Content uploaded by Maria Pąchalska
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Maria Pąchalska on Aug 30, 2014
Content may be subject to copyright.
SUMMARY
The aim of this study was to determine whether relative beta neu-
rofeedback training (RBNT), applied to regulate brain dynamics,
would be useful for a patient with benign partial epilepsy with
Rolandic Spikes (BPERS), accompanied by symptoms of attention
deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).
The patient, AG, age 8:2, presented with neuropsychiatric symp-
toms, cognitive dysfunction (especially attention deficits), and be-
havioral disorders, rendering her unable to function independently
in many home, preschool and school situations. She was being
treated for epilepsy, without apparent progress. Forty sessions of
RBNT were applied to regulate the dynamics of frontal lobe func-
tioning. In standardized neuropsychological testing and ERPs be-
fore and after the neurotherapy program, there were multiple cog ni ti
ve
and neurobehavioral deficits at baseline, along with Rolandic
spikes. After RBNT, there was a statistically significant reduction of
slow activity frontally and a considerable lowering of the theta:beta
ratio. These changes were accompanied by decreased reaction
time and fewer omission errors in the cued GO/NOGO task. The
post-training changes in EEG spectra were also accompanied by
complete cessation of Rolandic spikes in resting EEG. AG’s verbal
and non-verbal IQ also increased significantly, and her cognitive
functions improved, including immediate and delayed logical and
visual recall on the WMS-III, attention on the WMS-III, and execu-
tive functions.
RBNT was effective in the treatment of this patient with BPERS,
accompanied by ADHD symptoms. ERPs in a GO/NOGO task can
be used to assess functional brain changes induced by neurother-
apeutic programs.
Key words: ERPs, ADHD, attention problems, graphomotor deficit,
behavioral problems
Background:
Case study:
Conclusions:
AN EVALUATION OF PERSONALIZED
NEUROFEEDBACK TRAINING
FOR ROLANDIC EPILEPSY: A CASE STUDY
Andrzej Mirski1(A,B,D,E,F), Maria Pachalska1,2(A,B,C,D,E,F),
Natalia Mirska1(A,B,E), Olga Jauer Niworowska3(A,B,D,E),
Kinga Gryglicka1(A,B,E), Iurii D. Kropotov1,4,5(A,B,C,D,E)
1 Department of Neuropsychology, Andrzej Frycz-Modrzewski Krakow University,
Krakow, Poland
2 Center for Cognition and Communication, New York, NY
3Institute of Assisted Human Development and Education, The Maria
Grzegorzewska Academy of Special Education, Warsaw, Poland
4Laboratory of the Institute of the Human Brain of Russian Academy of Sciences,
St. Petersburg, Russia
5Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway
CASE STUDY
ACTA
ACTA
Vol. 12, No. 3, 2014, 271-287
NEUROPSYCHOLOGICA
NEUROPSYCHOLOGICA
Received: 11.01.2014
Accepted: 26.08.2014
A – Study Design
B – Data Collection
C – Statistical Analysis
D – Data Interpretation
E – Manuscript Preparation
F – Literature Search
G – Funds Collection
271
INTRODUCTION
Many different kinds of epileptic syndromes are most common in childhood.
Among these is Rolandic epilepsy (RE), which is more and more frequently di-
agnosed by pediatricians and pediatric neurologists [1,2,3,4,5].
According to the 2006 classification published by the International League
Against Epilepsy (ILAE), RE is a developmental epilepsy. The complex genetic
background of this particular type of epilepsy has not yet been fully explained
[4,6,7,8]. The presence of centrotemporal spikes (CTS) in the EEG is pathogno-
monic [4,5,7,8].
RE is also designated as an idiopathic epilepsy syndrome. It has been de-
scribed in the literature as “benign Rolandic epilepsy in children” (BREC) or “be-
nign epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes” (BECTS) [9,10]. The term “Rolandic”
stems from the fact that the seizures begin in the Rolandic region of the brain.
These are classified as “partial” seizures, since only this one part of the brain is
involved [11,12,13].
RE usually manifests between the ages of 3 and 10 years, and often resolves
in adolescence (14-18 years) [14,15]. It is considered a “benign” epilepsy pre-
cisely because the prognosis is positive: nearly all children with RE grow out of
it as they mature. It occurs in almost one out of every five children with epilepsy,
which means that it is one of the most common types of childhood epilepsy. Boys
and girls are equally affected.
Some children with RE function relatively well and display no learning diffi-
culties, though some may have special difficulties with reading and/or writing, or
with drawing and other visuospatial skills [8,10,16,17,18]. The great majority of
children with RE, however, display symptoms similar to Attention Deficit Hyper-
activity Disorder (ADHD) [4,5,18,19]. These symptoms may be associated with
a neuropsychiatric syndrome [9], though there has been relatively little attention
given in the literature to this co-occurence of syndromes [5,20].
