ArticlePDF Available

The Influence of Work-Related Chronic Stress on the Regulation of Emotion and on Functional Connectivity in the Brain

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Despite mounting reports about the negative effects of chronic occupational stress on cognitive and emotional functions, the underlying mechanisms are unknown. Recent findings from structural MRI raise the question whether this condition could be associated with a functional uncoupling of the limbic networks and an impaired modulation of emotional stress. To address this, 40 subjects suffering from burnout symptoms attributed to chronic occupational stress and 70 controls were investigated using resting state functional MRI. The participants' ability to up- regulate, down-regulate, and maintain emotion was evaluated by recording their acoustic startle response while viewing neutral and negatively loaded images. Functional connectivity was calculated from amygdala seed regions, using explorative linear correlation analysis. Stressed subjects were less capable of down-regulating negative emotion, but had normal acoustic startle responses when asked to up-regulate or maintain emotion and when no regulation was required. The functional connectivity between the amygdala and the anterior cingulate cortex correlated with the ability to down-regulate negative emotion. This connectivity was significantly weaker in the burnout group, as was the amygdala connectivity with the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the motor cortex, whereas connectivity from the amygdala to the cerebellum and the insular cortex were stronger. In subjects suffering from chronic occupational stress, the functional couplings within the emotion- and stress-processing limbic networks seem to be altered, and associated with a reduced ability to down-regulate the response to emotional stress, providing a biological substrate for a further facilitation of the stress condition.
Content may be subject to copyright.
The Influence of Work-Related Chronic Stress on the
Regulation of Emotion and on Functional Connectivity in
the Brain
Armita Golkar
1
, Emilia Johansson
1
, Maki Kasahara
1
, Walter Osika
1,2
, Aleksander Perski
3
, Ivanka Savic
4
*
1Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institute, Stockholm, Sweden, 2Center for Social Sustainability, Department of Neurobiology, Care Sciences and Society,
Karolinska institute, Stockholm, Sweden, 3Stress Research Institute, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden, 4Department of Women’s and children’s health, and
Neurology Clinic, Karolinska Institute and Hospital, Stockholm, Sweden
Abstract
Despite mounting reports about the negative effects of chronic occupational stress on cognitive and emotional functions,
the underlying mechanisms are unknown. Recent findings from structural MRI raise the question whether this condition
could be associated with a functional uncoupling of the limbic networks and an impaired modulation of emotional stress.
To address this, 40 subjects suffering from burnout symptoms attributed to chronic occupational stress and 70 controls
were investigated using resting state functional MRI. The participants’ ability to up- regulate, down-regulate, and maintain
emotion was evaluated by recording their acoustic startle response while viewing neutral and negatively loaded images.
Functional connectivity was calculated from amygdala seed regions, using explorative linear correlation analysis. Stressed
subjects were less capable of down-regulating negative emotion, but had normal acoustic startle responses when asked to
up-regulate or maintain emotion and when no regulation was required. The functional connectivity between the amygdala
and the anterior cingulate cortex correlated with the ability to down-regulate negative emotion. This connectivity was
significantly weaker in the burnout group, as was the amygdala connectivity with the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the
motor cortex, whereas connectivity from the amygdala to the cerebellum and the insular cortex were stronger. In subjects
suffering from chronic occupational stress, the functional couplings within the emotion- and stress-processing limbic
networks seem to be altered, and associated with a reduced ability to down-regulate the response to emotional stress,
providing a biological substrate for a further facilitation of the stress condition.
Citation: Golkar A, Johansson E, Kasahara M, Osika W, Perski A, et al. (2014) The Influence of Work-Related Chronic Stress on the Regulation of Emotion and on
Functional Connectivity in the Brain. PLoS ONE 9(9): e104550. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0104550
Editor: Daniel Margulies, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Germany
Received March 6, 2014; Accepted July 11, 2014; Published September 3, 2014
Copyright: ß2014 Golkar et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits
unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: The authors confirm that all data underlying the findings are fully available without restriction. Relevant data are included within the
Supporting Information files.
Funding: The Swedish Council for Working Life and Social Research (FAS), the Swedish Research Council, and VINNOVA are acknowledged for their financial
support. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
* Email: ivanka.savic-berglund@ki.se
Introduction
Stress is common and hard to avoid. When stress becomes
chronic, it may have negative effects on cognitive functioning and
even lead to psychiatric conditions such as anxiety and depression
[1–2]. In recent years, the mounting reports about occupational
stress and the substantial costs for society that are associated with
it, mainly due to impaired mental health, have been gaining more
attention [3]. These worldwide reports signal the pressing need for
scientific investigations of the underlying pathophysiological
mechanisms.
Cognitive and emotional dysfunctions attributed to
occupational stress – ‘the burnout syndrome’
Occupational ‘burnout’ is characterized by stress-related
symptoms among otherwise healthy and high-performing persons
who report that they have not experienced any major negative life
events [4–8]. The described symptoms are attributed to occupa-
tional stress. They are stereotyped, and include memory and
concentration problems, sleeplessness, diffuse aches, profound
fatigue, irritability, anxiety, and a feeling of being emotionally
drained. The underlying mechanisms are largely unknown.
Measurements of cortisol levels after awakening in these subjects
have hitherto yielded inconclusive results [9], with reports of
normal [10–11], reduced [12–15], and elevated levels [16–18].
Recent data from brain imaging studies, although still limited,
suggest, however, that burnout from occupational stress is
associated with an affection of the limbic structures, the amygdala,
and the mesial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), in particular [19–23].
These initial findings call for further research, considering the
amygdala’s key role in evoking stress responses [24–25] and
considering that the regulation of stress responses during
emotional conflict is processed via functional connectivity between
the amygdala and the mPFC and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)
[26–30]. During the cognitive reappraisal of emotion, it has, for
example, been demonstrated that the activity of the amygdala is
down-regulated (measured as change in BOLD signal), whereas
the activity in portions of the lateral and medial prefrontal cortex is
upregulated [31–36]. Moreover, it was recently shown that the
ability to cognitively down- regulate negative emotion was severely
PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 1 September 2014 | Volume 9 | Issue 9 | e104550
jeopardized after stress exposure [37]. It is, thus, possible that
subjects reporting cognitive and emotional dysfunction due to
chronic occupational stress could have an impaired ability to
modulate emotional stress and emotionally stressful stimuli,
rendering them less apt to cope with psychosocial stress.
Furthermore, in these individuals, the amygdala connectivity with
the mPFC and ACC, and perhaps also to the hippocampus and
the insular cortex, could have undergone alterations. Such changes
could constitute a stress vulnerability factor, or be a consequence
of prolonged occupational stress. Both scenarios would be in line
with our previous observations of structural and neuroreceptor
changes along the limbic circuits in affected subjects [20–23].
These limbic changes, and their apparent overlap with the
networks that are reported to be involved in emotional regulation
led us to design a combined behavioral and MR study to test two
specific hypothesis: (1) That subjects suffering from occupational
stress have an impaired ability to modulate stressful emotions; and
(2) That these subjects show altered amygdala functional
connectivity. To test these, forty subjects with occupational
burnout along with seventy unstressed healthy controls were
investigated using a cognitive emotion regulation task as well as
resting state fMRI.
Emotion regulation and the acoustic startle reflex
In order to assess emotion regulation, we measured the
magnitude of the fear-potentiated startle reflex, which is a highly
conserved, fast defensive reflex that consists of a series of muscular
contractions and is mediated by a well-characterized neural
circuitry [38–39]. In humans, this reflex can be elicited by a
sudden and intense auditory stimulus (acoustic startle probe). The
amplitude of this reflex is measured through facial electromyog-
raphy (EMG), [40], and is potentiated when the individual is in an
aversive or fearful state [41]. The startle reflex is a reliable and
well-validated measure of emotion modulation [40] and has
previously been successfully used as an index of cognitive emotion
regulation in a healthy population [42–43].
In the present study the acoustic startle reflex was measured to
investigate possible group differences in emotional reactions to
negative visual stimuli and in the ability to regulate negative
emotion. We also measured functional resting state connectivity
from the amygdala, paying special attention to the connectivity to
other nodes of the limbic system and to the mPFC and ACC in
particular. In addition, a possible association between the ability to
modulate emotion and the resting state functional connectivity was
tested with linear regression analysis.
Methods
Participants
Forty right-handed [44] subjects (27 females; age 3866 years,
range 19–46 years; education 1763 years), who had been
diagnosed as having a ‘reaction to severe stress and an adjustment
disorder’ according to the International Classification of Diseases
(ICD-10, F43), were recruited from the Stress Research Institute at
Stockholm University. The selection was limited to subjects who
attributed their illness to prolonged work-related stress, after
working 60 to 70 hours per week continuously over several years
prior to the onset of symptoms. Inclusion criteria consisted of a
characteristic symptom course of sleeplessness, diffuse aches,
palpitations and fatigue, a subsequent onset of irritability, anxiety,
memory and concentration problems, feeling of depersonalization,
and reduced work capacity (confirmed by the employers) [8],[19].
All of the subjects attributed their symptoms to chronic stress and
had no other known etiology for their distress.
Subjects were also required to have had a symptom duration of
at least one year (their histories of stress-related burnout symptoms
ranged from 1.5 to 3.5 years), to have been on sick leave ($50%)
for stress-related symptoms for a minimum of 6 months before
entering the study, and to have an average stress-burnout score of
$3.0 on the Maslach Stress-Burnout Inventory – General Survey
(MBI-GS) [45]. This 7-point rating scale, ranging from 0 (never) to
6 (daily), consists of three subscales: exhaustion (five items),
cynicism (five items) and lack of professional efficacy (six items).
When rating perceived stress, subjects were asked to take into
consideration the last six months, and not only the actual time-
point. The average scores for Scandinavian populations are
around 2 for MBI-GS [4],[46].
