ArticlePDF Available

Parental Validating and Invalidating Responses and Adolescent Psychological Functioning: An Observational Study

Authors:

Abstract

The current study assessed the extent to which parental validating and invalidating behaviors (a) could be reliably measured in parent–adolescent relationships, (b) differed significantly between clinic and nonclinic families, and (c) were associated with measures of adolescent emotion dysregulation, behavior problems, and parent–adolescent relationship satisfaction. Adolescents (N = 29; age range = 12–18; 62% female) and their parents completed a variety of self-report and parent-report measures of adolescent functioning. Ratings of parents’ validating and invalidating responses during video-recorded social support and problem-solving interactions were obtained. Results indicated that parental validating and invalidating behaviors (a) were measured with a high degree of reliability, (b) differed significantly between clinic and nonclinic families, and (c) were correlated, in expected directions, with adolescent emotion dysregulation, externalizing problem behaviors, and adolescent relationship satisfaction. The implications of these findings are discussed in terms of both research and potentially improved family interventions.
http://tfj.sagepub.com/
The Family Journal
http://tfj.sagepub.com/content/22/1/43
The online version of this article can be found at:
DOI: 10.1177/1066480713490900
2014 22: 43 originally published online 20 June 2013The Family Journal
Chad E. Shenk and Alan E. Fruzzetti
Observational Study
Parental Validating and Invalidating Responses and Adolescent Psychological Functioning: An
Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com
On behalf of:
International Association of Marriage and Family Counselors
can be found at:The Family JournalAdditional services and information for
http://tfj.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:
http://tfj.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:
http://tfj.sagepub.com/content/22/1/43.refs.htmlCitations:
What is This?
- Jun 20, 2013OnlineFirst Version of Record
- Nov 25, 2013Version of Record >>
at UNIV OF NEVADA RENO on August 19, 2014tfj.sagepub.comDownloaded from at UNIV OF NEVADA RENO on August 19, 2014tfj.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Article
Parental Validating and Invalidating
Responses and Adolescent Psychological
Functioning: An Observational Study
Chad E. Shenk
1
and Alan E. Fruzzetti
2
Abstract
The current study assessed the extent to which parental validating and invalidating behaviors (a) could be reliably measured in
parent–adolescent relationships, (b) differed significantly between clinic and nonclinic families, and (c) were associated with measures
of adolescent emotion dysregulation, behavior problems, and parent–adolescent relationship satisfaction. Adolescents (N¼29; age
range ¼12–18; 62% female) and their parents completed a variety of self-report and parent-report measures of adolescent
functioning. Ratings of parents’ validating and invalidating responses during video-recorded social support and problem-solving inter-
actions were obtained. Results indicated that parental validating and invalidating behaviors (a) were measured with a high degree of
reliability, (b) differed significantly between clinic and nonclinic families, and (c) were correlated, in expected directions, with
adolescent emotion dysregulation, externalizing problem behaviors, and adolescent relationship satisfaction. The implications of
these findings are discussed in terms of both research and potentially improved family interventions.
Keywords
validating behaviors, invalidating behaviors, emotion dysregulation, adolescents
Emotion dysregulation, or the difficulty in deploying
behavioral strategies that effectively modulate the form, fre-
quency, or magnitude of an emotional response (Diamond &
Aspinwall, 2003; Gross, 1998), is a process variable associated
with a variety of psychological outcomes in children and ado-
lescents, including higher incidences of externalizing behaviors
(Eisenberg et al., 2001), anxiety (Suveg & Zeman, 2004),
hyperactivity (Walcott & Landau, 2004), depression (Kobak
& Ferenz-Gillies, 1995; Silk, Steinberg, & Morris, 2003), and
suicidal behaviors (Tamas et al., 2007). The relationship
between emotion dysregulation and a variety of child and ado-
lescent clinical outcomes has led to a growing body of research
identifying key determinants of emotion regulation that inform
models of emotional development and clinical intervention.
Much of this research has focused on person-level variables
that affect a child’s ability to regulate emotions, such as physio-
logical reactivity (Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp, & Mead, 2007;
Cole, Zahn-Waxler, Fox, Usher, & Welsh, 1996; Shipman
et al., 2007), temperament (Eisenberg et al., 2005; Ellis, Roth-
bart, & Posner, 2004), and specific behavioral strategies
acquired to regulate emotions (Blair, Denham, Kochanoff, &
Whipple, 2004; Hannesdottir & Ollendick, 2007; Zeman, Ship-
man, & Suveg, 2002). In addition to person-level variables,
specific aspects of the family environment are linked to varying
degrees of child and adolescent emotion dysregulation. When
parents respond negatively to a child’s emotional expression, the
child is more likely to react negatively (Eisenberg, Cumberland,
& Spinrad, 1998). More specifically, when a parent responds
with emotional invalidation and minimization of an emotional
expression, the child has more difficulty regulating his or her
emotions (Gottman & Katz, 2002) and is more likely to learn
problematic means of regulating emotions that are linked to clin-
ical outcomes (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schweizer, 2010;
Berlin & Cassidy, 2003; Krause, Mendelson, & Lynch, 2003).
In contrast, when parents demonstrate warm, understanding, and
accepting responses to an expressed emotion, children are more
likely to develop understanding of their emotional experience,
accurately express emotions, regulate emotional reactions, and
comply with parental directions (Calkins, Smith, Gill, & John-
son, 1998; Eisenberg & Fabes, 1994; Shipman, Zeman, Penza,
& Champion, 2000; Spinrad, Stifter, Donelan-McCall, &
Turner, 2004). The family context then, and in particular par-
ent–child interactions, can play an important role in regulating
a child’s emotions, shaping a child’s ability to learn emotion
1
Department of Human Development and Family Studies, The Pennsylvania
State University, University Park, PA, USA
2
Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno, NV, USA
Corresponding Author:
Chad E. Shenk, Department of Human Development and Family Studies, The
Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, 16802, USA.
Email: ceshenk@gmail.com
The Family Journal: Counseling and
Therapy for Couples and Families
2014, Vol 22(1) 43-48
ªThe Author(s) 2013
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1066480713490900
tfj.sagepub.com
at UNIV OF NEVADA RENO on August 19, 2014tfj.sagepub.comDownloaded from
regulation strategies, while serving as an important target in fam-
ily interventions.
