Conference PaperPDF Available

Concurrent activation potentiation enhances performance of swimming race start

Authors:

Abstract

Concurrent activation potentiation (CAP) is recently developed method for enhancing athletic performance in speed-strength exercises. Th is phenomenon is based on the stimulatory eff ect of the voluntary contraction of remote mus-cles, which facilitates activation of prime movers and synergists for targeted exercise. Physiological prerequisites for this ergogenic eff ect are associated with the integrative function of the cerebral motor cortex and increased excit-ability of spinal motor neurons. Th e present study was aimed at evaluating the ergogenic eff ect producing by CAP treatment on the performance of the race start in competitive swimming. Th e CAP modifi ed start technique presupposed the teeth clenching and voluntary contraction of abdominal muscles aft er the preliminary starting command. Eight elite and sub-elite swimmers executed four crawl stroke starts: two CAP modifi ed and two conventional trials with objective registration of start reaction and time to complete a 15-m segment. Statistical analysis included a repeated measures two-way ANOVA test and size eff ect. Both the p-value and d-value between experimental and control sets were calculated. Th e average benefi t of the CAP modifi ed technique was equal to 0.08 s, which includes an advantage in start reaction equal to 0.05 s. As a result, application of the CAP modifi ed technique to swimming race start resulted in a signifi cant ergogenic eff ect, which was evaluated by start reaction and performance time on the 15-m segment of distance.
Concurrent activation potentiation enhances performance of swimming race start | 41
V. B. Issurin, O. Verbitsky
Concurrent activation potentiation enhances performance of swimming race start
CONCURRENT ACTIVATION POTENTIATION
ENHANCES PERFORMANCE OF SWIMMING
RACE START
V B. I  O V
Wingate Institute for Physical Education and Sport, Netanya 42902, Israel
Technion, Institute of Technology, Haifa 32000, Israel
ABSTRACT
Concurrent activation potentiation (CAP) is recently developed method for
enhancing athletic performance in speed-strength exercises.  is phenomenon
is based on the stimulatory e ect of the voluntary contraction of remote mus-
cles, which facilitates activation of prime movers and synergists for targeted
exercise. Physiological prerequisites for this ergogenic e ect are associated
with the integrative function of the cerebral motor cortex and increased excit-
ability of spinal motor neurons.  e present study was aimed at evaluating the
ergogenic e ect producing by CAP treatment on the performance of the race
start in competitive swimming.  e CAP modi ed start technique presupposed
the teeth clenching and voluntary contraction of abdominal muscles a er the
preliminary starting command. Eight elite and sub-elite swimmers executed
four crawl stroke starts: two CAP modi ed and two conventional trials with
objective registration of start reaction and time to complete a 15-m segment.
Statistical analysis included a repeated measures two-way ANOVA test and
size e ect. Both the p-value and d-value between experimental and control
sets were calculated.  e average bene t of the CAP modi ed technique was
equal to 0.08 s, which includes an advantage in start reaction equal to 0.05 s.
As a result, application of the CAP modi ed technique to swimming race start
resulted in a signi cant ergogenic e ect, which was evaluated by start reaction
and performance time on the 15-m segment of distance.
Key words: concurrent activation potentiation, ergogenic e ect, swimming start,
split-unit design
Acta Kinesiologiae Universitatis Tartuensis, 2013. Vol. 19, pp. 41–47
http://dx.doi.org/10.12697/akut.2013.19.04
42 | V. B. Issurin, O. Verbitsky
INTRODUCTION
e stimulatory e ect of concurrent activation of remote muscles on the
targeted muscular performance has been known since the late 19th century,
when Hungarian physician Erno Jendrassik developed this original technique,
termed as the Jendrassik Manoeuver (JM).  e JM technique proposes the
examination of tendon re ex in the lower limbs when the patient interlocks
his/her hands together, pulling them apart and clenching the teeth. Since then
the JM has been known as a procedure allowing for potentiating the tendon
re ex in neurologically impaired patients [16]. Correspondingly, the JM can
be considered as a classic example of the case when remote voluntary contrac-
tion (RVC) ampli es the motor output and neural excitability of other muscle
groups.