Subjects were excluded if they had a previous history of
psychosis, personality disorder, major or bipolar depression,
alcohol or substance abuse, chronic fatigue, chronic pain,
fibromyalgia, or neurological or endocrine disease. Those who
had experienced prominent stress factors in their private life or a
major traumatic event at any time in their life, including sexual
abuse, were also excluded. No daily medication was allowed
during the two months prior to the study, except contraceptives.
According to a review of their pharmacological treatment
histories, none of them had taken drugs that are known to affect
brain structure (e.g., psychopharmaca). Subjects who were sleep
deprived the night before the scan/testing procedures were
rescheduled, in order to exclude the acute effects of sleep
deprivation.
Seventy healthy, right-handed, non-smoking volunteers (45
females; age 3366 years, range 24–45 years; education 1763
years) with no history of chronic stress or heredity for neuropsy-
chiatric disorders comprised the control group. The patient and
control groups had similar gender distributions, and both were
predominately female to accord with the female-dominated
epidemiology of the condition studied [4].
The two groups were matched for socioeconomic status assessed
on the basis of years of education, type of occupation, and
organizational position (employee, middle management, supervi-
sor). The study was approved by the Ethics Committee at the
Karolinska Institute and written informed consent was received
from each participant.
Before the interview, participants completed questionnaires in
order to evaluate their stress symptoms and assess their previous
life events [47]. In addition, the occurrence of major life events
among the subjects was assessed through a clinical psychiatric
interview based on the non-work-related items of the Holmes and
Rahe Scale [48]. The participants were asked to answer yes or no
to whether they had experienced any non-work- related stressful
life events (e.g., death of a relative or spouse, recent divorce, forced
family relocation). Subjects were excluded if they answered
positively to having experienced such an event in their lives.
Patients also received a medical screening (physical examination,
test of thyroid and liver function). A structured interview, the
Swedish version of the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric
Interview, MINI [49] was performed, along with a test for
depression using the Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating
Scale [50]. Although some subjects had high scores in the
MADRS they did not fulfill the MINI criteria for depression, and
were therefore not excluded.
Out of the participants who matched the inclusion criteria, 8
subjects with occupational burnout and 9 controls failed to display
a startle response to the probe. The results from the emotion
regulation experiment and the correlation analyses with fMRI are
therefore based on data from the remaining 32 subjects with
occupational burnout (20 females; mean age = 37.6 years,
Effect of Stress on Emotion Regulation and Brain Connectivity
PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 2 September 2014 | Volume 9 | Issue 9 | e104550
SD = 6.5) and 61 controls (33 females; mean age = 30.9 years,
SD = 6.7), whereas the analysis of resting state amygdala
connectivity is based on the entire study group.
Salivary cortisol
Salivary cortisol was sampled according to a previously
established protocol [51]. Saliva sampling was chosen because
the method is simple, non-invasive, and non- stressful; the samples
are shown to readily reflect the levels of the free fraction of cortisol
in plasma [52]. Participants were instructed carefully on how to
collect their own salivary samples. Samples were collected seven
times on an ordinary weekday using Salivette cotton rolls (Sarstedt,
Rommelsdorf, Germany), which participants were instructed to
place in the mouth for 2 minutes. The first sample was collected
immediately upon awakening in the morning, irrespective of time.
The second sample was collected 15 minutes later, before eating
or brushing teeth, and the third sample was collected 15 minutes
after that. The fourth sample was collected around noontime,
before lunch. The fifth sample was collected at about 3 p.m., the
sixth at 8 p.m., and the seventh at bedtime, after having rested in
bed for 15 minutes, before falling asleep. The samples were frozen
(218uC) until analyzed. The levels of salivary cortisol were
measured with radioimmunoassay using the Spectria (
125
I) coated
tubes radioimmunoassay kit (Orion Diagnostica, FIN-02101
Espoo, Finland). The within-assay coefficients of variation ranged
from 0.8 to 0.9, and those between assays never exceeded 10
percent. All samples from each group were analyzed simulta-
neously in duplicate.
Emotion regulation task
Before the experiment, the participants were given written as
well as verbal explanations of the task and instructions. Partici-
pants were informed that they would receive three different
instructions during the experiment and that these instructions
would be symbolically represented by three different arrows: (1) an
upward arrow indicated that the participant should make an effort
to reinforce the feelings that are elicited by the picture, so that he/
she experiences the image as more emotionally charged (‘‘up-
regulate’’); (2) a horizontal arrow indicated that the participant
should focus on the feeling the picture elicits, without trying to
manipulate the emotion (‘‘maintain’’); and (3) a downward arrow
indicated that the participant should make an effort to down-
regulate the feelings that the picture elicits, so that he/she
experiences the image as less emotionally charged, or as neutral as
possible (‘‘down-regulate’’). Participants were thoroughly informed
of the importance of following the instructions during the
experiment and not distracting themselves from their feelings by
thinking of something else or by looking away from the image or
closing their eyes. The subjects were however free to choose the
strategy to regulate their emotion.
The experiment began with a practice session during which the
participants were first subjected to the auditory startle probe six
times to allow for habituation to the sound. This was followed by
twelve practice trials that mirrored the experimental procedure.
After the practice session, the participants were asked to describe
the strategy they had used to regulate emotion. None of the
participants reported that they were confused about how to adopt
a reappraisal strategy for the neutral and negative trials before or
after completing the experimental task.
An example of an experimental trial is shown in Figure 1.
During each trial, the participant was presented with a picture for
5 s, which was then replaced by an instruction cue for 1 s. For
negative pictures, participants were instructed to suppress (down-
regulate), enhance (up-regulate) or maintain their emotional
response. Based on previous work [42] and to avoid confusion
due to ambiguous instructions (e.g., to suppress emotional
reactions to neutral pictures), neutral pictures were only coupled
with the instruction to maintain the emotional response. Imme-
diately following the instruction cue, the same picture was
presented again for 5 s, during which time the participants carried
out the regulation instruction. During each trial, startle probes
were presented 3 s after picture onset during the first (pre-
instruction) and the second (post-instruction) picture-viewing
phases. Lastly, the participants were given 4 s to rate on a scale
of 1–7 how well they had managed to carry out the instructions.
Between each trial, a fixation cross was presented for 4–6 s (mean
5 s). Each trial lasted for 20 s. There were 60 trials, and the entire
testing session lasted approximately twenty minutes, with a 15-
second pause after the first 30 trials. The presentation of pictures
was synchronized with the monitor’s refresh rate and presented
with the software Presentation (Neurobehavioural systems, www.
neurobs.com).
Material
We selected three sets of 15 negative pictures and one set of 15
neutral pictures from the International Affective Picture Set (IAPS)
[53]. Each of the three sets of negative pictures was assigned to one
of the three task instructions (maintain, down-regulate, up-
regulate), and this assignment was counterbalanced between
participants (male and female controls, and patients). Pictures
were selected to match valence and arousal scores of pictures used
in a similar report [54].
Electromyographic recordings: response definition and
data reduction
The eye-blink component of the startle response was measured
through electromyographic (EMG) recordings of the left orbicu-
laris oculi muscle using two miniature Ag/AgCl electrodes
prepared with electrolyte gel. A third ground electrode was placed
behind the left ear over the mastoid. Startle probes were 50-ms
bursts of approximately 95-db[A] white noise with a near
instantaneous rise time (,1 ms), delivered through sound-proof
headphones (Bose AE21, Bose Co. Framingham, Massachusetts).
The raw EMG signal was amplified and filtered through a 28–
50 Hz bandpass filter, rectified and integrated with a time
constant of 20 ms. Startle eye-blink magnitude (microvolts) was
measured as the amplitude from onset to peak, and trials with
excessive baseline activity or recording artifacts were rejected. To
assess initial, unaltered startle responses, pre-instruction (Startle 1)
startle scores for negative and neutral images were normalized
using z-standardization to ensure that all participants contributed
equally to the group means, as has been described previously [55–
56]. The z-score calculation is a within-individual normalization,
resulting in a distribution with an overall mean of 0 and a standard
deviation of 1 for each participant. To assess the regulation of the
startle response according to instruction, for each participant, we
calculated the change in startle response by subtracting the raw
startle 1 response from the raw startle 2 response separately for
each instruction (maintain neutral, maintain negative, down-
regulate negative, up-regulate negative) of the task. This way, we
defined emotion regulation ability as the magnitude during
emotion regulation controlling for baseline levels before the
regulation cues.
Statistical analyses
Initial startle reactions were assessed in a 262 repeated
measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Valence (Negative,
Effect of Stress on Emotion Regulation and Brain Connectivity
PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 3 September 2014 | Volume 9 | Issue 9 | e104550
Neutral) as a within-subjects variable and Group (Burnout,
Control) as a between subjects variable. To test the hypothesis
that burnout patients would differ from controls in their startle
responses during down-regulation of negative emotion, we ran a
262 repeated measures ANOVA with Instruction (Down-regulate,
Maintain) as the within-subject variable and Group (Burnout,
Control) as the between-subject variable. As a control, we similarly
assessed whether there were any group differences in startle
responses during up-regulation of negative emotions in a 2 (Up-
regulate, Maintain)62 (Burnout, Control) repeated-measures
ANOVA. Possible group difference in salivary cortisol levels was
tested with a repeated measure ANOVA (p,.05).
fMRI data acquisition
MR experiments were carried out on a separate day to avoid
contamination by possible effects of the emotional regulation tasks.
Functional MRI time series data were collected from all of the
participants at rest over 8 minutes in a 3 Tesla MR scanner
Discovery 750 (GE Healthcare), using a 32-channel head coil.
