Available via license: CC BY 3.0
Content may be subject to copyright.
Fourteenth Annual Conference of the
International Society for Intelligence Research (ISIR)
Melbourne, Australia
December 12-14, 2013
Julian C. Stanley
Swinburne University of Technology
Hawthorn Campus, Advanced Technology Centre
PO Box 218, Hawthorn
Victoria, Australia 3122
Phone: +613 9214 8000
Fax: +613 9819 5454
i
Acknowledgements
2013 ISIR Board of Directors
David Lubinski, President
Vanderbilt University
Aljoscha Neubauer, President Elect
University of Graz
Linda Gottfredson, Past President
University of Delaware
Keith Widaman, Board Member (exp. 2013)
University of California, Davis
Richard Haier, Board Member (exp. 2014), Secretary-Treasurer
University of California, Irvine
Yulia Kovas, Board Member (exp. 2015)
Goldsmiths, University of London
The Board would like to thank all the ISIR committees for their hard work
in making this conference possible.
We also thank Elsevier for helping to sponsor the Reception.
ii
2013 ISIR Committees
Program Committee
Yulia Dodonova, Chair
Tom Coyle, Harrison Kell, Paul Irwing, Yulia Kovas
Conference Site Committee
Con Stough & Andrew Scholey, Co-Chairs
Distinguished Contributor Interview and Keynote Speaker Committee
Rich Haier, Chair
Buz Hunt, Jelte Wickerts, Doug Detterman, & Sherif Karama
Holden Memorial Address on Science Writing & Website Committee
Tim Bates, Chair
Jonathan Wai, Linda Gottfredson
Lifetime Achievement Award Committee
Tim Keith, Chair
Robert Plomin, Yulia Kovas, Wendy Johnson, Mike McDaniel
Nominations and Elections Committee
Aljoscha Neubauer, Chair
Heiner Rindermann, Jelte Wickerts, Rex Jung, David Schroeder
President’s Symposia Committee
David Lubinski, Chair
Student Engagement Committee
Magda Chmiel, Chair
Linda Gottfredson
Student Awards Committee
Matthew Reynolds, Chair
Keith Widaman & Roberto Colom
Graduate Student Presentation Judges
Meredith Frey, Chair
Ted Nettelbeck, Lazar Stankov, Matthew Reynolds, & Keith Widaman
Best Poster Judges
Tom Coyle, Chair
Doug Detterman
iii
In Memoriam
Joseph F. Fagan, III
1941-2013
iv
v
vi
1
Short schedule for ISIR 2013
(#) – Page of Abstract
DAY 1: Thursday, Dec. 12
@BA201
8:20–8:30
Opening Remarks
George Collins
8:30–9:30
Lifetime Achievement
Award:
Linda Gottfredson (90)
9:30–10:00
Break
10:00–11:20
Talks: Basic Predictors
10:00–10:20
Neubauer
(51)
Neural
Efficiency
10:20–10:40
Frey (41)
Perceived Workload
10:40–11:00
Giofrè (43)
Working Memory
11:00–11:20
Mosing (50)
Musical Discrimination&IQ
11:20–12:20
Dodonova &Dodonov (60)
Worst Performance Rule
Symp.
11:20–11:40
Dodonova (61)
Sources of WPR
11:40–12:00
Borter (62)
WPR & Task Difficulty
12:00–12:20
Dodonov (63)
WPR & Accuracy
12:20–1:50
Lunch
Luncheon for Awardees
1:50–2:50
President's Invited
Address:
Nick Martin (92)
2:50–4:10
Talks: Genetics
2:50–3:10
Bates (38)
SES & Heritability of IQ
3:10–3:30
Hill (44)
Functional SNPs & IQ
3:30–3:50
Luciano (47)
Rare Genetic Variants & g
3:50–4:10
Plomin (52)
Genetics of Low IQ
4:10–4:40
Break
4:40–5:30
Business Meeting
5:30–6:30
Interview:
Nicholas Mackintosh
6:30–9:00
Elsevier Reception,
Poster Sessions
(see the next page)
DAY 2: Friday, Dec. 13
@BA201
7:00–8:20
Student breakfast with
Lifetime Achievement
Awardee
8:20–10:00
Stankov & Nettelbeck (64)
Confidence & IQ Symp.
8:20–8:40
Stankov (65)
Non
-Cognitive Predictors
8:40–9:00
Kleitman (66)
IQ & Confidence
9:00–9:20
Jackson (67)
Decision-Making
9:20–9:40
Lee (68)
Confidence & Achievement
9:40–10:00
Welsh (69)
Anchoring
10:00–10:30
Break
10:30–11:30
Talks: g &Special Abilities
10:30–10:50
Gignac (42)
Mutualism Theory of g
10:50–11:10
Coyle (39)
Ability Tilt on SAT & ACT
11:10–11:30
Song (55)
IQ, Creativity & Processing
11:30–12:30
Pase, Stough, & Bates (70)
CHC Taxonomy Symp.
11:30–11:50
Pase (71)
Cattell-Horn-Caroll Model
11:50–12:10
Stough(72)
Smart Drug Effects
12:10–12:30
Scholey (73)
Natural Medicines & CHC
12:30–2:00
Lunch
2:00–2:40
Talks: Life Outcomes
2:00–2:20
de Manzano (49)
Creative Achievement
2:20–2:40
Kell (45)
Life Outcomes in Gifted
2:40–3:40
Talks: Health & Aging
2:40–3:00
Sörberg (56)
IQ, Health & Mortality
3:00–3:20
Macpherson (48)
Dietary Interventions
3:20–3:40
Ritchie (54)
Alcohol
Consumption
3:40–4:10
Break
4:10–5:30
Arden & Plotnik (74)
IQ in Non-Humans Symp.
4:10–4:30
Arden (
75)
g
in Dogs
4:30–5:10
Plotnik (76)
The Elephant Model
5:10–5:30
Taylor (77)
Tool-Making Crows
5:30-6:30
Keynote Address
Randall Engle (93)
6:30-9:00
Conference Banquet
DAY 3: Saturday, Dec. 14
@ATC101
8:20–8:40
Talk: Survey of Expert
Opinion
Rindermann (53)
8:40–9:40
Holden Memorial
Address on Science
Writing:
Elizabeth Finkel (94)
9:40–10:10
Break
10:10–12:00
President's Symposium
(83)
10:10–10:35
Lubinski (84)
SMPY 40 Years Later
10:35–10:50
Makel (85)
Profound Intellectual Talent
10:50–11:10
Plomin (86)
Genetics of High Abilities
11:10–11:30
Tellier (87)
DNA Sequencing
11:30–11:50
Lee (88)
Fourth Law of Behavior
Genetics
11:50–12:00
Q&A
12:00–1:30
Lunch
1:30–2:50
Kovas & Malykh (78)
Self-Concept, Motivation
& Achievement Symp
1:30–1:50
Kovas (79)
Motivation to Learn
1:50–2:10
Morosanova (80)
Self-Regulation
2:10–2:30
Ovcharova (81)
Abilities in Different School
Populations
2:30–2:50
Voronin (82)
Academic Self
-Concept
2:50–3:20
Break
3:20–4:20
Talks: Other Associations
3:20–3:40
Ledovaya (46)
Metaphors & IQ
3:40–4:00
Findlay (40)
Emotional Intelligence
4:00–4:20
Woodley (57)
Selection & Dysgenics
4:20–4:40
Carroll Student Award
ISIR Student Award
Best Poster Award
2
International Society for Intelligence Research (ISIR)
2013 Program: Posters
Thursday. December 12
6:30 – 9:00 P.M.
(#) – Page of Abstract
P1: Posters, generally about Structure and Measurement
P1.1:
What one intelligence test measures: The distinct roles of working memory and rule
induction in matrix reasoning. Patrick Loesche* & Marcus Hasselhorn (23)
P1.2:
Does education
boost general intelligence (g) or specific cognitive abilities?
Stuart J. Ritchie* et al. (29)
P1.3:
Structure of the Woodcock
-
Johnson III Cognitive in a
Clinical Sample.
Marley W.
Watkins & Tracy Strickland (35)
P1.4:
Cross
-
Cultural Validity of
IQ Tests among African Children: Artificial or Accurate
Measure of Intelligence? Cissé Dramé* (20)
P1.5:
Investigating causal direction in neuroimaging studies of cognitive ability; an
application of non-normal structural equation modelling. Tom Booth et al. (17)
P1.
6
:
Outliers Matter, Distance
-
Based Weighting Helps:Some
Examples Based on Speed
-
Ability Associations. Yury S. Dodonov & Yulia A. Dodonova (19)
P2: Posters, generally about
Effects of Substances on Cognition
P2.1:
Low levels of lead exposure
may impact children
's general
intelligence
.
Rachel M. Earl et al.(21)
P2.2:
Bacopa monnieri in child and adolescent populations with and without developmental
dysfunction. James D Kean* et al. (22)
P2.3:
The chronic
effects of Bacopa, Pycnogenol
and an antioxidant/micronutrient
combination formula on cognitive and cardiovascular function in a healthy older
population. Con Stough et al. (33)
P2.4:
An investigation of the
influence of antioxidant defense genes on cognitive ability in
older individuals. Karen J. Nolidin* et al. (28)
P2.5:
The cognitive enhancing effects of acute and chronic curcumin supplementation in a
healthy elderly population. Katherine H. M. Cox* et al. (18)
P2.
6
:
A Preliminary Study
Investigating the Effects of
Resveratrol on Cognitive Function.
Andrew Scholey et al. (31)
P2.
7
:
Neurocognitive effects of
multivitamin supplementation: A behavioural and fMRI
study. David J. White et al. (36)
3
Posters
-
cont.
P2.8:
Cognitive Performance Following Alcohol and
Energy Drink
.
Sarah Benson* et al. (15)
P2.
9
:
Effects of alcohol consumption on academic performance
.
Joris C. Verster
et al.
(
34
)
P3: Posters, generally about
Emotional
Intelligence
, Personality and
Metacognition
P3.1:
Emotional
Intelligence and
Scholastic Achievement in Preadolescent Children
.
Clare E W Billings et al. (16)
P3.2:
Adolescent Peer
-
Relations and Emotional
Intelligence
.
Justine
E. Lomas
et
al.
(2
4
)
P3.3:
Emotional Intelligence, victimisation,
bullying behaviours and attitud
es: a
replication
and extension. Chantelle M. Schokman et al. (30)
P3.4:
Challenging the intelligence compensation theory: Are conscientiousness and
intelligence really negatively correlated? Aja L. Murray* et al. (27)
P3.5:
Cognitive abilities,
self
-
regulation and academic achievements of gifted students
.
Varvara I. Morosanova et al. (26)
P3.
6
:
Knowing the knowing: Exploring the relationship between metacognition and
intelligence. John H. Song & Brooke Nickeas (32)
P
4
: Posters, generally about Intelligence
in a Broader Context
P
4
.1:
The role of Intelligence on the human development Tatiana A. Badaró* (13)
P
4
.2:
Expert opinion on the causes of international differences in intelligence
–
2013 survey
of expert opinion on intelligence. David Becker* et al. (14)
P
4
.3:
The Rule
-
Dependence Model: a New Model for Secular IQ Gains
.
Elijah Z. Armstrong & Michael A. Woodley (12)
P
4
.4:
Investigating relationships between cognition and life history in a sample of 6364
individuals. Guy Madison et al. (25)
Reception and poster sessions will be held at Hawthorn/Library Atrium
4
International Society for Intelligence Research (ISIR)
2013 Program: Talks
(#) – Page of Abstract
Thursday, December 12 (Day 1), BA201
8:
2
0
–
8:
3
0 AM
Opening
Ceremony
Professor George Collins, Deputy Vice Chancellor for Research, Swinburne
University
8:30
–
9:30 AM
Lifetime Achievement Award:
Linda
Gottfredson
Empirical Treasure, Lost and Found (90)
9:30
–
10:00 AM
Coffee Break
10:00
–
11:20 AM
T1: Talks, generally about basic predictors
Chair:
Aljoscha C. Neubauer
10:00
–
10:20 AM
T1.1: Neural efficienc
y as a function of task demands
.
Aljoscha C. Neubauer et al. (51)
10:20
–
10:40 AM
T1.2:
Perceived workload and performance in difficult nonverbal cognitive
tasks predict academic achievement. Meredith C. Frey & Cynthia Laurie-Rose
(41)
10:40
–
11:00 AM
T1.3:
The structure of working memory and how it relates to intelligence in
children. David Giofrè et al. (43)
11:00
–
11:20 AM
T1.4: Genetic and environmental influences on the relationship between
musical discrimination tasks and IQ. Miriam A. Mosing et al. (50)
11:20
–
12:2
0 AM
Symposium 1:
Worst performance rule: sources, moderating variables
and implications for intelligence research (60)
Organizers: Yulia A. Dodonova & Yury S. Dodonov
11:20
–
11:40 AM
S1.
1
:
Are slowest response times more g
-
loaded
than fastest reactions?
Evidence, hypotheses and implications of worst performance rule.
Yulia A. Dodonova et al. (61)
11:40
–
12:00 AM
S1.
2
:
Cognitive tasks with increasing complexity: should worst performance
rule be taken into account? Natalie Borter* et al. (62)
12
:
0
0
–
12
:
2
0 AM
S1.
3
:
Slowest and fastest responses are not equally good predictors of
intelligence, but does accuracy matter? Yury S. Dodonov et al. (63)
12:20
–
1:50 PM
Lunch
Luncheon hosted by ISIR officers for Lifetime Achievement Awardee,
Distinguished Contributions Awardee, Keynote Speaker, President's
Invited Speaker and Holden Science Writer
Glenferrie Hotel
324 Burwood Road
5
Thursday
-
cont.
1:50
–
2:50 PM
President's Invited Address:
Nick Martin
(9
2
)
Emerging Evidence on the Molecular Genetics of Cognition
2
:
5
0
–
4
:10
PM
T2:
Talks, generally about genetic
s
Chair:
Yulia Kovas
2
:
5
0
–
3
:
1
0
P
M
T2.1:
When does socioeconomic status moderate the heritability of IQ? Data
from Australia and the USA. Timothy C. Bates et al. (38)
3
:
1
0
–
3
:
3
0
P
M
T2.
2
:
Do functional SNPs show an enriched association for intelligence?
W. David Hill* et al. (44)
3:30
–
3:50 P
M
T2.3:
A pilot study of rare genetic variants and g.
Michelle Luciano
et al.
(
47
)
3:50
–
4:10 PM
T2.4:
The
genetics of low IQ and diagnosed mental retardation: 370,000
sibling pairs and 9000 twin pairs. Robert Plomin et al. (52)
4:
1
0
–
4:
40 PM
Coffee Break
4:
4
0
–
5:30 PM
Business Meeting
5:30
–
6:30 PM
Interview with recipient of Distinguished Contributor
Award:
Nicholas Mackintosh
6:30
–
9
:
0
0 PM
Elsevier Reception and Posters (11
-
3
6
)
Hawthorn/Library Atrium
6
Friday, December 13 (Day 2), BA201
7:00
–
8:20 AM
Student breakfast with Lifetime Achievement Awardee
Linda Gottfredson
Quest Hawthorn 616 Glenferrie Road
8:20
–
10:00 AM
Symposium 2:
The relationship between confidence, intelligence,
academic achievement and decision-making (64)
Organizers: Lazar Stankov & Ted Nettelbeck
8:20
–
8
:40 AM
S2.1:
Noncognitive
predictors of intelligence and academic achievement
.
Lazar Stankov (65)
8:40
–
9:00 AM
S2.2:
Intelligence and confidence in relationship to competence, arrogance
and close-mindedness. Sabina Kleitman (66)
9:00
–
9:20 AM
S2.3:
Intelligence and
confidence as respective predictors of quality and
erroneous decision-making.
Simon A Jackson* & Sabina Kleitman (67)
9:20
–
9:40 AM
S2.4:
Confidence: a better predictor of academic achievement than self
-
efficacy, self-concept and anxiety?
Lazar Stankov, Jihyun Lee et al.(68)
9:40
–
10:00 AM
S2.5:
Individual differences in anchoring: Traits and experience
Matthew B
Welsh et al. (69)
10:00
–
10:30 AM
Coffee Break
10:30
–
11:30 AM
T
3
: Talks, generally about
g and special abilities
Chair: Linda Gottfredson
10:30
–
10:50 AM
T
3
.1:
An Empirical Test of the Dynamic Mutualism Theory of g
.
Gilles E. Gignac (42)
10:50
–
11:10 AM
T
3
.2:
Validity of Ability Tilt on the SAT and ACT
.
Thomas R. Coyle et al. (39)
11:10
–
11:30 AM
T
3
.3:
Exploring
the relationship between intelligence, creativity, inspection
time, and inhibition. John H. Song & John G. Chetwynd (55)
11:30
–
12:30 AM
Symposium 3:
Contemporary applications of the Cattell
-
Horn
-
Carroll
cognitive taxonomy (70)
Organizers: Mathew Pase, Con Stough & Timothy C Bates
11:30
–
11:50 AM
S3.1:
An overview of the Cattell
-
Horn
-
Caroll model of cognition:
implications for contemporary use. Mathew Pase* & Con Stough (71)
11:50
–
12:10 AM
S3.2:
Understanding smart drug effects using the Cattell
-
Horn
-
Caroll model
of cognition. Con Stough et al. (72)
12:10
–
12:30 AM
S3.3:
Understanding the effects of natural medicines and substances using
the Cattell-Horn-Caroll model of cognition.
Andrew Scholey et al. (73)
7
Friday
–
cont.
12:30
–
2:00 PM
Lunch
2:00
–
2
:
40 PM
T4: Talks, generally about life outcomes
Chair:
Aljoscha C. Neubauer
2:00
–
2:20 PM
T4
.1:
Associations between creative achievement in scientific and artistic
domains, intelligence, personality and sex. Örjan de Manzano et al. (49)
2:20
–
2:40 PM
T4
.2:
"Wrecked by Success?" No! Linkages of Career Accomplishment,
Health, and Relationship Satisfaction among the Gifted.
Harrison J. Kell et al. (45)
2:40
–
3:40 PM
T5: Talks, generally about health and aging
Chair:
Aljoscha C.
Neubauer
2:40
–
3:00 PM
T5
.
1
:
Intelligence and somatic health in early adulthood, and mortality up to
age 59 -a longitudinal study of 49 000 men. Alma Sörberg* et al. (56)
3:00
–
3:20 PM
T5
.
2
:
Dietary interventions to offset cognitive decline.
Helen N. Macpherson et al. (48)
3:20
–
3:40 PM
T5.3:
Alcohol consumption and lifetime change in cognitive ability: A
Mendelian randomization study. Stuart J. Ritchie* et al. (54)
3:
4
0
–
4
:
10 PM
Coffee Break
4
:
1
0
–
5:30 PM
S4
:
Intelligence in non human
animals
(
74
)
Organizers: Rosalind Arden & Joshua Plotnik
4:10
–
4:30 PM
S4.
1
:
Fetch! g in dogs
.
Rosalind Arden
et al.
(
75
)
4:30
–
5
:
1
0 PM
S4.
2
:
Big Brains, Big Smarts? The Elephant Model for The Study of
Convergent Cognitive Evolution Across Species
Joshua M. Plotnik (76)
5:10
–
5:30 PM
S4.3:
What can tool
-
making crows teach us about our minds?
Alex H Taylor (77)
5:30
–
6:30 PM
Keynote Speaker:
Randall W. Engle
(9
3
)
Working memory capacity as a mediating variable.
6:30
–
7:00 PM
Pre
-
D
inner
D
rinks
Deck HawthornCommon 302 Burwood Road
7:00
–
9:00 PM
Conference Banquet
HawthornCommon 302 Burwood Road
8
Saturday, December 14 (Day 3), ATC101
8:
2
0
–
8
:
4
0 AM
T
6
:
Talk, Survey of Expert Opinion
T6.1: 2013 survey of expert opinion on intelligence.
Heiner Rindermann et al. (53)
8
:
4
0
–
9
:
4
0 AM
Holden Memorial Address on Science Writing:
Elizabeth Finkel (94) Navigating the Straits: when Scientists Disagree
9:40
–
10:10 AM
Coffee Break
10
:
1
0
–
1
2
:
00 AM
PS
:
President's Symposium
(
83
)
The Study of Mathematically Precocious Youth, DNA Sequencing,
Genetics, and the Phenotypic Accomplishments of Profound Intellectual
Talent at Midlife
10:10
–
1
0
:
35
AM
PS
.1:
Forty Years Later:What
Happens to Mathematically Precocious Youth
Identified at Age 12? David Lubinski et al. (84)
10:
35
–
1
0
:
50
AM
PS
.2:
Profound Intellectual Talent:
A Compelling Phenotype for Behavioral
Genetics and Neuroscience Inquiry. Matthew C. Makel et al. (85)
10:50
–
11:10 AM
PS.
3
:
The Genetics of High Cognitive Abilities.
Robert Plomin et al.
(
86
)
11:10
–
11:30 AM
PS.
4
:
Petaflop Computer Clusters Crunch Genomes of Giant Minds:
Collection, Sequencing, Analysis of the World’s Largest Extreme-IQ
Genomic Cohort. Laurent Tellier et al. (87)
11:30
–
11:50 AM
PS.5: The Fourth Law of Behavior Genetics.
James J. Lee & Christopher F. Chabris (88)
1
1
:
5
0
–
1
2
:
0
0 AM
Questions & Answers
12:00
–
1:30 PM
Lunch
1
:
3
0
–
2
:
5
0
P
M
Symposium
5
:
Sources of individual differences in academic self
-
concept, motivation, and achievement in different cultures and ability
groups (78)
Organizers: Yulia Kovas & Sergey Malykh
1:30
–
1:50 PM
S
5
.1:
Why
do children differ in motivation to learn? Insights from a large
twin study. Yulia Kovas et al. (79)
1:50
–
2:10 PM
S
5
.2:
Individual differences in conscious self
-
regulation and cognitive
characteristics as predictors of academic achievement
Varvara Morosanova et al. (80)
2:10
–
2:30 PM
S
5
.3:
Is the structure of interrelationships among intelligence, cognitive
abilities and achievement different in selected, self-selected, and unselected
school populations? Olga Ovcharova* et al. (81)
2:30
–
2:50 PM
S
5
.4:
Testing internal/external frame of reference model across academic
domains and school years. Ivan Voronin* et al. (82)
9
Saturday
–
cont.
2:50
–
3:20 PM
Coffee Break
3
:
2
0
–
4
:
20 PM
T
7
: Talks, generally about associations with
other variables
Chair: Aljoscha C. Neubauer
3
:
2
0
–
3
:
40 PM
T
7
.1:
Metaphorical representations of psychological concepts in students and
their relation with verbal and nonverbal intelligence.
Yanina Ledovaya & Ksenia Mikhalchenko (46)
3
:
4
0
–
4:
00 PM
T
7
.2:
Emotional intelligence mediates the relationship between humour
styles and psychological well-being. Bruce Findlay & Robyn Brown (40)
4:
0
0
–
4
:
20 PM
T
7
.3:
Climatic variability, group selection and dysgenics: Testing a multi
-
level selection model. Michael Woodley et al. (57)
4
:
2
0
–
4
:
4
0 PM
Announcement of
ISIR
Best Poster Award
Announcement of ISIR Prize for Best Student Paper
Announcement of John B. Carroll Award for Research Methodology
10
P
O
S
T
E
R
S
POSTERS
Abstracts are organized in alphabetical order
12
The Rule-Dependence Model: a New Model for Secular IQ Gains
Elijah Z. Armstrong
1
, Michael A. Woodley
2
1
Independent Researcher
2
Umeå University
elijahlarmstrong@gmail.com; mawoodley03@gmail.com
Novelty. The audience will learn about a theory linking the ability to process rules to
the Flynn effect. Beforehand, specific proximate causes of the Flynn effect have been
understudied. This presentation identifies a plausible such proximate cause - the ability
to process, infer, reapply, and note the existence of repeated patterns or pieces of
information (for example, solution rules on the Raven's Progressive matrices). The
presentation will cover both general increases in this capacity and exposure to specific
rules that are duplicated on IQ tests.
