ArticlePDF Available
Chimpanzee Wears a Knotted Skin "Necklace"
W.C. McGrew and L.F. Marchant
Anthropology and Zoology,
Miami University
The tying of knots by animals is usually associated with construction of shelters (3), most notably nest-building by the Ploceinae
or weaver birds (2). Among primates, even in bed-making by Great Apes, we can find no previous record of knots; the closest is of
rehabilitated orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) trying unsuccessfully to knot rope to secure a hammock (4). Here we report a knot tied
in a piece of skin of red colobus monkey (Procolobus badius tephrosceles) worn by a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii)
in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania.
On 7 November, 1996, chimpanzees of M group at Mahale killed and ate a red colobus monkey in the afternoon. When the
chimpanzees were found by observers again the next morning, several individuals were still carrying "left-overs". This is not
unusual, as skin is not eaten and youngsters, especially, may play with scraps of it. On the morning of 8 November we observed a
female juvenile, Ai, playing with, and grooming a strip of skin. She was pursued in play by a juvenile male, Primus, who "stole"
the skin. Some time later that same morning we observed Ako, a young adult female probably born in 1981, to be wearing what
appeared to be the skin draped around her neck. When she removed and abandoned it on the trail at 10:45 hr., we found it to be
tied in a single overhand knot. As can be seen from the figure, the knot creates a "necklace" of 68 cm circumference, with a "tail"
of 31 cm. length. We recovered the artifact, photographed it fresh, and then dried it in silica gel.
Draping is one of 21 modes of tool use described by Beck (1); it is not uncommon in both captive and wild apes (e.g. Beck's
book's cover photograph shows an orangutan with a cloth strip draped over the head). However, we know of no other non-human
case in which a draped adornment is made, that is, a necklace created by tying a knot. However, it should be made clear that we
did not see the knot tied. It may not have been done by Ako, and it may have occurred by accident, but the result was functional.
Like all anecdotes, it can only serve to alert us to a possibility; a single case is only an hypothesis, not a conclusion.
We are grateful to M. Bunengwa, M. Huffman, K. Kawanaka, M. Nakamura, H. Sasaki, and S. Uehara for assistance in the field,
and R. O'Malley for the drawings.
REFERENCES
1. Beck, B.B., 1980. Animal Tool Behavior. Garland STPM Press, New York.
2. Crook, J.H., 1960. Nest form and construction in certain West African weaver birds. Ibis 102:1-25.
3. Hansell, M.H. 1984. Animal Architecture and Building Behavior. Longman, London.
4. Russon, A.E., and B.M.F. Galdikas, 1993. Imitation in free-ranging rehabilitant orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) J. Comp.
Psychol. 107:147-161.
Back to Contents
... For instance it is clear that the faculty of self-awareness in a social animal would logically lead to strategies of consciously expressing individualism. Most such evidence is of a nature possessing very low taphonomic thresholds (Bednarik 1994b; but see McGrew and Marchant 1998;McGrew 2004 for apparent 'self-decoration' of a chimpanzee) [22,103,102], but beads and pendants are notable exceptions (Bednarik 1997(Bednarik , 2005(Bednarik , 2008a [27,30,31] providing glimpses of very early self-adornment. The several species indicating degrees of self-awareness (De Veer and Van Den Bos 1999; Gallup 1970Gallup , 1998Gallup et al. 2002;Heyes 1998; Keenan et al. 2003;Mitchell 1993Mitchell , 1997Mitchell , 2002 [62,[73][74][75]85,89,[105][106][107] are much the same as those shown to possess von Economo neurons (Seeley et al. 2006;Butti et al. 2009;Hakeem et al. 2009) [126,55,84]. ...
... For instance it is clear that the faculty of self-awareness in a social animal would logically lead to strategies of consciously expressing individualism. Most such evidence is of a nature possessing very low taphonomic thresholds (Bednarik 1994b; but see McGrew and Marchant 1998;McGrew 2004 for apparent 'self-decoration' of a chimpanzee) [22,103,102], but beads and pendants are notable exceptions (Bednarik 1997(Bednarik , 2005(Bednarik , 2008a [27,30,31] providing glimpses of very early self-adornment. The several species indicating degrees of self-awareness (De Veer and Van Den Bos 1999; Gallup 1970Gallup , 1998Gallup et al. 2002;Heyes 1998; Keenan et al. 2003;Mitchell 1993Mitchell , 1997Mitchell , 2002 [62,[73][74][75]85,89,[105][106][107] are much the same as those shown to possess von Economo neurons (Seeley et al. 2006;Butti et al. 2009;Hakeem et al. 2009) [126,55,84]. ...