Resting fMRI blood oxygenation-level dependent (BOLD) data
were acquired in a standard gradient echo-planar-imaging (EPI)
acquisition, TR = 2.5 s, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 90u, resolu-
tion = 36363 mm, whole-head coverage. The participants were
asked to lie with their eyes closed, to think of nothing in particular,
and not to fall asleep. Structural brain images were acquired using
a T1-weighted 3D brain imaging volume imaging sequence with
whole-head coverage, TR = 7.91 s, TE = 3.06 s, flip angle = 12u,
and resolution 16161 mm. These structural images were used to
aid the registration of the functional data into a common standard
brain coordinate system (MNI152).
Seed region analysis
Seed region analysis is based on calculating cross-correlation
coefficients of the time series in a particular seed region-of-interest
(ROI) with all other voxels in the brain, which reveals the strength
of functional connectivity with respect to this seed region [57]. The
seed regions consisted of the right and left amygdala, and were
delineated with the guidance of the WFU-pick atlas, and after
adaptation to the gray matter template of our own population.
The MNI coordinates for the amygdala seeds where (sphere of
5 mm radius, co-ordinate 222, 27219, and 22 27219); the
seed regions covered the amygdala, with the exception of the most
medial 2 mm of the basomedial amygdala, which was excluded to
avoid the susceptibility artifact that was detected in some subjects.
Given the amygdala’s pivotal role in stress perception, we first
evaluated whether and how the functional connectivity from the
amygdala seeds differed between patients and controls. We then
used multiple regression analysis to investigate whether the degree
of perceived stress interacted with the pattern of connectivity from
the amygdala seed. Spatial preprocessing and statistical analysis of
functional images were performed using SPM8 (Welcome
Department of Cognitive Neurology). Functional images were
slice-timed and realigned, and then registered to structural T1
SPGR (spoiled gradient) images for each subject. Next, the
individual T1 SPGR images were segmented into gray matter,
white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid, and the gray matter image
was used to determine the parameters of normalization for the
standard Montreal Neurological Institute gray matter template.
The spatial parameters were then applied to the slice-timed and
realigned functional volumes that were finally resampled to
26262 mm voxels and smoothed with a 6-mm full-width at
half-maximum kernel. Each voxel’s time series was corrected for
noise using standard resting-state low-pass filtering with a cut-off
frequency of 0.1 Hz. In addition, voxel-wise multidimensional
regression analysis was employed in a standardized manner to
remove artifacts due to motion and changes in ventricle and white
matter signals. This was done by adding six movement regressors
obtained from rigid-body head motion correction (SPM 8
statistical package). Segmented WM (white matte) and CSF
(cerebro spinal fluid) were used as ROI for correction of signals
from non-gray matter tissue. To ensure that signals from WM and
CSF ROIs did not contain signals from gray matter, these ROIs
were superimposed on the individual EPIs and, when needed,
adapted to the respective subject, based on intensity differences
between white matter, gray matter, and ventricular regions.
Global signal correction was not employed, as it has been reported
that regression against the global signal may artificially introduce
Figure 1. Overview of one experimental trial with the maintain instruction. Participants were presented with a picture, which was replaced
by an instruction cue. For negative picture trials, this cue indicated whether the participants’ task was to maintain (horizontal arrow), down- regulate
(downward arrow) or enhance (upward arrow) their emotional response. Immediately following the instruction cue, participants implemented the
regulation instruction while being exposed to the same picture again. Lastly, participants rated how well they managed to implement the regulation
instruction on a scale of 1–7.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0104550.g001
Effect of Stress on Emotion Regulation and Brain Connectivity
PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 4 September 2014 | Volume 9 | Issue 9 | e104550
anticorrelations into fMRI data sets [58]. For each subject, the
average fMRI time course within the seed region was used as the
regressor of interest. Individual time series in each seed region
were extracted with MarsBar toolbox (http://marsbar.
sourceforge.net/). Each subject’s seed region time course was
then regressed voxel-wise against the subject’s fMRI time course
using the entire brain as search space. The t-values of the
corresponding regression coefficients at each voxel were used as
each subject’s connectivity map.
Statistical analysis
Group comparisons between stressed subjects and healthy
controls were carried out in SPM8 using one-way ANOVA, with
p,.001 voxel threshold, FWE corrected at cluster level, p,.05,
and controlling for age and gender, which were used as nuisance
covariates.
Results
Demographics
No group differences were found with regard to education or
gender distribution. The controls were younger than the stressed
subjects (Table 1). The subjects with occupational burnout scored
significantly higher on the MBI-GS scale (3.860.8 vs. 2.560.7
p = .001; F = 64.3) as well as on the MADRS (16.865.5 vs.
3.863.8; p = .001, F = 206.8) (Table 1). However, no group
difference was detected in cortisol levels (p = .56, F = .08).
Emotion regulation task
To verify that the data could be collapsed across female and
male controls, we first confirmed that there was no significant
difference between female and male controls in their initial startle
response to negative and neutral pictures (Valence6Group
interaction: F(1,30) = .13, p = .72). Furthermore, no sex differences
were found regarding the startle response to negative and neutral
pictures across instructions (Instruction6Group interaction: F
(2,60) = 2.23, p = .12). Because no significant differences were
detected between male and female controls, all of the comparisons
with the subjects with burnout were based on data from the entire
gender-mixed control group.
The burnout group and control group did not differ in their
initial startle response to negative and neutral pictures (Main effect
of Valence: F(1,91) = 39.97, p,.001; Valence6Group interaction:
F,1); both groups showed significantly higher startle responses to
negative images than to neutral images (burnout: t(31) = 3.87,
p = .001; control: t(60) = 5.65, p,.001). However, group differ-
ences emerged in the emotional regulation task (see Figure 2). A
262 repeated measures ANOVA with Instruction (Down-regulate,
Maintain) as the within-subject variable and Group (burnout,
control) as the between-subject variable revealed that the burnout
population showed overall higher startle responses across instruc-
tions (Main effect of Instruction: F(1,91) = 16.32, p,.001 and
Group, F(1,91) = 5.55, p = .02). As predicted, follow-up analysis
revealed that compared to the controls, the startle response among
the burnout patients was significantly higher during negative
down-regulation, [t(91) = 2.38, p = .02], but did not reach signif-
icance during the negative maintain instruction, [t(91) = 1.55,
p = .13]. No significant group difference in startle response was
detected during the up-regulate negative condition,
[F(1,91) = 6.04, p = .02], or during the maintain neutral condition
[t(91) = .06; p = .96]. Lastly, we compared the emotion regulation
success ratings of the burnout patients and the controls (see
Figure 3). The burnout patients differed from the controls
(Instruction6Group: F(2,160) = 4.63, p = .01): the burnout patients
had overall lower success ratings after viewing negative images, an
effect that was particularly pronounced with regard to being
instructed to down-regulate [t(80) = 4.70, p,.001] and maintain
negative emotion [t(80) = 3.12, p = .003] but that did not reach
significance for up-regulation of negative emotion [t(80) = 1.77;
p = .08]. These results parallel those observed with the startle
response. Critically, there were no differences between groups with
respect to rating after viewing neutral images [t(80) = .94, p = .35].
To test for possible effects of stress (MBI-GS) and depression
(MADRS) scores on the ability to down-regulate negative emotion,
two separate correlational analyses were run (Pearsson’s linear
correlation analysis). Bonferroni correction was not employed
Table 1. Demographics.
Stressed subjects (n = 40) Controls (n = 70) P and F values
Age (years) 38.266.8 33.265.8 p = 0.00 F = 17.4
Education (years) 16.963.4 16.862.9 p = 0.88 F = 0.22
MBI- GS (score) 3.860.8 2.560.7 p = 0.00 F = 64.3
Nexhaustion 4.461.1 1.260.8 p = 0.00 F = 269.0
Ncynicism 3.361.3 1.361.0 p = 0.00 F = 75.6
MADRS (score) 16.865.5 3.863.8 p = 0.00 F = 206.8
Cortisol sample 1 15.8612.6 5.763.9 p = 0.86 F = 0.33
Cortisol sample 2 24.2615.7 15.268.5 p = 0.78 F = 0.08
Cortisol sample 3 22.9614.9 20.3612.2 p = 0.17 F = 1.92
Cortisol sample 4 8.8610.0 23.1616.2 p = 0.96 F = 0.00
Cortisol sample 5 5.863.5 9.7613.9 p = 0.73 F = 0.12
Cortisol sample 6 3.664.3 3.764.9 p = 0.92 F = 0.01
Cortisol sample 7 4.2610.8 2.663.9 p = 0.33 F = 0.86
Age and education are expressed in years; MBI-GS is a questionnaire to score perceived work-related stress. Raw 3 indicates the mean total score, raw 4–5 the sub-scores
for the exhaustion and cynicism. MADRS = Montgomery Asberg Depression Scale. There was no overall group difference in cortisol levels (p = 0.56; F = 0.08, repeated
measure ANOVA). Time of the day for cortisol samples: Sample 1: 06.30–07.30; Sample 2: 15 minutes after sample 1; Sample 3: 30 minutes after sample 1; Sample 4:
12.00–13.00; Sample 5: 15.00–16.00; Sample 6: 20.00–21.00. Sample 7: 22.30–23.30.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0104550.t001
Effect of Stress on Emotion Regulation and Brain Connectivity
PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 5 September 2014 | Volume 9 | Issue 9 | e104550
because significant correlations were hypothesized for both score
types. Correlation analyses were carried out using both groups,
and also within each separate group. Both higher stress scores
(MGI-GS: r = 22, p = .02) and higher depression scores (MADRS:
r = . 37, p = .02) were related to a decreased ability to down-
regulate negative emotion, as indexed by higher differential startle
responses during the negative down-regulation condition. How-
ever, neither of these scores correlated with the differential startle
responses during down-regulation of negative emotion within the
burnout or control groups (both p’s..1).
