Content uploaded by Susan Ebbels
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Susan Ebbels on Nov 16, 2016
Content may be subject to copyright.
Available via license: CC BY 3.0
Content may be subject to copyright.
INT J LANG COMMUN DISORD,JULY–AUGUST 2014,
VOL. 49, NO. 4, 377–380
Editorial
Introducing the SLI debate
Susan Ebbels
It is my great pleasure to introduce this special issue
on specific language impairment (SLI). The special
issue re-examines the diagnostic criteria for SLI and
questions whether the term ‘SLI’ should continue be
used as a diagnostic label for children with ‘unexplained
language problems’ (the term used by Bishop 2014 in
her lead article).
This special issue has come about because of increas-
ing dissatisfaction in many quarters with the wide vari-
ability in the diagnostic criteria used and the labels given
to children with unexplained language problems. This
variability is contributing to a lack of equity of access to
services and limited recognition and understanding of
children’s language problems both by the general pub-
lic and the scientific community (Bishop 2010). Recent
population studies (e.g., Tomblin et al. 1996, Reilly et al.
2010) allow examination of the validity of the diagnostic
criteria from a new perspective and thus reconsideration
of our use of these criteria is timely. The debate about
labels has been highlighted by the exclusion of SLI in the
recently published Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders (DSM-5) (American Psychiatric Asso-
ciation 2013), as recommended by the American Speech
and Hearing Association (ASHA). ‘Language Disorder’
is used instead. The International Classification of Dis-
eases and Related Health Problems (ICD-11), currently
in preparation, is also unlikely to use ‘SLI’.
In the UK, a live debate entitled ‘What is Specific
Language Impairment?’ in May 2012 raised several is-
sues about diagnostic criteria, labels and services. The
main issues regarding diagnosis were: the role of non-
verbal IQ in diagnosis of language problems, differential
diagnosis from autistic spectrum disorder (ASD) and
the label that should be used for children with unex-
plained language difficulties. The debate also revealed
that diagnostic labels and criteria were being used cre-
atively in disputes over access to services both by those
seeking to obtain services for children (often parents
and their lawyers) who could be accused of ‘diagnos-
tic shopping’ and also by those seeking to deny services
(often due to financial constraints) who may use particu-
larly restrictive criteria in order to reduce the number of
children qualifying for services (see also Wright 2014).
Videos and slides from the debate can been viewed at
http://www.moorhouseschool.co.uk/sli-debate/.
This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction
in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Following this live debate, discussions with the
Royal College of Speech and Language Thera-
pists (RCSLT) about how to broaden the de-
bate led to development of the SLI webpage
(http://www.rcslt.org/members/clinical_areas/SLI) and
a series of articles in the Bulletin from April to October
2013 (Bishop 2013, Clark et al. 2013, Dockrell 2013,
Ebbels 2013, Lascelles 2013, McCartney 2013, Norbury
2013, Slonims 2013) focusing on diagnosis of SLI, la-
bels used, how to meet language needs in the classrooms
and the parental perspective.
Widening the debate further was still high on the
agenda, thus I was delighted to learn that Sheena Reilly
had been invited to present the IJLCD winter lecture in
December 2013 and she was proposing to talk about
the diagnostic criteria and label of SLI. Meanwhile, the
RALLI (Raising Awareness of Language Learning Im-
pairments) team, who aim to raise public awareness
of children with unexplained language problems (see
http://www.youtube.com/RALLIcampaign), found that
terminology was a major stumbling block. As a result
of lack of progress in this area, Dorothy Bishop sug-
gested to the IJLCD editorial board that a special issue
on SLI with lead articles and commentaries from a range
of perspectives would be a good way to move the de-
bate forward. Thus, the special issue was born, with
two lead articles: one from Bishop entitled ‘Ten ques-
tions about terminology for children with unexplained
language problems’ and one from Reilly and colleagues
based on her winter lecture entitled: ‘Specific language
impairment: a convenient label for whom?’ Each article
is followed by 10 commentaries from a range of ex-
perts from several different countries commenting from
their various perspectives as academics, speech and lan-
guage therapists, educational psychologists, special ed-
ucational needs lawyers, and representatives of charities
working for and with children with unexplained lan-
guage problems and their parents. The discussion is
then continued in a response article jointly authored
by Reilly, Bishop and Bruce Tomblin entitled: ‘Termi-
nological debate over language impairment in children:
forward movement and sticking points’.