There have been reports of a close connection between benign partial
epilepsy with Rolandic spikes (BPERS) and acquired epileptic aphasia (Landau-
Kleffner syndrome), which was the first example of a primarily “cognitive”
epilepsy in children [8,18]. Later, some children were diagnosed with persistent
but reversible oral-motor deficits during the active phase of RE [18].
With further research, it has become apparent that this type of epilepsy can
cause long-term “epileptic” deficits [20]. There has been some neuropsycholog-
ical research supported clinical observations that children with BPERS display
normal intelligence, but a larger percentage have attention disorder and other
deficits (linguistic, visuospatial, etc.) when compared to a control group [4,5,18,
20]. In the last decade or so, research on transient cognitive and behavioral dis-
turbances occurring in children with BPERS has suggested that these symptoms
are directly related to the epilepsy [18,20,21]. Though studies of this kind have
been infrequent, some longitudinal neuropsychological research has recently in-
dicated that acquired transient cognitive and behavioral symptoms correlate in
272
Mirski A. et al. Neurofeedback training for Rolandic epilepsy
some children with epileptic activity in the EEG [4,5,8,18,20], which may explain
some of the learning and school-related problems experienced by these children
when they are in the active phase of the syndrome [5]. Many of these studies
have found deviations from the norm in categories similar to those of the very
familiar pathophysiological patterns in the population of children with ADHD, such
as an increased theta-beta ratio [18,22], a decrease in the P3b component
[4,14,15,18], and a decrease in the P3 component in a NOGO task [4,20,21].
Quantitative analysis has also been used to analyze the ERP data without any
a priori decisions about peak performance, amplitude, or latency. It turned out
that the frequency of Rolandic spikes in children with ADHD is much higher than
epidemiological research would predict [23]. ERPs have also shown much
greater amplitudes in persons with epilepsy in the frontal and central regions
during the temporal window between 250 and 425 ms after stimulus, which cor-
responds with the temporal window of differentiation between target and non-
target stimuli [8,10,13].
Yet another question raised by researchers is the efficacy of therapy for chil-
dren with RE. Promising results have been obtained by applying carefully chosen
neurotherapy programs. Our previous research has shown that “the cognitive
deficits characteristic for ADHD in a child with BPERS may be unresponsive to
antiepileptic treatment, but are reversible after a carefully selected neurotherapy
program, combined with antiepileptic treatment” [5]. Relative beta neurofeedback
training (RBNT), applied to regulate the dynamics of frontal lobe cortical function
by increasing beta waves, has shown promise in reducing epileptic spikes
around the Rolandic fissure [24].
The next question involves the connection, if any, between the symptoms of
ADHD and the presence of Rolandic spikes in this subgroup of the ADHD pop-
ulation, and how these symptoms could be treated using RBNT. In what follows,
a case that addresses this question will be presented.
1. The purpose of the present study was to determine:
• whether or not a child with BPERS, accompanied by symptoms of attention
deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), presents with:
• a specific developmental deficit in learning or, more generally, disturbances
of cognitive and executive functions;
• variant patterns of cortical activation while performing operations in working
memory in comparison to children without epileptic seizures;
2. whether or not RBNT, applied to regulate the dynamics of frontal lobe cortex
function by increasing beta waves, registered with an electrode placed in the
frontal region, can help to:
• alleviate the disturbances of cognitive and executive functions;
• reduce the number of spikes
• alleviate the syndrome of neuropsychiatric disturbances and cognitive and
executive dysfunctions characteristic for ADHD;
• improve the patient’s independent functioning in school.
Mirski A. et al. Neurofeedback training for Rolandic epilepsy
273
CASE HISTORY
Female patient AG was first examined when she was six-and-a-half years old.
Neuropsychiatric symptoms had been occurring for some time, along with dis-
turbances of cognitive and executive functions, especially attention deficits. This
was making it difficult for her to function independently in daily life, as well as in
the preschool setting (she changed preschools three times) and in school. Since
AG’s problems with learning new material were considerable, so that she was
basically unable to learn anything, her teachers suggested that she should be
transferred to a special school.
After neuropsychiatric and neuropsychological consultations, AG was diag-
nosed with ADHD, according to DSM-IV-TR diagnostic criteria, and she initially
received neuropsychological therapy for her attention problems. In her mother’s
opinion, these disturbances could be attributed to an intensification of the symp-
toms of a particularly troublesome neuropsychiatric syndrome diagnosed by a
psychiatrist, which had persisted since her first seizure at the age of 3. The symp-
toms consisted primarily in behavioral disturbances, such as impulsivity and ag-
gressiveness, along with defiance (i.e. refusal to follow parental instruction).