Seed region fMRI connectivity
There was a significant difference between stressed subjects and
controls with respect to functional connectivity of the right and left
amygdala. Stressed subjects showed significantly weaker correla-
tions with clusters in the mPFC, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
(dlPFC), and the motor cortex, whereas their functional connec-
tivity during resting state with clusters in the cerebellum (vermis
cerebelli and the anterior cerebellum in particular) and the insular
cortex were stronger than in controls (Table 2, Figure 4a and 4b,
clusters calculated at p = .001, cluster level FWE correction at p,
.05). These differences were constitutes by differences in positive
connectivity and not anticorrelations (please see Figure 4c, which
shows within group connectivity patterns).
Post hoc analyses
Because the co-variation pattern from the amygdala differed
between the two groups of participants, we explored if this could
be related to degree of perceived stress or, perhaps, to the
MADRS scores. Each subject’s MGI-GS and MADRS scores
were, therefore, regressed on the individual connectivity maps
from the right and left amygdala seed (voxel threshold corre-
sponded to p = .001, FWE cluster correction at p,.05). In
addition, given the pivotal role of the mPFC and the ACC in
emotional regulation, we tested in the same manner whether the
ability to down-regulate negative emotion could be linked to the
connectivity between the amygdala and these two regions. Because
this analysis was hypothesis based, we employed small volume
correction (FWE corrected peak level at p,.05), using a search
area defined by a box covering both the ACC and the mPFC,
according to Montreal Neurological Institutes (MNI) atlas, the
MNI co-ordinates x = 210 to 10; y = 16 to 66; z= 4 to 24.
Only correlational data from the entire study group (thus,
without subdivision into burnout subjects and controls) are
presented. The subject groups were too limited to allow
explorative calculations of possible group differences in the
interaction between functional connectivity and emotional regu-
lation or to investigate the respective correlations in each group
separately.
Figure 2. Comparison between burnout patients and controls regarding startle reactions across task instructions. The burnout group
displayed overall higher responses when implementing instructions during negative pictures and this pattern was particularly pronounced during
down- regulation of negative emotion. Note that the y-axis represents post-instruction response – pre- instruction response; * = p,.05.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0104550.g002
Effect of Stress on Emotion Regulation and Brain Connectivity
PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 6 September 2014 | Volume 9 | Issue 9 | e104550
The MBI-GS scores (total mean scores) were found to be
positively correlated to the functional connectivity between the left
amygdala and the insular cortex, and the thalamus (covering a
portion of the hypothalamus); the more pronounced the stress
perception, the stronger the functional connection was (Table 3,
Figure 5). The corresponding analysis involving the right amyg-
Figure 3. Comparison between burnout patients and controls regarding rated regulation success across task instructions.
The burnout group rated themselves as generally less successful at implementing the task instruction after viewing negative pictures. * = p,.05,
#=p,.1.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0104550.g003
Table 2. Group differences in functional resting state connections from the amygdala.
Region Z level Size, cm
3
Coordinates
Controls
.
stressed subjects, R amygdala
RPFC+motor cortex 3.8 3.2 44 6 34
Controls
.
stressed subjects, L amygdala
L mPFC+L dlPFC 4.3 4.8 248 20 10
218 50 22
Stressed subjects
.
controls, R amygdala
R insular cortex 4.2 4.0 44 14 2
L insular cortex 3.9 3.2 228 20 6
Cerebellum 4.1 8.0 222 222 228
26 232 226
Stressed subjects
.
controls, L amygdala
Cerebellum 4.3 3.3 222 244 228
Clusters calculated using voxel threshold at p = 0.001, cluster level FWE correction at p,0.05.
R = right; L = left. The cere bellar clusters covered the anterior cerebellum and the vermis.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0104550.t002
Effect of Stress on Emotion Regulation and Brain Connectivity
PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 7 September 2014 | Volume 9 | Issue 9 | e104550
dala did not show any significant clusters. There were no negative
correlations with the stress scores, nor any interactions with
MADRS scores. The ability to down-regulate negative emotion
was associated with an increased functional connection between
the right amygdala seed and the ACC (the MNI co-ordinate was 6
26 14, z = 3.6, p = .027, FWE corrected, small volume correction).
Discussion
The present study represents part of a larger effort to
characterize the potential neurobiological underpinnings of
occupational burnout, an increasingly reported condition in
Western societies. The finding that subjects suffering from chronic
occupational stress had an impaired ability to modulate emotion,
and weaker functional connectivity between the amygdala and the
mPFC (two key structures for orchestrating defensive reactions to
environmental threats including stress) supports our previous
notion that we are dealing with a condition affecting the limbic
system.
Notably, stressed subjects showed higher startle responses
specifically during down-regulation of negative emotion, whereas
no group difference was detected in the initial response to negative
images or in the startle response during up-regulation of negative
emotion. Cortisol response during the image presentation was not
specifically measured, and it was not possible to objectively verify
that the images eliciting negative emotions were perceived as
stressful. Such an association has, however, been documented in
several previous studies which showed increased cortisol levels as
well as increased skin conductance responses during the presen-
tation of negatively valenced IAPS images [59–60]. It is, therefore,
reasonable to assume that the higher startle response among the
burnout group during negative down-regulation reflected an
impaired ability to modulate a stressful emotion, although we
cannot exclude that this effect was accompanied by differences in
attentional resources required to perform the tasks Emotion
regulation, as well as stress relies on an intact functional
connection between the amygdala, the mPFC, and the ACC
[24],[35]. The presently detected functional disconnection be-
tween the amygdala and the mPFC in the burnout group as well as
the detected interaction between the ability to down-modulate
negative emotion and the amygdala–ACC connection confirm this
notion. These findings are in line with our previous observation
based on PET measurements of resting state connectivity [22],
although the methodology and participant sample were different.
They are also in accordance with the reduction of gray matter
volume in the dlPFC and ACC and the cortical thinning of the
mPFC observed in subjects suffering from occupational stress
[21],[23]. Taken together, these data support the postulation that
Figure 4. Group difference in resting state functional connectivity from the right amygdala (R amy). Red clusters were calculated from
the burnout group - control-group contrast (A), and blue clusters from the reverse contrast (B), (p,0.05 FWE corrected). Clusters are superimposed
on the grey matter template (in the MNI space) from the entire study group. (C) Within group connectivity (positive) from the right amygdala. Blue
clusters show connectivity clusters in controls, red clusters in the burnout group.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0104550.g004
Table 3. Functional resting state connections from the amygdala in relation to stress perception.
Region Z level Size, cm
3
Coordinates
L amygdala connectivity and MBI-GS
L hypothalamus+thalamus 3.9 1.5 28220 6
26266
L insular cortex 4.0 1.0 232 24 4
R amygdala connectivity vs MBI-GS
R insular cortex 3.7 3.0 36 22 0
The interaction with MBI-GS is calculated at voxel threshold corresponding to p = 0.001, cluster level FWE correction at p,0.05. R = right; L = left. There were no negative
covariations from the amygdala seed regions.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0104550.t003
Effect of Stress on Emotion Regulation and Brain Connectivity
PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 8 September 2014 | Volume 9 | Issue 9 | e104550
stress-processing limbic networks are affected in subjects suffering
from occupational burnout.
The cluster showing impaired functional connectivity from the
amygdala in the burnout population comprised portions of the
motor cortex, which may be due to the fact that our groups were
gender mixed. In men there are strong connectivity between the
amygdala and motor cortex, while women have strong connec-
tivity between the amygdala and both the mPFC and ACC [61].
Thus, assuming that the amygdala–frontal lobe connectivity were
impaired in both sexes among the burnout population, it is not
surprising that a cluster encompassing both the mPFC and the
motor cortex was observed when comparing the entire control
group with the entire burnout group.
The MGI-GS scores interacted with functional connectivity
between the amygdala, the thalamus, and a minor portion of the
hypothalamus (Figure 5); thus, the more stressed the subject, the
stronger the connectivity was. This finding is congruent with the
well-established finding that stress leads to the activation of the
HPA via amygdala connectivity with the paraventricular hypo-
thalamic nuclei. It also fits well with the notion that the insular
cortex relays stress signals from the amygdala to the autonomous
nervous system. The observance of a stress-related enhancement of
the amygdala–insular cortex connection is in accordance with
previous findings for other stress conditions [62].
The enhanced connectivity observed between the amygdala and
the cerebellum in the stressed group was, on the other hand, not
directly expected. Nevertheless, there are several previous reports
suggesting that cerebellum may have a modulatory role in the
processing of psychosocial stress. The amygdala relays the
emotional salience of incoming signals to the rest of the brain,
and via cholinergic connections to the pontine nuclei and the
cerebellum, the neuronal traffic from the amygdala leads to
increased arousal [24–25]. The cerebellum is part of the
amygdala’s resting state connectivity network [63], and via
inhibitory (GABAergic) output from Purkinje, the excitation of
the amygdala is modulated by the cerebellum. Via this ability to
modulate the excitation of the amygdala the cerebellum is involved
in the processing of emotion, and potentially also the psychosocial
stress. Interestingly, the cerebellum has dense glucocorticoid
binding sites as well as reciprocal monosynaptic connections to
the hypothalamus, that provide a biological substrate for the
regulation of HPA, and the stress response. Animal experiments
show an enlargement of Purkinje cell spines in response to
corticotropin-releasing factor [64]. The involvement of the
cerebellum in stress is also indicated by reports about a reduction
in the cerebellar volume in patients with PTSD [65–67]. Thus,
although there have not been any comparable previous studies
showing stress- related increases in the functional connectivity
between the amygdala and cerebellum, one may speculate that, in
our burnout population, a compensatory enhancement of the
modulatory pathway from the cerebellum could have occurred
due to weakened amygdala–mPFC connectivity.
