Content uploaded by Rachael Gabriel
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Rachael Gabriel on Mar 12, 2015
Content may be subject to copyright.
10
EDUCATIONAL L EADERSHIP / MARCH 2012
Richard L. Allington
and Rachael E. Gabriel
Every child a reader” has been the goal
of instruction, education research, and
reform for at least three decades. We
now know more than ever about how to
accomplish this goal. Yet few students in
the United States regularly receive the best reading
instruction we know how to give.
Instead, despite good intentions, educators often
make decisions about instruction that compromise
or supplant the kind of experiences all children
need to become engaged, successful readers. This is
especially true for struggling readers, who are much
less likely than their peers to participate in the kinds
of high-quality instructional activities that would
ensure that they learn to read.
Six Elements for Every Child
Here, we outline six elements of instruction that
every child should experience every day. Each of
these elements can be implemented in any district
and any school, with any curriculum or set of mate-
rials, and without additional funds. All that’s nec-
essary is for adults to make the decision to do it.
1. Every child reads something he or she chooses.
The research base on student-selected reading
is robust and conclusive: Students read more,
understand more, and are more likely to continue
reading when they have the opportunity to choose
what they read. In a 2004 meta-analysis, Guthrie
and Humenick found that the two most powerful
instructional design factors for improving reading
motivation and comprehension were (1)student
access to many books and (2)personal choice of
what to read.
Every Child,
Every Day
The six elements of effective reading instruction don’t require
much time or money—just educators’ decision to put them in place.
Allington.indd 10 2/6/12 1:41 PM
ASCD / WWW.ASCD.ORG
11
We’re not saying that students should never read
teacher- or district-selected texts. But at some time
every day, they should be able to choose what they
read.
The experience of choosing in itself boosts moti-
vation. In addition, offering choice makes it more
likely that every reader will be matched to a text
that he or she can read well. If students initially
have trouble choosing texts that match their ability
level and interest, teachers can provide limited
choices to guide them toward successful reading
experiences. By giving students these opportunities,
we help them develop the ability to choose appro-
priate texts for themselves—a skill that dramati-
cally increases the likelihood they will read outside
school (Ivey & Broaddus, 2001, Reis et al., 2007).
Some teachers say they find it difficult to provide
a wide selection of texts because of budget con-
straints. Strangely, there is always money available
© TIM PANNELL/CORBIS
Allington.indd 11 2/7/12 8:02 AM
12
Educational l EadErship / March 2012
for workbooks, photocopying, and com-
puters; yet many schools claim that they
have no budget for large, multileveled
classroom libraries. This is interesting
because research has demonstrated that
access to self-selected texts improves
students’ reading performance (Krashen,
2011), whereas no evidence indicates
that workbooks, photocopies, or com-
puter tutorial programs have ever done
so (Cunningham & Stanovich, 1998;
Dynarski, 2007).
There is, in fact, no way they ever
could. When we consider that the
typical 4th grade classroom has students
reading anywhere from the 2nd to the
9th grade reading levels (and that later
grades have an even wider range), the
idea that one workbook or textbook
could meet the needs of every reader is
absurd (Hargis, 2006). So, too, is the
idea that skills developed through iso-
lated, worksheet-based skills practice
and fill-in-the-blank vocabulary quizzes
will transfer to real reading in the
absence of any evidence that they ever
have. If school principals eliminated
the budget for workbooks and work-
sheets and instead spent the money on
real books for classroom libraries, this
decision could dramatical ly improve
students’ opportunities to become better
readers.
2. Every child reads accurately.
Good readers read with accuracy
almost all the time. The last 60 years of
research on optimal text difficulty—a
body of research that began with Betts
(1949)—consistently demonstrates
the importance of having students
read texts they can read accurately and
understand. In fact, research shows
that reading at 98percent or higher
accuracy is essential for reading accel-
eration. Anything less slows the rate
of improvement, and anything below
90percent accuracy doesn’t improve
reading ability at all (Allington, 2012;
Ehri, Dreyer, Flugman, & Gross, 2007).
