ArticlePDF Available

Examination of Verb and Verb Phrase in Boier Ahmadi Lori Based on X-bar Syntax

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The big Chomskyan revolution took place by publication of Syntactic structures in 1957 but it evolved gradually during the time. In Syntactic structures, Chomsky introduced a grammar known as Generative Transformational Grammar which has many branches. One of its branches is Government and Binding theory / GB theory, sometimes called "Principle-and-parameters Theory", also has some branches. One of its branches is "X-bar syntax". It seems that X-bar syntax is able to describe verb and verb phrase in Boier Ahmadi Lori adequately. So, a free conversation of Boier Ahmadi Lori's informant was recorded. Then all of its simple sentences were analyzed based on X-bar syntax. The research comes up with this conclusion that X-bar syntax is not adequate enough to justify all characteristics of the verb and verb phrase in Boier Ahmadi Lori language.
Content may be subject to copyright.
International Journal of Linguistics
ISSN 1948-5425
2012, Vol. 4, No. 3
www.macrothink.org/ijl
509
Examination of Verb and Verb Phrase in Boier Ahmadi
Lori Based on X-bar Syntax
Fahime Mohammadpour (Corresponding author)
Faculty of Literature and Humanities, University of Sistan and Baluchestan
No. 19 Arash Alley, Arash street, Zahedan, Iran
Tel: 98-915-846-9037 E-mail: fahime_mohammadpour@yahoo.com
Received: June 19, 2012 Accepted: July 7, 2012 Published: September 1, 2012
doi:10.5296/ijl.v4i3.2375 URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.5296/ijl.v4i3.2375
Abstract
The big Chomskyan revolution took place by publication of Syntactic structures in 1957 but
it evolved gradually during the time. In Syntactic structures, Chomsky introduced a grammar
known as Generative Transformational Grammar which has many branches. One of its
branches is Government and Binding theory / GB theory, sometimes called „Principle-and-
parameters Theory‟, also has some branches. One of its branches is „X-bar syntax‟.
It seems that X-bar syntax is able to describe verb and verb phrase in Boier Ahmadi Lori
adequately. So, a free conversation of Boier Ahmadi Lori's informant was recorded. Then all
of its simple sentences were analyzed based on X-bar syntax. The research comes up with
this conclusion that X-bar syntax is not adequate enough to justify all characteristics of the
verb and verb phrase in Boier Ahmadi Lori language.
Keywords: Government and binding theory, X-bar syntax, Verb, Verb phrase, Head,
Complement, Adjunct
International Journal of Linguistics
ISSN 1948-5425
2012, Vol. 4, No. 3
www.macrothink.org/ijl
510
1. Introduction
In first days of emergence of generative transformational grammar, Chomsky assumed that
linguistic competence and linguistic performance must be separated. Since he supposed that
language is the mirror of mind (Darzi, 2007), he did his best to explain universal grammar/
UG in detail. “UG is a term used by Chomsky to point out to a theory about innate and
biological linguistic knowledge and this is the same as language Acquisition Device / LAD”
(Dabirmoghadam , 2006:15).
Government and Birding theory is known as the most formal theory in linguistics (Dabir
moghadam, 2006). Some may know it as Principle-and- parameters Theory. Government and
Binding theory is a theory about UG. Chomsky thinks that UG must have two conditions.
First, it must be compatible with all known facts about all languages (Radford, 1988:37). In
other words, it must be flexible enough to be able to explain diversity of languages. Second,
it must be contain a highly constrained set of principles, because Chomsky maintains that
“the evidence which the child has to rely on in acquiring his native Language (=the speech of
those around him) is degenerate (i.e. incomplete) and imperfect).” (ibid: 35) Chomsky
continues that:
In many cases that have been carefully studied in recent work, it is near certainty that
fundamental properties of the attained grammars are radically underdetermined by
evidence available to the language learner and must therefore be attributed to UG
[Universal Grammar] itself.
(Chomsky, 1981:3, cited in Radford, 1988)
So, it is expected that UG contains some universal and fundamental principles, which
constraints the structures of human languages and grammar that human brain can learn, and
also some parameters. In other words, parameters are fixed limits within which languages
seem to vary (Radford, 1988).
UG constitutes from „Deep-structure/D-structure‟, „Surface -structure/ S-structure‟, „logical
form‟, and „phonetic form‟ (Miremadi, 1999).