The symptoms began to intensify, with stronger nocturnal “episodes” [5], until
the development of active epilepsy, which slowed the normal course of AG’s de-
velopment. The seizures were simple, partial, motor and sensory, and involved
the lower part of the face and the throat. Tremors most often occurred during
sleep (immediately before falling asleep and just before waking up in the morn-
ing), and involved mostly an upper or lower limb, sometimes the entire half of
the body. The seizures sometimes began when AG awoke at night or in the morn-
ing; she would experience a prickling sensation (something like needles) at one
corner of her mouth, involving also the tongue, the lips, and the inside of her
cheek. There were also automatisms, consisting in lateral movements of the jaw.
She usually cried out or wept, and could not be comforted. At times the seizures
involved the throat as well, causing slurred and nearly incomprehensible speech.
She would produce odd throaty, gurgling sounds, and in this way she often
alarmed her parents that something bad was happening. She usually knew what
she wanted to say at such times, but could not pronounce the words properly.
AG was also observed to have increasing and serious problems with drawing
and writing. The writing disorders were diagnosed as an acquired, isolated
graphomotor deficit, an example of a selective “epileptic” developmental deficit
(see Fig. 1A).
Despite antiseizure mediation with carbamazepine (Tegretol), her condition
worsened, and she exhibited a dramatic loss of skills already acquired (see Fig.
2). This situation could be explained, however, in terms of the psychological and
neuropsychological sequelae of the epilepsy [23,24,25,26].
Mirski A. et al. Neurofeedback training for Rolandic epilepsy
274
Mirski A. et al. Neurofeedback training for Rolandic epilepsy
275
Fig. 1. Writing samples from AG, a female patient diagnosed with RE, at the age of 6.5 years. In B
and C, the patient was asked to write from dictation the Polish sentence “Mama gotuje obiad,”
which means “Mom is cooking dinner,” and then to write the date beneath. [Please note that the
European convention is used: DD/MM/YY, not the American MM/DD/YY]: a). writing sample at the
age of 6:5, indecipherable scribble; b). at baseline, age 7:1: acquired, isolated graphomotor deficit,
an example of selective “epileptic” development disorders; mixing of upper and lower case letters,
rotation; c). at followup (after 40 sessions of neurotherapy), age 8:2: correct writing. Date of exa-
mination corrected by AG spontaneously: initially, she said the wrong date aloud (April 23), but
quickly corrected to the right date, August 3
Fig. 2. Acquired, isolated graphomotor deficit in AG at the age of 6:5, an example of selective “epi-
leptic” development problems. Retardation of the process of development. Cognitive and executive
dysfunctions at age 7:1. Significant recovery of disturbed cognitive and exectuve functions at age
8:2. From: [5], with modifications
THE NEUROTHERAPY PROGRAM
Based on the data given above, a neurotherapy program was administered
to AG, involving 40 sessions of relative beta training, conducted by a neurofeed-
back therapist.
Electrodes were placed at the Fz and Cz points, for bipolar recording. The
goal of the therapy was to increase the activization of the frontal cortex by in-
creasing beta activity in the EEG in the combined theta and alpha frequency
bands. The beta frequency band was in the range from 4 to 12 Hz, and the com-
bined theta and alpha bands were in the same range. During the first four ses-
sions, neurofeedback training was administered daily for 30 seconds. In sub se
quent
sessions, the duration was gradually increased, as the patient’s capacity in-
creased. The last 20 sessions of neurofeedback lasted for about 20 minutes [26].
AG was evaluated twice, using a comprehensive diagnostic protocol (psycho-
metric and neurometric). The baseline examination was performed in an active
period of the epilepsy, before the application of neurotherapy, when the patient’s
age was 7:1, while the followup examination took place after 40 sessions of neu-
rotherapy, at the age of 8:2.
In neuropsychological examination, standard neuropsychological batteries
were used, including clinical observation, a structured interview, the Wechsler
Memory Scale (WMS III, Polish version), the Wechsler Children’s Intelligence
Test (Polish version), the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST, Polish version for
children), the Trail-Making Test (TMT) and Pachalska’s Semantic Figure Test for chil-
dren [26]. For the neurophysiological testing, we used EEG, quantitative electroen-
cephalography (QEEG), and event-related potentials (ERPs). The patient’s EEG and
ERP spectra with inter-seizure Rolandic spikes were compared to a normative base
(the HBI data base) in order to evaluate AG’s neurophysiological deficits.
The experiment was consulted with and approved by the local ethics commitee.
The parents gave written consent to the anonymous publication of AG’s history.