Earlier studies of the potentiation e ects of teeth clenching were performed
by Japanese researchers, who revealed that teeth clenching produces a signi -
cant potentiation e ect on shoulder adduction [15] and ankle plantar  exion
[13].  e researchers investigated variations of H-re ex following RVC and
teeth clenching in a series of in-depth studies. H-re ex, which is evoked by
electricalstimulation of the sensory  bers of the innervating nerve, serves as
the world-recognized indicator of the excitability of spinal motor neurons; its
variables characterize acute and chronic neuromuscular adaptation to maxi-
mal e ort and resistance training. It has been established that teeth clenching
increases the excitability of the soleus H-re ex (i), and the greater force level in
teeth clenching produces greater facilitation e ect on the soleus H-re ex (ii);
facilitation of H-re ex is associated with both the descending in uence from
the cerebral cortex and the a erent input from the oral-facial region (iii) [10,
11].  e similar facilitation of H-re ex induced by teeth clenching has been
found in the forearm muscles [14]. Hiroshi [7] reported that teeth clench-
ing before and during hand griping elicits a signi cant increase of maximal
force and rate of force development as compared with no clenching conditions.
Furthermore, a number of studies evaluated the potentiation e ect of RVC
combined with the performance of various speed-strength exercises, termed
Concurrent Activation Potentiation (CAP) [2].  us, a signi cant ergogenic
e ect of CAP has been found during the performance of the high jump, squat
jump, back squat, and knee  exion and extension [3–5].  ese publications
reported gains of maximal force, rate force development, and jump height
that varied within 4.2–32.2% depending on the character of the variables and
experimental conditions.
e possible mechanisms underlying the CAP e ects were considered
with regards to two major theories a ributed to the integrative function of the
Concurrent activation potentiation enhances performance of swimming race start | 43
cerebral motor cortex and increased excitability of spinal motor neurons.  e
rst one is focused on intercortical connections between di erent motor areas
of the brain, when activation of one part of the motor cortex that is induced by
RVC, such as teeth clenching, a ects the activation of neural centers located in
another part of the brain.  ese centers descend a motor command to prime
movers, which initiates e orts for targeted exercise [2]. Another theory under-
lines the role of RVC in the increase of the excitability of spinal motor neurons.
is increased excitability is supported by extensive  ndings of H-re ex investi-
gated during the administration of various RVCs, such as jaw clenching, JM,
etc. [6, 13].  e present study was aimed at evaluating the ergogenic e ect pro-
duced by the application of CAP to the movement technique of the swimming
competitive race start.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Subjects
Eight elite and sub-elite male swimmers took part in the study (mean±standard
deviation [SD]: age: 19.1±2.13 yrs, height: 179.2±3.2 cm, body mass:
74.3±3.4kg, training experience 8.9±1.3 yrs). All subjects were su ciently
familiar with the CAP modi ed start, using this technique in practice and
competitions during at least two months. All subjects were informed of the
purpose and study design of the present research.
Procedures
e experimental model of the CAP modi ed swimming start has been elabo-
rated upon based on the outcomes of previous studies [3–5], and taking into
account our experience in start technique investigation in Olympic swimmers
[9].  e technique protocol included teeth clenching and voluntary contrac-
tion of abdominal muscles, which were initiated immediately a er the preli-
minary starting command (“take your marks”).  e time interval between the
preliminary command and the starting signal varied between 0.9–1.2 s.  is
time course completely corresponds to the three-phasic character of the poten-
tiation process: initial teeth clenching and abdominal contraction embraces
the 1
st
phase, whereas the takeo was corresponded to the 2
nd
phase, where
intensity of the potentiation impact reaches maximum [1].  e subjects were
required to stop the teeth clenching immediately a er takeo , while abdominal
contraction was maintained according to a habitual coordinative pa ern.