In Chomsky's view, bounding theory, government theory, theta theory, binding theory, case
theory, control theory and X-bar syntax are sub-branches of GB theory (Cook and Newson,
1997).
Since this research is based on X-bar syntax, only this theory is clarified.
1.1 X-bar Syntax
X-bar syntax is one of sub-branches of GB theory which examines the phrase structure rules
within a sentence. In fact, it illustrates the relationships present within each phrase of a
sentence (ibid). Each phrase may be made of one or more than one constituent. To display
relationships of constituents present in a phrase, a tree-diagram is drawn. Each tree-diagram
has some branches and nodes.
International Journal of Linguistics
ISSN 1948-5425
2012, Vol. 4, No. 3
www.macrothink.org/ijl
511
What X-bar syntax emphasizes at is that each phrase has a head, as its main part, and one or
more than one (pre- or post-) modifier. The modifiers can be either specifier or complement
or adjunct.
Complement of a head is its sister. Where head is presented, head and its complements
together form an x-bar constituent and that x-bar can be expanded by the addition of
appropriate specifiers into an x-double-bar constituent. The following tree-diagram shows
these above mentioned rules:
X''
Specifier (X)
X' Adjunct
X complement
Radford (1988:277) writes the following constituency rules for the above tree-diagram:
X'' x' , (Yp) (Specifier Rule)
X' x' , Yp (Adjunct Rule)
X' x , Yp* (Complement Rule)
To sum up, X bur syntax believes that there is an intermediate constituent which exists
between phrase-level and word-level.
1.2 Boier Ahmadi Lori:
Lori is one of clusters of Indo-Iranian languages with over four million speakers (Anonby,
2003). It has some dialects which are spoken by both settled and migratory folk over a large
area of western Iran, including parts of Hamadan Province (at least from Nehavand
southward) through Lorestan to Khuzestan, Chahar Maal and Batiari, Kohgiluya and Boier
Amadi, and Fars. They belong, together with Persian, to the southern branch of Western
Iranian (MacKinnon, 2011).
MacKinnon (Ibid) believes that the river Āb-e Dez which acts as the geographical dividing
line between Lori dialects has divided them naturally into two quite distinct groups
corresponding to the distinction between the so-called Greater Lors (Lor-e bozorg) and
Lesser Lors (Lor-e kuchek). Greater Lors' dialects include Batiari and Mamasani of the
International Journal of Linguistics
ISSN 1948-5425
2012, Vol. 4, No. 3
www.macrothink.org/ijl
512
Boier Amadi group, other Boier-Amadi, and Kohgiluya. On the other hand, Lesser Lors'
dialects are dialects spoken in settled communities of Khuzestan such as Ramhormoz and
Masjed Solayman.
Boier Ahmadi Lori dialect is selected to be examined based on X-bar Syntax.
2. Methodology
First of all, a free conversation of an informant was recorded. Then all of its simple sentences
were transcribed. After that, all sentences were analyzed based on X-bar syntax.
Radford (1988) puts forward some evidence in favor of positing V, V', and V'' constituents,
which will be explained and examined in Boier Ahmadi Lori dialect.
2.1 Verb Constituent
Radford (1988) assumes that generally each individual word, and especially each verb, can be
identified according to the range of inflections which it permits. So, verb‟s inflection forms in
Boier Ahmadi Lori will be reviewed.
Gholamalizadeh (1997:42) thinks that “verb, as a lexical category, is a linguistic element
which functions as head of a verb phrase. Thus, a verb may be representative of a sentence.”
Meshkatodini (2004) presents a framework for paradigms of verb in Persian, which is applied
for description of the verb „to eat‟ in Boier Ahmadi Lori.