RESULTS
Neuropsychological results
Table 1 shows the results of neuropsychological testing. The baseline exam-
ination showed disturbances of cognitive and executive functions. In the follow-up
examination, AG showed considerable improvement in all tested neuropsy -
chological functions. The greatest improvement was seen in the verbal and non-
verbal IQ (WCIS), concentration (WMS III), visuospatial functions (WMS III),
immediate and delayed logical memory (WMS III), and executive functions
(WCST), where at baseline, as Table 1 shows, there were errors in every cate-
gory (Table 1). Of particular interest is the disappearance of perseveration, which
was no longer present in either drawing or writing (cf. Figs. 1 and 3).
Probably the most interesting result that AG achieved after neurotherapy was
in the domain of memory capacity. In aural learning of verbal material, at baseline
Mirski A. et al. Neurofeedback training for Rolandic epilepsy
276
Mirski A. et al. Neurofeedback training for Rolandic epilepsy
277
Table 1. Results of neuropsychological testing at baseline and follow-up
she remembered only 2 of 12 words after distraction, while after 30 minutes she
had forgotten all of them. At follow-up, she remembered all 12 words, both after
distraction and 30 minutes later. This is an above-average score compared to
the Polish norm, which is 10.
Baseline testing also showed executive dysfunction in drawing a semantic
figure (Fig. 3). At baseline, she was unable to copy the figure (Fig. 3A). She
worked very fast but without precision, and quickly dropped the task. At follow-
up, however, she was able to make a much better copy of the figure (Fig. 3C).
Her writing (cf. Fig. 3C) and reading skills were also improved.
Her successes at school were particularly noteworthy. AG received the high-
est possible marks for a first-grader in all subjects, including conduct. The growth
of her range of interests should also be emphasized, especially her excellent
piano playing.
Mirski A. et al. Neurofeedback training for Rolandic epilepsy
278
Fig. 3. Drawing of Semantic Figure by the female patient AG with benign partial epilepsy with Ro-
landic Spikes (BPERS) and the diagnosis of ADHD. a). The pattern for drawing; b). in the 1st exa-
mination (before neurotherapy); c) in the 2nd examination (after neurotherapy)
Neurophysiological analysis
Event Related Potentials (ERPs)
Event related potentials (ERPs) were used to assess functional changes man-
ifested by the patient after rehabilitation. We used this approach for several rea-
sons. First of all, ERPs have a superior temporal resolution (on the order of
milliseconds) as compared to other imaging methods, such as fMRI and PET
(which have time resolutions of 6 seconds and more) [4]. Secondly, ERPs have
been proven to be a powerful tool for detecting changes induced by neurofeed-
back training in ADHD children [4,19,28,29]. And finally, in contrast to sponta-
neous EEG oscillations, ERPs reflect stages of information flow within the brain
[4,5,18,19,23,24]. The diagnostic power of ERPs has been enhanced by the re-
cent emergence of new methods of analysis, such as Independent Component
Analysis (ICA) and Low Resolution Electromagnetic Tomography (LORETA) [4].
A modification of the visual two-stimulus GO/NO GO paradigm was used (Fig. 4).
T
hree categories of visual stimuli were selected:
• 20 different images of animals, referred to as “A”;
• 20 different images of plants, referred to as “P”;
Mirski A. et al. Neurofeedback training for Rolandic epilepsy
279
Fig. 4. Schematic representation of the two stimulus GO/NOGO task. From top to bottom: time dy-
namics of stimuli in four categories of trials. Abbreviations: A, P, H stimuli are “Animals”, “Plants”
and “Humans” respectively. GO trials occur when A-A stimuli require the subject to press a button.
NOGO trials are A-P stimuli, which require suppression of a prepared action. GO and NOGO trials
represent “continue set,” in which subjects have to prepare for action after the first stimulus pre-
sentation (A). Ignore trials are stimuli pairs beginning with a P, which require no preparation for ac-
tion. Novel trials are pairs requiring no action, with presentation of a novel sound as the second
stimuli. Ignore and Novel trials represent “Discontinue set,” in which subjects do not need to prepare
for action after the first stimulus presentation. Time intervals are depicted at the bottom
• 20 different images of people of different professions, presented along with
an artificial “novel” sound, referred to as “H+Sound”.
All visual stimuli were chosen so as to have a similar size and luminosity. The
randomly varying novel sounds consisted of five 20-ms fragments filled with
tones of different frequencies (500, 1000, 1500, 2000, and 2500 Hz). Each time
a new combination of tones was used, while the novel sounds appeared unex-
pectedly (the probability of appearance was 12.5%).
The trials consisted of presentations of paired stimuli with inter-stimulus in-
tervals of 1 second. The duration of stimuli was 100 ms. Four categories of trials
were used (see Fig. 3): A-A, A-P, P-P, and P-(H+Sound). The trials were grouped
into four blocks with one hundred trials each. In each block a unique set of five
A, five P, and five H stimuli were selected. The subject practiced the task before
the recording started.