Methodological limitations and future directions
The amygdala seed covered the entire amygdala except for its
most medial portion, which was excluded because of the signal loss
in the fMRI images; thus, no differentiation could be made
between the basomedial and dorsolateral nuclei. In previous
studies of stress with resting state fMRI, primarily carried out in
patients with PTSD, it has been reported that amygdala–mPFC
(and also the amygdala-ACC) connectivity is decreased when
seeding from the entire amygdala [68–69]. However, when
separating the medial and lateral portions of the amygdala,
Brown et al. found elevated connectivity between the basomedial-
amygdala and the insular and dorsomedial PFC in PTDS patients,
whereas the connection between the lateral amygdala and the
inferior frontal cortex was stronger in controls [70]. It is, thus,
possible that the results with respect to amygdala connectivity
would be slightly different if the two major portions of the
amygdala were separated. In this initial study, our priority was,
however, to minimize the noise in the seed ROI. Also, there was
no primary hypothesis that the mesial and lateral portions of the
amygdala would be affected differently in the burnout population.
The relatively small size of the subject sample did not allow us to
test for possible gender-related differences in our results among the
burnout group. This important issue will be investigated in a
separate study. The groups were, however, matched with respect
to gender distribution.
One important question worth discussing is whether and to
what extent the present findings could reflect depression. For
several reasons we find this to be unlikely. First of all, none of the
stressed subjects reported dysthymia, and none were judged to be
depressed according to the psychiatrist in charge and the SCID-
MINI ratings. Although the depression scores were significantly
higher among the stressed group, for those subjects who had high
MADRS scores, the only items that contributed to these scores
were anxiety and poor sleep, which does not necessarily imply
depression. Higher MADRS scores were related to a decreased
ability to down-regulate negative emotion, but these scores
covaried highly with the stress scores, and did not moderate the
ability to down-regulate negative emotion within the burnout
group. While the MGI-GS scores interacted with the amygdala
connectivity with the insular cortex and the thalamus, no such
interaction was detected with the MADRS scores, not even when
employing small volume correction. Finally, the cortisol levels
were normal in our stressed subjects, whereas they have been
found to be high in a large portion of patients with genuine
depression [71]. We recently also found that women suffering from
chronic occupational stress had an elevated reaction to allopreg-
nanolone [20] which differed from the diminished allopregnano-
lone response that has been observed among depressed women
[72]. Emotional reactions to chronic stress and major depression
may, thus, represent separate constructs. They share, however,
certain symptoms perhaps due to the affection of similar limbic
networks. Indeed, the higher MADRS scores among the stressed
subjects who were not diagnosed as depressed could be an effect of
this comorbidity. Stress may lead to depression, and the impaired
ability to specifically down-regulate negative emotions that was
Figure 5. Yellow clusters denote significant interaction be-
tween the left amygdala connectivity map and the MBI-GS
score merging both groups. Pink clusters denote corresponding
clusters from the right amygdala. Clusters calculated at p,0.05 FWE
corrected, and superimposed on the grey matter template (in the MNI
space) from the entire study group.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0104550.g005
Effect of Stress on Emotion Regulation and Brain Connectivity
PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 9 September 2014 | Volume 9 | Issue 9 | e104550
demonstrated in the present study may by itself render stressed
individuals more prone to depressive thoughts and explain the
comorbidity between the two conditions. Because the study was
cross-sectional, it is difficult to know whether the detected changes
represent effects of stress or of a pre-existing condition that could
have rendered the brain more vulnerable to the development of
pathological stress responses and reduced the ability to modulate
emotion.
Conclusion
In subjects suffering from chronic emotional stress, there seems
to be a dysregulation of the emotion- and stress-processing
networks, which prevents the restoration of internal homeostasis in
response to negative emotional stress. An impairment of the ability
to down-regulate negative emotions in subjects suffering from
occupational stress may render them more vulnerable to
depressive symptoms. This finding needs to be further explored,
as it may potentially explain the link between stress and
psychological ill health.
Acknowledgments
Tom Perski, Edin Fazlic, and Alexander Berglund are acknowledged for
excellent data collection.
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: AG MK WO AP IS. Performed
the experiments: MK EJ. Analyzed the data: MK AG EJ. Wrote the paper:
IS AG.
References
1. Eisler RM, Polak PR (1971) Social stress and psychiatric disorder. J Nerv Ment
Dis 153: 227–233.
2. Weber K, Miller GA, Schupp HT, Borgelt J, Awiszus B, et al. (2009) Early life
stress and psychiatric disorder modulate cortical responses to affective stimuli.
Psychophysiology 46: 1234–1243.
3. Kalia M (2002) Assessing the economic impact of stress–the modern day hidden
epidemic. Metabolism: clinical and experimental 51: 49–53.
4. Ahola K, Honkonen T, Isometsa E, Kalimo R, Nykyri E, et al. (2006) Burnout
in the general population. Results from the Finnish Health 2000 Study. Soc
Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 41: 11–17.
5. Copertaro A, Barbaresi M, Tarsitani L, Battisti F, Baldassari M, et al. (2007)
[Fast stress evaluation in nurses]. Giornale italiano di medicina del lavoro ed
ergonomia 29: 350–352.
6. Copertaro A, Bracci M, Amati M, Mocchegiani E, Barbaresi M, et al. (2010)
[Biological risk and health care workers: analysis of the effects of work
chronobiology on the immune system]. Med Lav 101: 427–436.
7. Fernandez Torres, B, Roldan Perez LM, Guerra Velez A, Roldan Rodriguez T,
Gutierrez Guillen A, et al. (2006) [Prevalence of burnout among anesthesiol-
ogists at Hospital Universitario Virgen Macarena de Sevilla]. Rev Esp Anestesiol
Reanim 53: 359–362.
8. Rydmark I, Wahlberg K, Ghatan PH, Modell S, Nygren A, et al. (2006)
Neuroendocrine, cognitive and structural imaging characteristics of women on
longterm sickleave with job stress-induced depression. Biological psychiatry 60:
867–873.
9. Fries E, Dettenborn L, Kirschbaum C (2009) The cortisol awakening response
(CAR): facts and future directions. Int J Psychophysiol 72: 67–73.
10. Langelaan S, Bakker AB, Schaufeli WB, van Rhenen W, van Doornen LJ (2007)
Is burnout related to allostatic load? Int J Behav Med 14: 213–221.
11. Mommersteeg PM, Heijne CJ, Verbraak MJ, van Doorn en, LJ(2006a) Clinical
burnout is not reflected in the cortisol awakening response, the day-curve or the
response to a low-dose dexamethasone suppression test. Psychoneuroendocri-
nology 31: 216–225.
12. Chida Y, Steptoe A (2009) Cortisol awakening response and psychosocial
factors: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Biol Psychol 80: 265–278.
13. Moch SL, Panz VR, Joffe BI, Havlik I, Moch JD (2003) Longitudinal changes in
pituitary- adrenal hormones in South African women with burnout. Endocrine
21: 267–272.
14. Mommersteeg PM, Keijsers GP, Heijnen CJ, Verbraak MJ, van Doornen LJ
(2006b) Cortisol deviations in people with burnout before and after
psychotherapy: a pilot study. Health Psychol 25: 243–248.
15. Pruessner JC, Hellhammer DH, Kirschbaum C (1999) Burnout, perceived stress,
and cortisol responses to awakening. Psychosom Med 61: 197–204.
16. Grossi G, Perski A, Ekstedt M, Johansson T, Lindstrom M, et al. (2005) The
morning salivary cortisol response in burnout. J Psychosom Res 59: 103–111.
17. Grossi G, Perski A, Evengard B, Blomkvist V, Orth-Gomer K (2003).
Physiological correlates of burnout among women. J Psychosom Res 55: 309–
316.
18. Melamed S, Ugarten U, Shirom A, Kahana L, Lerman Y, Froom P (1999)
Chronic burnout, somatic arousal and elevated salivary cortisol levels.
J Psychosom Res 46: 591–598.
19. Sandstrom A, Rhodin IN, Lundberg M, Olsson T, Nyberg L (2005) Impaired
cognitive performance in patients with chronic burnout syndrome. Biological
psychology 69: 271–279.
20. Backstrom T, Bixo M, Nyberg S, Savic I (2013) Increased neurosteroid
sensitivity–an explanation to symptoms associated with chronic work related
stress in women? Psychoneuroendocrinology 38: 1078–1089.
21. Blix E, Perski A, Berglund H, Savic I (2013) Long-term occupational stress is
associated with regional reductions in brain tissue volumes. PLoS ONE 8:
e64065.
22. Jovanovic H, Perski A, Berglund H, Savic I (2011) Chronic stress is linked to 5-
HT(1A) receptor changes and functional disintegration of the limbic networks.
Neuroimage 55: 1178–1188.
23. Savic I. Structural changes of the brain in relation to occupational stress. Cereb
Cortex 2013 Dec 18. [Epub ahead of print]
24. LeDoux JE (2000) Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual review of neuroscience
23: 155–184.
25. Phillips ML, Drevets WC, Rauch SL, Lane R (2003) Neurobiology of emotion
perception I: The neural basis of normal emotion perception. Biological
Psychiatry 54: 504–514.
26. Egner T, Etkin A, Gale S, Hirsch J (2008) Dissociable neural systems resolve
conflict from emotional versus nonemotional distracters. Cereb Cortex 18:
1475–1484.
27. Etkin A, Egner T, Peraza DM, Kandel ER, Hirsch J (2006) Resolving emotional
conflict: A role for the rostral anterior cingulate cortex in modulating activity in
the amygdala. Neuron 51: 871–882.
28. Gianaros PJ, Sheu LK, Matthews KA, Jennings JR, Manuck SB, et al. (2008)
Individual differences in stressor-evoked blood pressure reactivity vary with
activation, volume, and functional connectivity of the amygdala. J Neurosc 28:
990–999.
29. Pezawas L, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Drabant EM, Verchinski BA, Munoz KE, et
al. (2005) 5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala
interactions: a genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression. Nature
neuroscience 8: 828–834.