Linehan (1993) and colleagues (Fruzzetti, Shenk, &
Hoffman, 2005) assert that emotion dysregulation results from
an ongoing transaction between parents and children, including
vulnerabilities from a child’s temperament, prior learning, and
problematic interactions with parents, which leads to the
development and maintenance of various forms of psycho-
pathology. This model proposes that specific aspects of family
communication, validating and invalidating behaviors, are key
determinants of a child’s ability to regulate emotions. A validat-
ing behavior occurs when a child or adolescent expresses his or
her private experience to a parent and this expression is met with
understanding, legitimacy, and acceptance of this experience
(Linehan, 1997). A validating behavior does not directly seek
to change or alter a child’s emotional experience; instead, it
seeks to highlight the emotional experience in order to facilitate
an individual’s acceptance and experiencing of the emotion.
Validating responses can influence individual emotion regula-
tion in several ways. First, validating behaviors can promote the
learning of skills for regulating emotions because they promote
more disclosures of emotional states which facilitate the experi-
encing of an emotion and consequently its expression and
regulation (Fruzzetti & Shenk, 2008; Fruzzetti & Worrall,
2010). Second, validating behaviors minimize the frequency,
intensity, and duration of an emotional reaction, making regula-
tion more likely. Conversely, an invalidating behavior is ‘‘one in
which communication of private experiences is met by erratic,
inappropriate, and extreme responses. In other words, the
expression of private experiences is not validated; instead it is
often punished or trivialized’’ (Linehan, 1993, p. 49). Such a
response conveys to a child or adolescent that his or her
emotional experience in a given situation is incorrect and attri-
butes that experience to socially unacceptable or undesirable
standards. Parental invalidating behaviors have a significant
impact on emotion dysregulation by worsening a child’s emo-
tional reactivity and by impeding his or her ability to learn and
use skills for regulating emotions. Validating and invalidating
behaviors have demonstrated moderate to large effect size differ-
ences (d¼0.73–1.10) on emotion regulation outcomes, such as
heart rate, skin conductance, and negative affect (Shenk &
Fruzzetti, 2011).
However, there is no research directly examining the rela-
tionship between parental validating and invalidating behaviors
and child and adolescent outcomes. The current study is a pre-
liminary test of parental validating and invalidating behaviors
and their relationship to broad domains of adolescent function-
ing. There were several aims of the current research: (a) deter-
mine whether validating and invalidating behaviors can be
reliably measured in parent–adolescent relationships, (b) test
whether validating and invalidating behaviors discriminate
between clinic and nonclinic families, and (c) examine whether
validating and invalidating behaviors are related to adolescent
emotion dysregulation, externalizing and internalizing beha-
viors, and relationship satisfaction as predicted by biosocial
models of psychopathology (e.g. Linehan, 1993).
Method
Sample
Clinic (n¼14) and nonclinic (n¼15) families were recruited
for participation. Clinic families, defined as an adolescent cur-
rently participating in family-based psychological treatment,
were recruited from local behavioral health clinics. Nonclinic
families, where no family member was currently receiving psy-
chological treatment, were recruited through public service
announcements and advertisements in local newspapers.
Families responding to recruitment efforts contacted the pro-
gram coordinator for the study and scheduled the research
assessment. Inclusion criteria were (a) families with at least one
caregiver with custodial rights, (b) an adolescent child between
the ages of 12 and 18, and (c) a willingness to participate in two
videotaped interaction tasks including one parent and his or her
adolescent child. In the case of two parent homes, each parent
was required to participate in the study in order to be eligible.
The mean age of children in the sample was 14.86 (SD ¼1.55),
the median family income was $40,000–$49,000, 62%of the
children were female, with 93%identifying themselves as
Caucasian. See Table 1 for detailed demographic information
by clinic status membership.
Measures
Validating and Invalidating Behaviors Coding Scale (VIBCS; Fruzzetti,
2001). The VIBCS is an observational rating scale used to mea-
sure levels of validating and invalidating behaviors within fam-
ilies (Fruzzetti, 2001). The VIBCS uses an ordinal rating scale
ranging from 1 to 7 where family members are given a global
Table 1. Demographic Characteristics.
Clinic Nonclinic
(n¼14) (n¼15)
M(SD)ornM(SD)orn
Age 15.00 (1.41) 14.73 (1.71)
Race
Caucasian 14 13
Minority 0 2
Sex
Male 6 5
Female 8 10
Single-parent family
Yes 9 7
No 5 8
LPI 115.43 (39.79) 90.00 (22.32)*
CBCL
Internalizing 60.43 (15.89) 49.67 (9.16)*
Externalizing 64.71 (15.75) 51.33 (8.97)**
AFLSI 22.43 (5.60) 26.93 (5.71)*
Validating behaviors 3.29 (0.99) 4.33 (1.63)*
Invalidating behaviors 4.43 (1.56) 2.33 (1.23)***
Note. AFLSI ¼Adolescent Family Life Satisfaction Index–Parental subscale;
CBCL ¼Child Behavior Checklist; LPI ¼Life Problems Inventory.
*p< .05. **p< .01. ***p< .001.
44 The Family Journal: Counseling and Therapy for Couples and Families 22(1)
at UNIV OF NEVADA RENO on August 19, 2014tfj.sagepub.comDownloaded from
rating on validating and invalidating behaviors observed in each
interaction (the coding manual is available upon request from the
second author). The VIBCS has demonstrated good interrater
reliability when rating couples’ interactions in previous research
with an intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) of .77. The con-
current validity of the VIBCS was also examined in prior
research with couples where validating behaviors were associ-
ated with greater relationship satisfaction (r¼.37, p<.001),
invalidating behaviors were associated with greater interpartner
aggression (r¼.39, p< .001), and where moderate to large
effect size differences (Z
2
¼.13 to .20) were observed between
distressed and nondistressed couples (Lowry, Mosco, Shenk, &
Fruzzetti, 2002). The VIBCS was used in this study to establish
the initial reliability of measuring validating and invalidating
behaviors in parent–adolescent relationships. Only parents were
assigned ratings of validating and invalidating behaviors in the
current study. When a two-parent home completed the study, the
highest level of validating and invalidating behaviors on the
VIBCS across the two parents was used for analysis.