Article
Full-text available
This comprehensive overview considers the currently known Pleistocene palaeoart of Asia on a common basis, which suggests that the available data are entirely inadequate to form any cohesive synthesis about this corpus. In comparison to the attention lavished on the corresponding record available from Eurasia’s small western appendage, Europe, it is evident that Pleistocene palaeoart from the rest of the world has been severely neglected. Southern Asia, in particular, holds great promise for the study of early cognitive development of hominins, and yet this potential has remained almost entirely unexplored. Asia is suggested to be the key continent in any global synthesis of ‘art’ origins, emphasising the need for a comprehensive pan-continental research program. This is not just to counter-balance the incredible imbalance in favour of Europe, but to examine the topic of Middle Pleistocene palaeoart development effectively.
... In a child it is at the age of about 40 months that the ToM surpasses that of the extant great apes, and homology would suggest that australopithecines probably had considerably more developed cognitive faculties at their disposal than chimpanzees or bonobos. Body adornment is an obvious consequence of self-awareness and in incipient form may be present in some chimpanzees (McGrew and Marchant 1998;McGrew 2004;Nishida et al. 2009). Therefore from a biological perspective, and ignoring archaeological doctrine, it might be expected that the earliest physical evidence of body decoration should be of the late Pliocene. ...
... Robert a. Dielenberg) can safely be assumed to predate them in the primate ancestry by dozens of millions of years. The view that the first appearance of 'the human ability to be reflectively conscious of one's own perspective on the world' must be deducible from archaeological finds is itself anthropocentric, considering the incidence of Tom and self-awareness among non-human animals (Gallup 1970;Call and Tomasello 1998;Gallese and Goldman 1998;mcgrew and Marchant 1998;McGrew 2004;Iacoboni et al. 2005;Sommerville and Decety 2006;Keysers and Gazzola 2007;Nishida et al. 2009;Bednarik 2011a). ...
Article
Full-text available
Palaeoart constitutes the entire surviving corpus of exograms available from the distant hominin past, providing a principal resource for accessing cognitive dimensions of early humans. The potential of exogrammatic review has remained largely unexplored so far in palaeoart research. The reasons for this are explored, and the role of exograms in the development of semiotic capacities and mental constructs is examined. It is shown that, among the classes of evidence available to reason about the cognitive evolution of hominins, exograms, although truncated taphonomically, are the most comprehensive and dependable source of information. However, it also emerges that this record needs to be considered in scientific rather than simplistic humanistic terms. Its potential in exploring neuroscientific aspects of hominin evolution is investigated.
... Self-awareness is also present in a variety of other animals, as apparently demonstrated by the mirror test (Keenan et al. 2003; for detailed discussions of self-awareness, Theory of Mind and consciousness see Bednarik 2011bBednarik , 2013c. Body adornment seems to be limited to humans, although one instance has been NeanderArt 2018Proceedings NeanderART 2018 reported in which chimpanzees may have engaged in incipient self-decoration behaviour (McGrew and Marchant 1998;McGrew 2004;Nishida et al. 2009). If that observation is correct, one might expect to find early examples of body decoration soon after the debut of Homo. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Most of the differences between humans and other animals still endorsed in the 20th century have now been refuted. Even theory of mind, self-awareness, recursion and metarepresentation are losing their eminence as exclusively human variables. This may leave us with just one distinguishing trait: the talent of creating and using memory traces external to the brain. Palaeoart is the principal empirical evidence of this ability from the human past, a corpus that has been misconstrued in various respects. For instance, the discussion of the dawn of art-like productions and the behavioural range they facilitate has been consistently marred by humanistic banalities and lapses. Among the most damaging are to treat such productions as art or as symbols, and to impose commensurate but false taxonomies. Another fatal shortcoming of this debate has been the rejection of exogrammatic evidence on the basis that it is not art. This presentation endeavours to correct some of these misunderstandings.
... This possibility was first hinted at by observations of a female orangutan at a zoo, who would decorate herself by gathering lettuce leaves from her cage to pile them onto her head while inspecting herself closely in the mirror [33] (Fig 4). Similarly, chimpanzees sometimes adorn themselves by walking around with the skin of monkey prey around their necks or develop a group-wide "fashion" to insert grass into their ears [34,35]. Only with a richer theory of the self and a larger test battery will we be able to determine all of the various levels of selfawareness, including where exactly fish fit in. ...
Article
Full-text available
The mirror mark test has encouraged a binary view of self-awareness according to which a few species possess this capacity whereas others do not. Given how evolution works, however, we need a more gradualist model of the various ways in which animals construe a self and respond to mirrors. The recent study on cleaner wrasses (Labroides dimidiatus) by Kohda and colleagues highlights this need by presenting results that, due to ambiguous behavior and the use of physically irritating marks, fall short of mirror self-recognition. The study suggests an intermediate level of mirror understanding, closer to that of monkeys than hominids.