Table 1. Present simple of xrdn „to eat‟
Person / number
Singular
Plural
1st person
ixrom
(„I eat‟)
ixrim
(„we eat‟)
2nd person
ixri
(„you eat‟ )
ixrit
(„you eat‟)
3rd person
ixre
(„he/she eats‟)
ixren
(„they eat‟)
International Journal of Linguistics
ISSN 1948-5425
2012, Vol. 4, No. 3
www.macrothink.org/ijl
513
Table 2. Present continuous of xrdn „to eat‟
Person / number
Singular
Plural
1st person
hey ixrom
(„I am eating‟)
hey ixrim
(„we are eating‟)
2nd person
hey ixri
(„you are eating‟ )
hey ixrit
(„you are eating‟)
3rd person
hey ixre
(„he/she is eating‟)
hey ixren
(„they are eating‟)
Table 3. Present Subjunctive mood of xardn „to eat‟
Singular
Plural
bexrom
(„I should eat‟)
bexrim
(„we should eat‟)
bexri
(„you should eat‟ )
bexrit
(„you should eat‟)
bexre
(„he/she should eat‟)
bexren
(„they should eat‟)
Table 4. Past simple of xardn „to eat‟
Person / number
Singular
Plural
1st person
xrdom
(„I ate‟)
xrdim
(„we ate‟)
2nd person
xrdi
(„you eat‟ )
xrdit
(„you ate‟)
3rd person
x
(„he/she ate‟)
xrden
(„they ate‟)
International Journal of Linguistics
ISSN 1948-5425
2012, Vol. 4, No. 3
www.macrothink.org/ijl
514
Table 5. Past continuous of xrdn „to eat‟
Person / number
Singular
Plural
1st person
(hey) ixrdom
(„I was eating‟)
(hey) ixrdim
(„we were eating‟)
2nd person
(hey) ixrdi
(„you were eating‟)
(hey) ixrdit
(„you were eating‟)
3rd person
(hey) ix
(„he/she was eating‟)
(hey) ixrden
(„they were eating‟)
Table 6. Present Subjunctive mood of xrdn „to eat‟
Person / number
Singular
Plural
1st person
xrd buyom
(„I have eaten‟)
xrd buim
(„we have eaten‟)
2nd person
xrd bui
(„you have eaten‟ )
xrd buit
(„you have eaten‟)
3rd person
xrd bu
(„he/she has eaten‟)
xrd buen
(„they have eaten‟)
Table 7. Present perfect of xrdn „to eat‟
Person / number
Singular
Plural
1st person
xrdeme
(„I have eaten‟)
xrdime
(„we have eaten‟)
2nd person
xrdi
(„you have eaten‟ )
xrdite
(„you have eaten‟)
3rd person
xrde
(„he/she has eaten‟)
xrdene
(„they have eaten‟)
International Journal of Linguistics
ISSN 1948-5425
2012, Vol. 4, No. 3
www.macrothink.org/ijl
515
Table 8. Past perfect of xrdn „to eat‟
Table 9. Imperative form of xrdn „to eat‟
Person / number
Singular
Plural
2nd person
bexa
(„eat ( you)‟ )
bexrit
(„eat( you)‟)
All the above verb inflections prove that V-constituent exists as Radford (1988) believes.
2.2 Verb Phrase
In Radford‟s view (1988), in every type of phrase, there is a head which is obligatory and
some other elements, which are optional and play the role of (pre-or post) modifier for the
head of phrase. Thus, a verb phrase is a phrase with a verb as its head. The verb phrase may
or may not have some modifiers.
He (ibid) gives some syntactic evidence to prove existence of verb phrase. They are applied
for Boier Ahmadi Lori:
2.2.1 Movement
“Only phrasal constituents can undergo movement (from one position in a sentence to
another)” (ibid: 72)
Person / number
Singular
Plural
1st person
xrd biyom
(„I had eaten‟)
xrd biim
(„we had eaten‟)
2nd person
xrd byi
(„you had eaten‟ )
xrd biit
(„you had eaten‟)
3rd person
xrd by
(„he/she had eaten‟)
xrd biyen
(„they had eaten‟)
International Journal of Linguistics
ISSN 1948-5425
2012, Vol. 4, No. 3
www.macrothink.org/ijl
516
1. a. / vo bi Ali mdres r /
He / she with Ali to the school went
„He/she and Ali went to the school‟
1. b. / mdres r vo bi Ali /
To the school went he / she with Ali
„He/she and Ali went to the school‟
2.2.2 Sentence-fragment:
“Only phrasal constituents (i.e. whole phrases) can serve as sentence-fragments (in an
appropriate context)” (ibid: 72)
2. a. SPEAKER A: what did he do?
2.b. SPEAKER B : / bi Ali mdres r /
With Ali to the school went
„He/she and Ali went to the school‟
2.2.3 Coordination
“Only identical categories can be conjoined, idiomatically.”
A) Ordinary Coordination
3.a. / Reza om. Reza esteraht ke/
Reza came. Reza slept.
3.b. / Reza om o Reza esteraht ke /
Reza came and Reza slept.