The articles and commentaries raise many important
issues about the diagnosis of children with unexplained
language problems. They discuss identification of a
International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders
ISSN 1368-2822 print/ISSN 1460-6984 online
C2014 The Authors International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of Royal College of Speech and Language Therapists
DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12119
378 Editorial
language problem and the validity of exclusionary crite-
ria. They argue that the precise criteria used may need
to be different for research versus identification of those
who need access to services (Bellair et al. 2014, Reilly
et al. 2014b). Indeed, Rutter (2014) discusses how di-
agnosis of a disorder is not tantamount to a need for
treatment, and Lauchlan and Boyle (2014) state that
there is little evidence that a diagnostic label dictates the
intervention a child should receive.
In terms of diagnostic criteria, even the identification
of a language problem is not straightforward. Should
language tests be used and if so, which tests, with which
cut-points? Reilly et al. (2014a) suggest using a cut-
point of –1.25 SD, with children scoring <–1 SD also
being monitored. Several commentators point out prob-
lems with this approach, e.g. many false-positives may
be identified (Norbury 2014) and standardized tests are
particularly unreliable for identifying bilingual children
(Bellair et al. 2014). There were also concerns that lim-
ited resources would be severely stretched by the identi-
fication of so many children (Norbury 2014, Parsons et
al. 2014). The addition of functional assessments is ad-
vocated by both lead articles and many commentators,
not only to reduce false-positives, but also to ensure
that children with functional difficulties have access to
services (Grist and Hartshorne 2014, Norbury 2014,
Snowling 2014, Whitehouse 2014). In terms of provi-
sion of services, some commentators stress the need to
consider the holistic profile of the child, the range of
needs and how these impact on each other (Bellair et al.
2014, Strudwick and Bauer 2014).
The validity and usefulness of exclusionary criteria
is a major focus of the articles in this special issue.
While both lead articles recognize there may be a
place for exclusionary criteria in some research studies
(although they stress that these ‘pure cases’ will not
then be representative of children in clinical contexts),
they both argue that use of most exclusionary criteria
is probably not justified for deciding who should
receive intervention. Most commentators agree. Indeed,
Dockrell and Lindsay (2014), see dropping exclusionary
criteria as a positive step towards a common language
between professionals and academics. However, some
commentators recommend that criteria previously used
to exclude a diagnosis of SLI should still be noted,
but should no longer exclude diagnosis (Rutter 2014,
Strudwick and Bauer 2014).
The evidence against using non-verbal IQ is dis-
cussed in some detail. Both lead articles and the com-
mentators agree that requiring a gap between non-verbal
IQ and language abilities (‘cognitive referencing’) should
be dropped, as its use is ‘largely discredited’ (Bishop
2014), ‘conceptually unsound’ (Reilly et al. 2014a) and
‘misinformed’ (Leonard 2014). However, there is dis-
agreement about whether there should be some mini-
mal level of non-verbal ability, and if so, what that level
should be. Hansson et al. (2014) state that a cut-off of
70 is used in most Swedish research on SLI.
In terms of what label to give to children with un-
explained language problems, the articles and commen-
taries are more mixed in their views. However, in their
response article, Reilly et al. (2014b) rule out three po-
tential labels: Language delay because it ‘implies eventual
catch-up in skills, which is not typically what is seen’
and is ‘often used to deny services to children’ (see also
Wright 2014); primary language impairment because it
is difficult to judge which condition is primary in a
child with more than one impairment (Conti-Ramsden
2014) and it could be confused with primary school-age
(Clark and Carter 2014); and language disorder because
it yields too many results unrelated to children’s un-
explained language problems when entered as a search
term (Bishop 2014).