Importance. The rule-dependence model is a possible cause of the Flynn effect,
which makes it extremely theoretically important. It also identifies specific
environmental factors that may increase rule-processing capacity, and the real-world
effects of these factors.
Methods. We found a sample of studies giving Flynn effects on 14 diverse tests and
subtests. Using the method of correlated vectors, we found that Flynn effect magnitudes
correlated to rule dependence (as rated on a 1-4 point scale) at .597, p < 0.05.
13
P
O
S
T
E
R
S
The Role of Intelligence on the Human Development
Tatiana A. Badaró
Instituto Baiano de Inteligência
badarotatiana@gmail.com
Novelty. This research has established a link between intelligence and Integral human
development. The adopted methodology was a bibliographical research from a survey of
the scientific production of the themes involved, characterized, therefore a literature
review. The State-of-the-art analysis of these two topics, led to confirmation of initial
hypothesis that there is a directly proportional relationship among intelligence and
Integral human development, demonstrating that both implicit theories about
intelligence as those relating to Human Development tend to be incorporated into the
culture as a defining aspect of reality, hence of the socio-educational setting. It is
concluded, therefore, that urges the moment that intelligence can be treated as what it
really is, a human faculty that allows 1) self-knowledge; 2) easy adaptation to the
environment into which it is inserted; and 3) creativity, to then be able to commit the
full human development.
Importance. Intelligence is a subject of the psychology of great social impact and
therefore is characterized as one of the main themes of the area. Overall, society
demonstrates to know concepts and definitions of intelligence, but there is still the
question of what intelligence really is and what its main features.
In the early days of scientific study on this topic, dualists such as Descartes, Spinoza
and Bayle defined Intelligence as a faculty inherent in the spirit, and one of its highest
forms would be the language. The emergence of intelligence is therefore a legacy of
evolution, which, again according to this stream, is not a matter of degree or quantity,
but a qualitative matter (Khalfa, 1996).
Methods. Our work aims to investigate the contributions of theoretical and practical
study of intelligence for the human development, by demonstrating the need, relevance
and urgency of the work of intelligence to it, but mostly we propose the implementation
of educational activities in educational institutions of any levels for the development of
human intelligence.
Therefore we conducted qualitative research as indicated by Rey Gonzalez (1997,
1999, 2004, 2007). It is evident that this was done based on our perception of the
apparent change in conceptions of human development on the part of individuals who
participated in the course Essays on Intelligence, for this reason this course was chosen
by us to be the key point of this discussion.
14
Expert Opinion on the Causes of International Differences in Intelligence – 2013
Survey of Expert Opinion on Intelligence
David Becker
1
, Heiner Rindermann
1
, Thomas R. Coyle
2
1
Department of Psychology, Chemnitz University of Technology, Germany
2
Department of Psychology, University of Texas at San Antonio, San Antonio, USA
david.becker@s2009.tu-chemnitz.de; heiner.rindermann@psychologie.tu-chemnitz.de;
Thomas.Coyle@utsa.edu
Novelty. We examined expert opinions of the causes of international differences in
intelligence using responses from a larger survey (in these questions N≈60; overview on
the 2013 survey of expert opinion on intelligence presented in Rindermann et al. at ISIR
2013). In the present study, expert opinions of international and group differences in
intelligence were evaluated in the following regions and countries: Finland, East Asia,
sub-Sahara Africa, Southern Europe (compared to North-Western-Middle
Europe/NWME), Arabian-Muslim World (compared to NWME), Latin America
(compared to North America), Israel (compared to NWME), Jews in the Western world,
Roma in Europe, and immigrants from the Middle East in Europe. The following causes
of international differences in intelligence were evaluated: culture (religion, tradition,
etc.), genes (evolution), quantity of education, quality of education, wealth, health,
geography, current climate, politics, modernization, sampling error, test knowledge,
discrimination, test bias, and migration (the last three with possible negative or positive
effects).
Importance. Intelligence research addresses global differences in cultural, political,
economic, social and psychological disparities. According to the experts in our sample,
the most important causes of international differences in intelligence were quality of
education, followed by quantity of education, health, wealth, genes, modernization,
culture, test knowledge, politics, sampling error, migration, test bias, geography,
discrimination, and current climate. Group differences in intelligence between specific
regions and ethnic groups were most strongly attributed to differences in education.
Ratings of the causes of intelligence varied across the following countries: Finland
(stronger focus on quality of education), East Asia (relatively strong focus on culture,
quantity of education, and genes), sub-Sahara Africa (relatively strong politics),
Arabian-Muslim World (culture), Roma (culture), Israel (migration, culture, and genes),
and Jews (genes and culture).
Methods. The survey of expert opinions consisted of 62 main questions with follow-
up questions and space for comments. Experts were sampled from publications
addressing intelligence (including Intelligence), cognitive abilities, and student
achievement. Notice of the study was emailed to ISIR members and posted to the ISSID
homepage, and colleagues were asked to inform other researchers about the study. Only
people who received a participation code could participate. The present study was based
on responses from about 60 experts to questions concerning international and group
differences in intelligence. Experts rated the importance of possible causes of
intelligence (e.g., education, genes, and culture) and could provide comments for each
item.
15
P
O
S
T
E
R
S
Cognitive Performance Following Alcohol and Energy Drink
Sarah Benson
1
, Brian Tiplady
2
, Andrew Scholey
1
1
Centre for Human Psychopharmacology, Swinburne University
2
University of Edinburgh
sarahbenson@swin.edu.au; ascholey@swin.edu.au
Novelty. Alcohol consumption has consistently demonstrated impairment to
information processing, particularly demonstrated by an increase in errors made while
processing speed remains unaffected. This relationship is characteristic of a speed-
accuracy trade-off (SATO). Since caffeine enhances cognitive performance, there is
concern that consumers mix alcohol with caffeinated beverages to reverse or mask some
of the impairment effects associated with alcohol intoxication. The current study found
that alcohol increased [processing errors while reaction time remained unaffected. The
addition of the energy drink to the alcohol did not result in any reversal of cognitive
impairment.
Importance. If the addition of energy drink to alcohol masks the effects of
intoxication, consumers may stay out later drinking more alcohol or even feel capable of
driving even though breath alcohol concentrations may be high. Very little research
supports the suggestion that the addition of energy drink to alcohol can reverse
cognitive impairment caused by intoxication, however, this notion is regularly
supported in the media. It is important that consumers are aware of the cognitive effects
to avoid any false expectations.
Methods. This study used a factorial, double-blind, placebo-controlled and crossover
design. Participants were randomised to a treatment sequence consisting of alcohol (0.6
g/kg) energy drink (250 ml), alcohol and energy drink (0.6 g/kg and 250 ml) and
placebo. Participants completed an array of computerised cognitive tasks at baseline and
45, 90 and 180 minutes post treatment.
16
Emotional Intelligence and Scholastic Achievement in Preadolescent Children
Clare E. W. Billings
1
, Justine E. Lomas
1
, Luke Downey
1,2
, Con Stough
1
1
Swinburne University;
2
Swansea University
cbillings@swin.edu.au; jlomas@swin.edu.au; ldowney@swin.edu.au;
cstough@swin.edu.au
Novelty. The results of the current study shows that Emotional Intelligence, a
particular type of intelligence based on Mayer and Salovey’s (1997) model, can be
reliably measured in pre-adolescent children. Emotional Intelligence was also shown to
relate to important educational outcomes for pre-adolescents such as their scholastic
achievement overall and specifically in literacy and numeracy.This is the first study
showing a relationship between an ability based measure of EI for pre-adolescents and
scholastic performance. This suggests that basic emotional processing abilities are
important for the development of knowledge.
Importance. We now know that Emotional Intelligence can be assessed in
preadolescent children and that this assessment relates to important educational
outcomes. This knowledge can help in the assessment of children’s Emotional
Intelligence as part of a holistic assessment of their education development. The ability
to assess Emotional Intelligence in pre-adolescent children also allows us to assess the
impact of Emotional Intelligence development programs for this age group. Improving
educational attainment in primary school children is a vital area for educational
psychology and the community in general.
Methods. The study was correlational in nature, investigating the association between
branches of children’s Emotional Intelligence and outcomes of scholastic performance.
Significant positive correlates were found between all scholastic achievement variables
(literacy, numeracy, overall achievement) and the ‘Understanding and Analysing
Emotions’ branch of the model, suggest that Emotional Intelligence is related to
scholastic achievement in this age group.
17
P
O
S
T
E
R
S
Investigating Causal Direction in Neuroimaging Studies of Cognitive Ability; an
Application of non-Normal Structural Equation Modelling
Tom Booth
1
, Aja L. Murray
1
, Mark E. Bastin
2
, John M. Starr
3
, Joanna M. Wardlaw
2
,
Ian J. Deary
1
1
Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, Department of Psychology,
University of Edinburgh, UK
2
Brain Research Imaging Centre, Division of Neuroimaging Sciences, University of
Edinburgh, UK
3
Geriatric Medicine Unit, The University of Edinburgh, Royal Victoria, UK.
tom.booth@ed.ac.uk
Novelty. The current paper discusses the flow of causality from neuroimaging
markers to performance on cognitive ability measures. We discuss the use of non-
normal structural equation modelling (nnSEM) as a method for explicitly testing the
assumption that the brain is a primary causal agent of cognitive performance. nnSEM
uses higher order moments to identify models which would not be comparable using
conventional SEM or regression methods. We demonstrate nnSEM using the example
of white matter lesions and processing speed. It is generally assumed that the increased
presence of white matter lesions, specifically here in ageing, is one of the causal drivers
of decline in cognitive performance. Using data from the Lothian Birth Cohort 1936
(n=650), nnSEM models suggested that the best fitting, and thus most plausible causal
model, was one in which white matter lesion volume predicted simple reaction time
mean score.
Importance. Researchers often wish to provide causal interpretations in the presence
of cross-sectional associations. This is not possible. Whilst the use of nnSEM cannot
prove causality, it provides a methodology which, when data are suitable, can discern
between competing models which posit competing causal hypotheses. The current paper
provides a proof in principle of such an application.
Methods. The current work makes no explicit claims, but presents nnSEM as a
potentially useful methodology for assessing the causal direction of cross-sectional
associations between neuroanatomical variables and cognitive ability. nnSEM is a
relatively new methodology which is yet to be widely applied in research contexts. We
suggest here that some neuroimaging and cognitive variables are meaningfully skewed
and or kurtotic, making this area of research one which is suited to applications of
nnSEM. We acknowledge and discuss some limitations with the method which requires
further research through simulation studies.
18
The Cognitive Enhancing Effects of Acute and Chronic Curcumin
Supplementation in a Healthy Elderly Population
Katherine H. M. Cox, Andrew B. Scholey, Andrew Pipingas
Centre for Human Psychopharmacology, Swinburne University of Technology
kcox@swin.edu.au; ascholey@swin.edu.au; apipingas@swin.edu.au
Novelty. Around the world millions of people take dietary supplements in an attempt
to improve health, mood and cognitive function. Curcumin, a polyphenol found in the
spice turmeric, has been shown to exhibit a great variety of health benefits which may
extend to improving cognitive function.
To our knowledge this is the first study to investigate the potential cognitive
enhancing effects of curcumin supplementation in a healthy elderly population. This
poster will present findings showing acute and chronic facilitatory effects of curcumin
supplementation on working memory and attention resulting in improved task
performance. Curcumin supplementation may also reduce the strain of undertaking
cognitive demanding tasks.
Importance. It is predicted that by 2050 approximately one third of the population of
more developed nations will be over the age of 60. However it has been estimated that
in the United State alone 2.3 million community dwelling adults over the age of 65
suffered limitations caused by cognitive impairment. Age related cognitive decline and
age related conditions such as dementia are known to have a multifactorial etiology.
Therefore therapeutics that show multiple modes of action, such as nutraceuticals, may
be superior to traditional, single target pharmaceuticals in treating of such conditions.
The findings of this study have valuable real world applications as they suggest that
curcumin may represent readily available therapeutic agent for improving cognitive
function and by extension quality of life among our growing elderly population.
Methods. This study used a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled design,
allowing us to conclude that the benefits found with curcumin supplementation are over
and above those a placebo. Assessment of cognitive functions was carried out using
standardized, computerized tasks known to be sensitive to changes in cognitive
function.
19
P
O
S
T
E
R
S
Outliers Matter, Distance-Based Weighting Helps:
Some Examples Based on Speed-Ability Associations
Yury S. Dodonov
1
, Yulia A. Dodonova
1,2
1
Moscow State University of Psychology and Education
2
Peoples' Friendship University of Russia
ys.dodonov@gmail.com; ya.dodonova@mail.ru
Novelty. Most of the statistical analyses applied within the field of intelligence
research mathematically involve the computation of correlations between variables.
However, the question of how to choose a correlation coefficient that is robust to
outlying values and noisy observations is, by itself, an important methodological issue.
This poster introduces a distance-weighted estimator of correlation and compares its
behavior to that of conventionally used coefficients. The presented examples include
empirical RT-IQ correlations as well as the results of the simulation studies.
Importance. This poster presentation provides examples of why the choice of an
estimator matters in studies of speed-ability associations. First, it compares the
magnitudes of the RT-IQ correlations obtained using different estimators. Second, it
questions whether certain tendencies, such as the worst performance rule (an
observation that the RT-IQ correlation is higher for the slowest reactions than it is for
the fastest reactions obtained for the same task), are sensitive to a particular measure of
correlation used in the analysis. Third, it uses simulation studies to analyze the
behaviors of different correlation coefficients, including distance-weighted correlation,
under conditions that are plausible for empirical distributions of the RT and intelligence
scores.
Methods. Contrary to the commonly recommended robust methods that essentially
implement some kind of truncation of the original distribution, distance-based
weighting considers all observations while emphasizing those that lie close to each
other and the downweighting distant data points. Hence, distance-based weighting does
not use any statistics as input information, does not require an arbitrary threshold
delimiting outlying and inlying values, and does not produce any controversy on the
number of observations to be downweighted. The simulation results that are shown in
this poster suggest that the distance-weighted correlation is at least no worse in its
behavior under various noise conditions than the measures commonly recommended as
robust statistics.
20
Cross-Cultural Validity of IQ Tests among African Children:
Artificial or Accurate Measure of Intelligence?
Cissé Dramé
Texas A&M International University
cissedrame@dusty.tamiu.edu
Novelty. My research work demonstrates that Western standardized IQ tests are not
valid measure of intelligence across cultures.
The purpose of this study is to examine whether commonly used intelligence tests
such as the Ravens Progressive Matrices are valid indices of cognitive functioning
among children in Mali, Africa where previous research indicate significant sub-average
intelligence, below 70. No studies have yet ventured to examine if any correlation exists
between IQ score, achievement score and adaptive functioning level. The Woodcock-
Johnson will be used measure their academic achievement. The Vineland Behavior
Scale(VABS) will be used to indicate their adaptive functioning level. In the proposed
study, tests of IQ will be compared against adaptive functioning and academic
achievement, to examine whether IQ scores measured among African populations are
artificially lowered or are an accurate measure of performance.
Importance. As we may know racial differences in intelligence has been by far one
of the most intriguing and contested subject of a protracted discussion since the
inception of standardized cognitive tests one about hundred years ago. While decades of
research studies consistently show that average IQ lies below 70 in Sub-Saharan Africa
(e.g, Nissan, Machover, and Kinder, 1935;Laroche, 1959; Boissiere, Knight, & Sabot
985; Fernandez-Ballesteros; Juan-Espinoza, Colom, & Calero;1997), yet no studies
have ventured to examine the significant discrepancies between individuals’ subaverage
IQ scores and their adaptive functioning level in Africa. The question of whether IQ
scores measured among African populations are artificially lowered or are an accurate
measure of performance is rather important. One important consideration in this study is
the social and political implications.
Methods. Approximately 200 children were recruited from a school district in Mali,
West Africa. The sample was constituted of 200 African children ranged in age from 6
to 14 years old.
Three measures were used:
- Non Verbal IQ Test: Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices (RSPM)
- Adaptive Functioning Scale: Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale (VABS)
- Achievement Test: Woodcok-Johnson (Math section)
Proposed statistical analysis: Paired-samples T-tests to compare Ravens and
Vineland scores. Also multiples regression to examine how the Ravens scores, Vineland
and demographics predict the WRAT math scores.
21
P
O
S
T
E
R
S
Low Levels of Lead Exposure May Impact Children's General Intelligence
Rachel M. Earl, Ted Nettelbeck, Nicholas R. Burns, Peter Baghurst
University of Adelaide
rachel.earl@adelaide.edu.au; ted.nettelbeck@adelaide.edu.au;
nicholas.burns@adelaide.edu.au; baghurst@internode.on.net
Novelty. It is already well established that exposure to lead has a negative impact on
the development of intelligence. However, until recently there has been wide acceptance
that levels of exposure below a concentration 10 micrograms per decilitre of blood does
not constitute a risk to normal childhood development. Our results suggest that there
may be no safe level of lead exposure.
Importance. Extensive evidence supports the critical importance of general
intelligence as a major predictor of academic achievement and subsequent job
performance. If subsequent research confirms an effect on IQ of even very low levels of
exposure to lead, this will have profound implications for the long-term protection of
populations living in lead-contaminated environments.
Methods. Participants were 106 children (mean = 8 years, SD = .6) and their parents
living in two Australian communities where lead mining and smelting remain a source
of exposure. Data were collected on a wide range of cognitive measures, including
Wechsler scales and Raven Progressive Matrices for adults and children, several
subtests from the Woodcock Johnson-III (children only)and other tests selected to
represent the CHC model of cognitive abilities. Measures also included a range of
potential social and environmental confounding factors. An unadjusted curvilinear
continuous trend (correlation = -.31; p < .001)was found between children\'s blood lead
levels and a general factor extracted from the battery of cognitive tests, suggesting that
there may be no threshold safety level for exposure to lead during childhood. In
covariate adjusted analyses consistent findings emerged, suggesting that even low level
lead may detrimentally impact the cognitive development of children, in particular
speed of information processing and long term storage and retrieval.
22
Bacopa Monnieri in Child and Adolescent Populations with and without
Developmental Dysfunction
James D. Kean, Luke A. Downey, Con Stough
Swinburne University of Technology
jkean@swin.edu.au; ldowney@swin.edu.au; cstough@swin.edu.au
Novelty. The Ayurvedic medicinal system is becoming more popular in Western
medicine. Bacopa monnieri has been used in the Ayurvedic medicinal system for
approximately 3000 years and is classified as a medhyarasayana, a drug used to improve
memory and intellect (medhya). To date clinical trials in Western research using Bacopa
have focused on adult populations in acute and chronic settings but very few have
looked at a younger population and its possible benefits. A recent systematic review
completed by the authors covers every clinical trial that has looked at Bacopa monnieri
and its effects on the child & adolescent population. The review explores how
efficacious Bacopa has been in terms of improving developmental dysfunction and
increasing areas of cognitive function in the younger population.
Importance. Complementary and alternative medicines (CAM) are a common
treatment avenue for parents to take for their children who might be struggling with
school work, social situations, behavioural issues, developmental delay or even mild to
moderate learning difficulties. Although there is an aura of safety surrounding the use
of natural products to relieve symptoms associated with certain disorders, the safety and
tolerability research into many of these products has been far from conclusive. Half of
all parents with children diagnosed with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
(ADHD), will give their child CAM without the proper consultation of their child’s
physician. There is a need for greater restraint in the field of natural medicine to ensure
the benefits and risks of every vitamin, plant extract, and natural compound can be
elucidated through research and the replication of results, to ensure the safe treatment of
the child and adolescent population.
Methods. The aim of current review was to summarize and assess the data from
clinical trials of Bacopa monnieri and its effects on the mental health of children and
adolescents. Each trial involving Bacopa monnieri in any form will be looked at in
terms of its extraction process, its overall weight (mg), the percentage of bacosides in
the extract (the suggested mechanism of action), the intervention time period, the
population of children and adolescents on which it is being tested (clinical or non-
clinical group), and the impact of any other extracts if in a combination formula. The
main outcome will be the safety and tolerability of the extract being used, whether any
side-effects (if any) were reported.
23
P
O
S
T
E
R
S
What one Intelligence Test Measures: the Distinct Roles of Working Memory and
Rule Induction in Matrix Reasoning
Patrick Loesche, Marcus Hasselhorn
German Institute for International Educational Research
loesche@dipf.de; hasselhorn@dipf.de
Novelty. The solution process underlying problems from Raven’s APM has been
conceptualized to consist of two subprocesses: rule induction and goal management.
The present research aims at giving an answer to the question whether both of these
subprocesses are related to working memory capacity. This was done by testing whether
the correlation between Raven’s APM and working memory tasks can be artificially
raised by eliminating the subprocess of rule induction. The experimental design
included a variation of the instructions to Raven’s APM which already gave the rules
necessary to solve the problems, hence making rule induction unnecessary. An effect of
the rule induction affordance on the correlation between APM and working memory
was confirmed in a sample of 644 secondary school students. The results suggest that
the solution process in the APM is twofold (at least), and that one of the subprocesses
(goal management) does almost exclusively rely on working memory capacity whereas
the other (rule induction) does not.
Importance. The research that has linked working memory to gf has, to this point,
mainly focused on the part that is not involved in generating rules. That is, prevailing
accounts for this relationship envision some sort of information processing that involves
storage, maintenance, inhibition, supervision, attention, or updating but none of these
accounts can explain how a mental representation of a rule or abstract relationship is
actually formed. It lies in the nature of working memory tasks that they are free of
inductive processes. That is, in typical working memory tasks the participants are fully
informed about the task and about the relation of the task material to a correct response.
In our view, this aspect is fundamentally different from intelligence tests like the APM,
where the relationship among elements is unknown to the subject.
Methods. The study combines methods from differential and cognitive psychology
jointly in an experimental design. The design allows for the manipulation of a narrowly
circumscribed subprocess in matrix reasoning. Teaching the rules needed to solve the
problems, should make the induction of these rules unnecessary. Participants should be
able to recall the rules, so they would just have to verify if any is applicable to the
problem at hand and then store the intermediary result before going to the next problem.
The downside of the methodology though is that a rather large test power is required to
detect the predicted changes in the correlation. As a result of a test power analysis we
aimed at recruiting at least 600 subjects.
24
Adolescent Peer-Relations and Emotional Intelligence
Justine E. Lomas, Luke A. Downey, Con Stough
Centre for Human Psychopharmacology, Swinburne University
jlomas@swin.edu.au; ldowney@swin.edu.au; cstough@swin.edu.au
Novelty. Research into schoolyard bullying has highlighted not only the undesirable
prevalence of bullying behaviours exhibited by adolescents but also the associated
adverse consequences. In order to better understand bullying behaviours, a pilot study
was conducted to examine for the first time the relationship between emotional
intelligence (EI) of adolescents, bullying behaviours and peer victimisation. Results of
the study indicated that the EI dimensions of Emotions Direct Cognition and Emotional
Management and Control, significantly predicted the propensity of adolescents to be
subjected to peer victimisation. The EI dimension of Understanding the Emotions of
Others was found to be negatively related with bullying behaviours. A larger study
subsequently provided support for these findings.