The patient sat upright in a comfortable chair looking at a computer screen.
The task was to press a button with the right hand in response to all A-A pairs as
fast as possible, and to withhold pressing the button in response to other pairs:
A-P, P-P, P-(H+Sound) (Fig. 3). According to the task design, two preparatory
sets were distinguished: a “continue set,” in which A is presented as the first stim-
ulus and the subject is presumed to prepare to respond; and a “discontinue set,”
in which P is presented as the first stimulus, and the subject does not need to
prepare to respond. In the “Continue set,” A-A pairs will be referred to as “GO
trials,” A-P pairs as “NO GO trials.” Averages for response latency and response
variance across trials were calculated. Omission errors (failure to respond in GO
trials) and commission errors (failure to suppress a response to NO GO trials)
were also computed.
EEGs were recorded from 19 scalp sites. The electrodes were applied ac-
cording to the International 10-20 system. The EEG was recorded referentially
to linked ears, allowing computational re-referencing of the data (remontaging).
The patient’s performance is shown in Table 2).
The reaction time decreased by 80 ms and the number of omission errors
dropped twice. The ERPs did not change. The most dramatic changes were ob-
served in clinical EEG and EEG spectra (Fig. 5)
Mirski A. et al. Neurofeedback training for Rolandic epilepsy
280
Table 2. Behavioral parameters in the GO/NOGO task.
DISCUSSION
Our study has shown that AG was displaying a specific deficit related to the
epilepsy, and not a generalized dysfunction, since the cognitive symptoms re-
solved along with the disappearance of Rolandic spikes. We did observe variant
patterns of cortical activation during the performance of operations in working
memory, as compared to children without epilepsy.
Rolandic spikes appear in the EEG of children with ADHD at a significantly
higher rate than would be expected from epidemiological studies [27, 28]. The
question of how ADHD symptoms are related to Rolandic spikes in this ADHD
subgroup remains to be answered. On the other hand, many previous EEG stud-
ies have reported an elevated theta-beta ratio in ADHD [29,14,30]. In the present
study, we used the theta-beta ratio as a neurophysiological index of ADHD, and
the number of Rolandic spikes as an index of the neurological status of the brain.
We have shown that the QEEG based neurofeedback sessions in this patient,
intended to increase the frontal beta activity, resulted in the disappearance of
Rolandic spikes and a statistically significant decrease of frontal theta activity,
accompanied by a substantial lowering of the theta-beta ratio.
The first application of EEG based neurofeedback for the treatment of
epilepsy was done by Sterman and colleagues (for a review of these studies see
Mirski A. et al. Neurofeedback training for Rolandic epilepsy
281
Fig. 5. Clinical EEG/EEG spectra changes after neurofeedback treatment. a). EEG spectra diffe-
rences. Red – pre, green – post, black – difference. b). Changes in spike detection. Pre – 12 Ro-
landic spikes were detected during 6 minutes of resting state conditions. Post – no spikes were
detected. c). Reduction in the theta-beta ratio after neurofeedback
[32]). In these studies, a standard SMR (sensory-motor rhythm) training was
used at one or more central sites (C3, Cz and C4).
Although 82% of patients demonstrated more than a 30% reduction in seizures,
complete cessation of seizures has been rare. Walker and Kozlowski [33] were
the first to report on a study of applying QEEG as a guide to neurofeedback train-
ing for people with untreatable seizures. The QEEG findings included one or
more focal slow abnormalities and one or more coherence abnormalities. The
neurofeedback included rewarding inhibition of slow activity in the areas with ex-
cessive slow activity (1–10 Hz) and rewarding an increase in 15–18 Hz at the
same time. Within 20–35 sessions,all the studied patients became seizure-free
and remained so for an average of 7 years.
In the present study we followed this strategy. The19-channel EEG of the pa-
tient was recorded in the resting state with eyes open and eyes closed, the EEG
spectra were computed and compared with the HBI reference data base. We
found increased slow activity frontally in comparison to healthy controls, which
resulted in an elevated theta-beta ratio at the Fz and Cz electrodes. Relative
beta training [23] was suggested as a neurofeedback protocol.
The implementation of a program of neurofeedback in this patient resulted in
a statistically significant reduction of slow activity frontally and substantial low-
ering of the theta-beta ratio. These changes were accompanied by decreased
reaction time and fewer omission errors in the cued GO/NOGO task. The post-
training changes in EEG spectra were also accompanied by complete cessation
of Rolandic spikes in the resting state EEG.
The present study thus supports the effectiveness of RBNT for mitigating the
symptoms of both ADHD and epilepsy. In our opinion, however, the question
about the possibility that Rolandic spikes (or rather the neurological abnormalities
that lead to appearance of these spikes in EEG) are actually the cause of ADHD
in this patient still remains to be answered, despite the therapeutic success.