Half of the commentators are in favour of dropping
the term SLI to reflect the relaxation of exclusionary
criteria which all agreed is required. But others feel that
changing the label risks breaking the link with past re-
search (Gallagher 2014, Rice 2014, Taylor 2014) and
prefer to keep the term, but revise the meaning of the
term ‘specific’ to mean ‘idiopathic’ (i.e., ‘of unknown
origin’; Bishop 2014). However, concern is expressed
that keeping the term ‘specific’ would encourage people
to continue to use inappropriate exclusionary criteria.
Of the remaining possible terms, language learning
impairment was viewed favourably by most, except par-
ents (H¨
uneke and Lascelles 2014). The fewest objections
were raised to developmental language disorder,wheredis-
order is used to refer to conditions without obvious aeti-
ology (Baird 2014), not to whether the child’s language
profile is ‘spikey’ or ‘flat’; a distinction which Reilly
et al. (2014b) say ‘has no validity as an indicator of
either aetiology or prognosis’.
Ultimately, Reilly et al. (2014b) argue in their re-
sponse article that we need a diagnostic label which
works for services, families and individuals. This is an
ambitious goal, but in order to achieve it, the authors
(and several of the commentators) call for an interna-
tional and multidisciplinary panel to be formed that
should aim to build consensus about first the diagnos-
tic criteria and second the diagnostic label. This panel
should take account of the views of families and people
with language problems and policy-makers and could
produce a position statement on the issue. In the mean-
time, we would like to solicit the views of readers of this
special issue and encourage you to join in the discussion
via www.rcslt.org/news/news/2014 news archive/ijlcd
discussion forum.
It has been a great pleasure to work with Dorothy
Bishop, Sheena Reilly and Bruce Tomblin while editing
this special issue and also to work with the 32 com-
mentators who between them provided 20 wonderfully
insightful commentaries. I thank them all for meeting
Editorial 379
extremely tight deadlines to enable this project to come
to fruition under a year from its inception.
Associate Editor, IJLCD
References
AMERICAN PSYCHIATRIC ASSOCIATION, 2013, Diagnostic and Statis-
tical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) (Arlington, VA:
American Psychiatric Association).
BAIRD, G., 2014, Lumping, splitting, drawing lines, statistical cut-
offs and impairment. Commentary on Bishop, D. V. M., Ten
questions about terminology for children with unexplained
language problems. International Journal of Language and
Communication Disorders,49, 381–415. doi: 10.1111/1460-
6984.12101
BELLAIR,J.,CLARK, S. and LYNHAM, S., 2014, Can any label work
for both intervention and research purposes? Commentary
on Bishop, D. V. M., Ten questions about terminology for
children with unexplained language problems. International
Journal of Language and Communication Disorders,49, 381–
415. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12101
BISHOP, D. V. M., 2010, Which neurodevelopmental disorders get
researched and why?, PLOS One,5, e15112.
BISHOP, D. V. M., 2013, SLI: the invisible neuro-developmental
disorder. RCSLT Bulletin,April(732), 14.
BISHOP, D. V. M., 2014, Ten questions about terminology for chil-
dren with unexplained language problems. International Jour-
nal of Language and Communication Disorders,49, 381–415.
CLARK, A. and CARTER, G., 2014, What should we call children
with unexplained language difficulties? A practical perspec-
tive. Commentary on Bishop, D. V. M., Ten questions about
terminology for children with unexplained language prob-
lems. International Journal of Language and Communication
Disorders,49, 381–415. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12101
CLARK,A.,CARTER,G.,CHALMERS,C.,ROSS, E. and SIMPSON,
S, 2013, Tangled terminology: what’s in a name? RCSLT
Bulletin,September(737), 20–21.
CONTI-RAMSDEN, G., 2014, What should we call children who
struggle to talk? Taking a developmental, global perspective
of diagnostic labels—reflections on Bishop (2014). Commen-
tary on Bishop, D. V. M., Ten questions on terminology for
children with unexplained language problems. International
Journal of Language and Communication Disorders,49, 381–
415. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12101
DOCKRELL, J., 2013, Meeting language needs in classrooms. RCSLT
Bulletin,August(736), 20–21.