Life Problems Inventory (LPI). The LPI is a 60-item self-report
questionnaire measuring adolescent emotion regulation consis-
tent with the biosocial theory of emotion dysregulation (Rathus
& Miller, 1995). Example items include ‘‘When I don’t get my
way, I quickly lose my temper,’’ ‘‘Once I get upset, it takes me a
long time to calm down,’’ and ‘‘Relationships with people I care
about have a lot of ups and downs.’’ Items are rated on a 5-point
scale ranging from 1 (not at all like me)to5(extremely like me).
Reliability of the LPI in the current study is a¼.96. The total
score on the LPI was used in this study and is derived by sum-
ming all 60 items with higher scores indicating greater emotion
dysregulation.
Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 (CBCL). Parents completed the
CBCL, a well-established, comprehensive multiaxial parent
report measure of children’s behavioral functioning with reli-
abilities ranging from a¼.72 to .96 and stability coefficients
ranging from r¼.70 to .74 in prior research (Achenbach,
1991). The CBCL generates standardized scores for broadband
scales of internalizing and externalizing behavior problems.
T-scores derived from the internalizing and externalizing scales
of the CBCL were used in the current study as indicators of
global adolescent problem behaviors. An average score was
used in cases where two parents each provided a score on exter-
nalizing and internalizing behaviors for a single adolescent.
Adolescent Family Life Satisfaction Index—Parental subscale
(AFLSI). The AFLSI is a self-report questionnaire assessing global
family satisfaction as reported by the adolescent (Henry, Ostran-
der, & Lovelace, 1992; Henry & Plunkett, 1995). In prior
research, the AFLSI has demonstrated reliability (a¼.90) and
concurrent validity (r¼.72) with other measures of family satis-
faction (Henry et al., 1992). The AFLSI has a 7-item, Parental
subscale measuring the degree to which an adolescent agrees
with an item assessing how satisfied heor she is with the parental
relationship. Items are ranked on a scale ranging from 1
(strongly disagree)to5(strongly agree) with higher scores indi-
cating greater satisfaction. Example items include ‘‘How much
my parents approve of me and the things I do’’ and ‘‘The amount
of freedom my parents give me to make my own choices.’ The
Parental subscale of the AFLSI was used in this study as an index
of parent–adolescent relationship satisfaction. Reliability and
concurrent validity of the Parental subscale in prior research is
a¼.88 and r¼.78, respectively (Henry et al., 1992). The relia-
bility of the Parental subscale in the current sample is a¼.82.
Procedure
All procedures were approved by the local institutional review
board prior to beginning the study. Upon the family’s arrival
for the research assessment, informed consent and child assent
was reviewed with the parent/parents and adolescent. Follow-
ing consent and assent, each family member was given the
appropriate questionnaires to complete in private. Once the
questionnaires were completed, each parent and adolescent
participated in two, 10-min videotaped interactions. In two-
parent homes, the adolescents participated in two 10-min
videotaped interactions with each of their parents. Each family
was prompted to discuss two topics. The first topic involved a
discussion of an issue that promotes closeness between the ado-
lescent and parent. The second topic involved a discussion of
an issue that the adolescent and parent agreed was a mild to
moderate conflict in their relationship. Families were asked
to discuss each of these topics for 10 min while being
videotaped. Once the family had completed both the question-
naires and the videotaped portions of the assessment, they were
financially compensated for participating in the study.
Graduate and undergraduate students were trained in the
VIBCS prior to coding the videotaped interactions. Training con-
sisted of 10 weekly meetings with each meeting lasting 90 min.
The first five meetings involved an overview of observational rat-
ing systems with families and detailed instruction on the VIBCS,
including the theoretical background, coding structure, content of
each level of validating and invalidating behaviors, and decision
rules for promoting reliability. The final five meetings involved
consensus coding of specified training sessions. After the 10th
meeting, coders rated a new set of training sessions to determine
whether their ratings met a sufficient criterion of reliability. An
ICC of .75 was adopted as the lower bound criterion of reliability
as coefficients of .75 and above indicate excellent reliability
(Fleiss, 1986; Shrout & Fleiss, 1979). Coders meeting the ICC
¼.75 criterion were permitted to code the interactions. Coders not
meeting this criterion were provided with additional training.
Coder drift was minimized via weekly checks of adherence on
a videotape rated by each coder. All coders were blind to the
family’s clinic status and all other data.
Data Analytic Strategy
The data analytic strategy involved several planned analyses to
establish the preliminary evidence of parental validating and
Shenk and Fruzzetti 45
at UNIV OF NEVADA RENO on August 19, 2014tfj.sagepub.comDownloaded from
invalidating behaviors. First, ICCs were obtained to estimate
the interrater reliability of parental validating and invalidating
behaviors measured in parent–adolescent interactions. Second,
ratings obtained from the VIBCS were analyzed using multi-
variate analysis of variance (MANOVA) to determine whether
parental validating and invalidating behaviors differed signifi-
cantly between clinic and nonclinic families. Finally, multiple
regression determined whether validating and invalidating
behaviors were related to proposed processes of change (emo-
tion dysregulation) and adolescent outcomes (externalizing
behaviors, internalizing behaviors, and parent–adolescent rela-
tionship satisfaction). Validating and invalidating behaviors
were entered simultaneously as predictors in the regression
models. Regression models were then examined for outliers
and model assumptions.
Results
Preliminary Data Analysis
Demographic and study-related variables were assessed using
chi-square and analysis of variance (ANOVA) to detect signif-
icant differences between clinic and nonclinic families. Results
from chi-square tests indicated that clinic and nonclinic fami-
lies did not differ significantly on race, sex of the adolescent,
whether the family was a single-parent or dual parent home,
or family income. The ANOVA revealed significant mean
differences between clinic and nonclinic families on the LPI,
CBCL externalizing, CBCL internalizing, and AFLSI scores
(see Table 1). There were no significant age differences
between clinic and nonclinic families.
Reliability of the VIBCS in Parent–Adolescent Dyadic
Interactions
Four coders provided ratings of validating and invalidating
behaviors observed during the parent–adolescent interactions.