Although the idea that students read
better when they read more has been
supported by studies for the last 70
years, policies that simply increase the
amount of time allocated for students to
read often find mixed results (National
Reading Panel, 2000). The reason is
simple: It’s not just the time spent with
a book in hand, but rather the intensity
and volume of high-success reading,
that determines a student’s progress in
learning to read (Allington, 2009; Kuhn
et al., 2006).
When students read accurately,
they solidify their word-recognition,
decoding, and word-analysis skills.
Perhaps more important, they are likely
to understand what they read—and, as
a result, to enjoy reading.
In contrast, struggling students who
spend the same amount of time reading
texts that they can’t read accurately are
at a disadvantage in several important
ways. First, they read less text; it’s slow
going when you encounter many words
you don’t recognize instantly. Second,
struggling readers are less likely to
understand (and therefore enjoy) what
they read. They are likely to become
frustrated when reading these difficult
texts and therefore to lose confidence in
their word-attack, decoding, or word-
recognition skills. Thus, a struggling
reader and a successful reader who
engage in the same 15-minute inde-
pendent reading session do not neces-
sarily receive equivalent practice, and
they are likely to experience different
outcomes.
Sadly, struggling readers typical ly
encounter a steady diet of too-
challenging texts throughout the school
day as they make their way through
classes that present grade-level material
hour after hour. In essence, traditional
instructional practices widen the gap
between readers.
3. Every child reads something
he or she understands.
Understanding what you’ve read is the
goal of reading. But too often, struggling
readers get interventions that focus on
basic skills in isolation, rather than on
Allington.indd 12 2/6/12 1:42 PM
ASCD / www.ASCD.org
13
reading connected text for
meaning. This common
misuse of intervention
time often arises from a
grave mis interpretation
of what we know about
reading difficulties.
The findings of neuro-
logical research are some-
times used to reinforce the
notion that some students
who struggle to learn to
read are simply “wired dif-
ferently” (Zambo, 2003)
and thus require large
amounts of isolated basic
skills practice. In fact, this
same research shows that
remediation that empha-
sizes comprehension can
change the structure of
struggling students’ brains.
Keller and Just (2009)
used imaging to examine
the brains of struggling
readers before and after
they received 100 hours of reme-
diation—including lots of reading and
rereading of real texts. The white matter
of the struggling readers was of lower
structural quality than that of good
readers before the intervention, but it
improved following the intervention.
And these changes in the structure of
the brain’s white matter consistently
predicted increases in reading ability.
Numerous other studies (Aylward
et al., 2003; Krafnick, Flowers, Napo-
liello, & Eden, 2011; Shaywitz et al.,
2004) have supported Keller and Just’s
findings that comprehensive reading
instruction is associated with changed
activation patterns that mirror those
of typical readers. These studies show
that it doesn’t take neurosurgery or
banging away at basic skills to enable
the brain to develop the ability to read:
It takes lots of reading and rereading
of text that students find engaging and
comprehensible.
The findings from brain research
align well with what we’ve learned
from studies of reading interven-
tions. Regardless of their focus, target
population, or publisher, interventions
that accelerate reading development
routine ly devote at least two-thirds of
their time to reading and rereading
rather than isolated or contrived skill
practice (Allington, 2011). These
findings have been consistent for the
last 50 years—yet the typical reading
intervention used in schools today has
struggling readers spending the bulk of
their time on tasks other than reading
and rereading actual texts.
Studies of exemplary elementary
teachers further support the finding
that more authentic reading develops
better readers (Allington, 2002; Taylor,
Pearson, Peterson, & Rodriguez, 2003).
In these large-scale national studies,
researchers found that students in more-
effective teachers’ classrooms spent a
larger percentage of reading instruc-
tional time actually reading; students
in less-effective teachers’ classrooms
spent more time using worksheets,
answering low-level, literal questions,
or complet ing before-and-after reading
activities. In addition, exemplary
teachers were more likely to differentiate
instruction so that all readers had books
they could actually read accurate ly,
fluent ly, and with understanding.