DOCKRELL, J. and LINDSAY, G., 2014, The view from the chalk face.
Commentary on Reilly, S., Tomblin, B., Law, J., McKean, C.,
Mensah, F. K., Morgan, A., Goldfeld, S., Nicholson, J. M.
and Wake, M., 2014, Specific language impairment: a conve-
nient label for whom? International Journal of Language and
Communication Disorders,49, 416–451. doi: 10.1111/1460-
6984.12102
EBBELS, S. H., 2013, Guest Editorial: Introducing the Bulletin SLI
series. RCSLT Bulletin,April(732), 3.
GALLAGHER, A., 2014, What’s in a name? Some thoughts on Reilly
et al. (2014). Commentary on Reilly, S., Tomblin, B., Law,
J., McKean, C., Mensah, F. K., Morgan, A., Goldfeld, S.,
Nicholson, J. M. and Wake, M., 2014, Specific language im-
pairment: a convenient label for whom? International Journal
of Language and Communication Disorders,49, 416–451. doi:
10.1111/1460-6984.12102
GRIST,M.andHARTSHORNE, V. M., 2014, Please! No more spaghetti
....CommentaryonReilly,S.,Tomblin, B., Law, J., McK-
ean, C., Mensah, F. K., Morgan, A., Goldfeld, S., Nichol-
son, J. M. and Wake, M., 2014, Specific language impair-
ment: a convenient label for whom? International Journal of
Language and Communication Disorders,49, 416–451. doi:
10.1111/1460-6984.12102
HANSSON,K.,SANDGREN, O. and SAHLEN, B., 2014, Changing
labels for a concept in change. Commentary on Bishop,
D. V. M., Ten questions about terminology for children
with unexplained language problems. International Journal
of Language and Communication Disorders,49, 381–415. doi:
10.1111/1460-6984.12101
H¨
UNEKE, A. and LASCELLES, L., 2014, Terminology mayhem: why it
matters—the ramification for parents and families. Commen-
tary on Bishop, D. V. M., Ten questions about terminology
for children with unexplained language problems. Interna-
tional Journal of Language and Communication Disorders,49,
381–415. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12101
LASCELLES, L., 2013, The parental perspective. RCSLT Bulletin,
October(738), 19.
LAUCHLAN,F.andBOYLE, C., 2014, To label or not to label: Isthis the
question? Commentary on Bishop, D. V. M., Ten questions
about terminology for children with unexplained language
problems. International Journal of Language and Communica-
tion Disorders,49, 381–415. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12101
LEONARD, L., 2014, Replacing one imperfect term with another.
Commentary on Reilly, S., Tomblin, B., Law, J., McKean,
C., Mensah, F. K., Morgan, A., Goldfeld, S., Nicholson, J.
M. and Wake, M., 2014, Specific language impairment: a con-
venient label for whom? International Journal of Language and
Communication Disorders,49, 416–451. doi: 10.1111/1460-
6984.12102
MCCARTNEY, E., 2013, Specific language impairment in schools.
RCSLT Bulletin,June(734), 20–21.
NORBURY, C. F., 2013, Where are the boundaries? RCSLT Bulletin,
April(732), 12–13.
NORBURY, C. F., 2014, Language Impairment: where do we draw the
line? Commentary on Reilly, S., Tomblin, B., Law, J., McK-
ean, C., Mensah, F. K., Morgan, A., Goldfeld, S., Nichol-
son, J. M. and Wake, M., 2014, Specific language impair-
ment: a convenient label for whom? International Journal of
Language and Communication Disorders,49, 416–451. doi:
10.1111/1460-6984.12102
PARSONS,S.,JORDAN, E. and BRANAGAN, A., 2014, Changes to
specific language impairment: the service perspective. Com-
mentary on Reilly, S., Tomblin, B., Law, J., McKean, C.,
Mensah, F. K., Morgan, A., Goldfeld, S., Nicholson, J. M.