Seventeen percent of the families in the sample were randomly
selected as a means to assess interrater reliability. Interrater
reliability on this subset of families was determined via ICC
using a two-way random effects model with absolute agree-
ment among coders (McGraw & Wong, 1996). Interrater relia-
bility was estimated using a single measure ICC, which is based
on each individual rating across raters, a conservative estimate
of interrater reliability. The resulting estimate was ICC ¼.86,
indicating excellent interrater reliability. The correlation
between parental validating and invalidating behaviors was
r¼.57, p¼.001.
Validating and Invalidating Behaviors in Clinic and
Nonclinic Families
A MANOVA compared ratings of validating and invalidating
behaviors obtained using the VIBCS between clinic and noncli-
nic families. The results of the MANOVA demonstrated signif-
icant between-group differences on levels of validating
behaviors, F(1, 27) ¼4.23, p¼.05, d¼.80, and invalidating
behaviors, F(1, 27) ¼16.27, p< .001, d¼1.55. Specifically,
clinic families had significantly lower levels of validating
behaviors and significantly higher levels of invalidating
behaviors when compared to nonclinic families (see Table 1)
with large effect size differences observed between the groups.
Validating and Invalidating Behaviors and Global
Adolescent Functioning
LPI. The multiple regression model with validating and invali-
dating behaviors as predictors of LPI scores provided a good fit
to the data, F(2, 26) ¼4.77, p¼.02, with validating and inva-
lidating behaviors accounting for 27%of the variance in LPI
scores. Validating behaviors significantly predicted LPI scores,
b¼11.88, p¼.01, indicating that a one-level increase in
ratings of validating behaviors was associated with an approx-
imate 12-point decrease in LPI scores. Invalidating behaviors
were not significantly related to LPI scores.
CBCL. Validating and invalidating behaviors were simultane-
ously estimated as predictors of CBCL externalizing behavior
scores. This model produced a good fit to the data, F(2, 26) ¼
6.62, p¼.01, that accounted for 34%of the variance in externa-
lizing scores. Invalidating behaviors significantly predicted
CBCL externalizing scores, b¼3.83, p¼.01, with a one-
level increase in invalidating behaviors associated with an
almost 4-point increase in externalizing T-scores. Validating
behaviors did not significantly predict externalizing scores after
accounting for invalidating behaviors. This same model was
used to fit CBCL internalizing behavior scores. Results indicated
a poor fit to the model, F(2, 26) ¼1.06, p¼ns, with validating
and invalidating behaviors accounting for only 8%of the
variance in internalizing scores. Neither validating nor invalidat-
ing behaviors significantly predicted internalizing scores.
AFLSI. A final model was fit where validating and invalidating
behaviors were entered as predictors of AFLSI scores. Results
demonstrated a good fit to the model, F(2, 26) ¼8.42, p< .01,
that accounted for 39%of the variance in AFLSI scores. Both
validating behaviors, b¼1.51, p¼.03, and invalidating beha-
viors, b¼1.19, p¼.04, predicted AFLSI scores, indicating
the mutual importance of both variables when assessing adoles-
cent relationship satisfaction.
Discussion
Results provide preliminary support to theoretical models (Dia-
mond & Aspinwall, 2003; Fruzzetti et al., 2005) and prior
research (Shenk & Fruzzetti, 2011) examining the role of
family-level determinants, specifically validating and invali-
dating behaviors, of emotion dysregulation and corresponding
behavioral outcomes. Parental validating and invalidating
behaviors can be measured with a high degree of reliability
using a global observational rating scale. These behaviors
differed significantly between clinic and nonclinic families
where families receiving treatment had significantly lower
46 The Family Journal: Counseling and Therapy for Couples and Families 22(1)
at UNIV OF NEVADA RENO on August 19, 2014tfj.sagepub.comDownloaded from
levels of validating behaviors and significantly higher levels of
invalidating behaviors when compared to families who were
not in treatment. Even with a modest sample size, analyses
were sufficiently powered to detect the large effect size differ-
ences between clinic and nonclinic families on ratings of vali-
dating and invalidating behaviors, highlighting the potential
importance of evaluating these behaviors in parent–adolescent
relationships. Validating and invalidating behaviors were also
differentially related to process variables and adolescent
outcomes as predicted. Validating parent behaviors were asso-
ciated with more effective emotion regulation and greater
satisfaction in parent–child relationships. Invalidating parent
behaviors, on the other hand, were associated with higher exter-
nalizing behavior problems and lower relationship satisfaction.
These results support previous research indicating that how
parents react to emotional responses and disclosures is related
to the psychological functioning of their children currently and
later in development (Krause et al., 2003; Spinrad et al., 2004).
It is also important to note that neither validating behaviors nor
invalidating behaviors were associated with internalizing beha-
vior problems. Thus, validating and invalidating behaviors may
be more useful when understanding the development of exter-
nalizing behaviors as opposed to internalizing behaviors,
although future research will be needed to support this claim.
Future longitudinal research will also need to examine the rela-
tionship between validating and invalidating responses and other
ratings of interests (e.g., parent social support or criticality) to
evaluate the predictive utility of various theoretical models.
Overall, the results support existing research while extending the
literature through the identification of specific parenting beha-
viors linked to both processes of change and clinical outcomes.
There are several potential clinical implications based on
findings from this study. The VIBCS is a readily applicable
assessment tool that could be used when conceptualizing clin-
ical cases and developing a family treatment plan. For instance,
if invalidating behaviors are related to clinical outcomes, then
focusing on decreasing invalidating behaviors, and potentially
increasing validating behaviors, may serve as important treat-
ment targets. Based on present outcomes, the extent to which
invalidating behaviors are contributing to individual or
relationship outcomes can be assessed reliably and efficiently
before starting therapy. By rating interaction samples from
clients at various times throughout therapy, clinicians can track
changes in invalidating behaviors as a result of implementing a
treatment plan that includes these behaviors as targets. Also,
because brief interactions can be coded in real time, immediate
in-session feedback can be provided to families in therapy.
Reducing the level of invalidating behaviors in parent–adoles-
cent interactions may help in reducing problem behavior while
removing important barriers to relationship satisfaction
throughout adolescence. In turn, increasing the use of parental
validating behaviors, both in their frequency and intensity, can
be used to facilitate children labeling their emotional experi-
ences, the accurate expression of their emotional states, as well
as their abilities to regulate their emotional reactivity. In this
context, validating behaviors can help improve adolescent
functioning during the course of treatment while promoting
relationship enhancement with parents.