... Knot tying is rare in captive great apes and possibly absent in the wild (McGrew and Marchant 1998;Herzfeld and Lestel 2005). In the present context, it would require a stem or frond being looped around a trunk and the two ends overlapped and pulled tight; however, I did not examine nests sufficiently to determine this (but see Fig. 1d, e). ...
Article
With data accumulating from a growing pool of chimpanzee field studies, new behaviors as well as novel variants on common behaviors continue to be described. Nest construction is a universal behavior in wild great apes. Among chimpanzee populations, reported variation in nest building behavior mostly reflects environmental constraints. Despite the ubiquity of nest making by chimpanzees, only ground nesting has been recognized as a behavioral variant, potentially determined by both environmental and social factors. In a study of nests made by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in Bulindi, Uganda, I identified a hitherto undescribed nest construction technique, termed ‘nest tying’. Five observed nests lacked strong weight-bearing structures beneath them, such as large branches or a supporting trunk. Instead, the nests appeared ‘tied’ (or ‘tethered’) to an adjacent trunk by looping leafy stems or palm fronds around it and interweaving these into the nest mattress, securing the nest against the trunk; thus, nest tying presumably functions to provide stability and added support. This preliminary report presents a description of the observed nests. Irrespective of whether nest tying constitutes true knot making – commonly considered absent in wild great apes – this nest construction technique would seem to require advanced dexterity and a sophisticated understanding of the mechanical properties of the plants used. Forest fragments in Bulindi are highly degraded. Thus, nest tying – and construction of integrated nests (i.e., utilizing multiple plants, often small trees and shrubs) generally – may be promoted by a relative paucity of suitable nesting trees at this site. Still, insofar as nest building is learned in chimpanzees, different construction techniques including nest tying are potentially acquired through social learning. Further investigation is required to ascertain the prevalence and acquisition of this nest construction technique at Bulindi, and to verify its presence or absence in other habitats.
... Birds and mammals also routinely handle objects to eat, play, groom, defend, or hide themselves (Beck 1980;Bentley-Condit and Smith 2010;Shumaker et al. 2011;Mann and Patterson 2013), yet rarely self-decorate with them. The best example of self-decoration is a free-ranging female chimpanzee who wore a strip of skin from a fresh monkey kill like a necklace for a day, albeit a one-time observation (McGrew and Marchant 1998). Captive elephants twisted vegetation into ropes and rings that they placed on their bodies, behavior that suggested self-decoration but which Chevalier-Skolnikoff and Liska (1993) classified as ambiguous. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
... Apparently useless materials are also collected (Kahlenberg and Wrangham 2010;S. Krief, personal observation;McGrew and Marchant 1998;Wrangham and Peterson 1996). Since the 1950s, several studies have been conducted in captivity to investigate curiosity and exploratory behaviors and their determining factors in different animal species (e.g., Butler 1960;Corey 1978;Harlow 1950;Hughes 1997; see also Byrne 2013 for an overview). ...
Article
Archaeological discoveries suggest that human interest in odd objects emerged as early as the end of the Lower Paleolithic with Homo erectus, although it is still difficult to understand why early humans collected these objects. Several studies show that nonhuman primates are able to appreciate the physical characteristics of their tools, but their interest in the physical properties of nonutilitarian objects has, to our knowledge, never been tested. Here, objects of different brightness, color, and shape were proposed to five orangutans to test whether odd objects intrigue orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) and whether they induce specific and noticeable behavior. This preliminary experiment shows that orangutans are responsive to the physical properties of nonutilitarian objects, particularly visually striking, bright, and colorful objects. Our experiment shows that these are the most manipulated pieces, regardless of their respective proportions. However, unlike (early) humans, orangutans did not show any tendency to preserve objects.
... Knot tying behaviours have never been described up to now in great apes living in the wild except one single case (McGrew & Marchant, 1998). No even available study reports that captive chimpanzees can be trained to make knots (Vauclair, 2000). ...
Article
Full-text available
Ethology and comparative psychology have shown that some animals could have abilities that were unthinkable not a long time ago. It is not only more developped abilities that what has been said before but also a structure of abilities that were even not supposed to exist as such. Unlike classical ethology (objectivist, sociobiological or even cognitive main stream ethology), some ethologists have described abilities of some animals that do not refer only to the species but also to the group within which they live and to particuliarities that individualise them. At least among the more complex species, each animal has to be understood at the crossroad of three different stories: a phylogenetic history (history of the species), a cultural history (history of the group) and a biographical history (history of the individual itself). In that way, some particular animals show not only rare but also very exceptional cognitive abilities. A good example of that situation is given by the young orang-utan female that we have observed at the Ménagerie du Jardin des Plantes of Paris and who has the very unusual ability to tie knots. Experiments have confirmed that ability and have shown the bright mastering of that ape in that task. We will first recall experimental results we have obtained before to discuss them. An interesting question is to know what does actually do that orang utan female when she ties complex knots with great ease and when she manipulates fibres. We wish to defend here a strong interpretation: Wattana, the orang utan female, performs a kind of mathematics and the study of her knots tying behavior allows us to bring the question of origins of mathematics on new grounds in an ethological perspective – a task that has never been done up to now. Indeed, origins of mathematics have only been discussed by historians of mathematics in an historical uniquely human way.