and Wake, M., 2014, Specific language impairment: a conve-
nient label for whom? International Journal of Language and
Communication Disorders,49, 416–451. doi: 10.1111/1460-
6984.12102
REILLY,S.,BISHOP,D.V.MandTOMBLIN, B., 2014b, Termino-
logical debate over language impairment in children: for-
ward movement and sticking points. International Journal of
Language and Communication Disorders,49, 452–562. doi:
10.1111/1460-6984.12111
REILLY,S.,TOMBLIN,B.,LAW ,J.,MCKEAN,C.,MENSAH,F.,MOR-
GAN,A.,GOLDFELD,S.,NICHOLSON, J. and WAKE,M.,
2014a, SLI: a convenient label for whom? International Jour-
nal of Language and Communication Disorders,49, 416–451.
doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12102
REILLY,S.,WAKE,M.,UKOUMUNNE,C.O.,BAV IN,E.,PRIOR,M.,
CINI,E.,CONWAY,L.,EADIE, P. and BRETHERTON, L., 2010,
Predicting language outcomes at 4 years of age: findings from
Early Language in Victoria Study. Pediatrics,126(6), e1530–
e1537.
RICE, M. L, 2014, Advocating for SLI. Commentary on Bishop,
D. V. M., Ten questions about terminology for children
380 Editorial
with unexplained language problems. International Journal
of Language and Communication Disorders,49, 381-415. doi:
10.1111/1460-6984.12101
RUTTER, M., 2014, Classification purposes and diagnostic concepts.
Commentary on Reilly, S., Tomblin, B., Law, J., McKean, C.,
Mensah, F. K., Morgan, A., Goldfeld, S., Nicholson, J. M.
and Wake, M., 2014, Specific language impairment: a conve-
nient label for whom? International Journal of Language and
Communication Disorders,49, 416-451. doi: 10.1111/1460-
6984.12102
SLONIMS, V., 2013, A test for the profession. RCSLT Bulletin,
June(734), 22.
SNOWLING, M. J., 2014, SLI—not just a semantic issue. Commen-
tary on Reilly, S., Tomblin, B., Law, J., McKean, C., Mensah,
F. K., Morgan, A., Goldfeld, S., Nicholson, J. M. and Wake,
M., 2014, Specific language impairment: a convenient label
for whom? International Journal of Language and Communica-
tion Disorders,49, 416–451. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12102
STRUDWICK, M. and BAUER, A., 2014, Getting behind the la-
bel: practitioners’ point of view. Commentary on Bishop,
D. V. M., Ten questions about terminology for children
with unexplained language problems. International Journal
of Language and Communication Disorders,49, 381–415. doi:
10.1111/1460-6984.12101
TAYLOR , C. L., 2014, The SLI construct is a critical link to the
past and a bridge to the future. Commentary on Bishop,
D. V. M., Ten questions about terminology for children
with unexplained language problems. International Journal
of Language and Communication Disorders, 49, 381–415. doi:
10.1111/1460-6984.12101
TOMBLIN,J.B.,RECORDS, N. and ZHANG, X., 1996, A system for
the diagnosis of specific language impairment in kindergarten
children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research,39(6), 1284–
1294.
WHITEHOUSE, A. J. O., 2014, Refining language impairment: re-
searchers must pay their part too. Commentary on Reilly, S.,
Tomblin,B.,Law,J.,McKean,C.,Mensah,F.K.,Morgan,A.,
Goldfeld, S., Nicholson, J. M. and Wake, M., 2014, Specific
language impairment: a convenient label for whom? Interna-
tional Journal of Language and Communication Disorders,49,
416–451. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12102
WRIGHT, E., 2014, Special educational needs provision in the real
world. Commentary on Reilly, S., Tomblin, B., Law, J., McK-
ean, C., Mensah, F. K., Morgan, A., Goldfeld, S., Nichol-
son, J. M. and Wake, M., 2014, Specific language impair-
ment: a convenient label for whom? International Journal of
Language and Communication Disorders,49, 416–451. doi:
10.1111/1460-6984.12102