There are also important considerations that limit broad con-
clusions about parental validating and invalidating behaviors
from this study. The sample size is modest (N¼29) and,
although representative in terms of sex and family constellation,
not racially or ethnically diverse. The implication of having a
modest sample size with primarily Caucasian families raises the
possibility that findings may not generalize to the larger popula-
tion of families, although previous research with the VIBCS has
included ethnically and racially diverse samples (Lowry et al.,
2002; Shipman et al., 2007). The research design is cross-
sectional and causal inferences are not appropriate despite sig-
nificant relationships among validating and invalidating beha-
viors, adolescent emotion dysregulation, and clinical
outcomes. Only longitudinal research examining the temporal
relations between these variables can tease out whether adoles-
cent problem behavior develops as stated in biosocial models
of psychopathology. Overall, this study provides a base from
which to launch further research on parental validating and inva-
lidating responses, advancing prior research by identifying spe-
cific parent behaviors contributing to the well-established
connection between parenting and adolescent outcomes (Eisen-
berg et al., 1998). This study also offers theoretically informed
targets for intervention to aid clinicians treating emotion dysre-
gulation concerns with adolescents and their families.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Funding
The author(s) received no financial support for the research, author-
ship, and/or publication of this article.
References
Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Integrative guide for the 1991 CBCL/4-18,
YSR, and TRF profiles. Burlington: University of Vermont.
Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-
regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic
review. Clinical Psychology Review,30, 217–237.
Beauchaine, T. P., Gatzke-Kopp, L., & Mead, H. K. (2007). Polyvagal
theory and developmental psychopathology: Emotion dysregulation
and conduct problems from preschool to adolescence. Biological
Psychology,74, 174–184. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.008
Berlin, L. J., & Cassidy, J. (2003). Mothers’ self-reported control of
their preschool children’s emotional expressiveness: A longitudi-
nal study of associations with infant-mother attachment and
children’s emotion regulation. Social Development,12, 477–495.
Blair, K. A., Denham, S. A., Kochanoff, A., & Whipple, B. (2004).
Playing it cool: Temperament, emotion regulation, and social beha-
vior in preschoolers. Journal of School Psychology,42, 419–443.
Calkins, S. D., Smith, C. L., Gill, K. L., & Johnson, M. C. (1998).
Maternal interactive style across contexts: Relations to emotional,
behavioral, and physiological regulation during toddlerhood.
Social Development,7, 350–369.
Shenk and Fruzzetti 47
at UNIV OF NEVADA RENO on August 19, 2014tfj.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Cole, P. M., Zahn-Waxler, C., Fox, N. A., Usher, B. A., & Welsh, J. D.
(1996). Individual differences in emotion regulation and behavior
problems in preschool children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
105, 518–529.
Diamond, L. M., & Aspinwall, L. G. (2003). Emotion regulation
across the life span: An integrative perspective emphasizing
self-regulation, positive affect, and dyadic processes. Motivation
& Emotion,27, 125–156.
Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental
socialization of emotion. Psychological Inquiry,9, 241–273.
Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., Spinrad, T. L., Fabes, R. A., Shepard,
S. A., & Reiser, M., ...Guthrie, I. K. (2001). The relations of
regulation and emotionality to children’s externalizing and interna-
lizing problem behavior. Child Development,72, 1112–1134.
Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1994). Mothers’ reactions to children’s
negative emotions: Relations to children’s temperament and anger
behavior. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,40, 138–156.
Eisenberg, N., Zhou, Q., Spinrad, T. L., Valiente, C., Fabes, R. A., &
Liew, J. (2005). Relations among positive parenting, children’s
effortful control, and externalizing problems: A three-wave
longitudinal study. Child Development,76, 1055–1071.
Ellis, L. K., Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (2004). Individual differ-
ences in executive attention predict self-regulation and adolescent
psychosocial behaviors. Annals of the New York Academy of
Science,1021, 337–340. doi:10.1196/annals.1308.041
Fleiss, J. L. (1986). The design and analysis of clinical experiments.
New York, NY: John Wiley.
Fruzzetti, A. (2001). Validating and invalidating behaviors coding
scale. Reno: University of Nevada.
Fruzzetti, A. E., & Shenk, C. (2008). Fostering validating responses in
families. Social Work in Mental Health,6, 215–227.
Fruzzetti, A. E., Shenk, C., & Hoffman, P. D. (2005). Family interaction
and the development of borderline personality disorder: A transac-
tional model. Development and Psychopathology,17, 1007–1030.
Fruzzetti, A. E., & Worrall, J. M. (2010). Accurate expression and
validating responses: A transactional model for understanding
individual and relationship distress. In K. T. Sullivan & J. Davila
(Eds.), Support processes in intimate relationships (pp.
121–150). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Gottman, J. M., & Katz, L. F. (2002). Children’s emotional reactions to
stressful parent-child interactions: The link between emotion
regulation and vagal tone. Marriage & Family Review,34, 265–283.
Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An
integrative review. Review of General Psychology,2, 271–299.
Hannesdottir, D. K., & Ollendick, T. H. (2007). The role of emotion
regulation in the treatment of child anxiety disorders. Clinical
Child & Family Psychology Review,10, 275–293.
Henry, C. S., Ostrander, D. L., & Lovelace, S. G. (1992). Reliability
and validity of the adolescent family life satisfaction index.
Psychological Reports,70, 1223–1229.
Henry, C. S., & Plunkett, S. W. (1995). Validation of the adolescent
family life satisfaction index: An update. Psychological Reports,
76, 672–674.
Kobak, R., & Ferenz-Gillies, R. (1995). Emotion regulation and
depressive symptoms during adolescence: A functionalist perspec-
tive. Development and Psychopathology,7, 183–192.
Krause,E.D.,Mendelson,T.,&Lynch,T.R.(2003).Childhood
emotional invalidation and adult psychological distress: The
mediating role of emotional inhibition. Child Abuse & Neglect,
27,199213.
Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of borderline
personality disorder. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Linehan, M. M. (1997). Validation and psychotherapy. In A. C. Bohart
& L. S. Greenberg (Eds.), Empathy reconsidered: New directions
in psychotherapy (pp. 353–392). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Lowry, K., Mosco, E., Shenk, C., & Fruzzetti, A. E. (2002).