44 | V. B. Issurin, O. Verbitsky
e data were collected during the competitive period. Two VHS video
cameras operating at a nominal rate of 50 Hz were positioned perpendicular to
the axis of the swimmers’ movement. One camera was placed opposite the start
block at 5 m from the swimmers’ location; the second one was installed at the
side of the pool and served for measuring time on the mark 15 m.  e starting
command was produced in accordance with the competition’s rules, and a LED
signal was transmi ed to the videosystem.  e videotape data were analyzed
with the Ariel Performance Analysis System (Ariel Dynamics Inc., CA, USA);
start reaction (SR) was indicated as the time from start signal to takeo ; start
e ciency (SE) was assessed with the time to complete the segment 15 m.
Experimental design
e subjects randomly performed four trials with the crawl stroke, using either
the conventional or the CAP modi ed technique, with the rest interval about
8min. Eight swimmers were de ned as experimental units, while 32 individual
measurements, which were performed on eight swimmers, were de ned as
subunits, correlated the evaluations. A type of start was de ned as a repeated
treatment design, which was applied to the experimental units. Repeated e orts
(trials) within each type of start, de ned as a repeated treatment design, were
applied to the subunits, correlated observations. Start reaction and start e -
ciency were de ned as a response variable structure.  e experimental design
structure can be de ned as a 2x2 factorial within-subject repeated experiment
with a completely randomized split-unit design structure [8].
Statistical analysis
In the present study we used a repeated measures two-way ANOVA test with
Greenhouse-Geisser epsilon adjustment. E ect size was evaluated by the
Cohens d-test for dependent variables. We reported d-test value as a measure of
e ect size, which generally considered 0.20 as a small e ect, 0.50 as a medium
e ect, 0.80 as a large e ect, and 1.30 a very large e ect.  e descriptive statis-
tics were presented by average di erence between two sets in s.  e statistical
signi cance was established at p<0.05.
RESULTS
As shown in Table 1, the di erence between the experimental and the control
sets reached signi cance for start reaction time (F
1,7
=20.70, p=0.0029) and for
Concurrent activation potentiation enhances performance of swimming race start | 45
start e ciency (F
1,7
=11.137, p=0.0125).  e start reaction time was shorter
in the experimental set as compared with a control set; average di erence
= 0.05 s, e ect size, d=1.59.  e start e ciency was higher in experimental set
as compared with a control set; average di erence 0.08 s, e ect size, d=1.01.
Table 1. The effect of start type on the start reaction and start efficiency Repeated
Measures two-way ANOVA test
Source of Variance df Start reaction Start efficiency
Fp F p
Factor A, Control vs. Experiment sets 1 20.070 0.0029 11.137 0.0125
Residual 7
Factor B, Trial I vs. Trial II, 1 1.346 0.2840 0.806 0.3990
Residual 7
A x B interaction 1 2.944 0.1299 0.023 0.8835
Residual 7
Number of total observations 31
Note: The statistical significance is established at p< 0.05.
DISCUSSION
e study outcomes give strong evidence of the signi cant ergogenic e ect
of the CAP application, which was shown both for the start reaction and start
e ciency evaluated by time on 15 m. It can be expected that possible e ects of
CAP treatment may be the increased rate force development in takeo followed
by increased horizontal velocity and  ight distance, which may be the major
contributors of higher start e ciency. e study revealed one more factor of
the ergogenic e ect – shortening reaction time. To our knowledge, this is the
rst evidence that CAP may produce more rapid motor reaction. It is worth
noting that the time course of swimming start provides a very small chance
for shortening motor reaction, although this possibility was realized in a study
where the Grab start was replaced by Track technique [9].
e observations during competitions give additional support for the
occurrence of an ergogenic e ect during implementation of the CAP modi-
ed swim start technique.  e monitoring of start reaction is a routine practice
of o cial competitions, where the “Omega” electronic time system presents
on-line data and protocols. In our observations, several swimmers employing
the CAP start enhanced their reaction time by 0.12–0.16 s as compared with
their habitual data. Of course, these extraordinary gains exceed the range that
46 | V. B. Issurin, O. Verbitsky
can be expected following neurophysiological and biomechanical prerequisites.