How should we interpret the spectacular improvement achieved in this case,
especially as pertains to memory capacity, which is of fundamental importance
for learning?
In microgenetic theory, as in other theories of working memory, the primary
challenge is to solve two basic problems:
1. How long does information remain in working memory before it is either trans-
ferred to long-term memory to be available for later recall, or forgotten?
2. How much information can be processed simultaneously in working memory?
The history of various efforts to answer these questions, based on theoretical
reflection and/or experimental research, is long and complex. For the present
purposes, it will suffice to say that these questions remain very much open [26].
It is difficult, but not impossible, to combine these two problems into a single
concept. In the multilevel process (beginning with peripheral receptors and end-
ing in the cortex) of sifting through the enormous mass of information that comes
to the senses, we choose primarily those stimuli which have some meaning for
us. This protects our brain from overload, and makes it possible to construct a
Mirski A. et al. Neurofeedback training for Rolandic epilepsy
282
relatively stable picture of the world. Originally, the selectivity of attention served
the purposes of biological survival; now, it makes it possible for us to function in
a complicated social environment.
In the synchronic model of memory [26], developed according to the principles
of process neuropsychology and presented in Fig. 6, the attempt has been made
to determine, on the basis of neuropsychological research, the functional princi-
ples for a new clinical approach, based on process theory (i.e. microgenetic the-
ory) and evolutionary-genetic theory [34]. This model does not presume that
there exists an absolute cause-and-effect relation, but at the same time it does
not deny the possibility of rigorous scientific research. It does presume, however,
that there is an overarching connecting principle among the phenomena involved
in explicit and implicit memory, operating alongside the principle of causation.
Synchronicity is defined as the appearance in parallel lines of two (or several) phe-
nomena, event, or mental states that have, for the observer, a common meaning,
though they are not linked causally. According to some scientists, synchronicity
can be offered as an alternative to complete randomness [35,36,37,38].
The synchronic model of memory is derived from the holographic model of
the universe, which, according to Peat [36,37], reflects the synchronicity of reality.
The spatial arrangement of the model enables it to present, on the xand yaxes,
the relation between the general structure of the attention and memory systems
in terms of the number, content, and complexity of the items being processed,
and the time needed to process them. It is assumed, as in Pribram’s concept
[35], that thanks to the change of the angle at which bursts radiating from two
lasers affect a photographic image, it is possible to keep many different images
on the same surface.
The synchronic pattern of the model (the dotted line) in turn reflects the holo-
graphic interference of waves, corresponding to what goes on in the brain: the
pulsing of mental states and changes in neuronal connections (including new
connections arising in brain tissue).
In this model, consciousness and self-awareness have been represented by
a separate circle, since these are prerequisites for the normal course of cognitive
processes (including memory) and emotional processes. The outer (yellow) spi-
ral refers to the fractal concept of consciousness and self-awareness in relation
to mind, and to the synchronic image of reality formed by the self in relation to
the world and the universe.
The tunnels through which the small spheres are swimming represent the var-
ious kinds of working and long-term memory, thanks to which the conscious self
forms its own synchronic reality in the relation between the self, the world, and
the universe [26]. Thanks to plasticity and new connections in the brain, there
appears a kind of dependency between events, in which every causal connection
is possible. The large yellow circles are buffers:
• attention;
• working memory;
Mirski A. et al. Neurofeedback training for Rolandic epilepsy
283
• long-term memory;
• perception.
According to data recently obtained from neurophysiological research, the at-
tention system buffers the transmission of data to the working memory system.
The attention system processes the smallest number of elements in the shortest
time (seconds, even milliseconds). When the number of elements being pro -
cessed and/or the processing time exceeds a certain threshold, there is a gradual
transition from the attention system (a few stimuli, a few milliseconds) to the
working memory system (a dozen or more stimuli, several milliseconds, seconds,
or minutes), depending on the capacity of the working memory buffer.
The transition from working memory to long-term memory takes place in a si -
milar fashion. The boundary for transition is difficult to establish precisely, and in
reality it is probably rather blurred. In the human brain there is a constant
process, lasting from milliseconds to whole years, when information is committed
to memory, stored, recalled, and forgotten.
Mirski A. et al. Neurofeedback training for Rolandic epilepsy
284
Fig. 6. The synchronic model of memory. The spatial arrangement of the model is based on a ho-
logram. Interpretation: x axis: the number, content, and complexity of the elements a given system
must use. y axis: the time needed to process these elements. The synchronic pattern of the spiral
(dotted line): pulsing of mental states and changes in neuronal connections (including new ones).