Validating and invalidating behaviors coding scale (VIBCS):
Reliability and validity. Paper presented at the 36th Annual
Convention of the Association for the Advancement of Behavior
Therapy, Reno, NV.
McGraw, K. O., & Wong, S. P. (1996). Forming inferences about some
intraclass correlation coefficients. Psychological Methods,1, 30–46.
Rathus, J. H., & Miller, A. L. (1995). The life problems inventory.
Unpublished manuscript. Long Island University, Brookville, NY.
Shenk, C. E., & Fruzzetti, A. E. (2011). The impact of validating and
invalidating responses on emotional reactivity. Journal of Social
and Clinical Psychology,30, 163–183.
Shipman, K., Zeman, J., Penza, S., & Champion, K. (2000). Emotion
management skills in sexually maltreated and nonmaltreated girls:
A developmental psychopathology perspective. Development and
Psychopathology,12, 47–62.
Shipman, K. L., Schneider, R., Fitzgerald, M. M., Sims, C., Swisher,
L., & Edwards, A. (2007). Maternal emotion socialization in mal-
treating and non-maltreating families: Implications for children’s
emotion regulation. Social Development,16, 268–285.
Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses
in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin,86,
420–428.
Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. S. (2003). Adolescents’ emotion
regulation in daily life: Links to depressive symptoms and problem
behavior. Child Development,74, 1869–1880.
Spinrad, T. L., Stifter, C. A., Donelan-McCall, N., & Turner, L.
(2004). Mothers’ regulation strategies in response to toddlers’
affect: Links to later emotion self-regulation. Social Development,
13, 40–55.
Suveg, C., & Zeman, J. (2004). Emotion regulation in children with
anxiety disorders. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent
Psychology,33, 750–759.
Tamas, Z., Kovacs, M., Gentzler, A. L., Tepper, P., Gadoros, J., &
Kiss, E., ...Vetro, A. (2007). The relations of temperament and
emotion self-regulation with suicidal behaviors in a clinical sample
of depressed children in Hungary. Journal of Abnormal Child
Psychology,35, 640–652. doi:10.1007/s10802-007-9119-2
Walcott, C. M., & Landau, S. (2004). The relation between disinhibi-
tion and emotion regulation in boys with attention deficit hyperac-
tivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent
Psychology,33, 772–782.
Zeman, J., Shipman, K., & Suveg, C. (2002). Anger and sadness regula-
tion: Predictions to internalizingand externalizing symptoms in chil-
dren. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology,31,
393–398.
48 The Family Journal: Counseling and Therapy for Couples and Families 22(1)
at UNIV OF NEVADA RENO on August 19, 2014tfj.sagepub.comDownloaded from
... By validating their own experience, parents can remain more engaged with themselves and their child (Smith et al., 2023). Studies indicate that validation can reduce adverse emotional and behavioral manifestations, such as adolescent self-harm (Adrian et al., 2018), emotion dysregulation (Shenk and Fruzzetti, 2014), and negative affect in adults (Benitez et al., 2019(Benitez et al., , 2022. Assimilating validation as a primary skill is particularly crucial during infancy, a period marked by increased parental stress and a need for sensitive and responsive caregiving. ...
Article
Full-text available
Infancy is a formative period in which high-quality parental care plays a vital role in setting solid foundations that guide a child’s development. Mindfulness has been recognized for enhancing parental awareness and sensitivity to both self and child and can be utilized in clinical practice to facilitate healthy development. To adapt mindful parenting practice for implementation in pediatric care settings and the specific needs and challenges of parenting infants, the current study introduces a novel theoretical framework, combining mindfulness with elements from calming cycle theory, dialectical behavior therapy (DBT), trauma-informed care, emotion-focused therapy, schema therapy, and Vygotsky’s learning theory. These elements are not merely complementary but cardinal in meeting the diverse needs of parents during infancy, both in typical developmental contexts and following preterm birth, where additional stressors are often present. The study delineates the theoretical foundations of this integrative mindfulness-based approach and openly provides a novel comprehensive protocol of an 8-week group intervention program that operationalizes the proposed framework. This program focuses on enhancing parental mindful observation, non-judgmental acceptance, and goal-driven behavior to strengthen the resilience of the parent-infant relationship. Emphasizing the bi-directional nature of this relationship and the role of co-regulation with the child, the theory-derived program is designed to scaffold calming dyadic cycles, promote reconsolidation of birth-related adverse experiences, and facilitate flexibility in parental modes. The novelty of this intervention lies in its holistic approach to mindful parenting, conjoining diverse theoretical perspectives into a coherent, culturally adaptable, and clinically oriented protocol that can be assimilated in pediatric community clinics. The provided protocol may now enable the evaluation of the framework’s effectiveness in attaining positive effects for parents and children.
... Literatur yang ada menunjukkan bahwa ketidakvalidan emosi pada masa kanakkanak dapat menjadi faktor risiko bagi masalah hubungan pada individu dengan Gangguan Kepribadian Borderline (Selby, Braithwaite, Joiner, & Fincham dalam Herr et al., 2015). Selain itu, perilaku orangtua yang tidak memvalidasi emosi juga berkaitan dengan disregulasi emosi yang buruk pada remaja (Buckholdt et al., 2014;Shenk & Fruzzetti, 2014). ...