4. Every child writes about something
personally meaningful.
In our observations in schools across
several states, we rarely see students
writing anything more than fill-in-the-
blank or short-answer responses during
their reading block. Those who do have
the opportunity to compose something
longer than a few sentences are either
responding to a teacher-selected prompt
or writing within a strict structural
formula that turns even paragraphs and
essays into fill-in-the-blank exercises.
As adults, we rarely if ever write to
a prompt, and we almost never write
about something we don’t know about.
Writing is called composition for a good
reason: We actually compose (construct
something unique) when we write. The
opportunity to compose continuous text
about something meaningful is not just
something nice to have when there’s
free time after a test or at the end of the
school year. Writing provides a different
modality within which to practice the
skills and strategies of reading for an
authentic purpose.
When students write about some-
thing they care about, they use conven-
tions of spelling and grammar because
it matters to them that their ideas are
communicated, not because they will
lose points or see red ink if they don’t
(Cunningham & Cunningham, 2010).
They have to think about what words
will best convey their ideas to their
readers. They have to encode these
words using letter patterns others will
recognize. They have to make sure they
use punctuation in a way that will help
their readers understand which words
go together, where a thought starts and
ends, and what emotion goes with it.
They have to think about what they
know about the structure of similar
texts to set up their page and organize
their ideas. This process is especially
important for struggling readers because
Students read more, understand more, and are
more likely to continue reading when they have
the opportunity to choose what they read.
© Monalyn Gracia/corbis
Allington.indd 13 2/6/12 1:43 PM
14
Educational l EadErship / March 2012
it produces a comprehensible text
that the student can read, reread, and
analyze.
5. Every child talks with peers about
reading and writing.
Research has demonstrated that con-
versation with peers improves compre-
hension and engagement with texts in a
variety of settings (Cazden, 1988). Such
literary conversation does not focus
on recalling or retelling what students
read. Rather, it asks students to analyze,
comment, and compare—in short, to
think about what they’ve read. Fall,
Webb, and Chudowsky (2000) found
better outcomes when kids simply
talked with a peer about what they read
than when they spent the same amount
of class time highlighting important
information after reading.
Similarly, Nystrand (2006) reviewed
the research on engaging students in lit-
erate conversations and noted that even
small amounts of such conversation (10
minutes a day) improved standardized
test scores, regardless of students’ family
background or reading level. Yet strug-
gling readers were the least likely to
discuss daily what they read with peers.
This was often because they were doing
extra basic-skills practice instead. In
class discussions, struggling readers
were more likely to be asked literal
questions about what they had read,
to prove they “got it,” rather than to be
engaged in a conversation about the
text.
Time for students to talk about their
reading and writing is perhaps one
of the most underused, yet easy-to-
implement, elements of instruction. It
doesn’t require any special materials,
special training, or even large amounts
of time. Yet it provides measurable ben-
efits in comprehension, motivation, and
even language competence. The task of
switching between writing, speaking,
reading, and listening helps students
make connections between, and thus
solidify, the skills they use in each. This
makes peer conversation especially
important for English language learners,
another population that we rarely ask to
talk about what they read.
6. Every child listens to a fluent
adult read aloud.
Listening to an adult model fluent
reading increases students’ own fluency
and comprehension skills (Trelease,
2001), as well as expanding their vocab-
ulary, background knowledge, sense
of story, awareness of genre and text
structure, and comprehension of the
texts read (Wu & Samuels, 2004).
Yet few teachers above 1st grade
read aloud to their students every day
(Jacobs, Morrison, & Swinyard, 2000).
This high-impact, low-input strategy
is another underused component of
the kind of instruction that supports
readers. We categorize it as low-input
because, once again, it does not require
special materials or training; it simply
requires a decision to use class time
more effectively. Rather than conducting
whole-class reading of a single text that
fits few readers, teachers should choose
to spend a few minutes a day reading to
their students.