30. Wager TD, Davidson ML, Hughes BL, Lindquist MA, Ochsner KN (2008)
Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation.
Neuron 59: 1037–1050.
31. Goldin PR, McRae K, Ramel W, Gross JJ (2008) The neural bases of emotion
regulation: Reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion. Biological
Psychiatry 63: 577–586.
32. Golkar A, Lonsdorf TB, Olsson A, Lindstrom KM, Berrebi J, et al. (2012)
Distinct contributions of the dorsolateral prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex
during emotion regulation. PLoS ONE 7: e48107.
33. Koenigsberg HW, Fan J, Ochsner KN, Liu X, Guise K, et al. (2010) Neural
correlates of using distancing to regulate emotional responses to social situations.
Neuropsychologia 48: 1813–1822
34. Levesque J, Eugene F, Joanette Y, Paquette V, Mensour B, et al. (2003) Neural
circuitry underlying voluntary suppression of sadness. Biological Psychiatry 53:
502–510.
35. Ochsner KN, Bunge SA, Gross JJ, Gabrieli JDE (2002) Rethinking feelings: An
fMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience 14: 1215–1229.
36. Ochsner KN, Ray RD, Cooper JC, Robertson ER, Chopra S, et al. (2004) For
better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-
regulation of negative emotion. Neuroimage 23: 483–499.
37. Raio CM, Orederu TA, Palazzolo L, Shurick AA, Phelps EA (2013) Cognitive
emotion regulation fails the stress test. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110: 15139–
15144.
38. Davis M, Parisi T, Gendelman DS, Tischler M, Kehne JH (1982) Habituation
and sensitization of startle reflexes elicited electrically from the brain-stem.
Science 218: 688–690.
39. Yeomans JS, Frankland PW (1995) The acoustic startle reflex: Neurons and
connections. Brain research reviews 21: 301–314.
40. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN (1990) Emotion, attention, and the startle
reflex. Psychological Review 97: 377–395.
41. Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN, Lang PJ (1990) Startle reflex modification - emotion
or attention. Psychophysiology 27: 513–522.
42. Dillon DG, LaBar KS (2005). Startle modulation during conscious emotion
regulation is arousal- dependent. Behav Neurosci 119: 1118–1124.
Effect of Stress on Emotion Regulation and Brain Connectivity
PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 10 September 2014 | Volume 9 | Issue 9 | e104550
43. Jackson DC, Malmstadt JR, Larson CL, Davidson RJ (2000) Suppression and
enhancement of emotional responses to unpleasant pictures. Psychophysiology
37: 515–522.
44. Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh
inventory. Neuropsychologia 9: 97–113.
45. Schaufeli WB, Van Dierendonck D (1995) A cautionary note about the cross-
national and clinical validity of cut-off points for the Maslach Burnout Inventory.
Psychological reports 76: 1083–1090.
46. Stenlund T, Ahlgren C, Lindahl B, Burel l G, Knutsson A (2007) Patients with
burnout in relation to gender and a general population. Scandinavian journal of
public health 35: 516–523.
47. Deykin EY, Keane TM, Kaloupek D, Fincke G, Rothendler J, et al. (2 001)
Posttraumatic stress disorder and the use of health services. Psychosom Med 63:
835–841.
48. Holmes TH, Rahe RH (1967) The Social Readjustment Rating S cale.
J Psychosom Res 11: 213–218.
49. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J (1998) The Mini-
International Neuropsychiatri c Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and
validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and
ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry 59 Suppl 20: 22–33;quiz 34–57.
50. Montgomery S, Asberg M, Traskman L, Montgomery D (1978) Cross cultural
studies on the use of CPRS in English and Swedish depressed patients. Acta
Psychiatrica Scandinavica Suppl 271: 33–37.
51. Karlamangla AS, Friedman EM, Seeman TE, Stawksi RS, Almeida DM (2013)
Daytime trajectories of cortisol: Demographic and socioeconomic differences-
Findings from the National Study of Daily Experiences. Psychoneuroendocri-
nology 38: 2585–2597.
52. Galbois A, Rudler M, Massard J, Fulla Y, Bennani A, et al. (2010) Assessment of
adrenal function in cirrhotic patients: salivary cortisol should be preferred.
J Hepatol 52: 839–845.
53. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN (1997) International Affective Picture
System (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. Gainesville, FL: NIMH
Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, University of Florida.
54. Jackson DC, Mueller CJ, Dolski I, Dalton KM, Nitschke JB, et al. (2003) Now
you feel it, now you don’t: Frontal brain electrical asymmetry and individual
differences in emotion regulation. Psychological science 14: 612–617.
55. Golkar A, Bellander M, Ohman A (2013) Temporal properties of fear
extinction–does time matter? Behav Neurosci 127: 59–69.
56. Weike AI, Schupp HT, Hamm AO (2007) Fear acquisition requires awareness
in trace but not delay conditioning. Psychophysiology 44: 170–180.
57. Biswal B, Yetkin FZ, Haughton VM, Hyde JS (1995) Functional connectivity in
the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn Reson
Med 34: 537–541.
58. Weissenbacher A, Kasess C, Gerstl F, Lanzenberger R, Moser E, et al. (2009)
Correlations and anticorrelations in resting-state functional connectivity MRI: a
quantitative comparison of preprocessing strategies. Neuroimage 47: 1408–
1416.
59. Bradley MM, Codispoti M, Cuthbert BN, Lang PJ (2001) Emotion and
Motivation I: Defensive and Appe titive Reactions in Picture Processi ng.
Emotion, 1: 276–298.
60. Gerra G, Somaini L, Manfredini M, Raggi MA, Saracino MA, et al. (2014)
Dysregulated responses to emotions among abstinent heroin users: Correlation
with childhood neglect and addiction severity, Progress in Neuro-Psychophar-
macology and Biological Psychiatry, 48: 220–228.
61. Savic I, Lindstrom P (2008) PET and MRI show differences in cerebral
asymmetry and functional connectivity between homo- and heterosexual
subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105: 9403–9408.
62. Hong JY, Kilpatrick LA, Labus J, Gupta A, Jiang Z, et al. (2013) Patients with
chronic visceral pain show sex-related alterations in intrinsic oscillations of the
resting brain. J Neurosci 33: 11994–12002.
63. Sang L, Qin W, Liu Y, Han W, Zhang Y, et al. (2012) Resting-state functional
connectivity of the vermal and hemispheric subregions of the cerebellum with
both the cerebral cortical networks and sub.cortical structures. Neuroimage 61:
1213–1225.
64. Gounko NV, Swinny JD, Kalicharan D, Jafari S, Corteen N, et al. (2013)
Corticotropin-releasing factor and urocortin regulate spine and synapse
formation: structural basis for stress-induced neuronal remodeling and
pathology. Molecular psychiatry 18: 86–92.
65. Anderson CM, Teicher MH, Polcari A, Renshaw PF (2002) Abnormal T2
relaxation time in the cerebellar vermis of adults sexually abused in childhood:
potential role of the vermis in stress-enhanced risk for drug abuse.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 27: 231–244.
66. De Bellis MD, Kuchibhatla M (2006) Cerebellar volumes in pediatric
maltreatment-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 60: 697–703.
67. Baldacara L, Jackowski AP, Schoedl A, Pupo M, Andreoli SB, et al. (2011)
Reduced cerebellar left hemisphere and vermal volume in adults with PTSD
from a community sample. J Psychiatr Res 45: 1627–1633.
68. Rabinak CA, Angstadt M, Welsh RC, Kenndy AE, Lyubkin M, et al. (2011)
Altered amygdala resting-state functional connectivity in post-traumatic stress
disorder. Frontiers in psychiatry 2: 62.
69. Sripada RK, King AP, Garfinkel SN, Wang X, Sripada C.S, et al. (2012).
Altered resting-state amygdala functional connectivity in men with posttraumatic
stress disorder. J Psychiatry Neurosci 37: 241–249.
70. Brown VM, Labar KS, Haswell CC, Gold AL, Mid-Atlantic MIRECC
Workgroup, et al. (2014) Alt ered resting-state functional connectivity of
basolateral and centromedial amygdala complexes in posttraumatic stress
disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 39: 351–359.
71. Kasckow JW, Baker D, Geracioti TD Jr (2001) Corticotropin-releasing hormone
in depression and post-traumatic stress disorder. Peptides 22: 845–851.
72. Girdler SS, Klatzkin R (2007) Neurosteroids in the context of stress: implications
for depressive disorders. Pharmacology & therapeutics 116: 125–139.
Effect of Stress on Emotion Regulation and Brain Connectivity
PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 11 September 2014 | Volume 9 | Issue 9 | e104550
... Parents raising a child with a disability must deal with the heavy burden on their daily lives caused by the numerous demands related to caring for their child's needs (Caicedo, 2014). These demands include managing the child's challenging behaviors (Thwala et al., 2015), financial demands, and employment restrictions (Goudie et al., 2014), as well as finding the time for therapies and diagnoses, all these may lead to burnout (Caicedo, 2014). In addition, such parents are at risk of experiencing stigma, social isolation (Cantwell et al., 2015), worry, and uncertainty about their child's future (Coughlin & Sethares, 2017). ...
... These stressors can challenge these parents' ability to effectively manage their child's difficult behaviors (Deater-Deckard, 2008) and lead to chronic stress (Hayes & Watson, 2013). Studies have shown that compared to parenting a child without a disability, parents of children with a disability experience higher levels of anxiety and depression (Scherer et al., 2019), more strained family relationships, and a lower sense of parental capacity (Emerson, 2014;Goudie et al., 2014;Hayes & Watson, 2013). These parents also express higher levels of fatigue and burnout (Giallo et al., 2016;Seymour et al., 2013) and are vulnerable to chronic feelings of sorrow (Coughlin & Sethares, 2017). ...