The tunnels through which the small spheres are flowing: different kinds of working and long-term
memory. Large yellow circles: buffers for attention, working memory, long-term memory, and per-
ception. Outer (yellow) spiral: the fractal concept of consciousness and self-awareness in relation
to the mind and the synchronic image of reality created by the self
From: Pachalska, Kaczmarek & Kropotov [26], with permission
The longest duration of storage is naturally provided by long-term memory,
which is why we have placed it near the base of the model. Memory (both retro-
spective and prospective) is closely associated with the creation of a model of
the world, thanks to the perception buffer placed at the very base of the model.
Without a properly functioning memory system, there is no properly functioning
perception system, or other cognitive processes. It is also essential to note that
in the process of perception, the feeling that an object exists and that it belongs
to a primitive functional category precedes the awareness of its particular per-
ceptual features [26,34].
On the edges of the model of memory, outside the circle of consciousness
and self-consciousness, is unawareness and amnesia. It requires a major com-
mitment of brain resources to recover information from this domain.
This model makes it easier (though admittedly not easy) to understand the
improvement AG displayed as a result of neurotherapy. Thanks to the reduction
of the Rolandic spikes and the regulation of the brain’s electrical activity, the
management of brain resources was improved. As a result, the attention and
working memory buffers were expanded, which in turn caused new information
to become available to long-term memory. The final effects of this could be seen
in AG’s spectacular results, not only in neuropsychological testing (including tests
for memory storage capacity), but also in her very gratifying achievements, both
at school and at home, especially her piano playing skills.
CONCLUSIONS
RBNT was successful in the treatment of a patient with benign partial epilepsy
with Rolandic Spikes (BPERS), accompanied by the symptoms of attention
deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Event Related Potentials (ERPs) in the
GO/NOGO task can be used to assess functional brain changes induced by neu-
rotherapeutic programs.
REFERENCES
Holtmann, M., Becker, K., Kentner-Figura, B. & Schmidt, M.H. Increased frequency of rolandic
spikes in ADHD children. Epilepsia, 2003; 44, 1241-1244.
Bishop DVM, Snowling MJ. Developmental dyslexia an specific language impairment: same or
different? 2004; Psychol Bull 130:858–886.
Kramer U: Atypical Presentations of Benign Childhood Epilepsy With Centrotemporal Spikes:
A Review. J Child Neurol July 2008; 23(7) 785-790.
Kropotov JD: Quantitative EEG, event related potentials and neurotherapy. San Diego: Academic
Press, Elsevier, 2009.
Pąchalska M., Kropotov I.D., Mańko G., Lipowska M., Rasmus A, Łukaszewska B., Bogdanowicz
M., Mirski A: Evaluation of a neurotherapy program for a child with ADHD with Benign Partial
Epilepsy with Rolandic Spikes (BPERS) using event-related potentials. Medical Science Mo-
nitor. 2012; 18(11) CS 94-104.
Bali B, Kugler SL, Pal DK: Genetic influence on Rolandic epilepsy. Ann Neurol, 2005; 57(3): 464–65.
Chahine LM, Mikati MA: Benign pediatric localization-related epilepsies. 2006; Epileptic Disord 8
(4): 243–58.
Mirski A. et al. Neurofeedback training for Rolandic epilepsy
285
Clarke T, Strug LJ, Murphy PL et al: High risk of reading disability and speech sound disorder in
rolandic epilepsy families: case – control study.Epilepsia, 2007; 48(12): 2258–65.
Deonna T, Zesiger P, Davidoff V et al: Benign partial epilepsy of childhood: a neuropsychological
and EEG study of cognitive function. Dev Med Child Neurol, 2000; 42: 595–603.
Huang-Pollock CL, Nigg JT, Halperin JM: Single dissociation findings of ADHD deficits in vigilance
but not anterior or posterior attention systems. 2006; Neuropsychology 20:420–429.
Pan A, Luders HO: Epileptiform discharges in benign focal epilepsy of childhood. Epileptic Disord,
2000; 2S1: 29–36
de Saint-Martin AD, Carcangiu R, Arzimanoglou A et al: Semiology of typical and atypical rolandic
eplepsy: a video-EEG analysis. Epileptic Disord, 2001; 3: 173–82.
Datta A, Sinclair DB. Benign epilepsy of childhood with rolandic spikes: typical and atypical vari-
ants. Pediatr Neurol 2007 March;36(3):141-5
Monastra, V. J., Lubar, J. F., Linden, M., VanDeusen, P., Green, G., Wing, W., & Fenger, T. N: As-
sessing attention deficit hyperactivity disorder via quantitative electroencephalography: An
initial validation study. Neuropsychology, 1999; 13, 424-433.
Pan A, Luders HO: Epileptiform discharges in benign focal epilepsy ofchildhood. Epileptic Disord,
2000; 2S1: 29–36.