However, factors such as cognitive input, bene cial mental concentration, and
a more favorable emotional state can also be taken into account as contributors
to these motor e ects. To date, the swimmers who practiced a CAP modi ed
start have utilized it in European Championships in the event 50 m backstroke
and earned bronze (2011) and gold (2012) medals.
In conclusion, unlike previously conducted studies [e.g., 3–5, 7, 12, 15],
the targeted technical component in the present research was not a separate
exercise, but rather a relatively short part of the entire athletic performance.
erefore, this study is one of the  rst a empts to implement CAP treatment
into athletic performance that is regulated by o cial competition protocol.  e
study outcomes evidence that this implementation is of distinct value and per-
spective for competitive practice and further research projects in other sports.
REFERENCES
1.
Delwade PJ, Toulouse P. (1981) Facilitation of monosynaptic reflexes by
voluntary contraction in remote parts of the body. Mechanisms involved in the
Jendrassik maneuver. Brain, 104 (Pt 4), 701–709
2.
Ebben WP. (2006) A brief review of concurrent activation potentiation:  eoreti-
cal and practical constructs. J Strength Cond Res, 20 (4), 985–991
3.
Ebben WP, Flanagan EP, Jenssen RL. (2008) Jaw clenching results in concurrent
activation potentiation during the countermovement jump. J Strength Cond Res,
22 (6), 1850–54
4.
Ebben WP, Kaufmann CE, Fauth ML. (2010) Kinetic analysis of concurrent
activation potentiation during back squats and jump squats. J Strength Cond
Res, 24 (6), 1515–19
5. Ebben WP, Petushek EJ, Fauth ML. (2010) EMG analysis of concurrent activa-
tion potentiation. Med Sci Sports Exerc, 42 (3), 556–62
6.
Gregory JE, Wood SA, Proske U. (2001) An investigation into mechanisms of
re ex reinforcement by the Jendrassik maneuver. Exp Brain Res, 138 (3), 366–74
7.
Hiroshi C. (2003) Relation between teeth clenching and grip force production
characteristics. Kokubyo Gakkal Zasshi, 70 (2), 82–88
8.
Hurlbert SH. (2013) Pseudofactorialism, response structures and collective
responsibility. Austral Ecology 38: 6, 646–663
9.
Issurin VB, Verbitsky O. (2003) Track start vs. Grab start: evidence of the Sydney
Olympic games. In: Chatard J-K. (Ed.). Biomechanics and Medicine in Swim-
ming IX. Saint-Etienne University, France, pp. 213–218
10. Miyahara T. (1991) Modulation of soleus H-re ex by teeth clenching. Kokubyo
Gakkal Zasshi, 58 (4), 670–686
Concurrent activation potentiation enhances performance of swimming race start | 47
11.
Miyahara T, Haqiya N, Ohyama T. (1996) Modulation of human soleus H-re ex
in association with voluntary clenching of the teeth. J Neurophysiol, 76 (3),
2033–41
12.
Sasaki Y, Ueno T, Taniquchi H, Ohyama T. (1998) E ect of teeth clenching on
isometric and isokinetic strength of ankle plantar  exion. J Med Dent Sci, 45
(1), 29–37
13.
Takada Y, Miyahara T, Tanaka T, Ohyama T, Nakamura Y. (2000) Modulation
of H-re ex of pretibial muscles and reciprocal inhibition of soleus muscle during
voluntary teeth clenching in humans. J Neurophysiol, 83, 2063–2070
14.
Takahashi T, Ueno T, Ohyama T. (2003) Modulation of H-re ex in the fore-
arm during voluntary teeth clenching in humans. Eur J Appl Physiol, 90 (5–6),
651–53
15.