... Acceptance may be non-intuitive to implement as it requires relinquishing the illusion of control, which can be especially difficult for individuals who experience high levels of anxiety and depression (Singer & Dobson, 2009). Therefore, populations experiencing high levels of stress, fatigue, and emotional overload, such as parents raising a child with a disability (Hayes & Watson, 2013), may find it difficult to implement acceptance and may turn instead to other, less adaptive, strategies (Golkar et al., 2014). ...
Article
Full-text available
To deal with the stress involved in parenting a child with a disability, parents might benefit from using adaptive emotion regulation (ER) strategies. ER may serve as a protective factor against psychological distress among parents of children with disabilities, as well as promote the healthy development of their child. In this paper, we delineate the factors that contribute to the use of adaptive and maladaptive ER strategies among parents of children with disabilities. To this end, a systematic review using the PRISMA guideline was conducted, targeting studies that examine ER in this population. Thirty-three eligible articles were identified and discussed. We first review which ER strategies have been examined among parents of children with disabilities and the implication of using these strategies on parents’ well-being, parenting style, and child ER. Findings suggest that compared to their counterparts, parents of children with disabilities show emotion dysregulation patterns, which are associated with negative outcomes for both the parents and the child. Conversely, these parents utilize adaptive ER strategies to a lesser degree. Based on this literature review, we propose the Strained Parenting and Emotion Regulation (SPER) model, which posits that ER among parents of children with disabilities may be compromised due to three factors: intensified negative emotions, depleted cognitive resources, and challenging parent-child interactions. The SPER model emphasizes the presence of a vicious cycle, in which these three factors are presumably promoted by the use of maladaptive ER strategies, and their presence in tern limits the use of adaptive ER strategies, leading to the establishment of emotion dysregulation patterns in parents of children with disabilities.
... A longitudinal study by Armon et al. (2008) found a bidirectional association, whereas Jansson-Frojmark and Lindblom (2010) found that insomnia predicted new onsets of burnout, but not vice versa. Underlying mechanisms for burnout is largely unknown, but as for insomnia, research on regulation of emotions and their functional connection in the brain has shown promising results (Arnsten et al., 2015;Golkar et al., 2014;Savic, 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
Insomnia and burnout are highly prevalent in the general population, calling for understanding of its causes. Taking a broad approach, the aim of this study was to determine various mental and somatic risk factors for development of insomnia and burnout and stratifying for sex and age group. Questionnaire data were used from a Swedish population-based sample aged 18-79 years, from which cohorts without insomnia (n=1702) and without burnout (n=1972) at baseline were followed-up after three years. Self-reports of eight mental and somatic conditions at baseline were used as independent variables in logistic regression analyses to predict development of insomnia and burnout at 3-year follow-up. All eight studied conditions were significant risk factors for development of both insomnia (odds ratio, OR = 1.62-2.73) and burnout (OR = 2.20-3.21). Burnout and poor self-rated health had the highest ORs for insomnia, and poor self-rated health, anxiety and somatic symptoms had the highest ORs for burnout. The ORs were generally similar between men and women, whereas age groups tended to differ in some of the risk factors. The study highlights the importance of a broad assessment of both mental and somatic conditions in the prevention of insomnia and burnout. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
... Specifically, workers experiencing stressful situations may have a reduced ability to down-regulate emotions and may not be able to process negative contextual stimuli, which are perceived as more threatening [46]. For example, the results of the study conducted by Golkar et al. [47] in individuals suffering from chronic workrelated stress highlighted an alteration in functional couplings within the emotion-and stress-processing limbic networks associated with a reduced ability to regulate negative emotions. At the neurophysiological level, Rosenkranz et al. [48] demonstrated that chronic stress increases fear and the amygdala's lateral nuclei neural excitability. ...
Article
Full-text available
Although a growing body of research has analyzed the determinants and effects of technostress, it is still unclear how and when technostress would impact workers’ psycho-physical health and work-family interface during the pandemic. To fill this gap, this study tests the mediating mechanisms and the boundary conditions associated with the impact of technostress on workers’ psycho-physical well-being and work-family conflict. A total of 266 Italian workers completed online questionnaires measuring (traditional vs. remote) working modalities, technostress, fear of COVID-19, working excessively, psycho-physical distress, work-family conflict, loss of a loved one due to COVID-19, and resilience. Structural equation models were performed. Results indicated that technostress was positively related to psycho-physical distress and work-family conflict, as mediated by fear of COVID-19 and working excessively, respectively. The loss of a loved one exacerbated the effects of fear of COVID-19 on psycho-physical health, while resilience buffered the effects of working excessively on work-family conflict. Since numerous organizations intend to maintain remote working also after the COVID-19 emergency, it is crucial to study this phenomenon during its peaks of adoption, to prevent its potential negative outcomes. The implications of these findings for theory and practice are discussed.
... Some of these anatomic changes may be reversible if uncontrolled stress is removed [15], but the time frame available in which reversibility is possible is not currently clear. Further evidence of the effect of this high occupational stress includes telomere shortening (accelerated cellular aging) [19], cortisol levels becoming high, then low, creating coronary artery plaques [16] [17] [18]. The medical culture of endurance and silence, has impeded recognition of the human limitations of clinicians. ...
... According to Gavelin et al., cognitive impairments in burnout might be prefrontal in nature i.e., executive dysfunction could cause impairments in other cognitive domains by impairment in cognitive control [43]. Furthermore, structural and functional changes in the prefrontal cortex, as well as in the amygdala and the striatum, have been found in many neuroimaging studies [50][51][52], providing the underlying neural basis for executive dysfunction related to burnout. Structural changes in brain regions might be caused by elevated glucocorticoid levels caused by the prolonged uncontrollable stress underlying burnout [53]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Burnout is becoming a global pandemic jeopardizing brain health, with a huge impact on quality of life, available workforce, and the economy. Knowledge of the impact of burnout on cognition, physiology, and physical activity (PA) in daily life allows for an improved understanding of the health consequences and everyday ramifications of burnout. Twenty-eight volunteers participated in a three-day recording of daily physiology and PA, including heart rate (HR) and daily steps, with a wearable device. They filled in questionnaires screening for burnout (BBI-15), depression (BDI), and executive functions (EFs) in daily life (BRIEF-A). The subjects with burnout had more challenges in EFs, higher average HRs and lower numbers of steps in daily life than those without it. The BBI-15 scores correlated positively with the BDI scores and BRIEF-A indices and negatively with the awake HR variability (HRV) and daily steps. The metacognition index correlated negatively with the HRV. In conclusion, burnout is linked with compromised EFs along with alterations in cardiac physiology and PA in daily life. Such alterations may be easily detected with wearable devices, opening possibilities for novel biomarkers of burnout and other neuropsychiatric disorders. We suggest that physical activity and heart and brain health are intimately intertwined and that burnout interacts with each of them bidirectionally.
... Work-related fatigue is common among workers, such as those in car factories [66]. Serious illnesses (stroke, neuromuscular diseases) and injuries can affect job burnout and the ability to be employed and work, but knowledge of individual factors is limited [67,68]. Further diagnostic methods need to be linked to being an employee. ...
Article
Full-text available
Occupational burnout, manifested by emotional exhaustion, lack of a sense of personal achievement, and depersonalization, is not a new phenomenon, but thusfar, there is no clear definition or diagnostic guidelines. The aim of this article wasto summarize all empirical studies to date that have used medical neuroimaging techniques to provide evidence or links regarding changes in brain function in occupational burnout syndrome from a neuroscientific perspective, and then use these to propose a fuzzy-based computational model of burnout.A comprehensive literature search was conducted in two major databases (PubMed and Medline Complete). The search period was 2006–2021, and searches were limited to the English language. Each article was carefully reviewed and appropriately selected on the basis of raw data, validity of methods used, clarity of results, and scales for measuring burnout. The results showed that the brain structures of patients with job burnout that are associated with emotion, motivation, and empathy weresignificantly different from healthy controls. These altered brain regions included the thalamus, hippocampus, amygdala, caudate, striatum, dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, anterior insula, inferior frontal cingulate cortex, middle frontal cingulate cortex, temporoparietal junction, and grey matter. Deepening our understanding of how these brain structures are related to burnout will pave the way for better approaches fordiagnosis and intervention. As an alternative to the neuroimaging approach, the paper presents a late proposal of the PLUS (personal living usual satisfaction) parameter. It is based on a fuzzy model, wherein the data source is psychological factors—the same or similar to the neuroimaging approach. As the novel approach to searching for neural burnout mechanisms, we have shown that computational models, including those based on fuzzy logic and artificial neural networks, can play an important role in inferring and predicting burnout. Effective computational models of burnout are possible but need further development to ensure accuracy across different populations. There is also a need to identify mechanisms and clinical indicators of chronic fatigue syndrome, stress, burnout, and natural cognitive changes associated with, for example, ageing, in order to introduce more effective differential diagnosis and screening.
Article
Stress-related exhaustion is associated with cognitive deficits, measured subjectively using questionnaires targeting everyday slips and failures or more objectively as performance on cognitive tests. Yet, only weak associations between subjective and objective cognitive measures in this group has been presented, theorized to reflect recruitment of compensational resources during cognitive testing. This explorative study investigated how subjectively reported symptoms of cognitive functioning and burnout levels relate to performance as well as neural activation during a response inhibition task. To this end, 56 patients diagnosed with stress-related exhaustion disorder (ED; ICD-10 code F43.8A) completed functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) using a Flanker paradigm. In order to investigate associations between neural activity and subjective cognitive complaints (SCCs) and burnout, respectively, scores on the Prospective and retrospective memory questionnaire (PRMQ) and the Shirom-Melamed burnout questionnaire (SMBQ) were added as covariates of interest to a general linear model at the whole-brain level. In agreement with previous research, the results showed that SCCs and burnout levels were largely unrelated to task performance. Moreover, we did not see any correlations between these self-report measures and altered neural activity in frontal brain regions. Instead, we observed an association between the PRMQ and increased neural activity in an occipitally situated cluster. We propose that this finding may reflect compensational processes at the level of basic visual attention which may go unnoticed in cognitive testing but are reflected in the experience of deficits in everyday cognitive functioning.