Importance. The implications of these findings are particularly important in terms of
managing bullying and peer victimisation within schools. The findings suggest that
measures of EI may be utilised to identify students with lesser developed EI
competencies. Using this information, schools may be able to identify students at
greater risk of being subjected to peer victimisation. The ability to identify students at
greatest risk of peer victimisation may allow for more targeted, accurate or timely
intervention to protect the student from the potential harmful consequences that are
associated with exposure to bullying. The ability to identify students at greater risk of
being subjected to peer victimisation may also assist in the development of improved
anti-bullying programs in the educational setting.
Methods. Initial correlational analysis revealed that self-reported Understanding of
Emotions was significantly correlated with self-reported engagement in bullying
behaviours. Further to this, a regression analysis of self-reported EI and peer relations
found that the EI factors Emotions Direct Cognition and Emotional Management and
Control accounted for a significant amount of variance in self-reported experience of
peer victimisation.
25
P
O
S
T
E
R
S
Investigating Relationships between Cognition and Life History in a Sample of
6364 Individuals
Guy Madison
1
, Michael A. Woodley
1
, Fredrik Ullén
2
1
Umeå University;
2
Karolinska Institutet
guy.madison@umu.se; Michael.Woodley@umu.se; fredrik.ullen@ki.se
Novelty. A recent meta-analyses shows that life history and intelligence do not
correlate at the level of latent variables (Woodley, 2011). An objection to this is based
on the observation that most studies involve student convenient samples and are
therefore nor population representative. Here, we explore the relations amongst a higher
order cognition factor (Gc) comprised of a general creativity measure (CAQ) and a fluid
intelligence measure (WMT) and a super-K factor comprised of subjective health, GFP,
and the mini-K. We utilised a large twin sample (N=6364 Swedes) as the basis for
creating one replicate for each group of twins in a pair. We found significant but small
correlation magnitudes (.06,.07) between Gc and Super-K. Interestingly, fluid
intelligence correlated negatively and significantly (-.04) with mini-K. The GFP was a
significant predictor of CAQ (.17,.18) as was mini-K (.09,.07). GFP did not correlate
with WMT and health correlated with WMT in only one sample (.03,.06). Health did
not correlate with CAQ (-.01,-.02). These results are in line with results of meta-
analysis.
Importance. Never before have life history and intelligence been studied in such a
large individual differences sample. These findings indicate that contrary to predictions
from differential K (i.e. that g and K should be significantly and substantively
correlated) what correlations exist whilst, for the most part in the theoretically expected
direction (with the exception of fluid intelligence and mini-K), are of a magnitude
equivalent to that found in the meta-analysis of Woodley (2011) which supports the idea
presented there that life history and cognition are largely distinct sources of individual
differences in behaviour.
Methods. We used correlative analysis to explore the nomological network of
associations between cognition and life history in two very large samples. Most
previous research into the question have been correlative in nature owing to the small
effect sizes typically recovered. Are findings are in line with these previous analyses
hence the methods are sound.
26
Cognitive Abilities, Self-Regulation and Academic Achievements of Gifted
Students
Varvara I. Morosanova, Irina N. Bondarenko, Elena I. Shcheblanova
Psychological Institute of Russian Academy of Education
morosanova@mail.ru; pondi@inbox.ru
Novelty. There is a well-known fact: intellectually gifted pupils may have poor
academic achievements. The reason might be low level of their self-regulation (SR).
The aim of the study was to investigate the relationship between psychometric
intelligence and conscious SR of learning activity and its effect on academic
achievements of gifted students.
A comparison of intellectual and regulation characteristics showed that gifted
adolescents have higher levels of conscious SR.
The correlations between academic achievements and SR were slightly lower than
correlation coefficients between academic achievements and intelligence, but were
found in all major subjects. High academic achievements correlated positively with
well-developed SR and personal characteristics (initiative and autonomy). Dependant
students and students with no initiative were inferior to their peers in terms of verbal
and mathematical intelligence, level of conscious SR and general level of academic
achievements. Regulatory properties (initiative and autonomy) were the link between
motivation, intelligence, and SR system.
Importance. We take interest in the problem of gifted children’s low achievements
for a reason. The results of our investigation make it possible to predict and optimize
their training activities and to offer the psycho-educational recommendations to improve
the learning process.
Low levels of conscious SR, as well as impulsivity, disorganization and lack of
attention factors (Reis, McCoach, 2002), may prevent gifted students from fulfilling
their intellectual and creative potential.
The intellectual level of gifted students is high as it is. That is why we think that the
key for improving their academic performance is to focus on developing their self-
regulation of learning activity. Students, who take initiative in learning process, achieve
high results. It was proved that developing SR results in increase in verbal,
mathematical, and, as a consequence, the overall levels of intelligence. Which leads to
increased academic achievements in all major disciplines.
Methods. The study was carried out on a sample of 87 gifted and 360 “normal”
students aged 14-16 years.
We used the following methods:
Questionnaire of self-regulation in learning activity (Morosanova at al., 2011) allows
assessing regulatory processes and features, such as goal planning, modeling of goal-
achievement conditions, programming of actions, results evaluation, regulative
flexibility, independence, reliability and responsibility.
The Russian version of the Munich cognitive abilities test for gifted students - KFT
(Kognitiver Fähigkeit Test). This test is designed for students who are two years ahead
of their peers in general intelligence level and mental abilities. The test involves three
scales - verbal (V), math (Q) and nonverbal (N).
We used the final exams results to assess academic achievements of our sample.
27
P
O
S
T
E
R
S
Challenging the Intelligence Compensation Theory: are Conscientiousness and
Intelligence Really Negatively Correlated?
Aja L. Murray
1
, Wendy Johnson
1
, Matt McGue
2
, William G. Iacono
2
1
Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, Department of Psychology,
University of Edinburgh, UK
2
Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota- Twin Cities, USA
s0785823@staffmail.ed.ac.uk
Novelty. A consistently observed negative association between intelligence and
conscientiousness has been explained in terms of an ‘intelligence compensation theory’
whereby higher conscientiousness develops in compensation for lower cognitive ability.
In the present study we tested the hypothesis that a better explanation for previous
observations of a negative association is the use of selected research samples which are
comprised of participants with levels of educational and occupational attainment above
certain thresholds. The associations between the traits in our samples - which were not
selected in this way - were either zero or positive. Further, artificially introducing
selection into the samples biased the associations between intelligence and
conscientiousness in the negative direction. Together, these results are consistent with
the hypothesis that the true association between these constructs may be zero or positive
at the population level but that the use of selected research samples has resulted in the
appearance of a negative association in empirical studies.
Importance. In observational studies it is uncommon for the selection processes
leading to the composition of convenience samples to be explicitly considered, even less
to be measured and modelled. Unless such selection processes are given due
consideration, researchers risk being misled as to the direction and magnitude of the
association between IQ and conscientiousness-related traits. This, in turn, can lead to
the development of lines of research which are wasteful because they aim to explain
what are actually non-existent phenomena.
Methods. Our methods of evaluating the correlation between IQ and our two
measures of conscientiousness (the MPQ Scales of Control and Achievement-Striving)
were designed to mimic the methods that have previously been employed in studies
finding negative associations between IQ and Conscientiousness. Thus, we used
Pearson’s correlations between the scale scores on the personality measures and IQ. The
primary methodological advantage of the current study was the ability to introduce
selection into a sample which was unselected for the traits of interest. We did this by
discarding all individuals who were below progressively increasing thresholds of
educational or occupational attainment in order to mimic processes of selection into
populations (e.g. undergraduate students, or assessment centre participants) to some
degree dependent on educational or occupational attainment.
28
An Investigation of the Influence of Antioxidant Defense Genes on Cognitive
Ability in Older Individuals
Karen J. Nolidin, Con Stough, Chad Bousman, Andrew Scholey, Andrew Pipingas
Swinburne University of Technology
knolidin@swin.edu.au; cstough@swin.edu.au; cbousman@unimelb.edu.au;
ascholey@swin.edu.au; apipingas@swin.edu.au
Novelty. Previous research has indicated that increased oxidative stress is associated
with poorer cognitive performance and ability. Oxidative stress tends to be more
pronounced in older individuals, possibly due to dysregulation and poorer control of the
maintenance of optimal oxidative levels. Supplementation with antioxidants has shown
some promising improvements to cognitive performance, but more research is needed to
investigate the body\'s own endogenous antioxidant defense mechanisms. Genetic
variation within antioxidant defense genes may mediate the cognitive ageing process
and predict variability in cognition and biological ageing parameters.
Importance. Old age is associated with declining cognitive abilities and increased
risk of neurodegenerative disorders. Since populations around the world are living
longer, determining possible risk factors related to poorer cognition in later life is
important. While research suggests oxidative stress is associated with the ageing
process, more research is needed to explore the possible mediating role that antioxidant
defense genes play in the cognitive ageing process. Since the selected genes are related
to antioxidant defense this research may help with the future development of
interventions as well.
Methods. A wider range of cognitive tasks is being utilised than in previous research.
This will enable better determination of which cognitive abilities are most sensitive to
genetic effects. The selected SNPs have previously been associated with cognitive
performance in older people, and there is the addition of being able to investigate
oxidative stress markers as well.
29
P
O
S
T
E
R
S
Does Education Boost General Intelligence (G) or Specific Cognitive Abilities?
Stuart J. Ritchie, Timothy C. Bates, Ian J. Deary
The University of Edinburgh
stuartjritchie1@gmail.com; tim.bates@ed.ac.uk; i.deary@ed.ac.uk
Novelty. Previous studies using causal methods have shown that education raises
cognitive ability, but the mechanism of its influence is unclear. Here, we tested whether
education is associated with cognitive test score improvements via general cognitive
ability (g), or via specific cognitive skills. Results from structural equation modeling of
data from a large, longitudinal sample, with a measure of IQ at age 11 years and ten
tests covering a diverse range of cognitive abilities taken at about age 70, indicated that
the association of education with improved cognitive test scores is not mediated by g,
but consists of direct links to specific cognitive subtests.
Importance. Given the importance of g in everyday life, it is extremely useful to
know whether it can be improved via environmental inputs such as education. Our
results showed that education did not raise g, but we argue that the domain-specific
effects of education are still an important benefit. Thus, our study has the potential to
defuse some of the perceived conflicts between intelligence researchers and
educationalists: the two ostensibly opposing conceptualizations, of a largely general
cognitive ability and a malleable IQ score, are not mutually exclusive.
Methods. The relationship between intelligence and education has been difficult to
investigate in most samples, due to issues of confounding and reverse causality. To
address these issues, we used a large, longitudinal sample (the Lothian Birth Cohort
1936) with IQ measured early in life and a wide range of cognitive tests (which
provided a representative g-factor) taken in later life. We modelled this data using
structural equation modelling, and so were able to compare complex models of
education, g, and specific abilities.
30
Emotional Intelligence, Victimisation, Bullying Behaviours and Attitudes: a
Replication and Extension
Chantelle M. Schokman
1
, Luke A. Downey
1,2
, Justine E. Lomas
1
, Dirk Wellham
3
,
Andrew Wheaton
3
, Nathan Simmons
3
, Con Stough
1
1
Centre for Human Psychopharmacology, Swinburne University of Technology,
Hawthorn, Australia.
2
Department of Psychology, Swansea University, Wales, UK
3
Churchie, Anglican Church Grammar School, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
cschokman@swin.edu.au; ldowney@swin.edu.au; jlomas@swin.edu.au;
dirk.wellham@churchie.com.au; cstough@swin.edu.au
Novelty. This study extended the literature through an examination of the relationship
between attitudes towards bullying and Emotional Intelligence (EI) on outcomes of
bullying, victimisation, and pro-social behaviours.
As a replication and extension of a 2012 pilot study, the present study built on the
prediction model of levels of victimisation with the EI variables and attitudes towards
bullying. The skills involved in management and control of emotions and attitudes
towards bullying were observed to be significant predictors of victimization.
Results from this study also revealed significant associations between bullying,
victimisation, pro victim attitudes and the EI dimensions Emotion Recognition and
Expression (ERE), Emotion Management and Control (EMC), Understanding the
Emotions of Others (UEO) and Emotions Direct Cognition (EDC).
Importance. In the context of adolescent development, EI has been suggested to be
integral for successful social interaction (Romasz, Kantor & Elias, 2004), with more
highly evolved EI skills serving to enhance emotional awareness, coordinate decision
making and improve conflict resolution.
These results reinforce previous school based EI research findings in consistently
showing that EI not only has significant associations with bullying and victimisation but
is also a significant predictor of the propensity for peer victimisation and problematic
behaviours (Downey et al., 2010; Lomas et al., 2012).
Investigation of the influence of EI and pro-victim attitudes on victimisation further
revealed significant independent contributions to the prediction model of victimisation.
The results of this study have overall implications for the management and education
of bullying and victimisation in secondary schools.
Methods. This study used three self-report measures to test its hypotheses. EI was
measured using the adolescent version of the Swinburne University Emotional
Intelligence Test, a validated EI measure reported to have high reliability for total EI
(Luebbers et al., 2007).
Bullying behaviours were measured using the Peer Relations Questionnaire (Rigby
& Slee, 1993).
Attitudes to Bullying were measured via the Shortened Version of Rigby and Slee’s
(1991) Attitude to Victims Scale, a reliable measure of the degree to which a person
justifies bullying behaviour, supports bullies and their desire to reject children who are
victims of bullying based on supposed weakness (Rigby & Slee, 1991).
Correlation and regression analyses indicated that adolescents’ understanding of
others emotions and recognition and accurate expression of emotions were predictive of
engaging in pro-social behaviours.
31
P
O
S
T
E
R
S
A Preliminary Study Investigating the Effects of Resveratrol on Cognitive
Function
Andrew Scholey
1
, Sarah Benson
1
, Con Stough
1
, Creina Stockley
2
1
Centre for Human Psychopharmacology, Swinburne University
2
Australian Wine Research Institute
ascholey@swin.edu.au; sarahbenson@swin.edu.au
Novelty. There is evidence from preclinical studies, epidemiology and human clinical
trials suggesting beneficial effects of resveratrol to systems underpinning cognitive
function. Nevertheless, direct evidence of cognitive benefits from resveratrol has proved
elusive. This pilot study compared mental performance in an older cohort following red
wine alone or enriched with resveratrol wine. The results suggest that there are
differential effects of the treatments depending on the type of task.
Importance. Over the past decade evidence has steadily accumulated suggesting that
there is a j-shaped relationship between the consumption of alcoholic beverages such as
wine and cognitive function. Cognitive function is defined as the intellectual or mental
processes by which knowledge is acquired, including perception, attention, working
memory, secondary memory and executive functioning. Cognitive functions underpin
thought processes including reasoning, acts of creativity, problem-solving and possibly
intuition. Dementia is a form of cognitive dysfunction whereby an individual loses the
capacity to think, remember and reason due to physical changes in the brain. Currently,
there are few options for the treatment of cognitive dysfunction and dementia. Simple
dietary measure such as moderate wine consumption to supplement a healthy diet and
exercise routine, or as an adjunct to prescription medicines when appropriate, are thus
needed to maintain an ageing population.
Methods. The present study investigated the effects of a daily moderate dose of
resveratrol-enhanced red wine on mood and cognitive performance in older adults using
a placebo-controlled, double-blind and crossover design. Participants were randomised
to consume 100 ml red wine and 100 ml resveratrol-enhanced wine over two testing
sessions. Participants completed a cognitive demand battery and had blood samples
taken at baseline and 45, 90 and 120 minutes post treatment.
32
Knowing the Knowing: Exploring the Relationship Between Metacognition and
Intelligence
John H. Song, Brooke Nickeas
De Montfort University
jsong@dmu.ac.uk
Novelty. Metacognition (MC) can be defined as an individual’s knowledge about
their own cognitive structure or processes and having the ability to organise their
structure (Akturk, 2011). Commonly examined in the context of learning situations, MC
is less examined within the context of cognitive ability task performance. With this in
mind, a good candidate, Raven’s Progressive Matrics (RPM) presents an opportunity to
examine individuals’ MC related to their own performance. Due to the nature of the task
whereby matrices can be solved through identification of relationships, it is well-suited
to analysis of this kind. One hundred participants completed the RPM followed by an
adapted MC Questionnaire specifically designed to be used in relations to RPM. Of the
various sub-components of metacognition, Knowledge about Strategies significantly
predicted RPM performance. In addition, self-ratings of confidence level having
provided correct solutions also significantly predicted RPM performance.
Importance. Relatively few studies have examined the completion of intelligence test
in relations to participants’ awareness of their own cognitive structure and processes.
The basic question of whether having knowledge about one’s own cognitive structure
and being able to organise that structure is beneficial to intelligence test performance
has rarely been explored. Uniquely this opportunity arose due to the design of this
particular intelligence test – Raven’s Progressive Matices. Other tests, by comparison,
tended to be more varied in terms of content, and format, making such studies more
difficult. Ultimately, this study may be helpful in identifying or characterising
individuals who performs better on intelligence tests compared to those who performed
less well. It will also contribute towards knowledge about components underlying
intelligence.
Methods. This is one of very few studies that examined metacognition within the
context of intelligence test performance. More often, metacognition is examined within
the context of learning, for example most recently, in foreign language achievement
(e.g. Pishghadam & Khajavy, 2013). Through this study, it was found that having the
knowledge about strategies predicts better outcome in terms of intelligence test
performance. In contrast, other subcomponents of metacognition such as Knowledge
about Tasks did not predict intelligence test performance. This study utilised a
metacognition questionnaire that has been specifically adapted to suit the task
(Raven’s), rather than a generic learning-related metacognition questionnaire. The use
of this method therefore enabled examination of data that is related to the task at hand.
33
P
O
S
T
E
R
S
The Chronic Effects of Bacopa, Pycnogenol and an Antioxidant/Micronutrient
Combination Formula on Cognitive and Cardiovascular Function in a Healthy
Older Population
Con Stough, Karen M. Savage, Karen J. Nolidin, Andrew Scholey, Andrew Pipingas,
Chad Bousman, Antoinette Goh
Swinburne University of Technology
cstough@swin.edu.au; ksavage@swin.edu.au; knolidin@swin.edu.au;
ascholey@swin.edu.au; apipingas@swin.edu.au; cbousman@unimelb.edu.au;
agoh@swin.edu.au
Novelty. A number of biomarkers and physiological processes involved in the ageing
brain are associated with measurable changes in cognitive abilities. The current study
seeks to examine markers of changes in cognitive abilities in an older population,
utilising a number of cognitive tests and biological (cardiovascular, biochemical and
genetic) measures.
With Australia's ageing population, interventions capable of ameliorating age-related
neurocognitive change are becoming vital areas of research. Natural supplements such
as Bacopa Monnieri and French Maritime Pine Bark extract have demonstrated positive
effects on cognition-relevant mechanisms and in cardiovascular health.
The current study examines domains of cognitive abilities in this population, and
whether nutraceutical supplementation elicits improvements to areas of learning,
memory and attention. Further, the mechanisms through which these improvements are
observed are also investigated.
Importance. No such studies have comprehensively examined markers of ageing and
cognitive performance in a normal older population. The literature points to a need to
explore the efficacy of bioactive constituents and a rigorous methodological approach
that best informs researchers and consumers seeking cognitive benefit over the life span.
Methods. The study employs cognitive tasks over several visits over 52 weeks:
measures involving speed of information processing, decision-making time, working
memory and memory consolidation. It involves the biochemical analysis (select SNPs,
telomeres, C-reactive protein, F2 isoprostanes, immunological measures),
cardiovascular (blood pressure, arterial stiffness, blood flow, HDL and cholesterol), also
measures of personality and intelligence (WASI, MMSE, NEO-FFI). All these measures
have been utilised previously in relevant literature designed to investigate the variables
relating to cognitive abilities and performance.
The three interventions used in the study (Bacopa monnieri, French Maritime Pine
Bark Extract, and a novel micronutrient blend) have been associated with improvements
in cognitive ability, mood, and cardiovascular health.
34
Effects of Alcohol Consumption on Academic Performance
Joris C. Verster
1,2
, Renske Penning
3
, Reinout W. Wiers
4
1
Utrecht University, Division of Pharmacology, Utrecht, The Netherlands
2
Centre for Human Psychopharmacology, Swinburne University, Melbourne, Australia
3
Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
4
University of Amsterdam, Department of Psychology, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
j.c.verster@uu.nl; r.penning@uu.nl; R.W.H.J.Wiers@uva.nl
Novelty. Alcohol consumption can have various negative effects on the daily life of
students, and result in not attending lectures and poor academic performance. Increased
alcohol consumption has been related to poor academic performance among US
students, but European data on this relationship is scarce and yielded inconclusive
results. This study examined the relationship of alcohol use and misuse with academic
performance of two Dutch student samples, and examined potential moderating factors
such as gender or being member of a fraternity.
Importance. Alcohol abuse is a significant problem, especially among the student
population. This study showed that alcohol consumption is negatively associated with
academic performance, especially in male fraternity members. This knowledge is
important to make students aware of the consequences of consuming too much alcohol.
Methods. Two surveys (N=1248)were conducted among Dutch university students in
the cities Nijmegen and Utrecht. Weekly alcoholic consumption was recorded and the
number of study credit points they earned over the past year. Students were divided into
five different drinking groups (0, 1-14, 15-21, 22-42 and over 42 weekly alcoholic
drinks).
The drinking groups differed significantly on number of study credit points
(p<0.0001). Paired comparisons showed no significant difference between non-drinkers
and those who consumed less than 21 alcoholic drinks a week. The other groups
differed significantly from the non-drinkers (p<0.004). A significant negative
correlation was found between the number of weekly alcoholic drinks and the earned
study credit points (r = - 0.260, p< 0.0001). Males and members of fraternities or
sororities consumed significantly more alcohol (p<0.0001), and earned significantly less
study credit points (p <0.0001) when compared to females and non-fraternity members.
35
P
O
S
T
E
R
S
Structure of the Woodcock-Johnson III Cognitive in a Clinical Sample
Marley W. Watkins
1
, Tracy Strickland
2
1
Baylor University;
2
Arizona State University
Marley_Watkins@baylor.edu; Tracy.Strickland@asu.edu
Novelty. The Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities III (WJ-III) are based
on the Cattell-Horn-Carroll (CHC) theory of intelligence, which combined the Gf-Gc
cognitive theories of Horn with the three-stratum theory of Carroll. The WJ-III purports
to measure 7 broad cognitive abilities and one higher-order ability. Alignment of the
structure of the WJ-III with its theoretical structure would provide evidence of structural
validity. Many prior investigations of the WJ-III have applied confirmatory factor
analysis (CFA) to data from the normative sample. Results have consisently supported
the CHC structure. Some researchers have suggested that the WJ-III has been over-
factored and should be analyzed with exploratory factor analytic (EFA) methods.
However, there are few studies that have investigated the structure of the WJ-III among
clinical samples and few investigations have included EFA methods. The current study
will apply both EFA and CFA methods to WJ-III data from a clinical sample to
determine if the structure of the WJ-III is consistent with CHC theory for these children.
Importance. The WJ-III is widely used in schools and clinics with children and
adolescents referred for assessment to determine their eligibility for special education
services. Its interpretation is based on theory and CFA results from analyses of the
normative sample. Recent EFA analyses of the WJ-III school-aged normative sample
failed to confirm the CFA model (Dombrowski & Watkins, 2013) and some researchers
(Frazier & Youngstrom, 2007) have suggested that the WJ-III is over-factored.