... Beads and pendants, however, may in rare circumstances survive taphonomy. An incipient form of body decoration may have been observed by McGrew and Marchant ( [194]; cf. [195,196]) in 1996. ...
Article
Full-text available
This paper begins with a refutation of the orthodox model of final Pleistocene human evolution, presenting an alternative, better supported account of this crucial phase. According to this version, the transition from robust to gracile humans during that period is attributable to selective breeding rather than natural selection, rendered possible by the exponential rise of culturally guided volitional choices. The rapid human neotenization coincides with the development of numerous somatic and neural detriments and pathologies. Uniformitarian reasoning based on ontogenic homology suggests that the cognitive abilities of hominins are consistently underrated in the unstable orthodoxies of Pleistocene archaeology. A scientifically guided review establishes developmental trajectories defining recent changes in the human genome and its expressions, which then form the basis of attempts to extrapolate from them into the future. It is suggested that continuing and perhaps accelerating unfavorable genetic changes to the human species, rather than existential threats such as massive disasters, pandemics, or astrophysical events, may become the ultimate peril of humanity.
Article
Full-text available
We made an observational study of spontaneous imitation in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Previous studies may have underestimated great apes' imitative capacities by studying subjects under inhibiting conditions. We used subjects living in enriched environments, namely, rehabilitation. We collected a sample of spontaneous imitations and analyzed the most complex incidents for the likelihood that true imitation, learning new actions by observing rather than by doing, was involved in their acquisition. From 395 hr of observation and other reports on 26 orangutans, we identified 354 incidents of imitation. Of these, 54 complex incidents were difficult to explain by forms of imitation based on associative processes grounded in experimental learning alone; they were, however, congruent with acquisition processes that include true imitation. These findings suggest that orangutans may be capable of true imitation and point to critical eliciting factors.
Article
Ploceine nests are small globes with entrances at the side or below with or without some extension of the entrance in a spout or tube. An “initial ring” is first constructed and thereafter the male bird sits in this to carry out stitching and shaping. The form and dimensions of a nest are thus determined by the use of a fixed position and the distances to which the bird can reach up, out and around from it. The egg chamber develops first and the initial ring is thus the original entrance. Later construction involves the extension of the roof down over the front of the initial ring aperture to form porch, antechamber or funnel etc. (Construction in Amblyospiza albifront is aberrant.) Four types of nest are characterised: “globular”, “kidney-shaped”, “incomplete kidney-shaped” and “retort-shaped”. The latter may be subdivided into nests with a funnel entrance (Type A) and those with tubes (Type B). Nest construction is described in detail for Quelea quelea and compared with that of Ploceus {Sitagra) cucullatus and other species. The stages are classified and stitching and shaping movements described. The differences between stitching movements correlate with successive stages of construction and the differences are regarded as due to different orientations of the same basic movements to the developing nest. Proximate factors determining nest form are: the methods of attachment, the size of the bird, and the construction of much of the nest from a fixed position in the initial ring. Extension of the entrance is a function of the extent to which building continues after the completion of the egg chamber. It is thus in part an expression of the length of time spent building relative to the total duration of the breeding season. In considering the ultimate factors responsible for the evolution of nest forms a correlation is noted between long breeding seasons, predominantly insect diet, nest siting in trees and long tubular entrances. Likewise there is a rough correlation between short breeding seasons (and/or rapid breeding) and globular or kidney-shaped nests. A high speed of reproduction in a short breeding season seems correlated with a cessation of nest building at an earlier stage than where the breeding season is a long one. However, nests of a particular species often show characteristic features, which suggests that the nest of a species is the product of selection for its survival value in the habitat concerned. Nest form is thus considered not merely the expression of the speed of reproduction but in addition to be directly adapted to its site. The evidence for the four types of nest is discussed and the need for further study emphasised. Building behaviour is discussed. The production of a properly developed nest requires constant repetition of the building sequences, especially at the fixed building position, and also building activity sufficiently sustained to produce roof and walls and shaping sufficient to mould the form of the nest. Low motivation results in aberrant shapes through the use of irregular building positions and inadequate shaping.
  • B B Beck
Beck, B.B., 1980. Animal Tool Behavior. Garland STPM Press, New York.