Ueno T. (1995) Study on relationship between teeth clenching in intercuspal
position and isometric movement of upper limbs. Kokubyo Gakkal Zasshi, 62
(2), 212–253
16.
Zehr EP, Stein RB. (1999) Interaction of the Jendrassik maneuver with segmental
pre-synaptic inhibition. Exp Brain Res, 124, 474–480
Correspondence to:
Vladimir B. Issurin,
Wingate Institute of Physical Education and Sport
Netanya 42902
Phone: 972-528470347
Fax: 972 -8639536
e-mail: v.issurin@gmail.com
... The influence of the RVC on the reaction time has also been investigated. 8,16,26 The possible improvements have been associated with stress reduction on the temporomandibular structure, as well as with better neural transmission, higher blood flow and an increased oxygen unloading in other areas of the head and neck. 26 Additionally, Issurin and Verbitsky 8 suggested that factors such as mental concentration, and a more favourable emotional state can also contribute to these motor effects. ...
... Also, nonsignificant differences were found in the 15-m freestyle. These findings are in contrast with Issurin et al. 8 who found significant ergogenic effects due to jaw clenching combined with abdominal contraction in the start reaction time (average difference ¼ 0.05 s, p ¼ 0.003) and in the 15-m distance (average difference ¼ 0.08 s, p ¼ 0.013). The authors supported that the possible effects of CAP such as cognitive input, beneficial mental concentration and a more favourable emotional state might be the main contributors to these motor effects. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background/Objectives The potential advantages of wearing customized bite-aligning mouthguards on several performance parameters such as muscular strength, power and reaction time have been reported. Literature shows that the concurrent activation potentiation phenomenon, elicited by a powered and balanced jaw clenching, can provide athletes with several neuromuscular advantages. The aim of the present study was to investigate the acute effects of jaw clenching while wearing a customized bite-aligning mouthguard on swimming start, countermovement jump and swim bench test, in contrast to two other conditions: non-jaw clenching and jaw clenching without mouthguard. Methods A randomized, repeated measure within study design was used to compare the condition effect on eight highly trained elite male and female swimmers. Results Statistical analysis revealed a significant increase in the countermovement jump height (p = 0.041) when comparing the use of mouthguards with the non-jaw condition. In the swim bench, a significant greater time to peak force (p = 0.049) was found when comparing the use of mouthguards with the jaw condition. Although, non-significant effects, small differences were found in the start reaction time and 15-m freestyle swimming when comparing the use of mouthguards with the non-jaw condition. Conclusion This study demonstrated that wearing customized, bite-aligning mouthguards had an ergogenic effect on specific measures of vertical jump and swim bench test, whereas non-meaningful but small differences were found in swimming start.
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated the possible contributing effect of oral motor function on maximal and explosive grip force production characteristics. Fourteen healthy male subjects (age 22.5 +/- 2.1 years) were asked to exert maximal explosive grip strength with their dominant hands under the following four conditions: 1. Teeth clenching before and during grip strength exertion (C-C), 2. Teeth clenching before grip strength exertion and mandibular resting position during grip strength exertion (C-R), 3. Mandibular resting position before grip strength exertion and teeth clenching during grip strength exertion (R-C), and 4. Mandibular resting position before and during grip strength exertion (R-R). Maximal force (maxF), average force for every 0.1 s (aveF), maximal rate of force development (maxRFD) and time required to reach 90% of maxF (T 90% max) were analyzed for 1 s from the onset of grip force production. MaxF under C-C and R-C were significantly greater than that under R-R by 12.1% and 12.3%, respectively. AveF under C-C was significantly greater by 10.0-41.2% than that under R-R for all ten periods. AveF under C-R was significantly larger by 9.8-19.0% than that under R-R conditions from 0 to 0.4 s. Compared with under R-R conditions, maxRFD under C-C and C-R increased by 15.8% and 8.5%, respectively, and T 90% max under C-C, C-R and R-C decreased by 22.3%, 12.3% and 12.8%, respectively. These findings suggest that oral motor functions such as teeth clenching may influence not only maximal grip strength generation but also the rapidity of grip force production.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Two concurrent swimming start techniques-Track start (TS) and Grab start (GS), are presently particularly popular among swimmers and researchers. Despite the relatively large amount of publications, the outcome as to which is remains inconclusive. The present study investigates those start techniques based on the outcome of the Sydney Olympic Games. There analyzed were the start reaction (1), the start efficiency-the time on the 15m mark (2), and the start mode – track or grab start technique (3). The final and semifinal performances in all strokes beside backstroke were analyzed and statistically compared by t-test with reference to the start technique, gender effect and whether distance had any influence. The outcome of the analysis revealed start technique particularities related to start reaction and efficiency, distance and gender specificity. The advantage of TS with regards to the start reaction was noted in major part of events. When start efficiency in TS was compared to GS there was an improvement in major part of events and in five events it was significantly higher. Key words: olympic performance, start mode, start reaction and start efficiency.