Additionally, children referred for evaluation differ in many ways from children who
are not experiencing academic or behavioral problems. Another recent study applied
CFA methods to data from students with learning disabilities and a matched sample of
students without disabilities and was unable to achieve full measurement invariance
(Benson & Taub, in press). Consequently, the structure of the WJ-III must be confirmed
among clinical samples to allow confident use of the instrument with those children.
Methods. This study included 529 participants who were 6 to 13 years of age (M =
9.47, SD = 1.81). Of the 528 participants whose gender was specified, 62% were male
and 38% were female. Participants\' race/ethnicity was around 49% white, 32%
Hispanic, 8% black, 5% native American, 3% multiracial, and 3% other. Educational
diagnoses included 69% with learning disabilities, 11% with speech impairments, 10%
with other health impairments, 5% without a disability, 3% with emotional disabilities,
and 2% with a variety of other diagnoses (e.g., autism, orthopedic, etc.). The WJ-III
standard battery claims to measure 7 broad ability constructs with 7 tests whereas its
extended battery claims to measure the same constructs with 14 tests. Scores from the
14 extended battery tests were included in these analyses. Both EFA (following the
procedures detailed by Carroll as well as those recommended by Frazier and
Youngstrom) and CFA methods were applied to ascertain the structure of this data.
Results from both factor analytic methods will be reported.
36
Neurocognitive Effects of Multivitamin Supplementation: a Behavioural and
FMRI Study
David J. White
1
, Katherine H.M. Cox
1
, Chris Neale
1
, David A. Camfield
1
, Isabelle
Bauer
1
, Karen Savage
1
, Silvia Maggini
2
, Andrew Pipingas
1
, Con Stough
1
, Matthew
Hughes
1
, Andrew Scholey
1
1
Centre for Human Psychopharmacology, Swinburne University, Melbourne, VIC, 3122
2
Bayer Consumer Care Ltd, Basel, Switzerland 4002
dawhite@swin.edu.au; kcox@swin.edu.au; chrisneale02@gmail.com;
davidcamfield@gmail.com; ibauer@swin.edu.au; ksavage@swin.edu.au;
silvia.maggini@bayer.com; apipingas@swin.edu.au; cstough@swin.edu.au;
matthewhughes@swin.edu.au; ascholey@swin.edu.au
Novelty. This research examined both the acute (single-dose) and chronic (4-week)
effects of MV supplementation on neurocognition. Two double-blind, placebo-
controlled studies examined, for the first time, the neural substrates of these effects
using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and Steady State Topography
(SST). Findings revealed positive effects of acute doses of MV supplementation with
guaraná on attentional and working memory processes, with SST recordings showing
that MV + guaraná increased neural activity and processing speed while MV alone
improved cortical efficiency. fMRI revealed that both supplements increased activation
of a well-characterised fronto-parietal working memory network with respect to
placebo. The effects of 4-weeks MV supplementation further revealed increased
activation during working memory task performance within task-relevant brain regions.
These findings describe, for the first time, acute and chronic activation of the human
brain following multivitamin administration consistent with benefits to attentional and
working memory systems.
Importance. There is growing evidence supporting the potential for multivitamin
(MV) supplementation, including those with additional ingredients such as guaraná, in
cognitive enhancement. However, there is little known about the mechanisms by which
such supplementation may exert these positive effects. Given the widespread use of MV
supplements, understanding the potential mechanisms by which MV supplementation
may exert such effects represents an important step in fully understanding the impact of
MV supplementation.
Methods. Two double-blind, placebo-controlled studies examined the neural
substrates of MV supplementation. In the first experiment, healthy adults consumed a
MV, a MV with guaraná and a placebo using a balanced, crossover design. The second
experiment explored the effects of 4-weeks MV supplementation using a parallel groups
design. Neurocognitive effects were explored using functional magnetic resonance
imaging (fMRI) and Steady State Topography (SST), whilst mood and cognitive
function were also assessed. Double-blind, placebo-controlled studies represent the gold
standard in clinical trials design.
37
T
A
L
K
S
P
O
S
T
E
R
S
INDIVIDUAL
PRESENTATIONS
Abstracts are organized in alphabetical order
Names of students eligible for the John B. Caroll Award for Research
Methodology and ISIR Prize for Best Student Paper
are marked with an asterisk
38
When Does Socioeconomic Status Moderate the Heritability of IQ? Data from
Australia and the USA
Timothy C. Bates
1
, Narelle K. Hansell
2
, Nicholas G. Martin
2
, Margaret J. Wright
2
1
University of Edinburgh
2
Queensland Institute of Medical Research
timothy.c.bates@gmail.com; Narelle.Hansell@qimr.edu.au; Nick.Martin@qimr.edu.au;
Margie.Wright@qimr.edu.au
Novelty. When is the heritability of intelligence dependent on socio economic status
(SES), and when it is not? US studies largely suggest that cognitive ability is more
heritable among those raised in higher socioeconomic status (SES) families. However,
the mechanism of this effect is unclear, and the effect may not be universal. We tested
for gene × SES interaction effects on Full-scale IQ in 2,307 adolescent Australian twins
(mean age 16.2 years). While mean scores in were higher among those from higher SES
backgrounds, the magnitude of genetic influences on IQ, constant across the range of
SES.
Importance. The heritability of intelligence was high, and unrelated to social status.
This suggests that during the development of intelligence, genes multiply cultural inputs
supportive of intellectual growth. It suggests also, however, that this interaction can be
decoupled from parental SES, possibly via factors such as quality of school provision.
Methods. We used data from the United States (Bates Lewis, and Weiss, 2013) as
well as from Australian twins to test for gene × SES interaction effects on Full-scale IQ.
The Australian sample is large (over 1000 twin pairs) and representative. We explicit
testing of variance components with widely accepted measures of IQ and SES.
39
T
A
L
K
S
Validity of Ability Tilt on the SAT and ACT
Thomas R. Coyle, Jason M. Purcell, Anissa C. Snyder, Miranda C. Null, Robert Vargas
University of Texas at San Antonio
thomas.coyle@utsa.edu; jmpurcell@satx.rr.com; anissa.snyder@utsa.edu;
miranda.null@gmail.com; ndz163@my.utsa.edu
Novelty. This research examined the validity of ability tilt, defined as within-subject
differences in math and verbal test scores. Ability tilt was examined using the SAT and
ACT, two widely used college admissions tests. The SAT and ACT produce two types
of tilt: math tilt, which occurs when math scores are higher than verbal scores, and
verbal tilt, which occurs when verbal scores are higher than math scores. Park et al.
(2007) examined ability tilt in gifted subjects who took the SAT at age 12 and scored in
the top 1%. Math tilt predicted achievements in science, technology, engineering, and
math (patents obtained) 25-years later, while verbal tilt predicted achievements in arts
and letters (book published). The present study examined the validity of ability tilt in
subjects in the normal range of ability. Such subjects are assumed to show less cognitive
specialization and lower levels of tilt (compared to gifted subjects), which may decrease
tilt effects. The present study also estimated tilt effects after removing g, guaranteeing
that tilt effects could not be attributed to g.
Importance. This research extends our prior research on the predictive validity of
non-g factors in cognitive tests (Coyle et al., 2013). Non-g factors are unrelated or
weakly related to g, which generally explains the predictive validity of mental tests.
Ability tilt is a non-g factor in cognitive tests: It is unrelated (or weakly related) to g,
yet, as Park et al. (2007) show, it still predicts later achievements. The present study
provides further evidence of the validity of ability tilt using a representative sample
(rather than gifted subjects) and extracting g with a variety of tests (rather than relying
solely on the SAT). After removing g, math tilt correlated positively with math ability
(measured using the ASVAB) but negatively with verbal ability, whereas the opposite
pattern was observed with verbal tilt. The overall pattern is consistent with investment
theories, which argue that investing in the development of a specific ability (e.g., math)
improves that ability but weakens competing abilities (e.g., verbal).
Methods. Subjects (N = 1951) and test scores were drawn from the National
Longitudinal Survey of Youth, a nationally representative sample of youth in the United
States. Test scores included SAT and ACT scores, which were used to calculate ability
tilt (math minus verbal scores), and the 12 cognitive tests of the ASVAB, which were
used to estimate g. The ASVAB was also used to estimate four abilities (verbal, math,
speed, and shop). These four abilities were correlated with ability tilt from the SAT and
ACT after removing g, guaranteeing that ability tilt effects were not attributable to g.
Thus, the observed tilt effects (which supported investment theories) were attributable
to non-g factors, providing further evidence that the validity of cognitive tests is
attributable to factors other than g.
40
Emotional Intelligence Mediates the Relationship Between Humour Styles and
Psychological Well-Being
Bruce Findlay, Robyn Brown
Swinburne University of Technology
bfindlay@swin.edu.au; robynbrown1@hotmail.com
Novelty. They will be introduced to the associations between humour use, emotional
self-efficacy and psychological well-being. There is relatively little known about the
relationships between intelligence in general and humour styles, and although there has
been some publication about emotional intelligence and humour, self-report scales of
emotional intelligence have been criticised on the grounds that people may not
accurately assess their own emotional intelligence. This study found that the effects of
positive humour styles (affiliative and self-enhancing) and the negative humour style of
self-defeating humour on psychological well-being were partially mediated by
emotional self-efficacy, a measure of what emotional intelligence people think they
have. It also found that social desirability remained a significant predictor.
Importance. Humour use is ubiquitous, and its relationships with well-being have
been the focus of some perhaps far-reaching claims. Learning that at least some of this
relationship is mediated by emotional self-efficacy gives a more nuanced view of the
association. Similarly, the importance of controlling for social desirability when using
self-report measures of self-referential constructs cannot be overstated.
Methods. An international sample of 438 participants responded to an online survey
including measures of humour styles (Martin et al., 2003), emotional self-efficacy
(Schutte et al., 2008), psychological well-being (Ryff, 1989) and social desirability
(Marlowe-Crowne, in Reynolds, 1982). The mediational effects were calculated using
Preacher & Hayes' (2008) INDIRECT method of multiple regression. Implications of
the results will be discussed.
41
T
A
L
K
S
Perceived Workload and Performance in Difficult Nonverbal Cognitive Tasks
Predict Academic Achievement
Meredith C. Frey, Cynthia Laurie-Rose
Otterbein University
MFrey@Otterbein.edu; CLaurie-Rose@Otterbein.edu
Novelty. Previous research presented at this conference indicated that, contrary to a
faster is smarter claim, high-achieving students sometimes take longer to solve difficult
nonverbal cognitive task items. The current study attempted to confirm and further
explore these findings with the addition of two difficult dual-task paradigms. The first
was a vigilance task under memory load and the second was a dual n-back working
memory task. Along with a previously studied battery of cognitive tasks these
experiments indicated, once again, that higher ability students took longer to complete
difficult trials, and they responded more cautiously in some cases. Further, higher
perceived mental workload in very difficult tasks was associated with higher academic
achievement and ability, perhaps reflecting a more accurate appraisal of a task’s
demands by the higher ability students.
Importance. Despite a wealth of information about the relationship between
intelligence and academic achievement, relatively little is known about the underlying
cognitive components that influence academic achievement in university students.
Further knowledge of these underlying processes may allow for the development of
shorter, experimentally controlled assessments that can be used to make admissions
decisions without measuring learned information.
Methods. Measures of speed, accuracy, and self-reported psychological workload
were collected from over 100 participants at a non-selective private university. Each
participant completed a 90-minute battery measuring fluid reasoning ability (from a
short form of the Advanced Progressive Matrices), reaction time, stimulus
discrimination (Frey, 2011), working memory (modeled after Jaeggi, et al., 2008) and
attention (Helton & Russell, 2011). Psychological workload was measured at 11 points
during the battery using the multidimensional NASA-TLX assessment (Hart &
Staveland, 1988). Academic transcript records of grade point averages (GPA) and ACT
scores were obtained from all participants, and transformed GPA was regressed onto
speed, accuracy, and workload measures to investigate relationships among variables.
42
An Empirical Test of the Dynamic Mutualism Theory of g
Gilles E. Gignac
University of Western Australia
gilles.gignac@uwa.edu.au
Novelty. According to the dynamic mutualism theory of the general (g) factor, the
positive manifold (and the g factor) is contended to be an epiphenomenon. It is an
epiphenomenon because the positive correlations between subtests (or group factors) are
theorised to emerge during human development, as a consequence of mutually
beneficial interactions between originally uncorrelated cognitive processes. Despite the
increasing popularity of the dynamic mutualism theory of g, the theory has not yet been
tested empirically.
According to dynamic mutualism theory, the general factor should not be observed in
infants and should be observed to increase in strength in very young children.
To test the dynamic mutualism theory of g, the strength of the g factor was plotted
across the ages of 2.5 to 90 years (N = 5,200). Although there was an observed increase
in the strength of the g factor from the ages of 2.5 to approximately 10.0, the slope was
so weak in magnitude that the dynamic mutualism theory of g was interpreted as largely
unsupported.
Importance. The competing theories that were tested in this paper (dynamic
mutualism versus g factor theory) are fundamental to the manner in which researchers
think about intelligence. Also, the dynamic mutualism theory of g has gained a notable
amount of popularity since its introduction in 2006. Arguably, it is important that it be
tested empirically.
Methods. There are two elements of the method used in this investigation which
arguably support the findings/conclusions.
First, very high quality data were used. Specifically, the Wechsler scales normative
sample data are arguably second to none with respect to normal population
representation. All of the analyses reported in this presentation (N = 5,200) are based on
Wechsler scale normative samples.
Secondly, the method used to estimate the strength of the g factor is, arguably,
modern, sophisticated, and relatively unambiguous. Specifically, when applied to a
bifactor solution, omega hierarchical represents the percentage of true score variance
unique to the g factor.
43
T
A
L
K
S
The Structure of Working Memory and How It Relates to Intelligence in Children
David Giofrè
1
, Irene C. Mammarella
2
, Cesare Cornoldi
1
1
Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Italy
2
Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, University of Padova, Italy
david.giofre@gmail.com; irene.mammarella@unipd.it; cesare.cornoldi@unipd.it
Novelty. This study explored the structure of working memory, and its relation with
intelligence in 176 typically-developing children in the 4th and 5th grades at school.
Different measures of working memory (WM), and intelligence (g) were administered.
Confirmatory factor analyses showed that WM involves an attentional control system
and storage aspects that rely on domain-specific verbal (STM-V) and visuospatial
(STM-VS) resources. The structural equation models showed that WM predicts a large
portion (66%) of the variance in g, confirming that the two constructs are separable but
closely related in young children. Findings also showed that only WM and STM-VS are
significantly related to g, while the contribution of STM-V is moderate.
Importance. The structure of WM and its relationship with intelligence has not
received enough attention in children. In the present study, we found that our data fitted
poorly with a unitary WM model, and with two-factor models that were either modality
dependent (distinguishing between visuospatial and verbal components) or modality
independent (distinguishing between STM and WM). Our findings indicate that
children's WM can be well represented by three components, which distinguishes
between a WM component and two storage components relying on domain-specific
verbal and visuospatial resources. This result is consistent with previous research on
populations of developmental age (e.g., Alloway et al., 2009). In addition, we found that
STM-VS (typically involving unfamiliar situations) predicts a unique portion of the
variance not explained by active WM whereas the verbal component (i.e., STM-V)
(typically involving more familiar material) is less relevant. Our results also confirm
that WM predicts a substantial portion of the g variance even when the effect of STM is
taken into account.
Methods. The method used was particularly appropriate for investigating the
structure of WM and the relationship between intelligence and WM. For a start, a large
sample of children from 4th- and 5th grades was included. Second, we used a large
number of tests of WM tests, which allowed us to test several alternative WM models.
Finally, in order to measure general intelligence, we used three tasks (two verbal and
one visuospatial task). This allowed us to have a more robust measure of general
intelligence which was, for example, not biased toward visuospatial tasks.
44
Do Functional SNPs Show an Enriched Association for Intelligence?
David W. Hill*
1
, Silviu A. Bacanu
2
, Riccardo E. Marioni
1
, Gail Davies
1
, Dave C.
Liewald
1
, Xianging Chen
2
, John M. Starr
1
, David J. Porteous
1
, Generation Scotland,
Timothy C. Bates
1
, Ian J. Deary
1
1
Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology Department of Psychology
University of Edinburgh, UK
2
Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth
University, Richmond, VA, USA
W.D.Hill@sms.ed.ac.uk; sabacanu@vcu.edu; rmarioni@staffmail.ed.ac.uk;
gail.davies@ed.ac.uk; dave.liewald@ed.ac.uk; xchen@vcu.edu;
jstarr@staffmail.ed.ac.uk; dporteou@staffmail.ed.ac.uk; timothy.c.bates@gmail.com;
iand@staffmail.ed.ac.uk
Novelty. In the present study we examined the association of intelligence with
neuronally-expressed functional Single Nucleotide Polymorphisms (SNPs), i.e. SNPs
that alter gene expression and function in the brain. These include promoter SNPs,
SNPs that change gene expression via microRNA binding, methylated SNPs and
eQTLs: SNPs that correlate with quantitative measures of gene expression. Methylated
SNPs and eQTLs were further divided according to their region of activation within the
brain, including frontal cortex, temporal cortex, cerebellum and pons giving a total of
10 functional SNP sets. Using subjects in the family-based Generation Scotland sample,
we tested whether any of these 10 categories of SNP show significant enrichment for
fluid or crystallised intelligence by comparison with non-functional SNPs. Results to
date support the idea that functional SNPs significantly enriched for association with
both gf and gc compared to SNPs which do not alter gene expression in the brain. We
will also present data testing whether this minority of SNPs accounts for the majority of
heritability
Importance. Intelligence is heritable and predicts wealth, health and mortality.
However, whilst twin studies have demonstrated that a heritable component accounts
for around 50% of the variation in intelligence differences, Genome Wide Association
Studies (GWAS) carried out to identify individual variants are currently too small to
reliably reveal such variants. The next challenge is to identify which genetic variants
contribute to variation in human intelligence. If SNPs can be pruned according to their
functional role, the power of genetic studies will be enhanced, bringing the prospect of
identifying small but true genetic effects on intelligence closer.
Methods. Experimentally validated promoter SNPs were extracted from dbQSNP
(http://qsnp.gen.kyushu-u.ac.jp/) with the functional categories of microRNA,
methylated SNPs, and eQTL SNPs being based on published literature. These functional
SNP sets were analysed using robust statistics using empirical tests of significance
based on simulations which account for both average background signal enrichment and
preserve the linkage disequilibrium (LD) between SNP statistics. This matching strategy
ensures that it is the biological group to which the SNPs belong, rather than extraneous
genomic features such patterns of LD which link these functional categories to cognitive
abilities.
45
T
A
L
K
S
“Wrecked by Success?” No! Linkages of Career Accomplishment, Health, and
Relationship Satisfaction among the Gifted
Harrison J. Kell, David Lubinski, Camilla P. Benbow
Vanderbilt University
harrison.kell@vanderbilt.edu; david.lubinski@vanderbilt.edu;
camilla.benbow@vanderbilt.edu
Novelty. Career success has long been anecdotally held to cost those who achieve it
their personal relationships and their mental and physical health; Freud (1917) coined
the phrase “wrecked by success.” Recent popular (e.g., Joiner, 2011) work suggests this
idea enjoys widespread appeal, but it has been the subject of little empirical scrutiny.
The current study examines the tenability of being “wrecked by success” in the modern
economic climate using an intellectually gifted sample drawn from the Study of
Mathematically Precocious Youth (SMPY). The pattern of results does not support the
idea that achieving noteworthy career success must entail enormous personal cost.
Overall, scores on health and relationship items did not covary significantly with
objectively measured career success. When significant differences were observed, they
tended to favor those who were more successful in their careers (e.g., lower divorce
rates, fewer health problems). These findings indicate that intellectually gifted
individuals can achieve career success without sacrificing their health and personal
lives.
Importance. The modern international economy is distinguished by intense
competition and pressure to produce frequent innovations in complex domains,
especially STEM (Friedman, 2007). Due to their capacity to analyze and manipulate
abstract symbols, intellectually gifted individuals are not only in demand in
government, business, and academia, they are also those most likely to consistently be
relied upon to push the boundaries of knowledge and skill. Intellectually talented
individuals are not some resource to be mined (Benbow & Stanley, 1996), however, and
it is appropriate to ask what the physical, mental, and interpersonal costs might be for
gifted individuals who are put under intense demands to be successful in their careers.
This study asks if gifted individuals handle the pressure put on them to actualize their
full potential, or are they "wrecked by success" when they do so? The results of this
study suggest they can handle it and that gifted individuals can thrive in their
professional and personal lives without sacrificing their mental and physical health in
the process.
Methods. Relationships between career success and a wide variety of well-known
health and interpersonal measures was examined in a sample of 1,650 intellectually
talented individuals (in the top 1% to .5% of ability). Participants were identified in
early adolescence using above-level testing and followed-up periodically. Career
success was assessed at midlife (mean age = 50), where participants were classified into
three “success groups” according to their primary incomes. Participants also completed
comprehensive surveys detailing the state of their mental, emotional, and physical
health, in addition to their relationship satisfaction and family status. Large sample sizes
contribute to the stability of results. The longitudinal nature of the data allows for the
examination of the long-term interplay of career success and important personal
variables among participants.
46
Metaphorical Representations of Psychological Concepts in Students and Their
Relation with Verbal and Nonverbal Intelligence
Yanina Ledovaya, Ksenia Mikhalchenko
Saint Petersburg State University
ledovaya@gmail.com; k.mihalchenko@mail.ru
Novelty. Because metaphor is considered to be the very essence of human intelligence
capacity, we investigated some characteristics of metaphor creation in connection with the
level of verbal and nonverbal components of intelligence. "The essence of metaphor is
understanding and experiencing one kind of thing in terms of another" (Lakoff, Johnson,
1980, p.6). In our study the task was to create pictographical metaphors conveying the
meaning of scientific psychological terms (specialized abstract concepts). The participants
were the students of 1st and 4th years.
We distinguish the two objects of research. On the one hand, it is: 1) the relationship of
verbal and nonverbal components of intelligence and the accuracy of metaphorical
representations of psychological concepts; and on the other hand, it is: 2) the influence of
verbalization of the formal definition of a psychological concept on the quality of
metaphorical representation.
Thus we aimed to find if verbal or nonverbal intelligence is more important in metaphor
creation; and if formal knowledge could help in metaphor creation.
Importance. There are commonsence ideas that metaphor is closely related to creativity.
But the study made by our colleague M.Avanesyan showed that the ability to create a good
metaphor doesn't relate to the level of creativity (Avanesyan, 2013). We aimed to test if
verbal or nonverbal intelligence contribute more into good metaphor creation.
During qualitative analysis we developed two indicator of a metaphor (metaphorical
representation) quality – "the meaningfulness of an image" and "metaphoricity" (how far
semantically is the idea of a metaphor from the concept itself).
Our results show that:
The main contribution to the accuracy of the meaning of a concept within a metaphor is
made by verbal intelligence.
Moreover, nonverbal intelligence doesn't affect neither the accuracy of metaphorical
representation, nor the accuracy of formal verbal representation of an abstract concept.
"Metaphoricity" remains an "elusive" indicator: we didn't find its analogues measured by
IQ testing.
Verbalization of the definition of a psychological concept doesn't promote improvement
of quality of a metaphor.