„Reza came and slept.‟
B) Shared Coordination
4.a. / mma qza pot. ima qza xrdim /
International Journal of Linguistics
ISSN 1948-5425
2012, Vol. 4, No. 3
www.macrothink.org/ijl
517
Mother the meal cooked. We the meal ate.
„Mother cooked the meal. We ate the meal. ‟
4.b. / mma qza pot o ima qza xrdim /
Mother the meal cooked and we the meal ate.
„Mother cooked the meal and we ate the meal. ‟
2.2.4 Pronominalisation
“Proforms generally replace phrase-level constituents, not word-level constituents.” (ibid:79)
5.a. / Zahra bayd rastgu bu. Maryam bayd rastgu bu/
Zahra must honest to be. Maryam must honest to be
„Zahra must be honest. Maryam must be honest. ‟
5.b. / Zahra bayd rastgu bu. Maryam hm hminto/
Zahra must honest to be. Maryam the same.
„Zahra must be honest. As must be Maryam. ‟
2.2.5 Ellipsis
6.a. / Ali nitre xub ranndegi kene. mo itrom xub ranndegi kenom./
Ali NEG-can well drive (a car). I can well drive (a car).
„Ali cannot drive a car well. I can drive a car well. ‟
6.b. / Ali nitre xub ranndegi kene. mo itrom ./
Ali NEG-can well drive (a car). I can.
„Ali cannot drive a car well. I can. ‟
2.3 Verb Phrase in Boier Ahmadi Lori
Verb phrase in Boier Ahmadi Lori, like other Iranian languages, is the most prominent
constituent of a sentence. This language, like Persian, is a pro-drop language. It means that
International Journal of Linguistics
ISSN 1948-5425
2012, Vol. 4, No. 3
www.macrothink.org/ijl
518
subject may or may not exist in this language. Instead, affix attached to the end of a verb is
person and number marker. Thus, many sentences can be observed in which a verb phrase is
representative of a whole sentence.
In Boier Ahmadi Lori (the same as Persian), verb phrase are endocentric structures (i.e.
structures where the category of a given phrase matches that of its head) (Stowell, 1981: 70;
cited in Radford, 1988:259) and they follow modifier maximality constraint (i.e. every
non-head term in the expansion of a rule must itself be a maximal projection some category)
(Ibid: 263).
Chomsky (1981) supposes that subject of a sentence play the role of „external argument‟ for
the main verb of the sentence and other arguments play the role of „internal argument‟.
Carnie (2002:79) reckons that:
Direct object is daughter of verb phrase;
Prepositional object is daughter of prepositional phrase.
The same is true for the Boier Ahmadi Lori. Look at the tree diagram of the following
sentences:
7. / extelaf-l vel ikenan/
Discrepancy-PLURAL care of
„ [They] do not care of discrepancies.‟
VP
NP V
extelafl vel ikenan
As it is evident, extelaf
l as direct object is dominated by verb phrase.
8. / men behtri mdreseye shhr kar gerotom /
In the best school of city work
„I worked in the best school of the city. ‟
International Journal of Linguistics
ISSN 1948-5425
2012, Vol. 4, No. 3
www.macrothink.org/ijl
519
VP
PP V
kar gerotom
P NP
men behtri mdreseye shhr
beht
ri m
dreseye sh
hr as prepositional object is daughter of the prepositional phrase.
However, strict adjacency principle, which of states that an NP complement must appear
strictly adjacent to its verb, is not true in this language:
9. / reshtem- xeili dus darom /
my field- OBJECT MARKER very much love
„I love my field of study very much. ‟
The above sentence shows that xeili which is the adjunct of the verb dus darom, has appeared
between verb and its complement, i.e. reshtem
. This flouts the strict adjacency principle.
2.3.1 Differences between complement and adjunct in verb phrase:
Radford (1988:233-38) puts forth some differences between complement and adjunct. These
differences are examined in Boier Ahmadi Lori:
Complement is within V' containing head V, but adjunct is outside of it.
This difference is true to some extent in Boier Ahmadi Lori, but there are some cases which
are not true. For example:
10. / srdste-ye goruy-al ostad- daxel-e goru diye/
head- POSSESIVE group-PLURAL the professor-OBJECT MARKER in department
seen
„The head of the group had seen the professor in the English Language Department.‟
International Journal of Linguistics
ISSN 1948-5425
2012, Vol. 4, No. 3
www.macrothink.org/ijl
520
VP
V'
NP V'
PP V
ostad- daxel-e goru diye
In this tree diagram, though daxele goru is adjunct of verb, it is within the V' containing the
head verb. On the contrary, ostad
is complete of verb but it is outside of the V' containing
the head verb.