Article
Full-text available
A burnout kialakulásáért elsősorban munkahelyi tényezők tehetők felelőssé, de az egyén tulajdonságai (személyiségjegyek) is fontos szerepet játszanak benne. A kezdeti kutatások a munkahelyi tényezők szerepére koncentráltak, csak később került fókuszba az egyéni tulajdonságok szerepe. Ma már egyértelmű, hogy a munkahelyi és személyes jellemzőket együttesen kell tanulmányozni.
Article
Activation of the insula is found in all anxiety-related disorders and increased insular-prefrontal cortex (PFC) functional connectivity is associated with reduced anxiety. In this study, the combined stimulation of the insula and PFC using the dTMS H4 (insula+LPFC) and H2 (PFC) coils were used to reduce anxiety in 13 subjects experiencing occupational stress, and 55 participants suffering from generalized anxiety disorder (GAD). The combined HF stimulation of the insula and PFC significantly decreased anxiety scores according to the HARS, CAS, and STAI anxiety scales, leading to a reduction in anxiety according to HARS of 88.7% and 70.7% in participants with occupational stress and the clinical sample of participants diagnosed with GAD, respectively. The findings suggest that the prefrontal-insular axis is critical for the regulation of anxiety and its stimulation can be used for the treatment of anxiety in people suffering from occupational stress and GAD.
Article
Full-text available
The amygdala is a major structure that orchestrates defensive reactions to environmental threats and is implicated in hypervigilance and symptoms of heightened arousal in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). The basolateral and centromedial amygdala (CMA) complexes are functionally heterogeneous, with distinct roles in learning and expressing fear behaviors. PTSD differences in amygdala-complex function and functional connectivity with cortical and subcortical structures remain unclear. Recent military veterans with PTSD (n=20) and matched trauma-exposed controls (n=22) underwent a resting-state fMRI scan to measure task-free synchronous blood-oxygen level dependent activity. Whole-brain voxel-wise functional connectivity of basolateral and CMA seeds was compared between groups. The PTSD group had stronger functional connectivity of the basolateral amygdala (BLA) complex with the pregenual anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, and dorsal ACC than the trauma-exposed control group (p<0.05; corrected). The trauma-exposed control group had stronger functional connectivity of the BLA complex with the left inferior frontal gyrus than the PTSD group (p<0.05; corrected). The CMA complex lacked connectivity differences between groups. We found PTSD modulates BLA complex connectivity with prefrontal cortical targets implicated in cognitive control of emotional information, which are central to explanations of core PTSD symptoms. PTSD differences in resting-state connectivity of BLA complex could be biasing processes in target regions that support behaviors central to prevailing laboratory models of PTSD such as associative fear learning. Further research is needed to investigate how differences in functional connectivity of amygdala complexes affect target regions that govern behavior, cognition, and affect in PTSD.
Article
Full-text available
Cognitive emotion regulation has been widely shown in the laboratory to be an effective way to alter the nature of emotional responses. Despite its success in experimental contexts, however, we often fail to use these strategies in everyday life where stress is pervasive. The successful execution of cognitive regulation relies on intact executive functioning and engagement of the prefrontal cortex, both of which are rapidly impaired by the deleterious effects of stress. Because it is specifically under stressful conditions that we may benefit most from such deliberate forms of emotion regulation, we tested the efficacy of cognitive regulation after stress exposure. Participants first underwent fear-conditioning, where they learned that one stimulus (CS+) predicted an aversive outcome but another predicted a neutral outcome (CS-). Cognitive regulation training directly followed where participants were taught to regulate fear responses to the aversive stimulus. The next day, participants underwent an acute stress induction or a control task before repeating the fear-conditioning task using these newly acquired regulation skills. Skin conductance served as an index of fear arousal, and salivary α-amylase and cortisol concentrations were assayed as neuroendocrine markers of stress response. Although groups showed no differences in fear arousal during initial fear learning, nonstressed participants demonstrated robust fear reduction following regulation training, whereas stressed participants showed no such reduction. Our results suggest that stress markedly impairs the cognitive regulation of emotion and highlights critical limitations of this technique to control affective responses under stress.
Article
Full-text available
Abnormal responses of the brain to delivered and expected aversive gut stimuli have been implicated in the pathophysiology of irritable bowel syndrome (IBS), a visceral pain syndrome occurring more commonly in women. Task-free resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) can provide information about the dynamics of brain activity that may be involved in altered processing and/or modulation of visceral afferent signals. Fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation is a measure of the power spectrum intensity of spontaneous brain oscillations. This approach was used here to identify differences in the resting-state activity of the human brain in IBS subjects compared with healthy controls (HCs) and to identify the role of sex-related differences. We found that both the female HCs and female IBS subjects had a frequency power distribution skewed toward high frequency to a greater extent in the amygdala and hippocampus compared with male subjects. In addition, female IBS subjects had a frequency power distribution skewed toward high frequency in the insula and toward low frequency in the sensorimotor cortex to a greater extent than male IBS subjects. Correlations were observed between resting-state blood oxygen level-dependent signal dynamics and some clinical symptom measures (e.g., abdominal discomfort). These findings provide the first insight into sex-related differences in IBS subjects compared with HCs using resting-state fMRI.
Article
Despite mounting reports about the negative effects of chronic occupational stress on cognitive functions, it is still uncertain whether and how this type of stress is associated with cerebral changes. This issue was addressed in the present MRI study, in which cortical thickness (Cth) and subcortical volumes were compared between 40 subjects reporting symptoms of chronic occupational stress (38 ± 6 years) and 40 matched controls (36 ± 6 years). The degree of perceived stress was measured with Maslach Burnout Inventory. In stressed subjects, there was a significant thinning of the mesial frontal cortex. When investigating the correlation between age and Cth, the thinning effect of age was more pronounced in the stressed group in the frontal cortex. Furthermore, their amygdala volumes were bilaterally increased (P = 0.020 and P = 0.003), whereas their caudate volumes were reduced (P = 0.040), and accompanied by impaired fine motor function. The perceived stress correlated positively with the amygdala volumes (r = 0.44, P = 0.04; r = 0.43, P = 04). Occupational stress was found to be associated with cortical thinning as well as with selective changes of subcortical volumes, with behavioral correlates. The findings support the hypothesis that stress-related excitotoxicity might be an underlying mechanism, and that the described condition is a stress related illness.
Article
The aim of this paper was to investigate the subjective responses of abstinent heroin users to both neutral and negative stimuli and the related hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal reactions to emotional experience in relationship to their perception of childhood adverse experiences. Thirty male abstinent heroin dependents were included in the study. Emotional responses and childhood neglect perception were measured utilizing the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory Y-1 and the Child Experience of Care and Abuse Questionnaire. Neutral and unpleasant pictures selected from the International Affective Picture System and the Self-Assessment Manikin procedure have been used to determine ratings of pleasure and arousal. These ratings were compared with normative values obtained from healthy volunteers used as control. Blood samples were collected before and after the experimental sessions to determine both adrenocorticotropic hormone and cortisol plasma levels. Basal anxiety scores, cortisol and adrenocorticotropic hormone levels were higher in abstinent heroin users than in controls. Tests showed that anxiety scores did not change in controls after the vision of neutral slides, whilst they did in abstinent heroin addicts, increasing significantly; and increased less significantly after the unpleasant task, in comparison to controls. Abstinent heroin users showed significantly higher levels of parent antipathy and childhood emotional neglect perception than controls for both the father and the mother. Plasma adrenocorticotropic hormone and cortisol levels did not significantly increase after unpleasant slide set viewing among addicted individuals, because of the significantly higher basal levels characterizing the addicted subjects in comparison with controls. Multiple regression correlation showed a significant relationship between childhood neglect perception, arousal reaction, impaired hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis response and addiction severity. Early adverse experiences seem to affect the entire interaction between hyper-arousal, reduced hormonal response to stress and addiction severity. Our findings, although obtained in a small number of subjects, indicate a significant link between the perception of parental style/care/support during childhood and the ability to cope with stressful emotional stimuli in adulthood and addiction severity.
Article
The Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) is a short diagnostic structured interview (DSI) developed in France and the United States to explore 17 disorders according to Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM)-III-R diagnostic criteria. It is fully structured to allow administration by non-specialized interviewers. In order to keep it short it focuses on the existence of current disorders. For each disorder, one or two screening questions rule out the diagnosis when answered negatively. Probes for severity, disability or medically explained symptoms are not explored symptom-by-symptom. Two joint papers present the inter-rater and test-retest reliability of the Mini the validity versus the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) (this paper) and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IH-R patients (SCID) (joint paper). Three-hundred and forty-six patients (296 psychiatric and 50 non-psychiatric) were administered the MINI and the CIDI ‘gold standard’. Forty two were interviewed by two investigators and 42 interviewed subsequently within two days. Interviewers were trained to use both instruments. The mean duration of the interview was 21 min with the MINI and 92 for corresponding sections of the CIDI. Kappa coefficient, sensitivity and specificity were good or very good for all diagnoses with the exception of generalized anxietydisorder (GAD) (kappa = 0.36), agoraphobia (sensitivity = 0.59) and bulimia (kappa = 0.53). Inter-rater and test-retest reliability were good. The main reasons for discrepancies were identified. The MINI provided reliable DSM-HI-R diagnoses within a short time frame, The study permitted improvements in the formulations for GAD and agoraphobia in the current DSM-IV version of the MINI.