Things That Really Matter
Most of the classroom instruction
we have observed lacks these six
research-based elements. Yet it’s not
difficult to find the time and resources
to implement them. Here are a few
suggestions.
First, eliminate almost all worksheets
and workbooks. Use the money saved to
purchase books for classroom libraries;
use the time saved for self-selected
reading, self-selected writing, literary
conversations, and read-alouds.
Second, ban test-preparation activities
and materials from the school day.
Although sales of test preparation
materials provide almost two-thirds of
the profit that testing companies earn
(Glovin & Evans, 2006), there are no
studies demonstrating that engaging stu-
dents in test prep ever improved their
reading proficiency—or even their test
performance (Guthrie, 2002). As with
eliminating workbook completion, elim-
inating test preparation provides time
and money to spend on the things that
really matter in develop ing readers.
It’s time for the elements of effective
instruction described here to be offered
more consistently to every child, in
© SUSIE FITZHUGH
Allington.indd 14 2/10/12 8:54 AM
ASCD / www.ASCD.org
15
every school, every day. Remember,
adults have the power to make these
decisions; kids don’t. Let’s decide to
give them the kind of instruction they
need. EL
References
Allington, R. L. (2002). What I’ve learned
about effective reading instruction from a
decade of studying exemplary elementary
classroom teachers. Phi Delta Kappan,
83(10), 740–747.
Allington, R. L. (2009). If they don’t read
much...30 years later. In E. H. Hiebert
(Ed.), Reading more, reading better
(pp.30–54). New York: Guilford.
Allington, R. L. (2011). Research on reading/
learning disability interventions. In S. J.
Samuels & A. E. Farstrup (Eds.), What
research has to say about reading instruction
(4th ed., pp. 236–265). Newark, DE:
International Reading Association.
Allington, R. L. (2012). What really matters
for struggling readers: Designing research-
based programs (3rd ed.). Boston: Allyn
and Bacon.
Aylward, E. H., Richards, T. L., Berninger,
V.W., Nagy, W. E., Field, K. M.,
Grimme, A. C., Richards, A. L., Thomson,
J. B., & Cramer, S. C. (2003). Instruc-
tional treatment associated with changes
in brain activation in children with dys-
lexia. Neurology, 61(2), E5–6.
Betts, E. A. (1949). Adjusting instruction to
individual needs. In N. B. Henry (Ed.),
The forty-eighth yearbook of the National
Society for the Study of Education: Part II,
Reading in the elementary school (pp.266–
283). Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
Cazden, C. B. (1988). Classroom discourse:
The language of teaching and learning.
Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E.
(1998). The impact of print exposure
on word recognition. In J. Metsala & L.
Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning
literacy (pp. 235–262). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Cunningham, P. M., & Cunningham,
J. W. (2010). What really matters in
writing: Research-based practices across the
elementary curriculum. Boston: Allyn and
Bacon.
Dynarski, M. (2007). Effectiveness of reading
and mathematics software products: Findings
from the first student cohort. Washington,
DC: Institute for Education Sciences, U.S.
Department of Education. Retrieved from
http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/pubs/20074005
Ehri, L. C., Dreyer, L. G., Flugman, B., &
Gross, A. (2007). Reading Rescue: An
effective tutoring intervention model for
language minority students who are strug-
gling readers in first grade. American Edu-
cational Research Journal, 44(2), 414–448.
Fall, R., Webb, N. M., & Chudowsky, N.
(2000). Group discussion and large-scale
language arts assessment: Effects on stu-
dents’ comprehension. American Educa-
tional Research Journal, 37(4), 911–941.
Glovin, D., & Evans, D. (2006, December).
How test companies fail your kids.
Bloomberg Markets, 127–138. Retrieved
from http://timeoutfromtesting .org/
bloomberg_education.pdf
Guthrie, J. T. (2002). Preparing students
for high-stakes test taking in reading. In
A. Farstrup & S. J. Samuels (Eds.), What
research has to say about reading instruction
(pp. 370–391). Newark, DE: Inter national
Reading Association.
