among a community sample of adolescents girls (M age = 13 years). Objectification theory posits that the consequences of sexual objectification involve the process of self-objectification. As such, we hypothesized that music television consumption would first and foremost be associated with self-objectification, which would, in turn, predict a number of body-related consequences. The findings support a model in which self-objectification mediates a direct relation between music television viewing and body esteem, dieting, depressive symptoms, anxiety, and confidence in math ability.