Article
Full-text available
Emerging adulthood is when individuals begin to explore and experience many changes in their lives. Social support can improve psychological well-being during this challenging developmental stage. However, research on the influence of emotional invalidation on psychological well-being is still minimal. This study aims to determine the role of emotional invalidation and social support on psychological well-being in early adulthood. This study involved 266 samples consisting of male and female Indonesian citizens aged 18-29 years and applied quantitative methods with purposive sampling techniques. The instruments used for data collection were the Psychological Well-Being Scale, the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support, and The Perceived Invalidation of Emotion Scale, which were then analyzed using multiple linear regression. The results showed that the role of emotional invalidation and social support on psychological well-being had a high influence with a significant value (p
... The current study shows that parents may use distraction as a strategy when their children face more challenging emotions. However, distracting a child implies ignoring the emotion, which does not enhance emotional competence, as redirecting the child's attention does not provide them with the opportunity to discuss and understand their feelings (Shenk & Fruzzetti, 2014). While some studies consider distraction a coping emotion regulation strategy (Davis et al., 2010), other studies suggest that distraction is not seen as an effective method, as children may not feel comfortable with recognizing, understanding and discussing their emotions (Bjørk et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Emotion-focused parenting refers to parents teaching their children about emotions, talking about emotions within the family, and expressing their reactions to their children’s emotions. This study aims at examining the emotion-focused parenting approaches of parents with preschool-aged children (3–6 years). In this line, the purpose of this study is to explore the ways in which parents in Türkiye incorporate emotions into their interactions with their children and to discover their life experiences in this regard. To achieve this purpose, the study employed the descriptive phenomenological research design, one of the qualitative research methods. Since the participants were selected based on specific criteria, the criterion sampling method was preferred in the study. 16 parents were interviewed during the study period. The researchers created a semi-structured interview form, based on emotion-focused parenting literature to collect the study data. The data were analyzed using Giorgi’s phenomenological method and the computer-assisted data analysis program NVivo 14. At the end of the study, three themes were identified: identifying emotions, expressing emotions, and strategies used in emotion management. The study findings show that it is necessary for parents to first recognize, notice and manage their own emotions before they can recognize their children’s emotions and respond appropriately. Emotions are taught to children by parents in a natural process in daily life. Considering the study findings, parents are recommended to review their parenting styles in the process of teaching emotions to their children, and experts are recommended to develop various psychoeducation programs for parents.
... 13 Conversely, invalidation means responding to those same experiences and behaviours with disinterest, disbelief, or active criticism. 13,29 How, when, and who has their experiences (in)validated will depend on the characteristics (eg, age, sex and racialised identity) of those providing (eg, caregivers and clinicians) and receiving (in)validation. 31 In the adult pain literature, 3 models of validation have been considered 7 ; however, direct evidence supporting the use of one over the other is lacking. ...
... The current study seeks to understand exposure to abuse rather than neglect per se, therefore we only used the five-item subscales of physical abuse and sexual abuse. Given that adolescents with BPD commonly struggle with familial invalidation (Fruzzetti & Shenk, 2008;Shenk & Fruzzetti, 2014), which likely has considerable overlap with reports of emotional abuse, we did not include the emotional abuse subscale. ...
Article
Objective: Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD) is characterized by extreme behavioral, emotional, interpersonal and cognitive dysregulation, frequently including suicidality and self-harm. Dialectical Behavioral Therapy (DBT) is an empirically supported treatment for BPD and related problems that has been shown to reduce emotion dysregulation, suicidality and self-harm and improve a variety of other outcomes in adolescents and adults. However, the extent to which prior traumatic events affect treatment outcome is not well understood. This study investigated the effectiveness of an intensive residential DBT program for adolescents with BPD-related problems. Given previously demonstrated links between exposure to childhood abuse and emotion dysregulation, we also evaluated the impact of severity and type of childhood abuse on treatment outcomes. Method: Participants (N = 46) were female-identifying adolescents 13 to 20 years of age enrolled in an intensive residential DBT treatment program. Abuse severity and abuse subtype (Childhood Trauma Questionnaire, Short Form (CTQ-SF), and difficulties with emotional dysregulation (DERS) were assessed at admission and DERS was assessed again after one month of treatment. Results: Bivariate correlation between DERS Total Score and Abuse Exposure Total Score indicated that severity of abuse exposure had a medium positive association with emotion dysregulation at admission. Mixed model ANOVA results showed significant reductions in emotion dysregulation following one month of treatment, with a large effect size. Exposure to different subtypes of childhood abuse was tested as a moderator of treatment effects and was non-significant. Conclusions: Emotion dysregulation showed significant reductions following a brief, intensive DBT residential intervention in a population of adolescent females with severe BPD-related problems. Reductions were consistent across subtypes of abuse exposure despite the relationship between abuse exposure and emotional dysregulation at admission. Implications for the treatment of adolescents with abuse histories and current BPD-related problems, and the relationship between child abuse exposure and emotion dysregulation overall, are discussed.
Article
The present study aimed to find out differences of social support, perceived emotion invalidation, psychological needs, and use of adaptive and maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies in maritally adjusted and maladjusted after controlling for age, education, employment status, and depressive symptomatology. The cross‐sectional study uses a matched pairs design. The sample was divided into two groups; maritally adjusted and maladjusted women ( n = 40 pairs) on basis of scores obtained on revised‐dyadic adjustment scale. Forty maritally adjusted women were matched with 40 maritally maladjusted women according to age, education, and employment status. Social support questionnaire, perceived invalidation of emotion scale, basic psychological need satisfaction frustration scale, cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire, and center for epidemiologic studies depression scale were administered. One‐way ANCOVA revealed that maritally maladjusted women had lower level of social support [mean difference; −5.65(−9.97, −1.33), p < 0.05, partial η ² = 0.08] and more emotional invalidation [mean difference; 15.36(13.08, 17.65), p < 0.001, partial η ² = 0.71] compared to maritally adjusted women after controlling for the effect of depressive symptomatology. Maritally maladjusted women had more need frustration [mean difference; 10.75(7.59, 13.92), p < 0.001, partial η ² = 0.38] compared to maritally adjusted women. However, maritally adjusted women had more need satisfaction [mean difference; 13.36(9.67, 17.05), p < 0.001, partial η ² = 0.41] compared to maritally maladjusted women. Maritally adjusted women used more adaptive CER strategies (acceptance, refocus on planning and putting into perspective) [mean difference; 4.66(2.36, 6.95), p < 0.001, partial η ² = 0.18] compared to maritally maladjusted women whereas, maritally maladjusted women used more maladaptive strategies (self‐blame, catastrophizing and blaming others) [mean difference; 4.66(2.77, 6.54), p < 0.001, partial η ² = 0.25] compared to maritally adjusted women. Maladjusted women had less social support and more emotional invalidation of emotions and psychological needs frustration. They used more maladaptive strategies to manage their negative emotions in comparison to maritally adjusted women. Identification of these cognitive emotion regulation strategies will help clinicians and counselors to devise psychological intervention targeting the use of adaptive strategies to minimize the negative mental health consequences.