Methods. The methods we used: 1)to create and to draw metaphors for 3 psychological
terms – "insight", "introversion", "sensory deprivation", 2)to give the verbal definitions of
these terms, 3)to draw new metaphors 4)nonverbal Raven’s IQ test, 5)verbal subtest
"Similarities" taken from D.Wechsler’s IQ test.
Two experts evaluated the metaphorical drawings in a way similar to D. Wechsler’s test
system (0 pts–unsuccessful, 1 pt–partly successful, 2 pts-successful).
The idea of qualitative and quantitative analyses of formal verbal representation, as well
as the indicator of "the meaningfulness of an image"–were both borrowed from M.A.
Holodnaya's method "The integrated conceptual structures", but in the modified way.
By means of one-way ANOVA test we showed the relationship between the verbal
intelligence and the quality of formal verbal representation (definition) with "the
meaningfulness of an image" (ANOVA, p=0,006, p=0,036).
Wilcoxon's crit. showed the distinctions in the quality of "the meaningfulness of an image"
"before" and "after" the formal definition verbalization (p=0,032).
This work was supported by Saint Petersburg State University, research
№ 8.38.191.2011 “Information and Energetic Issues in Cognitive Activity”.
47
T
A
L
K
S
A Pilot Study of Rare Genetic Variants and G
Michelle Luciano
1
, Chris Haley
2
, Nick Hastie
2
, Caroline Hayward
2
, James Prendergast,
Pippa Thomson, Veronique Vitart, Alan Wright, David Porteous, Ian Deary, Generation
Scotland
1
Psychology, University of Edinburgh
2
MRC Human Genetics Unit, University of Edinburgh
michelle.luciano@ed.ac.uk
Novelty. Various genetic linkage, genome-wide SNP association, and genetic copy
number variant studies have investigated genetic variation in cognitive ability, but no
studies have investigated genetic rare variants. These may be a potentially important
source of genetic variation; exome sequencing studies have already met with success in
discovering novel trait-gene associations. Here, we investigate the effects of rare
variants on general cognitive ability. 150 unrelated individuals from the Generation
Scotland: Scottish Family Health Study were selected for high scores (>2.3 SD from the
mean ability score) on a general component of intelligence (g) based on Logical
Memory immediate and delayed, Digit Symbol, Verbal Fluency, and Mill Hill
Vocabulary test. The DNA of these individuals was exome-sequenced and the
frequency of rare genetic variants compared with those from a control sample who
scored in the lower to middle range of the g distribution. The results of single-SNP and
multi-SNP tests will be presented, including a discussion of the merits of these
approaches and their power.
Importance. This is one of the first genetic rare variant studies of general cognitive
ability and increases our understanding of the genetic architecture of general cognition.
Methods. Genetic sequencing is expensive, so we were limited to genotyping the
high extreme of our sample. But these methods are becoming more affordable.
Therefore, these preliminary results will be able to inform our future larger genetic rare
variant study design. And the sequencing data can be integrated with existing SNP data
to improve genome coverage and potential to identify genetic variants associated with
cognitive ability.
48
Dietary Interventions to Offset Cognitive Decline
Helen N. Macpherson, Andrew Pipingas, Andrew Scholey
Swinburne University
hmacpherson@swin.edu.au; apipingas@swin.edu.au; ascholey@swin.edu.au
Novelty. Cognitive decline is a feature of the normal ageing process and is
exacerbated in Alzheimer’s disease. Evidence suggests that vitamin intake and other
dietary factors may be important for cognition as individual’s age. We propose that the
cognitive domains most vulnerable to age-related decline will demonstrate the greatest
benefits from dietary interventions.
We have conducted several trials which have investigated the effects of multivitamin
supplementation on cognitive performance in older adults. These studies have
demonstrated that multivitamin supplementation improved working memory and
episodic memory, both processes which decline with age. Measures of brain electrical
activity (EEG) confirmed findings that memory was influenced by the multivitamin.
Findings suggest that chronic multivitamin supplementation may be effective to
improve neurocognition in older adults. The use of both behavioural and brain electrical
activity measures of cognition enables a thorough examination of potential mechanisms
of cognitive enhancement following dietary intervention.
Importance. With an ageing global population there is an increasing scientific
interest in the potential of health and lifestyle interventions to improve cognitive
function in the elderly. It is important to clearly differentiate interventions which are not
capable of influencing cognitive function from those which have shown small or
inconsistent effects due to poor choice of cognitive assessment instruments. Findings
from our research into the cognitive effects of multivitamins supports our hypothesis
that the cognitive processes which decline with age are most responsive to dietary
intervention.
Methods. The trials were randomised, placebo-controlled and double-blind.
Cognitive performance was assessed using a battery of computerised memory and
attentional tasks and it was predicted that chronic treatment with a combined
multivitamin and herbal supplement would enhance the cognitive domains most
vulnerable to age-related decline. Measures of working memory and episodic memory
were demonstrated to improve following multivitamin supplementation. A spatial
working memory delayed response task was also performed during the recording of
brain electrical activity (EEG). These results confirmed the finding that memory was
enhanced by the multivitamin and suggested that cognitive improvements may be due to
an increase in neural efficiency.
49
T
A
L
K
S
Associations Between Creative Achievement in Scientific and Artistic Domains,
Intelligence, Personality and Sex
Örjan de Manzano
1
, Guy Madison
2
, Fredrik Ullén
1
1
Karolinska Institutet, Department of Neuroscience
2
Umeå University, Department of Psychology
orjan.demanzano@ki.se; guy.madison@umu.se; fredrik.ullen@ki.se
Novelty. We correlated creative achievement scores (CAQ) from seven domains
(Visual arts, Music, Dance, Theater, Writing, Invention, Science) and found positive
associations between all domains. A PCA of the CAQ showed two factors with
eigenvalues > 1; a general creativity factor (cf1), explaining 29% of the variance, and a
second factor (cf2) separating scientific from artistic achievements, explaining 18%. In
two separate models we regressed factor scores of cf1 and cf2 respectively, on IQ, big
five personality, psychosis proneness, sex and age. cf1 was associated with IQ, sex (f),
E, A, -C, N, O, psychosis proneness and age. Only IQ (β = .14) and O (β = .45) had a β
> .1. cf2 (science) was associated with IQ, sex (m), -E and O. Only sex (β = .27) and IQ
(β = .21) reached a β > .1. The domain specific results (β > .1): Visual arts – O, sex (f);
Music – O, IQ; Dance – O, sex (f); Theater – O; Writing – O; Invention – O, IQ, sex
(m); Science – O, IQ. In summary, O moderately predicts CAQ in all domains. IQ has a
weaker general effect but is more associated with Science, Invention and Music.
Importance. Though psychometric studies on creativity suggest that
creative/divergent thinking can be distinguished from fluid reasoning, individuals
demonstrating eminent CAQ nonetheless typically display high levels of IQ. The
precise nature and strength of the relationship between CAQ and IQ is not well
understood. Nor is it clear if and to what extent the association with IQ varies between
creative domains. Scientific and artistic domains will e.g. presumably pose different
demands on cognitive abilities and it can be hypothesized that IQ is related to both self-
selection and level of achievement. Various personality traits have also been associated
with creativity, in particular O (from the Big Five Inventory), but also psychosis
proneness. The importance of certain traits has also been argued to differ between
creative domains but this has yet to be demonstrated in a large scale study. The
prevalence of mental illness is higher among individuals within creative domains and
their relatives, but it is not known how e.g. psychosis proneness actually relates to
achievement.
Methods. The data were collected in a web-based survey. 32,500 twins born 1958-85
were sent personal login details by mail. The present sample included 6,603 individuals.
The survey included several self-report questionnaires. For the present study, we
collected CAQ (seven levels; ranging from no involvement, to amateur production, to
professional excellence), in seven domains (Visual art, Dance, Music, Theater, Writing,
Invention, Science); IQ using the Wiener Matrizen Test; Big Five personality, using the
BFI-44, with measures of extraversion (E), agreeableness (A), conscientiousness (C),
neuroticism (N), openness (O); psychosis proneness, using the positive symptoms scale
of the CAPE-42; age and sex. These preliminary analyses were performed, firstly by
correlating CAQ scores from different domains, secondly by doing a PCA of the CAQ
scores and thirdly, by regressing factor scores from the PCA on all other variables
(random split-half sample analyses, twins separated). Only results which were
significant in both samples are reported.
50
Genetic and Environmental Influences on the Relationship Between Musical
Discrimination Tasks and IQ
Miriam A Mosing
1
, Nancy L. Pedersen
2
, Guy Madison
3
, Fredrik Ullén
1
1
Department of Neuroscience, Karolinska Institute
2
Dept of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institute
3
Department of Psychology, Umeå University
Miriam.Mosing@ki.se; Fredrik.Ullen@ki.se
Novelty. Performance on many sensory discrimination tasks is positively related to
intelligence. Such associations have been demonstrated for various sensory dimensions,
e.g. the pitch and loudness of auditory stimuli, and the colour and shape of visual
figures. Studies using latent variable modelling suggest that a common, general
discrimination ability factor may account for a substantial component of such
associations, although there is also evidence for additional, more specific influences of
e.g. temporal discrimination ability on intelligence. Past studies have generally focused
on phenotypic associations, and little is known about the role of genetic factors for these
associations. In the present study, for the first time a large genetically informative
sample of Swedish twins (N=10,000) is used to explore this issue.
Importance. The study will help to elucidate fundamental mechanisms mediating
individual differences in intelligence and its relationship to temporal and non-temporal
sensory discrimination abilities.
Methods. More than 10,000 twin individuals completed an extensive online
questionnaire that included auditory discrimination tasks as well as a timed intelligence
test (the Wiener Matrizen Test), similar in construction to the Raven progressive
matrices. Three auditory discrimination tasks of a type commonly used to assess music
aptitude were employed, i.e. rhythm, melody and pitch discrimination. As expected, the
phenotypic relationships between intelligence and the discrimination tasks were
moderate and highly significant with correlations ranging between 0.22 – 0.39. The twin
model allows for partitioning of the variance in and covariance between the variables
into that due to genetic and environmental influences. Potential sex-differences will be
explored and results will be discussed in relation to the past literature.
51
T
A
L
K
S
Neural Efficiency as a Function of Task Demands
Aljoscha C. Neubauer, Beate Dunst, Emanuel Jauk, Mathias Benedek
Department of Psychology, University of Graz, Austria
aljoscha.neubauer@uni-graz.at
Novelty. The neural efficiency hypothesis describes the phenomenon that brighter
individuals show lower brain activation than less bright individuals when working on
the same cognitive tasks. Consequently, the same task is more easy for individuals with
higher cognitive ability, but more difficult for less intelligent individuals. Since the
person-specific level of task difficulty can be expected to be associated with the time
and effort spent on the cognitive task, it could also be responsible for observed
differences in brain activation. The present study investigated whether the brain
activation-intelligence relationship still applies when more versus less intelligent
individuals perform tasks with the same person-specific task difficulty.
Differences in task performance and in brain activation were only found for the
subset of tasks with same sample-based task difficulty, but not when comparing tasks
with the same person-specific task difficulty. These results suggest that neural
efficiency reflects an ability-dependent adaption of brain activation to the respective
task demands.
Importance. The results provide evidence that neural efficiency is a function of both
intelligence and task demands. Results indicate that the neural efficiency hypothesis
needs to be refined. According to the refined definition, neural efficiency describes the
phenomenon that more intelligent individuals show lower brain activity than less
intelligent ones only when working on cognitive tasks with a comparable sample-based
difficulty. We hypothesize that this reflects a lower investment of mental effort due to
lower person-specific challenge. However, when equal person-specific challenge is
established lower versus higher IQ brains show similar brain activity levels. These
results suggest that the neural efficiency phenomenon may actually be explained by the
adaption of brain activation to the person-specific task demands.
Methods. In an fMRI-study, 28 lower and 30 higher intelligent individuals worked on
20 numerical inductive reasoning tasks (Arendasy et al., 2008). The used items are
automatically generated Rasch-calibrated items which provide the opportunity to
estimate the task difficulty relative to different levels of intelligence (Rasch, 1980).
While higher intelligent participants worked on number series of medium and high
difficulty, lower intelligent participants completed easy and medium difficult number
series. In this manner all participants completed number series with expected mean
person specific solution probability of .80 and .50, respectively. This enabled us to
evaluate, whether differences in task performance and brain activation between IQ
groups are attributable to group differences in person-specific solution probabilities. In
addition, we were able to compare the task performance and brain activation of lower
and higher intelligent participants when working on identical item set, since both groups
worked on items of medium difficulty (same sample-based task difficulty).
52
Low IQ and Mild Mental Retardation are Heritable But Severe Mental
Retardation is Not: a Genetic Analysis of 740,000 Siblings and 18,000 Twins
Robert Plomin
1
, Martin Cederlöf
2
, Paul Lichtenstein
2
1
Institute of Psychiatry, King\'s College London
2
Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institute
robert.plomin@kcl.ac.uk; Martin.Cederlof@ki.se; Paul.Lichtenstein@ki.se
Novelty. Despite the obvious societal importance of mental retardation (MR) in our
increasingly technological world, the most fundamental question about the genetic and
environmental origins of MR has not been definitively addressed. We will report new
results showing that diagnosed mild MR (IQ 50-70) and low IQ in the general
population are caused by the same genetic and environmental factors responsible for the
normal range of intelligence. In contrast, most severe MR (IQ <50) is not inherited,
despite hundreds of known single-gene causes of MR. Because severe MR is not
inherited, its most likely causes are environmental factors, some of which are well
known, such as prenatal trauma, neurotoxicity, and infections. Although severe MR is
not inherited, MR is a burgeoning area of genetic research using new techniques (exome
and whole genome sequencing) to identify non-inherited (de novo) mutations.
Importance. Molecular genetic research attempting to identify de novo mutations
responsible for MR will benefit from focusing on severe MR (IQ < 50), which is not
inherited. Although research on de novo mutations is an exciting new direction for
identifying causes of severe MR, severe MR is fortunately rare (.001). Much more of
the societal burden of intellectual disability lies with mild MR, which is the low extreme
of the same genetic and environmental factors responsible for the normal distribution of
intelligence. In other words, genes responsible for the substantial heritability of
intelligence are the same genes responsible for MR. Finding genes for MR other than
severe MR will involve the same problems faced throughout the life sciences for
common disorders and complex traits: many genes of very small effect and ‘missing
heritability’. A possible advantage for IQ is that it has a high end as well as a low end:
It might be easier finding genes associated with high IQ than low IQ, even though these
genes are expected to be associated with low IQ as well as MR.
Methods. We used data from 3 million 18-year-old males assessed for cognitive
abilities as part of compulsory military service in Sweden 1950-1990, which included
370,000 sibling pairs and 9000 twin pairs. We then linked these individuals to the
Swedish National Patient Register to identify 308 siblings with a diagnosis of severe
MR and 813 siblings with a diagnosis of mild MR. Two major findings emerged from
our analyses of sibling and twin correlations and concordances and model-fitting. First,
severe MR is not heritable in that siblings of individuals with severe MR had IQ means
and variances not significantly different from the population. Second, mild MR as well
as the lowest 3% of IQ scores in the population is familial, heritable, and caused by the
same genetic and environmental factors responsible for the normal distribution of
intelligence.
53
T
A
L
K
S
2013 Survey of Expert Opinion on Intelligence
Heiner Rindermann
1
, Thomas R. Coyle
2
, David Becker
1
1
Department of Psychology, Chemnitz University of Technology, Germany
2
Department of Psychology, University of Texas at San Antonio, San Antonio, USA
heiner.rindermann@psychologie.tu-chemnitz.de; Thomas.Coyle@utsa.edu;
david.becker@s2009.tu-chemnitz.de
Novelty. In 1988 (conducted 1984), Snyderman and Rothman published a survey of
expert opinions on intelligence and its treatment by the media. Since their survey, new
findings have appeared in the literature. To examine current opinions of experts, we
created a new questionnaire (“Expert Questionnaire on Cognitive Ability“), partly based
on the questions of Snyderman and Rothman, reflecting the current state of research.
We added questions on contemporary topics such as the FLynn effect and international
intelligence comparisons. Other questions addressed the definitions of terms (e.g.,
intelligence and cognitive ability); the validity of tests (e.g., WAIS/WISC, SAT, PISA);
the estimation of genetic and environmental influences; the treatment of intelligence
research by the media; the importance of genetic testing (e.g., DNA screening); and the
development of intelligence in global regions and populations.
Reference:
Snyderman, M. & Rothman, S. (1988). The IQ controversy, the media and public
policy. New Brunswick: Transaction.
Importance. We present the results of our survey and compare them with the one
from Snyderman and Rothman 30 years ago. The use of the terms “intelligence”,
“cognitive ability”, and “cognitive competence” was not uniform. However, there was
broad consensus that the most important attributes of intelligence were reasoning,
abstract thinking, problem solving, and the g-factor, with little importance attributed to
motivation, personality, and sensory acuity. Twin studies were still rated as providing
the best evidence for heritability, followed by adoption studies, molecular genetic
studies, and patchwork family studies. There was broad consensus that tests show little
or no bias against ethnic or social groups, with test taker motivation being seen as one
factor with potential for bias. Immigrants were viewed as a group that might be
disadvantaged by the use of tests. Intelligence was rated as being most important in
special education planning, followed by decisions at universities, primary and
secondary schools, and work. Concerns were expressed about the treatment of
intelligence by the media.
Methods. The survey consisted of 62 main questions with follow-up questions and
space for comments. Experts were sampled from publications addressing intelligence,
cognitive abilities, and student achievement. Authors of the past 4 volumes (2010-13) in
Intelligence were included. Notice of the study was emailed to ISIR members and
posted to the ISSID homepage, and colleagues were asked to inform other researchers
about the study. Only people who received a participation code could participate. 226
people responded (from March to July 2013); 94 people completed the questionnaire.
Participants had affiliations in psychology (84%), education (7%), economics (4%),
biology (4%) and sociology (2%). 38% of experts were from the US, 20% Germany,
8% UK, 8% Scandinavia, 8% Spain, 7% Canada, 3% Australia and New Zealand, and
3% from Latin America. The representativeness of the expert sample, possible biases,
and reasons for not participating are discussed.
54
Alcohol Consumption and Lifetime Change in Cognitive Ability: A Mendelian
Randomization Study
Stuart J. Ritchie*, Timothy C. Bates, Janie Corley, Geraldine McNeill, Gail Davies,
David C. Liewald, John M. Starr, Ian J. Deary
The University of Edinburgh
stuartjritchie1@gmail.com; tim.bates@ed.ac.uk; janie.corley@ed.ac.uk;
g.mcneill@abdn.ac.uk; gail.davies@ed.ac.uk; dave.liewald@ed.ac.uk;
jstarr@staffmail.ed.ac.uk; i.deary@ed.ac.uk
Novelty. We used a Mendelian Randomisation design to test the hypothesis that a
genetic score for alcohol processing capacity moderated the association between alcohol
consumption and lifetime change in cognitive ability. We found a significant interaction
between a 4-SNP alcohol metabolism genotype and alcohol consumption on lifetime
cognitive change (with cognition assessed at age 11 and age 70), such that individuals
with fewer rare alleles (high alcohol metabolising ability) had a positive association
between alcohol consumption and successful cognitive ageing. This was not found for
individuals with low metabolising ability, who declined in cognitive ability as a
function of alcohol consumption.
Importance. There is widespread concern about the determinants of healthy cognitive
ageing, and conflicting evidence about the cognitive and health effects of alcohol
consumption. A better understanding of, first, the effects of alcohol on cognition, and
second, the possible genetic mediators of this relationship, could lead to better-informed
efforts to prevent cognitive decline in old age.
Methods. The Mendelian Randomisation design is a useful way to avoid the serious
problems of confounding and reverse causation in observational epidemiological
research, since the genetic variants involved are unrelated to possible confounders, such
as socioeconomic status. Our large longitudinal sample (The Lothian Birth Cohort 1936,
valid n = 800) also allowed us to control for intelligence in early life, and thus assess
lifetime cognitive change, rather than intelligence measured at only one point, and also
used a well-validated measure of alcohol consumption, the Food Frequency
Questionnaire (FFQ).
55
T
A
L
K
S
Exploring the Relationship Between Intelligence, Creativity, Inspection Time, and
Inhibition
John H. Song, John G. Chetwynd
De Montfort University
jsong@dmu.ac.uk
Novelty. The relationship between intelligence and creativity has been examined and
debated relatively extensively. There were investigations about the nature of
intelligence-creativity relationship which were examined through higher order latent
variables, personality, executive processes, and strategy-use. The present study
examines the relationship between intelligence and creativity through the use of higher
level inhibitory processes measures and elementary cognitive tasks. A sample of largely
university students completed computerised Raven’s Progressive Matrices, Unusual
Uses task, Self-rated creativity measure, inspection time task, Stroop and Latent
Inhibition task. The results showed that intelligence, as measured using Raven’s
Progressive Matrices was significantly predicted only by inspection time, but not
inhibition variables, or creativity measured using Unusual Uses task and Self-rated
Creativity score. The result suggests that apart from inspection time, other inhibitory
processes do not significantly contribute toward intelligence.
Importance. Formal definitions of intelligence have often included problem-solving
ability. However, therein lay the tension between intelligence and creativity. The
former often involves completion of problems with a unique solution whereas creativity
encourages generation of multiple solutions. In order to derive a correct solution an
intelligent individual would engage in some selection processes. To do so successfully,
it may be that inhibition processes which prevents competing solutions from distracting
the individual would be necessary. On the contrary, creativity, would usually involve
generating as many solutions as possible. These are higher level processes. The current
study will add to the current knowledge in this field by examining inhibition processes
as higher level processes, but it has also include the use of an elementary cognitive task.
It will lead to further refinement of research methodologies and further examination of
important variables explaining the intelligence-creativity relationship.
Methods. Although it is moderately small in sample size, this ambitious and labour
intensive study examined intelligence as measured by Raven’s Progressive Matrices;
Creativity as measured subjectively and objectively through Self-rated Creativity scale,
and Unusual Uses task respectively; two types of inhibition processes using Cognitive
Inhibition (Stroop task) and a Latent Inhibition task; and processing speed measured
using elementary cognitive task – Inspection time. The number of variables examined
here enables a unique opportunity to explore the relationship between these variable,
notably between intelligence and creativity. Multiple regression analyses enabled
examination of the ability of various variables in predicting intelligence.
56
Intelligence and Somatic Health in Early Adulthood, and Mortality up to Age 59 -a
Longitudinal Study of 49 000 Men
Alma Sörberg*
1
, Peter Allebeck
2
, Tomas Hemmingsson
3,4
1
Institute for Environmental Medicine, Karolinska Institute, Stockholm, Sweden
2
Department of Public Health Sciences, Karolinska Institute, Stockholm, Sweden
3
Institute for Environmental Medicine, Karolinska Institute, Stockholm, Sweden
4
Centre for Social Research on Alcohol and Drugs, Stockholm University, Stockholm,
Sweden
alma.sorberg@ki.se; peter.allebeck@ki.se; tomas.hemmingsson@ki.se
Novelty. The association between intelligence in youth and later illness and early
death is established, but the mechanisms underlying these associations are not fully
understood (Calvin et al, 2011). One suggestion is that higher bodily system integrity
promotes faster intellectual processing as well as better health, and thus act as a
confounder in the association (Deary, 2012). We found that among a broad range of
clinical diagnoses in youth, several were cross-sectionally associated with intelligence.
However, the prevalence of somatic disorders in youth did not explain the association
between intelligence and later mortality.