Complement comes closer to the head than adjunct.
What is inferred from the analyzed data is that this difference is not always true for this
language. What makes this matter important is that if adjunct appears closer to the head than
complement, „No Crossing Branches Constraint‟ will be flouted. Look at the following:
11. /ey ka xbre peyda krdnesh vm n-idadn/
I wish the news of finding him to me NEG-give
„I wish they had not given the news to me that he had been found. ‟
The adjunct vm is closer to the head than the complement x
b
re peyda k
rd
nesh. So,
the tree-diagram of the sentence (11) will be as follows:
*VP
V'
NP
xbre peyda krdne V'
V
vm nidadn
International Journal of Linguistics
ISSN 1948-5425
2012, Vol. 4, No. 3
www.macrothink.org/ijl
521
Complement is obligatory but adjunct is optional. This is true for this language.
12.a. / qza -n bei qaoq xrdom /
the meal- OBJECT MARKER with spoon ate
„I ate the meal with the spoon.‟
Deletion of complement makes the sentence ungrammatical:
12.b.* / bei qaoq xrdom/
with spoon ate
*„I ate with the spoon.‟
But deletion of adjunct does not make it ungrammatical:
12.c. / qza -n xrdom /
the meal- OBJECT MARKER ate
„I ate the meal.‟
Ellipsis: under appropriate discourse conditions, a verb and all its complements, with or
without its adjuncts can be ellipsed. This matter is true for this language, too.
3. Results and Discussion
X-bar syntax, as one of sub-branches of Government and Binding theory, must be both
compatible with all known facts about language and also be flexible enough to explain
diversities of languages.
This research aimed to examine the adequacy of X-bar syntax for description of verb and
verb phrase in Boier Ahmadi Lori. The findings of this research suggest that hough X-bar
syntax is adequate for describing some of properties of Boier Ahmadi Lori's verb, there were
found some cases that X-bar syntax could not justify them at all. It seems that the reason lay
on the fact that this language has a flexible word order, while a language such as English has
a rigid one. Another reason may be that this language is a pro-drop language and as a result in
this language, a verb can play the role of a complete sentence with all its parts.
It is worth mentioning that the research was done on very small samples and the results of
this study are confined to Boier Ahmadi Lori. So, the same research on other languages may
produce different results. Indeed, to generalize results, more studies must be done.
The question remains that if X-bar syntax can present an adequate explanation of other
Iranian languages or not.
International Journal of Linguistics
ISSN 1948-5425
2012, Vol. 4, No. 3
www.macrothink.org/ijl
522
References
Anonby, E. J. (2003). Update on Luri: How many languages? Journal of the Royal Asiatic
Society (Third Series), 13(2), 171-197. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1356186303003067
Carnie, A. (Eds.). (2007). Syntax: A Generative Introduction. USA: Blackwell Publishing.
Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on Government and Binding. Foris: Dordrecht.
Cook, V. J., & Newson, M. (1997). Chomsky s Universal Grammar: An Introduction (2nd
Ed.). USA: Blackwell Publishing.
Dabir Moghadam, M. (2004). Theorical Linguistics: Emergence and Development of
Generative Grammar (2
nd
ed.). Tehran: Samt.
Darzi, A. (2007). Syntactic Argumentation. Tehran: Samt.
Gholamalizadeh, Kh. (1997). Persian's Structure. Tehran: Ehya Ketab.
MacKinnon, C. (2011). Lori Dialects. Iranica Journal. [Online] Available:
http://iranicaonline.org
Meshkatodini, M. (2004). Grammar of Persian (based on Transformational Grammar) (2nd
Ed.). Mashhad: Ferdowsi University of Mashhad Press.
Miremadi, A. (1999). Syntax of Persian (based on Government and Binding Theory). Tehran:
Samt.
Radford, A. (1988). Transformational Grammar: A First Course. USA: CAMBRIDGE
UNIVERSITY PRESS. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511840425
Stowell, T. (1981). Origins of Phrase Structure. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). MIT
Cambridge, Mass.