Article
Pseudofactorialism is defined as ‘the invalid statistical analysis that results from the misidentification of two or more response variables as representing different levels of an experimental variable or treatment factor. Most often the invalid analysis consists of use of an (n + 1)‐way anova in a situation where two or more n‐way anovas would be the appropriate approach’. I and my students examined a total of 1362 papers published from the 1960s to 2009 reporting manipulative experiments, primarily in the field of ecology. The error was present in 7% of these, including 9% of 80 experimental papers examined in 2009 issues of Ecology and the Journal of Animal Ecology. Key features of 60 cases of pseudofactorialism are tabulated as a basis for discussion of the varied ways and circumstances in which the error can occur. As co‐authors, colleagues, editors and anonymous referees and editors who approved them for publication, a total of 459 persons other than the senior authors shared responsibility for these 60 papers. Pseudofactorialism may sometimes be motivated by a desire to test whether different response variables respond in the same way to treatment factors. Proper procedures for doing that are briefly reviewed. A major cause of pseudofactorialism is the widespread failure in statistics texts, primary literature and documentation for statistics software packages to distinguish the three major components of experimental design – treatment structure, design structure, response structure – and clearly define key terms such as experimental unit, evaluation unit, split unit, factorial and repeated measures. A quick way to check for the possible presence of the pseudofactorialism is to determine whether the number of valid experimental units in a study is smaller than (i) the error degrees of freedom in a multi‐way anova; or (ii) the total number of tallies (N) in a multi‐way contingency table. Such situations also can indicate the commission of pseudoreplication, however.
Article
Concurrent activation potentiation enhances muscular force during open kinetic chain isometric and isokinetic exercises via remote voluntary contractions (RVCs). The purpose of this study was to evaluate the effect of RVCs on the performance of closed kinetic chain ground-based exercises. Subjects included 13 men (21.4+/-1.5 years) who performed the back squat and jump squat in 2 test conditions. The RVC condition included performing the test exercises while clenching the jaw on a mouth guard, forcefully gripping and pulling the barbell down into the trapezius, and performing a Valsalva maneuver. The normal condition (NO-RVC) included performing the test exercises without RVCs. Exercises were assessed with a force platform. Peak ground reaction force (GRF), rate of force development (RFD) during the first 100 milliseconds (RFD-100), RFD to peak GRF (RFD-P), and jump squat height (JH) were calculated from the force-time records. Data were analyzed using an analysis of variance. Results reveal that GRF and RFD-100 were higher in the RVC compared with the NO-RVC condition for both the back squat and jump squat (p<or=0.05). The RFD-P was higher in the RVC compared with NO-RVC condition for the jump squat (p<or=0.05) but not for the back squat (p=0.82). The JH was higher in the RVC compared to the NO-RVC condition for the jump squat (p<or=0.05). This study demonstrates that RVCs enhance the performance of closed kinetic chain exercises for most of the outcome variables assessed, yielding a 2.9-32.3% greater performance. Practitioners should encourage athletes to use RVCs to improve the acute training effect of exercises such as those used in this study.