Importance. Bodily system integrity, as potential underlying factor in the association
between IQ and mortality, is difficult to examine directly. Here, we had the opportunity
to investigate if somatic diagnoses in youth, a plausible indicator of system integrity,
would affect the association. This would indicate that the path from lower intelligence
to early death is noticeable already in youth, and thus provide some evidence of system
integrity at play. A better understanding of how intelligence predicts illness and death is
crucial for appropriate prevention and treatment, and yields valuable pointers for further
research in cognitive epidemiology.
Methods. In a cohort comprising 49 321 Swedish men born 1949-51, IQ test
performance and medical diagnoses were recorded at the two-day conscription process
in 1969-70, at ages 18-20. In the thorough medical examination, any diagnoses were
given according to the ICD-8, the diagnostic system used at that time. The National
Cause of Death Register provided information on all-cause mortality up to age 59 (3351
cases). We adjusted for socioeconomic position in childhood by record linkage with the
national census in 1960.
Although several diagnostic groups (by ICD-8 chapters) were associated with IQ,
adjusting for somatic diagnoses had minimal impact on the association of IQ with later
mortality. Since bodily system integrity is suggested to manifest as reduced health as
well as lower IQ, this finding lends limited support for the hypothesis of bodily system
integrity as an underlying cause for the association between intelligence and mortality.
Implications and alternative interpretations will be discussed.
57
T
A
L
K
S
Climatic Variability, Group Selection and Dysgenics:
Testing a Multi-Level Selection Model
Michael A Woodley
1
, Heitor B. F. Fernandes
2
, Aurelio José Figueredo
3
1
Umeå University, Sweden & Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Belgium
2
Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil
3
University of Arizona
Michael.Woodley@psy.umu.se; heitor.barcellos@ufrgs.br; ajf@u.arizona.edu
Novelty. A multi-level selection model published recently by Woodley and Figueredo
proposes that climatic changes have historically affected the direction of gene-frequency
changes for g via their alternating impact on group vs. individual level fitness. In the West,
colder environments imposed high extrinsic mortality on peasantry, whilst also creating
fitness opportunities for those with higher-g. This also encouraged group selection for ultra
high-g but low-fitness geniuses, whose innovations facilitated range expansion (i.e.
colonialism).
Warmer environments led to lower mortality amongst those with low-g. Concomitant
social and scientific innovations (i.e. welfare, medicine) further increased the mildness of
Western environments, leading to diminished group selection, coupled with greater
individual level selection for those with low-g responding to the fitness-incentives provided
by the improved ecology, in addition to individual level selection against those with high-g,
whose fitness diminished in the face of improved fertility control and redistributionist
economic policy.
Importance. Multiple indexes of major innovation in science and technology indicate a
pronounced per capita decline commencing in the latter half of the 19th century. Indexes of
per capita scientific genius show a similar decline. Woodley and Figueredo, using
hierarchical structural equations modelling, found that both of these trends relate to
simulated declines in heritable g estimated on the basis of the negative relationship between
fertility and IQ throughout this period. Recently, much attention was paid to the finding that
simple reaction time performance seems to have been slowing throughout this same period,
suggesting that the dysgenic decline in g predicted in previous works might be an actuality.
These findings are all consistent with the multi-level selection model as described above.
Understanding how these ecological factors might explicitly relate to real changes in the
patterns of selective pressure is important for understanding the interconnected and varied
nature of the determinants of accelerating adaptive biological and cultural evolution
amongst Holocene populations.
Methods. Here we test the multi-level selection model over the last 1.5 centuries using a
cascade General Linear Model. Climate warming is operationalized using three convergent
indicators of global temperature anomaly means spanning the period from 1859 to 1975. A
lexical approach to measuring historical attitudes was used to determine group selection
strength in the US+UK. Three convergent group selection \'loaded\' words were selected,
and their diminishing frequencies across printed matter were measured using Google
Ngram. Declining g was measured using meta-analytically matched simple reaction time
trend data from the UK+US corrected for various sources of error, spanning the period 1889
to 1993. US+UK innovations were taken from a database of global innovations and
weighted on the basis of US+UK population growth.
The model: Climate warming -> Group selected attitudes (with a lag of ten years) ->
Declining g (with a lag of one generation) -> Innovation rates; fit excellently. The fit
improved when war years were excluded from the innovation index. This is consistent with
the model.
59
S
Y
M
P
O
S
I
A
SIMPOSIA
Abstracts are organized in order of presentation time
Names of students eligible for the John B. Caroll Award for Research
Methodology and ISIR Prize for Best Student Paper
are marked with an asterisk
60
SYMPOSIUM 1
Worst Performance Rule:
Sources, Moderating Variables, and Implications for Intelligence Research
Yulia A. Dodonova
1,2
Yury S. Dodonov
1
1
Moscow State University of Psychology and Education
2
Peoples' Friendship University of Russia
ya.dodonova@mail.ru; ys.dodonov@gmail.com
To obtain a reliable measure of the speed of information processing, elementary
cognitive tasks (ECTs) are designed to include multiply repeated trials, for which
response times (RTs) are recorded. This produces a set of individual-level RTs, which
do not necessarily equally well predict cognitive ability. A frequently observed effect is
that when individual-level RTs are ordered from fastest to slowest, the slowest RTs
produce higher correlations with cognitive ability than the fastest RTs. This effect is
called the worst performance rule (WPR). An explanation for WPR suggested by
previous studies has implied that individuals differ in efficiency of memory and
attention, or in some basic characteristics of neuron functioning, and this is what matters
when both ECTs and intelligence-like tests are performed. However, previous studies
on WPR have been too rare to accumulate consistent evidence of its causes,
confounding the variables and the implications for an analysis of the association
between intelligence and the speed of processing of various ECTs.
Taken together, the three papers of this symposium reconsider the issue of different
RT-IQ associations reported for best- and worst-performance trials and discuss their
possible causes and implications for studies examining the association between
cognitive ability and the speed of information processing. In the first paper, Yulia
Dodonova analyzes a set of elementary cognitive tasks and questions whether WPR is
indeed present in these tasks and whether it can be explained via the effect of
confounding variables and statistical artifacts.
In the second paper, Natalie Borter discusses the implications of WPR when
analyzing individual performance in tasks of varying complexity. Such tasks imply that
individual differences in the speed of basic constant processes and task-specific
experimentally-induced processes can be analyzed at the latent level, and their
associations with cognitive ability can be evaluated. However, as shown in this study,
averaging across best and worst trials within each complexity level can mask another
source of variance, which can also be meaningful and can provide additional insights
into the associations between the speed of task processing and intelligence.
Finally, a paper by Yury Dodonov analyzes WPR-like effects in the context of
speed-accuracy relations that are always present when a participant performs speeded
tasks. This study suggests that accuracy rate is another factor that must be considered in
any WPR-type analysis of associations between individual-level response times and
intelligence.
61
S
Y
M
P
O
S
I
A
Are the Slowest Response Times More G-Loaded Than the Fastest Reactions?
Evidence, Hypotheses, and Implications of the Worst Performance Rule
Yulia A. Dodonova
1,2
, Yury S. Dodonov
1
, Sergey I. Kudinov
2
1
Moscow State University of Psychology and Education
2
Peoples' Friendship University of Russia
ya.dodonova@mail.ru; ys.dodonov@gmail.com
Novelty. The worst performance rule (WPR) suggests that when response times
(RTs) obtained for repeated trials of a speeded task are ordered from fastest to slowest,
the slowest RTs are more strongly correlated with cognitive ability than the fastest RTs.
If the rule truly holds, this raises important questions about what lies beyond the
conventionally analyzed correlations between average processing speed and cognitive
ability. However, to accept that the slowest RT trials are indeed higher g-loaded, one
should rule out the possibility that the effect is caused by some source of statistical
artifact or is confounded by some other meaningful effect. Previous studies have been
too rare to provide sufficient evidence that would allow for critically testing the
hypotheses on the underlying causes of the WPR. Hence, this study reconsiders the
issue of variations in RT-IQ correlations across multiply-repeated RT trials. Based on
data obtained from a set of elementary cognitive tasks (ECTs), it questions whether
there is indeed a WPR-like effect and (if yes) whether this effect can be explained via
various confounding variables.
Importance. The observation that the slowest RTs obtained during the processing of
various ECTs seem to be better predictors of g than the fastest RT reactions readily
suggests that the explanations are related to cognitive variables, such as lapses in
memory and attention (e.g., Larson & Alderton, 1990) or neuron errors (e.g., see Coyle,
2003), which are hypothesized to be more frequent in low-ability individuals then in
high-ability individuals. If so, the WPR provides insights into the nature of the
association between intelligence and speed of information processing. However, such an
interpretation is meaningful if and only if the WPR is not an artifact in a statistical
sense. Therefore, this study considers the possible non-cognitive sources of WPR and
discusses the implications of WPR for studies examining speed-ability associations.
Methods. The ECTs analyzed in this study included simple RT, choice RT, and
different versions of discrimination RT (e.g., discrimination between shapes or colors).
Intelligence entered the analysis as a single score (Raven’s APM) or a sum score based
on a number of subtests. Data were obtained from samples of high school and college
students. In addition, the simulated variables were involved where appropriately to
differentiate between the g-related effects and the purely statistical effects. Various
possible explanations for WPR were considered, including trial sequence effects (e.g.,
the sequence number of a trial in a set, switching between responses vs. repetition of the
same response) and statistical issues (e.g., the skewness of the sample distribution of the
respective variables). In addition, the question of whether WPR is indeed truer for more
difficult tasks compared to easier tasks, as suggested by previous studies, is addressed.
62
Cognitive Tasks With Increasing Complexity: Should Worst Performance Rule Be
Taken Into Account?
Natalie Borter*
1
, Stefan Troche
1
, Yulia Dodonova
2
, Thomas Rammsayer
1
1
University of Bern
2
Moscow State University of Psychology and Education
natalie.borter@psy.unibe.ch; stefan.troche@psy.unibe.ch; ya.dodonova@mail.ru;
thomas.rammsayer@psy.unibe.ch
Novelty. Homogenous tasks with increasing complexity imply that observed RT
variance can be decomposed into parts that represent (1) a constant process representing
basic aspects of information processing unrelated to the experimentally manipulated
task complexity and (2) an experimental process increasing with task complexity. In
contrast the worst performance rule (WPR) states that on cognitive task with multiple
trials, worst performance trials predict general intelligence (g) better than best
performance trials. Previous studies suggested that with increasing task complexity,
WPR may be more pronounced. This conclusion, however, was mainly based on
comparisons between tasks that largely differed in demands, not only in complexity.
The present study is the first one to explicitly address the question of whether WPR-like
effects vary with task complexity. It, thus, focuses on the question of whether WPR-
related effects affect task performance, interpretability of baseline RT, and
experimentally-induced RT-change, as well as their associations with intelligence.
Importance. Performance measures obtained from cognitive tasks may represent not
only the concept of interest but can also be considered an outcome of various
underlying processes and, thus, result in so-called impure measurements. With our
approach, we could avoid the impurity problem and investigate the influence of two
different sources of variance (i.e., complexity manipulation and processes unrelated to
the complexity manipulation) on psychometric intelligence. In 2003, Coyle reviewed
evidence, theory, and alternative hypotheses of the WPR and stressed complexity as a
possible mediating factor. He ended with a call for research on the causes of the WPR
and for research on the correlates of best performance. Therefore, in addition, WRP-like
effects were included in the model and the links to the two different sources of variance
as well as to psychometric intelligence were investigated.
Methods. Three computerized, homogenous cognitive tasks with increasing
complexity as well as a paper pencil version of Cattell's Culture Fair Test (CFT-20)
were completed by 130 participants (mostly students). From the four subtests of CFT-
20, a latent psychometric intelligence factor was extracted. For the cognitive tasks two
latent variables were modeled: One latent variable represented the constant process and,
therefore, factor loadings were fixed to equal unity for all conditions. The other latent
variable represented the shape of the experimental process and factor loadings were
therefore fixed according to a strictly increasing function to reflect the increasing task
complexity. With this approach, the links between the latent intelligence factor and the
experimental and non-experimental process were investigated. In addition, worst
performance analysis was incorporated by either explicit modeling of an additional
latent variable or by comparing models based on worst- and best-performance indexes.
63
S
Y
M
P
O
S
I
A
The Slowest and Fastest Responses are Not Equally Good Predictors of
Intelligence, But Does Accuracy Matter?
Yury S. Dodonov
1
, Yulia A. Dodonova
1,2
, Sergey I. Kudinov
2
1
Moscow State University of Psychology and Education
2
Peoples' Friendship University of Russia
ys.dodonov@gmail.com; ya.dodonova@mail.ru
Novelty. A commonly acknowledged problem in studies on individual differences in
processing speed and speed-ability associations is that, when performing elementary
cognitive tasks (ECTs), each participant necessarily adopts some speed-accuracy
strategy. Moreover, a participant can speed up or slow down while processing the same
task, thus compromising or emphasizing the accuracy of responding. On the other hand,
individual-level RTs are not equally good predictors of intelligence; those responses
that are slowest seem to be better predictors of ability than the fastest responses, an
observation known as the worst performance rule (WPR). However, previous WPR-
type analyses considered only RTs (or only accuracy rate), thus discarding the other
important source of information. To our knowledge, this is the first study to analyze
how speed-ability association changes for RTs obtained across trials of a speeded task
while simultaneously considering speed-accuracy associations.
Importance. This study suggests that the accuracy of responding might act as an
explanatory mechanism for WPR or, more generally, for any tendency of some
individual-level RTs to be more g-loaded than other RTs obtained within the same task.
In addition, the study raises an important question by seeking to determine what
measure of RT most adequately represents the individual-level speed of processing in
studies on individual differences. Our results suggest that the indexes of processing
speed are most meaningful when the accuracy rate is simultaneously taken into account
in the computations. The implications for studies on speed-ability associations are
discussed.
Methods. This study analyses ECT-performance for two types of tasks:
discrimination tasks, wherein stimuli were presented until a response occurred, and a
memory-type task, wherein participants had to judge the identity of the test stimuli
when a probe was briefly presented. The samples consisted of high school and college
students; Raven’s APM and Amthauer’s IST were used as intelligence tests. For the
discrimination tasks, the RT-IQ correlations were consistent with WPR: they were
higher for the slowest responses than they were for the fastest responses. For the
memory-type task, the U-shaped relations between the speed of responding and the
magnitude of the RT-IQ correlation were observed; contrary to the WPR, there seemed
to be some “optimal” speed of responding (corresponding to the most g-loaded RTs),
while both the fastest and the slowest reactions were not as strongly related to ability
level. Importantly, for both types of tasks the accuracy rate was consistently highest for
the responses given at the speed level that was optimal for the respective task. In other
words, most g-loaded RTs were those for which the highest accuracy rate was observed,
regardless of the shape of the RT-IQ and the RT-accuracy associations.
64
SYMPOSIUM 2
The Relationship between Confidence, Intelligence,
Academic Achievement and Decision-Making
Lazar Stankov
1
Ted Nettelbeck
2
1
University of Western Sydney
2
University of Adelaide
lazondi@rocketmail.com; ted.nettelbeck@adelaide.edu.au
The aim of this symposium is to present findings from several recent studies carried
out in Singapore (Nanyang University) and Australia (The University of Sydney,
University of Western Sydney, University of Adelaide). These studies have focused on
the relationship between one's confidence in the accuracy of the answer to a cognitive
test item and measures of intelligence, educational achievement and decision-making.
They also include a range of measures of thinking styles, self-beliefs and personality.
The Stankov paper is an overview of recent findings showing that non-cognitive
measures can be ordered with respect to their predictive validity. Many e.g., measures
of motivation, depression, and most personality traits - are poor predictors of
intelligence and achievement. Measures of self-beliefs - self-efficacy, self-concept and
anxiety - have moderate correlations with cognitive performance but tend to be domain-
specific. The best predictors of any kind of cognitive performance are measures of
confidence that can capture a major part of predictive validity of the self-beliefs.
The Kleitman paper explores the latent structure of intelligence and confidence,
along with a broad range of self-report measures of need for structure, outward
assuredness, rigid thinking, openness to experience and metacognitive beliefs. The
findings highlight the distinctions and relationships between intelligence, arrogance, and
rigid thinking.
The Jackson paper examines the generality of metacognitive and decision-making
measures derived from a variety of intelligence tests, as well as the predictive validity of
intelligence and metacognitive constructs to the quality and types of decisions made.
The particular decision making task is tailored along the lines of a test taking scenario.
The J. Lee paper report the results from a study that assessed confidence together
with scales measuring self-belief (i.e., self-efficacy, different kinds of self-concepts, and
anxiety) among the 15-year old students from Singapore. A distinct confidence factor
was identified in the domains of mathematics and English. The results show that
confidence is: a) a robust individual differences dimension and it captures much of the
predictive variance of other self-beliefs that are, in turn, among the best known
predictors of achievement.
The Welsh paper is focused on anchoring a well-known effect leading to bias in
estimation in various decision-making contexts. Anchoring was examined in a
simulated poker-like card game. While there were few significant demographic and
cognitive predictors of the overall performance, cognitive ability measures and decision
styles were related to decreases in anchoring susceptibility over the period od practice in
playing the card game.
65
S
Y
M
P
O
S
I
A
Noncognitive Predictors of Intelligence and Academic Achievement
Lazar Stankov
University of Western Sydney
lazondi@rocketmail.com
Novelty. Recent evidence about the relationship between cognitive tests and
psychological noncognitive variables is reviewed. Noncognitive measures can be
ordered with respect to their predictive validity. Many are poor predictors of
intelligence and achievement. Measures of rationality, selfassessment of intelligence,
Openness to Experience and self-concept correlate up to .35 with cognitive
performance. Some domain-specific self-beliefs (self-efficacy and anxiety) have
correlations with appropriate achievement tests that can reach .45. The best predictors of
any kind of cognitive performance are measures of confidence (frequently reported
correlations of .45 and above) that can capture a major part of predictive validity of the
three self-beliefs. The role of self-beliefs has attracted much interest in education but
their role in predicting performance on tests of fluid intelligence is likely to be low.
However, self-beliefs and confidence in particular may prove to be the most potent
noncognitive influences on the development of acculturated knowledge that is captured
by measures of Gc.
Importance. There is a plethora of noncognitive constructs studied by educational
and organizational psychologists that is claimed to influence cognitive performance.
This paper points out that many of these constructs have low predictive validity and
should be withdrawn from further consideration. The most important appear to be self-
belief constructs and confidence in particular.
Methods. This is a review paper that summarizes the outcomes from several studies
by ourselves and others. There is no presentation of new empirical data.
66
Intelligence and Confidence in Relationship to Competence,
Arrogance and Close-Mindedness
Sabina Kleitman
University of Sydney
sabinak@psych.usyd.edu.au
Novelty. Using hierarchical CFA, this research investigated the higher-order
relationships between Intelligence, self-assessments of ability (confidence and various
self-beliefs) and a range of personality and thinking style measures. The findings
demonstrated that Intelligence and self-assessments of ability defined a broad second-
order Competence factor. However, when not linked to Intelligence, these self-beliefs
also defined a broad second-order Arrogance factor along with first-order Outward
Assuredness and Openness dimensions. Competence and Arrogance factors were
unrelated. Results also revealed a second-order Rigid Thinking factor defined by a need
for structure and rigid thinking styles. It shared a strong negative correlation with
Competence and a moderate positive correlation with Arrogance. Overall, Intelligence
shared no relationship with Arrogant/Decisive traits, but low Intelligence was associated
with Rigid thinking styles. Furthermore, self-assessments of ability can define two
orthogonal higher-order traits: Competence and Arrogance.
Importance. The accurate assessment of our own abilities is important for a range of
outcomes, including decision-making, learning and academic achievement. For
example, well-calibrated confidence in one's various Intellectual abilities is needed to
select university courses that suit those abilities. Similarly, accurate self-beliefs play key
significance in decision-making. It is therefore important to understanding how
Intelligence relates to those assessments, as well as the broader network of constructs
that may influence them (thinking styles and personality).
Methods. Over 500 undergraduate psychology students completed a range of
Intelligence tests accompanied by confidence ratings, along with a broad range of self-
report measures of need for structure, outward assuredness, rigid thinking, openness to
experience and metacognitive beliefs. The sample was split. EFA and hierarchical CFA
were used. The Exploratory Factor Analysis was used to identify first order factors
using the first half of the sample. Confirmatory Factor Analysis was conducted to
replicate these factors and then investigate their higher order relationships.
67
S
Y
M
P
O
S
I
A
Intelligence and Confidence as Respective Predictors of Quality and Erroneous
Decision-Making
Simon A. Jackson*, Sabina Kleitman
University of Sydney
sabinak@psych.usyd.edu.au; sjac9712@uni.sydney.edu.au
Novelty. This research investigated the predictive relationships of Intelligence and
Confidence, and its traditional and novel derived indices, on decision-making
behaviour. The results demonstrated that Intelligence was the strongest incremental
predictor of quality decision-making (Optimal and Realistic), while confidence
variables were best predictive of the types of decision errors participants made in a
typical test-taking scenario (Incompetent or Hesitant). This implies that Intelligence sets
a threshold for the quality of an individual's decisions while confidence controls
decision behaviour. In turn, we consider Intelligence as a construct responsible for the
formation of accurate judgements upon which decision behaviour is based.
Importance. Decision-making is a complex process of making choices in order to
achieve our goals, and identifying the psychological constructs that contribute to this
process can be of great importance. Intelligence is often considered to be a suitable
proxy for decision-making competence. For example, Intelligence tests are routinely
administered for selection/profiling purposes in the workplace. However, beyond being
considered desirable, the role that intelligence plays in the decision-making process is
poorly understood.
Methods. A preliminary sample of 116 undergraduate psychology students
completed three intelligence tests, with all items being accompanied by confidence
ratings and a decision to submit the answer for marking. On each test, participants were
scored for accuracy (Intelligence), confidence, a range of calibration indices, and four
decision tendencies: Optimal, Realistic, Incompetent and Hesitant. The decision
tendency variables were regressed on Intelligence and the confidence variables in a
hierarchical fashion to assess their incremental validity.
68
Confidence: A Better Predictor of Academic Achievement than
Self-Efficacy, Self-Concept and Anxiety?
Lazar Stankov
1
, Jihyun Lee
2
, Wenshu Luo
3
, David Hogan
3
1
Universtiy of Western Sydney
2
The University of New South Wales
3
National Institute of Education, Singapore
lazondi@rocketmail.com
Novelty. In this paper we report the results from a study that assessed Confidence
together with scales measuring self-beliefs (i.e., self-efficacy, different kinds of self-
concepts, and anxiety) among the 15-year old students from Singapore.. A distinct
Confidence factor was identified in the domains of Mathematics (N = 1940) and English
(N = 1786). Our results show that Confidence is: a) a robust individual differences
dimension; b) that can be combined with accuracy information to obtain bias scores that
may be useful for group comparisons and for identification of misconceptions about
particular topics. Confidence as studied in our work to date has been c) the best
predictor of achievement in both Mathematics and English; d) is related to both
cognitive and self-beliefs measures; and e) it captures much of the predictive variance
of other self-beliefs that are, in turn, among the best known predictors of achievement.
Importance. It provides evidence that confidence is likely to be a general factor and it
shows that it is a better predictor of cognitive performance than other non-cognitive
traits.
Methods. It reports the data based on some 4000 15-years old students from
Singapore. Factor analysis and regression analyses are used to support the claims.