Article
While we know childhood experiences are influential on a child’s later socioemotional awareness and behavior, we are still searching for specific mechanisms that influence the transferability of childhood experiences and adult relationship functioning. In this study, we seek to further this area of investigation by examining the interpretation of ambiguous social interactions and perceptions of emotional invalidation as potential mediators of the relationship between perceptions of childhood emotional invalidation and current relationship quality. Participants completed online measures of hostile intent attributions, perceptions of childhood emotional invalidation, and current relationship quality with a significant other. They read emotionally provocative interpersonal scenarios and then reported likely emotionally invalidating reactions from their significant other to measure current perceptions of emotional invalidation. Results supported our hypotheses that perceptions of current emotional invalidation in a close, personal relationship would mediate the relationship between perceptions of childhood maternal emotional invalidation and both current relationship support and relationship conflict. Furthermore, the strength of this mediational pathway outweighed the influence of a more general hostile attribution bias. These findings have implications for prevention and intervention strategies designed to enhance interpersonal functioning.
Article
Persistence is a critical factor that significantly predicts life outcomes. Although individual differences in persistence emerge early in life, the knowledge of effective strategies for cultivating persistence in young children remains limited. Based on these two studies, we suggest that emotional validation, defined as the acceptance of emotions without judgment, is a beneficial technique for promoting persistence in the wake of frustration. Study 1 examined 150 parents of children aged 4–6 years and found that parents’ tendency to validate their children's emotions was positively associated with children's trait persistence. We conducted a randomized experiment ( N = 93, aged 4–6 years) in Study 2 to establish a causal relationship between emotional validation and persistence in preschoolers. Children who received emotional validation feedback ( n = 31) exhibited higher levels of persistence than those who received emotional invalidation ( n = 31) and no feedback (baseline, n = 31) on a frustrating task. The persistence between the emotional invalidation and no‐feedback conditions was not statistically different. This finding highlights the vital role of emotional validation in promoting persistence in children at trait and state levels. In summary, this study offers a compelling strategy for empowering young children with resilience and determination when they encounter challenges. Research Highlights Parental emotional validation predicts the trait‐level persistence of children aged 4−6 years. Children who received emotion‐validation feedback persisted longer on a task than those who received emotion‐invalidation feedback or no feedback. Children in the emotion invalidation condition did not differ from those in the no‐feedback condition in terms of persistence. Future studies are required to investigate the mechanisms underlying emotional validation in promoting task persistence in children.
Article
Full-text available
In this commentary, we build upon the papers featured in this 2-part special issue to advance an integrative perspective on emotion regulation that emphasizes the developmentally specific goal-contexts of emotional phenomena. We highlight the importance of (1) multilevel longitudinal investigations of interactions among biological, affective, cognitive, and behavioral processes with respect to emotion regulation; (2) the integration of emotion-regulation processes with self-regulatory processes across the life course; (3) the dynamic relationship between positive and negative affect and their respective influence on regulatory processes; and (4) greater consideration of the dyadic context of emotion-regulation processes. From this perspective, the optimal developmental outcome with respect to emotion regulation is not affective homeostasis, but rather a dynamic flexibility in emotional experience, the ability to pursue and prioritize different goals, and the capacity to selectively and proactively mobilize emotions and cognitions in the service of context-specific and developmentally specific goals.
Article
Full-text available
This paper examines children's physiological reactions to stressful parent-child interactions and tests the notion that vagal tone is a physiological index of the ability to regulate emotion. Basal vagal tone and the suppression of vagal tone at age 4-5 were examined as predictors of mother ratings of child's emotion regulation ability at age 8. Two hypotheses about the mechanism by which vagal tone predicts emotion regulation were examined: a stress inoculation hypothesis and a recovery from arousal hypothesis. Path analyses showed that age 4-5 regulatory physiology predicted child emotion regulation scores at age 8, and that this was partially mediated by the 4- to 5-year-old child's ability to maintain a low heart rate during stressful parent-child interactions. Interrupted time-series analyses of these events as a function of the child's basal vagal tone showed that children with higher basal vagal tone have both a larger heart rate increase to these events as well as faster recovery than children with lower vagal tone.
Article
Full-text available
AIthough intraclass correlation coefficients (lCCs) are commonIy used in behavioral measurement, pychometrics, and behavioral genetics, procodures available for forming inferences about ICC are not widely known. Following a review of the distinction between various forms of the ICC, this article presents procedures available for calculating confidence intervals and conducting tests on ICCs developed using data from one-way and two-way random and mixed-efFect analysis of variance models. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Chapter
This chapter presents a transactional model for understanding the linkages between individual and relationship distress within couple interactions, describing how specific patterns of ineffective communication can develop over time. The role of emotion (and dysregulated emotion) in relationships, along with the roles of accurate (vs. inaccurate) expression and validating (vs. invalidating) responses are highlighted, and treatment implications and directions for future research are discussed.
Article
Along with individual factors, such as emotional reactivity and regulatory skill, a biosocial perspective of emotion regulation incorporates social factors, such as parent-child or romantic partner interactions, as key determinants of psychological outcomes. Consistent with this perspective, the current study tested whether two social factors, validating and invalidating responses, influenced affective and physiological reactions to stress while accounting for individual skill in regulating emotions. Hierarchical linear modeling demonstrated that participants exposed to invalidating responses experienced significantly higher levels of negative affect, heart rate, and skin conductance over time when compared to participants exposed to validating responses. Results are discussed as support for models incorporating social factors as key determinants of individual emotional reactivity and regulation.
Article
A Statistical Model for ReliabilitySome Consequences of UnreliabilityThe Simple Replication Reliability StudyThe Control of Unreliability by ReplicationThe Interexaminer Reliability Study
Article
Families and family interactions can play a role in the development (vs. prevention), maintenance (or remediation), and treatment of borderline personality disorder (BPD); and, having a family member with BPD can have a significant impact on family functioning. This paper reviews a transactional model for the development and maintenance of BPD, with implications for treatment, particularly from the perspective of dialectical behavior therapy (DBT). The paper also describes a subset of DBT interventions specifically developed for work with couples and families to turn the destructive “inaccurate expression/invalidation cycle” into the constructive “accurate expression/validation cycle,” which is illustrated by a case example.