Mayor C, Zesiger P, Roulet-Perez E et al: Acquired epileptic dysgraphia: a longitudinal study. Dev
Med Child Neurol, 2003; 45: 807–12.
Chaix Y, Laguitton V, Lauwers-Cancès V, Daquin G, Cancès C, Démonet J.F, Villeneuve N: Read-
ing abilities and cognitive functions of children with epilepsy: Influence of epileptic syndrome.
Brain & Development 28 2006;122–130.
Barry RJ, Clarke AR, McCarthy R et al: Event-related potentials in adults with Attention-Deficit/Hy-
peractivity Disorder: an investigation using an inter-modal auditory/visual oddball task. Int J
Psychophysiol, 2009; 71(2): 124–31.
Kropotov JD, Muller A: What can event related potentials contribute to neuropsychology. Acta Neu-
ropsychologica, 2009; 7(3): 169–81.
Garcia-Penas JJ: Interictal epileptiform discharges and cognitive impairment in children. Revista
de Neurologia, 2011; 52(Suppl.1): S43–52.
Mayor C, Zesiger P, Roulet-Perez E et al: Acquired epileptic dysgraphia: a longitudinal study. Dev
Med Child Neurol, 2003; 45: 807–12.
Baglietto MG, Battaglia FM, Nobili L et al: Neuropsychological disorders related to interictal epileptic
discharges during sleep in benign epilepsy of childhood with centrotemporal or rolandic spikes.
DMCN, 2001; 43: 407–12.
Kropotov JD, Grin-Yatsenko VA, Ponomarev VA et al: ERPs correlates of EEG relative beta training
in ADHD children. Int J Psychophysiol, 2005; 55(1): 23–34
Danielsson J, Petermann F. Cognitive deficits in children with benign rolandic epilepsy of childhood
or rolandic discharges: a study of children between 4 and 7 years of age with and without
seizures compared with healthy controls. Epilepsy Behav 2009 December;16(4):646-51.
Maquet P, Hirsch E, Metz-Lutz MN et al: Regional cerebral glucose metabolism in children with
deterioration of one or more cognitive functions and continuous spike-and-wave discharges
during sleep. Brain,1995; 118: 1497–520.
Pąchalska M., Kaczmarek B.L.J., Kropotov JuD. Neuropsychologia kliniczna: od teorii do praktyki.
Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Naukowe PWN, 2014.
Diaz-NegrilloA. Benign infantile epilepsy. A new case with positive response to levetiracetam. Jour-
nal of Pediatric Epilepsy 3 (2014) 117–119. DOI 10.3233/PEP-14084.
Holtmann M, Becker K, Kentner-Figura B, Schmidt MH. Increased frequency of rolandic spikes in
ADHD children. Epilepsia. 2003 Sep;44(9):1241-4.
Lubar, J. F: Discourse on the development of EEG diagnostics and biofeedback for attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorders. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 1991;16, 201-225
Monastra, V. J., Lubar, J. F., & Linden, M; The development of a quantitative electroencephalo-
graphic scanning process for attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder: Reliability and validity stud-
ies. Neuropsychology, 2001;15, 136-144.
Mirski A. et al. Neurofeedback training for Rolandic epilepsy
286
Martijn Arns, C. Keith Conners and Helena C. Kraemer A Decade of EEG Theta/Beta Ratio Re-
search in ADHD : A Meta-Analysis. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2013; 17: 374-383
Sterman, M. B: Basic concepts and clinical findings in the treatment of seizure disorders with EEG
operant conditioning. Clinical Electroencephalography, 2002; 64, 45–55.
Walker, J. E., & Kozlowski, G. P: Neurofeedback treatment of epilepsy. Child and Adolescent Psy-
chiatric Clinics of North America, 2005;14, 163–176.
Brown, J. W: Simultaneity and serial order. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 2010: 17, 7-40.
Pribram K: The Holographic Hypothesis of Brain Functioning, [In:] S. Grof (ed.): Ancient Wisdom,
Modern Science, State University of New York Press, 1984, s. 174-175.
Peat D.F:Synchronicity: The Bridge Between Matter and Mind: New York: Bantam,1987.
Peat D.F: A Flickering Reality: Cinema and the Nature of Reality, Pari Publishing, 2011.
Bloch, D. P: Complexity, Connections, and Soul-Work. Journal of Catholic Education, 2013; 11
(4): 543- 554.
Address for correspondence:
Maria Pachalska, M.D., Ph.D.
Chair of Neuropsychology,
Andrzej Frycz Modrzewski Krakow University,
Herlinga-Grudzinskiego 1
PL-30-705 Krakow, Poland
neuropsycholgia23@o2.pl
Mirski A. et al. Neurofeedback training for Rolandic epilepsy
287