69
S
Y
M
P
O
S
I
A
Individual Differences in Anchoring: Traits and Experience
Matthew B. Welsh, Paul H. Delfabbro, Nicholas R. Burns, Steve H. Begg
University of Adelaide
matthew.welsh@adelaide.edu.au; paul.delfabbro@adelaide.edu.au;
nicholas.burns@adelaide.edu.au; steve.begg@adelaide.edu.au
Novelty. Anchoring is a well-known effect leading to bias in estimation in various
decision-making contexts. Previous research examining the role of individual
differences in anchoring susceptibility has found weak and unreliable results. In this
study anchoring was examined in a simulated poker-like card game, among people with
varying levels of academic achievement and using a wide variety of psychometric tests
for both cognitive ability and decision style/personality factors. Overall, anchoring
susceptibility was largely unrelated to demographic and cognitive measures but weakly
correlated to measures of preference and aptitude for rationality. Performance generally
improved during the course of the card game task, suggesting that participants became
less susceptible to anchoring with experience and these improvements were weakly-to-
moderately related to demographic, cognitive and decision style measures. That is,
while there were few significant predictors of overall performance, cognitive ability
measures and decision styles were related to decreases in anchoring susceptibility.
Importance. These results suggest that the conclusion from the judgement and
decision making (JDM) literature that intelligence is a poor predictor of bias
susceptibility may have been premature.
The observation that cognitive ability predicts not anchoring susceptibility but rather
changes in that susceptibility over time underlines the need for better measures of
individual differences in peoples\' responses to different biases.
Additionally, the paper highlights the fact that many JDM papers have relied solely
on correlates of general intelligence such as self-reported SAT scores, which may
underestimate the relationship between bias susceptibility and more specific cognitive
abilites.
Methods. Given the overreliance of previous JDM research on very general measures
of intelligence and the resultant conclusion that biases are better predcited by decision
style or personality measures, our methodology included a number of measures of
specific cognitive abilities including Gs and Gq from the Catell-Horn-Carroll model,
Working Memory and an Executive Functioning measure for comparison with various
decision style measures previously related to bias susceptibility.
The anchoring measure was also a methodological improvement over many previous
studies, which have generally looked at only group differences in anchoring. By
developing a poker-like game to test for anchoring bias, we were able to have
participants complete 140 anchoring tasks with known correct answers and thus track
their susceptibility to the anchoring bias, controlling for the effect of participant
knowledge.
70
SYMPOSIUM 3
Contemporary Applications of the Cattell-Horn-Carroll Cognitive Taxonomy
Mathew Pase
1
Con Stough
1
Timothy C Bates
2
1
Centre for Human Psychopharmacology, Swinburne University
2
Dept of Psychology University of Edinburgh
mpase@swin.edu.au; cstough@swin.edu.au; tim.bates@ed.ac.uk
Cognitive tests ranging from paper and pencil to computerised measures are
implemented across many different scientific disciplines. In areas such as clinical
nutrition, psychopharmacology and medicine, cognitive tests are often used to measure
the effect of a specific intervention on a set of cognitive processes. In these studies it is
commonplace for cognitive test results to be grouped into a set of broader cognitive
factors prior to statistical analysis. By limiting the number of cognitive outcomes this
method reduces the number of statistical comparisons, reducing the risk of a type-I
error. However, the grouping of cognitive test results is often executed in an arbitrary
manner without justification from either a validated cognitive taxonomy or factor
analysis. Both Carroll's model of human cognitive ability and the Cattell-Horn-Carroll
model of intelligence provide empirically based taxonomies of human cognition. These
models provide a cognitive "map" that can be used to guide the handling and analysis of
cognitive outcomes in any discipline that deals with cognitive test data. This talk will
provide an overview of cognitive taxonomies and discuss how their application can
improve the standardization and validity of cognitive outcomes in disciplines such as
psychopharmacology and nutrition.
In the first talk by Matthew Pase the general Carroll framework for cognition is
outlined. Within this framework, several data sets from recent studies (ranging from
cardiovascular, fish oil and vitamin interventions are mapped onto the framework. The
mapping of these relationships onto the Carroll model provides a unique ability to
understand differences across intervention studies that have utilized a variety of
different cognitive and intelligence assessments.
In the second presentation, Con Stough maps the differential cognitive effects of
different pharmaceutical cognitive enhancers including Modafinil and amphetamines on
to the Carroll framework. The general area of cognitive enhancers is at present difficult
to summarise because of the vast heterogeneity in different cognitive assessments used
across pharmaceutical trials. The effects of amphetamines for instance are particularly
difficult to understand because of the variety of methodologies used in previous studies.
the Carroll framework allow a clearer understanding of what cognitive components are
changed by amphetamine and other pharmaceutical cognitive enhancers.
In the third presentation Andrew Scholey uses the Carroll framework to map the
cognitive effects of a wide range of natural substances. The cognitive effects from a
range of both acute and chronic acting neutraceuticals are examined.
The three presentations taken together illustrate the contemporary use of a
comprehensive model of human intelligence.
71
S
Y
M
P
O
S
I
A
An Overview of The Cattell-Horn-Caroll Model of Cognition: Implications for
Contemporary Use
Matthew Pase*, Con Stough
Centre from Human Psychopharmacology, Swinburne University
mpase@swin.edu.au; cstough@swin.edu.au
Novelty. The Cattell-Horn-Carroll model of intelligence is a comprehensive model of
cognitive abilities. It has been grossly underused in contemporary research utilizing
cognitive assessments. As such there are many different cognitive tests and batteries all
measuring different components of cognition and these are routinely used across many
different areas of research with often results that are hard to interpret. The Cattell-Horn-
Carroll model provides a cognitive ‘map’ that can be used to guide the handling and
analysis of cognitive outcomes in any discipline that deals with cognitive test data. This
talk will provide an overview of cognitive taxonomies and discuss how their application
can improve the standardization and validity of cognitive outcomes in disciplines such
as psychopharmacology and nutrition.
Importance. There is growing dispute about the relationship between cognition and
many different other areas. For instance it is difficult to know whether fish oil
supplementation improves cognitive functioning because different studies utilize
different measures of cognition and therefore assess different aspects of cognition thus
making comparison between studies very difficult. By utilizing the Cattell-Horn-Carroll
model it is possible to map the different results on to the different components of
intelligence and to make better conclusions about cognitive effects. The talk will
provide a framework in which we can apply this model to a range of interesting
cognitive phenomena.
Methods. The methodology of this talk utilizes an analysis of the Cattell-Horn-
Carroll model. It then maps data from clinical trials studying relationships between
cardiovascular processes, pharmacological interventions and cognition. Thus it utilizes
data from several randomized controlled trials and assesses relationships between these
variables.
72
Understanding Smart Drug Effects Using the Cattell-Horn-Caroll Model of
Cognition
Con Stough, Matthew Pase, Andrew Scholey
Centre for Human Psychopharmacology, Swinburne University
cstough@swin.edu.au; mpase@swin.edu.au; ascholey@swin.edu.au
Novelty. Presently there is no consensus about whether pharmaceutical drugs such as
Modafinil and amphetamines improve cognitive performance and if they do which
cognitive processes they specifically enhance (if any). One of the reasons why there is
no consensus in this area of research is because different studies examining the effects
of cognitive enhancing pharmaceutical drugs have utilized different methodologies and
therefore different cognitive measures. By utilizing the Cattell-Horn-Carroll model of
cognition it is possible to map the results from different studies on to a coherent
cognitive framework and to ascertain at which level of the model (if any) that there may
be a cognitive enhancement. This information will be helpful in the design of future
studies developing cognitive enhancing drugs as well as contributing to a better
understanding of the cognitive effects of current drugs.
Importance. Amphetamines and other pharmaceutical drugs are widely abused in our
society. They are currently being taken by a wide cross-section of the community to
improve intelligence and cognitive performance. However little is known about their
effectiveness. The presentation will provide some conclusions using the Carroll
framework about the effectiveness and specificity of action in improving human
intelligence. This information is important and may influence drug usage and drug
development.
Methods. The presentation will utilize data from randomized controlled trials
(RCTs)using pharmaceutical drugs. Results will be mainly taken from well controlled
scientific studies and then mapped onto the Carroll cognitive framework.
73
S
Y
M
P
O
S
I
A
Understanding the Effects of Natural Medicines and Substances Using the Cattell-
Horn-Caroll Model of Cognition
Andrew Scholey, Con Stough, Matthew Pase
Centre for Human Psychopharmacology, Swinburne University
ascholey@swin.edu.au; cstough@swin.edu.au; mpase@swin.edu.au
Novelty. As well as a pharmaceutical approach to cognitive enhancement there is
growing evidence that nutraceuticals (natural medicines and nutritional supplements)
may also improve cognitive function. These effects are likely to involve multiple
biological processes. This paper will review evidence for this poly-pharmacological
approach to cognition enhancement in the context of the Carroll framework and
compare these effects from pharmaceutical approaches.
Importance. A huge number of people in our community take natural supplements
for cognitive enhancement usually without any empirical evidence. This talk will
present the growing evidence base and mechanisms for cognitive enhancement from
natural products and map these effects using the Carroll model.
Methods. We will utilize data from Randomized Controlled Trials (RCTs) from our
group and elsewhere. The talk will briefly consider methodological shortcomings of
certain trials.
74
SYMPOSIUM 4
Intelligence in Non Human Animals
Rosalind, Arden
1
Joshua Plotnik
2
1
King's College London
2
Division of Biological and Natural Resources Sciences, Mahidol University,
Kanchanaburi, Thailand
arden.rosalind@gmail.com; joshua.plotnik@gmail.com
The ubiquity of g in humans raises many questions to students of evolutionary
theory. Is g a uniquely human construct? Is it an ability or adaptation that emerged at a
discernible point in our phylogeny? What selection pressures influence g, and what
fitness optimum/optima are associated with it? These questions are not answered and
rarely asked. The reason for this is simple: intelligence research in humans is mostly the
child of Francis Galton and has focused on individual differences; animal cognition is
mostly the child of Charles Darwin and is mostly directed towards the species-typical.
Researchers from these two fields generally do not attend the same conferences; publish
in the same journals, nor realise how much each has to offer/learn from the other.
Although g has received a great deal of psychometric attention, it merits attention
from evolutionary-minded researchers. The many and varied phenotypic correlates of g
in humans suggest that it has a positive relationship with fitness. In humans g is
associated positively with fitness-related characters such as better health, higher life
expectancy, sperm quality, (and xxx in inversely with minor health abnormalities).
Higher g reduces the risk of alzheimer's disease, psychopathology and dementia. This
raises several causal possibilities. Advantage may beget advantage: auspicious family
backgrounds may promote the likelihood of bright, healthy children through non genetic
pathways, genetic pathways or a mixture of both. It would be useful to know to what
extent g, independently of socio-economic background, is a fundamental cause in the
matrix of phenotypic benefits. Animal models of g will be a useful aid to answering this
question and others pertaining to ageing.
75
S
Y
M
P
O
S
I
A
Fetch! G in Dogs
Rosalind Arden
1
, Mark James Adams
2
, Robert Plomin
3
1
King's College London
2
University of Sheffield
3
King's College London
arden.rosalind@gmail.com; r.plomin@kcl.ac.uk
Novelty. We assessed individual differences in cognitive abilities in one breed of
domestic dog (Border Collie) to find out whether there is a g factor of general
intelligence in dogs. We administered four configurations of a detour test and repeated
trials of two choice tasks (pointing and quantity). We used confirmatory factor analysis
to test alternative models explaining test performance. The best fitting model was a
hierarchical model with three lower-order factors for the detour time, choice time, and
choice score and a higher order factor accounting for 40% of the reliable variance in
performance. Dogs that completed the detour tasks quickly also tended to score higher
on the choice tasks and take less time to make a choice; this could be explained by a
general intelligence factor.
Importance. Animal models of g are currently restricted to humans and mice.
Learning about the phylogeny of g will inform us about its evolutionary history.
In addition to species-specific cognitive traits many animals show within-species
variation in cognitive ability. In humans most of the variation in ability in different
cognitive domains can be explained by single underlying g factor. Whether cognitive
abilities are domain general or domain specific has implications for the evolution and
function of cognitive traits.
Methods. We used confirmatory factor analysis to test different models against our
data.
76
Big Brains, Big Smarts? The Elephant Model for The Study of Convergent
Cognitive Evolution Across Species
Joshua M. Plotnik
Conservation Biology Program, Mahidol University, Thailand and Dept of Psychology,
University of Cambridge, U.K.
joshua.plotnik@gmail.com
Novelty. To help understand the evolution of intelligence in humans, psychologists
have, for nearly a century, looked to the study of non-human primates, particularly
chimpanzees, for answers. By studying specific markers of physical (tool-use, causal
reasoning, problem-solving) and social (cooperation, perspective taking, empathy)
intelligence in non-human primates, scientists have gained a better understanding of the
evolutionary trajectory of complex cognition. Within the past twenty years, however,
the field of animal cognition has expanded beyond primates to species that have also
demonstrated remarkable intellectual capacity. Animals such as dolphins, elephants and
the corvid bird family use tools, reason causally, engage in complex cooperation, and
take the perspective of others. These capacities most likely evolved through convergent
cognitive evolution, a process by which evolutionarily distant species evolve similar
intelligence as a result of similar environmental pressures.
Importance. The study of convergent cognitive evolution provides an interesting
avenue for understanding what drives the evolution of intelligence in animals, including
humans, and how such intelligence may be expressed across species.
Methods. In this talk, I will focus primarily on the study of cooperation in animals
from a proximate perspective, using elephants as an example. How do elephants "think"
about cooperation, and does any of their cooperative behavior mimic that of humans? I
will briefly review recent research on cooperative problem-solving, and behavioral
economics (including inequity and prosociality) in a number of animal species,
including my own work on elephants in Thailand. The continued study of animal
intelligence provides a window into the evolution of the mind, and increasing evidence
against the argument that complex cognition is uniquely human.
77
S
Y
M
P
O
S
I
A
What Can Tool-Making Crows Teach Us About Our Minds?
Alex H. Taylor
University of Auckland
alexander.taylor@auckland.ac.nz
Novelty. One of the defining features of Homo sapiens is the complexity of our tool
use and manufacture. However, it is unclear what effect these tool behaviours have had
on the evolution of our minds. I will discuss recent work on the cognition of another
prolific tool user, the New Caledonian crow. This species is capable of sophisticated
tool behaviours including context-dependent tool use, metatool use and hook tool
manufacture. These crows are, therefore, an ideal model species for understanding the
effect of tool use on cognitive evolution. I will present evidence that tool behaviours do
not lead to the evolution of the cognition involved in cooperation, but may lead to the
evolution of sophisticated planning and causal reasoning.
Importance. Two major hypotheses for the evolution of intelligence are the social
intelligence hypothesis and technical intelligence hypothesis. The social intelligence
hypothesis suggests that the selection pressures created by the need to manipulate social
interactions between conspecifics led to an increase in intelligence, while the technical
intelligence hypothesis suggests that the need to understand how to make tools
increased intelligence. Due to hominins being both highly social, and the manufactures
of sophisticated tools, it is impossible to uncover how social and technical selection
pressures sculpted the human mind. However, the New Caledonian crow makes tools
even more sophisticated than those seen in our closest relatives, the chimpanzee, and yet
is relatively unsocial. This makes it a perfect study species to for uncovering the effect
that tools have had on our minds over evolutionary time.
Methods. My talk will present work from a number of papers published in highly
prestigious journals. In each study discussed carefully controlled behavioural
experiments allowed us to make various claims about the cognition of New Caledonian
crows.
78
SYMPOSIUM 5
Sources of Individual Differences in Academic Self-Concept, Motivation, and
Achievement in Different Cultures and Ability Groups
Yulia Kovas
1,2
, Sergey Malykh
1,3
1
Laboratory for Cognitive Investigations and Learning, Tomsk State University, Russia
2
Psychology Department, Goldsmiths, University of London, UK
3
Psychological Institute of Russian Academy of Education, Moscow, Russia
y.kovas@gold.ac.uk; malykhsb@mail.ru
The current symposium brings together four studies investigating the etiology of
individual differences in different aspects of academic self-concept, motivation, and
achievement, as well as the structure of interrelationships among them. The four studies
provide new insights through the use of multivariate, longitudinal and/or cross-cultural
data. The first study (Kovas et al.) applied twin methodology to investigate the relative
contribution of genetic and environmental factors to variation in self-perceived ability
and intrinsic motivation in different twin samples from 6 different countries. The second
study (Morosanova et al.) investigated whether conscious self-regulation differentially
contributes to different aspects of mathematical performance and achievement, above
and beyond general intelligence and specific cognitive abilities. The third study
(Ovcharova et al.) examined whether the structure of the interrelationships between
intelligence, cognitive abilities, and academic achievement differs across ability and
selection range. Finally, the fourth study (Voronin et al.) used the multivariate
longitudinal cross-lag design to examine the relationships between academic self-
concept and achievement in different academic domains, controlling for intelligence.
Together these four studies provide new insights into the complex interrelationships
between intelligence, motivation, ability, and achievement, and identify new directions
for research in this important area.
79
S
Y
M
P
O
S
I
A
Why Do Children Differ in Motivation to Learn?
Insights From a Large Twin Study
Yulia Kovas
1
, Robert Plomin
2
, Gabrielle Garon-Carrier
3
, Michel Boivin
3
, Sergey B.
Malykh
4
, Frank Spinath
5
, Kou Murayama
6
, Stephen A. Petrill
7
1
Laboratory for Cognitive Investigations and Learning, Tomsk State University, Russia;
2
King’s College London, MRC Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre,
Institute of Psychiatry, UK;
3
Université Laval, Québec, Canada;
4
Laboratory for
Cognitive Investigations and Behavioural Genetics, Tomsk State University, Russia;
5
Department of Psychology, Saarland University, Saarbrücken, Germany;
6
Department
of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, USA;
7
The Ohio State University,
Columbus, Ohio, USA
y.kovas@gold.ac.uk; robert.plomin@kcl.ac.uk
Novelty. Academic motivation, encompassing a wide range of concepts related to
individuals’ educationally relevant beliefs, perceptions, interests, and attitudes plays a
central role in school achievement. Research suggested that the links between
motivation and achievement can largely be explained by overlapping genetic effects,
some of which are also involved in intelligence. We explored the etiology of two
constructs: enjoyment of learning (intrinsic motivation), and self-perceived ability
(academic self-concept) for different school subjects across school years, in several
thousand twins from 6 countries. The results showed a striking consistency across ages,
school subjects, and cultures. Overall, these constructs were found to be moderately
heritable. In terms of environmental influences, individual specific rather than family-
wide or classroom-wide environmental factors contributed to variance in enjoyment and
self-perceived ability. Being in the same class did not make children more similar in
their enjoyment and perceived ability than those attending different classes.
Importance. These results from 6 large representative samples provide convincing
evidence for the importance of non-shared environmental influences rather than family-
wide or class-wide environmental influences on enjoyment of learning and self-
perceived ability. Further research should focus on clarifying how the same educational
environment can create different experiences for different individuals. Moreover,
measures of motivation are not independent of intelligence and achievement as
motivation develops partly through feedback on performance. Future multivariate
genetic analyses should explore whether many of the genetic and non-shared
environmental effects on enjoyment and self-perceived ability are the same as those on
intelligence.
Methods. Self-reported evaluations of enjoyment and self-perceived ability, using
similar measures, were collected from several thousand participants from six different
twin studies: the UK twins at ages 9, 12 and 16; the Canadian twins at ages 10 and 12;
Japanese twins at ages 10, 11, 12, 13 and 16; the German twins at ages 9, 11 and 13; the
US twins at age 12; and the Russian twins at age 16. The relative contribution of genetic
and environmental factors to variation in enjoyment and self-perceived ability were
estimated from monozygotic and dizygotic intraclass correlations. For our analyses at
age 9 in the UK sample we used structural equation model fitting to assess whether
being in the same class for 8 or more months led to more similar level of enjoyment and
perceived ability for the two twins.
80
Individual Differences in Conscious Self-Regulation and Cognitive Characteristics
as Predictors of Academic Achievement
Varvara Morosanova
1
, Tatiana Fomina
1
, Irina Bondarenko
1
, Kseniya Sharafieva
2
, Elena
Ginku
2
, Olga Bogdanova
2
1
Psychological Institute, Russian Academy of Education, Moscow, Russia
2
Laboratory for Cognitive Investigations and Behavioural Genetics, Tomsk State
University, Russia
morosanova@mail.ru
Novelty. The importance of general intelligence and specific cognitive abilities for
learning success has been firmly demonstrated by the existing literature. In addition,
research also suggests an important role of non-cognitive factors in ensuring progress in
learning. In this study, we investigated whether self-regulation contributed to students’
academic success in mathematics, above and beyond intelligence and specific cognitive
abilities. The results showed that self-regulation was a significant independent predictor
of mathematical success, beyond the contribution of general intelligence and cognitive
abilities. Moreover, we found that different aspects of mathematical success were
explained by partly different cognitive and regulatory factors. We discuss the results
with the view that conscious self-regulation may serve as a metacognitive factor that is
involved in intellectual activity.
Importance. Understanding the complexity of the interrelationships among the
factors involved in individual differences in different aspects of academic achievement
can lead to progress in optimizing education.
Methods. The study used the data from 300 14-16 year old male and female students
in the 9th grades from several secondary schools. We assessed cognitive abilities
(spatial memory, number sense, nonverbal intelligence, and reaction time); general
intelligence; as well as 4 aspects of mathematical ability and achievement, including
standardized tests, school grades, and exam scores. To study the regulatory features, we
used "Style of Self-Regulation of Learning Activity Questionnaire", measuring the level
of self-regulation of students' learning activity. We used a grade for the year and
mathematics state exam score as measures mathematical achievement. Analyses of
variance examined any potential average and variance differences across different
classes and schools. Multiple regression analyses examined the predictors of different
aspects of mathematical success.
81
S
Y
M
P
O
S
I
A
Is the Structure of Interrelationships among Intelligence, Cognitive Abilities and
Achievement Different in Selected, Self-Selected, and Unselected School
Populations?
Olga Ovcharova
1,2
, Sergey Malykh
1,2
, Tatiana Tikhomirova
1,2
, Tatiana Kolienko
1
,
Yulia Kovas
1
1
Laboratory for Cognitive Investigations and Learning, Tomsk State University, Russia
2
Psychological Institute of the Russian Academy of Education
ovcharova.on@gmail.com; malykhsb@mail.ru; tikho@mail.ru; y.kovas@gold.ac.uk
Novelty. Previous research has identified a number of cognitive predictors of
mathematical achievement, including spatial ability, speed of information processing,
spatial memory, the so called ‘number sense’, as well as general intelligence. However,
it remains unclear whether the structure of the associations between such cognitive
processes and mathematical achievement is the same across the ability spectrum. For
example, does mathematical talent develop on the basis of different abilities from those
supporting mathematical performance in the normal range?
Our results support previous findings of the relationship between intelligence,
cognitive abilities, general intelligence, and achievement. However, the structure of the
interrelationships differed across the ability spectrum. In particular, factoring in
intelligence led to significant changes in this structure for the mathematically gifted
students, but not for students in the normal ability range.
Importance. Better understanding of how mathematical ability develops has
important implications for mathematical education overall, and for fostering
mathematical talent.
Methods. The study involved 855 students, divided into 4 groups: Mathematically
Gifted (371 students), Unselected (258 students), Self-selected (62 students) and Sports-
selected (164 students). Mathematically gifted students were recruited from specialist
Physics and Mathematics Schools, to which students are selected based on their
excellence in mathematics. Unselected students came from State secondary schools.