Guthrie, J. T., & Humenick, N. M. (2004).
Motivating students to read: Evidence for
classroom practices that increase moti-
vation and achievement. In P. McCardle
& V. Chhabra (Eds.), The voice of evidence
in reading research (pp. 329–354). Bal-
timore: Paul Brookes.
Hargis, C. (2006). Setting standards: An
exercise in futility? Phi Delta Kappan,
87(5), 393–395.
Ivey, G., & Broaddus, K. (2001). Just plain
reading: A survey of what makes students
want to read in middle schools. Reading
Research Quarterly, 36, 350–377.
Jacobs, J. S., Morrison, T. G., & Swinyard,
W. R. (2000). Reading aloud to students:
A national probability study of classroom
reading practices of elementary school
teachers. Reading Psychology, 21(3),
171–193.
Keller, T. A., & Just, M. A. (2009). Altering
cortical activity: Remediation-induced
changes in the white matter of poor
readers. Neuron, 64(5), 624–631.
Krafnick, A. J., Flowers, D. L., Napoliello,
E. M., & Eden, G. F. (2011). Gray matter
volume changes following reading inter-
vention in dyslexic children. Neuroimage,
57(3), 733–741.
Krashen, S. (2011). Free voluntary reading.
Santa Barbara, CA: Libraries Unlimited.
Kuhn, M. R., Schwanenflugel, P., Morris,
R.D., Morrow, L. M., Woo, D., Meis-
inger, B., et al. (2006). Teaching children
to become fluent and automatic readers.
Journal of Literacy Research, 38(4),
357–388.
National Reading Panel. (2000). Teaching
children to read: An evidence-based
assessment of the scientific research literature
on reading and its implications for reading
instruction. Rockville, MD: National
Institutes of Child Health and Human
Development. Retrieved from www
.nationalreadingpanel .org/publications
/summary.htm
Nystrand, M. (2006). Research on the role of
classroom discourse as it affects reading
comprehension. Research in the Teaching of
English, 40, 392–412.
Reis, S. M., McCoach, D. B., Coyne, M.
Schreiber, F. J., Eckert, R. D., & Gubbins,
E. J. (2007). Using planned enrichment
strategies with direct instruction to
improve reading fluency, comprehension,
and attitude toward reading: An evidence-
based study. Elementary School Journal,
108(1), 3–24.
Shaywitz, B., Shaywitz, S., Blachman, B.,
Pugh, K., Fulbright, R. K., Skudlarski, P.,
et al. (2004). Development of left occipto-
temporal systems for skilled reading
in children after phonologically based
intervention. Biological Psychiatry, 55(9),
926–933.
Taylor, B. M., Pearson, P. D., Peterson, D.S.,
& Rodriguez, M. C. (2003). Reading
growth in high-poverty classrooms:
The influence of teacher practices that
encourage cognitive engagement in lit-
eracy learning. Elementary School Journal,
104, 3–28.
Trelease, J. (2001). Read-aloud handbook (5th
ed.). New York: Viking-Penguin.
Wu, Y., & Samuels, S. J. (2004, May). How
the amount of time spent on independent
reading affects reading achievement. Paper
presented at the annual convention of the
International Reading Association, Reno,
Nevada.
Zambo, D. (2003). The importance of pro-
viding scientific information to children
with dyslexia. Dyslexia [online magazine].
Retrieved from Dyslexia Parents Resource
at www.dyslexia-parent.com/mag47.html
Richard L. Allington is a professor at
the University of Tennessee in Knoxville;
richardallington@aol.com. Rachael E.
Gabriel is assistant professor at the Uni-
versity of Connecticut in Storrs; rachael
.gabriel@uconn.edu.
First, eliminate
almost all worksheets
and workbooks.
Allington.indd 15 2/6/12 1:43 PM
Copyright of Educational Leadership is the property of Association for Supervision & Curriculum Development
and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright
holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.