Article
This study evaluated the effect of remote voluntary contractions(RVC) on concentric isokinetic knee extensor and flexor peak torque, rate of torque development, power, and work, the activation of the affected muscles, and gender differences therein. Eleven men and 12 women were evaluated with EMG and isokinetic dynamometry during knee extension and flexion tests in RVC and baseline (NO-RVC) test conditions. The RVC condition included jaw clenching, hand gripping, and the Valsalva maneuver. A two-way mixed ANOVA with repeated measures for test condition was used to evaluate the main effects for each isokinetic measure, as well as the EMG of the prime movers, their antagonist,and the muscles involved in the RVC, and the interaction between test condition and gender. Significant interactions between test condition and gender indicate differences in response to RVC during knee extension tests for power and work (P < or = 0.05) and for knee flexion tests for peak torque and power (P < or = 0.05). All subjects produced higher peak torque and power during knee extension in the RVC condition (P < or = 0.05). Men produced a higher rate of torque development and work during knee extension (P < or = 0.05) and a higher peak torque and power during knee flexion in the RVC condition (P < or = 0.05). Prime mover activation was greater in the RVC condition for most tests (P < or = 0.05). Women demonstrated lower bilateral flexor digitorum superficialis activation than men during all tests in the RVC condition (P < or = 0.05). RVC increased the performance of several outcome variables assessed, which coincides with the concomitant increase in EMG of the prime movers.
Article
The study assessed the effect of current activation potentiation by evaluating jaw clenching and its effect on the rate of force development (RFD), time to peak force (TTPF), and peak force (PF) during the countermovement jump. Fourteen subjects performed the countermovement jump on a force platform while maximally clenching their jaw on a dental vinyl mouthguard (JAW) as well as without clenching their jaw by jumping with an open mouth (NON-JAW). Results reveal that the RFD was 19.5% greater in the JAW compared with the NON-JAW condition (p < 0.05). The TTPF was 20.15% less in the JAW compared with the NON-JAW condition (p < 0.05). There were no significant differences (p = 0.60) in PF between the JAW and NON-JAW conditions. These findings indicate that concurrent activation potentiation is manifested through jaw clenching during the countermovement jump. As a result, athletes may employ this strategy of maximally clenching their jaws to gain an ergogenic advantage during the countermovement jump.
Article
Modulation of the human soleus H-reflex (Sol. H-R) was studied during voluntary teeth clenching in 11 healthy adult volunteers, to investigate the possible correlation of motor functions between the jaw and body. The following results were obtained: (1) Sol. H-R was facilitated during the maximum voluntary teeth clenching more than during the maximum voluntary efforts in the other parts of the body, i.e., Jendrassik maneuver and clenching of both hands. (2) The amount of the Sol. H-R facilitation increased with the masseter EMG activity. (3) Facilitation of the Sol. H-R in association with teeth clenching started ca 60ms before the onset of the masseter EMG activity, reached its peak ca. 100ms after the onset and thereafter decreased to a certain plateau level in accordance with the masseter EMG activity level. (4) During the inhibition of the masseter EMG reflexively evoked by electrical stimulation of the lower lip, the amount of the facilitation of the Sol. H-R during teeth clenching decreased compared with that without lip stimulation, though the facilitation was still retained. It was concluded that: 1) The excitability of the Sol. H-R is remarkably elevated during teeth clenching. 2) There is a positive correlation between the amount of the facilitation of the Sol. H-R and the teeth clenching force level. 3) The facilitation is due to both the descending influence from the higher brain and the afferent inputs from the oral structures. It is assumed that the oral motor activity exerts a strong influence on the bodily motor function in general.