Students in the self-selected group came from a specialist Physics and Mathematics
School, which can be attended by anyone. However, it is expected that only highly
STEM motivated and achieving students select this school. Sports selected students
came from the vocational school which selects students based on good sports
achievement track record. A web-based battery of tests measured different aspects of
cognitive ability and intelligence, and several measures of mathematical performance
and achievement.
82
Testing Internal/External Frame of Reference Model across Academic Domains
and School Years
Ivan Voronin
1,2
, Maria Tosto
3
, Yulia Kovas
1,2,3,4
1
Laboratory for Cognitive Investigations and Behavioural Genetics, Tomsk State
University, Russia
2
Psychological Institute, Russian Academy of Education, Moscow, Russia
3
Goldsmiths, University of London, London, UK
4
King’s College London, MRC Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre,
Institute of Psychiatry, UK
ivan.a.voronin@gmail.com
Novelty. Academic achievement reflects a wide range of factors, from general
intelligence, to specific cognitive abilities, to personality and motivational factors, to
educational practices. Moreover, complex interrelationships have been suggested
among these different factors. Reciprocal internal/external frame of reference model
(Möller et al., 2011) is aimed to describe and explain the manifold relationships
between academic achievement and academic self-concept. In our study we show that
these relationships do not depend on general cognitive ability. We found that positive
within-domain effects and (weak) negative cross-domain effects of self-perceived
ability on achievement and vice versa persist over a long period of time.
Importance. Academic self-concept is a set of personal beliefs about one\'s academic
abilities and achievement. It is considered as one of the most important components of
general self-concept since it is strongly associated with motivation, achievement, and
life satisfaction. Within a specific domain the variability of academic self-concept is
explained mostly by academic achievement in the corresponding domain (however, this
effect is moderate: 0.21, Hansford & Hattie, 1982). Academic self-concept has small
positive reciprocal effect on the corresponding academic achievement (Marsh & Martin,
2011). Academic self-concepts in different domains are clearly differentiated. Across
domains academic achievement has negative effect on the academic self-concept. This
effect is explained by the processes of social comparison and internal dimensional
comparison of achievement in the development of the academic self-concept (Marsh,
1986). The reciprocal internal/external frame of the reference model was proposed to
integrate these findings (Möller et al., 2011).
Methods. We used longitudinal data from the Twin Early Development Study
(TEDS: Oliver & Plomin, 2007; Haworth, Davis, & Plomin, 2013) collected from the
same children when they were 9, 12 and 16 years of age. One child from each twin pair
was selected randomly to form the sample (7712 participants in total). Our measures of
achievement were literacy and mathematics teacher ratings at 9, web tests scores at 12,
and GCSE scores at 16. Language and mathematical self-concepts were assessed by
questionnaires measuring self-perceived ability. The data were adjusted by sex, age, and
g to exclude the potential bias. We fit a cross-lag cross-domain model to estimate within
and cross-domain relationships between self-concept and achievement. For the analysis
we used OpenMx package for R environment for statistical computations (Boker et al.,
2011, R Development Core Team, 2012).
83
S
Y
M
P
O
S
I
A
PRESIDENT'S
SYMPOSIUM
The Study of Mathematically Precocious Youth,
DNA Sequencing, Genetics, and the Phenotypic
Accomplishments of Profound Intellectual Talent
at Midlife
84
Forty Years Later:
What Happens to Mathematically Precocious Youth Identified at Age 12?
David Lubinski, Camilla P. Benbow, Harrison J. Kell
Vanderbilt University
david.lubinski@vanderbilt.edu; camilla.benbow@vanderbilt.edu;
harrison.j.kell@vanderbilt.edu
Preliminary findings from the first midlife follow-up of 1,650 participants from
the Study of Mathematically Precocious Youth’s (SMPY) two oldest cohorts will be
presented. During 1972-1974 and 1976-1978, participants were identified at age 12 as
in the top 1% in mathematical reasoning ability. They were surveyed over the web from
January 2012 to February 2013 on their accomplishments, family, and personal well-
being. Particular attention will be devoted to their occupational status, creative
accomplishments, and mate preferences, as well as how they invest their time currently
and plan to in the future. Sex differences in occupational preferences, personal views,
and life values will be reviewed. The presentation will conclude with a discussion of
participants’ satisfaction with their careers, personal relationships, and lives in general.
85
S
Y
M
P
O
S
I
A
Profound Intellectual Talent:
A Compelling Phenotype for Behavioral Genetics and Neuroscience Inquiry
Matthew C. Makel
1
, Harrison J. Kell
2
, David Lubinski
2
, Martha Putallaz
1
,
Camilla P. Benbow
2
Duke University
1
; Vanderbilt University
2
mmakel@tip.duke.edu; harrison.j.kell@vanderbilt.edu; david.lubinski@vanderbilt.edu;
putallaz@duke.edu; camilla.benbow@vanderbilt.edu
Two cohorts identified before age 13 as having profound mathematical or verbal
reasoning abilities (top 1 in 10,000) were tracked over three decades. One cohort (N =
320) was taken from the Study of Mathematically Precocious Youth (SMPY), the
second (N = 271) was identified through Duke University’s Talent Identification
Program (TIP). Replicable findings across cohorts in terminal graduate degrees,
occupations, and creative accomplishments underscore that above-level assessment
procedures (e.g., administering college entrance exams to intellectually talented 12-
year-olds) are an efficient way to identify truly extra-ordinary human potential. That
the distinctiveness of their accomplishments is anticipated by early assessments of
ability level and pattern could inform multidisciplinary inquiry. Studying these special
populations from a behavioral genetics and neuroscience point of view could uncover
underlying mechanisms that give rise to differential development among the profoundly
gifted, and individual differences in intellectual talent more generally.
86
The Genetics of High Cognitive Abilities
Robert Plomin
1
, Michael A. Simpson
1
, Martin Cederlöf
2
, Paul Lichtenstein
2
King’s College London, United Kingdom
1
; Karolinska Institute, Stockholm, Sweden
2
robert.plomin@kcl.ac.uk; michael.simpson@kcl.ac.uk; martin.cederlof@ki.se;
paul.lichtenstein@ki.se
Persons with extremely high intelligence offer unique advantages for gene-
hunters. Our Genetics of High Cognitive Abilities (GHCA) collaboration obtained
DNA from more than 2,000 of the highest-IQ individuals in Duke University’s Talent
Identification Program (TIP). These individuals are estimated to have IQ scores greater
than 150 and will be used as a case cohort in our High-IQ (HiQ) case-control project.
We have genotyped these individuals on the Illumina Exome array (HumanExome-
12v1-1_A) with 240,000 single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in protein-coding
regions of the genome. The whole genomes of these samples are also being sequenced
at 40x coverage in collaboration with BGI. Although we do not as yet have results that
we can report from these ongoing analyses, we are able to report the results from the
first large-scale sibling and twin study of high IQ. Using cognitive assessments
administered to 3 million 18-year-old males as part of compulsory military service in
Sweden 1950-1990, we identified 370,00 sibling pairs and 9,000 twin pairs. The top
3% of intelligence composite scores was familial, heritable, and caused by the same
genetic and environmental factors responsible for the normal distribution of
intelligence. These results imply that any genetic effects identified in our HiQ project
will not be ‘genes for genius’; rather, they will apply to IQ throughout the distribution,
including low IQ.
87
S
Y
M
P
O
S
I
A
Petaflop Computer Clusters Crunch Genomes of Giant Minds:
Collection, Sequencing, Analysis of the World’s Largest Extreme-IQ Genomic
Cohort
Laurent C. Asker Melchior Tellier
1,2
, Christopher Chang
1
, Bowen Zhao
1
,
James J. Lee
1,3
, Steven Hsu
1,4
,
BGI Cognitive Genomics Lab
1
; University of Copenhagen Biocenter
2
;
University of Minnesota Twin Cities
3
; University of Michigan
4
laurent@cog-genomics.org; chris@cog-genomics.org; james@cog-genomics.org;
steve@cog-genomics.org
Over 2,200 DNA samples have been gathered from subjects with very high IQ.
Many (n=1,688) were assessed and identified early on, in nation-wide talent
programmes. Others (n=548) have been enrolled and evaluated through protocols
specially implemented by the Cognitive Genomics Lab project. The procedures applied
for cohort phenotype assessment, the characteristics of the samples gathered so far, the
technology involved in their treatment and sequencing, and the bioinformatics methods
deployed in the genomic analysis, are reviewed. We describe the model of the genetics
of intelligence, variance, and mutation used in the study design, the reasoning
underlying this model, the implications on what types of variants we look for, and the
prospects for the collection of further, equivalent cohorts in East Asia and Scandinavia.
We state in advance that samples are currently undergoing sequencing - and
resequencing - in BGI’s recently acquired Complete Genomics technology, and that
analysis results therefore will not be revealed prematurely in the lecture or ensuing
Q&A.
88
The Fourth Law of Behavior Genetics
James J. Lee
1,2
, Christopher F. Chabris
3
University of Minnesota Twin Cities
1
; BGI Cognitive Genomics Lab
2
; Union College
3
leex2293@umn.edu; chabrisc@union.edu
Genome-wide association studies (GWAS), broadly understood, can utilize one
of many possible approaches, including: (1) a comparison of allele frequencies in
extreme cases and unselected controls; (2) the linear regression of the phenotype on
gene counts in samples of unselected individuals; (3) tests of linkage and association
between markers and causal variants within families. The advantages and disadvantages
of these approaches are complementary, and all should be pursued by scientists
interested in the genetic architecture of a particular phenotype. The advantage of the
case-control design—discussed by the previous speakers in relation to the ongoing
whole-genome sequencing of the Duke TIP cohort and high-IQ volunteers—is that in
the absence of confounding it most offers the most statistical power to detect the
variants of small effect that typically account for the genetic architectures of
quantitative traits.
The evidence for the pervasiveness of small-effect architectures is now strong
enough that we propose a "Fourth Law of Behavior Genetics": genetic variation in a
typical behavioral trait is attributable to thousands of variants scattered across the
human genome, each of which has a small effect. Some commentators have expressed
disappointment at these small effects, which are denigrated as academic curiosities with
little enduring scientific value. At first blush these claims, which cite the writings of
authorities such as Paul Meehl, seem to have some merit and thus appear to throw the
entire enterprise of gene hunting into disrepute. In this lecture I examine the
epistemological implications of the Fourth Law and conclude that gene-mapping studies
are in fact scientifically well justified.
89
S
Y
M
P
O
S
I
A
K
E
Y
N
O
T
E
S
KEYNOTES
Lifetime Achievement Award Address
Linda Gottfredson
President's Invited Address
Nicholas G. Martin
Keynote Address
Randall W. Engle
Holden Memorial Address On Science Writing
Elizabeth Finkel
90
LIFETIME ACHIEVEMENT
AWARD ADDRESS
Linda Gottfredson
Empirical Treasure, Lost and Found
University of Delaware
gottfred@udel.edu
In 1904 Charles Spearman demonstrated that human intelligence is a general
capacity, that is, it aids performance in diverse activities and content domains. His
discovery lay fallow until Arthur Jensen realized how profound it was. This single fact
about intelligence, its generality, allowed him to predict that g’s genetic roots would be
dispersed in the genome and its physiological manifestations distributed across the
brain. He showed at the behavioral level that individual and group differences in g
generate predictable variations in performance whenever tasks require us to mentally
manipulate information—learn, reason, think abstractly, “connect the dots,” “figure
things out,” and so on. Jensen gradually built a theoretically coherent body of empirical
evidence, a nomological network, integrating many types of evidence and attracting
other scholars to the enterprise.
Jensen reintroduced Spearman’s discovery about the time I entered graduate
school (1973). It was the worst of times for objective inquiry into a trait so enmeshed in
socioeconomic outcomes. Social scientists were disparaging the notion of intelligence
and tests that measure it. Leading figures in my discipline, sociology, asserted that
differences in ability and achievement are manufactured by elites to maintain their
privileges. Some said that most everyone could do almost any job, and one that doctors
could work their way up from orderly.
Their assertions violated common sense, ignored evidence in other disciplines,
and assumed causal forces never demonstrated empirically. I therefore began looking
91
K
E
Y
N
O
T
E
S
more deeply into mechanisms that might generate occupational inequality — not just
differences in the occupations individuals prefer and enter, but also how today’s finely
graded occupational prestige hierarchy evolved in the first place. That search soon led to
g and, more importantly, to asking exactly which aspects of a job magnify the
advantages of higher g. The answer, found in job analyses data, was anything that
increases the complexity of a job’s information processing demands: irrelevant, abstract,
additional, or insufficient information; ambiguity, novelty, and uncertainty; need to
continually update knowledge, draw inferences, spot lurking hazards, visualize the
unobservable, and much more. Complexity is also the active ingredient in IQ tests and
what modern life heaps upon us. We all have to contend with its proliferating cognitive
burdens, but they weigh more heavily on individuals lower on the IQ continuum or
experiencing normal age-related cognitive decline.
Following g’s footprints across the social landscape led me from one discipline
to another, each stopover replicating my experience as a Peace Corps volunteer in South
East Asia—being the proverbial Man from Mars. It always yielded unexpected
insights. One was the transformative power of individually inconsequential effects that
cumulate over time, tasks, or populations. Another was that we can improve the welfare
of less able citizens—literally, reduce their disproportionately high odds of premature
death—without having to raise their intelligence. That is what occupies me now—
bringing critical tasks in health self-care within the cognitive reach of patients currently
unable to perform them effectively (i.e., “non-compliant” patients).
92
PRESIDENT'S INVITED
ADDRESS
Nicholas G. Martin
Emerging Evidence on the Molecular Genetics of
Cognition
Queensland Institute of Medical Research
nickM@qimr.edu.au
After decades of stagnation, there have been recent dramatic advances in the
genetics of cognition. These will be reviewed and future directions adumbrated.
93
K
E
Y
N
O
T
E
S
KEYNOTE ADDRESS
Randall W. Engle
Working Memory Capacity
as a Mediating Variable
Georgia Institute of Technology
randall.engle@gatech.edu
Working memory capacity has the qualities of a trait, with identifiable brain
structures, neurotransmitters, and genetic factors associated with individual differences
in working memory capacity. This trait is associated with performance on a huge array
of real world tasks from complex learning to mind wandering to the prevention of
intrusive thought. WMC can also be thought of as a state variable in that many
conditions can lead to a reduction in trait WMC. Some examples are sleep deprivation,
drugs, and stereotype threat. I argue for a model in which WMC at the construct level is
a mediating variable for many different tasks in which control of attention is
important. I will also present an argument that working memory capacity and fluid
intelligence reflect two highly related but different abilities and data supporting that
argument.
94
HOLDEN MEMORIAL
ADDRESS ON SCIENCE
WRITING
Elizabeth Finkel
Navigating the Straits of the Genetics of
Intelligence Research
Science Journalist
Editor in Chief, Cosmos Magazine
ella.finkel@cosmosmedia.com
It’s a tough time to be a science writer. It’s not just the constant challenge of
mastering new fields in order to adjudicate between contested scientific views. There’s
also the distortion of scientific information by vested interests, and all of this
complicated by seemingly laxer standards in science publication. One need not look far
for examples: climate change, GM crops, vaccination…
But intelligence research raises the bar a whole order of magnitude—particularly
research on the genetics of intelligence. Many regard it as a Pandora’s Box. To some
extent we have been saved from the worst by the fact that genetics has, so far, been
unable to dig up the genes that underlie intelligence. That safeguard is about to be
shredded as the most powerful dig in history is underway at the Beijing Genomics
Institute. In this talk, I will share my trepidation as I attempt to journalistically navigate
the straits of the genetics of intelligence research.
95
INDEX
A
Adams, Mark James ......................... 75
Allebeck, Peter ................................. 56
Arden, Rosalind .................. 1, 7, 74, 75
Armstrong, Elijah Z. .................... 3, 12
B
Bacanu, Silviu A. ............................. 44
Badaró, Tatiana A. ....................... 3, 13
Baghurst, Peter ................................. 21
Bastin, Mark E. ................................ 17
Bates, Timothy C. ................................
...................... 1, 5, 6, 29, 38, 44, 54, 70
Bauer, Isabelle ................................. 36
Becker, David ......................... 3, 14, 53
Begg, Steve H. ................................. 69
Benbow, Camilla P. .............. 45, 84, 85
Benedek, Mathias............................. 51
Benson, Sarah ......................... 3, 15, 31
Billings, Clare E. W. .................... 3, 16
Bogdanova, Olga ............................. 80
Boivin, Michel ................................. 79
Bondarenko, Irina N. .................. 26, 80
Booth, Tom .................................. 2, 17
Borter, Natalie ..........................1, 4, 62
Bousman, Chad .......................... 28, 33
Brown, Robyn .................................. 40
Burns, Nicholas R. ..................... 21, 69
C
Camfield, David A. .......................... 36
Cederlöf, Martin ........................ 52, 86
Chang, Christopher .......................... 87
Chen, Xianging ................................ 44
Chetwynd, John G. ....................... 6, 55
Collins, George .............................. 1, 4
Corley, Janie .................................... 54
Cornoldi, Cesare .............................. 43
Cox, Katherine H. M. .............. 2, 18, 36
Coyle, Thomas R. ......... 1, 6, 14, 39, 53
D
Davies, Gail ............................... 44, 54
de Manzano, Örjan ....................1, 7, 49
Deary, Ian J. .............. 17, 29, 44, 47, 54
Delfabbro, Paul H. ........................... 69
Dodonov, Yury S. 1, 2, 4,19, 60, 61, 63
Dodonova, Yulia A. .............................
..................... 1, 2, 4, 19, 60, 61, 62, 63
Downey, Luke ................. 16, 22, 24, 30
Dramé, Cissé ................................ 2, 20
Dunst, Beate ..................................... 51
E
Earl, Rachel M. ............................ 2, 21
Engle, Randall .......................... 1, 7, 91
F
Fernandes, Heitor B. F. .................... 57
Figueredo, Aurelio José .................... 57
Findlay, Bruce .......................... 1, 9, 40
Finkel, Elizabeth ...................... 1, 8, 92
Fomina, Tatiana ............................... 80
Frey, Meredith C. ..................... 1, 4, 41
G
Garon-Carrier, Gabrielle .................. 79
Gignac, Gilles E. ...................... 1, 6, 42
Ginku, Elena .................................... 80
Giofrè, David ........................... 1, 4, 43
Goh, Antoinette ................................ 33
Gottfredson, Linda ................1, 4, 6, 89
H
Haley, Chris ..................................... 47
Hansell, Narelle K. ........................... 38
Hasselhorn, Marcus ...................... 2, 23
Hastie, Nick ..................................... 47
Hayward, Caroline ........................... 47
Hemmingsson, Tomas ...................... 56
Hill, David W. .......................... 1, 5, 44
Hsu, Steven ...................................... 87
Hogan, David ................................... 68
Hughes, Matthew ............................. 36
I
Iacono, William G. ........................... 27
J
Jackson, Simon A. .................... 1, 6, 67
96
Jauk, Emanuel .................................. 51
Johnson, Wendy ............................... 27
K
Kean, James D. ............................ 2, 22
Kell, Harrison J. ............ 1, 7, 45, 84, 85
Kleitman, Sabina ................. 1, 6, 66, 67
Kolienko, Tatiana............................. 81
Kovas, Yulia ........... 1, 8, 78, 79, 81, 82
Kudinov, Sergey I. ..................... 61, 63
L
Laurie-Rose, Cynthia ................... 4, 41
Ledovaya, Yanina .....................1, 9, 46
Lee, Jihyun ...............................1, 6, 68
Lee, James J. ....................... 1, 8, 87, 88
Lichtenstein, Paul....................... 52, 86
Liewald, David C. ...................... 44, 54
Loesche, Patrick ........................... 2, 23
Lomas, Justine E. .............. 3, 16, 24, 30
Lubinski, David ............ 1, 8, 45, 84, 85
Luciano, Michelle .....................1, 5, 47
Luo, Wenshu .................................... 68
M
Mackintosh, Nicholas..................... 1, 5
Macpherson, Helen N. ..............1, 7, 48
Madison, Guy ................... 3, 25, 49, 50
Maggini, Silvia ................................ 36
Malykh, Sergey B. ........ 1, 8, 78, 79, 81
Mammarella, Irene C. ...................... 43
Marioni, Riccardo E. ........................ 44
Martin, Nicholas G.............. 1, 4, 38, 90
McGue, Matt .................................... 27
McNeill, Geraldine .......................... 54
Mikhalchenko, Ksenia ................. 9, 46
Morosanova, Varvara I.... 1, 3, 8, 26, 80
Mosing, Miriam A ....................... 1, 50
Murayama, Kou ............................... 79
Murray, Aja L. ........................ 3, 17, 27
N
Neale, Chris ..................................... 36
Nettelbeck, Ted ................... 1, 6, 21, 64
Neubauer, Aljoscha C. ..............1, 4, 51
Nickeas, Brooke ........................... 3, 32
Nolidin, Karen J. ..................... 2, 28, 33
Null, Miranda C. .............................. 39
O
Ovcharova, Olga ...................... 1, 8, 81
P
Pase, Matthew ......... 1, 6, 70, 71, 72, 73
Pedersen, Nancy L. .......................... 50
Penning, Renske ............................... 34
Petrill, Stephen A. ............................ 79
Pipingas, Andrew ...... 18, 28, 33, 36, 48
Plomin, Robert .... 1, 5, 8, 52, 75, 79, 86
Plotnik, Joshua M. ............... 1, 7, 74, 76
Porteous, David J. ............................ 44
Prendergast, James ........................... 47
Purcell, Jason M. .............................. 39
R
Rammsayer, Thomas ........................ 62
Rindermann, Heiner ............ 1, 8, 14, 53
Ritchie, Stuart J. .............. 1, 2, 7, 29, 54
S
Savage, Karen M. ....................... 33, 36
Schokman, Chantelle M. .............. 3, 30
Scholey, Andrew B. .............................
1, 2, 6, 15, 18, 28, 31, 33, 36, 48, 72, 73
Sharafieva, Kseniya ......................... 80
Shcheblanova, Elena I. ..................... 26
Simmons, Nathan ............................. 30
Snyder, Anissa C. ............................. 39
Song, John H. .................. 1, 3, 6, 32, 55
Sörberg, Alma .......................... 1, 7, 56
Spinath, Frank .................................. 79
Stankov, Lazar .............. 1, 6, 64, 65, 68
Starr, John M. .......................17, 44, 54
Stockley, Creina ............................... 31
Stough, Con 1, 2, 16, 22, 24, 28, 30, 31,
33, 36, 70, 71, 72, 73
Strickland, Tracy .......................... 2, 35
T
Taylor, Alex H. ........................ 1, 7, 77
Thomson, Pippa ............................... 47
Tikhomirova, Tatiana ....................... 81
Tiplady, Brian .................................. 15
Tosto, Maria ..................................... 82
Troche, Stefan .................................. 62
97
U
Ullén, Fredrik........................ 25, 49, 50
V
Vargas, Robert ................................. 39
Verster, Joris C. ........................... 3, 34
Vitart, Veronique ............................. 47
Voronin, Ivan ............................1, 8, 82
W
Wardlaw, Joanna M. ........................ 17
Watkins, Marley W. ..................... 2, 35
Wellham, Dirk ................................. 30
Welsh, Matthew B.................... 1, 6, 69
Wheaton, Andrew ............................ 30
White, David J. ............................ 2, 36
Wiers, Reinout W. ............................ 34
Woodley, Michael A. 1, 3, 9, 12, 25, 57
Wright, Alan .................................... 47
Wright, Margaret J. .......................... 38
Z
Zhao, Bowen .................................... 87