... In the short story "the setar," Nakhavaly (2012) concentrated on the analysis of grammatical voice. Mohammadpour (2012) investigated the verb and verb phrase in Boier Ahmadi lori using X-bar Syntax. Fu (2016) then turned his attention to semantic fuzziness and tts translation techniques. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study compares English and Buginese grammar constructions using Dixon's approach to assess the grammar of the STAB subtype. The researcher also wants to characterize and calculate the frequency of different sorts of categorical comparisons. This is a descriptive qualitative approach. Affected C (STAB Subtype) refers to a pointed or bladed Manip that penetrates below the surface of the Target, such as pierce, prick, stab, dig, sting, knife, spear; cut, prune, mow, saw, slice, chop, hack. In constructs I and III, all of these verbs appear. According to the Dixon's hypothesis, there are twelve Stab subtypes of verbs that can be used in textual construction, including stab, slice, cut, prune, mow, saw, chop, dig, pierce, prick, spear, and hack. There are six verbs in Buginese that pertain to the STAB subtype. Appacokengngi, angngampallasa, Meloka, Makkai, Nacui, and Matoddo' are the names of the animals.
... Nakhavaly (2012), focused on the analysis of grammatical voice in the short story "the setar". Mohammadpour (2012), concerned with examination of verb and verb phrase in Boier Ahmadi lori based on X-bar Syntax. Next, Fu (2016), focused on semantic fuzziness and tts translation strategies. ...
Article
This research focuses on Rub subtype verbs in and English Konjo language. The aims of the research were (1) to identify Rub subtype verbs in Konjo language and English. (2) to investigate the similarities and the differences of Rub subtype verbs in English and Konjo language in the aspect of semantic and grammatical construction. Furthermore, this research used descriptive qualitative method. English data were taken from COCA (Corpus of Contemporary American English). Meanwhile, Konjo language data were taken from observation and interview. Then, the results of this research indicated that (1) there are twelve Rub subtype verbs in English: rub, wipe, scrape, scratch, mark, sweep, brush, shave, rake, polish, lick and wash. Then, in Konjo language, there are nineteen verbs of Rub subtype: a’goso’, assossoro, angngampallasa, a’lu’lu’, angnga’ru’ akkankang, angngara’musu, annanrai, a’ca’ a’barrasa, assika’, accukkuru, kokkoro, assumer, a’lemong, angngemu’, a’bissa, assassa, and angngi’lasi. (2) The similiarities and the differences of Rub subtype verbs in English and Konjo language: The similarities are both of Rub subtype verbs in English and Konjo language can be applied in construction I (Agent + Target + Manip) and construction II (Agent + Manip + Target). The differences are the variant construction of English Rub subtype verbs and the extra constructions of Konjo language Rub subtype verbs. The variant construction of English Rub subtype verbs is variant construction Ib (Agent + Target (Adj) + Manip) and extra constructions of Konjo language are extra construction Ia (Agent + Target), Ib (Target + Agent), Ic (Agent + Target + Agent), Id (Agent), Ie (Target + Agent)*, IIa (Agent + Target + Manip)*, IIb (Manip + Agent + Target), IIIa (Manip + Target + Manip) and IIIb (Manip + Manip + Target).
Article
Luri is an Indo-Iranian language cluster with over four million speakers. The Luri ethnic area is found in southwestern Iran and southeastern Iraq, although smaller numbers of speakers have emigrated to elsewhere in Asia and a number of cities in the West.
Theorical Linguistics: Emergence and Development of Generative Grammar
  • M Dabir Moghadam
Dabir Moghadam, M. (2004). Theorical Linguistics: Emergence and Development of Generative Grammar (2 nd ed.). Tehran: Samt.
Grammar of Persian (based on Transformational Grammar
  • M Meshkatodini
Meshkatodini, M. (2004). Grammar of Persian (based on Transformational Grammar) (2nd Ed.). Mashhad: Ferdowsi University of Mashhad Press.
Chomsky s Universal Grammar: An Introduction
  • V J Cook
  • M Newson
Cook, V. J., & Newson, M. (1997). Chomsky s Universal Grammar: An Introduction (2nd Ed.). USA: Blackwell Publishing.
Syntactic Argumentation. Tehran: Samt
  • A Darzi
Darzi, A. (2007). Syntactic Argumentation. Tehran: Samt.
Origins of Phrase Structure. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation)
  • T Stowell
Stowell, T. (1981). Origins of Phrase Structure. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). MIT Cambridge, Mass.
Syntax of Persian (based on Government and Binding Theory)
  • A Miremadi
Miremadi, A. (1999). Syntax of Persian (based on Government and Binding Theory). Tehran: Samt.