Article
The facilitation of the tendon reflex of quadriceps, induced by voluntary contraction of a muscle group in the upper limb, evolves in the three phases and depends on several factos. These include the strength and type (ballistic or ramp) of the contraction. In the absence of any electromyographic activity in the conditioning muscle (from a lesion of its nerve trunk or by blockage of conduction by injection of xylocaine), a voluntary attempt to contract it causes only a moderate facilitation of the quadriceps motor nucleus; while reflex activation of the conditioning muscles, by vibration or sudden stretching. causes a more marked facilitation. Thus, it is concluded that at least two distinct mechanisms act sequentially. First, a general motor facilitation of supraspinal origin induces the first moderate phase of facilitations (phase I). This early facilitation precedes the onset of electromyographic activity in the conditioning muscle. Secondly, the more marked facilitation (phase II) is related to the stimulation of afferents originating from the conditioning muscles, either by fusimotor excitation in voluntary contraction or by manoeuvres which stimulate the spindle receptors mechanically. To investigate the transmission pathways of these facilitations, the latencies of these two phases were compared at two distant motor nuclei, the trigeminal motor nucleus and the quadriceps nucleus, following voluntary contractions of the tibialis anterior. Since the facilitations of the trigeminal motor nucleus precede those of the quadriceps motor nucleus, a slowly-travelling rostro-caudal facilitation is postulated during phase II when the facilitation is related to afferents coming from the contracting muscle. A long loop, therefore, is postulated to interpret the second phase of the facilitation curves.
Article
To investigate the relationship between teeth clenching in intercuspal position and isometric shoulder adduction movement in 12 normal subjects, the muscle strength and electromyogram (EMG) of the seven muscles concerned were measured by using Cybex 6000 Extremity Testing and Rehabilitation System and surface EMG analyzing system. Normalized peak torque (n-PT) and normalized average torque (n-AT) were analyzed to find muscle strength. Normalized integrated EMG (n-iEMG) was analyzed to find muscle activity. The results were as follows: 1. For n-AT, the value in 100%MVC significantly increased by 5.4% as compared with that in the rest position (RP). 2. There was a significant positive correlation between biting force of intercuspal position (ICP), 20%MVC, 50%MVC and 100%MVC and muscle strength of n-PT and n-AT. 3. For n-iEMG, the value of latissimus dorsi, teres major, triceps brachii and forearm extensor muscles in 100%MVC significantly increased by 8.4%, 13.0%, 9.3% and 18.9% respectively as compared with that in RP. 4. There was a significant positive correlation between biting force of intercuspal position (ICP), 20%MVC, 50%MVC and 100%MVC and muscle activity of pectralis major, latissimus dorsi, teres major, biceps brachii, triceps brachii, forearm extensor and forearm flexor muscles.
Article
1. To determine the effects of motor activity in the jaw on more general motor behavior, we examined the modulation of the soleus H reflex in association with voluntary clenching of the teeth in healthy adult volunteers. 2. The amplitude of the H reflex increased remarkably during teeth clenching. The increase during maximal teeth clenching was greater than that induced either by maximal voluntary isometric contraction of the wrist extensors or by maximal voluntary clenching of the fists. 3. The increase in amplitude of the soleus H reflex showed a positive correlation with the strength of teeth clenching, as monitored by recording electromyographic (EMG) activity from the masseter muscle. 4. The increase in amplitude of the H reflex associated with teeth clenching started before the onset of the EMG activity of the masseter muscle, reached a peak shortly after the onset, and then declined to a plateau that continued in parallel with the masseter EMG until the end of teeth clenching. 5. The increase in amplitude of the H reflex in association with teeth clenching was partially decreased during the inhibitory masseteric reflex evoked by innocuous electrical stimulation of the lip. 6. It is concluded that the soleus H reflex is facilitated in association with voluntary teeth clenching, and that descending influences from the cerebral cortex, as well as peripheral afferent impulses from the oral-facial region, are involved in this facilitation. It is proposed that oral motor activity can exert strong influences on the motor activity of the other parts of the body.